11.07.2015 Views

RUNNING HEAD: The IAT in Consumer Behavior - Wharton Marketing

RUNNING HEAD: The IAT in Consumer Behavior - Wharton Marketing

RUNNING HEAD: The IAT in Consumer Behavior - Wharton Marketing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>The</strong> <strong>IAT</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Consumer</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> 15slogans that potentially differ <strong>in</strong> valence, one would expect differential responses to thoseslogans to the extent that the viewer of those slogans was able to discern both mean<strong>in</strong>gs.To test these hypotheses, three versions of an experimental advertisement werecreated us<strong>in</strong>g two well-known automobile brands: Lexus and Mercedes-Benz. <strong>The</strong>advertisements were identical <strong>in</strong> there presentation of comfort and performanceattributes, but differed <strong>in</strong> the slogans that were <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the advertisements: “Unlikeany other” (literal positive), “No one comes close” (polysemous mixed, such that thesecondary negative read<strong>in</strong>g suggests that it is too expensive and thus unatta<strong>in</strong>able for thevast majority of automobile buyers) and “For the few who can afford it” (literalnegative). <strong>The</strong> <strong>IAT</strong> was used to measure subjects’ automobile brand associations withattributes related to “expensive” versus “affordable.” Consistent with expectations,Dimofte and Yalch found that subjects who were better able to access multiple mean<strong>in</strong>gsof the slogans exhibited a significant “Mercedes + Expensive” association suggest<strong>in</strong>g thatthese subjects implicitly understood the secondary slogan mean<strong>in</strong>gs. On the other hand,subjects who were unable to access the multiple mean<strong>in</strong>gs of the slogans did not appearto process the secondary, implicit mean<strong>in</strong>g of the slogan, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that differentcognitive processes were occurr<strong>in</strong>g between the groups.A follow-up experiment by Dimofte and colleagues (Dimofte, Yalch, &Greenwald, 2003) suggested that <strong>in</strong>cidental exposure to an object could produce novelimplicit associations with that object. A particular brand name (Trojan) that could bothrepresent a “party”-related product and the mascot of a major American university waschosen as a stimulus item. After <strong>in</strong>cidental exposure to the brand name and logo,subsequent implicit associations of that specific university and the concept of “party”

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!