30.11.2012 Views

[on] how people learn science - Learning Research and ...

[on] how people learn science - Learning Research and ...

[on] how people learn science - Learning Research and ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

esearchers Playing catch-Up<br />

in gauging Bey<strong>on</strong>d-School effects<br />

BY<br />

SaraH D.<br />

SParKS<br />

Emerging research s<strong>how</strong>s the <strong>science</strong><br />

school-age children <strong>learn</strong> in<br />

informal settings—from museums<br />

<strong>and</strong> clubs to <strong>on</strong>line communities<br />

<strong>and</strong> televisi<strong>on</strong> s<strong>how</strong>s—can have a<br />

big impact <strong>on</strong> their lives. Yet the<br />

open format <strong>and</strong> distinct structures<br />

of informal <strong>science</strong> make it next to<br />

impossible for researchers to evaluate<br />

the quality of those experiences<br />

in the same way they can gauge<br />

formal schooling.<br />

School assessments generally<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> cognitive measures, such<br />

as what a student knows <strong>and</strong> can<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate about particular c<strong>on</strong>tent.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, informal <strong>learn</strong>ing<br />

is dominated by n<strong>on</strong>cognitive<br />

measures such as motivati<strong>on</strong>, interest,<br />

<strong>and</strong> identity, according to<br />

Larry E. Suter, the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Science<br />

Foundati<strong>on</strong>’s program director<br />

for informal <strong>science</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Moreover, traditi<strong>on</strong>al “gold st<strong>and</strong>ard”<br />

research methods such as<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omized c<strong>on</strong>trolled trials can<br />

be detrimental to activities that<br />

base their strength <strong>on</strong> <strong>people</strong>’s<br />

choosing to participate, rather<br />

than being assigned.<br />

Such research raises the risk,<br />

Mr. Suter said, that “if you touch<br />

it, you’re going to kill that thing<br />

you’re trying to study.”<br />

That has led to some creative<br />

research alternatives. Alan J.<br />

Friedman, a former director <strong>and</strong><br />

chief executive officer of the New<br />

York Hall of Science <strong>and</strong> the editor<br />

of the n s f’s 2008 framework<br />

for evaluating informal <strong>science</strong><br />

educati<strong>on</strong>, recalls judging the effectiveness<br />

of an astr<strong>on</strong>omy exhibit<br />

by the number of visitors<br />

who chose an astr<strong>on</strong>omy poster<br />

over a different prize. Barbara N.<br />

Flagg, the director of the Multimedia<br />

<strong>Research</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sultant<br />

group in Bellport, N.Y., said<br />

she has used smudged museum<br />

walls indicating where<br />

visitors have touched exhibits,<br />

changes in Google <strong>and</strong> Amaz<strong>on</strong><br />

search terms over time, <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

interviews with parents <strong>and</strong> children.<br />

“If you’re comparing this to other<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> research, you’ve got to<br />

S6<br />

turn back the clock 30 or 40 years,”<br />

said Kevin J. Crowley, the director<br />

of the University of Pittsburgh’s<br />

Center for <strong>Learning</strong> in Out-of<br />

-School Envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> an associate<br />

professor of educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

psychology. “We are just now in the<br />

Wild West fr<strong>on</strong>tier, <strong>and</strong> <strong>people</strong> are<br />

just starting to gear up the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

studies <strong>on</strong> <strong>how</strong> this will coalesce<br />

into a coherent narrative of<br />

<strong>how</strong> <strong>people</strong> <strong>learn</strong> <strong>science</strong>.”<br />

Now, the tools being born of that<br />

creativity s<strong>how</strong> the potential to<br />

link children’s educati<strong>on</strong> across a<br />

lifetime of different experiences,<br />

<strong>and</strong> in the process uncover more<br />

of <strong>how</strong> <strong>and</strong> what children <strong>learn</strong><br />

than has ever been measured in a<br />

school test al<strong>on</strong>e. Children, after<br />

all, spend more than 80 percent<br />

of their waking hours outside the<br />

classroom.<br />

“The research has evolved,” Mr.<br />

Crowley said. “In the past, the<br />

great flaw of the informal-<strong>learn</strong>ing<br />

<strong>science</strong> was we looked at it in situ-<br />

“ We’re<br />

just now in<br />

the Wild<br />

West<br />

fr<strong>on</strong>tier<br />

... [<strong>on</strong>]<br />

<strong>how</strong> <strong>people</strong><br />

<strong>learn</strong> <strong>science</strong>.”<br />

KeViN J. crOWleY<br />

associate Professor of educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

University of Pittsburgh<br />

ati<strong>on</strong>s; we weren’t really<br />

looking at a lifel<strong>on</strong>g trajectory<br />

in <strong>science</strong> <strong>learn</strong>ing. For<br />

the first time, we’re asking questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

about <strong>how</strong> <strong>learn</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong><br />

are moving across place<br />

<strong>and</strong> across time.”<br />

“Brenda’s” school performance,<br />

for instance, didn’t really s<strong>how</strong> her<br />

scientific achievements or level of<br />

interest in the subject. Teachers<br />

reported the first-generati<strong>on</strong> Haitian<br />

daughter of a single mother<br />

in Seattle had little interest in<br />

<strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently “failed to<br />

engage” with the chemical-mixing<br />

tasks in her school lab.<br />

Yet because researchers at the<br />

University of Washingt<strong>on</strong>, in Seattle,<br />

had been observing the 4th<br />

grader across formal <strong>and</strong> informal<br />

settings for more than 2,000 hours,<br />

they knew that school didn’t tell<br />

Brenda’s whole story. Not <strong>on</strong>ly did<br />

she regularly measure <strong>and</strong> mix<br />

chemicals <strong>and</strong> record the results<br />

for her perfume-making hobby,<br />

but she also had told the researchers<br />

she was c<strong>on</strong>sidering becoming<br />

a chemist when she grew up.<br />

“School <strong>science</strong> underrepresents<br />

her developing expertise,” Philip<br />

Bell, an associate professor of<br />

<strong>learn</strong>ing <strong>science</strong>s at the university<br />

<strong>and</strong> the director of ethnographic<br />

<strong>and</strong> design-based research at the<br />

Everyday Science <strong>and</strong> Technology<br />

Group there, said during a recent<br />

lecture. “Just in terms of <strong>how</strong> <strong>people</strong><br />

<strong>learn</strong>, our literatures d<strong>on</strong>’t do<br />

justice to the varied pathways that<br />

<strong>people</strong> take through their experiences<br />

to make progress <strong>on</strong> things<br />

they care about.”<br />

iNTereST VS. graDeS<br />

That’s a dangerous disc<strong>on</strong>nect,<br />

experts say, because<br />

mounting evidence s<strong>how</strong>s<br />

that early engagement,<br />

even through informal pathways,<br />

eventually can lead to<br />

careers in the stEm fields of <strong>science</strong>,<br />

technology, engineering, <strong>and</strong><br />

mathematics more surely than top<br />

grades in school.<br />

In a 2006 study published in<br />

the journal Science, Robert H. Tai,<br />

an associate professor at the University<br />

of Virginia’s Curry School<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>, in Charlottesville,<br />

tracked thous<strong>and</strong>s of students via<br />

the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Educati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

Study. He found that<br />

students who had <strong>on</strong>ly average<br />

grades in middle school but expressed<br />

interest in <strong>science</strong> were<br />

two to three times more likely to<br />

earn bachelor’s degrees in a <strong>science</strong><br />

or engineering field 12 years<br />

later than high-achieving students<br />

who did not voice interest.<br />

The l<strong>and</strong>mark 2009 study that<br />

Mr. Bell co-wrote, “<strong>Learning</strong> Sci-<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> WEEK: sciEncE lEarning outsidE thE classroom l www.edweek.org/go/<strong>science</strong>report l april 6, 2011<br />

What’s measured in the<br />

classroom—what students<br />

know <strong>and</strong> can do— differs<br />

from what’s currently measured<br />

outside—such as motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> interest.<br />

ViTal liNKS<br />

<strong>learn</strong>ing Science in informal envir<strong>on</strong>ments: People, Places,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Pursuits (2009)<br />

The Nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Council of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academies,<br />

Edited by Philip Bell, Bruce Lewenstein, Andrew W. Shouse, <strong>and</strong> Michael<br />

A. Feder<br />

this l<strong>and</strong>mark study by the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academies’ Committee <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>Learning</strong> <strong>science</strong> in Informal Envir<strong>on</strong>ments documented evidence<br />

that children <strong>and</strong> adults do <strong>learn</strong> <strong>science</strong> outside of direct school<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. Both designed <strong>science</strong> settings such as zoos or<br />

museums <strong>and</strong> sp<strong>on</strong>taneous settings such as a walk in the park can<br />

help children underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>science</strong>. the book laid the foundati<strong>on</strong> for a<br />

more evidence-based approach to informal <strong>science</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Surrounded by Science: <strong>learn</strong>ing Science in informal<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments (2010)<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academies, Marilyn Fenichel <strong>and</strong> Heidi A. Schweingruber<br />

A follow-up to the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academies’ <strong>Learning</strong> <strong>science</strong> in Informal<br />

Envir<strong>on</strong>ments, this practiti<strong>on</strong>er-focused guide provides case studies,<br />

framework tools, <strong>and</strong> other examples of <strong>how</strong> high-quality informal<br />

<strong>science</strong> educati<strong>on</strong> programs <strong>and</strong> exhibits can look.<br />

Framework for evaluating impacts of informal Science<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> Projects (2008)<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Science Foundati<strong>on</strong>, edited by Alan J. Friedman<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> an NsF workshop <strong>on</strong> informal <strong>science</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>, this<br />

framework lays out the criteria for measuring informal <strong>science</strong>, based<br />

<strong>on</strong> a participant’s awareness, knowledge, or underst<strong>and</strong>ing of a<br />

<strong>science</strong> topic; engagement or interest in <strong>science</strong>; attitude toward<br />

<strong>science</strong> or careers in the field; changes in scientific behavior such<br />

as inquiry; <strong>and</strong> the improvement of specific skills related to <strong>science</strong>,<br />

such as experimenting or data analysis.<br />

Measuring the impact of a Science center <strong>on</strong> its<br />

community (2011)<br />

Journal of <strong>Research</strong> in Science Teaching, John H. Falk <strong>and</strong> Mark D.<br />

Needham<br />

By studying Los Angeles residents <strong>and</strong> museum-goers before <strong>and</strong> a<br />

decade after a massive overhaul of the city’s California <strong>science</strong> Center,<br />

researchers s<strong>how</strong>ed that the museum had increased the public’s<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>and</strong> interest in the <strong>science</strong> covered in the new<br />

exhibits. the researchers used public underst<strong>and</strong>ing of homeostasis as<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual marker to track improved scientific underst<strong>and</strong>ing.<br />

soURCE: Educati<strong>on</strong> Week<br />

Links to these reports are provided at edweek.org/links.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!