12.07.2015 Views

The effects of syntactic and lexical complexity on the comprehension ...

The effects of syntactic and lexical complexity on the comprehension ...

The effects of syntactic and lexical complexity on the comprehension ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2011, 4(1), 107-125.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>elementary science textsDiana J. ARYA ∗University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oslo, NorwayElfrieda H. HIEBERTTextProject <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> California, Santa Cruz, United StatesP. David PEARSONUniversity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> California, Berkeley, United StatesAbstractIn this study we examined <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> third-gradestudents' comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science texts. A total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 16 expository texts were designed to representsystematic differences in levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> across four science-related topics(Tree Frogs, Soil, Jelly Beans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Toothpaste). A Latin-square design was used to counterbalance <strong>the</strong>order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> administrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se 16 texts. After reading each text, students resp<strong>on</strong>ded to a post-testcomprehensi<strong>on</strong> measure (without access to <strong>the</strong> text). External measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading achievement <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>prior vocabulary knowledge were also ga<strong>the</strong>red to serve as c<strong>on</strong>trol variables. Findings show that<str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> had a significant impact <strong>on</strong> students' comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four topics.Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> performance was not influenced by <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts, regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>topic. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, no additi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> were found for English language learners. Potentially moderating<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>founding issues, such as <strong>the</strong> inference dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>topic familiarity, are discussed in order to explain <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>sistency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> findings across topics.Keywords: Text <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, science literacyIntroducti<strong>on</strong>Recently, scholars have highlighted <strong>the</strong> need for increased attenti<strong>on</strong> to informati<strong>on</strong>al texts inelementary schools, especially primary-level classrooms (D<strong>on</strong>ovan & Smolkin, 2001; Duke,2000). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> argument for this shift in textual diet is that increased attenti<strong>on</strong> to informati<strong>on</strong>altexts will improve many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> things that matter in students’ later development: world∗Diana J. Arya, Norwegian Center for Science Educati<strong>on</strong> at <strong>the</strong> University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oslo, Norway.E-mail: d.j.arya@naturfagsenteret.noISSN:1307-9298Copyright © IEJEEwww.iejee.com


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>knowledge, m<strong>on</strong>itoring <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> problem-solving strategies, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dispositi<strong>on</strong>s toward academicreading. While all disciplines have benefited from this shift in emphasis, science has received<strong>the</strong> most attenti<strong>on</strong>. For example, in 2010, an entire special issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Journal, Science, wasdevoted to <strong>the</strong> literacy-science interface—a remarkable departure for a public access journalthat normally focuses <strong>on</strong> research <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> policy for <strong>the</strong> hard sciences. Science requiresnumerous firsth<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experiences; however, appropriate texts can have a critical role inscience learning (Cervetti & Barber, 2009; Guthrie, McRae & Klauda, 2007). Science textsprovide readers with a purpose for reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> additi<strong>on</strong>al exposure to key science c<strong>on</strong>ceptsthat lead to deeper c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing (Guthrie, Anders<strong>on</strong>, Alao, & Rinehart, 1999;Palincsar & Magnuss<strong>on</strong>, 2001; Romance & Vitale, 1992; 2006).Although <strong>the</strong> academic benefits <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science texts are evident, <strong>the</strong>y pose challenges toteaching <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning. In particular, <strong>the</strong> vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science texts can be dense <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>complex (Armstr<strong>on</strong>g & Collier, 1990; Schleppegrell, 2004; Snow, 2010). Elementary sciencetexts have been criticized for being inaccessible because <strong>the</strong>y introduce <strong>the</strong> reader to manyunfamiliar words yet fail to explain <strong>the</strong>m in ways that c<strong>on</strong>nect with students’ experiences(Armbruster, 1993; Armstr<strong>on</strong>g & Collier, 1990; Norris & Phillips, 2003; Ru<strong>the</strong>rford, 1991). One<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> benefits <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> having a science text is to help clarify <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> extend scientific c<strong>on</strong>cepts thatstudents encounter during firsth<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> investigati<strong>on</strong>s (Duke & Bennett-Armistead, 2003;D<strong>on</strong>ovan & Smolkin, 2001). However, for young students who are still developing literacyskills, as well as academic vocabulary, science texts c<strong>on</strong>taining unfamiliar terms can be verydifficult to comprehend.In <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student science texts, <strong>the</strong>re is a tensi<strong>on</strong> between c<strong>on</strong>ceptualexplicitness (which <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten requires more complex <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>al realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rare, c<strong>on</strong>ceptorientedvocabulary words) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic simplicity (which generally requires less complex<str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> simpler vocabulary). Matters <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> were salient in<strong>the</strong> text comprehensi<strong>on</strong> research <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 1970s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> even into <strong>the</strong> early 1980s (Pears<strong>on</strong> &Camparell, 1981), but text structure yielded to o<strong>the</strong>r emphases, most notablycomprehensi<strong>on</strong> strategy work, in <strong>the</strong> 1990s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early 2000s (see Pears<strong>on</strong>, 2009). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> need tore-examine <strong>the</strong>se factors is greater than ever in light <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two recent developments. First <strong>the</strong>dramatic increase in <strong>the</strong> numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students with diverse linguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> culturalbackgrounds (US Census, 2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> challenges many linguistically diverse studentsexperience <strong>on</strong> tasks such as <strong>the</strong> NAEP Science assessment (Gutierrez & Rog<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, 2003; Lee, &Luykx, 2005; Shaw, 1997) requires us to take a closer look at text features that may proveespecially challenging or supportive for English language learners. A major challenge isidentifying text features that make informati<strong>on</strong> more accessible for ELLs. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong> advent<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> new Comm<strong>on</strong> Core State St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards in English language arts (CCSS, 2010) has upped<strong>the</strong> ante <strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>; educators are being challenged by <strong>the</strong>se st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardsto increase <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts students read at every grade level by at least a half gradein measured readability. This means that all students are going to be asked to read texts withmore complex syntax <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more difficult vocabulary. In this study, we investigate <strong>the</strong> extentto which <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g>ly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally complex realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent hinder or helpcomprehensi<strong>on</strong>—<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se two factors interact to provide ei<strong>the</strong>r unique scaffoldsor barriers to acquiring important science c<strong>on</strong>cepts.Gauging Text ComplexitySyntactic Complexity. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> “simple” view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> evident in readabilityformulas such as <strong>the</strong> Flesch Ease <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading formula (Flesch, 1948) holds that <strong>the</strong> fewer wordsin a sentence, <strong>the</strong> less difficult it is for readers to comprehend (Klare, 1984). This perspective,however, may be misleading; more words may simply be an alias for more ideas or, even108


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>more likely, more complex ideas. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, c<strong>on</strong>ceptual <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> could be drivingdifficulty, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in a sentence simply indexes (ra<strong>the</strong>r than causes) that<str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In psychological terms, <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>ger <strong>the</strong> sentence, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> likelihood thatmultiple discrete ideas, called propositi<strong>on</strong>s, are embedded in it (Kintsch, 1998). Examples 1-3illustrate this point.1. Tree frogs have red eyes.2. Tree frogs have red eyes that help <strong>the</strong>m see <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> find food.3. Tree frogs have red eyes that help <strong>the</strong>m see <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> find food at night.Example 1 c<strong>on</strong>veys two complete ideas or propositi<strong>on</strong>s: (a) tree frogs have eyes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (b)<strong>the</strong>se eyes are red. Example 2 has two additi<strong>on</strong>al propositi<strong>on</strong>s: <strong>the</strong>se red eyes help <strong>the</strong> frogto (a) see food <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (b) to find food. Example 3 actually adds three more propositi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>on</strong>eexplicit <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two implicit. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit propositi<strong>on</strong> is that <strong>the</strong> frogs do <strong>the</strong> seeing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> findingat night. That propositi<strong>on</strong> invokes two more entailments: that (a) that frogs are awake atnight <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (b) that <strong>the</strong>ir eyes help <strong>the</strong>m to see in <strong>the</strong> dark. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicitness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>informati<strong>on</strong> provided in each sentence increases as <strong>the</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded structures(e.g., adjectives, relative clauses, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> prepositi<strong>on</strong>al phrases) increases. Readers must be ableto unpack <strong>the</strong> propositi<strong>on</strong>s within complex sentences <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> establish <strong>the</strong>ir logical relati<strong>on</strong>s to<strong>on</strong>e ano<strong>the</strong>r to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> presented.Any account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text difficulty that uses sentence length to establish <strong>the</strong> readability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>texts assumes, at least implicitly, that unpacking <strong>the</strong> propositi<strong>on</strong>s within a complex sentenceis more difficult than making c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s across related propositi<strong>on</strong>s stated in simplesentences. A short sentence in itself may be easier to comprehend than a complex <strong>on</strong>e.However, <strong>the</strong> challenge may come when <strong>the</strong> reader needs to c<strong>on</strong>struct a cohesi<strong>on</strong> model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>meaning from a series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> short sentences. To illustrate this distincti<strong>on</strong>, a complex sentencesuch as <strong>the</strong> third example above can be broken up into five simple sentences, as in Example4:4. Tree frogs have eyes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se eyes are red. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se eyes help <strong>the</strong>m see. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y help <strong>the</strong>mfind food. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> tree frogs are awake at night.Just as <strong>the</strong> complex sentence required readers to unpack propositi<strong>on</strong>s within <strong>the</strong> sentence,having to c<strong>on</strong>nect ideas across discrete, simple sentences may place o<strong>the</strong>r task dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong>readers. C<strong>on</strong>nective cues (e.g., c<strong>on</strong>junctives, c<strong>on</strong>junctive adverbs, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative clauses) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r embedded structures serve as markers to guide readers to a full underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ideas presented. Eliminating <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>nective cues may increase <strong>the</strong> inference burden <strong>on</strong>readers (Bowey, 1986; Pears<strong>on</strong> & Camperell, 1981); relati<strong>on</strong>ships, such as cause-effect orproblem-soluti<strong>on</strong>, or sequence, that were explicitly cued in <strong>the</strong> more complex versi<strong>on</strong>s haveto be inferred in <strong>the</strong> less complex versi<strong>on</strong>s. Ozuru, Dempsy, Sayroo <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> McNamara (2005)found that adding cohesive devices such as c<strong>on</strong>nectives that made relati<strong>on</strong>ships betweensentences more explicit were beneficial for students reading science texts about unfamiliartopics. Students were able to correctly answer more questi<strong>on</strong>s when texts had <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>structures that made meaning more explicit than when texts were less cohesive. Similarly,Raws<strong>on</strong> (2004) found that texts that presented more ambiguous <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> structures withunmarked, reduced relative clauses (<strong>the</strong> girls told about <strong>the</strong> movie were excited) were moredifficult for college students (all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whom had high reading abilities) than texts with moreexplicit structures, c<strong>on</strong>taining marked clauses (<strong>the</strong> girls who were told about <strong>the</strong> movie wereexcited).While some <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> in sentences can support readers in comprehending text,presumably <strong>the</strong>re is a point where sentences can become too complex for novice or109


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>inexperienced readers. Several factors likely influence where this tipping point occurs.Developmental level <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reader pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency appear to be two such factors in that olderreaders <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient readers dem<strong>on</strong>strate greater comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammaticallycomplex structures than younger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> less pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient readers (Nati<strong>on</strong> & Snowling, 2000;Willows & Ryan, 1986).Background knowledge or c<strong>on</strong>ceptual familiarity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> topic may also influence readers’abilities to comprehend <strong>the</strong> embedded structures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex sentences. Goldman <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Bisanz (2002) reported that novice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> less pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient readers who did not have backgroundknowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a topic were less able than more knowledgeable or more pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient readers toavail <strong>the</strong>mselves <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded structural cues. However, McNamara, Kintsch, S<strong>on</strong>ger, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Kintsch (1996) found that science texts c<strong>on</strong>taining greater number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded structuresthat clarify or highlight informati<strong>on</strong> (e.g. use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>nectives or embedded explanati<strong>on</strong>s)benefit readers with less knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts whereas texts with “cohesive gaps” (i.e.,fewer c<strong>on</strong>nectives <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded clauses) that require students to make inferences aboutrelati<strong>on</strong>ships <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>cepts benefit readers with a str<strong>on</strong>g level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> prior knowledge(McNamara, 2001; McNamara et al., 1996); in short, less knowledgeable readers are aided by<strong>the</strong> scaffolding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit cues but more knowledgeable readers are aided by <strong>the</strong> challenge<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a text that needs “fixing”. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current study attempts to address this c<strong>on</strong>flict. Thus when itcomes to <strong>the</strong> issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, a trade-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f may well exist: What is made moreeasily accessible by <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> (seeing <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g propositi<strong>on</strong>s) is made moreesoteric by simplicity. What is made easier to comprehend by simplicity (getting unitaryideas through <strong>the</strong> veil <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> working memory) is rendered complex by <strong>the</strong> additi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>embedded structures.Lexical Complexity. Sentence length, typically an alias for <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten,indeed almost universally, coupled with vocabulary difficulty in readability formulas in orderto determine overall accessibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a given text (Flesch, 1948, 1979; Lenn<strong>on</strong> & Burdick, 2004).Vocabulary difficulty is generally indexed by how frequently a particular word generallyappears in texts (Zeno, Ivens, Millard, & Duvvuri, 1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> assumpti<strong>on</strong> is that <strong>the</strong> moreexposures a reader has to a particular word, <strong>the</strong> more a reader learns about it <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in turn,<strong>the</strong> more accessible that word (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> message in which it is embedded) becomes.Indicators <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiarity have l<strong>on</strong>g been used to estimate <strong>the</strong> readability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text (e.g.,Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998; Snow & Sweet, 2003; Stahl, 1999).Word frequency is str<strong>on</strong>gly correlated with word knowledge, which is a crucial aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (NICHD, 2000; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002); simply put, <strong>the</strong>more frequently a word occurs in a language <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> likelihood that students willknow its meaning. Research <strong>on</strong> vocabulary suggests that texts c<strong>on</strong>taining few unknownwords provide readers with an appropriate source from which to develop fluency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> wordknowledge (Beck & McKeown, 1991; Qian, 2002; Vellutino, 2003). Thus, <strong>the</strong> more studentsread texts with very few rare words, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong>ir chances in developing a solidunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> unfamiliar c<strong>on</strong>cepts that are present, allowing <strong>the</strong>m to comprehendtexts with additi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong> future (Nagy & Scott, 2000; Stanovich, 2000).However, it may be argued that <strong>the</strong> more frequent a word, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> likelihood that,while students will know its meaning, its meaning may be less precise (Carey, 1985; Gopnik,1996). This may especially be so with words in science where a less frequent word such asastr<strong>on</strong>aut c<strong>on</strong>veys a level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> precisi<strong>on</strong> that a generic word like man does not. C<strong>on</strong>versely, toomany unfamiliar or complex vocabulary words within science texts may inhibit readers’ability to learn c<strong>on</strong>cepts through reading (Shymansky, Yore, & Good, 1991; Stahl, 1999). Weexpect students to infer word meanings from c<strong>on</strong>text; it is a required part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> skilled, strategicreading. However, if <strong>the</strong>re are too many unknown words in <strong>the</strong> surrounding c<strong>on</strong>text, <strong>the</strong>re110


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>may be no meaning base from which a student could infer <strong>the</strong> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a particular word.C<strong>on</strong>trast <strong>the</strong> challenge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inferring <strong>the</strong> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> habitat in Examples X <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Y:X. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> soil in <strong>the</strong> alluvial plane, rich in nutrients <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> decomposers, provided an optimalhabitat for our earthworms.Y. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> soil al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> river provided a good habitat for our earthworms.Science texts are purported to have more than twice <strong>the</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> rare words as textsfrom any o<strong>the</strong>r discipline, thus creating a vexing challenge for developers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science literacycurricula: How can <strong>the</strong>y create c<strong>on</strong>siderate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> accessible texts for young readers that alsodo justice to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts students are supposed to acquire (Hayes & Ahrens, 1988)? Just aswith <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <strong>the</strong>re is a potential trade <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f in <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Rare words havea level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> precisi<strong>on</strong> that high frequency words do not. However, <strong>the</strong> presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> too manyrare words may make a text inaccessible to readers.Vocabulary familiarity (<str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>) has a direct relati<strong>on</strong>ship to readers’ knowledge about<strong>the</strong> topic, which has a great impact <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (Kintsch, 1998; RAND Reading StudyGroup, 2002; Smagorinsky, 2001; Snow & Sweet, 2003; Stahl, 1999). As <strong>on</strong>e becomes morefamiliar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experienced with a topic, knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>textualized meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> wordsdevelops as well (Anders<strong>on</strong> & Freebody, 1981; Kintsch, 1998). In experiments that useassociati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming tasks, skilled readers have been found to approach a text with anorganized network <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> knowledge called schemata. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se allow readers to integrate newinformati<strong>on</strong> with prior knowledge (Kintsch, 1998; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002;Smagorinsky, 2001; Snow & Sweet, 2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in <strong>the</strong> process, enhance <strong>the</strong>ir schemata evenmore. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> str<strong>on</strong>ger <strong>on</strong>e’s prior knowledge about a particular subject, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>on</strong>e’s abilityto read <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehend texts quickly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> efficiently (Kintsch, 1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s thatreaders make with text are dependent <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge base <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ability to retrieve <strong>the</strong>most relevant meaning from alternatives in <strong>the</strong>ir mental lexic<strong>on</strong>s (Kintsch, 1998;Smagorinsky, 2001; Wils<strong>on</strong> & Sperber, 1987).Just as students’ prior knowledge about particular c<strong>on</strong>cepts facilitates comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, alack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> knowledge about c<strong>on</strong>cepts within a text can have a detrimental impact <strong>on</strong>underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing. Bailey (2007) c<strong>on</strong>ducted a language analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> American st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardizedachievement tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> found that academic language (i.e., words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten used in tests such asexamine or cause) c<strong>on</strong>founds <strong>the</strong> ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English Language Learners (ELLs) to dem<strong>on</strong>strate<strong>the</strong>ir underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct that is being assessed in English. Similarly, Droop <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Verhoeven (1998) found in <strong>the</strong>ir study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> third grade students learning Dutch as a first orsec<strong>on</strong>d language that <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> (defined in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word frequency) as well ascultural relevance impacts text comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. However nei<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se studies examined<strong>the</strong> impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> or its interacti<strong>on</strong> with <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> in academiclanguage.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Current Study<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> aim <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> present investigati<strong>on</strong> was to compare <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> students’ underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science c<strong>on</strong>tent. Students’ comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> textswas examined as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> (simple, complex) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> (simple, complex); additi<strong>on</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> main <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>interacti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> were examined through <strong>the</strong> lenses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>reading ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> prior knowledge.Language status was also c<strong>on</strong>sidered as a potential c<strong>on</strong>founding factor <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se texts. Text accessibility is an important issue for ELLs because <strong>the</strong>ymust have <strong>the</strong> opportunity to read extensively in texts at <strong>the</strong>ir level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading ability in order111


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>to improve comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluency (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998; Elley, 1996; Grabe,1991; Snowling & Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1997). However, few studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> readability have investigated <strong>the</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text difficulty <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ELLs. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, no such study has focused <strong>on</strong>both <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> while holding issues <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural relevance c<strong>on</strong>stant.Specifically, <strong>the</strong> following questi<strong>on</strong>s are addressed in this investigati<strong>on</strong>:1. Do <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> affect comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science texts for thirdgraders?2. How do <strong>the</strong>se two forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> interact to produce unique combinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>?3. Are <strong>the</strong>re any additi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> for ELLs?We anticipated that, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a given science text (as measured byembedded clauses <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> difficult vocabulary), <strong>the</strong> more skilled a reader must be to successfullyunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text. Thus, scores <strong>on</strong> general reading assessments such as informal readinginventories <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> state tests should predict scores <strong>on</strong> an assessment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>science c<strong>on</strong>tent. Based <strong>on</strong> a l<strong>on</strong>g history <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> readability research, we also hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that<str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> might have a greater impact <strong>on</strong> performance than <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>,but that <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> would have <strong>the</strong> most debilitatingeffect <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. In short, <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> very best readers as defined by reading testscores would be able to h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>le <strong>the</strong> difficulty imposed by texts that are complex <strong>on</strong> both<str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> criteria.Questi<strong>on</strong>s about <strong>the</strong> manner in which prior knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>influence students’ comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> how <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>structs c<strong>on</strong>trast with <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> merit particular attenti<strong>on</strong> with science texts. It is possible that, when complexideas are communicated with accessible (i.e., high frequency) vocabulary, <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> does not matter as much as it does when technical (low frequency) vocabulary isused. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, a reader’s experience with certain subject matter may determine <strong>the</strong> degree towhich <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, or both affect underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing.MethodParticipantsThis study included all 142 third-graders who had returned parental c<strong>on</strong>sent in 10classrooms in four n<strong>on</strong>-charter public schools. According to California state regulati<strong>on</strong>soperating at <strong>the</strong> time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong>, no K-3 classroom could enroll more than 20students. An average <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 14.3 students per classroom (minimum = 12, maximum = 19), whichwas approximately 75% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total third grade enrollment across <strong>the</strong>se schools, participated in<strong>the</strong> study. All four schools were located in nor<strong>the</strong>rn California <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> varied according tourbanicity, ethnicity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ELLs, defined by language spoken at home. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>reas<strong>on</strong> for this ELL distincti<strong>on</strong> is that three <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> participating schools had no Englishlanguage program, thus having no school language designati<strong>on</strong> for students who arelearning English as a sec<strong>on</strong>d language. This informati<strong>on</strong> was reported by <strong>the</strong> participants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>firmed by <strong>the</strong> parental c<strong>on</strong>sent letters. Students who spoke <strong>on</strong>ly English at home werenot c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be ELL. All o<strong>the</strong>r students (49 students, 34% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total participants) werec<strong>on</strong>sidered to be ELL. Of <strong>the</strong> 49 ELL students, 23 (16%) spoke <strong>on</strong>ly Spanish at home while 11(8%) spoke a mix <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> remaining 15 students (11%) spoke <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>various Asian or European languages at home.Two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four participating schools (3 classrooms total) were urban, while <strong>on</strong>e waslocated within a suburban area (<strong>on</strong>e classroom) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e is rural (6 classrooms total). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>112


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>four schools ranged in percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ethnic minority (i.e., o<strong>the</strong>r than White, 44%--73%) aswell as language minority (o<strong>the</strong>r than English, 12%--59%) students. Half <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> totalpopulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> participants were Caucasian (71 students) while <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r half representedHispanic (44 students, 30% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total participants), African American (15 students, 10% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> totalparticipants), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Asian (eight students, 6% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total participants) ethnicities with a remainingfew (five students, 4% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total participants) representing o<strong>the</strong>r or mixed ethnicities. Note thatSES was not reported for this study: as <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2005, it is illegal to access this particulardemographic informati<strong>on</strong> according to California state regulati<strong>on</strong>s, even for classroomteachers, regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sent or approval <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> university’s internal review board.In <strong>the</strong> initial phase <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> study, each participant read a narrative passage orally from <strong>the</strong>Qualitative Reading Inventory (QRI) (Leslie & Caldwell, 2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> answered questi<strong>on</strong>s about it.Students who were not able to read at least 75% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> narrative text (five participants total)were excused from c<strong>on</strong>tinuing <strong>on</strong> with <strong>the</strong> assessment procedure in order to preventpotential frustrati<strong>on</strong>s. Thus, a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 142 participants c<strong>on</strong>tinued with <strong>the</strong> study. Participantsdem<strong>on</strong>strated a broad range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> abilities in fluency (words read within a minute, WPM) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses to questi<strong>on</strong>s about <strong>the</strong> passage reading) <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> QRI. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean WPM performance was 97 with a st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard deviati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 36. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> meancomprehensi<strong>on</strong> score was 5.8 (based <strong>on</strong> a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 8) with a st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard deviati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1.7.AssessmentsAssessments were administered across three sessi<strong>on</strong>s that spanned a three-week period. In<strong>the</strong> first sessi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> QRI <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students’ knowledge <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> specific topics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>experimental texts were administered. In <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> third sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurring threeweeks later, students were given <strong>the</strong> experimental texts. Two additi<strong>on</strong>al measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studentreading achievement were obtained: (a) teachers’ ranking <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student reading pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>(b) student scores <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized Testing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reporting Program (STAR) (CaliforniaDepartment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2007) from <strong>the</strong> previous spring. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> STAR was351 with a st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard deviati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 62. All assessments <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> scores described here were used toestablish a baseline <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading abilities for all participants.Qualitative Reading Inventory (QRI). As already described, students individually read a thirdgrade,narrative passage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> QRI <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> answered explicit <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inferential questi<strong>on</strong>s about it(Leslie & Caldwell, 2000). A student’s oral reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text was timed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> miscuesrecorded. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> oral readings <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>ses to comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s were tape-recordedto establish fidelity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different investigators’ <strong>on</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-spot recording <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> miscues. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> QRI report very high alternate form reliability (r = .9) as well as high correlati<strong>on</strong> with anunidentified st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized reading test (r = .7). This form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> assessment was used not <strong>on</strong>ly forits reliability, but also for <strong>the</strong> fact that participating teachers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> students are familiar withthis more qualitative format <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> assessment. Thus, <strong>the</strong> teachers could also use studentperformance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> QRI formatively for general educati<strong>on</strong>al purposes.Prior vocabulary knowledge. A prior knowledge measure was developed by identifying sixwords for each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four topics that were <strong>the</strong> focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> experimental porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>study: tree frogs, toothpaste, jelly beans, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> soil. Sixteen <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words in <strong>the</strong> 24-itemmeasure represented highlighted science c<strong>on</strong>cepts in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g>ly academic forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>experimental texts (four items per topic). All words were within <strong>the</strong> same general range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>frequency, from 46 to 53 <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> SFI index (Zeno et al., 1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> remaining eight itemsc<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ei<strong>the</strong>r words representing science c<strong>on</strong>cepts that were not part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>experimental texts (e.g., terrarium) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> or cross-disciplinary words (e.g., determine) that werenot in <strong>the</strong> experimental texts but were within <strong>the</strong> same range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency. This sec<strong>on</strong>d113


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words was included in order to obtain a measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> general lexic<strong>on</strong> as well as <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>specific topics in <strong>the</strong> study.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> 24 words were <strong>the</strong>n r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>omly organized into six groups. A student’s task was tomatch a word with its definiti<strong>on</strong> for each group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words. Definiti<strong>on</strong>s were short, everydaydescripti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words, such as to dig for burrow. This task was not timed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> wascompleted in small, investigator-supervised groups.Teacher rankings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students. Teachers were asked to rank students, beginning with 1 (<strong>the</strong>str<strong>on</strong>gest reader in <strong>the</strong> classroom). Teachers completed <strong>the</strong>se rankings without receivingfeedback <strong>on</strong> students’ performances <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> QRI or <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge measure.State assessment. Students’ performances <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> state’s St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized Testing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ReportingProgram (STAR) (California Department <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2007) from <strong>the</strong> end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prioracademic year were obtained as an external measure. This measure was used as a covariatewith <strong>the</strong> QRI to establish external validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> vocabulary pre-assessment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>comprehensi<strong>on</strong> measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> experimental texts. Forty-two scores were missing due torecord unavailability (15 total) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> missing teacher files (two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> six classroom teacherswithin <strong>the</strong> rural school were unable to locate scores).Experimental texts. Sixteen texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> approximately 200 words in length were written, with fourversi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four different science topics. Topics were identified from <strong>the</strong> nati<strong>on</strong>alscience educati<strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards (NRC, 2001) to represent <strong>the</strong> three str<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> life, earth, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>physical science.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> creati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> experimental texts began with a single text for each topic. This initialtext had three secti<strong>on</strong>s: (a) an introductory secti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 50 words that was comm<strong>on</strong> across allc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, (b) a manipulated secti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> approximately 100 words (within a 9-word range)that differed according to c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (c) a c<strong>on</strong>cluding secti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 50 words that wascomm<strong>on</strong> to all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> introductory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>cluding secti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text used“simple” <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “everyday” <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent.114


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>Table 1. Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Manipulated Versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Topical Texts: Jelly BeansSyntactically SimpleSyntactically SimpleEveryday VocabularyA scientist wanted to make anew flavor. He wanted to makegrass flavor. Grass is not safefood. He could not use realgrass. He used o<strong>the</strong>r things.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se things are safe. His newjelly bean smells like grass. Ittastes like grass.A scientist wanted to make anew flavor. He wanted to makegrass flavor. Grass is not safefood. He could not use realgrass. He used o<strong>the</strong>r things.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se things are safe. His newjelly bean smells like grass. Ittastes like grass.Academic VocabularyOne scientist wanted to invent 1a flavor. This was grass flavor.Grass is not edible. He couldnot manufacture <strong>the</strong> flavor. Heused different ingredients. Thisjelly bean had <strong>the</strong> odor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>grass. It had <strong>the</strong> taste <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grass.One scientist wanted to invent 1a flavor. This was grass flavor.Grass is not edible. He could notmanufacture <strong>the</strong> flavor. Heused different ingredients. Thisjelly bean had <strong>the</strong> odor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grass.It had <strong>the</strong> taste <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grass.Syntactically EmbeddedOne scientist wanted to make anew flavor, grass flavor, by 2using o<strong>the</strong>r things becausegrass is not safe to eat. Hecould not use real grass tomake <strong>the</strong> flavor, but it smelled<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> tasted like grass.1Academic vocabulary2Word indicating an embedded structureOne scientist wanted to invent aflavor, grass flavor. by usingdifferent ingredients becausegrass is not edible. He could notuse grass to manufacture <strong>the</strong>flavor, but this jelly bean had <strong>the</strong>odor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> taste <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grass.Table 2. Indexed Features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical ComplexitySyntactic Complexity (Average Number<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Propositi<strong>on</strong>s within Versi<strong>on</strong>)Lexical Complexity (Average St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardFrequency Index (SFI) within Versi<strong>on</strong>)Simple Embedded Everyday AcademicTree Frogs 2.9 7.3 65.9 52.1Soil 2.9 7.3 63 48.3Jelly Beans 2.6 7 67 47.5Toothpaste 2.7 6.8 63.4 47<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> middle secti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text was rewritten so that <strong>the</strong>re were four texts for each topic: (a)<str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple with everyday vocabulary (simple/everyday), (b) <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally complexwith everyday vocabulary (embedded/everyday), (c) <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple with academicvocabulary (simple/academic), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (d) <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally complex with academic vocabulary(embedded/academic).For this study, a high level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> was defined as <strong>the</strong> presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two ormore embedded structures within a sentence; sentences with <strong>on</strong>e or no embeddedstructures were deemed as low in <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Embedded structures includedrelative clauses, nominalizati<strong>on</strong>s, appositives <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> multiple modifiers. An illustrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>“treated” porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded structures appears in Table 1.115


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>A propositi<strong>on</strong>al analysis (Kintsch, 1998) was used to determine <strong>the</strong> difference between<str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex texts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> average number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> propositi<strong>on</strong>s per sentence for<strong>the</strong> simple <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> embedded versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts is summarized in Table 2. Across <strong>the</strong> fourtopics, <strong>the</strong> difference between <strong>the</strong> simple <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex versi<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistently about 4propositi<strong>on</strong>s per topic.Lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> was indexed by <strong>the</strong> presence or absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic (crossdisciplinaryor scientific) words that directly relate to science c<strong>on</strong>cepts or processes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> arebey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> 1000 most frequent words according to Zeno et al. (1995). High-frequency words(words within 1000 most frequent) are referred to as “everyday words.” To verify <strong>the</strong>differences across <strong>the</strong>se passages, <strong>the</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard frequency index (SFI) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words in eachpassage were computed. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> higher <strong>the</strong> SFI, <strong>the</strong> more frequently <strong>the</strong> word is used in texts(e.g., <strong>the</strong> = 88.3; sanitize = 25.6). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> average SFIs for academic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> everyday versi<strong>on</strong>s across<strong>the</strong> four topics are reported above in Table 2. Averaged across <strong>the</strong> four topics, <strong>the</strong> differencebetween <strong>the</strong> mean SFI values <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> academic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> everyday versi<strong>on</strong>s was 16 (<strong>the</strong> differencebetween <strong>the</strong> mean SFI value <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> individual topics were within three points <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this).Since <strong>the</strong> remaining porti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts (i.e., <strong>the</strong> first <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> last 25% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each text) areequivalent, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> since <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> words for all manipulated versi<strong>on</strong>s are high in frequency,<strong>the</strong> focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> analyses was <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> everyday <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic words.For each topic, 10 questi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>structed to measure students’ comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Half<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s were multiple-choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> half required short-answer resp<strong>on</strong>ses. An example<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> following: What makes plants grow? a. rocks; b. vitamins;c. bugs; d. wind. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> short-answer resp<strong>on</strong>ses were c<strong>on</strong>structed to elicit a specific resp<strong>on</strong>se,such as <strong>the</strong> following: Write two ways that animals help plants. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong>s for a given topicwere <strong>the</strong> same, regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> manipulated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that students received. Four <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>10 questi<strong>on</strong>s targeted <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> manipulated porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text; <strong>the</strong> remaining sixquesti<strong>on</strong>s referred to <strong>the</strong> first <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> last 25% porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text (three questi<strong>on</strong>s for eachporti<strong>on</strong>). Two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four questi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> treated porti<strong>on</strong> were explicit recall <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong>from <strong>the</strong> text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two required <strong>the</strong> student to make inferences based <strong>on</strong> what <strong>the</strong>y readfrom this porti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> remaining six questi<strong>on</strong>s also c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both direct recall <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>inferential questi<strong>on</strong>s.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> short-answer questi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., How do frogs get away from <strong>the</strong>ir enemies?) were scored<strong>on</strong> a scale <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 0-1-2. A rubric was c<strong>on</strong>structed to assign no, partial or full credit. No credit wasgiven to resp<strong>on</strong>ses that were irrelevant (e.g., <strong>the</strong>y like to swim). Partial credit was given toresp<strong>on</strong>ses that included part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> intended answer (e.g., <strong>the</strong>y hop around). Full credit wasgiven to complete <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> accurate answers (e.g., <strong>the</strong>y hop around really fast). A sample <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 20%<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses was double-scored; <strong>the</strong> inter-rater agreement was 95%.Reliability <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> measureAll experimenter-designed assessments were piloted to determine validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reliability.After revisi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge assessment had a Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s alpha coefficient <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> .85 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>correlated str<strong>on</strong>gly with <strong>the</strong> QRI timed miscue measure (.65, p < .01), teacher ranking <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>reading ability (.57, p < .01) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> STAR (.67, p < .01).<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> assessments for <strong>the</strong> experimental texts <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> four topics, Tree Frogs,Soil, Jelly Beans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Toothpaste, had a Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s alpha coefficient <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> .86. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>secomprehensi<strong>on</strong> assessments str<strong>on</strong>gly correlated with <strong>the</strong> state reading assessment (.56, .67,.74, .63; p < .01) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> QRI timed miscue measure (.51, .50, .51, .51; p < .01).116


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>ProceduresThree experienced researchers collected all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> data for <strong>the</strong> present study. To reduce <strong>the</strong>possibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming <strong>the</strong> participants <strong>on</strong> key vocabulary, <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge measure wasadministered individually in <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong>, al<strong>on</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> QRI task, at least three weeks prior to<strong>the</strong> experimental reading task. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> passage reading/comprehensi<strong>on</strong> tasks took place in twosessi<strong>on</strong>s as whole-class events <strong>on</strong> two separate days; each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se sessi<strong>on</strong>s lastedapproximately 50 minutes.All participants read four passages with <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>straint that each student received eachtopic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> each versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ce <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>ce. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were 4 topics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 4 versi<strong>on</strong>s per topic,yielding 16 unique reading tasks (a passage followed by <strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> itemsc<strong>on</strong>nected with that particular topic). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se reading tasks were assigned to participantsusing a Latin-square design, which resulted in complete counterbalancing for <strong>the</strong> order inwhich both topic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> versi<strong>on</strong> were presented. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 16 reading taskswas completed equally <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten in <strong>the</strong> first through fourth testing positi<strong>on</strong>s across students. Toavoid fatigue, participants completed two reading tasks <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> first day <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>sec<strong>on</strong>d day <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> testing. As an example, <strong>on</strong>e student might have read Tree Frogs in <strong>the</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple/everyday vocabulary versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Soil in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>allysimple/academy vocabulary versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> day <strong>on</strong>e, followed by Toothpaste in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>allycomplex/everyday vocabulary versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jelly Beans in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>allycomplex/academically vocabulary versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> day two. It required a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 64 participants tocomplete <strong>on</strong>e complete replicate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 4 topics X 4 versi<strong>on</strong>s X four serial testing positi<strong>on</strong>sdesign.Participants were given as much time as needed to read <strong>the</strong> text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n answer <strong>the</strong>questi<strong>on</strong>s, but each text was collected directly before distributing questi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y wererequired to answer each set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> memory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> what had been read, without<strong>the</strong> opportunity to look back at <strong>the</strong> text. Tables 3a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 3b show <strong>the</strong> total performance <strong>on</strong>each text by versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> topic as well as specific performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> treated porti<strong>on</strong>s.ResultsA series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2-step (students were level 1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> classrooms, level 2), hierarchical linear modelswere fit to <strong>the</strong> data to examine <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between treatment (<str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>/or <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> treated secti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> text, while simultaneouslyaccounting for variance due to <strong>the</strong> clustering <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students within classrooms. A r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>omintercept was included in <strong>the</strong> model; it permitted different mean performance levels acrossclassrooms. No r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>om slopes were included in this model due to <strong>the</strong> small number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>classrooms (N = 10) as well as <strong>the</strong> implausibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> irrelevance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> classroom-specific <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> treatment <strong>on</strong> performance. No additi<strong>on</strong>al classroom variables were c<strong>on</strong>sidered in <strong>the</strong>present study. Such analyses, which would have allowed for more level-2 covariates, wouldhave required a much larger sample <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> classrooms than was available.Error-variance histograms revealed that <strong>the</strong> error variance from each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> regressi<strong>on</strong>models fit was normally distributed. Also, predicted-versus-observed scatterplots <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>outcome variables revealed that <strong>the</strong> error variance was c<strong>on</strong>stant across <strong>the</strong> range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> data.Thus, <strong>the</strong> assumpti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> regressi<strong>on</strong> modeling were met for <strong>the</strong> data used in this study.This study uses a modest form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> HLM, with a r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>om intercept <strong>on</strong>ly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> no level-2covariates. In Raudenbush <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bryk’s (2002) notati<strong>on</strong>, our full model (which corresp<strong>on</strong>ds toModel 3 described below) is described by this formula:117


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>Table 3a. Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> SDs for Total performance <strong>on</strong> Designed TextsTopic Tree Frogs Soil Jelly Beans ToothpasteVersi<strong>on</strong> 110.6 (3.3) 9.5 (3.3) 6.2 (3.6) 9.7 (3.2)(simple/everyday)Versi<strong>on</strong> 210.7 (3.4) 8.7 (3.1) 5.1 (3.6) 9.7 (3.2)(complex/everyday)Versi<strong>on</strong> 310 (3.2) 7.5 (3) 6.3 (2.8) 10.2 (3.7)(simple/academic)Versi<strong>on</strong> 4(complex/academic)9.6 (3.1) 7.3 (2.6) 6.1 (2.9) 9.6 (3.1)Table 3b. Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> SDs for Treated Porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Designed TextsTopic Tree Frogs Soil Jelly Beans ToothpasteVersi<strong>on</strong> 14.4 (1.7) 3.4 (1.6) 2.2 (1.8) 3.4 (1.4)(simple/everyday)Versi<strong>on</strong> 24.5 (1.6) 3.2 (1.5) 1.8 (1.6) 3.5 (1.2)(complex/everyday)Versi<strong>on</strong> 34.0 (1.7) 2.6 (1.7) 2.1 (1.5) 3.9 (1.6)(simple/academic)Versi<strong>on</strong> 4(complex/academic)3.5 (1.7) 2.6 (1.6) 2.2 (1.5) 3.5 (1.3)In <strong>the</strong> present study, <strong>the</strong> model form described above was fit four times, <strong>on</strong>ce for each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>four topics. Although <strong>the</strong> multiple models were fit using <strong>the</strong> same participants, a B<strong>on</strong>fer<strong>on</strong>nilikecorrecti<strong>on</strong> was not applied in this situati<strong>on</strong> given that <strong>the</strong> same questi<strong>on</strong> was asked fourtimes, <strong>on</strong>ce for each topic. Naturally, we hoped that results from <strong>the</strong> four model sets wouldc<strong>on</strong>verge.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first model fit (Model 1) is a variance-comp<strong>on</strong>ents model with no covariates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ispresented to illustrate <strong>the</strong> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total variance in performance that can be attributed toclassroom-level <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Model 2 adds <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol variables, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Model 3 adds <strong>the</strong>independent variables. Since <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> between <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> wasnot significant, it was dropped for <strong>the</strong> final model (Model 4). This variance comp<strong>on</strong>entsmodel indicates that a significant amount (6.4%, p < .05) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong> in performance isbetween-classrooms. Since <strong>the</strong> various text c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were assigned r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>omly to studentswithin classrooms, it was important to c<strong>on</strong>trol for classroom-level <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> in order toaccurately assess treatment differences within all ten classrooms included in <strong>the</strong> analysis.Model 2 adds in <strong>the</strong> covariates, which are home language (i.e., ELL status) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> four pretestscores (STAR from grade 2, prior vocabulary knowledge, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> fluency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>scores for <strong>the</strong> 3 rd grade QRI passage). Pretest scores were a highly significant predictor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>performance; ELL status was not, after c<strong>on</strong>trolling for pretest scores. Thus, ELL status did notexplain any additi<strong>on</strong>al variance in performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> designed texts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> r<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>om interceptvariance remained significant, but its share <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> variance was reduced greatly incomparis<strong>on</strong> to Model 1, indicating that much <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> variance between classrooms isattributable to student background characteristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> prior achievement.Model 3 adds in <strong>the</strong> independent variables: presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an interacti<strong>on</strong> term between <strong>the</strong> two. All three <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se variables118


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>were n<strong>on</strong>-significant. We <strong>the</strong>n dropped <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> term from <strong>the</strong> model, leaving model4, in which <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> affected performance but <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> did not.Model 4 explains a significant amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variance for <strong>on</strong>ly two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four topics, TreeFrogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Soil. Similar results were not obtained for Jelly Beans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Toothpaste; for <strong>the</strong> lattertwo topics, nei<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> nor <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> affected performance.This final model suggests that high <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> (i.e., more low frequency words) in<strong>the</strong> text is associated with lower performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> test (p < .05). As would be predicted by<strong>the</strong> design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> passages, <strong>the</strong> impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> was limited to items in <strong>the</strong>middle 50% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> passage (<strong>the</strong> manipulated porti<strong>on</strong>s); <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> did not explainany significant porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variance in resp<strong>on</strong>ses for comprehensi<strong>on</strong> items relating to <strong>the</strong> first<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> final secti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts. A model with <strong>on</strong>ly <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> as a predictor variablewas also fit to <strong>the</strong> data, but was not significant at <strong>the</strong> 0.05 level. Model 4, with <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> predicting comprehensi<strong>on</strong> differences across forms, is presented in Table 4 for allfour topics.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se inc<strong>on</strong>sistent results prompted a series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> post-hoc investigati<strong>on</strong>s into <strong>the</strong> particularc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s under which <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a text may affect comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> that text.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most obvious c<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>idate to explain <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>sistent patterns is background knowledge<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> particular c<strong>on</strong>cepts across <strong>the</strong> four topics. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>cepts explanati<strong>on</strong> wasexplored in two ways. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first was an examinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> SFI indices <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency from <strong>the</strong>Zeno et al.’s (1995) corpus; <strong>the</strong>se data appear in Table 2. Differences between <strong>the</strong> SFIs for <strong>the</strong>academic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> everyday versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts for <strong>the</strong> four topics were calculated. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>observed average SFI differences between levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, which were (in order<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> magnitude), Jelly Beans: 19.5; Toothpaste: 16.8; Soil: 14.7; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tree Frogs: 13.8, would havepredicted <strong>the</strong> greatest between-versi<strong>on</strong> differences in comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Jelly beans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Toothpaste passages. Ir<strong>on</strong>ically, just <strong>the</strong> opposite pattern was evident in <strong>the</strong> data, with <strong>the</strong>greatest differences between academic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> everyday versi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> Soil <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tree Frogs, <strong>the</strong> twotopics with <strong>the</strong> smallest differences between <strong>the</strong> everyday <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic versi<strong>on</strong>s. Thus, SFIindex does not provide a suitable explanati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> apparent interacti<strong>on</strong> between topic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d way in which background knowledge was c<strong>on</strong>sidered was to examine <strong>the</strong>relati<strong>on</strong>ship <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge vocabulary measure to comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> topics.Recall that <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge vocabulary measure correlated str<strong>on</strong>gly with students’comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> manipulated porti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts: Tree Frogs: .52; Soil: .59; Jelly Beans:.65; toothpaste: .67 (p < .01). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean scores (out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a maximum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 4) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>arddeviati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prior vocabulary assessment items for <strong>the</strong> four topics are as follows: TreeFrogs: 2.3 (sd, 1.3); Soil: 1.7 (sd, 1.3); Jelly Beans: 2.9 (sd, 1.1); Toothpaste: 2.8 (sd, 1.0). When <strong>the</strong>simple <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> were calculated across <strong>the</strong>se four means, <strong>the</strong> analysis showed that “academicvocabulary” used to create <strong>the</strong> complex versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> passages yielded significantlydifferent pre-test vocabulary results across <strong>the</strong> four topics. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> pre-test academic vocabularyperformances for Toothpaste <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jelly Beans, which did not differ from <strong>on</strong>e ano<strong>the</strong>r, weresignificantly easier than ei<strong>the</strong>r Soil or Tree Frogs; additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Tree Frogs was easier than Soil (p< .01, in all cases); in sum: (Jelly Beans= Toothpaste) > (Tree Frogs > Soil). Thus, <strong>the</strong> empiricalmeasure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students’ prior knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words was a more accurate predictor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> than <strong>the</strong> SFI index. It is <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly plausible explanati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> differential effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> across topics.Discussi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> present study was designed to address <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whe<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> or <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>factors exert greater influence <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> elementary science texts. Based <strong>on</strong>119


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>previous research <strong>on</strong> text accessibility, it was expected that <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>would each affect students’ performance <strong>on</strong> science texts, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong>se two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text<str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> toge<strong>the</strong>r would additi<strong>on</strong>ally impact student performance. In order to test thishypo<strong>the</strong>sis, 16 texts that varied in <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> across four different topicswere c<strong>on</strong>structed. Students read texts that ranged in <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, each from a different topic.C<strong>on</strong>trary to our hypo<strong>the</strong>ses, <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> did not explain variance in performanceacross any <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> four topics. It is difficult to interpret our results <strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Asestablished in <strong>the</strong> review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> research <strong>on</strong> this topic, opini<strong>on</strong>s are divided as to whe<strong>the</strong>r or notexplicitness, as defined by embedded clauses <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>nective cues, hinders or aidscomprehensi<strong>on</strong>. It is possible that different sorts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive loads effectively canceled outdifferences between <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simple <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex versi<strong>on</strong>s: our <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally simpleversi<strong>on</strong>s required students to engage in a great deal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inferencing to create <strong>the</strong> logical linksbetween sentences (e.g., A caused B or A happened before B). By c<strong>on</strong>trast, <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>allycomplex versi<strong>on</strong>s required readers to hold many embedded c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cues in shortterm memory to unpack those logical links. However, since reading ability (as measured by<strong>the</strong> QRI <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> STAR test) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge assessment did not interact with <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, it is difficult to sort out what was happening across levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. We certainly were not able to replicate <strong>the</strong> McNamara et al (1996) finding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> aninteracti<strong>on</strong> between students’ level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> prior knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cohesi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> texts asindexed by str<strong>on</strong>g use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cohesive ties between clauses <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentences. Future studies mightinclude gradati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> in order to begin to unpack this mystery. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r possibility is that <strong>the</strong> methodology used for measuring comprehensi<strong>on</strong> obscured <strong>the</strong>real impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax. It may be that syntax achieves its effect <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong>“search” process readers engage in when <strong>the</strong>y c<strong>on</strong>sult <strong>the</strong> text to find exact answers toexplicit questi<strong>on</strong>s or clues to help <strong>the</strong>m draw inferences. By taking away <strong>the</strong> texts during <strong>the</strong>comprehensi<strong>on</strong> assessment, we may have pre-empted <strong>the</strong> very mechanism (text search)through which <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicitness achieves its effect.Table 4. Regressi<strong>on</strong> Results without Interacti<strong>on</strong> Terms (Model 4)Predictor Topic 1(Tree frogs)Topic 2(Soil)Topic 3(Jelly Beans)Topic 4(Toothpaste)Intercept 1.82** (.52) 1.23** (.34) .80 (.53) 3.77** (.64)Home language -.47 (.34) -.46 .(23) -.53 (.35) -.61 (.47)QRI (pretest) .46** (.07) .27** (.05) .24** (.08) .54** (.10)Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> .06 (.25) -.22 (.17) .06 (.26) -.46 (.35)Lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> -.55* (.24) -.54** (.16) .13 (.25) .36 (.34)Variance comp<strong>on</strong>ent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>:uClassroom mean,ij.044* .185* .086* .228*rLevel-1 effect,ij1.999 .854 2.103 3.599Note: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results represent a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-nested multilevel models, fit to <strong>the</strong> same participants usingdifferent topics. St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard errors are given in paren<strong>the</strong>ses.* p < .05; ** p < .01.Lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> significantly influenced comprehensi<strong>on</strong> performance for texts <strong>on</strong> two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> four topics, Tree Frogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Soil, but not for texts <strong>on</strong> Jelly Beans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Toothpaste. Thisfinding was c<strong>on</strong>sistent across all participant groups, including ELLs. A possible explanati<strong>on</strong> isthat prior knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary, ra<strong>the</strong>r than any established index <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word frequency,determines how difficult a <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g>ly complex text will be for a student. Although, for example,bacteria is c<strong>on</strong>sidered a very low frequency word, 62% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> participants were able to120


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>correctly identify its meaning. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, essential, a word with a comparable SFI value tobacteria (SFI=56) was a much less familiar word, at least for our sample <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students, in thatless than half (42%) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> students were able to correctly identify its meaning. Assumingthat world <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge is shaped by experience, it is plausible to assume that mosteight year olds (<strong>the</strong> average age <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> our sample) have visited <strong>the</strong> dentist several times <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>have learned about dental hygiene, including words such as bacteria. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptualfamiliarity as a predictor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text comprehensi<strong>on</strong> has been commented up<strong>on</strong> in previousresearch (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998; Kintsch, 1998; Smagorinsky, 2001; Snow & Sweet,2003; Stahl, 1999), thus giving strength to this admittedly speculative explanati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong>interacti<strong>on</strong> between topic familiarity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>. However, it is important to notethat our explanati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>sistent <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> effect are tentative at best <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>require fur<strong>the</strong>r investigati<strong>on</strong>. Future studies <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> shouldinclude measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students’ prior knowledge in order to assess c<strong>on</strong>ceptual familiarityadequately.A specific interest in <strong>the</strong> present study was <strong>the</strong> effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong>s in text <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ELLs. Language status did not explain any additi<strong>on</strong>al variance inperformance bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> general findings in this study. Thus, <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> was <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>lysignificant factor in comprehensi<strong>on</strong> performance for ELLs. This finding is c<strong>on</strong>sistent withresearch by Proctor, August, Carlo <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Snow (2005) who reported that L2 vocabularyknowledge was a significant predictor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> L2 text comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ELLs. Our findings did notreveal any significant differences in comprehensi<strong>on</strong> performance between native Englishspeakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ELLs, thus suggesting a global model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, seemingly c<strong>on</strong>trary toProctor et al.’s (2005) c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that we need an L2-<strong>on</strong>ly model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.However, due to differences in specific informati<strong>on</strong> about L1 pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency, comparis<strong>on</strong>sbetween this study <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> work by Proctor et al. are speculative at best.While <strong>the</strong> results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this study are intriguing, it is important to note significant limitati<strong>on</strong>s.First, <strong>the</strong> manipulated porti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> experimental texts (approximately 100 words in each<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 200-word texts) may not have been l<strong>on</strong>g enough to allow for <strong>the</strong> detecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> across all four topics. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong> fact that ELL statuswas dichotomously classified (ELL or n<strong>on</strong>-ELL) could limit our ability to explore <strong>the</strong> effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>first language (L1) expertise <strong>on</strong> performance. A multitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies highlight <strong>the</strong> significant<str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> L1 pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency <strong>on</strong> L2 acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (Jimenez, Garcia, & Pears<strong>on</strong>,1996; Proctor et al., 2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that students’ comm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> over <strong>the</strong>ir first languageinfluences <strong>the</strong>ir ability to comprehend both <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g>ly complex features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>texts was not c<strong>on</strong>sidered in <strong>the</strong> present study. Fur<strong>the</strong>r research is needed to determinepossible <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> varying gradati<strong>on</strong>s in L1 pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency <strong>on</strong> L2 text difficulty.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> findings within <strong>the</strong> present study have left questi<strong>on</strong>s regarding text accessibilityunanswered. Does <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> have absolutely no effect <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, or is<strong>the</strong>re some gradati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> difference that was not captured within <strong>the</strong> design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> our texts?Does prior knowledge, as defined by c<strong>on</strong>ceptual familiarity, trump <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g>, asindexed by frequency, in determining comprehensi<strong>on</strong>? If so, how much familiarity isnecessary to overcome difficult vocabulary? Finally, do EL learners face <strong>the</strong> same difficultiesas native English speakers in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text accessibility, even when c<strong>on</strong>sidering <strong>the</strong> effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>gradati<strong>on</strong>s in L1 pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency? We hope that future studies will shed fur<strong>the</strong>r light <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>seimportant questi<strong>on</strong>s. At <strong>the</strong> same time, our failure to elicit a <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> affectmight give us pause, when we design curriculum, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> being too rigid about keeping sentencelength to an absolute minimum. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>lexical</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> effect, which seemed to bemost powerful in situati<strong>on</strong>s in which students could not rely <strong>on</strong> prior knowledge from121


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>CCSS (2010). Comm<strong>on</strong> Core State St<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards. Retrieved September 14, 2011, fromhttp://www.corest<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards.org/.Cunningham, A. & Stanovich, K.E. (1998). What reading does for <strong>the</strong> mind. American Educator, 22(1& 2),8-15.D<strong>on</strong>ovan, C.A., & Smolkin, L.B (2001). Genre <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r factors influencing teachers’ book selecti<strong>on</strong>s forscience instructi<strong>on</strong>. Reading Research Quarterly, 36 (4), 412-440.Droop, M. & Verhoeven, L. (1998). Background knowledge, linguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>complexity</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d languagereading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Literacy Research, 30(2), 253-271.Duke, N. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> scarcity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong>al texts in <strong>the</strong> first grade. ReadingResearch Quarterly, 35, 202-224.Duke, N. K., & Bennett-Armistead, V. S. (2003). Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> writing informati<strong>on</strong>al text in <strong>the</strong> primarygrades: Research-based practices. New York: Scholastic.Elley, W. (1996). Using book floods to raise literacy levels in developing countries. In V. Greaney (Ed.),Promoting reading in developing countries: Views <strong>on</strong> making reading materials accessible toincrease literacy levels (pp. 148-163). Newark, DE: IRA.Flesch, R. (1979). How to write plain English. New York, NY: Harper <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Row.Flesch, R. (1948). A new readability yardstick. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Applied Psychology, 32, 221–23.Goldman, S.R., & Bisanz, G. L. (2002). Toward a functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> scientific genres: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s forunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning processes. In J. Ortero, J.A. Le<strong>on</strong>, & A.C. Graesser (Eds.), <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science text comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 19-50). New Jersey: LEA.Gopnik, A. (1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> scientist as child. Philosophy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science, 63(4), 485-514.Grabe, W. (1991). Current developments in sec<strong>on</strong>d language reading research. TESOL Quarterly, 25(3),375-406.Gutierrez, K. , & Rog<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, B. (2003). Cultural ways <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning: Individual traits or repertoires <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice.Educati<strong>on</strong>al Researcher, 32(5), 19 - 25.Guthrie, J. T., Anders<strong>on</strong>, E., Alao, S., & Rinehart, J. (1999). Influences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>cept- Oriented ReadingInstructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> strategy use <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual learning from text. Elementary School Journal,99(4), 343-366.Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A. C., & Klauda, S. L. (2007). C<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>cept-Oriented ReadingInstructi<strong>on</strong> to knowledge about interventi<strong>on</strong>s for motivati<strong>on</strong>s in reading. Educati<strong>on</strong>alPsychologist, 42, 237-250.Hayes, D. P., & Ahrens, M. (1988). Vocabulary simplificati<strong>on</strong> for children: A special case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ‘mo<strong>the</strong>rese.’Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Child Language, 15, 395-410.Jimenez, R.T., Garcia, G.E., & Pears<strong>on</strong>, P.D. (1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading strategies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilingual; Latina/o studentswho are successful English readers: Opportunities <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> obstacles. Reading Research Quarterly,31, 90-112.Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehensi<strong>on</strong>: A paradigm for cogniti<strong>on</strong>. NY: Cambridge University Press.Klare, G.R. (1984). Readability. In P. D. Pears<strong>on</strong> (Ed.), H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading Research (Vol. 1, pp. 681-744).New York: L<strong>on</strong>gman.Lee, O., & Luykx, A. (2005). Dilemmas in scaling up innovati<strong>on</strong>s in science instructi<strong>on</strong> withn<strong>on</strong>mainstream elementary students. American Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research Journal, 42(5), 411–438.Lenn<strong>on</strong>, C. & Burdick, H. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexile framework as an approach for reading measurement <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>success. MetaMetrics.Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (2000). Qualitative Reading Inventory-III. New York: L<strong>on</strong>gman.McNamara, D. S. (2001). Reading both high-coherence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> low coherence texts: Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> textsequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> prior knowledge. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 55, 51-62.McNamara, D.S., Kintsch, E., S<strong>on</strong>ger, N.B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Textcoherence, background knowledge, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing in learning from text.Cogniti<strong>on</strong> & Instructi<strong>on</strong>, 14, 1-43.Nagy, W.E., & Scott, J.A.(2000).Vocabulary processes. In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D. Pears<strong>on</strong>, & R.Barr (Eds.), H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading Research (Vol. III, pp. 269–284). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.Nati<strong>on</strong>, K., & Snowling, M.J. (2000). Factors influencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> awareness skills in normal readers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 229–241Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Child Health <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Human Development (NICHD) (2000). Report <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>alReading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> scientific123


Internati<strong>on</strong>al Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary Educati<strong>on</strong>research literature <strong>on</strong> reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s for reading instructi<strong>on</strong> (NIH Publicati<strong>on</strong> No.00-4769). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council. (2001). Classroom assessment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> nati<strong>on</strong>al science educati<strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards.Washingt<strong>on</strong> D.C.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy Press.Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (2003). How literacy in its fundamental sense is central to scientific literacy.Science Educati<strong>on</strong>, 87(2), 224-240.Ozuru, Y., Dempsey, K., Sayroo J., & McNamara, D.S. (2005). Effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text cohesi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> biology texts. Psychology Department, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Memphis.Palincsar, A. S. & Magnuss<strong>on</strong>, S. J. (2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interplay <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> first-h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> text-based investigati<strong>on</strong>s tomodel <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> support <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> scientific knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reas<strong>on</strong>ing. In S. Carver & D.Klahr (Eds.), Cogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong>: 25 years <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> progress (pp.151-194). Mahwah, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum.Pears<strong>on</strong>, P.D. (2009). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> roots <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. In S.E. Israel & G.G.Duffy (Eds.), H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> research <strong>on</strong> reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 3–31). New York:Routledge.Pears<strong>on</strong>, P.D., & Camperell, K. (1981). Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text structures. In J.T. Guthrie (Ed.),Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> teaching: Research reviews (pp. 448-468). Newark, DE: Internati<strong>on</strong>alReading Associati<strong>on</strong>.Proctor, C.P., August, D., Carlo, M., & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading inEnglish: Toward a model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 97(2), 246-256.Qian, D.D. (2002) investigating <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between vocabulary knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic readingperformance: an assessment perspective. Language Learning, 52, 513-536.RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing: Towards an R&D program in readingcomprehensi<strong>on</strong>.Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models. Thous<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oaks, CA: SagePublicati<strong>on</strong>s.Raws<strong>on</strong>, K.A. (2004). Exploring automaticity in text processing: <str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic</str<strong>on</strong>g> ambiguity as a test case.Cognitive Psychology, 49(4), 333-69.Romance, N.R., & Vitale, M.R. (1992). A curriculum strategy that exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s time for in-depth elementaryscience instructi<strong>on</strong> by using science-based reading strategies: Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a year-l<strong>on</strong>g study ingrade four. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Research in Science Teaching, 29, 545-554.Romance, N.R., & Vitale, M.R. (2006). Science IDEAS: Making <strong>the</strong> case for integrating reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>writing in elementary science as a key element in school reform. In R. Douglas,M. P. Klentschy,K. Worth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> W. Binder (Eds.), Linking science <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> literacy in <strong>the</strong> K–8 classroom (pp. 391-405).Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, VA: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Science Teachers Associati<strong>on</strong> (NSTA) Press.Ru<strong>the</strong>rford, J.F. (1991). Vital c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s: Children, books, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> science. In S.Jagusch & W. Saul (Ed.),Vital c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s (pp. 21-30). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Schleppegrell, M.J. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> language <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> schooling: A functi<strong>on</strong>al linguistics perspective. Mahwah, NJ:LEA.Shaw, J. M. (1997). Threats to <strong>the</strong> validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> science performance assessments for English languagelearners. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Research in Science Teaching, 34(7), 721-743.Shymansky, J.A., Yore, L.D., & Good, R. (1991). Elementary school teacher’s beliefs about <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>percepti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> elementary school science, science reading, science textbooks, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> supportiveinstructi<strong>on</strong>al factors. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Research in Science Teaching, 28(5), 437-454.Smagorinsky, P. (2001). If meaning is c<strong>on</strong>structed, what is it made from? Toward a cultural <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>reading. Review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research, 71(1), 133-169.Snow, C.E. (2010). Academic language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> challenge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading for learning about science.Science, 328, 450-452.Snow, C. E., & Sweet, A.P. (2003) Reading for Comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. In A. P. Sweet & C. E. Snow (Eds.),Rethinking reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (1-11). New York: Guilford Press.Snowling, M., & Nati<strong>on</strong>, K. (1997). Language, ph<strong>on</strong>ology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning to read. In C. Hulme & M.Snowling (Eds.). Dyslexia: Biology, cogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong>. San Diego, CA: SingularPublishing Group.Stahl, S.A. (1999). Vocabulary development. Cambridge, MA: Brookline.124


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Syntactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lexical / Arya, Hiebert & Pears<strong>on</strong>Stanovich, K. (2000). Progress in underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing reading: Scientific foundati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> new fr<strong>on</strong>tiers. NewYork: Guilford.U.S. Census Bureau, (2000). http://www.census.gov/main/www/citati<strong>on</strong>.html.Vellutino, F.R. (2003). Individual differences as sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variability in reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> inelementary school children. In A.P. Sweet & C.E. Snow(Eds.), Rethinking readingcomprehensi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 51-81). NY: Guilford Press.Willows, D. M., & Ryan E. B. (1986). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammatical sensitivity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its relati<strong>on</strong>ship toearly reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 253–266.Wils<strong>on</strong>, D, & Sperber, D. (1987). An outline <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relevance <strong>the</strong>ory. Notes <strong>on</strong> Linguistics, 39, 5-24.Zeno, S.M., Ivens, S.H., Millard, R.T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educator’s Word Frequency Guide. Brewster:Touchst<strong>on</strong>e Applied Science Associates, Inc.125

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!