12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong><strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong> offers sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic, ethnographic, andsocial-psychological perspectives on TESOL teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>troduces therelevant literature on second language acquisition. It presents <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> a variety of specific <strong>in</strong>stitutional, geographic and cultural contexts.The articles - which <strong>in</strong>clude both classic and specially commissioned pieces - have beencarefully chosen and edited to present the ma<strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g.They focus on the roles played by teachers and learners, recognise the <strong>in</strong>dividuality oflanguage learners, support teachers <strong>in</strong> the provision of active guidance for students’learn<strong>in</strong>g, and exam<strong>in</strong>e both positive and negative patterns of <strong>in</strong>teraction between learnersand teachers.This Reader offers people unfamiliar with research <strong>in</strong> this field an overall understand<strong>in</strong>g ofkey issues <strong>in</strong> contemporary <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g while allow<strong>in</strong>g the more experiencedreader the opportunity to relate his or her experiences to the theories presented.Articles by: Michael P. Breen; Anne Burns; A. Suresh Canagarajah; J. Keith Chick; RodEllis; Paul<strong>in</strong>e Gibbons; Paul Knight; Patsy M. Lightbown; Angel M.Y. L<strong>in</strong>; Michael H.Long; Neil Mercer; Rosamond Mitchell; Florence Myles; David Nunan; Jack C. Richards;Celia Roberts; Peter Skehan; Assia Slimani; N<strong>in</strong>a Spada; Joan Swann; Leo van LierChristopher N. Candl<strong>in</strong> is Chair Professor of Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Director of theCentre for <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Education and Communication Research at the CityUniversity of Hong I


Companion volumesThe companion volumes <strong>in</strong> this series are:Analys<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> a Global <strong>Context</strong> edited by Anne Burns and Carol<strong>in</strong>e Coff<strong>in</strong>Innovation <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> edited by David R. Hall and Ann Hew<strong>in</strong>gsThese three readers are part of a scheme of study jo<strong>in</strong>tly developed by MacquarieUniversity, Sydney, Australia, and the Open University, United I


<strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>‘Candl<strong>in</strong>’s and Mercer ’s Reader provides key <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to contemporary knowledgeof second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, the exploitation of this knowledge <strong>in</strong> classroom action,and subsequent assessment and analysis. By emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g the social context of thesethree processes, and the rclationship between thcm, the book providcs a reward<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>troduction to the <strong>in</strong>teraction between theory, research and professional practicewhich lies at the heart of applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics.’ Guy Cook, 1Jniversity $Read<strong>in</strong>g, 1JK‘This volume l<strong>in</strong>ks the teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong> to the development of autonomous<strong>in</strong>dividuals who prize debate, negotiation and <strong>in</strong>teraction, and who will ultimately beable to build global communications of likc-m<strong>in</strong>dcd <strong>English</strong> spcakers around theworld. Readers will f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> this collection of excellent papers some of the classic mile-stones <strong>in</strong> the field of ELT.’ Claire Kramsch, University of Calfornia, BerkelT, Calfornia<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> WorldwideA selection of readcrs’ comments on the series:‘This three-part series offers a map to ELT research and practice . . . it represents the bestthat ELT, as an Anglo-Saxon <strong>in</strong>stitution, has developed over the last thirty ycars for theteach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong> around thc world . . . Readers will f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> this series the Who’s Whoguide to this dynamic and expand<strong>in</strong>g community.’ Claire Krarnsch, Universiiy ._f‘ Cal@rnia,Berkelty, Calfornia‘Experienced <strong>English</strong> language <strong>in</strong>structors seek<strong>in</strong>g to deepen their knowledge antl abilitieswill f<strong>in</strong>d this scries forms a coherent basis to develop their understand<strong>in</strong>g of current trends,sociocultural tlivcrsity, and topical <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> as a second or foreignlanguagr around the world. All threc volumes provide ample flexibility for discussion,<strong>in</strong>terpretation, and adaptation <strong>in</strong> local sett<strong>in</strong>gs.’ Alister Cumrn<strong>in</strong>g, Ontario Institute-for Studies <strong>in</strong>Education, Ilnivercity of Eronto‘This series provides a collection of essential read<strong>in</strong>gs which will not only provide theTEFL/TESOL studcnt and teacher with access to the most up-to-date th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g andapproachcs to the subject but will give any person <strong>in</strong>tcrcsted <strong>in</strong> the subject an overview ofthe phenomenon of the uhe and usage of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the modern world. Perhaps moreimportantly, this series will be crucial to those students who do not have available to themarticles that provide both a wide spectrum of <strong>in</strong>formation and the necessary analytical toolsto <strong>in</strong>vestigate the language further.’ Joseph A. Foly, Southeast Asia M<strong>in</strong>isters of EducationOrganisation, Regional <strong>Language</strong> Centre, S<strong>in</strong>gapore‘The strong reprcsentation of the sem<strong>in</strong>al Anglo-Australian development of the Europeanfunctional tradition <strong>in</strong> the study of language antl language education makes this a rcfresh<strong>in</strong>glybrac<strong>in</strong>g scries, which should be widely used <strong>in</strong> teacher education for <strong>English</strong> languageteach<strong>in</strong>g.’ Euan Reid, Institute of Education, Ilniverciiy FfLondon‘In a pr<strong>in</strong>cipled and accessible manner, these three volumes br<strong>in</strong>g together major writ<strong>in</strong>gs onessential topics <strong>in</strong> the study of <strong>English</strong> languagc teach<strong>in</strong>g. They provide broad coverage ofcurrent th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and debate on major issucs, provid<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>valuable resource for thecontemporary postgraduate student.’ Gc~v Cook, University of’Read<strong>in</strong>g


<strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong><strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>A ReaderEdited byChristopher N. Candl<strong>in</strong> and Neil MercerudT :!aYTheOpen3a4%., 0.”MACQUARIUniversityE4 UNIVERSITY- SYDNEYLondon and New Yovk<strong>in</strong> association with Macquavie Universityand The Open University


First published 2001by Routledge11 New Fetter Lane, London EC4P 4EESimultaneously published <strong>in</strong> the USA and Canadaby Routledge29 West 35th Street, New York, NY 10001Routledge is an impr<strong>in</strong>t of the Taylor & Francis Group0 2001 Compilation, orig<strong>in</strong>al and editorial material Macquarie University andThe Open University; <strong>in</strong>dividual articles 0 their authorsTypeset <strong>in</strong> Perpetua and Bell Gothic by Keystroke, Jacaranda Lodge, WolverhamptonPr<strong>in</strong>ted and bound <strong>in</strong> Great Brita<strong>in</strong> by TJ International Ltd, Padstow, CornwallAll rights reserved. No part of this book may be repr<strong>in</strong>ted or reproduced orutilized <strong>in</strong> any form or by any electronic, mechanical or other means,now known or hereafter <strong>in</strong>vented, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g photocopy<strong>in</strong>g and record<strong>in</strong>g,or <strong>in</strong> any <strong>in</strong>formation storage or retrieval system, without permission<strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g from the publishers.British Library Catalogu<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Publication DataA catalogue record for this book is available from the British LibraryLibrary of Congress Catalog<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Publication Data<strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> social context / edited by Christopher N. Candl<strong>in</strong>and Neil Mercer.p. cm. - (<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>English</strong> language worldwide)Includes bibliographical references and <strong>in</strong>dex.1. <strong>English</strong> language-Study and teach<strong>in</strong>g-Foreign speakers. 2. <strong>English</strong>language-Study and teach<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>Social</strong> aspects. I. Candl<strong>in</strong>, Christopher.11. Mercer, Neil. 111. SeriesPE1128.A2 E49 200042 8’. 007 1-dc2 1 00-059195ISBN 0-415-24121-9 (hbk)ISBN 0-415-24122-7 (Dbk)


ContentsList of illustrationsAcknowledgementsXxiiChristopher N. Candl<strong>in</strong> and Neil MercerINTRODUCTIONPART ONEHow is language learn<strong>in</strong>g expla<strong>in</strong>ed?Rosamond Mitchell and Florence Myles1 SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: I


IllustrationsFigures1.12.13.13.25.18.18.28.38.48.510.112.120.120.220.320.420.520.620.720.820.920.1020.1120.12Spolsky’s general model of second language learn<strong>in</strong>gBar charts show<strong>in</strong>g the language levels of pre- and post-pubertylearners of <strong>English</strong>‘Moments’ of action researchRelat<strong>in</strong>g discipl<strong>in</strong>ary theory and language pedagogyIRF cont<strong>in</strong>uumSituational language teach<strong>in</strong>g materialA typical audio-l<strong>in</strong>gual drillCLT materials which encourage groupwork and participationA typical Prabhu taskAn example of unit objectives with<strong>in</strong> a text-based approachNoun phrase accessibility hierarchyThe teach <strong>in</strong>g-learn i ng cycleField-notes of an assembly <strong>in</strong> a school <strong>in</strong> south-east EnglandTranscription of teacher-student talkTranscription of small group talk: standard layoutTranscription of small group talk: column layoutTranscription of group talk: stave layoutRepresentation of nonverbal features <strong>in</strong> an oral narrativeRepresentation of teacher’s gaze towards female and male studentsTranscript illustrat<strong>in</strong>g alternation between <strong>English</strong> and MalteseTranscript illustrat<strong>in</strong>g alternation between Sanskrit and <strong>English</strong>Transcription of a conversation us<strong>in</strong>g Creole and London <strong>English</strong>Representation of pronunciation us<strong>in</strong>g phonetic symbolsIncidence of ’cos and because <strong>in</strong> primary school children’s talk1338586594150151156162164185202329331332333335336337337338339339342


ILLUSTRATIONS xiTables2.1 Comparison of language learn<strong>in</strong>g at different ages3.1 Attributes of <strong>in</strong>novation15.1 Some techniques that teachers use18.1 Average scores and percentage <strong>in</strong>crease for each group18.2 Effect of topicalisation18.3 Percentage of claims made by reporters on each l<strong>in</strong>guistic feature20.1 Number and type of student-<strong>in</strong>itiated moves <strong>in</strong> two types of lesson4162293296299340


Ac kn ow I e d g e me n t sThe editors and publishers would like to thank the follow<strong>in</strong>g for permission to usecopyright material:Michael P. Breen and Cambridge University Press for ‘The social context of languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g: a neglected situation’ <strong>in</strong> Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, 7, 1985.Michael P. Rreen and SEAMEO Regional <strong>Language</strong> Centre for ‘Navigat<strong>in</strong>g the discourse:on what is learned <strong>in</strong> the language classroom’ <strong>in</strong> Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of the 1997 RELCSem<strong>in</strong>ar.Anne Burns for ‘Genre-based approaches to writ<strong>in</strong>g and beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g adult ESL learners’,repr<strong>in</strong>ted from Prospect Vol. 5, No. 3, May 1990 with permission from the NationalCentre for <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Research (NCELTR), Australia. (MacquarieUnivcrsity). Includes material <strong>in</strong> Fig. 2 adapted from Learn<strong>in</strong>g Styles <strong>in</strong> Adult MigruntEducation by Will<strong>in</strong>g K., also with permission from the National Centre for <strong>English</strong><strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Research (NCELTR), Australia (Macquarie University).Cambridge University Press for Assia Slimani ‘Evaluation of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction’ <strong>in</strong> J. C.Alderson and A. Beretta (eds) Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Education, 1992.A. Suresh Cangaraja and TESOL for ‘Critical ethnography of a Sri Lankan classroom:ambiguities <strong>in</strong> student opposition to reproduction through ESOL’ <strong>in</strong> TESOL Quarterl,,Vol. 27, No. 4, (TESOL 1993).J. Keith Chick and Cambridge University Press for ‘Safe-talk: collusion <strong>in</strong> apartheideducation’ <strong>in</strong> H. Coleman (ed.) Society and the <strong>Language</strong> Classroom, 1996.Rod Ellis for ‘Second language acquisition research and language pedagogy’ <strong>in</strong> SLA Researchand <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> by Rod Ellis ( Rod Ellis 1997). Reproduced by permission ofOxford University Press.Patsy M. Lightbown and N<strong>in</strong>a Spada for ‘Factors affect<strong>in</strong>g second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> How<strong>Language</strong>s are Learned (Second Edition) by Patsy M. Lightbown and N<strong>in</strong>a Spada (Patsy M.Lightbown and N<strong>in</strong>a Spada 1999.) Reproduced by permission of Oxford UniversityPress.Angel M.Y. L<strong>in</strong> and TESOL for ‘Do<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>English</strong>-lessons <strong>in</strong> the reproduction ortransformation of social worlds?’ <strong>in</strong> TESOL Quarterl,,Vol. 33, No. 3, (TESOL 1999).Michael Long and John Benjam<strong>in</strong>’s Publish<strong>in</strong>g Co. for ‘Focus on form: a design feature <strong>in</strong>language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology’ <strong>in</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Research <strong>in</strong> a Cross-cultural Perspective.Edited by K. de Bot, R.B. G<strong>in</strong>sberg and C. Krausch. John Benjam<strong>in</strong>’s Publish<strong>in</strong>g Co.,1991.


A C I< N 0 W L E D G E M E N T SXi i iRosamond Mitchell and Florence Myles for ‘Second language learn<strong>in</strong>g: key concepts andissues’ <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Theories, 1999.David Nunan and ELTjournal for ‘<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> grammar <strong>in</strong> context’ <strong>in</strong> ELTJourna1,Vol. 52, No.2, 1998. Reproduced by permission of ELTjournal and Oxford University Press.Jack Richards and Cambridge University Press for ‘Beyond methods’ <strong>in</strong> The <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Matrix, 1990.Celia Roberts for ‘<strong>Language</strong> through acquisition or language socialisation <strong>in</strong> and throughdiscourse’ <strong>in</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers zn Apphed L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Vol. 4, Thames Valley University, 1998.Peter Skehan for ‘Comprehension and production strategies <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> ACognitiveApproach to Lanpaje Learn<strong>in</strong>g by Peter Skehan ( Oxford University Press 1998.)Reproduced by permission of Oxford university Press.Leo van Lier for ‘Constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resources <strong>in</strong> classroom talk: issues of equality andsymmetry’ <strong>in</strong> Learnmg Foreign and Second <strong>Language</strong>c. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of theModern <strong>Language</strong> Association of America.While the publishers and editors have madc every effort to contact authors and copyrightholders of works repr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> Englzhh <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>, this has notbeen possible <strong>in</strong> every case. They would welcome correspondence from <strong>in</strong>dividuals orcompanies they have been unable to trace.We would like to thank the authors who contributed their chapters, as well as colleagueswith<strong>in</strong> and outsideThe Open University and Macquarie University who gave advice on thecontents. Special thanks arc due to the follow<strong>in</strong>g people for their assistance <strong>in</strong> theproduction of this book.Helen Boyce (course manager)Pam Burns and Libby Brill (course ~ecretarics)Liz Freeman (Copublish<strong>in</strong>g)Nanette Reynolds, Frances Wilson and the staff of the Resource Centre of the NationalCentre for <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Research, Macquarie University.Critical readersProfessor Vijay K. Bhatia (Department of <strong>English</strong>, City University, Hong Kong)Geoff Thompson (Applied <strong>English</strong> Languagc Studies Unit, Liverpool University, UK)Professor Leo van Lier (Educational L<strong>in</strong>guistics, University of Monterey, USA).External assessorProfessor Ronald Carter (Department of <strong>English</strong> Studies, Nott<strong>in</strong>gham University, LIK)Developmental testersIlona Cziraky (Italy)Eladyr Maria Norbert0 da Silva (Brazil)Chitrita Mukerjee (Australia)Dorien Gonzales (UK)Patricia Williams (Denmark).


xivAC I


Christopher N. Candl<strong>in</strong>and Neil MercerINTRODUCTIONWhen Macquarie University, <strong>in</strong> Sydney, Australia, and The Open University, <strong>in</strong> MiltonKeynes, England, decided to collaborate on the development of new curriculum materialsfor study at Master’s level, the partnership brought together The Open University’sexperience <strong>in</strong> open learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the field of education, and Macquarie’s experience <strong>in</strong> appliedl<strong>in</strong>guistics and language education, backed by <strong>its</strong> own exist<strong>in</strong>g distance learn<strong>in</strong>g programme.The collection of articles <strong>in</strong> this book and <strong>its</strong> two companion volumes are one result of thatcollaboration. While the edited collections have been designed as one part of an overall studyprogramme, complemented by other learn<strong>in</strong>g and study materials compris<strong>in</strong>g study guidesand accompany<strong>in</strong>g video and audio record<strong>in</strong>gs, they stand alone as extensive yet focusedcollections of articles which address key contemporary issues <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>gand applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics.A major concern <strong>in</strong> edit<strong>in</strong>g these three volumes has been the desire to present <strong>English</strong>language teach<strong>in</strong>g (ELT) <strong>in</strong> a variety of specific <strong>in</strong>stitutional, geographic and cultural contexts.Hence, as far as possible across the three volumes, we have attempted to highlight debate,discussion and illustration of current issues from different parts of the <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g and<strong>English</strong>-us<strong>in</strong>g world, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those where <strong>English</strong> is not learned as a first language. In do<strong>in</strong>gthis we recognize that <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g comprises a global community of teachersand learners <strong>in</strong> a range of social contexts.It is <strong>English</strong> Languoge<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong> which is the title of this second volume<strong>in</strong> the series, and it will be useful to decide early on what we mean by this term. We havea number of <strong>in</strong>terpretations and perspectives <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. One that is central is that of theclassroom context <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>teractions betwecn teachers and learners have an effect on thenature and quality of language learn<strong>in</strong>g. No language teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g takes placehowever, <strong>in</strong> a classroom which is isolated from the world of experiences and personalengagements and <strong>in</strong>vestments of learners outside the classroom <strong>its</strong>elf. In that sense the widersociol context of life outside the classroom has an important effect on what takes place <strong>in</strong> these<strong>in</strong>teractions between learners and teachers, and among learners. For many learners, thecontexts outside the classroom are not only where they make use of the <strong>English</strong> they havelearned <strong>in</strong> class, but they can also constitute a powerful <strong>in</strong>centive (or dis<strong>in</strong>centive) for furtherlearn<strong>in</strong>g. Moreover, it is not only the contexts of learn<strong>in</strong>g and us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> that areimportant. We need also to understand the professional context of teachers’ practicesthemselves with<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>teractive process of classroom teach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally, weneed to take account of the cocio-cultural context by which communicat<strong>in</strong>g partners <strong>in</strong> this


2 INTRODUCTIONprocess evoke and create shared knowledge and use it for mak<strong>in</strong>g sense together, <strong>in</strong> a senseconstruct<strong>in</strong>g the overarch<strong>in</strong>g context for successful language learn<strong>in</strong>g.No collection of papers about <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> can hope to be comprehensive.The world of ELT <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> diversity, of learners, teachers, of schools and <strong>in</strong>stitutions, cultures,countries, contents, and pedagogies cannot bc captured even <strong>in</strong> a series ofthree books.Whata structured collection of selected papers like this can do is to map out the territory, and fill<strong>in</strong> enough of the topographical features so that the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g reader can obta<strong>in</strong> an overallimpression of <strong>its</strong> cartography, while the experienced reader can br<strong>in</strong>g hcr or his own richexperience of travell<strong>in</strong>g and map-mak<strong>in</strong>g to fill <strong>in</strong> the details of those territories of whichthey have special awareness and knowledge. We need to be cautious, however. No map isneutral.Thc first maps were products of the cartographers of Europe, so their world was aEuro-centric one, and, <strong>in</strong> their own S<strong>in</strong>o-centric way, those devised by the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese were justas biascd. Readers have been alerted, therefore, to a natural tendency towards a particularprojection. Our ELT map <strong>in</strong> this book offers a social and socio-cultural perspective onlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. At the same time, maps have to be true to thcir territories, and it wouldbe absurd to ignore a psychological perspectivc on language learn<strong>in</strong>g, one which highlightedthe cognitive processes of the <strong>in</strong>dividual learner, engag<strong>in</strong>g with the <strong>in</strong>tricacies of a newcommunicative code. Maps are not only to be followed, however. They have always servedas <strong>in</strong>centives for further and more rcf<strong>in</strong>cd map-mak<strong>in</strong>g. In the same way, teachers do not justfollow a sct of presented <strong>in</strong>structions, they actively create and chart their own progressthrough the territories of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, it is important thatsuch a focused collection as this gives a major placc to classroom-based research, <strong>in</strong> particular,research which exam<strong>in</strong>es the processes of teach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g, us<strong>in</strong>g that evidence whichis most to hand <strong>in</strong> classrooms, namely the productive talk of teachcrs and learners.What a collection of papers needs to have, is an argument, one which carries the readcrtowards engagement with particular issues and questions, offer<strong>in</strong>g through <strong>its</strong> structure justthat amount of guidance necessary. Ultimately, though, whether we have gauged the rightdegree of that guidance required, or simply led readers by the nose, only you can say. Whatwe have done as a guid<strong>in</strong>g structure is to take three ma<strong>in</strong> perspectives on <strong>English</strong> languageteach<strong>in</strong>g: an explanation of some hypotheses about language learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>its</strong> processes; an<strong>in</strong>terpretation of learncrs’ anti teachers’ strategies and goals <strong>in</strong> the classroom context, theirpurposes and their beliefs; and, f<strong>in</strong>ally, a description and analysis of teachers’ and learners’behaviours and practices, who they are, what they do, what they th<strong>in</strong>k about languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g and what their attitudes are.How is language learn<strong>in</strong>g expla<strong>in</strong>ed?The argument beg<strong>in</strong>s with a focus on the explanation oflanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g with a paper byRosamund Mitchell and Florence Myles. The authors outl<strong>in</strong>e a model of second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g and identify <strong>its</strong> key factors.Thrce key questions underp<strong>in</strong> all these factors: What isthe nature of language? What is the nature of the language learn<strong>in</strong>g process? What arethe characteristics of the second language learner! In address<strong>in</strong>g these questions the paperidentifies the complementarity of nature and nurture <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g, and relates whatresearch has to say about language learn<strong>in</strong>g with what we know about learn<strong>in</strong>g moregenerally. At the same time, the paper highlights one of the abid<strong>in</strong>g questions about teach<strong>in</strong>gand learn<strong>in</strong>g, the tension between yaternaticity and creativity <strong>in</strong> learners’ performance.<strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g is clearly not just about processes. It <strong>in</strong>volves learners. So, ask<strong>in</strong>g questionsabout who thcsc learners are and what learner characteristics and factors affect language


INTRODUCTION 3learn<strong>in</strong>g, and <strong>in</strong> which ways, is a central question for teachers of language. Patsy Lightbownand N<strong>in</strong>a Spada take up this necessary dualism <strong>in</strong> their account of the cognitive andbehavioural characteristics of what some researchers have referred to as the ‘good languagelearner’. As we will see later <strong>in</strong> the argument of this book, there has to be a third aspect toany such account, namely the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the social conditions of language learn<strong>in</strong>g on theeffectiveness of language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Many learners don’t learn languages <strong>in</strong> classrooms. Theylearn them more or less well or badly, on the street, <strong>in</strong> the community, and <strong>in</strong> the workplace.Certa<strong>in</strong>ly, Lightbown’s and Spada’s territory abuts that of Mitchell and Myles. Factors suchas motivation, aptitude, personality, <strong>in</strong>telligence, learner preferences and learner beliefs, willbe high on any teacher’s list, but so will factors of age, social background, gender andeducational atta<strong>in</strong>ment.Research<strong>in</strong>g second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, and explor<strong>in</strong>g the relationship between research<strong>in</strong>gand teach<strong>in</strong>g is a key element <strong>in</strong> what some have referred to as the teacher as ‘reflective’practitioner. Rod Ellis’ paper on research and pedagogy <strong>in</strong> the context of second languageacquisition squarely addresses this relationship. Questions of decision-driven researchemanat<strong>in</strong>g from practical classroom problems, or knowledge-driven research start<strong>in</strong>g fromtheoretical hypotheses, are but two sides of the same co<strong>in</strong>. At the heart are the practices ofthe classroom, or encounters with the target language <strong>in</strong> other contexts.That these worldsof teach<strong>in</strong>g and research have often been at odds is an issue for this paper, and for this bookas a whole to explore. What Ellis identifies, however, is the importance of mapp<strong>in</strong>g thecultures of teach<strong>in</strong>g and research<strong>in</strong>g and achiev<strong>in</strong>g at least mutual understand<strong>in</strong>g, if not activecollaboration. What is clear after read<strong>in</strong>g Ellis is that it isn’t go<strong>in</strong>g to be enough for teachersto write ‘Here be dragons’ and steer the teach<strong>in</strong>g ship away from the rocky coastl<strong>in</strong>e ofresearch. One useful and productive ground for such collaboration is that of research<strong>in</strong>glearners’styles and strategies <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g, loolung at what learners do as aspects of theirpersonality, or <strong>in</strong> response to problems and tasks that teach<strong>in</strong>g, or just life <strong>its</strong>elf, confrontsthem. Peter Skehan’s paper has this dual focus and he locates his discussion <strong>in</strong> the key areaof learners’ comprehension of foreign language texts, written or spoken, exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g therelationship between <strong>in</strong>put to the learner, what the learner confronts, and what the learnerproduces herself, the output of Icarn<strong>in</strong>g. Important for Skehan, and for our general argument<strong>in</strong> this book, are the ways <strong>in</strong> which learners negotiate mean<strong>in</strong>g, guided by teachers, <strong>in</strong> theirroad towards understand<strong>in</strong>g the foreign language.If negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g smacks of the marketplace, then perhaps that is no bad imagefor the exchange of language goods which characterizes both classrooms and social<strong>in</strong>teractions more generally. Estimat<strong>in</strong>g the values to be placed on these goods is, after all,what a good deal of teach<strong>in</strong>g (and learn<strong>in</strong>g) is all about. Leo van Lier’s, Celia Roberts’and Michael Breen’s papers are all sited <strong>in</strong> the markctplace of learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g. It is time,then, to beg<strong>in</strong> to look at the contexts oflearn<strong>in</strong>g. Now a new set of questions arise. Howlearners <strong>in</strong>teract with each other and other speakers, what do they do when they arc learn<strong>in</strong>ga language, what effect their attitudes, beliefs and feel<strong>in</strong>gs have on language learn<strong>in</strong>g, whatk<strong>in</strong>ds of personal <strong>in</strong>vestment they are prepared to make, how far they can draw on the supportof others, what effects teach<strong>in</strong>g has on learn<strong>in</strong>g, and to what extent the social conditions andpriorities of the social world outside the classroom, and the learners’ places <strong>in</strong> that world,affect what learners do <strong>in</strong> classrooms and how effectively they can learn.Address<strong>in</strong>g these questions suggests a need for some redraw<strong>in</strong>g of the dimensions of thesecond language learn<strong>in</strong>g map. In fact, as we will see <strong>in</strong> the papers which follow <strong>in</strong> thecollection, such questions make us redraw our projection <strong>in</strong> a number of important ways:to take account of the learn<strong>in</strong>g of strategic competence not merely of language competence;of the appraisal of learn<strong>in</strong>g sites, contexts and modes as key variables <strong>in</strong> language acquisition;


6 INTRODUCTIONcould be used by adult sccond language learners at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g a secondlanguage. Of particular <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the paper is her exposition of what she and her colleaguesrefer to as the ‘teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g cycle’.We have emphasized the importance to our understand<strong>in</strong>g of sccond language learn<strong>in</strong>gof explor<strong>in</strong>g the socio-cultural contexts of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>side and outside the classroom. Thishas been and is a core theme of many papers <strong>in</strong> this book.There has, howevcr, been a tacitassumption, though perhaps not so much <strong>in</strong> the paper by Roberts earlier, that such contextscalled up differentiated, but essentially cooperative learners.That this may not be so, and oftenis not so, is the theme of the two f<strong>in</strong>al papers <strong>in</strong> this second section of the book, those bySuresh Canagarajah and Keith Chick. Both paperr focus on the degree to which externalsocio-cultural factors, and learncrs’ self-perceptions of their identities as learners of <strong>English</strong>,affect what they do <strong>in</strong> class, and what they are prepared to do <strong>in</strong> class, and thus ultimatelyimp<strong>in</strong>ge on their second language learn<strong>in</strong>g performance. In particular, the papers identifyprocesses of learners’ resistance, <strong>in</strong> the case ofcanagarajah, and <strong>in</strong> the case of Chick, learners’and tcachers’ collusion to frustrate the succcssful implementation of particular methodologiesconsidercd as imported and as culturally alien. Such issues have recently taken on considerableimportance <strong>in</strong> discussions of the cultural appropriateness of some <strong>English</strong> languageteach<strong>in</strong>g. Both these papers have another significance, howevcr, one which relates to Ellis’earlier accounts of research<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Thc papers are valuable not only for their<strong>in</strong>novative re-exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the goals and practices of language teach<strong>in</strong>g, but also for theirclear and detailed account<strong>in</strong>g of a critical ethnographic research methodology <strong>in</strong>tended tobe revelatory not only of the go<strong>in</strong>gs-on of classrooms but more deeply explanatory of theway <strong>in</strong> which the learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> particular is deeply embedded <strong>in</strong> thepolitical, social and educational fabric of post-colonial societies. Once aga<strong>in</strong> they re<strong>in</strong>forceour view that the beliefs and ideologies of teachcrs about all aspects of thcir subject-matterand their practice have a profound cffect on the plann<strong>in</strong>g and the moment-by-momentdecisions thcy take <strong>in</strong> class.To rcfcr to these latter as <strong>in</strong>tuitive, or personal, downplays boththeir effect and our capacity to explore thcir underp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs.That these are dceply engenderedby the social contexualization of learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g, and the educational, social andpolitical contexts of classroom practice can, after read<strong>in</strong>g thcse latter papers, hardly be <strong>in</strong>doubt.Analys<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>gThe importance of the analysis of the <strong>in</strong>teractions among learners and between learnersand teachers to an understand<strong>in</strong>g of the processes of language learn<strong>in</strong>g has been a centralpart of the argument of this book. Explor<strong>in</strong>g these relationships has been both the prov<strong>in</strong>ceof researchers as well as of teachers, and several papers <strong>in</strong> this collection have argued fora closer l<strong>in</strong>k between them, given the tendency for both ‘cultures’ to be separate. Part ofthis distanc<strong>in</strong>g has been due to the difficulty of mak<strong>in</strong>g the results of research necessarily anddirectly applicable to changes <strong>in</strong> classroom practice, or to the design and delivery of <strong>in</strong>novativeteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g materials. Nonethclcss, there are stutlicr of classroom behaviourwhich can help teachers conceptualize those factors which <strong>in</strong>fluence life <strong>in</strong> classrooms,directed at explor<strong>in</strong>g the dual nature of classroom lessons, as pedagogic and as social events.The paper by Michael Breen, cited above, emphasiyes this social and <strong>in</strong>teractional naturc oflanguage Icarn<strong>in</strong>g.Influential <strong>in</strong> this context is the work of the Russian sociocultural psychologist LevVygotsky. Ccntral tovygotskyb thcories about lcarn<strong>in</strong>g is the place accorded to languagc as


INTRODUCTION 7not only a medium for exchang<strong>in</strong>g and construct<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation but also as a tool forth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Language</strong> is seen byvygotsky both as a cultural and a cognitive tool, help<strong>in</strong>g us toorganize our thoughts but also used for reason<strong>in</strong>g, plann<strong>in</strong>g and review<strong>in</strong>g. Of greatestsignificance for the argument and the map of this book, then, is Vygotsky’s <strong>in</strong>sistence thatlearn<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>teractive and social. Such a position resonates well with the earlier papers <strong>in</strong>this collection, notably those by van Lier and Breen, especially with their highlight<strong>in</strong>g of theimportance of study<strong>in</strong>g teacher and learner discourses. Neil Mercer’s paper provides anexample of an <strong>in</strong>-depth study of these discourses of classroom life, as the data from which<strong>in</strong>ferences may be drawn about the processes of language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Mercer’s socio-culturalapproach to the analysis of classroom behaviour s<strong>its</strong> well with earlier papers <strong>in</strong> Part I1 of thisbook, and paves the way for a detailed discursive and l<strong>in</strong>guistic analysis of such classroom<strong>in</strong>teraction provided by Paul<strong>in</strong>e Gibbons’ exhaustive example <strong>in</strong> her paper. She draws onHallidayan systemic functional grammatical analysis to provide her description, <strong>in</strong>cidentallysuggest<strong>in</strong>g a l<strong>in</strong>k between the work of Michael Halliday and that of LevVygotsky, one whichmany other contemporary researchers of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction have also made. Gibbons’paper is also noteworthy for her careful analysis of the immediate contexts of that mean<strong>in</strong>gnegotiation which we have earlier identified as central to language learn<strong>in</strong>g.It may be useful to recall here our comment at the outset of this Introduction that thepapers <strong>in</strong> this collection are all <strong>in</strong> different ways <strong>in</strong>timately concerned with the def<strong>in</strong>ition ofcontext, <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> various <strong>in</strong>terpretations.The relationship between language and context is neitherdirect nor unitary. We can see <strong>in</strong> the papers by Gibbons and Mercer two possible<strong>in</strong>terpretations of this relationship. On the one hand, context is a feature of texts, someth<strong>in</strong>gendur<strong>in</strong>g that belongs to the text-as-entity that l<strong>in</strong>guists seek to describe. In this sense,perhaps that found more <strong>in</strong> Paul<strong>in</strong>e Gibbons’ paper, context may be the texts that learnersand teachers produce, or the physical sett<strong>in</strong>gs with<strong>in</strong> which their texts are produced. On theother hand, perhaps more along the l<strong>in</strong>es suggested by Mercer, context is dynamic, a productof people’s th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, more the configuration of <strong>in</strong>formation that people use for mak<strong>in</strong>g senseof language <strong>in</strong> particular situations. In this sense, context is more of a mental rather than aphysical phenomenon, someth<strong>in</strong>g dynamic and momentary, but dependent for <strong>its</strong> creation<strong>in</strong> the classroom on the careful construct<strong>in</strong>g by thc teacher of a cont<strong>in</strong>uity and a communityof shared understand<strong>in</strong>g with learners.Such avygotskian view of context places a premium on the exploration of the emotionaland affective engagement of learners <strong>in</strong> the acts and processes of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Such anengagement is not explicable, however, only from an analysis <strong>in</strong> terms of the activities of theclassroom. As <strong>in</strong> earlier papers <strong>in</strong> this collection, wider social factors play a role. In her paper,Angel L<strong>in</strong>’s experience as a teacher-researcher <strong>in</strong>to second language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Hong Kongis l<strong>in</strong>ked to the work of the French sociologist Bourdieu <strong>in</strong> an attempt to expla<strong>in</strong> the natureof these factors. Are classrooms replicative of learners’ social worlds or do they have thepower to challenge and transform them? In read<strong>in</strong>g how L<strong>in</strong> addresses this question there isa clear resonance with the papcrs by Canagarajah and Chick <strong>in</strong> the second part of this book.One key example of a site for such a transformation is that of the cultural perspectives andideologies present <strong>in</strong> typical textbooks and the degree to which classroom practices ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>a conformist, or can exercise a challeng<strong>in</strong>g stance <strong>in</strong> relation to them.The papers by Mercer, Gibbons and L<strong>in</strong> all present analyses of the <strong>in</strong>teractive processesof teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Although rather different, the research described <strong>in</strong> each of themencourages the view that the quality of the <strong>in</strong>teraction between teachers and learners <strong>in</strong>thc language classroom, and between learners if they work together, is a strong determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gfactor on what, and how much, is learned and understood by learners. The issue of howclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction can be related to assessment of the outcomes of student learn<strong>in</strong>g is the


8 INTRODUCTIONkey theme <strong>in</strong> the paper by Assia Slimani which follows. From a teacher-researcherperspective, what is significant about her paper is the way <strong>in</strong> which she matches learners’own statements about what they believed they had learned, with thc evidence offered byanalyses of the recorded talk of the lessons concerncd.This provided Slimani with a meansof evaluat<strong>in</strong>g what themes, topics and learn<strong>in</strong>g items suggcsted by learners had actuallyfigured <strong>in</strong> their classroom <strong>in</strong>teractions. Closely connected with this comparative mode ofanalysis is Michael Breen’s second paper <strong>in</strong> this collection where he concentrates on what herefers to as the different discourses of the classroom that learners need to ‘navigate’. Aga<strong>in</strong>,our cartographic metaphor offers perhaps some cxplanatory valuc. For Breen, the classroomis full of dist<strong>in</strong>ctive discourses, <strong>in</strong> part pedagogically oriented, <strong>in</strong> part socially, <strong>in</strong> part<strong>in</strong>dividually. These discourses <strong>in</strong>voke a range of different mean<strong>in</strong>gs and contexts. Learnersare faced with the considerable challenge of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g their ways through this obscured terra<strong>in</strong>,draw<strong>in</strong>g on their natural language <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>cts and analytical capacity to make sense of asemantically and pragmatically complex environment.Mapp<strong>in</strong>g the territory of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g has been the guid<strong>in</strong>gmetaphor for this collection of papers.The cartography of this territory may be left as theprov<strong>in</strong>ce of researchers, or it may be also colonixd by reflective teachers eagcr to exploreand understand more of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> action <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms.Indispensable to such a project, however, is the capacity to describe classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction.This is the theme of the f<strong>in</strong>al paper <strong>in</strong> the collection, by Joan Swann, <strong>in</strong> which she setsout some procedures that <strong>English</strong> language teachers can uscfully follow if they wish todescribe, <strong>in</strong>terpret and expla<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>teractive processes of their own classrooms or those ofcolleagues. We th<strong>in</strong>k that Swann’s paper is an admirable way of clos<strong>in</strong>g a theoretical and apractical collection of papers.What are the general pr<strong>in</strong>ciples that we may derive at the end of this particular journey? Fromthe argumcnts <strong>in</strong> the papers hcre, we would likc to identify thc follow<strong>in</strong>g:A need to focus on thc dist<strong>in</strong>ct roles, activities and purposes for teachers and learnersthat are constructed through classroom practice;A need to recognize language learners as <strong>in</strong>dividuals, work<strong>in</strong>g together <strong>in</strong> theclassroom, but whose learn<strong>in</strong>g is shaped by the context of their wider expcricnce ofliv<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g outsidc the classroom;The requirement on teachers to take an active, guid<strong>in</strong>g role <strong>in</strong> ‘scaffold<strong>in</strong>g’ the learn<strong>in</strong>gof their students, remember<strong>in</strong>g that this is not to downgrade <strong>in</strong> any way the need forlearners to become actively and <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly engaged <strong>in</strong> the processes of classroomlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g and their direction;An appreciation that the patterns of <strong>in</strong>teraction between learners and teachers, and theuse of certa<strong>in</strong> procedures by teachers, can have both positive and negative effects onlanguage learners.


PART ONEHow is language learn<strong>in</strong>gexpla<strong>in</strong>ed?


Chapter 1Rosamond Mitchell and Florence MylesSECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING:I


12 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESis ‘picked up’ <strong>in</strong> the community). Some second language researchers have proposed apr<strong>in</strong>cipled dist<strong>in</strong>ction between formal, conscious learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>formal, unconsciousacquisition .This dist<strong>in</strong>ction attracted much criticism when argued <strong>in</strong> a strong form by StephenKrashen (1 98 1); it still has both <strong>its</strong> active supporters and <strong>its</strong> critics (e.g. Zobll995; Rob<strong>in</strong>son1997). We th<strong>in</strong>k it is difficult to susta<strong>in</strong> systematically when survey<strong>in</strong>g SLL research <strong>in</strong> thebroad way proposed here, and unless specially <strong>in</strong>dicated we will be us<strong>in</strong>g both terms<strong>in</strong>terchangeably.What makes for a good theory?Second language learn<strong>in</strong>g is an immensely complex phenomenon. Millions of human be<strong>in</strong>gshave experience of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, and may have a good practical understand<strong>in</strong>gof the activities which helped them to learn (or perhaps blocked them from learn<strong>in</strong>g). Butthis practical experience, and the common-sense knowledge which it leads to, are clearly notenough to help us understand fully how the process happens.We know, for a start, that peoplecannot reliably describe the language rules which they have somehow <strong>in</strong>ternalized, nor the<strong>in</strong>ner mechanisms which process, store and retrieve many aspects of that new language.We need to understand second language learn<strong>in</strong>g better than we do, for two basicreasons.1 Improved knowledge <strong>in</strong> this particular doma<strong>in</strong> is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>elf, and can alsocontribute to more general understand<strong>in</strong>g about the nature of language, of human learn<strong>in</strong>g,and of <strong>in</strong>tercultural communication, and thus about the human m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>its</strong>elf, as well as howall these are <strong>in</strong>terrelated and affect each other.2 The knowledge will be useful. If we becomc better at expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the learn<strong>in</strong>g process,and are better able to account for both success and failure <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g, there will be a payofffor millions of teachers, and tens of millions of students and othcr learners, who arestruggl<strong>in</strong>g with the task.We can only pursue a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an organi7ed and productiveway if our efforts are guided by some form of theory. For our purposes, a theory is a more orless abstract set of claims about the un<strong>its</strong> that are significant with<strong>in</strong> the phenomenon understudy, the relationships that exist between them, and the processes that br<strong>in</strong>g about change.Thus a theory aims not just at description, hut at explanation. Theories may be embryonicand restricted <strong>in</strong> scope, or more elaborate, explicit and comprehensive. (A theory of L2learn<strong>in</strong>g may deal only with a particular stage or phase of learn<strong>in</strong>g, or with the learn<strong>in</strong>g ofsome particular sub-aspect of language; or it may propose learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms which aremuch more general <strong>in</strong> scope.) Worthwhile theories arc collaborative affairs, which evolvethrough a process of systematic enquiry, <strong>in</strong> which the claims of the theory are assessed aga<strong>in</strong>stsome k<strong>in</strong>d of evidence or data. This may take place through a process of hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>gthrough formal experiment, or through more ecological procedures, where naturallyoccurr<strong>in</strong>g data is analysed and <strong>in</strong>terpreted. (See Brumfit and Mitchell 1990 for fullerdiscussion and exemplification of methods.) F<strong>in</strong>ally, the process of theory build<strong>in</strong>g is areflexive one; new developments <strong>in</strong> the theory lead to the need to collect new <strong>in</strong>formationand explore different phenomena and different patterns <strong>in</strong> the potentially <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite world of‘facts’ and data. Puzzl<strong>in</strong>g ‘facts’, and patterns which fail to fit <strong>in</strong>, lead to new theoretical<strong>in</strong>sights.


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 13To make these ideas more concrete, an example of a particular theory or ‘model’of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g is shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 1.1, taken from Spolsky 1989, p. 28. This.represents a ‘general model of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, as the proposer describesit (Spolsky 1989, p. 14).The model encapsulates this researcher’s theoretical views on theoverall relationship between contextual factors, <strong>in</strong>dividual learner differences, learn<strong>in</strong>gopportunities, and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes. It is thus an ambitious model, <strong>in</strong> the breadth ofphenomena it is try<strong>in</strong>g to expla<strong>in</strong>. The rectangular boxes show the factors (or variables)which the researcher believes are most significant for learn<strong>in</strong>g, i.e. where variation can leadto differences <strong>in</strong> success or failure.The arrows connect<strong>in</strong>g the various boxes show directionsof <strong>in</strong>fluence. The contents of the various boxes are def<strong>in</strong>ed at great length, as consist<strong>in</strong>g ofclusters of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g ‘Conditions’ (74 <strong>in</strong> all: 1989, pp. 16-25), which make languagek-provides<strong>Social</strong> contextleads toAttitudes(of various k<strong>in</strong>ds)which appear <strong>in</strong> thelearner asMotivationwhich jo<strong>in</strong>s with other personalcharacteristics such asknowledgeall of which expla<strong>in</strong> the use thelearner makes of the availableLearn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities (formal or <strong>in</strong>formal)4the <strong>in</strong>terplay between learnerand situation determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gL<strong>in</strong>guistic and non-l<strong>in</strong>guisticoutcomes for the learnerFigurr 1.1 Spolsky’s general modrl of sccond language learn<strong>in</strong>gSource: Spolsky 1989: 28


14 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESlearn<strong>in</strong>g success more or less likely.These summarize the results of a great variety of empiricallanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g research, as Spolsky <strong>in</strong>terprets them.How would we beg<strong>in</strong> to ‘evaluate’ this or any other model, or even more modcstly, todecide that this was a view of the language learn<strong>in</strong>g process with which we felt comfortableand with<strong>in</strong> which we wanted to work?This would depend partly on broader philosophicalpositions: e.g. are we satisfied with an account of human learn<strong>in</strong>g which sees <strong>in</strong>dividualdifferences as both relatively fixed, and also highly <strong>in</strong>fluential for learn<strong>in</strong>g? It would alsodepend on the particular focus of our own <strong>in</strong>terests, with<strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g; thisparticular model seems well adapted for the study of the <strong>in</strong>dividual learner, but has relativelylittle to say about the social relationships <strong>in</strong> which they engage, for example.But whatever the particular focus of a given theory, we would expect to f<strong>in</strong>d thefollow<strong>in</strong>g:1234clear and explicit statements of thc ground the theory is supposed to cover, and theclaims which it is mak<strong>in</strong>g;systematic procedures for confirm<strong>in</strong>g/disconfirm<strong>in</strong>g the theory, through datagather<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>terpretation;not only descriptions of L2 phenomena, but attempts to expla<strong>in</strong> why they are so, andto propose mechanisms for change;last but not least, engagement with other theories <strong>in</strong> the field, and serious attempts toaccount for at least some of the phenomena which arc ‘common ground’ <strong>in</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>gpublic discussion (Long 1990a). The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g sections of this chapter offer aprelim<strong>in</strong>ary overview of numbers of these.Views on the nature of languageLevels of languageL<strong>in</strong>guists have traditionally viewed language as a complex communication system, whichmust be analysed on a number of levels: phonology, syntax, morphology, semantics and lexis,pragmatics, discourse.They have differed about the degree of separateness/<strong>in</strong>tegration of theselevels; e.g. while Chomsky argued at one time that ‘grammar is autonomous and <strong>in</strong>dependentof mean<strong>in</strong>g’ (1 957, p. 17), another tradition <strong>in</strong>itiated by the British l<strong>in</strong>guist Firth claims that‘there is no boundary between lcxis and grammar: lexis and grammar are <strong>in</strong>terdependent’(Stubbs 1996, p. 36). In exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g different perspectives on second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, wewill first of all be look<strong>in</strong>g at the levels of language which they attempt to take <strong>in</strong>to account,and the relative degree of priority they attribute to the different levels. (Does languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g start with words, or with discourse?)We will also exam<strong>in</strong>e the degree of <strong>in</strong>tegration/separation that they assume, across the various levels. We will f<strong>in</strong>d that the control of syntaxis commonly seen as somehow ‘central’ to language learn<strong>in</strong>g, and that most general SLLtheories try to account for development <strong>in</strong> this area. Other levels of language receive muchmore variable attention, and some areas are commonly treated <strong>in</strong> a semi-autonomous way,as specialist fields; this is often true for SLL-oriented studies of pragmatics and of lexicaldevelopment (see e.g. Kasper 1996 on pragmatics; Meara 1996a, 1996b on vocabulary).Competence and performanceThroughout the twentieth century, l<strong>in</strong>guists have also disagreed <strong>in</strong> other ways over theirma<strong>in</strong> focus of <strong>in</strong>terest and of study. Should this be the collection and analysis of actual attestedsamples of language <strong>in</strong> use, for example by record<strong>in</strong>g and analys<strong>in</strong>g people’s speech? Or


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 15should it be to theorize underly<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and rules which govern language behaviour,<strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> potentially <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite variety?The l<strong>in</strong>guist Noam Chomsky has famously argued that it isthe bus<strong>in</strong>ess of theoretical l<strong>in</strong>guistics to study and model underly<strong>in</strong>g language competence,rather than the performance data of actual utterances which people have produced (Chomsky1965). By competence, Chomsky is referr<strong>in</strong>g to the abstract and hidden representation oflanguage knowledge held <strong>in</strong>side our heads, with <strong>its</strong> potential to create and understand orig<strong>in</strong>alutterances <strong>in</strong> a given language. As we shall see, this view has been <strong>in</strong>fluential <strong>in</strong> much secondlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g research.However, for l<strong>in</strong>guists committed to &s dualist position, there are difficulties <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>gcompetence. <strong>Language</strong> performance data are believed to be an imperfect reflection ofcompetence, partly because of the process<strong>in</strong>g complications which are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> speak<strong>in</strong>gor other forms of language production, and which lead to errors and slips. More importantly,it is believed that, <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, the <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite creativity of the underly<strong>in</strong>g system can neveradequately be reflected <strong>in</strong> a f<strong>in</strong>ite data sample (see e.g. Chomsky 1965, p. IS). Strictlyspeak<strong>in</strong>g, many students of language competence believe it can be accessed only <strong>in</strong>directly,and under controlled conditions, e.g. through grammaticality iudgement tests (roughly, whenpeople are offered sample sentences, which are <strong>in</strong> (dis)agreement with the rules proposedfor the underly<strong>in</strong>g competence, and <strong>in</strong>vited to say whether they th<strong>in</strong>k they are grammaticalor not: Sorace 1996).This split between competence and performance has never been accepted by all l<strong>in</strong>guists,however, with l<strong>in</strong>guists <strong>in</strong> the British tradition of Firth and Halliday argu<strong>in</strong>g for radicallydifferent models <strong>in</strong> which this dist<strong>in</strong>ction between competence and performance does notappear. In a recent review of this tradition, Stubbs quotes Firth as describ<strong>in</strong>g such dualismsas ‘a quite unnecessary nuisance’ (Firth 1957, p. 2n, quoted <strong>in</strong> Stubbs 1996, p. 44). In theFirthian view, the only option for l<strong>in</strong>guists is to study language <strong>in</strong> use, and there is no oppositionbetween language as system, and observed <strong>in</strong>stances of language behaviour; the onlydifference is one of perspective.Of course, the abstract language system cannot be ‘read’ directly off small samplesof actual text, any more than the underly<strong>in</strong>g climate of some geographical region of theworld can be modelled from today’s weather (a metaphor of Halliday’s: Stubbs 1996, pp.44-5). The arrival of corpus l<strong>in</strong>guistics, <strong>in</strong> which very large corpora compris<strong>in</strong>g millionsof words of runn<strong>in</strong>g text can be stored electronically and analysed with a grow<strong>in</strong>g range ofsoftware tools, has revitalized the writ<strong>in</strong>g of‘observation-based grammars’ (Aarts 1991 ), ofthe <strong>in</strong>tegrated k<strong>in</strong>d favoured by Firthian l<strong>in</strong>guistics. ‘Work with corpora provides new waysof consider<strong>in</strong>g the relation between data and theory, by show<strong>in</strong>g how theory can be grounded<strong>in</strong> publicly accessible corpus data’ (Stubbs 1996, p. 46). For example, the <strong>English</strong> corpusbasedwork of the COBUILD team directed by John S<strong>in</strong>clair has claimed to reveal ‘quiteunsuspected patterns of language’ (S<strong>in</strong>clair 1991, p. xvii), offer<strong>in</strong>g new <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to the<strong>in</strong>terconnectedness of lexis and grammar.In mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of contemporary perspectives on SLL, then, we will also need to takeaccount of the extent to which a competence/performance dist<strong>in</strong>ction is assumed.This willhave significant consequences for the research methodologies associated with variouspositions, e.g. the extent to which these pay attention to naturalistic corpora of learnerlanguage samples, or rely on more controlled and focused but more <strong>in</strong>direct ~ test<strong>in</strong>g oflearners’ underly<strong>in</strong>g knowledge. For obvious reasons, theorists’ views on the relationshipbetween competence and performance are also closely l<strong>in</strong>ked to their view of the languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>its</strong>elf, and <strong>in</strong> particular, to their view of the way <strong>in</strong> which language use(i.e. speak<strong>in</strong>g or writ<strong>in</strong>g a language) can contribute to language learn<strong>in</strong>g (i.e. develop<strong>in</strong>ggrammatical or lexical competence <strong>in</strong> the language) .


16 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESThe language learn<strong>in</strong>g processNature and nurtureDiscussions about processes of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g have always been colouredby debates on fundamental issues <strong>in</strong> human learn<strong>in</strong>g more generally. One of these is thenature-nurture debate. How much of human learn<strong>in</strong>g derives from <strong>in</strong>nate predispositions,i.e. some form of genetic pre-programm<strong>in</strong>g, and how much of it derives from social andcultural experiences which <strong>in</strong>fluence us as we grow up? In the twentieth century, the bestknown controversy on this issue as far as first language learn<strong>in</strong>g was concerned <strong>in</strong>volvedthe behaviourist psychologist B. F. Sk<strong>in</strong>ner and the l<strong>in</strong>guist Noam Chomsky. Sk<strong>in</strong>nerattempted to argue that language <strong>in</strong> all <strong>its</strong> essentials could be and was taught to the youngchild by the same mechanisms which he believed accounted for other types of learn<strong>in</strong>g.(In Sk<strong>in</strong>ner’s case, the mechanisms were those envisaged by general behaviourist learn<strong>in</strong>gtheory ~ essentially, copy<strong>in</strong>g and memoriz<strong>in</strong>g bchaviours encountered <strong>in</strong> the surround<strong>in</strong>genvironment. From this po<strong>in</strong>t of view, language could be learned primarily by imitat<strong>in</strong>gcaretakers’ speech.)Chomsky, on the other hand, has argued consistently for the view that human languageis too complex to be learned, <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> entirety, from the performance data actually available tothe child; we must therefore have some <strong>in</strong>nate predisposition to expect natural languages tobe organized <strong>in</strong> particular ways and not others. For example, all natural languages have wordclasses such as Noun and Verb, and grammar rules which apply to these word classes. It isthis type of <strong>in</strong>formation which Chomsky doubts children could discover from scratch, <strong>in</strong> thespeech they hear around them. Instead, he argues that there must be some <strong>in</strong>nate core ofabstract knowledge about language form, which pre-specifies a framework for all naturalhuman languages. This core of knowledge is currently known as Urnversa1 Grammar.For our purposes, it is enough to note that child language specialists now generallyaccept the basic notion of an <strong>in</strong>nate predisposition to language, though this cannot accountfor all aspects of language development, which results from an <strong>in</strong>teraction between <strong>in</strong>nateand environmental factors. That is, complementary mechanisms, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g active<strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> language use, are equally essential for the development of communicativecompetence (see e.g. Foster 1990).How does the nature-nurture debate impact on theories of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g?If humans are endowed with an <strong>in</strong>nate predisposition for language, then perhaps theyshould be able to learn as many languages as they need or want to, provided (importantprovisos!) that the time, circumstances, and motivation are available. On the other hand,the environmental circumstances for L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g differ systematically from L1 learn<strong>in</strong>g,except where <strong>in</strong>fants are reared <strong>in</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual surround<strong>in</strong>gs. Should we be aim<strong>in</strong>g toreproduce the ‘natural’ circumstances of L1 learn<strong>in</strong>g as far as possible for the L2 student?This was a fashionable view <strong>in</strong> the 1970s, but one which downplayed some very real socialand psychological obstacles. In the last twenty years there has been a closer and more criticalexam<strong>in</strong>ation of environmental factors which seem to <strong>in</strong>fluence L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g; some of theseare detailed briefly under ‘The relationship between second language use and second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g’, on page 2 1 .ModularityA further issue of controversy for students of the human bra<strong>in</strong> has been the extent to whichthe bra<strong>in</strong> should be viewed as modular or unitary.That is, should we see the bra<strong>in</strong> as a s<strong>in</strong>gle,flexible organism, with one general set of procedures for learn<strong>in</strong>g and stor<strong>in</strong>g different k<strong>in</strong>ds


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 17of knowledge and skills? Or, is it more helpfully understood as a bundle of modules, withdist<strong>in</strong>ctive mechanisms relevant to differcnt types of knowledge (e.g. Fodor 1983)?The modular view has consistently found support from with<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistics, most famously<strong>in</strong> the further debate between Chomsky and the child development psychologist, Jean Piaget.This debate is reported <strong>in</strong> Piatelli-Palmar<strong>in</strong>i (1 980), and has been re-exam<strong>in</strong>ed many times;a helpful recent summary is offered by Johnson (1 996, pp. 6-30). Briefly, Piaget argued thatlanguage was simply one manifestation of the more general skill of symbolic representation,acquired as a stage <strong>in</strong> general cognitive dcvelopment; no special mechanism was thereforerequired to account for first language acquisition. Chomsky’s general view is that not onlyis language too complex to be learned from environmental exposure (his criticism ofSk<strong>in</strong>ner), it is also too dist<strong>in</strong>ctivc <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> structure to be learnable by general cognitive means.Universal Grammar is thus endowed with <strong>its</strong> own dist<strong>in</strong>ctivc mechanisms for learn<strong>in</strong>g.There are many l<strong>in</strong>guists today who support the concept of a dist<strong>in</strong>ctive language module<strong>in</strong> the m<strong>in</strong>d.There are also those who argue that language competence <strong>its</strong>elfis modular, withdifferent aspects of language knowledge be<strong>in</strong>g stored and accessed <strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ctive ways.However, there is no general agreement on the number and nature of such modules, nor onhow they relate to othcr aspects of cognition.Modularity and second language learn<strong>in</strong>gThe possible role of an <strong>in</strong>nate, specialist language module <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g hasbeen much discussed <strong>in</strong> reccnt years. If such <strong>in</strong>nate mechanisms <strong>in</strong>deed exist, there are fourlogical possibilities:1 that they cont<strong>in</strong>uc to operate dur<strong>in</strong>g second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, and make key aspectsof second language learn<strong>in</strong>g possible, <strong>in</strong> the same way that they make first languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g possible;2 that after the acquisition of the first language <strong>in</strong> early childhood, these mechanismsccase to be operable, and second languages must be learned by other means;3 that the mechanisms themselves are no longer operable, but that the first languageprovides a model of a natural language and how it works, which can be ‘copied’ <strong>in</strong>some way when learn<strong>in</strong>g a second language;4 that dist<strong>in</strong>ctive learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms for language rema<strong>in</strong> available, but only <strong>in</strong> part,and must be supplcmented by other mcans.The first position was popularized <strong>in</strong> the sccond language learn<strong>in</strong>g field by Stephen Krashen<strong>in</strong> the 1970s, <strong>in</strong> a basic form. While Krashen’s theoretical views have been criticized, this hasby no means led to the disappearancc of modular proposals to account for SLL. Instead, thisparticular perspective has been rcvitalized by the cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g development of Chomsky’sUniversal Grammar proposals (Cook and Ncwson 1996).On the other hand, th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about those general learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms which maybe operat<strong>in</strong>g at least for adult learners of second languages has also developed further, s<strong>in</strong>cee.g. thc orig<strong>in</strong>al proposals of McLaughl<strong>in</strong> (1987, pp. 133-53). Most obviously, the work ofthe cognitive psychologist J. R. Anderson on human learn<strong>in</strong>g, from an <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>gperspective, has becn applied to various aspects of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g by differentresearchers (Johnson 1996; O’Malley and Chamot 199O;Towcll and Hawk<strong>in</strong>s 1994).


18 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESSystematicity and variability <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>gWhen the utterances produced by L2 lcarners are exam<strong>in</strong>ed and comparcd with targetlanguage norms, they are often condemned as full of crrors or mistakes. Traditionally,language teachers have often viewed thcse errors as the result of carelessness or lack ofconcentration on the part of learners. If only learners would try harder, surely theirproductions could accurately reflcct the TL rulcs which they had been taught! In the midtwentiethccntury, under the <strong>in</strong>fluence of behaviourist learn<strong>in</strong>g theory, errors were oftenviewed as the result of ‘bad hab<strong>its</strong>’, which could be eradicated if only learners did enoughrote learn<strong>in</strong>g and pattern drill<strong>in</strong>g us<strong>in</strong>g target language modcls.One of the big lessons which has been lcarned from the research of recent decades isthat though learners’ L2 Utterances may be deviant by comparison with target languagenorms, they are by no means lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> y am. Errors and mistakes are patterned, and thoughsome regular errors are due to the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the first language, this is by no means true ofall of them, or even of a majority of them. Instead, there is a good deal of evidence thatlearners work their way through a number of developmental stages, from very primitive anddeviant versions of the L2, to progressively morc elaborate and target-like versions. Just likefully proficient users of a language, their language productions can be described by a set ofunderly<strong>in</strong>g rules; thcse <strong>in</strong>terim rules have their own <strong>in</strong>tegrity and are not just <strong>in</strong>adequatelyapplied versions of theTL rules.A clear example, which has been studied for a range of target languages, has to do withthe formation of negative sentences. It has commonly been found that learners start off bytack<strong>in</strong>g a negative particle of some k<strong>in</strong>d on to the end of an utterance (no you are play<strong>in</strong>ghere); next, they learn to <strong>in</strong>sert a basic negative particle <strong>in</strong>to the verb phrase (Mariananot com<strong>in</strong>g today); and f<strong>in</strong>ally, they learn to manipulate modifications to auxiliaries andother details of negation morphology, <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with the full TL rules for negation ( I can’t playthat one) (examples from Ellis 1994, p. 100).This k<strong>in</strong>d of data has commonly been <strong>in</strong>terpretedto show that, at least as far as key parts of the L2 grammar are conccrned, learners’ developmentfollows a common route, even if the rate at which learners actually travel along thiscommon route may be very different.This ysternaticity <strong>in</strong> the language produced by L2 learners is of course paralleled <strong>in</strong> theearly stages through which first language learners also pass <strong>in</strong> a highly regular manner.Towel1and Hawk<strong>in</strong>s identify it as one of the key features which L2 lcarn<strong>in</strong>g theories are requiredto expla<strong>in</strong> (1 994, p. 5).However, learner language (or <strong>in</strong>terlanguage, as it is commonly called) is not onlycharacterized by systematicity. Learner language systems are presumably -<strong>in</strong>deed, hopefully- unstable and <strong>in</strong> course of change; certa<strong>in</strong>ly, they arc characterized also by high degrees ofvariability (Towell and Hawk<strong>in</strong>s 1994, p. 5). Most obviously, learners’ utterances seem to varyfrom moment to moment, <strong>in</strong> the types of‘errors’ which are made, and learners seem liableto switch between a rangc of correct and <strong>in</strong>correct forms over lengthy periods of time. Awell-known example offered by Ellis <strong>in</strong>volves a child learner of <strong>English</strong> as L2 who scemedto produce the utterances no look my card, don’t look my card <strong>in</strong>terchangeably ovcr an extcndedperiod (1 985). Myles et al. (1 998) have produced similar data from a classroom learner’sFrench as L2, who variably produced forms such as non animal, je n’ai pas de animal with<strong>in</strong>the same 20 m<strong>in</strong>utes or so (to say that he did not have a pet; thc correctTL form should beje n’ai pas d’animal). Here, <strong>in</strong> contrast to the underly<strong>in</strong>g systcmaticity earlier claimcd for thedevelopment of rules of negation, we see performance vary<strong>in</strong>g quite substantially frommoment to momcnt.Like systematicity, variability is also found <strong>in</strong> child language tlcvelopment. However, thevariability found among L2 learners is undoubtedly more ‘extreme’ than that found forI


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 19children; aga<strong>in</strong>, variability is described byTowell et a/. (1996) as a central feature of learner<strong>in</strong>terlanguage which L2 theories will have to expla<strong>in</strong>.Creativity and rout<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>gIn the last section, we referred to evidence which shows that learners’ <strong>in</strong>terlanguageproductions can be described as systematic, at least <strong>in</strong> part. This systematicity is l<strong>in</strong>kedto another key concept, that of creativity. Learners’ surface utterances can be l<strong>in</strong>ked tounderly<strong>in</strong>g rule systems, even if these seem primitive and deviant compared with the targetlanguage system. It logically follows that learners can produce orig<strong>in</strong>al utterances, i.e. thattheir rule system can generate utterances appropriate to a given context, which the learnerhas never heard before.There is of course plenty of eommon-sense evidence that learners can put their L2knowledge to creative use, even at the very earliest stages of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g. It becomes mostobvious that ths is happen<strong>in</strong>g, when learners produce Utterances like the highly deviant nonanimal (no animal ‘1 haven’t got any pet’), which we cited before.This is not an utterancewhich any native speaker of French would produce (other than, perhaps, a very young child);much the most likely way that the learner has produced it is through apply<strong>in</strong>g an extremelyprimitive <strong>in</strong>terlanguage rule for negation, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with some basic vocabulary.But how did this same learner manage to produce the near-targetje n’ai pas de anima/,with <strong>its</strong> negative particles correctly <strong>in</strong>serted with<strong>in</strong> the verb phrase, and correspond<strong>in</strong>galmost-perfect modification to the morphology of the noun phrase, with<strong>in</strong> a fcw m<strong>in</strong>utes ofthe other form? For us, the most likely explanation is that at this po<strong>in</strong>t he was reproduc<strong>in</strong>gan utterance which he has <strong>in</strong>deed heard before (and probably rehearsed), which has beenmemorized as an unanalysed whole, a formula or a prefabricated chunk.Work <strong>in</strong> corpus l<strong>in</strong>guistics has led us to the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g recognition that formulas androut<strong>in</strong>es play an important part <strong>in</strong> everyday language use by native speakers; when we talk,our everyday L1 utterances are a complex mix of creativity and prefabrication (S<strong>in</strong>clair1991). In L1 acquisition research also, the use of unanalysed chunks by young childrenhas been commonly observed. For L1 learners, the contribution of chunks seems limited byprocess<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>ts; for older L2 learncrs, however, mcmorization of lengthy, unanalysedlanguage rout<strong>in</strong>es is much more possible. (Th<strong>in</strong>k of those opera s<strong>in</strong>gers who successfullymemorize and deliver entire parts, <strong>in</strong> languages they do not otherwise control!)Analysis of L2 data produced by classroom learners <strong>in</strong> particular, seems to showextensive and systematic use of chunks to fulfil communicative needs <strong>in</strong> the early stages(Myles et al. 1998). Studies of <strong>in</strong>formal learners also provide some evidence of chunk use.This phenomenon has attracted relatively little attention <strong>in</strong> recent times, compared withthat given to learner creativity and systematicity (Wc<strong>in</strong>ert 1995). However, we bclieve it iscommon enough <strong>in</strong> L2 spontaneous production (and not only <strong>in</strong> the opera house), to needsome more susta<strong>in</strong>ed attention from L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g theory.Incomplete success and fossilizationYoung children learn<strong>in</strong>g thcir first language embark on the enterprise <strong>in</strong> widely vary<strong>in</strong>gsituations around the world, sometimes <strong>in</strong> conditions of extreme poverty and deprivation,whether physical or social.Yet with remarkable uniformity, at the end of five years or so,they have achieved a very substantial measure of success. Teachcrs and students know totheir cost that this is by no means the case with second languages, embarked on after thesecritical early years. Few, if any, adult learners ever come to blend <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guishably with the


20 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLEScommunity of target language ‘native speakers’; most rema<strong>in</strong> noticeably deviant <strong>in</strong> theirpronunciation, and many cont<strong>in</strong>ue to make grammar mistakes and to search for words, evenwhen well motivated to learn, after years of study, residencc and/or work <strong>in</strong> contact withthe target language.Second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, then, is typified by <strong>in</strong>complete success; the claimed systematicevolution of our underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terlanguage rules towards the target language system seemsdoomcd, most often, never to <strong>in</strong>tegrate completely with <strong>its</strong> goal. Indeed, while some learnersgo on learn<strong>in</strong>g, others seem to cease to make any visible progress, no matter how manylanguage classes they attend, or how activcly they cont<strong>in</strong>ue to use their second language forcommunicative purposes.The termfossilization is commonly used to describe this phenomenon,when a learner’s L2 system seems to ‘freeze’, or become stuck, at somc more or lessdeviant stage.These phenomena of <strong>in</strong>complete success and fossilization are also significant ‘facts’ aboutthe process of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g, which any serious theory must eventually expla<strong>in</strong>. As we willsee, explanations of two basic types have <strong>in</strong> fact been offered.The first group of explanationsare psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic: the language-specific learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms available to the young childsimply cease to work for older learners, at least partly, and no amount of study and effortcan recreate them.Thc second group of explanations are sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic: oldcr L2 learners donot have the social opportunities, or the motivation, to identify completely with the nativespeaker community, but may <strong>in</strong>stead valuc their dist<strong>in</strong>ctive identity as learners or asforeigners.Cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>jluences <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>gEveryday observation tells us that learncrs’ performance <strong>in</strong> a second language is <strong>in</strong>fluencedby the language, or languages, that they already know.This is rout<strong>in</strong>ely obvious from learners’‘foreign accent’, i.c. pronunciation which bcars traces of the phonology of their firstlanguage. It is also obvious when learners make certa<strong>in</strong> characteristic mistakes, e.g. when anativc speaker of <strong>English</strong> says someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> French likc je suis dome, an utterance parallel tothc <strong>English</strong> ‘1 am twelve’. (The correct French expression would of course bej’ai doure ansI have twelve years.)This k<strong>in</strong>d of phenomenon <strong>in</strong> learner productions is often called by the term languagetransfer. But how important is the phcnomenon, and what exactly is be<strong>in</strong>g transferred? Secondlanguage researchers have been through several ‘sw<strong>in</strong>gs of the pendulum’ on this question,as Gass puts it (1996). Behaviourist thcorists viewed language transfer as an important sourceof error and <strong>in</strong>terference <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g, because L1 ‘hab<strong>its</strong>’ were so tenacious and deeplyrootcd.The <strong>in</strong>terlanguage theorists who followcd downplayed the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the L1 <strong>in</strong> L2learn<strong>in</strong>g, however, because of their preoccupation with identify<strong>in</strong>g creative processes at work<strong>in</strong> L2 development; they po<strong>in</strong>ted out that many L2 errors could not lie traced to L1 <strong>in</strong>fluence,and were primarily concerned with discovcr<strong>in</strong>g pattcrns and dcvclopmental sequences onthis creative front.Thcorists today, as we shall see, would gcncrally accept once more that cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic<strong>in</strong>fluences play an important role <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g. Howevcr, we will still f<strong>in</strong>d widcly differ<strong>in</strong>gviews on the extent and nature of these <strong>in</strong>fluences. Some researchers have <strong>in</strong> fact claimedthat learners with different Lls progress at somewhat different rates, and even followdiffcrent acquisitional routes, at least <strong>in</strong> somc areas of the target grammar (c.g. Keller-Cohcn1979, Zobl 1982, quoted <strong>in</strong> Gass 1996, pp. 322-3).


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 21The relationship between second language use and second language learn<strong>in</strong>gIn an earlier section we considcred the dist<strong>in</strong>ction bctween language competence andperformance, which many l<strong>in</strong>guists havc found useful. Here, we look more closely at theconcept of pcrformance, and <strong>in</strong> particular, look at the possible relationship between us<strong>in</strong>g(i.e. perform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>) an L2, and learn<strong>in</strong>g (i.e. develop<strong>in</strong>g onc’s competence <strong>in</strong>) that samelanguage.We should note first of all, of course, that ‘perform<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> a language not only <strong>in</strong>volvesspeak<strong>in</strong>g it. Mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of the language data that wc hear around us is an equally essentialaspect of performance. Indeed, it is basic common ground among all theorists of languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g, of whatever description, that it is necessary to <strong>in</strong>terprct and to process <strong>in</strong>com<strong>in</strong>glanguage data <strong>in</strong> some form, for normal language development to take place.Therc is thusa consensus that language <strong>in</strong>put of some k<strong>in</strong>d is essential for normal language learn<strong>in</strong>g. In fact,dur<strong>in</strong>g the late 1970s and carly 1980s, the view was argued by Stephcn Krashen and othersthat <strong>in</strong>put (at thc right level of difficulty) was all that was necessary for L2 acquisition to takeplace (Krashen 1982, 1985). This position has been viewed by more reccnt theorists as<strong>in</strong>adequate, but a modified and ref<strong>in</strong>ed version has been developed.Krashen was unusual <strong>in</strong> not see<strong>in</strong>g any central role for language production <strong>in</strong> his thcoryof second language acquisition. Most other theoretical vicwpo<strong>in</strong>ts support <strong>in</strong> some formthe common-sense view that speak<strong>in</strong>g a language is helpful for learn<strong>in</strong>g it, though they offera wide variety of explanations as to why this should be the casc. For example, behaviouristlearn<strong>in</strong>g theory saw regular (oral) practice as helpful <strong>in</strong> form<strong>in</strong>g correct language ‘hab<strong>its</strong>’.This view has becomc less popular <strong>in</strong> reccnt decades, as part of l<strong>in</strong>guists’ general loss of<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> bchaviourist th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.However, various contcmporary theorists still lay stress on thc ‘practicc’ function oflanguage production, especially <strong>in</strong> build<strong>in</strong>g up fluency and control of an emergent L2 system.For example, <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g theorists commonly arguc that language competenceconsists of both a knowledge component (‘know<strong>in</strong>g that’) and a skill component (‘know<strong>in</strong>ghow’). While they may accept a variety of possible sources for the first component,researchers <strong>in</strong> this perspcctive agree <strong>in</strong> sec<strong>in</strong>g a vital role for L2 use/L2 performance <strong>in</strong>dcvelop<strong>in</strong>g the second skill component.An even more strongly contrast<strong>in</strong>g vicw to Krashcn’s is thc so-called comprehensible outputhypothesis, argued for by Merrill Swa<strong>in</strong> and collcagues (c.g. Swa<strong>in</strong> 1985; Swa<strong>in</strong> and Lapk<strong>in</strong>1995). Swa<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts out that much <strong>in</strong>com<strong>in</strong>g L2 <strong>in</strong>put is comprehensible, without any necdfor a full grammatical analysis. If we don’t need to pay attention to the grammar, <strong>in</strong> ordcr tounderstand the message, why should we bc compelled to learn it? On thc other hand, whenwe try to say sorncth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> our choscn second languagc, we arc forced to make grammaticalchoices and hypothescs, <strong>in</strong> ordcr to put our utterances together. Thc act of speak<strong>in</strong>g forcesus to try our ideas about how thc target grammar actually works, and of course gives us thechance of gett<strong>in</strong>g some feedback from <strong>in</strong>terlocutors who may fail to understand our efforts.So far <strong>in</strong> this section, we have secn that thcorists can hold different views on thecontribution both of language <strong>in</strong>put and languagc output to language learn<strong>in</strong>g Howcver,another way of dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g among current theories of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g from a ‘performance’perspective has to do with their view of L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction ~ when the speak<strong>in</strong>g and listcn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> which the learner is engaged arc viewcd as an <strong>in</strong>tcgral and mutually <strong>in</strong>fluential whole, e.g.<strong>in</strong> everyday conversation. Two major perspectives on <strong>in</strong>teraction are apparent, onepsychol<strong>in</strong>guistic, one sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic.From a psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic po<strong>in</strong>t of vicw, L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction is ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g because ofthe opportunities it offers to <strong>in</strong>dividual L2 learners to f<strong>in</strong>e-tune the language <strong>in</strong>put they arc


22 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESreceiv<strong>in</strong>g.This ensures that the <strong>in</strong>put is well adapted to thcir own <strong>in</strong>ternal needs (i.e. to thepresent state of development of their L2 knowledge). What this means is that learnersneed the chance to talk with native speakers <strong>in</strong> a fairly open-ended way, to ask questions, andto clarify mean<strong>in</strong>gs when they do not immctliately understand. Under these conditions, itis believed that the utterances that result will be at the right level of difficulty to promotelearn<strong>in</strong>g; <strong>in</strong> Krashen’s terms, they will provide true ‘comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put’. Conversationalepisodes <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the regular negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g have been <strong>in</strong>tensively studied by manyof the Krashen-<strong>in</strong>fluenced researchers.Interaction is also <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to l<strong>in</strong>guistic theorists, because of recent controversies overwhether the provision of negative evidence is necessary or helpful for L2 development. By‘negative evidence’ is meant some k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>put which lets the learner know that a particularform is not acceptable accord<strong>in</strong>g to target language norms. In L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction this might takethe shape of a formal correction offered by a teacher, say, or a more <strong>in</strong>formal rephras<strong>in</strong>g ofa learner’s L2 utterance, offered by a native-speak<strong>in</strong>g conversational partner.Why is there a controversy about negative evidence <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g? The problem is thatcorrection often seems <strong>in</strong>effective ~- and not only because L2 learners are lazy. It seems thatlearners often cannot benefit from correction, but cont<strong>in</strong>ue to makc the same mistakcshowever much fcedback is offered. For some current theorists, any natural language mustbe learnable from positive evidence alone, and corrective feedback is largely irrelevant. Otherscont<strong>in</strong>ue to see value <strong>in</strong> corrections and negative evidence, though it is generally acceptedthat these will be useful only when they relate to ‘hot spots’ currently be<strong>in</strong>g restructured <strong>in</strong>the learner’s emerg<strong>in</strong>g L2 system.These different (psycho)l<strong>in</strong>guistic views have one th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> common, however; they viewthe learner as operat<strong>in</strong>g and develop<strong>in</strong>g a relatively autonomous L2 system, and see <strong>in</strong>teractionas a way of feed<strong>in</strong>g that system with more or less f<strong>in</strong>e-tuned <strong>in</strong>put data, whethcr positive ornegative. Sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic vicws of <strong>in</strong>teraction are very different. Here, the language learn<strong>in</strong>gprocess is viewed as essentially social; both thc identity of the learncr, and their languagcknowledge, are collaboratively constructed and reconstructed <strong>in</strong> the course of <strong>in</strong>teraction.Some theorists strcss a broad view ofthc second language learn<strong>in</strong>g process as an apprenticeship<strong>in</strong>to a range of new discourse practices (e.g. Hall 1995); others arc more concerned withanalys<strong>in</strong>g the detail of <strong>in</strong>teraction between more expert and less expert speakers, to determ<strong>in</strong>ehow the learner is scaJolded <strong>in</strong>to us<strong>in</strong>g (and presumably learn<strong>in</strong>g) new L2 forms.Views of the language learnerWho is the second language learner, and how are they <strong>in</strong>troduced to us, <strong>in</strong> current SLLresearch? ‘Second language’ research generally deals with learners who embark on thelearn<strong>in</strong>g of an additional language, at least some years after they have started to acquire theirfirst 1anguage.This learn<strong>in</strong>g may take place formally and systematically, <strong>in</strong> a classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g;or it may take place through <strong>in</strong>formal social contact, through work, through migration, orother social forces which br<strong>in</strong>g speakers of different languages <strong>in</strong>to contact, and makecommunication a necessity.So, second language learners may be children, or thcy may be adults; they may belearn<strong>in</strong>g the target languagc formally <strong>in</strong> school or college, or ‘pick<strong>in</strong>g it up’ <strong>in</strong> thc playgroundor the workplacc.They may be learn<strong>in</strong>g a highly localized language, which will helpthem to become <strong>in</strong>siders <strong>in</strong> a local speech community; or the targct language may be alanguage of widcr communication relevant to thcir region, which givcs access to economicdevelopment and public life.


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 23Indeed, <strong>in</strong> the late twenticth century, the target language is highly likely to be <strong>English</strong>;a recent estimate suggests that while around 300 million people speak <strong>English</strong> as their firstlanguage, another 700 million or so are us<strong>in</strong>g it as a second language, or learn<strong>in</strong>g to do so(Crystal 1987, p. 358). Certa<strong>in</strong>ly it is true that much research on second language learn<strong>in</strong>g,whether with children or adults, is conccrned with the learn<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong>, or with a verysmall number of other languages, mostly European ones (French, German, Spanish). Thereare many multil<strong>in</strong>gual communities today (e.g. townships around many fast-grow<strong>in</strong>g cities)where L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volves a much wider range of languages. Howcver, these have beencomparatively little studied.The learner as language processorIt is possible to dist<strong>in</strong>guish three ma<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts of view, or sets of priorities, among SLLresearchers as far as thc learner is concerned. L<strong>in</strong>guists and psychol<strong>in</strong>guists have typicallybeen concerned primarily with analys<strong>in</strong>g and modell<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>ner mental mechanisms availableto the <strong>in</strong>dividual learner, for process<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g, and stor<strong>in</strong>g new language knowledge. Asfar as language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular is concerned, their aim is to document and expla<strong>in</strong> thedevelopmental route along which learners travel. Researchcrs for whom this is the primegoal are less concerned with the speed or rate of development, or <strong>in</strong>deed with the degreeof ultimate L2 success. Thus they tend to m<strong>in</strong>imizc or disregard social and contextualdifferences among learners; their aim is to document universal mental processes availableto all normal human be<strong>in</strong>gs.As we shall see, however, there is some controversy among rescarchcrs <strong>in</strong> thispsychol<strong>in</strong>guistic tradition on the question of age. Do child and adult L2 learners learn <strong>in</strong>essentially similar ways? Or, is thcre a critical uge which divides younger and older learners,a moment when early learn<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms atrophy and are replaced or at least supplementedby other compensatory ways of learn<strong>in</strong>g? The balance of evidence has been <strong>in</strong>terpretcd byLong (1 990b) <strong>in</strong> favour of the existence of such a cut-off po<strong>in</strong>t, and many other researchersagree with some version of a view that ‘younger better <strong>in</strong> the long run’ (S<strong>in</strong>gleton 1995,p. 3). However, explanations of why this should be are ?till provisional.Diflerences between <strong>in</strong>dividual learnersReal-life observation quickly tells us, however, that evcn if L2 learners can bc shown to befollow<strong>in</strong>g a common developmcntal routc, they differ greatly <strong>in</strong> the degrce of ultimatesuccess which they achieve. <strong>Social</strong> psychologists have argued consistently that thesedifferences <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes must be due to ~ndiv&~I dgerences between learners, andmany proposals have been made concern<strong>in</strong>g the characteristics which supposedly cause thesedifferences.In a recent two-part revicw (1 992, 1993), Gardner and MacIntyre divide what they seeas the most important learner tra<strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>to two groups, the cognitive and the afective (emotional).Here we follow their account, and summarize very briefly the factors claimed to havc themost significant <strong>in</strong>fluence on L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g success. For fuller trcatmcnt of this socialpsychological perspective on learner difference, we would refer the rcader to sources suchas Gardner (1985), Skehan (1989), and Ellis (1994, pp. 467- 560).


24 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE M Y L E SCognjtivefactorsIntelligence: Not very surpris<strong>in</strong>gly perhaps, there is clear evidence that L2 students who areabove average on formal measures of <strong>in</strong>telligence and/or general academic atta<strong>in</strong>ment tendto do well <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g, at least <strong>in</strong> formal classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gs.<strong>Language</strong> aptitude: Is there really such a th<strong>in</strong>g as a ‘gift’ for language learn<strong>in</strong>g, dist<strong>in</strong>ct fromgeneral <strong>in</strong>telligence, as folk wisdom often holds? The most famous formal test of languageaptitude was designed <strong>in</strong> the 1950s, by Carroll and Sapon (1 959, <strong>in</strong> Gardner and Maclntyre1992, p. 2 14).This ‘Modern <strong>Language</strong> AptitudcTest’ assesses a number of subskills believedto be predictive of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g success: (a) phonetic cod<strong>in</strong>g ability, (b) grammatical sensitivity,(c) memory abilities, and (d) <strong>in</strong>ductive language learn<strong>in</strong>g ability. In general, learners’ scoreson this antl other similar tests do <strong>in</strong>deed ‘correlate with. . . achievement <strong>in</strong> a secondlanguage’ (Gardner and MacIntyre 1992, p. 2 15), and <strong>in</strong> a range of contexts measures ofaptitude have been shown to be one of the strongest available predictors of success (Harleyand Hart 1997).<strong>Language</strong> Iearn<strong>in</strong>g strategies: Do more successful language learners set about the task <strong>in</strong>some dist<strong>in</strong>ctive way? Do they have at their disposal some special repertoire of ways oflearn<strong>in</strong>g, or strategies? If this were truc, could these even be taught to other, hitherto lesssuccessful learners? Much research has been done to describe antl categorize the strategiesused by learners at different levels, and to l<strong>in</strong>k strategy use to learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes; it is clearthat more proficient learners do <strong>in</strong>deed employ strategies that arc different from those usedby the less proficient (Oxford and Crookall 1989, quoted <strong>in</strong> Gardner and Maclntyre 1992,p. 2 17). Whether the strategies cause the learn<strong>in</strong>g, or the learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong>elf enables differentstrategies to be used, has not been fully clarified, however.Afecti vefactors<strong>Language</strong> attitudes: <strong>Social</strong> psychologists have long been <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the idea that the attitudcsof the learner towards the target language, <strong>its</strong> speakers, antl the learn<strong>in</strong>g context, may allplay some part <strong>in</strong> expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g success or lack of it. Research on L2 language attitudes haslargely been conducted with<strong>in</strong> the framcwork of broader research on motivation, of whichattitudcs form one part.Motivation: For Gardncr and MacIntyrc, the motivated <strong>in</strong>dividual ‘is one who wants toachieve a particular goal, devotes considerable effort to achieve this goal, and experiencessatisfaction <strong>in</strong> the activities associated with achiev<strong>in</strong>g this goal’ (1993, p. 2). So, motivationis a complex construct, def<strong>in</strong>ed by three ma<strong>in</strong> components: ‘desire to achieve a goal,effort extended <strong>in</strong> this direction, and satisfaction with the task’ (p. 2). Gardner and hisCanadian colleagues have carried out a long programme of work on motivation with <strong>English</strong>Canadian school students learn<strong>in</strong>g French as a sccond language, and have developed a rangeof formal <strong>in</strong>struments to measure motivation. Over the years consistent relationships havebeen demonstrated between language attitudes, motivation, and L2 achievement; Gardneraccepts that these relationships are complex, however, as the factors <strong>in</strong>teract, and <strong>in</strong>fluenceeach other (1 985, cited <strong>in</strong> Gardncr and Maclntyre 1993, p. 2).<strong>Language</strong> anxiety:Thc f<strong>in</strong>al learner charactcristic which Gardncr and Maclntyrc considerhas clearly been shown to have a relationship with learn<strong>in</strong>g success is language anxiety (and<strong>its</strong> obvcrse, self-confidence). For these authors, language anxiety ‘is seen as a stable pcrsonalitytrait referr<strong>in</strong>g to the propensity for an <strong>in</strong>dividual to react <strong>in</strong> a nervous manner whenspeak<strong>in</strong>g . . . <strong>in</strong> the second language’ (1 993, p. 5). It is typified by self-belittl<strong>in</strong>g, feel<strong>in</strong>gs ofapprchcnsion, and even bodily responses such as a faster heartbeat! The anxious learner is


SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING: CONCEPTS AND ISSUES 25also less will<strong>in</strong>g to speak <strong>in</strong> class, or to engage target language speakers <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>teraction.Gardner and MacIntyre cite many studies which suggest that language anxiety has anegative relationship with learn<strong>in</strong>g success, and some others which suggest the opposite, forlearner self-confidence.The learner as social be<strong>in</strong>gThe two perspectives on the learner which we have highlighted so far have concentratedfirst, on universal characteristics, and second, on <strong>in</strong>dividual characteristics. But it is alsopossible to view the L2 learner as essentially a social be<strong>in</strong>g, and such an <strong>in</strong>terest will lead toconcern with learners’ relationship with the social context, and the structur<strong>in</strong>g of thelearn<strong>in</strong>g opportunitics which it makes available. The learn<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>its</strong>elf may be viewedas essentially social, and <strong>in</strong>extricably entangled <strong>in</strong> L2 use aid L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction. Two majordifferences appear, which dist<strong>in</strong>guish this view of the learner from the last (for the socialpsychological view of the learner which we have just dipped <strong>in</strong>to is also clearly concernedwith the <strong>in</strong>dividual learners’ relationship with the ’socio-cultural milieu’ <strong>in</strong> which learn<strong>in</strong>gis tak<strong>in</strong>g place).First, <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the learner as a social be<strong>in</strong>g leads to concern with a range of sociallyconstructed dements <strong>in</strong> the learner’s identity, and their relationship with learn<strong>in</strong>g - so class,ethn<strong>in</strong>ty, and gender make their appearance as potentially significant for L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g research.Second, the relationship between the <strong>in</strong>dividual learner and the social context of learn<strong>in</strong>g isviewed as dynamic, reflexive and constantly chang<strong>in</strong>g. The ‘<strong>in</strong>dividual differences’ traditionsaw that relationship as be<strong>in</strong>g governed by a bundle of learner tra<strong>its</strong> or characteristics (suchas aptitude, anxiety, etc.), which were relatively fixed and slow to change. More sociallyoriented researchers view motivation, learner anxiety, ctc. as be<strong>in</strong>g constantly reconstructedthrough ongo<strong>in</strong>g L2 experience and L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction.L<strong>in</strong>ks with social practiceIs second language learn<strong>in</strong>g theory ‘useful’ ? Does it havc any immediate practical applications<strong>in</strong> the real world, most obviously <strong>in</strong> the L2 classroom? In our field, theorists have been andrema<strong>in</strong> divided on this po<strong>in</strong>t. Beretta and his colleagues havc argued for ‘pure’ theory-build<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> SLL, uncluttered by requirements for practical application (1993). Van Licr (1 994),Rampton (199%) and others havc argued for a socially engaged perspective, wheretheoretical development is rooted <strong>in</strong>, and responsive to, social practice, and languageeducation <strong>in</strong> particular.Yet others have argued that L2 teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular should be guidedsystematically by SLL research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g. Krashen 1985).This tension has partly been addressed by the emergence of ‘<strong>in</strong>structed languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g’ as a dist<strong>in</strong>ct sub-area of research (see recent reviews by Ellis 1994, pp. 561-663;Spada 1997). We th<strong>in</strong>k that language teachcrs, who will form an important segment of ourreadership, will themselves want to take stock of the relations between the theories wesurvey, and their own beliefs and experiences <strong>in</strong> the classroom.They will, <strong>in</strong> other words,want to make some judgement on the ‘usefulness’ oftheoris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of their ownexperience and their practice, while not necessarily chang<strong>in</strong>g it.


Chapter 2Patsy M. Lightbown and N<strong>in</strong>a SpadaFACTORS AFFECTING SECONDLANGUAGE LEARNINGL L N 0 R MA L C H I L D R EN , G I V EN a normal upbr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g, are successful <strong>in</strong> theA acquisition of their first language.This contrasts with our experience of second languagelearners, whose success varies greatly.Many of us believe that learners have certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics which lead to more or lesssuccessful language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Such beliefs are usually based on anecdotal evidence, often ourown experience or that of <strong>in</strong>dividual people we have known. For example, many teachersare conv<strong>in</strong>ced that extroverted learners who <strong>in</strong>teract without <strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>in</strong> their secondlanguage and f<strong>in</strong>d many opportunities to practise language skills will be the most successfullearners. In addition to personality characteristics, other factors generally considered to berclevant to languagc learn<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>tclligence, aptitude, motivation, and attitudes. Anotherimportant factor is the age at which learn<strong>in</strong>g beg<strong>in</strong>s.In this chapter, we will see whether anecdotal evidence is supported by research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.To what extent can we predict differences <strong>in</strong> the success of second language acquisition <strong>in</strong>two <strong>in</strong>dividuals if we have <strong>in</strong>formation about their personalities, their general and specific<strong>in</strong>tellectual abilities, their motivation, or their agc?ActivityCharacteristics of the @ad language learner'It seems that some people have a much easier time of learn<strong>in</strong>g than others. Rate ofdevelopment varies widely among first language learners. Some children can str<strong>in</strong>g togetherfive-, six-, and seven-word sentences at an age when other children are just beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g to labelitems <strong>in</strong> their immediate environment. Nevertheless, all normal children eventually mastertheir first language.In second languagc learn<strong>in</strong>g, it has been observed countless times that, <strong>in</strong> the sameclassroom sett<strong>in</strong>g, some students progress rapidly through the <strong>in</strong>itial stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g anew language while others struggle along mak<strong>in</strong>g very slow progress. Some learners neverachieve native-hke command of a second language. Are there personal characteristics thatmake one learner more successful than another, and if so, what are they?The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a list of some of the characteristics commonly thought to contributeto successful language learn<strong>in</strong>g. In your experience ~ as a second language learner and as a


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 29teacher ~ which characteristics seem to you most likely to be associated with succcss <strong>in</strong>second language acquisition <strong>in</strong> the classroom? Which ones would you be less <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed toexpect <strong>in</strong> a successful learner?In each case rate the characteristic as follows:1 =Very important2 = Quite important3 = Important4 = Not very important5 = Not at all importantA good language learner:a is a will<strong>in</strong>g and accurate guesserb tries to get a message across cvcn ifspecific language knowledge is lack<strong>in</strong>gc is will<strong>in</strong>g to make mistakesd constantly looks for patterns <strong>in</strong> the languagee practises as often as possiblef analyses his or her own speech and thespeech of othersg attends to whether his or her performancemeets the standards he or she has learnedh enjoys grammar exercisesi bcg<strong>in</strong>s learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> childhoodj has an above-average IQk has good academic skills1 has a good self-image and lots of confidence1 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 51 2 3 4 5All of the characteristics listed above can be classified <strong>in</strong>to five ma<strong>in</strong> categories: motivation,aptitude, personality, <strong>in</strong>tclligcnce, and learner preferences. However, many of the characteristicscannot be assigned exclusively to one category. For example, the characteristic ‘iswill<strong>in</strong>g to make mistakes’ can be considcrcd a personality and/or a motivational factor if thelearner is will<strong>in</strong>g to make mistakes <strong>in</strong> order to get the message across.


30 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAResearch on learner characteristicsPerhaps the best way to beg<strong>in</strong> our discussion is to describe how research on the <strong>in</strong>fluence oflearner characteristics on second language learn<strong>in</strong>g has been carried out. When researchersare <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out whether an <strong>in</strong>dividual factor such as motivation affects secondlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g, they usually select a group of learners and give them a questionnaire tomeasure the type and degree of their motivation.Thc learners arc then given a test to measuretheir second language proficiency. The test and the questionnaire are both scored and theresearcher performs a correlation on the two measures, to see whether learners with highscores on the proficiency test are also more likely to have high scores on the motivationquestionnaire. If this is the case, the researcher concludes that high levels of motivation arecorrelated with success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g. A similar procedure can be used to assess therelationship between <strong>in</strong>telligence and second language acquisition through the use of IQ tests.Although this procedure seems straightforward, there are several difficulties with it.The first problem is that it is not possible to directly observe and measure qualities such asmotivation, extroversion, or even <strong>in</strong>telligence. These are just labels for an entire range ofbchaviours and characteristics. Furthermore, because characteristics such as these arc not<strong>in</strong>dependent, it will come as no surprise that different researchers have often used the samelabels to describe different sets of behavioural tra<strong>its</strong>.For example, <strong>in</strong> motivation questionnaires, learners are often asked whether theywill<strong>in</strong>gly seek out opportunities to use their second language with native speakers and if so,how often they do this. The assumption beh<strong>in</strong>d such a qucstion is that learners who reportthat they often seek out opportunities to <strong>in</strong>tcract with speakcrs of the second languageare highly motivated to learn. Although this assumption seems reasonable, it is problematicbecause ifa learner responds by say<strong>in</strong>g‘yes’ to this qucstion, we may assume that the learnerhas more opportunities for languagc practice <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal contexts. Because it is usuallyimpossible to scparate these two factors (i.e. will<strong>in</strong>gness to <strong>in</strong>teract and opportunities to<strong>in</strong>teract), somc researchers have been criticized for conclud<strong>in</strong>g that it is the motivation ratherthan the opportunity which makes the greater contribution to success.Another factor which makes it difficult to reach conclusions about relationships between<strong>in</strong>dividual learner characteristics and sccond language learn<strong>in</strong>g is how language proficiencyis def<strong>in</strong>ed and mcasurcd.To illustrate this po<strong>in</strong>t let us refer once aga<strong>in</strong> to ‘motivation’. In thesecond language learn<strong>in</strong>g literature, somc studies report that learners with a higher level ofmotivation arc more successful language learners than those with lower motivation, whileother studies report that highly motivated learners do not perform any better on a proficiencytest than learners with much less motivation to learn the second language. One explanationwhich has been offered for these conflict<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs is that the language proficiency tests used<strong>in</strong> different studies do not mcasure the same k<strong>in</strong>d of knowledge.That is, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g sctt<strong>in</strong>gs, highly motivated lcarners may he more successful when the proficiencytests measure oral communication skills. In othcr studies, however, highly motivated learnersmay not lie more successful because the tests arc primarily measures of mctal<strong>in</strong>guisticknowledge. Rcsults such as these imply that motivation to learn a second language may bemore related to particular aspects of language proficiency than to others.F<strong>in</strong>ally, there is the problem of <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the correlation of two factors as be<strong>in</strong>gdue to a causal relationship between them. That is, the fact that two th<strong>in</strong>gs tend to occurtogether does not necessarily mean that one caused the other. While it may be that that onefactor <strong>in</strong>fluences the other, it may also be the case that both are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by someth<strong>in</strong>g elseentirely. Research on motivation is perhaps the best context <strong>in</strong> which to illustrate this.Learners who are successful may <strong>in</strong>deed be highly motivated. But can we conclude that they


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 31became successful because of their motivation? It is also plausible that early successheightened their motivation or that both success and motivation are due to their specialaptitude for language learn<strong>in</strong>g or the favourable context <strong>in</strong> which they are learn<strong>in</strong>g.IntelligenceThe term ‘<strong>in</strong>telligence’ has traditionally been used to refer to performance on certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dsof tests.These tests are often associated with success <strong>in</strong> school, and a l<strong>in</strong>k between <strong>in</strong>telligenceand second language learn<strong>in</strong>g has sometimes been reported. Over the years, many studiesus<strong>in</strong>g a variety of <strong>in</strong>telligence ( ‘IQ’) tests and different methods of assess<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>ghave found that IQ scores were a good means of predict<strong>in</strong>g how successful a learner wouldbe. Some recent studies have shown that these measures of <strong>in</strong>telligence may be more stronglyrelated to certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of second language abilities than to others. For example, <strong>in</strong> a studywith French immersion students <strong>in</strong> Canada, it was found that, while <strong>in</strong>telligence was relatedto the development of French second language read<strong>in</strong>g, grammar, and vocabulary, it wasunrelated to oral productive skills (Genesee 1976). Similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have been reported <strong>in</strong>other studies. What this suggests is that, while <strong>in</strong>telligence, especially as measured by verbalIQ tests, may be a strong factor when it comes to learn<strong>in</strong>g which <strong>in</strong>volves language analysisand rule learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>telligence may play a less important role <strong>in</strong> classrooms where the<strong>in</strong>struction focuses more on communication and <strong>in</strong>teraction.It is important to keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that ‘<strong>in</strong>telligence’ is complex and that <strong>in</strong>dividuals havemany k<strong>in</strong>ds of abilities and strengths, not all of which are measured by traditional IQ tests.In our experience, many students whose academic performance has been weak haveexperienced considerable success <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g.AptitudeThere is evidence <strong>in</strong> the research literature that some <strong>in</strong>dividuals have an exceptional‘aptitude’ for language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Lorra<strong>in</strong>e Obler (1 989) reports that a man, whom she callsCJ, has such a spccialized ability. CJ is a native spcaker of <strong>English</strong> who grew up <strong>in</strong> an <strong>English</strong>home. His first true cxperience with a second language came at the age of 15 when he beganlearn<strong>in</strong>g French <strong>in</strong> school. CJ also studied German, Spanish, and Lat<strong>in</strong> while <strong>in</strong> high school.At age 20, he made a brief visit to Germany. CJ reported that just hear<strong>in</strong>g German spokenfor a short time was enough for him to ‘recover’ the German he had learned <strong>in</strong> school. Later,CJ worked <strong>in</strong> Morocco where he reported learn<strong>in</strong>g Moroccan Arabic through both formal<strong>in</strong>struction and <strong>in</strong>formal immersion. He also spent some time <strong>in</strong> Spa<strong>in</strong> and Italy, where heapparently ‘picked up’ both Spanish and Italian <strong>in</strong> a ‘matter of weeks’. A remarkable talent<strong>in</strong>deed!Learn<strong>in</strong>g quickly is the dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g feature of aptitude.The ‘aptitude’ factor has been<strong>in</strong>vestigated most <strong>in</strong>tensively by researchers <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g tests which can be usedto predict whether <strong>in</strong>dividuals will be efficient learners of a foreign language <strong>in</strong> a classroomsctt<strong>in</strong>g.The most widely used aptitude tests are the Modern <strong>Language</strong> AptitudeTest (MLAT)and the Pimslcur <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Battery (PLAR). Both tests are based on the view thataptitude is composed of different types of abilities:(1) the ability to identify and memorize new sounds;(2) the ability to understand the function of particular words <strong>in</strong> sentences;(3) the ability to figure out grammatical rules from language samplcs; and(4) memory for new words.


32 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAWhile earlier research revealed a substantial relationship between performance on the MLATor PLAB and performance <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g, these studies were conducted at a timewhen second language teach<strong>in</strong>g was based on grammar translation or audiol<strong>in</strong>gual methods.With the adoption of a more communicative approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g, many teachers andresearchers came to see aptitudc as irrelevant to the process of language acquisition.Unfortunately, this means that relatively little research has actually explored whether hav<strong>in</strong>ga skill such as the ‘ability to identify and memoriLe new sounds’ is advantageous whenclassroom <strong>in</strong>struction is mean<strong>in</strong>g-oriented rather than focuscd on drills or mctal<strong>in</strong>guisticexplanations.Successful language learners may not be strong <strong>in</strong> all of the components of aptitude.Some <strong>in</strong>dividuals may have strong memories but only average abilities <strong>in</strong> the othercomponents of aptitude. Ideally, one could determ<strong>in</strong>e learners’ profiles of strengths andweaknesses and use this <strong>in</strong>formation to place students <strong>in</strong> appropriate teach<strong>in</strong>g programs. Anexample of how this can be done is described by Majorie Wesche (1981). In a Canadianlanguage program for adult lcarncrs of French, students were placed <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>structionalprogram which was compatible with their aptitude profile and <strong>in</strong>formation about thcirlearn<strong>in</strong>g experiences. Students who were high on analytic ability, but average on memory,were assigned to teach<strong>in</strong>g that focuscd on grammatical structures, while learners strong<strong>in</strong> memory but average on analytic skills were placed <strong>in</strong> a class where the teach<strong>in</strong>g wasorganized around the functional use of the second language <strong>in</strong> specific situations. Weschereported a high level of student and teacher satisfaction when students were matched withCompatible teach<strong>in</strong>g environments. In addition, some evidence <strong>in</strong>dicated that matchedstudents were able to atta<strong>in</strong> significantly higher levels of achievement than those who wereunmatched.While few second language teach<strong>in</strong>g contexts are able to offcr such choices to theirstudents, teachers may f<strong>in</strong>d that know<strong>in</strong>g the aptitude profile of their students will help thcm<strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g appropriate classroom activities for particular groups of students. Or, if they donot have such <strong>in</strong>formation, they may wish to ensure that their teach<strong>in</strong>g activities aresufficiently varied to accommodate learners with different aptitude profiles.PersonalityA number of personality characteristics have been proposed as likely to affect second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g, but it has not been easy to demonstrate thcir effects <strong>in</strong> empirical studies. As withothcr research <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the effects of <strong>in</strong>dividual characteristics on second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g, different studies measur<strong>in</strong>g a similar personality trait produce different results. Forexample, it is often argued that an extroverted person is well suited to language learn<strong>in</strong>g.However, research docs not always support this conclusion. Although some studies havefound that success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g is correlated with learners’ scores on characteristicsoften associated with extrovcrsion such as assertiveness and adventurousness, others havefound that many successful language learners do not get high scores on measures of extroversion.Another aspect of personality which has been studied is <strong>in</strong>hibition. It has been suggestedthat <strong>in</strong>hibition discourages risk-tak<strong>in</strong>g which is necessary for progress <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g.This is often considered to be a particular problem for adolescents, who are more selfconsciousthan younger learners. In a scrics of studies, Alexander Guiora and his colleaguesfound support for the claim that <strong>in</strong>hibition is a negative force, at lcast for second languagepronunciation performance. One study <strong>in</strong>volved an analysis of the effects of small doses ofalcohol on pronunciation (Guiora et al. 1972).They found that subjects who received small


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 33doses of alcohol did better on pronunciation tests than those who did not dr<strong>in</strong>k any alcohol.While results such as these are <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, as well as amus<strong>in</strong>g, they are not completelyconv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g, s<strong>in</strong>ce the experiments are far removed from the reality ofthe classroom situation.Furthermore, they may have more to do with performance than with learn<strong>in</strong>g. We may alsonote, <strong>in</strong> pass<strong>in</strong>g, that when larger doses of alcohol werc adm<strong>in</strong>istered, pronunciation rapidlydeteriorated!Several other personality characteristics such as self-esteem, empathy, dom<strong>in</strong>ance,talkativeness, and responsiveness have also been studied. However, <strong>in</strong> general, the availableresearch does not show a clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed relationship between personality and secondlanguage acquisition. And, as <strong>in</strong>dicated earlier, the major difficulty <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g personalitycharacteristics is that of identification and measurement. Another explanation which hasbeen offered for the mixed f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of personality studies is that personality variables maybe a major factor only <strong>in</strong> the acquisition of conversational skills, not <strong>in</strong> the acquisition ofliteracy skills.The confused picture of the research on personality factors may be due <strong>in</strong> partto the fact that comparisons are made between studies that measure communicative abilityand studies that measure grammatical accuracy or metal<strong>in</strong>guistic knowledge. Personalityvariables seem to be consistently related to the former, but not to the latter.Despite the contradictory results and the problems <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out research <strong>in</strong>the area of personality characteristics, many researchers believe that personality will beshown to have an important <strong>in</strong>fluence on success <strong>in</strong> language 1earn<strong>in</strong>g.This relationship is acomplex one, however, <strong>in</strong> that it is probably not personality alone, but the way <strong>in</strong> which itcomb<strong>in</strong>es with other factors, that contributes to second language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Motivation and attitudesThere has been a great deal of research on the role of attitudes and motivation <strong>in</strong> secondlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. The overall f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs show that positive attitudes and motivation are relatedto success <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g (Gardner 1985). Unfortunately, the research cannot<strong>in</strong>dicate precisely how motivation is related to learn<strong>in</strong>g. As <strong>in</strong>dicated above, we do not knowwhether it is the motivation that produces: successful learn<strong>in</strong>g or successful learn<strong>in</strong>g thatenhances motivation or whether both are affected by other factors. As noted by Peter Skehan(1989), the question is, are learners more highly motivated because they are successful, orare they successful because they are highly motivated?Motivation <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g is a complex phenomenon which can be def<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> terms of two factors: learners’ communicative needs and their attitudes towards the secondlanguage community. If learners need to speak the second language <strong>in</strong> a wide range of socialsituations or to fulfil professional ambitions, they will perceive the communicative value ofthe second language and will therefore be motivated to acquirc proficiency <strong>in</strong> it. Likewise, iflearners have favourable attitudes towards the speakers of the language, they will desire morecontact with thcm. Robert Gardner andwallace Lambert (1 972) co<strong>in</strong>ed the terms <strong>in</strong>tep~tivemotivat7on to refer to language learn<strong>in</strong>g for personal growth and cultural enrichment, and<strong>in</strong>strumental motivation for language learn<strong>in</strong>g for more immediate or practical goals. Researchhas shown that these types of motivation are related to success <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g.On the other hand, we should keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s identity is closely l<strong>in</strong>kedwith the way he or she speaks. It follows that when speak<strong>in</strong>g a new language one is adopt<strong>in</strong>gSome ofthe identity markers of another cultural group. Depend<strong>in</strong>g on the learner’s attitudes,learn<strong>in</strong>g a second language can be a source of enrichment or a source of resentment. If thespeaker’s only reason for learn<strong>in</strong>g the second language is external pressure, <strong>in</strong>ternalmotivation may be m<strong>in</strong>imal and general attitudes towards learn<strong>in</strong>g may be negative.


34 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAOne factor which often affects motivation is the social dynamic or power relationshipbetween the languages. That is, members of a m<strong>in</strong>ority group learn<strong>in</strong>g the language of amajority group may have different attitudes and motivation from those of majority groupmembers learn<strong>in</strong>g a m<strong>in</strong>ority language. Even though it is impossible to predict the exacteffect of such societal factors on second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, the fact that languages exist <strong>in</strong>social contexts cannot be overlooked when we seek to understand the variables which affectsuccess <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g. Children as well as adults are sensitive to social dynamics and powerrelationships.Motivation <strong>in</strong> the claxsroom sett<strong>in</strong>gIn a teacher’s m<strong>in</strong>d, motivated students arc usually those who participate actively <strong>in</strong>class, express <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the subject-matter, and study a great deal.Tcachers can easily recogni7echaracteristics such as these. They also have more opportunity to <strong>in</strong>fluence thesecharacteristics than students’ reasons for study<strong>in</strong>g the second language or their attitudestoward the language and <strong>its</strong> speakers. If we can make our classrooms places where studentsenjoy com<strong>in</strong>g because the content is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g and relevant to their age and level of ability,where the learn<strong>in</strong>g goals are challeng<strong>in</strong>g yet manageable and clear, and where the atmosphereis supportive and non-threaten<strong>in</strong>g, we can make a positive contribution to students’motivation to learn.Although little research has been done to <strong>in</strong>vestigate how pedagogy <strong>in</strong>teracts withmotivation <strong>in</strong> second language classrooms, considerable work has been done with<strong>in</strong> the fieldof educational psychology. In a review of some of this work, Graham Crookes and RichardSchmidt (1991) po<strong>in</strong>t to several areas where educational research has reported <strong>in</strong>creasedlevels of motivation for students <strong>in</strong> relation to pedagogical practices. Included among theseare:Motivat<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong>to the lesson At the open<strong>in</strong>g stages of lessons (and with<strong>in</strong> transitions), ithas been observed that remarks teachers make about forthcom<strong>in</strong>g activities can lead to higherlevels of <strong>in</strong>terest on the part of thc students.Vary<strong>in</strong>g the activities, tasks, and materials Students are reassured by the existence of classroomrout<strong>in</strong>es which they can depend on. However, lessons which always consist of the samerout<strong>in</strong>es, patterns, and formats have been shown to lead to a decrease <strong>in</strong> attention and an<strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> boredom. Vary<strong>in</strong>g the activities, tasks, and materials can help to avoid this and<strong>in</strong>crease students’ <strong>in</strong>terest levels.Us<strong>in</strong>g co-operative rather than competitive goals Co-operative learn<strong>in</strong>g activities are those <strong>in</strong>which students must work together <strong>in</strong> order to complete a task or solve a problem. Thesetechniques have been found to <strong>in</strong>crease the self-confidence of students, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g weakerones, because every participant <strong>in</strong> a co-operative task has an important role to play. Know<strong>in</strong>gthat their team-mates arc count<strong>in</strong>g on them can <strong>in</strong>crcasc students’ motivation.Clearly, cultural and age diffcrenccs will determ<strong>in</strong>e the most appropriate way for teachersto motivate students. In some classrooms, students may thrive on competitive <strong>in</strong>teraction,while <strong>in</strong> others, co-operative activities will be more successful.


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 35Learner preferencesLearners have clear preferences for how they go about learn<strong>in</strong>g new material. The term‘learn<strong>in</strong>g style’ has been used to describe an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s natural, habitual, and preferred wayof absorb<strong>in</strong>g, process<strong>in</strong>g, and reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g new <strong>in</strong>formation and skills (Reid 1995). We have allheard people say that they cannot learn someth<strong>in</strong>g until they have seen it. Such learnerswould fall <strong>in</strong>to the group called ‘visual’ learners. Other people, who may be called ‘aural’learners, seem to need only to hear someth<strong>in</strong>g once or twice before they know it. For others,who are referred to as ‘k<strong>in</strong>aesthetic’ learners, there is a need to add a physical action to thelearn<strong>in</strong>g process. In contrast to these perceptually based learn<strong>in</strong>g styles, considerable researchhas focused on a cognitive learn<strong>in</strong>g style dist<strong>in</strong>ction betweenfield <strong>in</strong>dependent andfielddependent learners. This refers to whether an <strong>in</strong>dividual tends to separate details from thegeneral background or to see th<strong>in</strong>gs more holistically. Another category of learn<strong>in</strong>g styles isbased on the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s temperament or personality.While recent years have seen the development of many learn<strong>in</strong>g style assessment<strong>in</strong>struments, very little research has exam<strong>in</strong>ed the <strong>in</strong>teraction between different learn<strong>in</strong>gstylcs and success <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition. At present, the only learn<strong>in</strong>g style that hasbeen extensively <strong>in</strong>vestigated is the field <strong>in</strong>dependencc/dependence dist<strong>in</strong>ction.The resultsfrom this research have shown that while field <strong>in</strong>dependence is related to some degree toperformance on certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of tasks, it is not a good predictor of performance on others.Although there is a need for considerably more research on learn<strong>in</strong>g styles, whenlearners express a preference for see<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g written or for memoriz<strong>in</strong>g materialwhich we feel should be learned <strong>in</strong> a less formal way, we should not assume that their waysof work<strong>in</strong>g are wrong. Instead, we should encourage them to use all means available to themas they work to learn another language. At a m<strong>in</strong>imum, research on learn<strong>in</strong>g styles shouldmake us sceptical of claims that a particular teach<strong>in</strong>g method or textbook will suit the needsof all learners.Learner beliefsSecond language learners are not always conscious of their <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>g styles,but virtually all learners, particularly older learners, have strong beliefs and op<strong>in</strong>ions abouthow their <strong>in</strong>struction should be delivered .These beliefs are usually based on previous learn<strong>in</strong>gexperiences and the assumption (right or wrong) that a particular type of <strong>in</strong>struction is thebest way for them to 1earn.This is another area where little work has been done. However,the available research <strong>in</strong>dicates that learner beliefs can be strong mediat<strong>in</strong>g factors <strong>in</strong> theirexperience <strong>in</strong> the classroom. For example, <strong>in</strong> a survey of <strong>in</strong>ternational students learn<strong>in</strong>gESL <strong>in</strong> a highly communicative program at an <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g university, Carlos Yorio(1 986) found high levels of dissatisfaction among the students. The type of communicative<strong>in</strong>struction they received focused exclusively on mean<strong>in</strong>g and spontaneous communication<strong>in</strong> group-work <strong>in</strong>teraction. In their responses to a questionnaire, the majority of studentsexpressed concerns about several aspects of their <strong>in</strong>struction, most notably, the absence ofattention to language form, corrective feedback, or teacher-centred <strong>in</strong>struction. Althoughthis study did not directly exam<strong>in</strong>e learners’ progress <strong>in</strong> relation to their op<strong>in</strong>ions about the<strong>in</strong>struction they received, several of them were conv<strong>in</strong>ced that their progress was negativelyaffected by an <strong>in</strong>structional approach which was not consistent with their beliefs about thebest ways for them to learn.Learners’ preferences for learn<strong>in</strong>g, whether due to their learn<strong>in</strong>g style or to their beliefsabout how languages are learned, will <strong>in</strong>fluence the k<strong>in</strong>ds of strategies they choose <strong>in</strong> order


36 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAto learn new material. Teachers can use this <strong>in</strong>formation to help learners expand theirrepertoire of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies and thus develop greater flexibility <strong>in</strong> their way ofapproach<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Age of acquisitionWe now turn to a learner characteristic of a different type: age.This characteristic is easierto def<strong>in</strong>e and measure than personality, aptitude, or motivation. Nevertheless, the relationshipbetween a learner’s age and his or her potential for success <strong>in</strong> second languageacquisition is the subject of much lively debate.It has been widely observed that children from immigrant families eventually speak thelanguage of their new community with native like fluency, but their parents rarely achievesuch high levels of mastery of the spoken language. To be sure, there are cases where adultsecond language learners have dist<strong>in</strong>guished themselvcs by their exceptional performance.For example, one often sees reference to Joseph Conrad, a native speaker of Polish whobecame a major writer <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong> language. Many adult second language learners becomecapable of communicat<strong>in</strong>g very successfully <strong>in</strong> the language but, for most, differences ofaccent, word choice, or grammatical features dist<strong>in</strong>guish them from native speakers andfrom second language speakers who began learn<strong>in</strong>g the language while they were very young.One explanation for this difference is that, as <strong>in</strong> first language acquisition, there is a criticalperiod for second language acquisition.The Critical Period Hypothesis suggests that there isa time <strong>in</strong> human development when the bra<strong>in</strong> is predisposed for success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Developmental changes <strong>in</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong>, it is argued, affect the nature of language acquisition.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this view, language learn<strong>in</strong>g which occurs after the end of the critical period maynot be based on the <strong>in</strong>nate biological structures believed to contribute to first languageacquisition or second language acquisition <strong>in</strong> early childhood. Rather, older learners dependon more general learn<strong>in</strong>g abilities ~ the samc one7 they might use to lcarn other k<strong>in</strong>ds of skillsor <strong>in</strong>formation. It is argued that these general learn<strong>in</strong>g abilities are not as successful forlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g as the more specific, <strong>in</strong>nate capacities which are available to the young child.It is most often claimed that the critical period ends somewhere around puberty, but someresearchers suggest it could be even earlier.Of course, it is difficult to compare children and adults as second language learners. Inaddition to the possible biological differences suggested by the Critical Period Hypothesis,the conditions for language learn<strong>in</strong>g are often very different. Younger learners <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formallanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g environments usually have more time to devote to learn<strong>in</strong>g 1anguage.Theyoften have more opportunities to hear and use the language <strong>in</strong> environments where they donot experience strong pressure to speak fluently and accurately from the very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g.Furthermore, their early imperfect efforts arc often praised or, at least, accepted. On theother hand, older learners are often <strong>in</strong> situations which demand much more complexlanguage and the expression of much more complicated ideas. Adults are often embarrassedby their lack of mastery of the language and they may develop a sense of <strong>in</strong>adequacy afterexperiences of frustration <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to say exactly what they mean.The Critical Period Hypothesis has been challenged <strong>in</strong> recent years from several differentpo<strong>in</strong>ts of view. Some studies of the second language development of older and youngerlearners who are learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> similar circumstances have shown that, at least <strong>in</strong> the early stagesof second language devclopment, older learners are more efficient than younger learners.In educational research, it has been reported that learners who began learn<strong>in</strong>g a secondlanguage at the primary school level did not fare better <strong>in</strong> the long run than those who began<strong>in</strong> early adolescence. Furthermore, there are countless anecdotes about older learners


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 37(adolescents and adults) who have reached high levels of proficiency <strong>in</strong> a second language.Does this mean that there is no critical period for second language acquisition?Critical Period Hypotheur: More than lu~taccent?Most studies of the relationship between age of acquisition and second language developmenthave focused on learners’ phonological (pronunciation) achievement. In general, thesestudies have concluded that older learners almost <strong>in</strong>evitably have a noticeable ‘foreign accent’.But what of other l<strong>in</strong>guistic features? Is syntax (word order, overall sentence structure) asdependent on age of acquisition as phonological development?What about morphology (forexample, grammatical morphemes which mark such th<strong>in</strong>gs as verb tense or the number andgender of nouns)? One study that attempted to answer these questions was done by MarkPatkowski (1980).Mastery of the spoken languageMark Patkowski studied the effect of age on the acquisition of features of a second languageother than accent. He hypothesized that, even if accent were ignored, only those who hadbegun learn<strong>in</strong>g their second language before the age of 15 could ever achieve full, nativelikemastery of that language. Patkowski exam<strong>in</strong>ed the spoken <strong>English</strong> of 67 highly educatedimmigrants to the United States. They had started to learn <strong>English</strong> at various ages, but allhad lived <strong>in</strong> the United States for more than five years.The spoken <strong>English</strong> of 15 native-bornAmerican <strong>English</strong> speakers from a similarly high level of education served as a sort of basel<strong>in</strong>eof what the second language learners might be try<strong>in</strong>g to atta<strong>in</strong> as the target language. Inclusionof the native speakers also provided evidence concern<strong>in</strong>g the validity of the researchprocedures.A lengthy <strong>in</strong>terview with each of the subjects <strong>in</strong> the study was tape recorded. BecausePatkowski wanted to remove the possibility that the results would be affected by accent,he did not ask the raters to judge the tape-recorded <strong>in</strong>terviews themselves. Instead, hetranscribed five-m<strong>in</strong>ute samples from the <strong>in</strong>terviews. These samples (from which anyidentify<strong>in</strong>g or reveal<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation about immigration history had been removed) wercrated by tra<strong>in</strong>ed native-speaker judges. The judges were asked to place each speaker on arat<strong>in</strong>g scale from 0, represent<strong>in</strong>g no knowledge of the language, to 5, represent<strong>in</strong>g a level of<strong>English</strong> expected from an educated native speaker.The ma<strong>in</strong> question <strong>in</strong> Patkowski’s rcsearch was: ‘Will there be a difference betweenlearners who began to learn <strong>English</strong> before puberty and those who began learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>later?’ However, <strong>in</strong> thc light of some of the issues discussed above, he also compared learnerson the basis of other characteristics and experiences which some people have suggestedmight be as good as age <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g or expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a learner’s eventual success <strong>in</strong> master<strong>in</strong>ga second language. For example, he looked at the relationship between eventual masteryand the total amount of time a speaker had been <strong>in</strong> the United States as well as the amountof formal ESL <strong>in</strong>struction each speaker had had.The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were quite dramatic.Thirty-two out of 33 subjects who had begun learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>English</strong> before the age of 15 scored at the 4+ or the 5 level. The homogeneity of the prepubertylearners seemed to suggest that, for this group, success <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a second languagewas almost <strong>in</strong>evitable (see Figure 2.1). On the other hand, there was much more variety <strong>in</strong>the levels achieved by thc post-puberty group. The majority of the post-puberty learnerscentred around the 3+ level, but there was a wide distribution of levels achieved.This varietymade the performance of this group look more like the sort of performance range one wouldexpect if one werc measur<strong>in</strong>g success <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g almost any k<strong>in</strong>d of skill or knowledge.


38 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADA2o I2+ 3 3+ 4 4+ 5Pre-puberty learnersPost-puberty learnersFigure 2.1 Bar charts show<strong>in</strong>g the language levels of prc- and post-puberty lcarncrs of <strong>English</strong>Source: Patkowski I980Patkowski’s first question, ‘Will there be a difference between learners who began tolearn <strong>English</strong> before pubcrty and those who began learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> later?’, was answercd witha very resound<strong>in</strong>g ‘yes’. When hc exam<strong>in</strong>ed the other factors which might be thought toaffect success <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, the picture was much less clear. There was,naturally, some relationship between these other factors and learn<strong>in</strong>g success. However, itoften turned out that age was so closely related to the other factors that it was not reallypossible to separate them completely. For example, length of residence <strong>in</strong> the United Statessometimes seemed to be a fairly good predictor. However, while it was truc that a personwho had lived <strong>in</strong> the country for 15 years might speak better than one who had been therefor only 10 years, it was often the case that the onc with longer residence had also arrivedat an earlier age. However, a person who had arrived <strong>in</strong> the United States at the age of 18and had lived there for 20 years did not score significantly better than someone who hadarrived at the age of 18 but had only lived there for 10 years. Similarly, amount of <strong>in</strong>struction,when separated from age, did not predict success to the extent that age of immigration did.Thus, Patkowski found that age of acquisition i.; a very important factor <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g lim<strong>its</strong>on the development of native-like mastery of a second language and that this limitation doesnot apply only to acccnt.These results gave added support to the Critical Period Hypothesisfor second language acquisition.Experience and research have shown that native-like mastcry of thc spoken language isdifficult to atta<strong>in</strong> by older learners. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, even the ability to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 39grammatical and ungrammatical sentences <strong>in</strong> a second language appears to be affected by theage factor, as we will see <strong>in</strong> the next study by Johnson and Newport.Intuitions of grammaticalityJacquel<strong>in</strong>e Johnson and Elissa Newport conducted a study of46 Ch<strong>in</strong>ese and Korean speakerswho had begun to learn <strong>English</strong> at different ages. All subjects were students or faculty at anAmerican university and all had been <strong>in</strong> the United States for at lcast three years.The studyalso <strong>in</strong>cluded 23 native speakers of <strong>English</strong> (Johnson and Newport 1989).The participants <strong>in</strong> the study were given a judgement of grammaticality task whichtested 12 rules of <strong>English</strong> morphology and syntax. They heard sentences on a tape andhad to <strong>in</strong>dicatc whether or not each sentence was correct. Half of the sentences weregrammatical, half were not.When they scored the tests, Johnson and Newport found that age of arrival <strong>in</strong> the UnitcdStates was a significant predictor of succcss on the test. When they grouped the learners <strong>in</strong>the same way as Patkowski, compar<strong>in</strong>g those who began their <strong>in</strong>tensive exposure to <strong>English</strong>between the ages of 3 and 15 with those who arrived <strong>in</strong> the United States between the agesof 17 and 39, once aga<strong>in</strong> they found that there was a strong relationship betwccn an earlystart to language learn<strong>in</strong>g and bcttcr performance <strong>in</strong> the second language. Johnson andNewport noted that for those who began before the age of 15, and especially before the ageof 10, there were few <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> second language ability.Those who began laterdid not have native-like language abilities and were more likely to differ greatly from oneanothcr <strong>in</strong> ultimate atta<strong>in</strong>ment.This study, then, furthcr supports the hypothesis that there is a critical period foratta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g full native-like mastery of a second language. Nevertheless, there is some researchwhich suggests that older learners may have an advantage, at least <strong>in</strong> the early stages of secondlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g.lryounger really better?In 1978, Cather<strong>in</strong>e Snow and Marian Hoefnagel-Hohle published an article based on aresearch project they had carried out <strong>in</strong> Holland.They had studied the progress of a groupof <strong>English</strong> speakers who were learn<strong>in</strong>g Dutch as a second language. What made their researchespecially valuable was that the learners they were follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cluded children as young asthrec years old as well as older chldren, adolescents, and adults. Furthermore, a large numberof tasks was used, to measure different types of language use and language knowledge.Pronunciation was tested by hav<strong>in</strong>g learners pronounce 80 Dutch words twice: the firsttime immediately after hear<strong>in</strong>g a native speaker say the word; the second time, a few m<strong>in</strong>uteslater, they were asked to say the word represented <strong>in</strong> a picture, without a model to imitate.Tape record<strong>in</strong>gs of the learners were rated by a native speaker of Dutch on a six-po<strong>in</strong>t scale.In an auditory discrim<strong>in</strong>ation test, learners saw pictures of four objects. In each groupof four there were two whose names formed a m<strong>in</strong>imal pair, that is, alike except for one sound(an example <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> would be ‘ship’ and ‘sheep’). Learners heard one of the words andwere asked to <strong>in</strong>dicate which picture was named by the word they heard.Morphology was tested us<strong>in</strong>g a procedure like the ‘wug test’, which required learnersto complete sentences by add<strong>in</strong>g the correct grammatical markcrs to words whichwere supplied by the researchers. Aga<strong>in</strong>, to take an cxample from <strong>English</strong>, learners wereasked to complete sentences such as ‘Here is one boy. Now there are two of them.There are,two


40 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAThe sentence repetition task required learners to repeat 37 sentences of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g lengthand grammatical complexity.For sentence translation, learners were given 60 sentences to translate from <strong>English</strong> toDutch. A po<strong>in</strong>t was given for each grammatical structure which was rendered <strong>in</strong>to the correctDutch equivalent.In the sentenceludgement task, learners were to judge which of two sentences was better.The same content was expressed <strong>in</strong> both sentences, but one sentence was grammaticallycorrect while the other conta<strong>in</strong>ed errors.In the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Est, learners saw four pictures and heard one isolatedword.Their task was to <strong>in</strong>dicate which picture matched the word spoken by the tester.For the story comprehension task, learners heard a story <strong>in</strong> Dutch and were then asked toretell the story <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> or Dutch (accord<strong>in</strong>g to their preference).F<strong>in</strong>ally, the storytell<strong>in</strong>g task required learners to tell a story <strong>in</strong> Dutch, us<strong>in</strong>g a set of picturesthey were given. Rate of delivery of speech mattered more than the expression of contentor formal accuracy.The learners were divided <strong>in</strong>to several age groups, but for our discussion we will dividethem <strong>in</strong>to just three groups: children (aged 3 to lo), adolescents (1 2 to 15 years), and adults(1 8 to 60 ycars).The children and adolescents all attended Dutch schools. Some of the adultsworked <strong>in</strong> Dutch work environments, but most of their Dutch colleagues spoke <strong>English</strong>well. Other adults were parents who did not work outside their homes and thus hadsomewhat less contact with Dutch than most of the other subjects.The learners were tested three times, at four- to five-month <strong>in</strong>tervals.They were firsttested with<strong>in</strong> six months of their arrival <strong>in</strong> Holland and with<strong>in</strong> six weeks of their start<strong>in</strong>gschool or work <strong>in</strong> a Dutch-language environment.ActivityCompar<strong>in</strong>g child, adolescent, and adult language learnersWhich group do you th<strong>in</strong>k (lid best on the first test (that is, who learned fastcst)?Which groupdo you th<strong>in</strong>k was best by the end of the year? Do you th<strong>in</strong>k some groups would do better oncerta<strong>in</strong> tasks than others? For example, who do you th<strong>in</strong>k would do best on the Pronunciationtasks, and who would do best on the tasks requir<strong>in</strong>g more mctal<strong>in</strong>guistic awareness? Compareyour predictions with the results for the different tasks which are presented <strong>in</strong>Table 2.1. An‘X’ <strong>in</strong>dicates that the group was the best on the test at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the year (an <strong>in</strong>dicationof the rate of learn<strong>in</strong>g), and a ‘Y’ <strong>in</strong>dicates the group that did best at the end of the year (an<strong>in</strong>dication of eventual atta<strong>in</strong>ment) .In the Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle study, the adolescents were by far the most successful1carners.They were ahead of everyone on all but one of the tests (pronunciation) on the firsttest session.That is, with<strong>in</strong> the first few months the adolescents had already made the mostprogress <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g Dutch.As the table <strong>in</strong>dicates, it was the adults who were better than thechildren and adolescents on pronunciation <strong>in</strong> the first test session. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, it was alsothe adults, not the children, whose scores were second best on the other tests at the first testsession. In other words, adolescents and adults learned faster than children <strong>in</strong> the first fewmonths of exposure to Dutch.By the end of the year, the children were catch<strong>in</strong>g up, or had surpassed, the adults onseveral measures. Nevertheless, it was the adolescents who reta<strong>in</strong>ed the highest levels ofperformance overall.


orofFACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 41Table 2.1 Comparison of language learn<strong>in</strong>g at different agesTask Child Adolescent AdultPronunciationAuditory discrim<strong>in</strong>ationMorphologySentence repetitionSentence translationSentence judgementPeabody picture vocabulary testStory comprehensionStorytell<strong>in</strong>gYYXYXYXYXY*XYXYYXYXx* These tests are too difficult for child learnersSnow and Hoefnagel-Hohle concluded that their results provide evidence that there isno critical period for language acquisition. However, their results can be <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> someother ways as well:1 Some of the tasks (for example, sentence judgement or translation) were too hard foryoung learners. Even <strong>in</strong> their native language, these tasks would have been unfamiliar anddifficult. In fact, young Dutch native speakers to whom the second language learners werecompared also had trouble with these tasks.2 Adults and adolescents may learn faster <strong>in</strong> the early stages of second language development(especially if they are learn<strong>in</strong>g a language which is similar to their first language).Youngchildren eventually catch up and even surpass them if their exposure to the language takesplace <strong>in</strong> contexts where they are surrounded by the language on a daily basis.3 Adults and adolescents can make considerable and rapid progress towards mastery of asecond language <strong>in</strong> contexts where they can make use of the language on a daily basis <strong>in</strong>social, personal, professional, or academic <strong>in</strong>teraction.At what age should second language <strong>in</strong>struction beg<strong>in</strong>?Even people who know noth<strong>in</strong>g about the critical period research are certa<strong>in</strong> that, <strong>in</strong> schoolprograms for second or foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g, ‘younger is better’. However, bothexperience and research show that older learners can atta<strong>in</strong> high, if not ‘native’, levels ofproficiency <strong>in</strong> their second language. Furthermore, it is essential to th<strong>in</strong>k carefully about thegoals of an <strong>in</strong>structional program and the context <strong>in</strong> which it occurs before we jump toconclusions about the necessity ~ even the desirability ~ the earliest possible start.The role of the critical period <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition is still much debated.For every rcsearcher who holds that there are maturational constra<strong>in</strong>ts on languageacquisition, there is another who considers that the age factor cannot be separated fromfactors such as motivation, social identity, and the conditions for learn<strong>in</strong>g. They arguethat older learners may well speak with an accent because they want to cont<strong>in</strong>ue be<strong>in</strong>gidentified with their first language cultural group, and adults rarely get access to the samequantity and quality of language <strong>in</strong>put that children receive <strong>in</strong> play sett<strong>in</strong>gs.


42 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAMany people conclude on the basis of studies such as those by Patkowski or Newportand Johnson that it is better to beg<strong>in</strong> second language <strong>in</strong>struction as early as possible.Yet itis very important to bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d the context of these studies. They deal with the highestpossible level of second language skills, the level at which a second language speaker is<strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guishable from a native speaker. But achiev<strong>in</strong>g a native-like mastery of the secondlanguage is not a goal for all second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> all contexts.When the objective of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g is native-like mastery of the targetlanguage, it is usually desirable for the learner to be completely surrounded by the languageas early as possible. However, early <strong>in</strong>tensive exposure to the second language may entail theloss or <strong>in</strong>complete development of the child’s first language.When the goal is basic communicative ability for all students <strong>in</strong> a school sett<strong>in</strong>g, andwhen it is assumed that the child’s native language will rema<strong>in</strong> the primary language, itmay be more efficient to beg<strong>in</strong> second orforeign language teach<strong>in</strong>g later. When learnersreceive only a few hours of <strong>in</strong>struction per week, learners who start later (for example, atage 10, 1 1 , or 12) often catch up with those who began earlier. We have often seen secondor foreign language programs which beg<strong>in</strong> with very young learners but offer only m<strong>in</strong>imalcontact with the language. Even when students do make progress <strong>in</strong> these early-startprograms, they sometimes f<strong>in</strong>d themselves placed <strong>in</strong> secondary school classes with studentswho have had no previous <strong>in</strong>struction. After years of classes, learners feel frustrated by thelack of progress, and their motivation to cont<strong>in</strong>ue may be dim<strong>in</strong>ished. School programsshould be based on realistic estimates of how long it takes to learn a second language. One ortwo hours a week will not produce very advanced second language speakers, no matter howyoung they were when they began.SummaryThe learner’s age is one of the characteristics which determ<strong>in</strong>e the way <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>dividualapproaches second language learn<strong>in</strong>g. But the opportunities for learn<strong>in</strong>g (both <strong>in</strong>side andoutside the classroom), the motivation to learn, and <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> aptitude forlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g are also important determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g factors <strong>in</strong> both rate of learn<strong>in</strong>g and eventualsuccess <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.In this chapter, we have looked at the ways <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>telligence, aptitude, personalityand motivational Characteristics, learner preferences, and age have been found to <strong>in</strong>fluencesecond language learn<strong>in</strong>g. We have learned that the study of <strong>in</strong>dividual learner variables isnot easy and that the results of research are not entirely satisfactory. This is partly becauseof the lack of clear def<strong>in</strong>itions and methods for the <strong>in</strong>dividual characteristics. It is also dueto the fact that these learner characteristics are not <strong>in</strong>dependent of one another: learnervariables <strong>in</strong>teract <strong>in</strong> complex ways. So far, researchers know very little about the nature ofthese complex <strong>in</strong>teractions.Thus, it rema<strong>in</strong>s difficult to make precise predictions about howa particular <strong>in</strong>dividual’s characteristics <strong>in</strong>fluence his or her success as a language learner.Nonetheless, <strong>in</strong> a classroom, a sensitive teacher, who takes learners’ <strong>in</strong>dividual personalitiesand learn<strong>in</strong>g styles <strong>in</strong>to account, can create a learn<strong>in</strong>g environment <strong>in</strong> which virtually alllearners can be successful <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a second language.


FACTORS AFFECTING SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING 43ReferencesCrookes, G. and Schmidt, R. (1991) ‘Motivation:“Reopen<strong>in</strong>g the research agenda”’, <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g 41 /4: 469-5 12.,Gardner, R. (1985) <strong>Social</strong> Psychology and Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g: The Role of Attitudes andMotivation. London: Edward Arnold.Gardner, R.C. and Lambert, W.E. (1972) Attitudes and Motivation <strong>in</strong> Second-<strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g.Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Genesee, F. (1976) ‘The role of <strong>in</strong>telligence <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g26/2: 267-80.Guiora, A., Beit-Hallahami, B., Brannon, R., Dull, C. and Scovel, T. (1972) ‘The effects ofexperimentally <strong>in</strong>duced changes <strong>in</strong> ego states on pronunciation ability <strong>in</strong> a second language:An exploratory study’, Comprehensive Psychiatry 13 /5: 42 1-8.Johnson, J. and Newport, E. (1989) ‘Critical period effects <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g: The<strong>in</strong>fluence of maturational state on the acquisition of <strong>English</strong> as a second language.’ CognitivePsychology 2 1 : 60-99.Obler, L. (1989) ‘Exceptional second language learners’, <strong>in</strong> Gass, S., Madden, C., Preston, D.and Sel<strong>in</strong>ker, L. (eds.) Variation <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, Vol. 11: Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic Issues.Clevedon, UK/Philadelphia, Pa. : Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters, pp. 141-9.Patkowski, M. (1980) ‘The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax <strong>in</strong> a second language’,<strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 30/2: 449-72.Skehan, P. (1989) Individual Dflerences <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. London: Edward Arnold.Reid, J. (ed.) (1995) Learn<strong>in</strong>g Styles <strong>in</strong> the ESL/EFL Classroom. NewYork: He<strong>in</strong>le & He<strong>in</strong>le.Snow, C. and Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1978) ‘The critical period for language acquisition: evidencefrom second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, Child Development 49/4: 1114-28.Wesche, M.B. (1981) ‘<strong>Language</strong> aptitude measures <strong>in</strong> stream<strong>in</strong>g, match<strong>in</strong>g students withmethods, and diagnosis of learn<strong>in</strong>g problems’, <strong>in</strong> Diller, K. (cd.) Individual Dgerences andUniversals <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Aptitude. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House. pp. 119-39.Yorio, C. (1986) ‘Consumerism <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g’, Canadian Modern<strong>Language</strong> Review 42/3: 668-87.


~ errorswereChapter 3Rod EllisSECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION:RESEARCH AND LANGUAGE PEDAGOGYIntroductionHE DOMINANT METHODS FOR TEACHING secondlanguages<strong>in</strong>the 1960sT were the grammar-translation method and thc audiol<strong>in</strong>gual method. These methodsrested on very differcnt theories of language learn<strong>in</strong>g. The grammar-translation methodrested on the belief that language learn<strong>in</strong>g was largely an <strong>in</strong>tellectual process of study<strong>in</strong>g andmemoriz<strong>in</strong>g bil<strong>in</strong>gual vocabulary lists and explicit grammar rules. The audiol<strong>in</strong>guist onedrew on bchaviourist theories of learn<strong>in</strong>g which emphasized habit formation throughrepeated practice and re<strong>in</strong>forcement. However, although there had been a number of studies<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the effects of teach<strong>in</strong>g on learn<strong>in</strong>g (Agard and Dunkcl 1948) very little wasknown about how learners actually lcarnt a second language. L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g, at that time, hadsimply not been rigorously studied.Start<strong>in</strong>g from the 196Os, two approaches to address<strong>in</strong>g this lacuna have been evident.The first, a cont<strong>in</strong>uation ofthe approach adopted <strong>in</strong> earlier research, consists of attempts to<strong>in</strong>vestigate the relative effectiveness of differcnt ways of teach<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> terms of thcproducts of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Experimental studies by Schcrer and Wertheimer (1 964) and Smith(1970), for example, compared the learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes of the grammar-translation andaudiol<strong>in</strong>gual methods. The results, however, were <strong>in</strong>conclusive. The studies failed todemonstrate the superiority of one method over the other.The second approach <strong>in</strong>volved the empirical study of how learners acquired an L2. Inthe first place, this took the form of studies oflearners' errors (e.g. Duskova 1969) and casestudies of <strong>in</strong>dividual learners learn<strong>in</strong>g a second language not <strong>in</strong> the classroom but throughexposure to it <strong>in</strong> natural sett<strong>in</strong>gs (c.g. Ravcm 1968).Thesc studics <strong>in</strong>volved forms ofrcsearchto which tcachers could easily relate if only because the constructs on which they were basedand <strong>in</strong>dividual learners ~ ones with which they were familiar. Also, thesestudies proved more rcward<strong>in</strong>g than the global method comparisons, provid<strong>in</strong>g clearevidence that L2 learners, like children acquir<strong>in</strong>g their first language (Ll), accumulatedknowledge of the language they wcrc learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a gradual and highly systematic fashion.Thus, whereas global method studics soon fell out of fashion,' studies of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g tookoff; SLA was born.Much of this early work <strong>in</strong> SLA was pedagogically motivatctl. That is, researchersconducted studies of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g with the cxpress <strong>in</strong>tention of address<strong>in</strong>g pcdagogic issues.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 45Many of these researchers were, <strong>in</strong> fact, orig<strong>in</strong>ally teachers themselves.’ The papers theywrote and published about their research typically concluded with a section <strong>in</strong> which theapplications and implications for language pedagogy were spelled out. The studies of learnererrors, for example, were used to address issues concern<strong>in</strong>g teachers’ attitude to errors, whaterrors should be corrected and how learner progress could be cvaluatcd.The case studies of<strong>in</strong>dividual learners were used to support the radical proposal that teachers should desistfrom try<strong>in</strong>g to ‘<strong>in</strong>tervene’ directly <strong>in</strong> the process of L2 acquisition and, <strong>in</strong>stead, developapproaches that would allow learners to learn ‘naturally’ (Newmark 1966 and Dulay and Burt1973).SLA has grown exponentially s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>its</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the 1960s. One of the outcomes of<strong>its</strong> growth and diversification is that much of the research is no longer directly concernedwith pedagogic issues. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to a theory advanced by Chomsky, children are able tolearn their mother tongue because they have <strong>in</strong>nate knowledge of the possible form that thegrammar of any language can take. Their task is to establish how the abstract pr<strong>in</strong>ciples thatconstitute this knowledge are manifest <strong>in</strong> the particular grammar they are learn<strong>in</strong>g. One ofthe ma<strong>in</strong> goals of UG-based SLA is to <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether and how these pr<strong>in</strong>ciples operate<strong>in</strong> L2 acquisition. This research, then, has been motivated by a desire to test a l<strong>in</strong>guistictheory rather than to address the practical problems of teach<strong>in</strong>g; it is oriented towardsl<strong>in</strong>guistics rather than language pedagogy (Gass 1989).jOther sub-fields of SLA have cont<strong>in</strong>ued the tradition of strong l<strong>in</strong>ks with languagepedagogy.Two <strong>in</strong> particular stand out.The first is the study of the role of <strong>in</strong>put and <strong>in</strong>teraction<strong>in</strong> L2 acquisition (c.g. Long 1981 and Pica 1992).The question of what constitutes optimal<strong>in</strong>put for language learn<strong>in</strong>g is potentially of considerable relevance to teachers. Indeed, oneway of characteriz<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong> terms of provid<strong>in</strong>g learners with opportunities to hearand use the L2. The theories and f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs which this research has generated have fed <strong>in</strong>toclassroom research, as, for example, <strong>in</strong> studies which have <strong>in</strong>vestigated the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>put and<strong>in</strong>teraction afforded by different types of language tasks (see Crookes and Gass 1993) andby different modes of classroom participation (e.g. Pica and Doughty 1985). The secondsub-field of SLA with clear l<strong>in</strong>ks to language pedagogy is the study of form-focused <strong>in</strong>struction.SLA researchers have <strong>in</strong>vestigated whether teach<strong>in</strong>g learners particular grammaticalstructures actually results <strong>in</strong> their be<strong>in</strong>g learnt (e.g Spada and Lightbown 1993) and, also,what methodological options for teach<strong>in</strong>g grammatical structures arc most effective (e.g.VanPatten and Cadierno 1993).However, irrcspectivc of whether SLA addresses issues of likely relevance to teachers,there is the problem of a gap between SLA and language pedagogy. Ultimately, this gap isnot so much a question of what issues SLA addresses, but of the manner <strong>in</strong> which SLA isconducted.The goal of SLA, like that of all academic discipl<strong>in</strong>es, is to contribute to technicalknowledge.This is reflected <strong>in</strong> the fact that SLA is, by and large, the preserve of univcrsitybasedresearchers, whose primary allegiance is to the conduct of well-designed studies andtheory development <strong>in</strong> their field.This is as true of those researchers who are concerned withareas of potential relevance to language pedagogy (c.g. <strong>in</strong>put/<strong>in</strong>teraction and the study ofform-focused <strong>in</strong>struction) as it is of researchers who see SLA as a means of contribut<strong>in</strong>g toother discipl<strong>in</strong>es such as l<strong>in</strong>guistics or cognitive psychology. In contrast, language pedagogyis concerned with practical knowledge. Textbook writers draw on their experience of thek<strong>in</strong>ds of activities that work <strong>in</strong> classrooms and, of course, on their familiarity with otherpublished materials. Teachers draw on their hands-on knowledge to perform the myriad oftasks that comprise teach<strong>in</strong>g.Given that a gap exists between SLA and language pedagogy and assum<strong>in</strong>g that SLA is,at least, of some potential relevance, the question arises as to how the gap can be bridged.


46 ROD ELLISMy perspective is that of the outsider-<strong>in</strong>sider, for an applied l<strong>in</strong>guist is not a practitioner oflanguage pedagogy (see Corder 1973) but rather someone who looks at language pedagogyfrom the vantage po<strong>in</strong>t of knowledge gleaned from technical sources. In my case, the sourceis SLA.Technical and practical knowledgeI have suggested that two types of knowledgc can be dist<strong>in</strong>guished: technical knowledge andpractical knowledge. This dist<strong>in</strong>ction is, <strong>in</strong> fact, common <strong>in</strong> the literature on professionalknowledge (e.g. Calderhcad 1988 and Eraut 1994).Technical knowledge is explicit; that is,it exists <strong>in</strong> a declarative form that has been codified. For these reasons it can be exam<strong>in</strong>edanalytically and disputed. Technical knowledge is acquired deliberatcly either by reflect<strong>in</strong>gdeeply about the object of enquiry or by <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g it empirically.The lattcr <strong>in</strong>volves theuse of a well-def<strong>in</strong>ed set of procedures designed to ensure the validity and reliability of theknowlcdge obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Technical knowledge is generalixd; that is, it takes thc form of statementsthat can be applied to many particular cases. For this reason, it cannot easily be applied‘off the shelf’ <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>d of rapid decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g needed to deal with problems as they occur<strong>in</strong> day-to-day liv<strong>in</strong>g.Over the years, SLA has provided a substantial body of technical knowledge about howpeople learn a second 1anguage.This is reflected <strong>in</strong> the ever-grow<strong>in</strong>g set of technical termsused to label this knowledge: overgcneralization and transfer errors, order and sequence ofacquisition, foreigner talk, <strong>in</strong>put and <strong>in</strong>take, notic<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g and communication strategies,the teachability hypothesis (see the glossary <strong>in</strong> Ellis 1994).This technical knowledgc and theterms that label it constitute the goods that are carefully guarded by practitioners of SLA.In contrast, practical knowledge is implicit and <strong>in</strong>tuitive. We are generally not aware ofwhat we practically know. For example, 1 know how to tic my shoe laces but I have littleawareness about the sequence of actions I must perform to do this and could certa<strong>in</strong>ly notdescribe them very well. In contrast to technical knowledge, practical knowledge is acquiredthrough actual experience <strong>in</strong> the context of perform<strong>in</strong>g actions by means of procedures thatare only poorly understood. Similarly, practical knowlcdge is fully expressible only <strong>in</strong>practice, although it may be possible, through reflection, to codify aspects of it. The greatadvantage of practical knowledge is that it is proceduralixd and thus can be drawn on rapidlyand efficiently to handle particular cases.Practis<strong>in</strong>g professionals (lawyers, doctors, and teachers) are primarily concerned withaction <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g particular case? and for this reason draw on practical rather than technicalknowledge <strong>in</strong> the pursuit of their work. Freidson (1 977, cited <strong>in</strong> Eraut 1994: 53) describeshow medical practitioners opcrate:Onc whose work requires practical application to concrete cases simply cannotma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the same frame of m<strong>in</strong>d as the scholar or scientist: he cannot suspend action<strong>in</strong> the absence of <strong>in</strong>controvertible evidence or be skeptical of himself, his experience,his work and <strong>its</strong> fruit. In emergencies he cannot wait for discoveries of the future.Deal<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>in</strong>dividual cases, he cannot rely solely on probabilities or on generalconcepts or pr<strong>in</strong>ciples: hc must also rely on his own senses. By the nature of his workthe cl<strong>in</strong>ician must assume responsibility for practical action, and <strong>in</strong> so do<strong>in</strong>g he mustrely on his concrete, cl<strong>in</strong>ical cxperiencc.Teachers, faced with thc need to makc countless decisions to accomplish a lesson, must also


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 47necessarily rely primarily on the practical knowledge they have acquired through teach<strong>in</strong>gor, perhaps, through their experiences of hav<strong>in</strong>g been taught. However, it may be possiblefor other practitioners of language pedagogy (e.g. syllabus designers, test constructors, andmaterials writers) to attempt some <strong>in</strong>tegration of technical and practical knowledge, as theiractivities are more amcnable to careful plann<strong>in</strong>g and deliberate decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g.The crucial issue is the nature of the relationship between technical and practicalknowledge.To what extent and <strong>in</strong> what ways can the technical knowledge derived from deepreflection and research <strong>in</strong>fluence actual practice? How can technical knowledge be utilized<strong>in</strong> the creation of the k<strong>in</strong>d of practical knowledge with which teachers must necessarilywork? Weiss (1 977) provides a way of address<strong>in</strong>g these questions. He describes three modelsof research use.Decision-driven model Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this model, research is aimed at <strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g a particulardecision.Thus, the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for research is not a theory of L2 acquisition or a previouspiece of research hut rather some practical issue of direct concern to teachers. There is aconsiderable body of SLA research that appears to fit <strong>in</strong>to this model. However, for thisresearch to be truly decision-driven it needs to be formulated <strong>in</strong> a manner that teachers willreadily understand. This is often not the casc, however. Rcscarchers prepare their articlesfor publication <strong>in</strong> journals and books that will be read by other researchers even if theyaddress issues of direct concern to teachers. In fact, then, much of the SLA research thatapparently belongs to the decision-driven model is more truly representative ofweiss’ yecondmodel - the knowledge-driven model.Knowledgedriven model Knowledge-driven research is <strong>in</strong>tended to contribute to a specificdiscipl<strong>in</strong>e. Its primary goal is to advance the knowledge base of the discipl<strong>in</strong>e by construct<strong>in</strong>g -and test<strong>in</strong>g explicit theories or by develop<strong>in</strong>g research methodology. As we have seen, oneway of characteriz<strong>in</strong>g the development of SLA as a field of study is <strong>in</strong> terms of a gradualmovement towards knowledge-driven research. Much of the earlier research was descriptive<strong>in</strong> nature (e.g. the studies of learner errors and the case studies of <strong>in</strong>dividual learners),motivated quite explicitly by a desire to <strong>in</strong>form pedagogy and published <strong>in</strong> a form that wasrelatively accessible to teachers. Later research, although certa<strong>in</strong>ly not all, has been designedto test specific SLA theories, has been <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly experimental <strong>in</strong> nature and has beenwritten about with other researchers as the <strong>in</strong>tended audience. Researchers may feel theirresearch is of relevance to language pedagogy but often see little need to consider <strong>its</strong>applications directly.Interactive model Here technical knowledge and practical knowledge are <strong>in</strong>ter-related <strong>in</strong> theperformance of some professional activity. The way <strong>in</strong> which this is achieved is highlycomplex. Weiss (1 977: 87-8) comments:the process is not of l<strong>in</strong>ear order from research to decision but a disorderly setof <strong>in</strong>terconnections and back-and-forthness that defies neat diagrams. All k<strong>in</strong>ds ofpeople <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> an issue area pool their talents, beliefs, and understand<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> aneffort to make sense of a problem.Not surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, then, the <strong>in</strong>teractive model is problematic. As Eraut (1 994) po<strong>in</strong>ts outthere are various factors that constra<strong>in</strong> the professional’s ability to make use of the knowledgecreated through research, particularly <strong>in</strong> a field such as teach<strong>in</strong>g. Few resources are availablefor effect<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>teraction. Fund<strong>in</strong>g for research, for example, is typically awarded to


48 ROD ELLISuniversity-based rescarchers concerned with knowledge-creation rather than to teams ofresearchers and teachers concerned with solv<strong>in</strong>g practical tcach<strong>in</strong>g problems through apool<strong>in</strong>g of expertise.Teachcrs rarely have the time to familiari7e themselves with publishedresearch. Also, the very nature of technical and practical knowledge makes it difficult to<strong>in</strong>ter-relate. Considerable effort and probably prolonged <strong>in</strong>teraction are needed to comb<strong>in</strong>ethe analytical skills of the researcher with the holistic and highly contextualized skills of theteacher.Similar problems exist regard<strong>in</strong>g the utilization of practical knowledge <strong>in</strong> the creation oftechnical knowledge. Practical knowledge is largely tacit and difficult to codify. Consequently,<strong>its</strong> reliability antl validity cannot be easily assessed. Given the requirement thattechnical knowlcdge is demonstrably reliable and valid, rcsearchcrs generally avoid referenceto practical knowledge. However, as Eraut (1 994) notes, researchers’ own practicalexperiences may often <strong>in</strong>fluence their work <strong>in</strong> subtle and unstated ways.To a certa<strong>in</strong> extent,then, the <strong>in</strong>teraction model may work implicitly.This discussion of technical and practical knowledge helps us to understand whySLA, as it has evolved s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>ception, cannot automatically be assumed to be of use<strong>in</strong> language pedagogy and, particularly, to classroom teachers. The gap between SLA andlanguage pedagogy is a product of both the typcs of knowledge these two fields typicallyemploy and the lack of opportunity to bridge the gap.The SLA researchers’ perspectiveThe nature of the relationship between SLA and language pedagogy has attracted theattention of a number of researchers over the years. A useful start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> our explorationof how SLA might <strong>in</strong>form pedagogy is to take a look at what these SLA researchers have hadto say.The application of SLA can take place <strong>in</strong> two rather tliffcrent ways. As Cordcr (1 977)has po<strong>in</strong>ted out, the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t can be thc rcsearch <strong>its</strong>elf with the applicd l<strong>in</strong>guist cast <strong>in</strong>the role of <strong>in</strong>novator or <strong>in</strong>itiator, advanc<strong>in</strong>g pedagogical proposals on the basis of his/herknowledge of SLA. This corresponds to Wciss’s knowledge-driven model of research use.Alternatively, the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t can be unsolved practical problems <strong>in</strong> language pcdagogy,<strong>in</strong> which case the SLA researcher takes on the role of a consultant who is approached bypractitioners for possible solutions. This corresponds to Weiss’s decision-driven model ofresearch use. We f<strong>in</strong>d both types of application discussed <strong>in</strong> the litcrature but it is probablythe first that is paramount, reflect<strong>in</strong>g, perhaps, the dom<strong>in</strong>ance of thc researcher’s perspectiveover that of the teacher’s.In general, SLA researchers with a strong <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> pedagogy have been cautious aboutapply<strong>in</strong>g SLA. Early articles by Tarone et ul. (1 976) and Hatch (1 978) emphasized thc needto be careful. Hatch lamcntcd that researchers have often heen over-ready to makeapplications to pedagogy, po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out ‘. . . our field must be known for the <strong>in</strong>credible leapsof logic we make <strong>in</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g our research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to classroom teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Tarone et ul.(1 976) advanced a number of reasons why SLA could not serve as an adcquate basis foradvis<strong>in</strong>g teachers. Among other po<strong>in</strong>ts, they argued that the research to date was too limited<strong>in</strong> scope, that the methodology for collect<strong>in</strong>g and analys<strong>in</strong>g data was unproven antl that toofew studies had been replicatctl.They also noted that the practices of research and teach<strong>in</strong>gwere very different <strong>in</strong> nature. Whereas researchers adopted a slow, bit-by-bit approach,teachers had immetliatc needs to meet. In the previous section, we considered this importantdifference <strong>in</strong> terms of the dist<strong>in</strong>ction between technical and practical knowledge.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 49The concerns voiced by Tarone et al. (1 976) and Hatch (1978) are very real ones. Theyreflect the understandable reticence of researchers to plunge <strong>in</strong> before they arc certa<strong>in</strong> oftheir results.This uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty about the quality of the research be<strong>in</strong>g produced may have beenone of the reasons why some researchers stopped add<strong>in</strong>g sections on the applications of theirresearch to their published articl~s.~ In retrospect, however, I am not so sure that researchersneed to be so cautious. As Corder (1980) noted, teachers cannot wait until researchers arecompletely satisfied that their results are robust and generalizable. Should teachers not bepermitted to base their pedagogical decisions on the best <strong>in</strong>formation available even if thisis still <strong>in</strong>adequate <strong>in</strong> the eyes of researchers? More importantly, the apply-with-cautionapproach makes certa<strong>in</strong> assumptions about the relationship between research/theory andpractice which are themselves challengeable. It appears to view the practitioner as a consumerof research. From such a stance, of course, it is essential to make sure that the product be<strong>in</strong>gmarketed is a sound one. But, as we will see later, th~s rather positivist view of the relationshipbetween research and practice 5 is not acceptable to many educators and may not serve as themost appropriate model for discuss<strong>in</strong>g how SLA can aid teach<strong>in</strong>g.There are alternatives to the <strong>in</strong>strumental view of SLA implicit <strong>in</strong> the early articles byTarone et al. (1976) and Hatch (1978). One is that SLA should not so much be used to tellpractitioners what to do, as to <strong>in</strong>form their understand<strong>in</strong>g of how L2 acquisition takes placeso that they will know better what it is possible to achieve <strong>in</strong> a classroom.This is the positionadopted by Lightbown ( 1985). She argues that SLA has noth<strong>in</strong>g to tell teachers about whatto teach but serves as a guide about how to teach. Lightbown recognizes that teachers willneed to rely primarily on their own practical experience of which approaches work andwhich do not but she suggests that familiarity with the results of SLA research will helpteachers make up their m<strong>in</strong>ds. For Lightbown, then, the value of SLA lies not <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>novative techniques or new teach<strong>in</strong>g approaches but rather <strong>in</strong> shap<strong>in</strong>g expectancies and <strong>in</strong>lend<strong>in</strong>g support to particular approaches, such as communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g. Fromthis perspective, however, SLA is of limited relevance to language pedagogy, for as Lightbown(ibid.: 182) comments:Second-language acquisition research does not tell teachers what to teach, and what <strong>its</strong>ays about how to teach they have already figured out.If this is all SLA can do for teachers, one might well ask whether it is worth their whilemak<strong>in</strong>g the effort to become familiar with it.Not all researchers/theorists have felt the need to play down the contribution that SLAcan make to language pedagogy. Some have looked for ways of bridg<strong>in</strong>g the gap betweenresearch and classroom practice. One way is to construct a theory of L2 acquisition that iscompatible with the available research but which also is tuned to the needs of teachers.Thisis what Krashen has tried to do. Krashen (1 98 3) argues that it is not the research <strong>its</strong>elf thatshould be used to address pedagogical issues but rather the theory derived from the research.Even applied SLA research should be related to practice via thcory. Theory is importantbecause it provides teachers with ‘an underly<strong>in</strong>g rationale for methodology <strong>in</strong> general’(Krashen 1983: 261) and thus helps them to adapt to different situations and constitutes abasis for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g new pedagogical ideas. Krashen argues that the theory must be a theoryof L2 acquisition as opposed to a l<strong>in</strong>guistic theory or a theory of general learn<strong>in</strong>g. Indeed,he claims that teachers have grown suspicious of ‘theory’ because of the failure of l<strong>in</strong>guisticand psychological theories to solve pedagogic problems. He believes that SLA theory, becauseit expla<strong>in</strong>s how learners actually learn a second language, is of more direct relevance. Krashenalso argues that theory must be empirically grounded (i.e. based on actual L2 research)rather than on armchair speculation.


50 ROD ELLISMuch of Krashen’s published work has been concerned with the applications of hisown forcefully promoted theory (i.c. the Monitor Model and, more recently, the InputHypothesis), as <strong>in</strong> Krashen andTerrell(l983). It should be noted, however, that contrary tosome criticisms levelled at him (see Widdowson 1990: 34) Krashen has never sought topreclude teachers explor<strong>in</strong>g pragmatic options derived from ideas outside his theoreticalframework. Krashen argues <strong>in</strong> favour of the utilization of theory <strong>in</strong> general, not just histheory. Also, he explicitly recognizes that teachers will and should br<strong>in</strong>g ideas and <strong>in</strong>tuitionsbased on their own practical expcrience to decision mak<strong>in</strong>g. As Krashen (1983: 261) says‘teach<strong>in</strong>g rema<strong>in</strong>s an art as well as a science’.There are obvious attractions of theory-based as opposed to research-based applications.A theory affords a composite view of L2 acquisition. Proposals based on it cannot bedismissed by po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out the limitations of specific research studies. A theory is general <strong>in</strong>nature and, thus, any proposals derived from it are potentially valid <strong>in</strong> a variety of teach<strong>in</strong>gcontexts. In contrast, <strong>in</strong>dividual research studies are necessarily located <strong>in</strong> specific contexts,mak<strong>in</strong>g it difficult to advance proposals of general applicability. Also, proposals bascd on atheory are likely to possess a coherence lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the piecemeal application of <strong>in</strong>dividualstudies. One of the attractions of Krashen’s theory is that it offers teachers an overarch<strong>in</strong>gview of what and how to teach.However, there are obvious dangers of theory-based applications. As Bcretta (1991) andLong (1993) have po<strong>in</strong>ted out, SLA theories do not tend to go away, even when they are <strong>in</strong>obvious opposition to each other. In a thoughtful discussion of why this is so, Schumann(1993) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that it is extremely difficult to falsify a theory. One reason is that whereashypotheses are typically tested <strong>in</strong> isolation they exist <strong>in</strong> ‘a network of auxiliary assumptions’(ibid.: 259) with the result that even if a particular hypothesis is not supported it cannot bedismissed because it is impossible to tell exactly where the problem lies. Thus, theoristsusually expcrience little difficulty <strong>in</strong> immuniz<strong>in</strong>g their theories aga<strong>in</strong>st counter f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs;they simply adjust an underly<strong>in</strong>g assumption or rcconceive the construct on which thehypothesis is based. Krashen has proven adept at ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g his own theory despiteconcerted criticism from prom<strong>in</strong>ent researchers antl applied l<strong>in</strong>guists. But if theories cannotbe falsified antl, therefore, are able to survive more or less <strong>in</strong>def<strong>in</strong>itely how, then, can teachersevaluate the legitimacy of proposals based on them? In the case of Krashen, for example, howcan teachers evaluate his pr<strong>in</strong>cipal proposal, namely that teachers should be primarilyconcerned with provid<strong>in</strong>g plentiful comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put so that acquisition (i .e.subconscious language learn<strong>in</strong>g) can take place? In short, applications based on an SLA theoryare risky because they have to be taken on faith.This might not matter so much if theory wereused to advance suggestions for teachers to test out <strong>in</strong> their own practice but, more oftenthan not, theory-derived applications are vested with an authority that works aga<strong>in</strong>st suchpedagogic experimentation. For example, Krashen’s claim that learn<strong>in</strong>g (i.c. the consciousstudy of l<strong>in</strong>guistic forms) has a relatively m<strong>in</strong>or role to play <strong>in</strong> L2 acquisition works aga<strong>in</strong>stteachers’ <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> the context of their own teach<strong>in</strong>g, how form-focused <strong>in</strong>structioncan complement and perhaps enhance acquisition.There is a more serious objection to Krashcn’s proposal that SLA theory should guidelanguage pedagogy ~ one that has already been h<strong>in</strong>ted at <strong>in</strong> the discussion of technicalknowledge and practical knowledge, SLA theories, such as Krashen’s, are typically thcproduct of the contemplative approach to enquiry that charactcrizes much modern scientificth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g (see Lantolf 1995). Such theories have been developed through formulat<strong>in</strong>gand systematically test<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses based on them. The result is ‘technical knowledge’ .However, such knowledge, because of the very form <strong>in</strong> which it is couched, is not readilyaccessible to practitioners <strong>in</strong> their day-to-day work, although Krashen has done as good a job


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 51as any to make it so. For a theory to be of maximum use to teachers it has to take the formof praxis ~ a theory of action. This is a po<strong>in</strong>t that will be taken up later <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Another way of bridg<strong>in</strong>g the gap between SLA and language pedagogy is through whatJohnston (1 987) has called ‘a technology of teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Johnston draws an analogy betweeneng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g. He argues that whereas eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g has successfully def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>its</strong> ownproblem space as <strong>in</strong>dependent from that of support<strong>in</strong>g discipl<strong>in</strong>es, such as physics, languageteach<strong>in</strong>g has not yet done so. This is because it lacks a sound body of practical knowledgedeveloped through experimentation <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>its</strong>elf. Johnston dist<strong>in</strong>guishes pureresearch (i.e. the research carried out by SLA researchers such as himself) and classroomresearch. He recognizes that pure research can only provide guidel<strong>in</strong>es and suggestions,which have to be put to the test. For Johnston, then, the gap between SLA and languagepedagogy needs to be filled by conduct<strong>in</strong>g experimental studies <strong>in</strong> actual classrooms. He isoptimistic that such research will ensure that ‘the language teach<strong>in</strong>g of 10 to 15 years hencewill be rather different from the hit and miss methods of today’ (ibid. : 38).There is a logical objection to Johnston’s position. If the k<strong>in</strong>d of classroom researchJohnston has <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d is controlled experimentation (where the realities of the classroom haveto be manipulated to control for unwanted variables that may <strong>in</strong>fluence the effect ofa given treatment), there may not, <strong>in</strong> fact, be any difference between pure and classroomresearch. In this respect, Wright’s (1 992) dist<strong>in</strong>ction between research <strong>in</strong> classrooms andresearch on classrooms is relevant. To develop the technology of teach<strong>in</strong>g that Johnstonconsiders necessary it is the latter that is required, for as Wright (ibjd. : 192) argues ‘anunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of the L2 classroom might best proceed . . . from <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigation as a culture<strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> own right’. However, controlled experimentation may not be the best way to carry outrcsearch on classrooms.The case for bas<strong>in</strong>g pedagogical decisions on L2 classroom research has been advancedby a number of other researchers and language educators. Jarvis (1983: 238), for example,argues that ‘Our knowledge must come from our own research’ and laments the fact that ithas typically not done so. Long (1983b) reports the results of a survey of methods courses<strong>in</strong> Masters programmes <strong>in</strong> TESOL <strong>in</strong> the United States and Canada. Only 18% <strong>in</strong>cludedreference to classroom-centred research (CCR). Long (ibid. : 284) suggests that this mayreflect the practical orientation of methods courses but he argues that classroom-centredresearch is ‘em<strong>in</strong>ently practical’ because it is ‘concerned with what actually goes on <strong>in</strong> theclassrooms, as opposed to what is supposed to go on’, a po<strong>in</strong>t that is only true, of course, ifthe researcher accepts the realities of classroom behaviour and makes no attempt to manipulateit for research purposes. Long gives three reasons why classroom-centred researchshould be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> methods courses: it has already produced some practical <strong>in</strong>formation;teachers can use thc research tools that have been employed to <strong>in</strong>vestigate their ownclassrooms; classroom-centred research will help teachers become sceptical about rely<strong>in</strong>gon s<strong>in</strong>gle teach<strong>in</strong>g methods. In a subsequent paper, Long (1990) argues the need for acommon body of knowledge which can be transmitted to teachers <strong>in</strong> much the same way asa common body of knowledge about medic<strong>in</strong>e is conveyed to doctors. He suggests thatalthough L2 classroom research is limited <strong>in</strong> a number of respects it constitutes ‘a grow<strong>in</strong>gbody of tangible evidence about language teach<strong>in</strong>g’ (ibid. : 1 16). For Long, this constituteshard evidence which is better than the prejudices and suppositions which he believescharacterize most pedagogical decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g. Like Johnston, then, Long envisages classroomresearch as the means by which researchers can most effectively <strong>in</strong>fluence languagepedagogy.There are serious reasons for disput<strong>in</strong>g the optimism that both Johnston, Long, andothers share regard<strong>in</strong>g the effect such rcsearch will have on language pedagogy. As Stenhousc


52 ROD ELLIS(1 979: 71-7) has so amus<strong>in</strong>gly demonstrated <strong>in</strong> his fictional account of how a teacher grappleswith the attempt to apply the results of research concern<strong>in</strong>g strategies for teach<strong>in</strong>g aboutrace relations, classroom research is unlikely to produce clear answers to teachers’ questionsbecause it only demonstrates what works by and large or for the most part whereas teachersare conccrncd with what will work <strong>in</strong> their own particular teach<strong>in</strong>g contcxts. Stenhouse(1975: 25) has stated the essential problem more formally elscwhcrc:The crucial po<strong>in</strong>t is that the proposal (from research) is not to be regarded as anunqualified rccomrnendation but rather as a provisional specification claim<strong>in</strong>g no morethan to be worth putt<strong>in</strong>g to the test of practice. Such proposals claim to be <strong>in</strong>telligentrather than correct.In other words, classroom rcscarch, although potentially closer to thc realities teachers haveto grapple with than non-classroom research, is still remote from actual practice. The gapbetween SLA and practice may be narrowed somewhat but it cannot be filled by classroomresearch, even when this is research on, rather than just <strong>in</strong>, classrooms.So far we have considered what various SLA researchers have had to say regard<strong>in</strong>g theapplication of rcscarch/theory to language pedagogy. The view of change implicit <strong>in</strong> all ofthe positions we have exam<strong>in</strong>ed is a top-down one. Applied l<strong>in</strong>guists draw on <strong>in</strong>formationfrom SLA to <strong>in</strong>itiate - tentatively or confidently - various pedagogic proposals.The proposalsmay be based on pure research, on a theory of L2 acquisition, or on classroom-centredresearch but <strong>in</strong> each case the presumed orig<strong>in</strong>ator of the proposal is the SLA researcher/theorist. It is timc now to briefly consider an alternative way <strong>in</strong> which SLA can bc used to<strong>in</strong>form language pedagogy.When the researcher functions as a consultant he or she functions AS a resource help<strong>in</strong>gteachers solve the practical problems they have identified. A good example of this approachcan be found <strong>in</strong> Pica (1 994). Pica’s start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t is not SLA <strong>its</strong>elf but rather the questionsthat teachers have asked her ‘both <strong>in</strong> the privacy of their classrooms and <strong>in</strong> the more publicdoma<strong>in</strong> of professional meet<strong>in</strong>gs’ (ibid.: 50). Pica offers a list of ten questions deal<strong>in</strong>g withsuch matters as the relative importance of comprehension and production, the rolc of explicitgrammar <strong>in</strong>struction, and thc utility of drill and practice.‘ Pica provides answers to thesequestions based on her understand<strong>in</strong>g of the SLA research literature.The obvious advantage of such an approach to apply<strong>in</strong>g SLA is that the <strong>in</strong>formationprovided is more likely to be heeded by teachers because it addresses issues they haveidentified as important. Bahns (1 990: 1 15) goes so far as to claim:The <strong>in</strong>itiative for apply<strong>in</strong>g research results of any k<strong>in</strong>d to any field of practicewhatsoever should come from the practitioners themselves.Such a statement ignores, however, some obvious limitations <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>sider approach.Teachers can only ask questions based on their own expericncc.They cannot ask questionsabout issues they have no knowledge of. If Bahns’s dictum were to be religiously adhered tomany of the developments <strong>in</strong> language pedagogy over the last twenty years would probablynot have taken place. For example, teachers would have been unlikely to ask‘what is the bestway to organize a syllabus - <strong>in</strong> terms of structures, notions, or tasks?’ because they wouldnot have known what ‘notions’ or ‘tasks’ (<strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> technical sense) wcrc.These concepts havebeen derived from the work of l<strong>in</strong>guists or applied l<strong>in</strong>guists, hut have not arisen spontaneouslythrough the practice of teach<strong>in</strong>g. Thus, although much can be said <strong>in</strong> favour of an <strong>in</strong>siderapproach, there is also a case for the outsider application of SLA.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 53A number of more recent discussions of the relationship between SLA and languagepedagogy have grappled with this issue. Gass (1 995: 16), for example, suggests that one wayround the <strong>in</strong>sider/outsider problem is for researchers and teachers to work ‘<strong>in</strong> tandem todetcrm<strong>in</strong>e how SLA f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs can be evaluated and be made applicable to a classroomsituation, and to detcrm<strong>in</strong>e which SLA f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to use’ .The k<strong>in</strong>d of collaborative endeavourGass has <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d is one where rcsearchers and teachers seek to understand each other’s goalsand needs and she suggests a number of areas where the concerns of the two groups co<strong>in</strong>cide(c.g. the issue of correction). However, true collaboration <strong>in</strong>volves not just agreement aboutwhat to <strong>in</strong>vestigate but also how. Gass partly addresses this by quot<strong>in</strong>g from Schachter (1 993:181):We need to create a m<strong>in</strong>dset <strong>in</strong> which both teachers and researchers view classroomsas laboratories where theory and practice can <strong>in</strong>teract to make both better practice andbetter theory.The problem here is that whereas researchers may feel comfortable <strong>in</strong> view<strong>in</strong>g classroomsas laboratories, teachers may not. It is also mistaken to imply ~ as Schachter seems to do-that researchers engage <strong>in</strong> theory and teachers <strong>in</strong> practice. SLA and language pedagogy areboth characterized by theory and practice, albeit of different k<strong>in</strong>ds. The issue of howresearchers and teachers can effectively collaborate is complex. It is one that has beenaddressed <strong>in</strong> some depth <strong>in</strong> the education literature. As Gass acknowledges, however, SLAresearchers have paid scant attention to this literature.From this <strong>in</strong>itial exploration of what it means to apply SLA research it is clear thatthere is no easy answer. For some, the immaturity of SLA as a field of enquiry precludesapplications. For others, SLA can only hope to shape teachers’ expectations of what is possible<strong>in</strong> the classroom. Others have developed specific proposals on the basis ofgeneral theoriesof L2 acquisition. Others have suggested that the gap between SLA and teach<strong>in</strong>g can be filledby conduct<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong> and on L2 classrooms. F<strong>in</strong>ally, some researchers have argued foran approach where they act as consultants address<strong>in</strong>g issues raised by teachers or where theyparticipate <strong>in</strong> collaborative research with t~achers.~As we have seen, each of these approacheshas someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> favour but none of them is entirely successful <strong>in</strong> clos<strong>in</strong>g the gap betwecnSLA rcscarch and language pedagogy. In the next section we consider the views of a numberof educators on how research can be made relevant to teachers.Educational perspectivesEarlier we noted that the once close connection which SLA researchers <strong>in</strong>itially envisagedbetween SLA and language pedagogy has not cont<strong>in</strong>ued.To understand the gulf that frequentlydivides the theory and practice of research on the one hand, and the theory and practice ofteach<strong>in</strong>g on the other, we need to exam<strong>in</strong>e the guid<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and assumptions of each. Weneed to consider the culture of research and the culture of teach<strong>in</strong>g.Let us beg<strong>in</strong> with research. It is customary to dist<strong>in</strong>guish two broad traditions <strong>in</strong> empiricalcnquiry ~ the confirmatory and the <strong>in</strong>tcrpretative. The confirmatory tradition is <strong>in</strong>tcrventionist.It is manifest <strong>in</strong> carefully designed experiments, such as the agricultural experimentsof R. A. Fischer (1935) <strong>in</strong> the United States, which wcrc designed to discovcr whichtreatment produced the best crop yields.The key characteristics of the confirmatory traditionare the use ofrandom sampl<strong>in</strong>g (i.e. subjects are randomly distributed <strong>in</strong>to an experimentaland a control group) and the careful control of cxtraneous variables (i.e. those variables that


54 ROD ELLISmight confound the study of the particular variable under <strong>in</strong>vestigation). The <strong>in</strong>terpretativetradition is reflected <strong>in</strong> Weber’s (1961) famous def<strong>in</strong>ition of sociology: ‘Sociology . . . is ascience which attempts the <strong>in</strong>terpretative understand<strong>in</strong>g of social action.’ It is manifest <strong>in</strong>non-<strong>in</strong>terventionist studies that seek to develop an understand<strong>in</strong>g of the social rules thatunderlie a particular activity by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the mean<strong>in</strong>g that the social actors <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> theactivity themselves put on it. As Van Lier (1 990) po<strong>in</strong>ts out, where confirmatory researchseeks causes, <strong>in</strong>terpretative research looks for reasons.Both of these traditions can be found <strong>in</strong> SLA. As we have already noted, SLA began withcase studies of learners (e.g. Ravem 1968). Thcsc studies focused on <strong>in</strong>dividual learners,collect<strong>in</strong>g samples of spoken language by observ<strong>in</strong>g the learners <strong>in</strong> naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>genvironments.’ These case studies <strong>in</strong>vestigatcd naturalistic lcarn<strong>in</strong>g by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g thelanguage produccd by lcarncrs, the processes and strategies they used and how <strong>in</strong>dividualand social factors affected their progress. One of the outcomes of this tradition of research<strong>in</strong> SLA was descriptive <strong>in</strong>formation about the order <strong>in</strong> which learners acquired diffcrcntgrammatical structures and the scqucncc of stages they followed <strong>in</strong> master<strong>in</strong>g particularstructures such as negatives, <strong>in</strong>terrogatives, and relative clauses. Another branch of the<strong>in</strong>terpretative tradition of research <strong>in</strong> SLA can be found <strong>in</strong> ethnographic studies of L2classrooms (c.g.Van Lier 1988, Markee 1994a).These studies have sought to describe thck<strong>in</strong>ds of discourse <strong>in</strong> which classroom learners engage and how thcse <strong>in</strong>fluence their L2development.The confirmatory tradition is evident <strong>in</strong> much of thc work based on Universal Grammar(e.g. Flynn and Martohardjono 1995), where data elicited by mcans of such <strong>in</strong>struments asgrammaticality judgement tests have been used to exam<strong>in</strong>e whether learners with diffcrcntfirst languages manifest access to particular pr<strong>in</strong>ciplcs of language. It is also evident <strong>in</strong> studiesof form-focuscd <strong>in</strong>struction (e.g. Spada and Lightbown 1993,VanPatten and Cadierno 1993).Where applications to teach<strong>in</strong>g arc conccrned, the confirmatory tradition frcqucntlyentails a particular view of what it means for a teacher to be professionally competent.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this vicw, education is an applied science. Rcsearchcrs do research, discover<strong>in</strong>gthe best ways to achieve predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed educational goals. These are then passed on toteachers, who function as tcchnicians carry<strong>in</strong>g out the researchers’ prescriptions. Thispresupposes a means-to-end view of education (Tyler 1949), where the curriculum is viewedas a delivery system, with research provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation about the most effective meansfor deliver<strong>in</strong>g the curriculum. Research, therefore, is concerned with means rather thanends, which are taken as given.This view of thc rclationship between research and educationis evident <strong>in</strong> thc op<strong>in</strong>ions of Johnston (1 987) and, <strong>in</strong> part, of Long (1 983a, 1990) discussed<strong>in</strong> the prcvious section.There arc many problems with the applied science view of thc rclationship betweenresearch and practice. As we have already notcd, the <strong>in</strong>formation provided by even the bestdesigned cxperirnental study may not be applicable to other tcach<strong>in</strong>g contexts. Also, it isdoubtful whether the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from experimental research has the objectivestatus often claimed for it, as subjcctivc and social factors play a crucial role <strong>in</strong> the productionof any k<strong>in</strong>d of knowledge, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g that obta<strong>in</strong>ed experimentally (see Kuhn 1970). As Carrand Kemmis (1986) po<strong>in</strong>t out, the separation of ends (or values) and means is not reallypossible. Also, ends should not bc takcn as given but should thcmsclves be the subject ofcritical scrut<strong>in</strong>y, as protagonists of critical pedagogy have argued (SCC Pcnnycook 1989). Agood example of the need to consider ends as well as means can be found <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigationsof teachers’ questions. A number of L2 studies have <strong>in</strong>vestigated the effect of display andreferential questions on learner output (e.g. Brock 1986). In these studies it is assumed thatteachers will and should ask questions and the only issue is what k<strong>in</strong>d of questions work best


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 55for language learn<strong>in</strong>g. One might legitimately challenge this assumption however. It has beensuggested that classroom L2 acquisition is likely to proceed most smoothly if learners enjoythe same participant rights as their teacher (Pica 1987). Howcvcr, teachers’ questions, <strong>in</strong> anyform, imply an asymmetrical power structure <strong>in</strong> the classroom and, therefore, may not bethe most effective way of creat<strong>in</strong>g conditions conducive to language learn<strong>in</strong>g. A more seriousproblem is that the applicd science view of teach<strong>in</strong>g allocates particular roles to researchersand to teachers, which are necessarily social and value ladcn <strong>in</strong> nature. Researchers are theproducers of knowlcdge while teachers are consumers; researchers are experts whereasteachers are mere technicians. The applied science view, therefore, implies a hicrarchicalrelationship between researchers and teachers (hence the term ‘top-down’), mirror<strong>in</strong>g thck<strong>in</strong>d of division which exists between teachers and students <strong>in</strong> traditional classrooms and,arguably, re<strong>in</strong>forc<strong>in</strong>g it.At first glance, the <strong>in</strong>terpretative tradition of research avoids many of these problems.By adher<strong>in</strong>g to what van Lier (1 990) calls the emic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple (i.e. try to understand how asocial context works through the perspectives of the participants) and thc holistic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple(i.c. try to understand someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>its</strong> natural surround<strong>in</strong>gs), it may makeapplication to differcnt contexts less problematic, if only because teachers will be able to seeclearly whether their own teach<strong>in</strong>g contexts are the same as, or different from, the contextsstudied <strong>in</strong> the research. Also <strong>in</strong>tcrpretative research does not claim to provide objectiveknowledge. Indeed, it makes a virtue of seek<strong>in</strong>g out subjective knowledgc.’Thus, even though<strong>in</strong>terpretative rcscarch may have theory construction as <strong>its</strong> ultimate goal, it can be consideredpractical <strong>in</strong> nature. Carr and Kemmis (1 986) expla<strong>in</strong> how <strong>in</strong>terpretative accounts facilitatedialogue between <strong>in</strong>terested parties (i.e. researchers and teachers).They can lead to changes<strong>in</strong> the way actors comprehend themselves and thcir situations; ‘practices arc understood bychang<strong>in</strong>g the ways <strong>in</strong> which they are understood’ ( hd.: 91). In fact, <strong>in</strong>terpretative researchachieves validity whcn it passes the test of participant confirmation.Thus, the beliefs, values,and perceptions of teachers are not ignorcd (or controlled) as <strong>in</strong> educational research <strong>in</strong> theconfirmatory tradition, but are given a constitutive place <strong>in</strong> the research. Thc traffic of ideasbetwecn researcher and teacher is, potentially at least, two way.Aga<strong>in</strong>, though, there arc problems. One is that because <strong>in</strong>terpretative rescarch<strong>in</strong>sists on explanations that are consistent with the participants’ own pcrceptions it runs therisk of accept<strong>in</strong>g accounts that are illusory. Obviously, actors can be mistaken, so their<strong>in</strong>terpretations of events need to be exam<strong>in</strong>ed critically. In other words, adherence to theemic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple can lead to faulty understand<strong>in</strong>gs. The holistic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple is also problematic.It can result <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation that is too rich, so detailed that the wood cannot be seen for thetrees. The major problem, however, as with confirmatory research, lies <strong>in</strong> the relationshipbetween the researcher and the teacher. For, although the gap has been narrowed, they still<strong>in</strong>habit different worlds. Carr and Kemmis (1 986: 99) put it this way:Despite thcir differences . . . both the ‘<strong>in</strong>terpretative’ and the positivist [;.e. confirmatory]approach convey a similar understand<strong>in</strong>g of cducational researchers and ofthe relationship to the research act. In both approachcs, the researcher stands outsidethc researched situation adopt<strong>in</strong>g a dis<strong>in</strong>terested stance <strong>in</strong> which any explicit concernwith critically evaluat<strong>in</strong>g and chang<strong>in</strong>g the educational realities be<strong>in</strong>g analysed isrejected.The truth of this is evident <strong>in</strong> what is perhaps the best piece of <strong>in</strong>terpretative research <strong>in</strong> SLAto date ~ van Lier’s (1988) study of aspects of classroom discourse (i.e. turn-tak<strong>in</strong>g, topicand activity, and rcpair work). Although van Lier offers a few comments on how teachers


56 ROD ELLISmight profitably engage <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretative research themselves (ibid. : 230 onwards), the bulkof his book is written from the standpo<strong>in</strong>t of the researcher function<strong>in</strong>g as a gatherer ofknowledge and concerned with truth rather than from the standpo<strong>in</strong>t of the practitionerconcerned with action.Researchers, then, follow agendas that arc set by the requirements of the researchtraditions to which they adhere. They also have their own social agendas. As members ofuniversity departments, researchers are expected to be producers of research and arerewarded accord<strong>in</strong>g to the quantity and quality of the research they produce.To publish theymust satisfy their peers (i.e. other researchers), who function as reviewers for the journals<strong>in</strong> which they seck to be published.Their research must demonstrate that it meets establishedcriteria of reliability and validity (i.e. that it is well designed and that the results warrant theconclusions made). Researchers are not obliged to make their research accessible to teachersor to demonstrate that it is relevant to them. Still less are they required to work with teachersto f<strong>in</strong>d ways <strong>in</strong> which research can be converted <strong>in</strong>to action. Indeed, it may well be that <strong>in</strong>the departments where the researchers work practical research receives less recognitionthan pure research.As we have seen, teachers have very different agendas and operate from a differentknowledge base. Whereas researchers are concerned <strong>in</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>g the truth, teachers are<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out what works.Tcachcrs select tasks that they believe will contributeto their students’ learn<strong>in</strong>g but they are rarely able to <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether their predictionsare borne out. They determ<strong>in</strong>e the succcss of the tasks <strong>in</strong> other ways (e.g. by impressionisticallyevaluat<strong>in</strong>g whether the task stimulates active participation by the learners).Teachers work from practical knowledge. They use their experience of teach<strong>in</strong>g (andof learn<strong>in</strong>g) <strong>in</strong> classrooms to develop a body of knowledge as habit and custom, as skillknowledge (c.g. how to deal with a student who dom<strong>in</strong>ates classroom discussion), ascommon-sense knowledge about practice, as contextual knowledge (i.c. regard<strong>in</strong>g theparticular class they are teach<strong>in</strong>g) and, over time, as a set of beliefs about how learners learnan L2. Polanyi (1958) refers to this k<strong>in</strong>d of knowledge as personal knowledge. As Schon(1 983) has observed, and as we noted earlier, much of this knowledge is only evident <strong>in</strong> use(i.c. it is revealed <strong>in</strong> actual teach<strong>in</strong>g but the teacher cannot articulate it) although some of itmay become espoused through reflection (i.c. the teacher can provide an explicit accountof it).Given these differences <strong>in</strong> goals and <strong>in</strong> what counts as knowledge, the gap betweenresearch and pedagogy and the gulf between researchers and teachers is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g.Zahorik (1 986), cited <strong>in</strong> Freeman and Richards (1 993), has identified a number of differentways <strong>in</strong> which teach<strong>in</strong>g can he conceptualized. Scientifically based Conceptions emphasizethe development of models of effective classroom practice based on the results of empiricalresearch. This is the k<strong>in</strong>d of conception we are likely to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> researchers. Alternativeconceptions are values-based (i.e. effective practice is that which takes <strong>in</strong>to account theidentity and <strong>in</strong>dividuality of learners) and art--craft (i.e. effective practice is built up graduallythrough experience and reflection). It is these conceptions that we arc more likely to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>teachers. As a consequence, some teachers may feel that research is of little value to them,not just because it is difficult to access (a familiar compla<strong>in</strong>t), but because it does not conformwith their own ideas of what teach<strong>in</strong>g is and, therefore, does not address their concerns.Other teachers, however, may feel that their own Conceptions of teach<strong>in</strong>g lack value andstatus <strong>in</strong> comparison to the scientifically-based Conceptions ofrcscarchcrs. As Bolitho (1 991 :25) notes, ‘teachers often take up extreme positions, often deferr<strong>in</strong>g bl<strong>in</strong>dly to theory orreject<strong>in</strong>g it out of hand as irrelevant to classroom issues’. In either case, the outcome isunsatisfactory’.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 57What then can be done about all this? Clearly, someth<strong>in</strong>g is needed to br<strong>in</strong>g the worldsof the researcher/theorist and the teacher closer together. One way might be to f<strong>in</strong>d waysof familiariz<strong>in</strong>g teachers with the technical knowledge obta<strong>in</strong>ed from research and, also, ofmak<strong>in</strong>g it mean<strong>in</strong>gful to them. Another way is by encourag<strong>in</strong>g teachers to become researchers<strong>in</strong> their own right. We will briefly exam<strong>in</strong>e both of these.An assumption of many educators, is that both pre-service and <strong>in</strong>-service teachereducation courses should provide studcnts with an undcrstand<strong>in</strong>g of a range of academicissues considered relevant to their work as teachers. Teacher preparation and furthereducation programmes, therefore, typically offer courses designed to familiarize teacherswith these basic clemcnts. In the case of programmes for L2 teach<strong>in</strong>g there is a broadconscnsus regard<strong>in</strong>g what these elements consist of: what language is; how it is used <strong>in</strong> speechand writ<strong>in</strong>g; how language reflects the work<strong>in</strong>gs of different social groups; how languagecurricula can be developed, taught, and evaluated and how language is learnt.” One of thegrounds for offer<strong>in</strong>g this k<strong>in</strong>d of education is to develop an awarcncss <strong>in</strong> teachers that thereis no one ‘best’ way to teach a language, but rathcr options from which teachers must select<strong>in</strong> accordance with the particular contexts <strong>in</strong> which thcy work.The need for a foundation <strong>in</strong> these basic elements has been strongly argued by Stern(1 983). In the <strong>in</strong>troduction to his book Fundamental Concepts In <strong>Language</strong>Each<strong>in</strong>g, Stern arguesthe need for guides to help the student teacher ‘pick his way through the mass of accumulated<strong>in</strong>formation, op<strong>in</strong>ion, and conflict<strong>in</strong>g advice; to make sense of the vast literature, and todist<strong>in</strong>guish between solid truth and ephemeral fads or pla<strong>in</strong> mis<strong>in</strong>formation’ (Ibd : 1-2). Hesees such guides as not tell<strong>in</strong>g teachers what to th<strong>in</strong>k but rather help<strong>in</strong>g them to sharpen theirown judgements. He works on the common-sense premise that judgements that are<strong>in</strong>formed, based on sound theoretical foundations, will produce better results than those thatarc not. Stern’s own guide is comprehensive, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g sections deal<strong>in</strong>g with historicalperspectives, concepts of language, concepts of society, concepts of language learn<strong>in</strong>g andconcepts of language teach<strong>in</strong>g. Other guides have focused on specific areas, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g SLA(e.g. Larsen-Freeman and Long 1991, Lightbown and Spada 1993, Ellis 1994).The aim of these guides is to make technical knowledge available to teachers <strong>in</strong> adigestible form. There is still the qucstion of’ how teachers are to <strong>in</strong>tegrate this knowledge<strong>in</strong>to their own practice. As Hirst (1 966: 40) has po<strong>in</strong>ted out:To try to understand the nature and pattern of some practical discourse <strong>in</strong> terms ofthe nature and patterns of some purely theoretical discourse can only result <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> be<strong>in</strong>gradically misconceived.Often enough, teachers <strong>in</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, particularly pre-service, compla<strong>in</strong> about the lack ofrelevance of the foundation courses they have taken to the actual task of teach<strong>in</strong>g (see forexample Schuyler and Sitterley 1995).This has led to the suggestion that teachers shouldbecome more than consumers of theories and research; they should become researchersand theorists <strong>in</strong> their own right.The case for teachers conduct<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms is now wellestablished <strong>in</strong> education, largcly as a product of the pioncer<strong>in</strong>g work of such educators asStenhouse (1975), Elliott and Ebutt (1985) and Kemmis and McTaggert (1981) amongothcrs. More recently, educators of language teachers (e.g. Nunan 1990 and Crookes 1993)have also argued the need for teachers to rcscarch their own classrooms. Onc form of teacherrcscarch that is commonly advocatcd is action research.Action research orig<strong>in</strong>ates <strong>in</strong> the work of Kurt Lew<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> the United Statcs (sccAdelman 1993 for a review of Lew<strong>in</strong>’s work and <strong>its</strong> contribution). Lew<strong>in</strong> was concerned withdecision-mak<strong>in</strong>g centred around changes <strong>in</strong> practice <strong>in</strong> the work place. He was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>


58 ROD ELLISwhat effect <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g workers <strong>in</strong> the decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g process (the research) had on factoryproduction (the action). His approach is exemplified <strong>in</strong> his experiment <strong>in</strong> the Harwoodfactory <strong>in</strong>Virg<strong>in</strong>ia. Lew<strong>in</strong> was able to show that when change was imposed on workers bymanagement, production dropped substantially, that when representatives of the workerswere <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> research<strong>in</strong>g the change, production <strong>in</strong>itially dropped but later recoveredand that when all the workers participated <strong>in</strong> the decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g, production rose markedlyafter only two days. The study demonstrated the practical benef<strong>its</strong> of <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g actors <strong>in</strong>decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g. ” More importantly for Lcw<strong>in</strong>, it demonstrated the need for and theadvantages of democracy <strong>in</strong> the workplace. Lew<strong>in</strong>’s work is of <strong>in</strong>terest because it reflects thetw<strong>in</strong> goals of action research, as it has been applied subsequently to education: action researchis <strong>in</strong>tended both to improve classroom practice and also to serve as a means for emancipat<strong>in</strong>gteachers. It has both an <strong>in</strong>strumental function and a social or ideological function. In thecase of the latter, it may be politically charged and, for that reason, potentially risky.It is customary to identify three k<strong>in</strong>ds of action research. First, there is technical actionresearch, where outside researchers co-opt practitioners <strong>in</strong>to work<strong>in</strong>g on questions derivedfrom theory or previous research. Crookes (1 993) characterizes this k<strong>in</strong>d of action researchas a relatively conservative l<strong>in</strong>e, not<strong>in</strong>g that it is likely to result <strong>in</strong> work published by scholarsfor academic audiences. Such research, he suggests, is approved because it fosters connectionsbetween universities and schools while ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the values and standards of traditionalresearch.Second, there is research undertaken by teachers <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms with a viewto improv<strong>in</strong>g local practices. Carr and Kemmis refer to this k<strong>in</strong>d of research as practicalaction research but Hopk<strong>in</strong>s (1 985) prefers the term tcachcr research. As Long (1 983a: 268)po<strong>in</strong>ts out, the aim of teacher research is not to turn teachers <strong>in</strong>to classroom researchers,but to provide a means by which they can monitor their own practice. It <strong>in</strong>volves a cyclc ofactivities as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 3.1, taken from Carr and Kemmis ( 1 986).The start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t isplann<strong>in</strong>g (i.e. the identification of some problem that needs solv<strong>in</strong>g). This results <strong>in</strong> action(i.e. the teach<strong>in</strong>g of a lesson <strong>in</strong> which the problematic behaviour will arise). Observation ofthe action provides material for reflection, which may then lead to further plann<strong>in</strong>g. Eachstep or moment <strong>in</strong> the cycle looks back to thc previous step and forward to the next step.The cycle serves to l<strong>in</strong>k the past with the future through the processes of reconstruction andconstruction. Furthermore, it l<strong>in</strong>ks discourse (i.e. talk<strong>in</strong>g about the action) with actualpractice (i.e. the action <strong>in</strong> context).Thc start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t of the cycle, plann<strong>in</strong>g, is generally seenas the most problematic. Ideally, teachers should form plans for action based on an analysisof their own experience, but <strong>in</strong> reality they are likely to pick out issues from thc educationalor applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics literature (see, for example, McDonough and McDonough’s (1 990)study of language teachers’ views about research). Carr and Kemmis acknowledge a role foran outside facilitator <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g teachers formulate appropriate plans of action.-Reconstructive ConstructiveDiscourse 4 Reflect 1 Planamong participantsPractice<strong>in</strong> the social contextFigure 3. I ‘Momcnts’ of action rcsearchSource: Carr and Kemmis 1986: 1861 J3 Observe t--- 2 Act


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 59The third type of action research is critical action research - research that is not onlydirected at improv<strong>in</strong>g practice but at emancipat<strong>in</strong>g those that participate <strong>in</strong> it. It is this k<strong>in</strong>dthat most closely reflects Lew<strong>in</strong>’s orig<strong>in</strong>al formulation. Crookes considers it more progressive.Teachersare required not only to understand local problems and identify solutions butto exam<strong>in</strong>e the underly<strong>in</strong>g social causes of problems and what needs to be done about them.Teachers need to become aware that their capacities for reflection (an essential part of theaction research cycle) are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by social factors. They need to recognize that theirunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs of classrooms may be distorted.The mechanism for achiev<strong>in</strong>g this is discourse<strong>in</strong> the sense <strong>in</strong>tended by Habermas (1 979) - free communication among participants whoshare equal discourse rights. In critical action research teachers need to take responsibilityfor carry<strong>in</strong>g out research and for discours<strong>in</strong>g on it. The presence of an outside researcher,while not outlawed, is seen as dangerous because it is likely to underm<strong>in</strong>e the social symmetryneeded to ensure collaborative discourse.Action research, then, bridges the gulfbetween the researcher and the teacher. Crookes(1 993) suggests that it overcomes the limitations of traditional research by ensur<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>its</strong>results are relevant to the needs of teachers; by encourag<strong>in</strong>g and support<strong>in</strong>g teacher reflectionand through this professional development; by encourag<strong>in</strong>g teachers to engage <strong>in</strong> other hndsof research and use the results of such research; and, <strong>in</strong> the case of critical action research,by prompt<strong>in</strong>g teachers to address the unquestioned values embodied <strong>in</strong> educational<strong>in</strong>stitutions. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Carr and Kemmis (1986) action research providcs a basis fordevelop<strong>in</strong>g truly educational theories through theoriz<strong>in</strong>g about practice.Action research is not without <strong>its</strong> critics, however. Hopk<strong>in</strong>s (1 985) argues that the actionresearch practised <strong>in</strong> education has departed from Lew<strong>in</strong>’s orig<strong>in</strong>al concept of externally<strong>in</strong>itiated <strong>in</strong>tervention for assist<strong>in</strong>g a client system. He also suggests that the models of actionresearch such as that shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 3.1 may strait-jacket teachers mak<strong>in</strong>g them reluctantto engage <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent action. These criticisms, however, do not seem to be especiallydamag<strong>in</strong>g as there is no reason why educationalists should adhere to Lew<strong>in</strong>’s <strong>in</strong>itial conceptionof action research nor is there any reason why teachers should not depart from the proposedcycle whenever they feel the nccd to do so. More serious are criticisms concern<strong>in</strong>g theimpracticality of ask<strong>in</strong>g teachers to engage <strong>in</strong> research and the quality of the research theyproduce.Teachers do not always f<strong>in</strong>d it easy to undertake research. Nunan (1990), draw<strong>in</strong>g onhis experience of work<strong>in</strong>g with teachers <strong>in</strong> Australia, lists a number of difficulties theyexperienced. Because the teachers were not used to observ<strong>in</strong>g each other teach, they foundcollaboration difficult.They tended towards excessive self-criticism when they first engaged<strong>in</strong> analys<strong>in</strong>g their own classrooms.Their proposals tended to be rather grand and unmanageable<strong>in</strong> nature because they did not f<strong>in</strong>d it easy to identify specific research questions, aproblem often commented on <strong>in</strong> the literature (see, for examplc, Hopk<strong>in</strong>s 1985, McDonoughand McDonough 1990). It proved extremely time-consum<strong>in</strong>g to design properly formulatedprojects.The teachers were unclear as to how the research should be reported because theywere uncerta<strong>in</strong> who their audience was. F<strong>in</strong>ally, there was a host of problems to do with therange and scope of the research. Over time, of course, such problems can be overcome asteachers accumulate experience of how to do research, but <strong>in</strong>itially the task they face canappear daunt<strong>in</strong>g.Another objection to action research concerns doubts about the quality of researchcarried out by teachers. Brumfit and Mitchell (1 990a: 9) argue that ‘there is no goodargument for action research produc<strong>in</strong>g less care and rigour (than other modes of research)unless it is less concerned with clear understand<strong>in</strong>g, which it is not’. Implicit <strong>in</strong> this statementis a belief that many teachers will not be able to acheve the standards professional researchers


theby60 ROD ELLISdeem necessary. Crookes (1 993), however, argues that when research is entirely local andno attempt to generalize is made it is less necessary to conform to the requirements ofreliability, validity, and trustworth<strong>in</strong>ess. He also suggests that action research reports do notneed to be academic <strong>in</strong> style.They can take the form of ‘teacher-oriented reports’ and thusbe more discursive, subjective, and anecdotal. The difference between the positions ofBrumfit and Mitchell and Crookes are <strong>in</strong>dicative of the lack of clear criteria for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gwhat constitutes good quality action research.From the educational perspective described above, the gap between the researcher/theorist on the one hand and the practitioner on the other is seen as the <strong>in</strong>evitable productof the social (and, one might add, political) worlds which they <strong>in</strong>habit. As Kramsch (1995)has po<strong>in</strong>ted out the behaviours that these two social groups typically manifest are symbolicof the value systems to which they adhere. The move to <strong>in</strong>volve teachers <strong>in</strong> research canbe seen, <strong>in</strong> part, as a move to reshape the symbolic capital of teachers’ behaviour by <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>git with the authority to be derived from research.This is one reason why the rationalefor action research so frequently makes reference to <strong>its</strong> contribution to professionalism <strong>in</strong>the teach<strong>in</strong>g fraternity.One way of view<strong>in</strong>g action research is as a means by which teachers can test ‘provisionalspecifications’ (Stenhouse 1975) <strong>in</strong> the context of their own classrooms.These specificationscan be drawn from the teacher’s own practical knowledge, <strong>in</strong> which case action research canhelp to make explicit the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, assumptions, and procedures for action that comprisethis k<strong>in</strong>d of knowledge. Alternatively, the specifications can be drawn from the technicalknowledge provided by research. Action research serves as an empirical test of whether thegeneralizations provided by confirmatory research or the understand<strong>in</strong>gs provided by <strong>in</strong>terpretativeresearch are applicable to specific classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gs. When teachers consistentlyf<strong>in</strong>d the results of their own research do not support the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of confirmatory or<strong>in</strong>terpretative rcscarch they need to be prepared to reject these as <strong>in</strong>applicable to theirown contexts. Action research, then, functions as a way of implement<strong>in</strong>g the third of Weiss’models of research use ~ <strong>in</strong>teractive model ~ bridg<strong>in</strong>g the gap between technicalknowledge and practical knowledge.The question arises as to whether the applicability of proposals based on research mustnecessarily be submitted to an empirical test by requir<strong>in</strong>g teachers to take on the role ofresearcher (as Stenhouse advocates) or whether it might be possible to predict which proposalsare likely to be acted on through an exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the proposals themselves. It seemsreasonable to suppose that some proposals are <strong>in</strong>herently more practical than others. Whatmakes them so?To address this question we turn to the study of the uptake of <strong>in</strong>novations.Innovationist perspectiveA number of applied l<strong>in</strong>guists have recently turned to work on <strong>in</strong>novation to help themunderstand the variable success they have observed <strong>in</strong> both large-scale language projects <strong>in</strong>the develop<strong>in</strong>g world and the variable response to new ideas among teachers <strong>in</strong> the developedworld. Kennedy (1 988), White (1 988 and 1993) and Markec (1 993) have all drawn on<strong>in</strong>novation research <strong>in</strong> a variety of discipl<strong>in</strong>es (e.g. Rogers (1 983) <strong>in</strong> sociology, Lambrightand Flynn (1 980) <strong>in</strong> urban plann<strong>in</strong>g, Cooper (1 989) <strong>in</strong> language plann<strong>in</strong>g and Fullan (1982)and (1 993) <strong>in</strong> education). Henrichsen (1 989), Bcretta (1 990), Stoller (1 994) and Markee(1 994b) have reported actual studies of <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g. It should be noted,however, that to date there has been no study of <strong>in</strong>novations stemm<strong>in</strong>g from proposals basedon SLA.‘~


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 61Innovation can be conceived of <strong>in</strong> two different ways ~ a dist<strong>in</strong>ction that is importantwhere SLA is concerned. First, we can talk about absolute <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> the sense that aproposal represents a completely new idea, not previously evident <strong>in</strong> practice anywhere.There are probably very few <strong>in</strong>stances of absolute <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g, although,arguably Wilk<strong>in</strong>s’ ( 1976) proposal for construct<strong>in</strong>g syllabuses around notions constitutedsuch an <strong>in</strong>novation. Second, there are perceived <strong>in</strong>novations.That is, the change is perceivedas <strong>in</strong>novatory by the practitioners who adopt it. Most <strong>in</strong>novations arc probably of thisk<strong>in</strong>d and, <strong>in</strong>deed, most def<strong>in</strong>itions of <strong>in</strong>novation make particular reference to adopters’perceptions.As Lightbown (1985) has observed, SLA has not produced much <strong>in</strong> the way of newpedagogic proposals.Thus, proposals derived from SLA typically lead to perccivcd rather thanabsolute <strong>in</strong>novations. For example, Krashen andTerrell(l983) view their Natural Approachas a re<strong>in</strong>stitution of the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and techniques of earlier methods rather than as orig<strong>in</strong>al.However, they clearly believe that their proposals will be new to many practitioners. SLAmay also serve to provide a rationale for <strong>in</strong>novations that have orig<strong>in</strong>ated elsewhere. Forexample, the idea of the <strong>in</strong>formation-gap task (Johnson 1982) orig<strong>in</strong>ated from a theory ofcommunicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g, but it has undoubtedly received support and, arguably,been ref<strong>in</strong>ed through SLA research (see, for example, Long 1981 and Skehan 1996).Innovation is <strong>in</strong>herently threaten<strong>in</strong>g, as Prabhu (1987: 105) has po<strong>in</strong>ted out <strong>in</strong> thecontext of discuss<strong>in</strong>g his proposal for a procedural syllabus <strong>in</strong> India:A new perception <strong>in</strong> pedagogy, imply<strong>in</strong>g a different pattern of classroom activity, is an<strong>in</strong>truder <strong>in</strong>to teachers’ mental framcs ~ an unsettl<strong>in</strong>g one, because there is a conflictof mismatch between old and new perceptions and, more seriously, a threat toprevail<strong>in</strong>g rout<strong>in</strong>es and to the sense of security dependent on them.What then determ<strong>in</strong>es whether and to what extent teachers cope with these threats?Theanswer to this question <strong>in</strong>volves a consideration of four sets of factors:the sociocultural context of the <strong>in</strong>novationthe personality and skills of <strong>in</strong>dividual teachersthe method of implementationattributes ofthe proposals themselves.First, as Kennedy (1 988) notes, there is a hierarchy of <strong>in</strong>terrelat<strong>in</strong>g sub-systems <strong>in</strong> which any<strong>in</strong>novation has to operate.Thus, the success of any proposal emanat<strong>in</strong>g from SLA (or any othersource) regard<strong>in</strong>g classroom practices may be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>in</strong>stitutional, educational,adm<strong>in</strong>istrative, political, or cultural factors. Kennedy comments: ‘the cultural system isassumed to be the most powerful as it will <strong>in</strong>fluence both political and adm<strong>in</strong>istrativestructures and behaviour’ (zbzd. : 332).This is apo<strong>in</strong>t thatWiddowson (1 993) also emphasizes.He cites an unpublished paper by Scollon and Scollon to the effect that ‘conversationalmethods’ may fail to take root <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a because they may appear <strong>in</strong>compatible with theConfucian emphasis on benevolence and respect between teacher and students.Second, the success of an <strong>in</strong>novation will also depend on the personality and qualitiesof <strong>in</strong>dividual teachers. Some teachers (e.g. those who are well-educated and upwardlymobile) may be more <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to adopt new practices than others. Rogcrs (1 983) dist<strong>in</strong>guishesfive categories of adopters: <strong>in</strong>novators, early adopters, early majority adopters, latemajority adopters, and laggards. Personal factors arc likcly to play a major part <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gwhich category a teacher belongs to.


62 ROD ELLISThird, the method of implementation is likely to <strong>in</strong>fluence to what extent an <strong>in</strong>novationtakes root. Havelock (1 971) dist<strong>in</strong>guishes three basic models of <strong>in</strong>novation. The research,development and diffusion modcl views the researcher as the orig<strong>in</strong>ator of proposals and theteachers as consumers and implementors of them. It is likely to be used <strong>in</strong> conjunction witha power-coercive strategy, whcre some authority takes a decision to adopt an <strong>in</strong>novation(e.g. a new syllabus) and then devises ways of provid<strong>in</strong>g teachers with the knowledge andskills they need to implement it. Innovation <strong>in</strong> this model, then, takes place top-down.Tbeproblem-solv<strong>in</strong>g model <strong>in</strong>volves engag<strong>in</strong>g teachers <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g problems, research<strong>in</strong>gpossible solutions and then try<strong>in</strong>g them out <strong>in</strong> their teach<strong>in</strong>g. Innovation <strong>in</strong> this model,then, orig<strong>in</strong>ates with the teachers. A social <strong>in</strong>teraction model emphasizes the importance ofsocial relationships <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g adoption and emphasizes the role of communication <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g uptake of an <strong>in</strong>novatory idea.To a large extent, these three models parallel thethree approaches to relat<strong>in</strong>g research and pedagogy discussed <strong>in</strong> the previous section. Thatis, the research development and diffusion modcl reflects the positivist, technical view; theproblem-solv<strong>in</strong>g model reflects the call for teacher rcscarch, while the importance placedon communication <strong>in</strong> the social <strong>in</strong>teraction model mirrors that placed on discourse <strong>in</strong> criticalaction rcsearch.The fourth set of factors govern<strong>in</strong>g thc uptake of <strong>in</strong>novatory proposals concerns theattributes of the proposals themselves. These arc of particular <strong>in</strong>terest to us because theymay provide the applied l<strong>in</strong>guist with a basis for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g proposals emanat<strong>in</strong>g from SLA.The pr<strong>in</strong>cipal attributes discussed <strong>in</strong> the literature (see Kelly 1980, Rogers 1983, and Stoller1994) are listed <strong>in</strong> Table 3.1, together with brief def<strong>in</strong>itions. Some of these attributes areTable 3. IAttributes of <strong>in</strong>novationAttributeInitial dissatisfactionFeasibilityAcceptabilityRelevanceComplexityExplicitnmEiabilityObservahili 9Orig<strong>in</strong>aliiy0wner.shipDef<strong>in</strong>itionThe level of dissatisfaction that teachers experience with some aspectof their exist<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g.The extent to which the <strong>in</strong>novation is seen as implcmentable giventhc conditions <strong>in</strong> which teachers work.The extent to which the <strong>in</strong>novation is seen as compatible with teachers’exist<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g style and ideology.The extent to which the <strong>in</strong>novation is viewed as match<strong>in</strong>g the needs ofthe teachers’ students.Thc extent to which the <strong>in</strong>novation is difficult or easy to grasp.The extent to which the rationale for the <strong>in</strong>novation is clear andconv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g.The extent to which the <strong>in</strong>novation can be easily tricd out <strong>in</strong> stages.The cxtcnt to which the results of <strong>in</strong>novation are visible to others.The cxtcnt to which the teachers arc required to demonstrate a highlevel of orig<strong>in</strong>ality <strong>in</strong> order to implement thc <strong>in</strong>novation (e.g. byprepar<strong>in</strong>g special materials).The extent to which teachers comc to feel that they ‘possess’ the<strong>in</strong>novation.


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 63seen as <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the likelihood of an <strong>in</strong>novation becom<strong>in</strong>g adopted (e.g. feasibility, relevanceand explicitness). That is why they are to be viewed positively. Other attributes are likelyto <strong>in</strong>hibit <strong>in</strong>novation (e.g. complexity). Still others may promote or <strong>in</strong>hibit <strong>in</strong>novationdepend<strong>in</strong>g upon the particular adopters. For example, <strong>in</strong> the case of orig<strong>in</strong>ality, some teachersmay be more likely to implement an <strong>in</strong>novation if it calls for their own orig<strong>in</strong>al contribution(e.g. <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g new teach<strong>in</strong>g materials) whereas others may be less likely to do so. Theattributes also vary <strong>in</strong> another way. Some (e.g. <strong>in</strong>itial dissatisfaction and relevance) seem tobe more relative than absolute <strong>in</strong> the sense that their application depends on the particularcontext <strong>in</strong> which teachers are work<strong>in</strong>g, whereas others (i .e. complexity, explicitness,triability, and observability) seem more concerned with the <strong>in</strong>herent characteristics of the<strong>in</strong>novation. Applied l<strong>in</strong>guists <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g proposals drawn from SLA are likelyto benefit from pay<strong>in</strong>g close attention to the <strong>in</strong>herent rather than the relative attributes ofproposals.In addition to these sets of factors that <strong>in</strong>flucnce the uptake of <strong>in</strong>novatory ideas, there isalso the question of what aspects of language pedagogy are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the change.This, too,can <strong>in</strong>fluence the likelihood of thc <strong>in</strong>novation be<strong>in</strong>g successful. Markee (1 994b), draw<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>particular on the work of Fullan (1982 and 1993) <strong>in</strong> education, suggests that <strong>in</strong>novations <strong>in</strong>the form of the development and use of new teach<strong>in</strong>g materials constitute the easiest k<strong>in</strong>dof change. Innovations requir<strong>in</strong>g change <strong>in</strong> methodological practices and, even more so <strong>in</strong>the teachers’ underly<strong>in</strong>g pedagogical values, are less likely to prove successful.There have been relatively few attempts to apply an <strong>in</strong>novationist perspective to languagepedagogy. Beretta (1 990) sought to evaluate the extent to which the methodological<strong>in</strong>novations proposed by Prabhu as part of the CommunicationalTcach<strong>in</strong>g Project (CTP) <strong>in</strong>India (Prabhu 1987) were actually implemented by the teachers <strong>in</strong>volved. This project isbased on the assumption that learners acquire grammar subconsciously when their attentionis focused on communicat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g-focused tasks. Although Prabhu did not drawdirectly on SLA rcsearch/theory, his proposal is very similar to that advanced by Krashenand for this reason is of considerable <strong>in</strong>terest here. Bcretta collected historical narratives from15 teachers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the project and then ratcd these accord<strong>in</strong>g to three levels of implementation:1 orientation (i.e. the teacher demonstrates he/she does not really understand the<strong>in</strong>novation and is unable to implement it)2 rout<strong>in</strong>e (i.e. the teacher understands the rationale of the CTP and is able to implementit <strong>in</strong> a relatively stable fashion), and3 renewal (i.e. the teacher has adopted a critical perspective on the <strong>in</strong>novation,demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g awareness of <strong>its</strong> strengths and weaknesses).Forty per cent of the teachers were rated at Level 1,47 per cent of teachers at Level 2and 1 3 per cent at Level 3. Beretta considered Levels 2 and 3 demonstrated an adequate levelof adoption. However, when he dist<strong>in</strong>guished between regular and non-regular classroomteachers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the project, he found that three out of four of the regular teachers wereat Level 1. He concluded that:. . . it seems reasonable to <strong>in</strong>fer that CTP would not be readily assimilable by typicalteachers <strong>in</strong> South Indian schools (or, by extension, <strong>in</strong> other schools elsewhere wheresimilar antecedent conditions perta<strong>in</strong>) (ibid. : 333).He po<strong>in</strong>ts out that the failure of the regular teachers to reach an acceptable level ofimplementation reflects their lack of ownership of the <strong>in</strong>novation and problems regard<strong>in</strong>g


64 ROD ELLISthe <strong>in</strong>novation’s feasibility because, for example, the teachers lacked the commandof <strong>English</strong> required for fluency-based teach<strong>in</strong>g.There are problems with Beretta’s study ~ forexample, we cannot be sure whether the regular teachers really failed to adopt the <strong>in</strong>novationor whether they simply lacked the <strong>English</strong> needed to produce narrative accounts of theirexperience ~ but, nevertheless, it demonstrates the potential of an <strong>in</strong>novationist perspectivefor evaluat<strong>in</strong>g pedagogic proposals derived from SLA theory and research.Probably the most comprehensive study of <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> language pedagogy is to befound <strong>in</strong> Stoller’s (1994) study of <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>English</strong> language programmes<strong>in</strong> the United States. Stoller obta<strong>in</strong>ed completed questionnaires from 43 such programmesand also conducted <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews with five programme adm<strong>in</strong>istrators. She foundthat the most frequently cited <strong>in</strong>novations related to the development of new curricula orthe restructur<strong>in</strong>g of the old. Some attributes were perceived as more important than othersfor successful <strong>in</strong>novation. Attributes rated as particularly important were usefulness(relevance), feasibility, improvement over past practices (which would seem to relate to<strong>in</strong>itial dissatisfaction) and practicality (which relates to acceptability). Stoller was able toidentify three major factors <strong>in</strong> the questionnaire responses. One factor was what she termeda ‘balanced divergent factor’. The attributes <strong>in</strong>volved here were explicitness, complexity,compatibility with past experiences, visibility, flexibility, and orig<strong>in</strong>ality. In the case of thisfactor, however, the attributes operated <strong>in</strong> a zone of <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> the sense that they facilitated<strong>in</strong>novation when they were present to a moderate degree but not when they were stronglyor weakly present.The second factor was dissatisfaction and the third factor viability. Stolleralso demonstrates that there appear to be different paths to <strong>in</strong>novation depend<strong>in</strong>g on thenature ofthc <strong>in</strong>novation.Thus, <strong>in</strong> the case of curricular <strong>in</strong>novation, viability was seen as themost important followed by dissatisfaction and f<strong>in</strong>ally the balanced divergent factor. Theemphasis that Stoller places on viability <strong>in</strong> this type of <strong>in</strong>novation reflects the importance thatBeretta attaches to feasibility <strong>in</strong> the communicational teach<strong>in</strong>g project.An <strong>in</strong>novationist perspective, then, would seem to afford applied l<strong>in</strong>guists a way ofevaluat<strong>in</strong>g the extent to which their propods are likely to succeed. It will not be possible,of course, to make very precise predictions about which proposals will be taken up andwhich ones will not, but, arguably, the very act of evaluat<strong>in</strong>g their potential will helpresearchers to make them more practical. One might also add that an <strong>in</strong>novationist analysis,us<strong>in</strong>g the k<strong>in</strong>ds of categories discussed <strong>in</strong> this section, may provide teachers with an explicitand relatively systematic way of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether specific proposals derived from SLAare of use to them.The study of <strong>in</strong>novations, therefore, offers another possible way ofbridg<strong>in</strong>gthe gap between SLA and language pedagogy.Applied l<strong>in</strong>guist’s perspectiveI have def<strong>in</strong>ed an applied l<strong>in</strong>guist as a person who seeks to apply ideas derived from l<strong>in</strong>guistics,psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics, sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics, education, and any other area of potentially relevantenquiry to language pedagogy. It is important to make a clear dist<strong>in</strong>ction between ‘appliedl<strong>in</strong>guistics’ and ‘l<strong>in</strong>guistics applied’. One obvious reason is that applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics utiliyes<strong>in</strong>formation sources other than l<strong>in</strong>guistics, as the above def<strong>in</strong>ition makes clear. There is,however, a deeper reason. Widdowson (1 984) argues that ‘it is the responsibility of appliedl<strong>in</strong>guists to consider the criteria for an educationally relevant approach to language’ (7bid. :17) and that this cannot be achieved by simply apply<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic thcory.This is because theway l<strong>in</strong>guists conceive of their task is <strong>in</strong>herently different from the way teachers conceiveof theirs. L<strong>in</strong>guists are concerned with the precise description of language and with <strong>its</strong>


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 65explanation. Teachers are concerned with the effective use of language and with <strong>its</strong>propagation.Just as we can dist<strong>in</strong>guish between applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics and l<strong>in</strong>guistics applied so we canalso dist<strong>in</strong>guish applied SLA and SLA applied. In the case of the latter, an attempt is made toapply SLA research and theory to language pedagogy.This is what many SLA researchers haveexpressed doubt about do<strong>in</strong>g, advis<strong>in</strong>g caution. In the case of applied SLA, however, anattempt is made to exam<strong>in</strong>e the relevance of SLA <strong>in</strong> educational terms; it requires the SLAresearcher to have knowledge of the theory and practice of both SLA and language pedagogy.Only when SLA researchers engage <strong>in</strong> applied SLA do they function as applied l<strong>in</strong>guists.’3A good example of applied SLA is to be found <strong>in</strong> Br<strong>in</strong>dley’s (1990) account of a coursehe taught as part of a postgraduate diploma <strong>in</strong> adult TESOL. Br<strong>in</strong>dley dismisses whathe sees as the traditional approach of SLA courses which he characterizes as ‘we give you thetheory ~ you apply it’ (the approach implicit <strong>in</strong> Stern’s 1983 advocacy of foundation studies)<strong>in</strong> favour of an approach that provides opportunities for the participants to analyse data.Thisencourages them to reformulate broad SLA research questions <strong>in</strong> terms of classroomimplications and <strong>in</strong>cludes a strong problem-pos<strong>in</strong>g/problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g element by <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g theparticipants to address specific classroom situations <strong>in</strong> the light of <strong>in</strong>sights drawn from theirstudy of SLA and to discuss options for classroom applications. Br<strong>in</strong>dley did <strong>in</strong>clude aknowledge component of the course (i.e. he provided an <strong>in</strong>troduction to key topics andterm<strong>in</strong>ology) but <strong>in</strong> accordance with his applied SLA stance, he <strong>in</strong>vited the participants toidentify those SLA topics they found most relevant to their concerns. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, he foundthat psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic studies of developmental sequences (generally considered of centralimportance by SLA researchers) came bottom of the list, possibly because the teachers’primary concern was with teach<strong>in</strong>g rather than learn<strong>in</strong>g.Applied SLA, then, as a branch of applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics, must necessarily concern <strong>its</strong>elf withrelevance. SLA is concerned with develop<strong>in</strong>g models ofhow L2 learners acquire knowledgeof a second language but it cannot be assumed that these models are of any value to teachers.Indeed, <strong>in</strong> many cases they probably are not. It is no more corrcct to assume that a theoryof language learn<strong>in</strong>g is of relevance to teachers than it is to assume that a theory of languageis. Relevance must necessarily be determ<strong>in</strong>ed not from with<strong>in</strong> SLA but from without ~ bydemonstrat<strong>in</strong>g how the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of SLA address the needs and concerns of practitioners.How then can SLA be made relevant to pedagogy? An answer to this question can befound <strong>in</strong> Widdowson’s (1990) discussion of the roles of the applied l<strong>in</strong>guist (see Figure 3.2).TheoryAPPRAISAL(<strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple)InterpretationConceptualevaluationI(of technique)OperationEmpiricalevaluationFigure 3.2 Relat<strong>in</strong>g discipl<strong>in</strong>ary theory and language pedagogySource: Widdowson 1990: 32


66 ROD ELLISFor Widdowson, the applied l<strong>in</strong>guist’s task is to mediate between discipl<strong>in</strong>ary theory/research and language pedagogy. He suggests that this mediation <strong>in</strong>volves two <strong>in</strong>terdependentprocesses. The first is appraisal, which <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>in</strong>terpretation (i.e. the explication of ideaswith<strong>in</strong> their own terms of reference), followed by conceptual evaluation (i.e. ‘the processof specify<strong>in</strong>g what might be called the transfer value of ideas’ (ibid. : 3 1 )).The second processis application, which also <strong>in</strong>volves two phases. In the case of operation, specific techniques areproposed based on the conclusions of the conceptual evaluation. Alternatively, specifictechniques taken from teachers’ customary practices can be subjected to scrut<strong>in</strong>y, a processthat both draws on the results of prior conceptual evaluation and potentially contributes toit.The result of this process is a rationale for proposed action.The second phase of applicationis whatwiddowson calls ‘empirical evaluation’ .This is undertaken by teachers, possibly withthe assistance of applied l<strong>in</strong>guists, and <strong>in</strong>volves monitor<strong>in</strong>g the effects of their actions byexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the relationship between teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. It calls for teacher research.Widdowson’s framework provides a basis for apply<strong>in</strong>g SLA <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g ways:1 Mak<strong>in</strong>g SLA accessibleThis function <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Because the bulk of SLA publications were written forresearchers and not practitioners, there is an obvious need for summaries of the ma<strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Such summaries will have four major purposes: to make a pr<strong>in</strong>cipled selection ofthose f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that arc likely to be of <strong>in</strong>terest to teachers; to provide surveys of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof a wide range of research which has addressed these issues; to evaluate the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> theirown terms (i.e. to establish which ones are valid, reliable and trustworthy); to present thesurveys <strong>in</strong> a language that makes them accessible to practitioners and which provide themeans by which teachers can receive a foundation <strong>in</strong> SLA.The organization of these summaries bears some thought. One possibility is to structurethem around the issues identified <strong>in</strong> the research. This would lead to surveys of such issuesas learner errors, <strong>in</strong>put and <strong>in</strong>teraction, fossilization, thc role of formal <strong>in</strong>struction, etc. Analternative, however, is to base the surveys on pedagogical concepts. This would lead tosurveys of research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that are relevant to such issues as error treatment, the use of thelearner’s L1 <strong>in</strong> the classroom, and options <strong>in</strong> grammar tcach<strong>in</strong>g.This latter approach is clearlymore demand<strong>in</strong>g but is likely to <strong>in</strong>crease the perceived relevance. It provides a bridgebetween <strong>in</strong>terpretation and conceptual evaluation.2 Theory development and <strong>its</strong> applicationOne way of conduct<strong>in</strong>g conceptual evaluation is through theory construction. As Krashen(1 983) has noted there are dangers <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to apply the results of <strong>in</strong>dividual research studiesand a more pr<strong>in</strong>cipled approach is to use research to construct a theory which can then beapplied. One advantage of such an approach to conceptualization is that it provides anopportunity for develop<strong>in</strong>g a pedagogically relevant theory. As Brumfit (1 983) has noted,teachers necessarily operate with category systems. A theory of <strong>in</strong>structed language acquisitioncan assist them <strong>in</strong> creat<strong>in</strong>g appropriate categories. We noted earlier, however, thatthere are also dangers <strong>in</strong> such an approach. In particular, so much <strong>in</strong>vestment may be made<strong>in</strong> a theory that it becomes petrified, resistant to modification <strong>in</strong> the light of counterarguments and new research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. If this happens, of course conceptual evaluation givesway to persuasion.The application of the hypotheses that comprise a theory is one way of operationalix<strong>in</strong>gSLA for pedagogy.This operationalization takes the form of specific proposals for the practiceof teach<strong>in</strong>g. The proposals may concern overall approaches, the aims of the languagecurriculum, the content and organization of a syllabus, teach<strong>in</strong>g activities, methodological


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 67procedures, and methods of test<strong>in</strong>g learners and evaluat<strong>in</strong>g curricula ~ <strong>in</strong> other words anyaspect of language pedagogy. These proposals, may take the form of orig<strong>in</strong>al ideas, but as Ihave already po<strong>in</strong>ted out, it is more likely that they will identify options already to be foundwith<strong>in</strong> pedagogy. Irrespective of their form, these proposals cannot have the status ofprescriptions. Rather they serve as illum<strong>in</strong>ative ideas. They are suggestions which practitioners,if they see fit, may or may not choose to experiment with.The provisional nature ofproposals is determ<strong>in</strong>ed not by doubts about the validity of the theory /research upon whichthey are based (even though such doubts may well exist) but by the recognition that no theoryand no research can claim to be applicable to the myriad contexts <strong>in</strong> which practitionersoperate. The applied SLA researcher, however, has a duty to ensure that any proposal haspotential for application and, <strong>in</strong> this respect, the attributes of different proposals can beexam<strong>in</strong>ed from the <strong>in</strong>novationist perspective described <strong>in</strong> the previous section. In this way,it may be possible to identify which proposals have a good chance of be<strong>in</strong>g adopted byteachers.3 Research<strong>in</strong>g the L2 classroomAs we have seen, another way of operationaliz<strong>in</strong>g constructs, whether these have been derivedfrom pure research or from teachers’ personal knowledge, is to carry out <strong>in</strong>vestigationsof classroom learners. However, this should <strong>in</strong>volve research on classrooms, not just research<strong>in</strong> classrooms (Wright 1992). Such research provides a means of empirically test<strong>in</strong>gpedagogic proposals (Long 1990). It provides some assurance that the proposals are sound.It helps to ward off attacks that proposals derived from teachers’ own experience or frommethodologists’ writ<strong>in</strong>gs are noth<strong>in</strong>g more than hunches or unproven prescriptions. Furthermore,practitioners are likely to attend to classroom research more seriously than to pureresearch because it directly addresses issues that they are concerned with.Classroom-centred research conducted by researchers, however, does not supply a bodyof <strong>in</strong>formation about effective pedagogy which can be transmitted to teachers as solutionsto their problems any more than does pure research. The most that can be said is thatproposals that are tested through classroom research may become more fully illum<strong>in</strong>ated.In accordance with the views of Stenhouse, Verma, Wild, and Nixon (1 982), the externalvalidity of any research, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g classroom research, can only be established by <strong>in</strong>dividualteachers <strong>in</strong> the contexts of their own classrooms. It follows, then, that what Widdowson(1 990) refers to as ‘outsider research’ needs to be complemented by <strong>in</strong>sider research, whichis research conducted by teachers themselves.4 The teacher as researcherWe saw earlier <strong>in</strong> our discussion of the educational perspective that there is a compell<strong>in</strong>g casefor <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g teachers <strong>in</strong> research<strong>in</strong>g their own classroom. Teacher research focuses onproblems identified by teachers. It provides means of enabl<strong>in</strong>g teachers to reflect on theirown practice and, thereby, of develop<strong>in</strong>g theories of language learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g that arerelevant to their own classroom contexts. The advocacy of teacher research (e.g. Nunan1990) <strong>in</strong> recent years reflects the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g awareness that language teachmg is an educationalenterprise and, thus, needs to be <strong>in</strong>formed by ma<strong>in</strong>stream educational th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Widdowson(1 990) sees the need for teachers to be engaged <strong>in</strong> the active process of experiment<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>their classrooms as a way of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the practical effect of ideas <strong>in</strong> action.There is still a role for SLA <strong>in</strong> teacher-led research, however. AsWiddowson (1 993) haspo<strong>in</strong>ted out, action research, like any other k<strong>in</strong>d of research, cannot take place withouttheoriz<strong>in</strong>g. Teachers need to engage <strong>in</strong> the process of conceptual evaluation <strong>in</strong> order toidentify research problems. A familiarity with SLA, then, can help teachers shape problems


68 ROD ELLIS<strong>in</strong> a way that makes them researchable. In so do<strong>in</strong>g, however, it must not impose issues onteachers but rather act as a resource by which teachers can ref<strong>in</strong>e questions derived from theirown experience. As Widdowson (1 993) puts it, theoriz<strong>in</strong>g must be client-centred.SLA can help <strong>in</strong> another way. It can providc tcachcrs with <strong>in</strong>formation about the k<strong>in</strong>dsof <strong>in</strong>struments and procedures they will necd to usc <strong>in</strong> order to collect and analyse data. Somehrty years of research<strong>in</strong>g L2 acquisition have led to the development of a number of researchtools (see Larsen-Freeman and Long 1991 ,AllWright and Bailey 1991), many of which canbe used by tcachers <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms.As we noted earlier, the idea of the tcachcr as rcsearcher will not always be welcomedby teachers. For some teachers, at least, however, SLA can be made real through thcdiscoveries they make about how their own learners learn a second language.From an applied l<strong>in</strong>guist’s perspective, thcn, SLA is rclcvant to language pedagogy <strong>in</strong>a number of ways. It can contribute to the appraisal of pedagogic issues. To this end, thcapplied SLA worker can assist by mak<strong>in</strong>g rcscarch accessible to teachers, by develop<strong>in</strong>gtheories of <strong>in</strong>structed L2 acquisition and by advanc<strong>in</strong>g pedagogic proposals based on thesetheories. SLA also has a role <strong>in</strong> application.The applied SLA researcher can scck to illum<strong>in</strong>atepedagogic problems and their possible solutions through conduct<strong>in</strong>g experimental and<strong>in</strong>terpretative studics <strong>in</strong> and, particularly on L2 classrooms. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the SLA worker can actas a facilitator of teachers’ own research by help<strong>in</strong>g thcm formulate research questions andchoose appropriate rcscarch methods. These functions can be sccn as strung out on acont<strong>in</strong>uum with‘outsider activity’ at onc pole and ‘<strong>in</strong>sider-activity’ at the other.While it canbe argued that the relevance of SLA <strong>in</strong>crcascs as one moves along the cont<strong>in</strong>uum, outsideractivity should not be disparaged, as has become fashionable <strong>in</strong> some quarters. Teachers canand do benefit from an understand<strong>in</strong>g of the issues discussed <strong>in</strong> SLA. Howcvcr, the determ<strong>in</strong>ationof relevancc is ultimately the duty of the tcachcr, not the applied SLA worker,although the lattcr can aid the process and, doubtlessly, should try to do so.F<strong>in</strong>ally, it must be clearly acknowledged that SLA does not constitutc a body ofknowledge that is necessary for the development of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g skills. As Brumfit(1 983: 61) has observed, ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g to pcrform competently is never the samc as learn<strong>in</strong>g howto understand the process of performancc and to expla<strong>in</strong> it’. SLA can contribute to teachers’undcrstand<strong>in</strong>g; it cannot ensurc competent practice and, to quote Brumfit aga<strong>in</strong>, ‘there isalways the possibility that practicc will run ahead of theory, as wcll as the reverse’ bid. : 68).Notes1 The failure of the comparative method studics to demonstrate the superiority of onemethod over another did not lead to thc abandonment of classroom research based onpedagogical constructs, however. Rather it led to a focus on particular aspects of teach<strong>in</strong>g,such as error trcatmcnt or learner participation. Allwright (1988) describes how the globalmethod studics gave way to the detailed study of classroom processes.2 SLA researchers who began their careers as teachers <strong>in</strong>clude Vivian Cook, Pit Corder,Mike Long, John Schumann, Ela<strong>in</strong>cTarone and myself.3 Precisely what counts as a relcvant ficld of enquiry <strong>in</strong> SLA where language pedagogy isconccrned is, of course, debatable. In Ellis (1999, I argue the case for the irrelevance ofUG-based research and theory. Another area <strong>in</strong> which I have personally been able to f<strong>in</strong>dlittle relevance is language transfer.The competition model (Bates and MacWh<strong>in</strong>ney 1982)has proved productive <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g research but to date has had little to say to teachers.However, this failure to f<strong>in</strong>d rclcvance should not be perceived as a criticism of these areas


SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 6945678910111213of enquiry.The study of language transfer, for example, is obviously of central importancefor understand<strong>in</strong>g L2 acquisition, the goal of SLA.Other factors to do with the relative status of pure research (i.e. research directedexclusively at the creation of technical knowledge) as opposed to applied research (i.e.research directed at address<strong>in</strong>g practical issues) <strong>in</strong> the university sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> whichresearchers typically work may also have contributed to the dim<strong>in</strong>ish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> add<strong>in</strong>gapplication sections to published papers.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the positivist view of the relationship between research and languagepedagogy, research provides technical knowledge which teachers use <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g decisionsabout what and how to teach. Research prescribes and proscribes what teachers shoulddo.Pica (1 994) does not <strong>in</strong>dicate how her teachers arrived at the questions they asked. Onepossibility is that their questions were <strong>in</strong>fluenced, <strong>in</strong> part at least, by their knowledge ofthe SLA literature and their perception of what this literature claims is important andrelevant. It would be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to know what k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions are asked by teacherswho are not familiar with SLA. I am grateful for Jim Lantolf for rais<strong>in</strong>g this po<strong>in</strong>t.It should be notcd that some researchers see a positive disadvantage <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to establishl<strong>in</strong>ks with language pedagogy. Newmeyer and Ste<strong>in</strong>berg (1 988), for example, consider thatone of the reasons for the immaturity of SLA is precisely the felt need to make applications.Sometimes, however, these natural samples of spoken language were supplemented withsamples of elicited language. For example, Cazden, Canc<strong>in</strong>o, Rosansky, and Schumann(1 975) used experimental elicitations by ask<strong>in</strong>g their subjects to imitate or transform amodel utterance.The <strong>in</strong>terpretative tradition of research, wedded to ideas borrowed from critical sociology,has more recently been used to exam<strong>in</strong>e a third type of knowledge -socially constructedknowledge. This post-modern approach has, until recently, not been strongly reflected <strong>in</strong>SLA .Richards (1 991), <strong>in</strong> a survey of 50 MATESOL programmes listed <strong>in</strong> theTESOL directory,found that 29 of them <strong>in</strong>cluded required courses on SLA.There is, of course, a dual application of Lew<strong>in</strong>’s model of action research to teach<strong>in</strong>g. Oneis that researchers <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g classroom practices need to work with teacherswith a similar <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> research<strong>in</strong>g change.The other is that teachers need to work withlearners <strong>in</strong> negotiat<strong>in</strong>g the activities they will engage <strong>in</strong>. The latter application is reflected<strong>in</strong> the idea of a process syllabus (Breen 1984), accord<strong>in</strong>g to which the content, methodology,and methods of evaluation for a language course are established jo<strong>in</strong>tly by teacherand students as the course takes place.To the best of my knowledge, however, proponentsof the process syllabus have not made direct l<strong>in</strong>ks between their ideas and those of Lew<strong>in</strong>.Markee’s (1 994b) study exam<strong>in</strong>ed task-based language teach<strong>in</strong>g, which, as Markee po<strong>in</strong>tsout, has been <strong>in</strong>fluenced by psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic theories of L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g.It should be clear from this that the SLA researcher and the applied l<strong>in</strong>guist can be one andthe same person. Indeed, many SLA researchers (myself <strong>in</strong>cluded) would considcrthemselves applied l<strong>in</strong>guists. It should be equally clear that the two roles need notbe related; there are many SLA researchers who are not applied l<strong>in</strong>guists. There are alsosome SLA researchers with no foundation <strong>in</strong> language pedagogy who engage <strong>in</strong> ‘SLAapplied’.ReferencesAdelman, C. (1 993) ‘Kurt Lew<strong>in</strong> and the orig<strong>in</strong>s of actions research’, EducationalAction Research1 : 7-24.


~ (170 ROD ELLISAgard, P. and Dunkel, H. (1948) An lnvestigation of Second <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Boston, Mass.:G<strong>in</strong>n .Agnew, J. (ed.) (1980) Innovation, Research and Public Policy. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University.Alatis, J. (ed.) (1991) University $Georgetown Round Table on <strong>Language</strong> and L<strong>in</strong>guistics. Wash<strong>in</strong>gtonD.C.: Georgetown University Press.Alatis, J., Stern, H. and Stcvcns, P. (cds) (1983) Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics and the Preparation ofTeachers:Toward a Rationale. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D.C. : Gcorgctown University Press.Allwright, R. (1988) Observation <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Language</strong> Classroom. London: Longman.Allwright, D. and Bailey, K. (1991) Focus on the <strong>Language</strong> C1assroom:An Introduction to ClassroomResearchfor Teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge Univcrsity Press.Bahns, J. (1990) ‘Consultant not <strong>in</strong>itiator: the role of the applied SLA researcher’. ELTjournal44: 110-16.Bates, E. and MacWh<strong>in</strong>ney, B. (1982) ‘Functionalist approaches to grammar’, <strong>in</strong> Wanncr andGlcitman (eds) (1982).Beebe, L. (ed.) (1988) Issues <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition: Multiple Perspectives. New York:Newbury House.Beretta, A. (1990) ‘Implcmcntation of thc Bangalore Project’, Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 11 : 32140.991) ‘Theory construction <strong>in</strong> SLA: complementarity and opposition’, Studies <strong>in</strong> Second<strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 13: 493-5 11.Bolitho, R. (1991) ‘A placc for sccond language acquisitions <strong>in</strong> teacher development and <strong>in</strong>teacher education programmcs’, <strong>in</strong> Sadtono (cd.) (1991).Breen, M. (1984) ‘Process syllabuses for the language classroom’ <strong>in</strong> Brumfit (cd.) (1984b).Brccn, M. (1985) ‘Authenticity <strong>in</strong> the language classroom’, Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 6: 60--70.Breen, M. and Candl<strong>in</strong>, C. (1987) ‘Which matcrials! a consumer’s and designer’s guide’ <strong>in</strong>Shcldon (ed.) (1987).Bright, J. and McGrcgor, G. ( 1970). <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>English</strong> as a Second <strong>Language</strong>. London: Longman.Br<strong>in</strong>dley, G. (1990) ‘Inquiry-based teacher cducation: a casc study’. Paper presented at 24thAnnual TESOL Convention, San Francisco March 6-1 0, 1990.Brock, C. (1986) ‘Thc cffccts of referential questions on ESL classroom discoursc’. TESOLQuarterly 20: 4748.Brooks, N. (1960) <strong>Language</strong> and <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. NcwYork: Harcourt Brace andworld.Brown, H. (ed.) (1975) Papers <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Ann Arbor, Mich.: LanguagcLearn<strong>in</strong>g, University of Michigan.Brown, H.,Yorio, C. and Crymcs, R. (eds) (1977) On TESOL ’77. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D.C.:TESOL.Brumfit, C. (1983) ‘Thc <strong>in</strong>tegration of theory and practice’, <strong>in</strong> Alatis et al. (cds) (1983).~- (cd.) (1984) General <strong>English</strong> Syllabus Design. ELT Documents 118. Oxford: Pergamon.Brumfit, C. and Mitchell, R. (1990a) ‘The language classroom as a focus for research’, <strong>in</strong>Brumfit and Mitchell (cds) (1990b).Brumfit, C. and Mitchell, R. (eds) (1990b) ‘Research <strong>in</strong> thc <strong>Language</strong> Classroom’. EL7Documents 133. London: Modern <strong>English</strong> Publications.Calderhead, J. (ed.) (1988) Teachers’ Prrofessional Learn<strong>in</strong>g. London: Falmer.Carr, W. and Kemmis, S. (1986) Becom<strong>in</strong>g Critical: Education, Knowledge and Action Research.London: Falmer.Caxden, C., Canc<strong>in</strong>o, E., Rosansky, E. and Schumann, J. (1975) Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition <strong>in</strong>Children, Adolescents and Adults. F<strong>in</strong>al Rcport. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D. C. : National Institute ofEducation.Clarke, M. and Handscombc, J. (cds) (1983) On TESOL ’82: Pactf;c Perspectives on <strong>Language</strong> and<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.: TESOL.Cook. G. and Seidlhofer, B. (cds) (1995) Pr<strong>in</strong>ciple and Practice <strong>in</strong> Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics: Studies <strong>in</strong>Honour ofH. G. Widdowson. Oxford: Oxford Univcrsity Prcss.Cooper, R. (I 989) <strong>Language</strong> Plann<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Social</strong> Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.


~ (1977)~ (1980)~ (1SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 71Corder, S. P. (1973) Introduc<strong>in</strong>g Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.‘<strong>Language</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g: a social encounter’, <strong>in</strong> Brown et al. (eds)(1977).‘Second language acquisition research and the teach<strong>in</strong>g of grammar’, BAALNewsletter 10: 1-12.Crookes, G. (1993) ‘Action research and second language teachers: go<strong>in</strong>g beyond teacherresearch’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 14: 1304.Crookes, G. and Gass, S. (eds) (1993) Tasks and <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g: <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g Theory and Practice.Clcvedon, North Somerset: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Dahl, H. (ed.) (1979) Spotlight on Educational Research. Oslo: Oslo university Press.Dulay, H. and Burt, M. (1973) ‘Should we teach children syntax?’ <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 23:245-58.Duskova, L. (1969) ‘On sources of errors <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, LML 7: 11-36.Eckman, F., Highland, D., Lee, P., Mileham, J. and Weber, J. (eds) (1995) Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition Theory and Pedagogy. Mahwa, N. J. : Lawrence Erlbaum.Elliott, J. and Ebutt, D. (1985) Facilitat<strong>in</strong>g Educational Action Research <strong>in</strong> Schools. New York:Longman.Ellis, R. (1994) The Study $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.995) ‘Apprais<strong>in</strong>g second language acquisition theory <strong>in</strong> relation to pedagogy’ , <strong>in</strong> Cookand Seidlhofer (eds) (1995).Eraut, M. (1994) Develop<strong>in</strong>g Prrofessional Knowledge and Competence. London: Falmer.Fischer, R. (1935) The Design $Experiments. Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh: Oliver and Boyd.Flowerdew, J., Brooks, M. and Hsia, S. (eds) (1992) Perspectives on Second <strong>Language</strong> TeacherEducation. Hong Kong: Hong Kong City Polytechnic.Flynn, S. and Martohardjono, G. (1995) ‘Towards theory-driven language pedagogy’, <strong>in</strong>Eckman et al. (eds) (1995).Freeman, D. and Richards, J. (1993) ‘Conceptions of teach<strong>in</strong>g and the education of secondlanguage teachers’ . TESOL Quarterl, 27: 193-2 17.Freidson, E. (1977) Prrofession rof Medic<strong>in</strong>e: A Study of Sociology $Applied Knowledge. New York:Dodd, Mead and Co.Fullan, M. (1982) The Mean<strong>in</strong>g $Educational Change. NewYork: Teachers College Press.Fullan, M. (1993) Change Forces: Prob<strong>in</strong>g the Depths ofEducationa1 Reform. London: Falmer.Galton, M. (ed.) (1980) Curriculum Change. Leicester: Leicester university Press.Gass, S. (1989) ‘<strong>Language</strong> universals and second language acquisition’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 39:497-534.Gass. S. (1995) ‘Learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g: the necessary <strong>in</strong>tersection’, <strong>in</strong> Eckman et al. (eds)(1995).Gass, S. and Schachter, J. (eds) (1994) Issues <strong>in</strong> Construct<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Classroom Research.Hillsdale, N. J. : Lawrence Erlbaum.Habermas, J. (1979) Communication and the Evolution $Society (tr.T. McCarthy). Boston: BeaconPress.Harley, B,Allen, P., Cumm<strong>in</strong>s, J. and Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (eds) (1990) The Development $Second <strong>Language</strong>Pr$ciency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Hatch, E. (1978) ‘Apply with caution’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 2: 12343.Havelock, R. (1971) ‘The utilization of educational research and development’. Britishjournalof Educational Technology 2 : 84-97.Henrichsen, L. (1989) Dflusion oflnnovations <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>:The ELEC EJort <strong>in</strong> japan. NewYork: Greenwood Press.Hirst, P. (1966) ‘Educational theory’ <strong>in</strong>Tibbles (ed.) (1966).Hopk<strong>in</strong>s, D. (1985) A Teacher’s Guide to Classroom Research. Milton Keynes: Open UniversityPress.


72 ROD ELLISJarvis, G. (1983) ‘Pedagogical knowledge for the second language teacher’, <strong>in</strong> Alatis et al. (cds)(1983).Johnson, K. (1982) ‘Five pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>in</strong> a“communicative”exercisc type’, <strong>in</strong> Johnson, K. (1982).Communicative Syllabus Design and Methodology, Oxford: Pcrgamon.Johnston, M. (1987) ‘Understand<strong>in</strong>g learner language’ <strong>in</strong> Numan (ed.) (1987).Kelly, P. (1980) ‘From <strong>in</strong>novation to adaptability’, <strong>in</strong> Galton (ed.) (1980).Kemmis, S. and McTagert, R. (198 1) The Action Research Planner. Victoria, Australia: Deak<strong>in</strong>University Press.Kennedy, G. (1988) ‘Evaluation and the management of change <strong>in</strong> ELT projects’. AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 9: 32942.Kramsch, C. (1995) ‘The politics of applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics’. Plenary lecture, Annual Conference ofthe American Association for Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Long Beach, California.Kramsch, C. and McConnell-G<strong>in</strong>et, S. (cds) (1992) Text and <strong>Context</strong>: Cross-Discipl<strong>in</strong>aryPerspectives on <strong>Language</strong> Study. Lex<strong>in</strong>gton, Mass. : D.C. Heath and Company.Krashen, S. (1983) ‘Second language acquisition theory and the preparation of teachers’, <strong>in</strong>Alatis et al. (eds) (1983).Krashen, S. and Terrell, T. (1983) The Natural Approach: <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition <strong>in</strong> the Classroom.Oxford: Pergamon.Kuhn, T. (1970) The Structure ofScient$c Revolutions (2nd edn.). Chicago: Chicago universityPress.Lambright, W. and Flynn, P. (1980) ‘The role of local bureaucracy-centered coalitions <strong>in</strong>technology transfer to the city’ <strong>in</strong> Agnew (cd.) (1980).Lantolf, J. (1995) ‘Second language acquisition theory’. Plenary address, 1995 Annual BAALConference, Southampton, UK.Larsen-Freeman, D. and Long, M. (199 1) An Introduction to Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research.London: Longman.Lightbown, P. (1985) ‘Great expectation: second language acquisition research and classroomteach<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 6: 173-89.Lightbown, P. and Spada, N. (1993) How <strong>Language</strong>s are Learned. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Long, M. (1981) ‘Input, <strong>in</strong>teraction and second language acquisition’ <strong>in</strong> W<strong>in</strong>itz (cd.) (1981 b)- -- (1983a) ‘Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the second language teacher as classroom researcher’ <strong>in</strong> Alatis et al.(eds) (1983).__ (I 983b) ‘Native speaker/non-native speaker conversation <strong>in</strong> the second languageclassroom’, <strong>in</strong> Clarke and Handscombe (eds) (1983).__- (1990) ‘Second language classroom research and teacher education’, <strong>in</strong> Brumfit andMitchell (eds) (I 990b).__ (1 993) ‘Assessment strategies for SLA theories’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 14: 22549.Markee, N. (1993) ‘The difference of <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Annual Review $AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 13: 229.~43.Markee, N. (1994a) ‘Towards an ethnomethodological rcspcctification of second-languageacquisition studies’, <strong>in</strong> Gass and Schachter (eds) (1994).__ (1994b) ‘Curricular <strong>in</strong>novation: issues and problems’. Applied <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 5: 1-30.McDonough, J. and McDonough, S. (1990) ‘What’s the use of research?’ ELT Journal 44:102-9.Newmark, L. (1966) ‘How not to <strong>in</strong>terfere <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. International Journal $American L<strong>in</strong>guistics 32: 77-87.Newmeyer, F. and Stc<strong>in</strong>berg (1988) ‘The ontogcncsis of the field of second language learn<strong>in</strong>gresearch’ <strong>in</strong> Flynn and O’Neill (eds) (1988).Noffke, S. and Stevenson, R. (eds) (1995) Educational Action Research: Becom<strong>in</strong>g PracticallyCritical. New York: Teachers’ College Press.


~ (1~ (1~ (1SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 73Nunan, D. (ed.) (1987) Apply<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research. National CurriculumResearch Centre, Adelaide, Australia: Adult Migrant Education Program.Nunan, D. (1990) ‘The teacher as researcher’, <strong>in</strong> Brumfit and Mitchell (eds) (1990b).Pennycook, A. (1989) ‘The concept of method, <strong>in</strong>terested knowledge, and the politics oflanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g’. TESOL Quarterly 25: 589-618.Pica, T. (1987) ‘Second language acquisition: social <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> the classroom’. AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 7: 1-25.992) ‘The textual outcomes of native speaker-non-native speaker negotiation: what dothey reveal about second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> Kramsch and McConnell-G<strong>in</strong>et (eds)(1992).994) ‘Qucstions from thc languagc classroom: research perspectives’. TESOL Quarterly28: 49-79.Pica, T. and Doughty, C. (1985) ‘The role of group work <strong>in</strong> classroom second languageacquisition’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 7: 23348.Polanyi, M. (1958) Personal KnowledgcTowards a Post-Critical Philosophy. London: Routledge.Prabhu, N. S. (1987) Second <strong>Language</strong> Pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Ravem, R. (1968) ‘<strong>Language</strong> acquisition <strong>in</strong> a second language environment’. IRAL 6: 165--85.Richards, J. (1991) ‘Content knowledge and <strong>in</strong>structional practice <strong>in</strong> second language teachereducation’, <strong>in</strong> Alatis (ed.) (1991).Rogers, R. (1983) The Dflusion oflnnovations (3rd ed.). London and NewYork: Macmillan andFree Press.Sadtono, E. (ed.) (1991) <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition and the Second/Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Classroom.S<strong>in</strong>gapore: SEAMFO Regional <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Centre.Schachter, J. (1993) ‘Second language acquisition: perception and possibilities’. Second <strong>Language</strong>Research 9: 173-87.Scherer, A. and Wertheimer, M. (1964) A Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic Experiment <strong>in</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>.NewYork: McGraw Hill.Schon, D. (1983) The Reflective Practitioner. NewYork: Basic Books.Schumann, J. (1993) ‘Some problems with falsification: an illustration from SLA research’.Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 14: 295-306.Schuyler, P. and Sitterley, D. (1 995) ‘Pre-service teachcr supervision and reflective practice’, <strong>in</strong>Nolfke and Stevenson (eds) (1995).Skehan, P. (1996) ‘Second language acquisition rcscarch and task-based <strong>in</strong>struction’, <strong>in</strong> Willisand Willis (eds) (1996).Smith, P. (1970) ‘A comparison of the Audiol<strong>in</strong>gual and Cognitivc Approaches to Foreign<strong>Language</strong> Instruction: The Pennsylvania Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Project.’ Philadelphia: Ccntcrfor Curriculum Development.Spada, N. and Lightbown, P. (1993) ‘Instruction and the development of questions <strong>in</strong> L2classrooms’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 15: 205-24.Stcnhousc, L. (1975) An lntroduction to Curriculum Research and Development. London:He<strong>in</strong>emann.979) ‘Us<strong>in</strong>g research means do<strong>in</strong>g research’, <strong>in</strong> Dah1 (ed.) (1 979).Stenhousc, L.,Verma, G., Wild, R. and Nixon, J. (1982) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> about Race Relations. London:Routledge.Stern, H. (1983) Fundamental Concepts of<strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Stoller, F. (1994) ‘The diffusion of <strong>in</strong>novations <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensive ESL programs’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics15: 300-27.Tarone, E., Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. and Fathman, A. (1976) ‘Some limitations to thc classroom applicationsof currcnt sccond language acquisition research’. TESOL Quarterly 10: 19-31.Tibbles, J. (ed.) (1966) The Study $Education. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Tyler, R. (1949) Basic Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples ofcurriculum and Instruction. Chicago: Chicago Univcrsity Prcss.


~ (1~ (1~ (174 ROD ELLISVan Lier, L. (1988) The Classroom and the <strong>Language</strong> Learner. London: Longman.990) ‘Ethnography: bandaid, handwagon, or contraband’ <strong>in</strong> Brumfit and Mitchell (eds)(1990b).VanPatten, B. and Cadierno, T. (1993) ‘Explicit <strong>in</strong>struction and <strong>in</strong>put process<strong>in</strong>g’. Studies <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 15: 22541.Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L. (eds) (1982) <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition: The State $the Art. New York:Cambridge University Press.Weber, M. (1961) The Theory $<strong>Social</strong> and Economic Organization. NewYork:The Free Press.Weiss, C. (ed.) (1977) Us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Social</strong> Research <strong>in</strong> Public Policy-mak<strong>in</strong>g. Lex<strong>in</strong>gton: D.C. Heath.White, R. (1988) The ELT Curriculum: Design, Innovation and Management. Oxford: BasilBlackwell.- (1993) ‘Innovation <strong>in</strong> program development’. Annual Review $ Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 13:244-59.Widdowson, H. (1984) ‘Applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics: the pursuit of relevance’ <strong>in</strong> Widdowson (1984)Explorations <strong>in</strong> Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press.990) Aspects of<strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.993) ‘Innovation <strong>in</strong> teacher development’. Annual Review $ Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 13:260-75.Wilk<strong>in</strong>s, D. (1976) Notional Syllabuses. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Willis, J. and Willis, D. (eds) (1996) Challenge and Change <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford:He<strong>in</strong>emann.W<strong>in</strong>itz, H. (ed.) (1981) ‘Native <strong>Language</strong> and Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition’. Annals ofthe NewYork Academy $Sciences 379.Wright, T. (1992) ‘Classroom research and teacher education: towards a collaborativeapproach’, <strong>in</strong> Flowerdew et al. (eds) (1992).Zahorik, J. (1986) ‘Acquir<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g skills’, journal $Teacher Education 27: 2 1-25.


Chaptev 4Pet e r S It e ha nCOMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTIONSTRATEGIES IN LANGUAGE LEARNINGN AN INFLUENTIAL PAPER WHICH discusses differences between firstI and second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, Bley-Vroman (1989) draws attention to the extent towhich second language (L2) learn<strong>in</strong>g often does not lead to success while first languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g, except <strong>in</strong> unusual cases, does. Faced with such an unsettl<strong>in</strong>g vote of no confidence,it is hardly surpris<strong>in</strong>g that the language teach<strong>in</strong>g profession has explored many alternatives<strong>in</strong> the search to f<strong>in</strong>d more effective methods (Larsen-Freeman 1986). And it is equallyunsurpris<strong>in</strong>g that one of the responses the profession has made is to see whether approachesto second language tcach<strong>in</strong>g which connect withfirst language acquisition hold out anypromise.This chapter will review two such <strong>in</strong>structional approaches. The first is broadlyconcerned with comprehension-driven learn<strong>in</strong>g, regard<strong>in</strong>g second language developmentas likely to proceed, under the right conditions, simply as a result of exposure to mean<strong>in</strong>gful<strong>in</strong>put.The second, which <strong>in</strong> some ways arose out of dissatisfaction with the first, proposesthat engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction and produc<strong>in</strong>g output will be sufficient to drive second languagedevelopment forward. In each case, clearly, <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development is seen to be theby-product of cngag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g-process<strong>in</strong>g ~ <strong>in</strong> the first case through comprchcnsion,and <strong>in</strong> the second through production. As a broader aim, the chapter develops the claim that<strong>in</strong>structional activities that emphasize mean<strong>in</strong>g, whether comprehension or productionbased,may <strong>in</strong>duce learners to rely on strategies for communication which result <strong>in</strong> abypass<strong>in</strong>g of the form of language.The place of comprehension <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>gThe clearest example of a comprehension-based account of second language developmentderives from Krashcn (1985). He proposed that comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put is the driv<strong>in</strong>g forcefor <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development and change, and that the effects of such change carry over to<strong>in</strong>fluence production ~ that is, one learns to speak by listen<strong>in</strong>g, a claim,which is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gbecause of <strong>its</strong> counter-<strong>in</strong>tuitive nature. Krashen argues that thc prcdictability of the contextmakes what is said function as a commentary on what is already understood. The result isthat it is more likely that thc <strong>in</strong>terlanguage system will be extended by the context-tolanguagemapp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volved.


76 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 771 Whenever you f<strong>in</strong>d a determ<strong>in</strong>er (a, an, the) or quantifier (some, all, many, two, six,etc.) beg<strong>in</strong> a new noun phrase.2 Whenever you f<strong>in</strong>d a co-ord<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g conjunction (and, or, but, nor) beg<strong>in</strong> a newconstituent similar to the one you just completed.3 Try to attach each new word to the constituent that came just before.(hid.: 66)They illustrate this last strategy through an advertis<strong>in</strong>g campaign run by a Londoneven<strong>in</strong>g paper with posters such as ‘Zoo keeper f<strong>in</strong>ds Jaguar queu<strong>in</strong>g for underground ticket’,and ‘Butler f<strong>in</strong>ds new station between Piccadilly and Oxford Strcct’.The paper wanted morepeople to realize how useful <strong>its</strong> small advertisements section was and to attract their attentionto posters they would normally glance at only briefly while pass<strong>in</strong>g. So they exploited the‘doublc-take’ that readers were led <strong>in</strong>to by us<strong>in</strong>g the third of the above micro-strategies.Readers then had to recognize the improbability of their first <strong>in</strong>terpretation of ‘queu<strong>in</strong>g’be<strong>in</strong>g attached to ‘Jaguar’ and ‘new station’ to ‘between Piccadilly and Oxford Street’, andmove the l<strong>in</strong>k to the first noun <strong>in</strong> each sentence.Clark arid Clark (~bzd.: 72-79) also discuss semantic strategies, such as:4 Us<strong>in</strong>g content words alone, build propositions that make sense and parse the sentence<strong>in</strong>to constituents accord<strong>in</strong>gly.Fillenbaum (1 97 1 ) illustrates the operation of this strategy by show<strong>in</strong>g that when peoplewere asked to paraphrase ‘perverse’ sentences like ‘John dressed and had a bath’, theynormalized them, with more than half of his subjects even assert<strong>in</strong>g there was ‘not a shredof difference’ between the paraphrase and the orig<strong>in</strong>al.Clark and Clark are, <strong>in</strong> effect, argu<strong>in</strong>g that native-speaker comprehension is probabilistic<strong>in</strong> nature, and does not follow any sort of determ<strong>in</strong>istic model which would rely on anexhaustive pars<strong>in</strong>g of the utterance concerned. Instead, listeners use a variety of means tomaximize the chances that they will be able to recover the <strong>in</strong>tended mean<strong>in</strong>g of what is be<strong>in</strong>gsaid to them.They are not, <strong>in</strong> other words, us<strong>in</strong>g some l<strong>in</strong>guistic model to retrieve mean<strong>in</strong>gcomprehensively and unambiguously. Instead, they cope with the problem of‘ hav<strong>in</strong>g toprocess language <strong>in</strong> real time by employ<strong>in</strong>g a variety of strategies which will probablycomb<strong>in</strong>e to be effective, even though there is no guarantee that this will be the case.Presumably if a comprehension difficulty arises dur<strong>in</strong>g ongo<strong>in</strong>g process<strong>in</strong>g, the listener canshift to a different mode of mcan<strong>in</strong>g extraction, as perhaps <strong>in</strong> the case of the 700 keeper andthe Jaguar (as was <strong>in</strong>tended by the authors of the poster). But this is not done rout<strong>in</strong>ely: theprimary strategy is to achieve effectiveness <strong>in</strong> very fast language process<strong>in</strong>g. Most listeners,<strong>in</strong> their native language, prefer to make a bcst-guess and keep up, rather than be accused ofbe<strong>in</strong>g slow-witted but accurate pedants (although we can all br<strong>in</strong>g to m<strong>in</strong>d some membersof this species).These ‘micro’ issues discussed by Clark and Clark (1 977) can be located with<strong>in</strong> a widermodel of comprehension, which has a more macro perspective.The follow<strong>in</strong>g table is adaptedfrom Anderson and Lynch (I 988: 1 3), who suggest that comprehension (aga<strong>in</strong>, for themoment, native-speaker comprehension) is dependent on three ma<strong>in</strong> sources of knowledge:Schematic knowledgebackground knowledge- factual- sociocultural


~ how~ physical78 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 79From comprehension to productionKrashen’s proposal (198S), that comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put drives forward language developmentand generalizes to speak<strong>in</strong>g was attractive. Claim<strong>in</strong>g that we learn through exposure tomean<strong>in</strong>gful material may not be very startl<strong>in</strong>g ~ we are unlikely to learn from material wedo not understand, after all. But claim<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>terlanguage change arises <strong>in</strong> a receptivemodality and later becomes available to production was by no means self-evident hencethe attraction of the argument.We have seen, though, that the evidcnce reported from evaluations of immersion wassupportive of the orig<strong>in</strong>al claim and so we have to accept that speahng docs not come ‘forfree’ simply through listen<strong>in</strong>g to comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put. In this respect, Long (1985) makesa three-level dist<strong>in</strong>ction between conditions for second language learn<strong>in</strong>g. He suggests thatit is valuable to consider whether factors such as <strong>in</strong>put are:1 necessary2 sufficient3 efficientLogically, an <strong>in</strong>fluence might operate at a level 1, 2, or 3, with 3 efficiency constitut<strong>in</strong>g themost search<strong>in</strong>g criterion, that an <strong>in</strong>fluence is not just causative (necessary and sufficient), butis likely to produce successful language learn<strong>in</strong>g most quickly. At the other extreme, level1, necessary, an <strong>in</strong>fluence would have to be present, but would not be enough, <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>elf toproduce successful learn<strong>in</strong>g (let alone accomplish this rapidly) s<strong>in</strong>ce it would act simply asa precondition. Krashen’s proposal was that <strong>in</strong>put is necessary, sufficient, and efficient, whilethe preced<strong>in</strong>g pages have argued aga<strong>in</strong>st this.Roles for outputSwa<strong>in</strong> (1985; Swa<strong>in</strong> and Lapk<strong>in</strong> 1982), an important contributor of immersion-basedevidence, was led to consider whether other factors besides <strong>in</strong>put might take us further <strong>in</strong>meet<strong>in</strong>g the three levels of condition proposed by Long, and account for how languagedevelopment might be driven forward. In particular, shc proposed the ComprehensibleOutput Hypothesis, that to lcarn to speak we have to actually speak! Draw<strong>in</strong>g on her specificsuggestions (Swa<strong>in</strong> 1985), as well as on other sources, several roles for output can beidentified that are relevant to language learn<strong>in</strong>g.The first two ofthc proposed roles still havea connection with <strong>in</strong>put, but rework this relationship <strong>in</strong> some way.The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g roles foroutput are more specifically targctcd on the productive modality <strong>its</strong>elf.To generate better <strong>in</strong>putParadoxically, one necds to start by draw<strong>in</strong>g attention to the way <strong>in</strong> which one could onlyget good quality <strong>in</strong>put by us<strong>in</strong>g output (speakmg) to give one’s <strong>in</strong>terlocutor feedback, so thatthe <strong>in</strong>put dircctcd to the listener is more f<strong>in</strong>ely tuned to the listener’s current competence(Long 198s). In this view, output is important as a signall<strong>in</strong>g dcvicc to negotiate better <strong>in</strong>put:<strong>in</strong>put would still be the major explanatory construct, but output would be necessary togenerate it most effectively. Simply listen<strong>in</strong>g would not ensure that good quality <strong>in</strong>put wouldbe received, s<strong>in</strong>ce one would have to rely on good luck or the sensitivity of one’s <strong>in</strong>terlocutor,neither of which is very dependable. The strongest form of this account concerns the


80 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 81opportunity to practise speech <strong>in</strong> languages where morphology plays a more prom<strong>in</strong>ent rolemay be all the more important.To develop discourse skillsThe previous arguments for the importance of output have not challenged the view thatlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g is essentially the development of a sentence-based <strong>in</strong>terlanguage system.But it has been claimed (Brown and Yule 1983) that much ELT work focuses excessively on‘short turns’, and that as a result learners’ capacities to take part <strong>in</strong> extended discourse arenot stretched. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly, current developments <strong>in</strong> discourse analysis suggest that there is alot to be learned if one is to become an effective communicator. Discourse management(Bygate 1987), turn-tak<strong>in</strong>g skills, and a range of similar capacities which underlie thenegotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g discourse (Cook 1989), can only be achieved by actuallyparticipat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> discourse. If mean<strong>in</strong>g-mak<strong>in</strong>g is a jo<strong>in</strong>tly collaborative activity, then wecannot read about these skills, or even acquire them passively, but <strong>in</strong>stead have to take part<strong>in</strong> discourse and realize how our resources are put to work to build conversations andnegotiate mean<strong>in</strong>g. Extensive speak<strong>in</strong>g practice is therefore unavoidable.To develop a personal voiceA learner who is completely dependent on what others say, is unlikely to be able to developa personal manner of speak<strong>in</strong>g. Such a learner will be dependent on the sorts of mean<strong>in</strong>gsthat he or she has been exposed to, and will not be able to exert an <strong>in</strong>fluence on conversationaltopics. This implies a strange, passive view of what language is used for, and how personalconcerns are manifested by it. It seems <strong>in</strong>evitable that if one wants to say th<strong>in</strong>gs that areimportant, one must have, dur<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>g, the opportunity to steer conversationsalong routes of <strong>in</strong>terest to the speaker, and to f<strong>in</strong>d ways of express<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual mean<strong>in</strong>gs.A role for output here seems unavoidable.The importance of outputThese six reasons for the importance of output provide yet another argument aga<strong>in</strong>st thesufficiency of a comprehension-based approach. They detail the <strong>in</strong>adequacy of simplylisten<strong>in</strong>g, and show that output too is a necessary condition for successful language learn<strong>in</strong>g.But the next question is to consider whether output, <strong>in</strong> turn, is sufficient and efficient as acondition for language.The six roles for output listed above might suggcst that it is.The first such use, obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gbetter <strong>in</strong>put (see p. 79), will not be pursued here s<strong>in</strong>ce it is only a more sensitive form ofKrashcn’s views. The last two roles, acquir<strong>in</strong>g discourse skills and develop<strong>in</strong>g a personalvoice (see above), are more concerned with the construct of communicative competence.The central roles for output <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development are forc<strong>in</strong>g syntacticprocess<strong>in</strong>g, test<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses, and develop<strong>in</strong>g automaticity. The first two of these centralroles focus on form while the third is more concerned with performance and fluency.The contrast implied here between attention to form and attention to performance,suggests a question which is susceptible to empirical <strong>in</strong>vestigation. We need to devise studieswhich can establish whether actual output favours form or emphasizes fluency at the expenseof form. Although output may generally be a good th<strong>in</strong>g, the roles it serve3 <strong>in</strong> specificsituations may not be so beneficial. It then becomes important to establish, through research,the conditions and constra<strong>in</strong>ts under which output promotes a focus on form.


82 PETER SKEHANIn the literature, two general accounts of the role of communication <strong>in</strong> language developmenthave been proposed: language development through the negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g(Pica 1994, for example); and development through the operation of strategic competence(such as Bialystok 1990). We will exam<strong>in</strong>e each of them <strong>in</strong> turn to assess whether they canclarify whether output and <strong>in</strong>teraction have a positive <strong>in</strong>fluence, and if so, what that <strong>in</strong>fluencemight be.Negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>gAdvocates of the negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g approach (Gass andVaronis 1994 and Pica 1994,for example) suggest that the ongo<strong>in</strong>g identification of difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractive encountersstimulates learners to overcome such difficulties. In so do<strong>in</strong>g, it is hypothesized thatmodifications which are made to speech <strong>in</strong> the service of repair<strong>in</strong>g conversational breakdownhave beneficial sp<strong>in</strong>-off effects on underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terlanguage. Conversation is then seen as theideal supportive mechanism to:12identify areas where <strong>in</strong>terlanguage is limited and needs cxtension;provide scaffold<strong>in</strong>g and feedback at precisely the po<strong>in</strong>t when it will be most usefuls<strong>in</strong>ce the learner will be particularly sensitive to the cues provided to enable newmean<strong>in</strong>gs to be encoded.Conversational moves such as comprehension checks, clarification requests, and thelike will reflect how conversation leads to engagement with an underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terlanguagesystem whcn it is made unusually malleablc.To l<strong>in</strong>k back with the roles for output discussedabove, such negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g provides itleal opportunities for hypotheses to be testedand a syntactic mode of process<strong>in</strong>g to be highlighted.There are, however, problems here. Aston (1 986), for example, has questioned thedesirability of contriv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong>tended to generate extensivc negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g,and whose value is judged accord<strong>in</strong>g to how well this is achieved. He proposes, <strong>in</strong> fact, thatsuch <strong>in</strong>teractions can he irritat<strong>in</strong>g for students, and unrepresentative as far as naturaldiscourse is concerned. The wider issue, essentially, is that it is one th<strong>in</strong>g for successfulnegotiation to take place, but quite another for this to have beneficial consequenccs for<strong>in</strong>terlanguage development. Far from scaffold<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development, negotiationsequences may distract the learners and overload the process<strong>in</strong>g systems they arc us<strong>in</strong>g, withthe result that even when successful scaffolded negotiations occur which produce morecomplex language, these may not have an impact upon underly<strong>in</strong>g change because there isno time to consolidate them.In any case, there is also the possibility that such studies may have over-estimated theempirical importance of negotiation for mean<strong>in</strong>g. Foster (1998) demonstrates that althoughone can, <strong>in</strong>deed, po<strong>in</strong>t to differences between <strong>in</strong>teraction types and participation patterns asfar as negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dices are concerned, global figures disguise the true state ofaffairs. In fact, unusually active students, whatever the task or participation pattern, engage<strong>in</strong> the same amount of negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g ~ nil. As a result, we have to conclude that formost students this aspcct of output does not have a def<strong>in</strong>ite impact on <strong>in</strong>terlanguage changeand development.Strategic competenceThe situation is not particularly different with respect to the operation of strategiccompetence and communication strategies, the other more general framework which mightI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 83provide a rationale for output-led <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development.This literature (Tarone 1981 ;Faerch and Kasper 1983; Bialystok 1990) has exam<strong>in</strong>ed the ways <strong>in</strong> which the strategiesthat learners adopt when faced by communication problems can be described clearly andclassified. Many categorization systems have been proposed, such as Faerch and Kasper’s(1983) dist<strong>in</strong>ction between achievement and avoidance strategies, and Bialystok’s (1990)contrast between l<strong>in</strong>guistic and cognitive factors. One attraction of such systems is that theyaccount for the range of strategies which are used as parsimoniously and yet as comprehensivelyas possible. In addition, it is useful if they can be grounded <strong>in</strong> related fields, as isthe case with Faerch and Kasper’s (1983) appeal to general psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic models.However, a central issue is whether the operation of such strategies of communicationat a particular time to solve particular problems has any implications for <strong>in</strong>terlanguage changeand development over time. ’ One could ask, for example, whether achievement strategies(that is, reta<strong>in</strong> the orig<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>tention of mean<strong>in</strong>g, and use resourccs creatively to solve acommunication problem) are more likely to lead to development than avoidance strategies(that is, do not extend one’s l<strong>in</strong>guistic repertoire, but <strong>in</strong>stcad change the message to becommunicated so that it comes with<strong>in</strong> available resources). Similarly, one could ask whetherthere are different implications from the use of l<strong>in</strong>guistic strategies compared with cognitiveones.A different way of exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g essentially the same po<strong>in</strong>t is to consider the relationshipbetween communication strategies and the Canale and Swa<strong>in</strong> (1980) model of communicativecompetence. This conta<strong>in</strong>s three (Canale and Swa<strong>in</strong> 1980) or four (Canale 1983)competences: l<strong>in</strong>guistic, sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic, discourse, and strategic (discourse be<strong>in</strong>g the addedfourth competence: see the discussion <strong>in</strong> McNamara 1995). L<strong>in</strong>guistic, sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic, anddiscourse competences are, <strong>in</strong> a sense, more basic, s<strong>in</strong>ce they represent areas of coherentcompetence <strong>in</strong> relation to different aspects of communication. Strategic competence, <strong>in</strong> thisformulation, has a less <strong>in</strong>tegrated quality <strong>in</strong> that it is meant to function <strong>in</strong> an improvisatorymanner when problems are encountered because other competences are lack<strong>in</strong>g (seeBachman 1990). Presumably the capacity to negotiatc mean<strong>in</strong>g would be part of a moregeneral strategic competence.A weak <strong>in</strong>terpretation of what is happen<strong>in</strong>g would be that such strategies have no otherfunction than to solve some sort of communicative breakdown <strong>in</strong> order that conversationcan proceed.With this <strong>in</strong>terpretation, all that happens when a problem is encountered is thatsome degree of resourcefulness is drawn on, and the problem <strong>in</strong> question may or may notbe solved. In this view, it is not assumed that there is much trace from thc activity of solv<strong>in</strong>gthe problem <strong>in</strong> question. Although the ‘solution’ may enable further <strong>in</strong>teraction to take place(which is, of course, not a bad th<strong>in</strong>g), <strong>its</strong> details are regarded as transitory and unimportant.However, a stronger <strong>in</strong>terpretation is that when communication strategies are used,they have implications for longer-term language development.There are three requirementsfor this to happen. First, it is necessary that solv<strong>in</strong>g current communicative problems leavessome sort oftrace. In other words, what is <strong>in</strong>itially an improvisation to convcy one’s mean<strong>in</strong>gwhen resources are limited is noticed and becomes more than a transitory but evanescentsuccess; there must be someth<strong>in</strong>g about the <strong>in</strong>teraction which is sufficiently salient, and/orthe process<strong>in</strong>g capacity available allows such attention. Second, the improvisation whichhas become a solution must be useful to future problems ~ it must have some transfer orgeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g power. Such an outcome would reflect the way the <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>its</strong>elf has ledto useful hypothesis generation or to syntactic process<strong>in</strong>g (Swa<strong>in</strong> 1985; 1995). Third,the communicative solution needs to becomc proceduralized, either because it is so strik<strong>in</strong>gdur<strong>in</strong>g one occurrence (Logan 1998), or because <strong>its</strong> strength is built up more graduallythrough repeated related solutions to essentially the same communicative problem


84 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 85be<strong>in</strong>g tra<strong>in</strong>ed to be obta<strong>in</strong>ed. The former is based on a five-step scale on which globalproficiency can be estimated (supplemented by plus scores for each numerical category) .Thelatter gives separate rat<strong>in</strong>gs for syntax, vocabulary, fluency, and other skill areas. In this way,the longitud<strong>in</strong>al development of the learners can be monitored through an exam<strong>in</strong>ation ofthe profiles generated by the analytic mark<strong>in</strong>gs scheme over several po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time.Higgs and Clifford (1 982) report that profiles of students at earlier po<strong>in</strong>ts of <strong>in</strong>structioncan be used predictively to estimate the likely later ga<strong>in</strong> of the candidates <strong>in</strong> question. Giventhe basic five-stcp scale, candidates whose grammar rat<strong>in</strong>gs were above or equal to theirrat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> vocabulary or fluency tended to cont<strong>in</strong>ue to progress and reach hgher performancelevels as they received more <strong>in</strong>struction. In other words, balanced analytic rat<strong>in</strong>gs or highergrammar predicted cont<strong>in</strong>ued ga<strong>in</strong> and capacity to profit from <strong>in</strong>struction. In contrast,students whose earlier profiles showed strong fluency and vocabulary skills did not manifestthe same degree of susta<strong>in</strong>ed improvement. Higgs and Clifford (1982) called these learners‘term<strong>in</strong>al 2’s’ (from the five-step scale), suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the earlier profile was associated witha probable plateau<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> achievement at around Level 2. It seemed as though the earlier fluencyand vocabulary ga<strong>in</strong>s comprised cont<strong>in</strong>ued development, and may have been associated withfossilization. These learners corresponded, <strong>in</strong> some ways, to Schmidt’s Wes, s<strong>in</strong>ce earliercommunicative effectiveness (and the higher fluency and vocabulary scores earlier <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>struction might be connected with a communicative orientation on the part of such learners)represented a short-term advantage which proved expensive <strong>in</strong> the longer run s<strong>in</strong>ce it wasassociated with an <strong>in</strong>terlanguage system which became less permeable. Once aga<strong>in</strong>, thesuggestion is that unless there is direct <strong>in</strong>volvement of the underly<strong>in</strong>g language system <strong>in</strong>communication, it need not develop, even though communicative effectiveness does change.Theoretically-based concernsIn addition to these empirically motivated concerns over the usefulness of communicationstrategies, there are somc more theoretically-based worries. First of all, there are what mightbe termed logical criticisms of the viewpo<strong>in</strong>t. For example, it is difficult to imag<strong>in</strong>e exactlyhow such strategies can leave a trace. It is likely that <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g operations will occur whenachievement strategies are used to cope with communicative problems whose solution willrequire some adaptation of the underly<strong>in</strong>g systcm. But <strong>in</strong> such cases thc need to solveunforeseen problems will ensure that thc lion’s share of cognitive resources will be directcdto convey<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs. As a result, it is not easy to see how memory of what exactly hasworked can be effectively reta<strong>in</strong>cd for the next occasion when the strategy may be useful,s<strong>in</strong>cc this outcome would require the spare capacity to fumble towards such a solution andsimultaneously to monitor <strong>its</strong> nature and <strong>its</strong> effect. It seems unlikely that the conflict<strong>in</strong>g callson limitcd resources will allow this with any dependability.VanPattcn (1 990) makes a similarpo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> relation to comprehension, where he demonstrates that syntactic and semanticproccss<strong>in</strong>g seem to conflict as far as attentional resources are concerned, and that attentionspan is too limited to allow both to be emphasized simultaneously. One can only assume thatspeak<strong>in</strong>g, as part of the <strong>in</strong>teraction, will pose significantly greater problems for learn<strong>in</strong>g.More generally, for the use of communication strategies to work to foster progresssystematically, it would be necessary to show not simply that they leave a trace, but also thatthe use of such strategies has some cumulative build<strong>in</strong>g potential. For if SLA research hasdemonstrated anyth<strong>in</strong>g, it is that dcvclopmental sequenccs have considerable importance.It would be necessary, therefore, to show that the progressive improvisations which solvecommunication problems build upon one another, and are not isolated chance manipulationsof language elements <strong>in</strong> one restricted area, but have system-develop<strong>in</strong>g potential, and push


86 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 87deal of the time. S<strong>in</strong>ce it is mean<strong>in</strong>gs which are primary, as long as the speaker feels thatcommunication is proceed<strong>in</strong>g satisfactorily, the need for precise syntax is dim<strong>in</strong>ished. Thiscontrasts very clearly with the younger language learner who has much less schematic andcontextual knowledge available personally, and who is also much less able to imag<strong>in</strong>e whathis or her <strong>in</strong>terlocutor has by way of knowledge <strong>in</strong> each of these areas. As a result, the childhas much less scope to take syntactic liberties and short cuts.We are now fac<strong>in</strong>g quite a changed picture regard<strong>in</strong>g the usefulness (or lack of it) ofconversation for language development.There is less need, for the older learner, to producecomplete and well-formed utterances, because most <strong>in</strong>teractions require collaborativeconstruction of mean<strong>in</strong>g rather than solipsistic party pieces. Further, when communicativeproblems occur, the strategies second language learners adopt are not likely to push forwardunderly<strong>in</strong>g system change <strong>in</strong> any cumulative way. F<strong>in</strong>ally, there is the issue that, even ifconversation were by means of complete, well-formed utterances, and attempts to copewith communicative problems were useful, there is still the likelihood that attempts to copewith ongo<strong>in</strong>g process<strong>in</strong>g demands would not allow the learner to capitali7e upon such atemporary breakthrough, establish a memory trace of it, and use it <strong>in</strong> the future.ConclusionThe central theme of this chapter has been that syntax has fragile properties. Normalcommunication is pervaded by the pressures of process<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> real time. Wecomprehend and produce language not by exhaustively analys<strong>in</strong>g and comput<strong>in</strong>g (althoughwe can do these th<strong>in</strong>gs if we have to, for reasons of creativity or precision) but <strong>in</strong>stead bydraw<strong>in</strong>g shamelessly on probabilistic strategies which work effectively enough (given thesupport and potential for retrieval of miscommunication that discourse provides) atconsiderable speed of process<strong>in</strong>g. We rely on time-creat<strong>in</strong>g devices, context, predictionskills, elliptical language, and a range of similar performance factors to reduce the process<strong>in</strong>gload that we have to deal with dur<strong>in</strong>g conversation. And the older we become (up to a po<strong>in</strong>t)the more adept we can be at exploit<strong>in</strong>g these resources.The central po<strong>in</strong>t is that language use, <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>elf, does not lead to the development of ananalytic knowledge system s<strong>in</strong>ce mean<strong>in</strong>g distracts attention from form. But clearlycommunication does proceed, so one can <strong>in</strong>fer that speakers draw upon other non-analyticknowledge systems which, one assumes, have qualities relevant to real-time communication.Note1 In one sense, of course, this po<strong>in</strong>t is addressed through the dist<strong>in</strong>ction betweencommunication and learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies. The former emphasizes solutions to immediatecommunication problems, while the latter are concerned with activities which are <strong>in</strong>tendedby the learner to lead to longer-term development. In some cases this dist<strong>in</strong>ction is clear,as when, for example, a communication strategy deals with (say) how to express an ideawhen a lexical item is miss<strong>in</strong>g (and has no last<strong>in</strong>g effect) or when a learner deliberatelyorgani7es a list of words for memorization, not attempt<strong>in</strong>g to use these words immediately,but <strong>in</strong>stead work<strong>in</strong>g towards the extension of an underly<strong>in</strong>g vocabulary. But the centralissue is that one can also regard the operation of many communication strategies asconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g potential, for example when a useful communication strategy becomesproceduralized and so reusable. It is precisely this type of communication strategy that isrelevant <strong>in</strong> this section.


88 PETER SI


COMPREHENSION AND PRODUCTION STRATEGIES 89Lowe, P. Jr. (1982) TLR Handbook on Oral Interview Test<strong>in</strong>g. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.: Defense <strong>Language</strong>Institute.Lyster, R. and Ranta, L. (1997) ‘Corrective feedback and learner uptake: negotiation of form<strong>in</strong> communicative classrooms’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 19: 37-66.McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, B. (1987) Theories $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. London: Edward Arnold.McNamara, T. (1995) ‘Modell<strong>in</strong>g performance: open<strong>in</strong>g Pandora’s box’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 16:159-79.Oller, J.W. (1983) (cd.). Issues <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Test<strong>in</strong>g Research. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Pica, T. (1994) ‘Research on negotiation: what does it reveal about second language learn<strong>in</strong>g,conditions, processes, outcomes?’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 44: 493-527.Postovsky, V. (1977) ‘Why not start speak<strong>in</strong>g later?’, <strong>in</strong> Burt, M., Dulay, H. and F<strong>in</strong>occhiaro,M. (eds) Viewpo<strong>in</strong>ts on <strong>English</strong> as a Second <strong>Language</strong>. NewYork: Regents.Schmidt, R. (1983) ‘Interaction, acculturation, and the acquisition of communicativecompetence’, <strong>in</strong> Wolfson, N. and Judd, E. (eds) Sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics and Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Skehan, P. (1984) ‘On the non-magical nature of foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. PolyBot 5: Fiche 1.Spolsky, B. (1985) ‘Formulat<strong>in</strong>g a theory of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second<strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 7: 269-88.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1985) ‘Communicative competence: some roles of comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put andcomprehensible output <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> development’, <strong>in</strong> Gass, S. and Madden, C. (eds) Input <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Rowley, Mass. : Newbury House.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1995) ‘Three functions of output <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> G. Cook and B.Seidlhofer (eds) Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and Practice <strong>in</strong> Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. and Lapk<strong>in</strong>, S. (1982) Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Education:A Canadian Case Study. Clevedon,Avon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Tarone, E. (1981) ‘Some thoughts on the notion of communication strategy’. TESOL Quarterly15: 2845-95.VanPatten, B. (1990) ‘Attend<strong>in</strong>g to content and form <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>put: an experiment <strong>in</strong>consciousness’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 12: 287-301.Widdowson, H.G. (1989) ‘Knowledge of language and ability for use’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 10:128-37.W<strong>in</strong>itz, H. (1978) ‘A reconsideration of comprehension and production <strong>in</strong> language tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g’.Applied Health and Behavioural Sciences 1 : 272-3 15.


Chapter 5Leo van LierCONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES INCLASSROOM TALK: ISSUES OF EQUALITYAND SYMMETRYOST CURRENT VIEWS OF LANGUAGE education are based on theM assumption that social <strong>in</strong>teraction plays a central role <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g processes, as aquick glance at the dom<strong>in</strong>ant term<strong>in</strong>ology shows. “Communication,” “negotiation ofmean<strong>in</strong>g,”“co-construction,”“coopcrative learn<strong>in</strong>g,”“responsive teach<strong>in</strong>g,” and many otherterms like them testify to a fundamental shift from condition<strong>in</strong>g, association, and otherlaboratory-based notions of learn<strong>in</strong>g to human learn<strong>in</strong>g as it is situated <strong>in</strong> the everyday socialworld of the learner.This shift to the social context (and construction) of language learn<strong>in</strong>g does not makethe <strong>in</strong>vcstigation of learn<strong>in</strong>g processcs any easier. On thc contrary. The security of isolat<strong>in</strong>gvariables and def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g them operationally, a security obta<strong>in</strong>ed by laboratory-like experimentsand statistical <strong>in</strong>ferences, is largely lost, as the researcher is forced to look for determ<strong>in</strong>antsof learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the fluid dynamics of real-time learn<strong>in</strong>g contexts.Traditionally we have thought of scientific research as a matter of look<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to causesand effects, and the benef<strong>its</strong> have been cast <strong>in</strong> the shape of generalizations from a sample toa population and of accurate predictions of future occurrcnces.This research scenario, whileadequate for simple physical processes and laboratory-controlled behaviors, will no longerwork once we venture forth <strong>in</strong>to the real world of complexity, <strong>in</strong> which many people andcircumstances act and <strong>in</strong>teract. Here there are no simple causes, and predictability mustyield to cont<strong>in</strong>gency. Research must be aimed at <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g our understand<strong>in</strong>g, bothholistically and <strong>in</strong> the smallest details, of the social sett<strong>in</strong>g as a complex adaptive system.Increased understand<strong>in</strong>g allows us not to generalize but to particularize, that is, to adaptour skills, ideas, and strategies to the chang<strong>in</strong>g circumstances and the multifarious <strong>in</strong>fluencesof the contexts <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>vestigated processes occur.It is of the utmost importance to realize how different the job ofresearch<strong>in</strong>g languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g becomes once we decide that the social context is central.To cont<strong>in</strong>ue look<strong>in</strong>g foroperationally def<strong>in</strong>ed, discretely measured, statistically manipulated, and causally predictivevariables would be to approach one job with tools that belong to another. It would belike go<strong>in</strong>g to an archaeological site with a comb<strong>in</strong>e harvester or like sh<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g shoes with anail file.In this essay I exam<strong>in</strong>e social <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> language-learn<strong>in</strong>g sett<strong>in</strong>gs from the po<strong>in</strong>tof view that such sett<strong>in</strong>gs are complex systems <strong>in</strong> which both attention to detail and global


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALI< 91understand<strong>in</strong>g are necessary. There are many different k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>teraction that may occur<strong>in</strong> these sett<strong>in</strong>gs, but I group them <strong>in</strong>to two broad types: teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction andlearner-lcarner <strong>in</strong>teraction. Both have been the subject of considerable research, and theirpotential to facilitate (or h<strong>in</strong>der) language learn<strong>in</strong>g has been much debated. I look attranscribed examples of learn<strong>in</strong>g talk to try to understand how social <strong>in</strong>teraction facilitateslearn<strong>in</strong>g.The first example is an extract from a teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction; the second, anextract from a learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction. (In the transcriptions that follow; x’s <strong>in</strong>parentheses <strong>in</strong>dicate an un<strong>in</strong>telligible, brief exclamation or word; a left square bracket<strong>in</strong>dicates overlap; colons <strong>in</strong>dicate lengthen<strong>in</strong>g of the previous sound; the equals sign <strong>in</strong>dicatesthat the turn cont<strong>in</strong>ues below at the next equals sign; and three ellipsis dots <strong>in</strong>dicate a pauseof about one second.Teacher: Put the umbrella . . .Student: Put the umbrella on theyoorTeacher: On thefloor. . .Student: . . . between . . .Teacher: . . . between. . .Student: . . . the booksheyand the TCiTeacher: good.In this example of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> an ESL classroom, it is easy to dist<strong>in</strong>guish teacher fromstudent. The teacher prompts and gives feedback, while the studcnt produces language aspart of a task (hcre, plac<strong>in</strong>g objects <strong>in</strong> a picture as a way of practic<strong>in</strong>g prepositions).That such classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction is easily recognizable is often taken as evidence of <strong>its</strong>artificiality. The characteristic pattern has the teacher do<strong>in</strong>g most of the talk<strong>in</strong>g while thestudents act as rather passive responders and followers of directions. As Anthony Edwardsand David Westgate (1987) put it, classroom talk seems to run along “deep grooves,” even <strong>in</strong>sett<strong>in</strong>gs that aim to break ncw ground. Students “have only very restricted opportunities toparticipate <strong>in</strong> the language of the cla~sroom,”as John S<strong>in</strong>clair and David Brazil (1982) note.What makes classroom talk the way it is? How does it differ from <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> othersett<strong>in</strong>gs, and how can it be brought <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with prescnt-day critical and constructivist goalsfor education?Learner 1:Learner 2:Learner 1:Learner 2:Learncr 1:Learner 2:Learner 1:Learner 2:Learner 1:Learner 2:Learner 1:Learner 2:Learner 1:Learncr 2:Learner 1:Here I - sometimes go to the beach (xxxxxs)Pebble Beach?Not Pebble Beach. My (sxxxxx)/They near - Oh,yeah.[UhuhWow. 1s it good?Yeah, I th<strong>in</strong>k so.But I th<strong>in</strong>k here the beach not beautfulO:h, re::ally?Yes. It’s not white. The sand is not white./UhuhAnd the water -you cannot swim.1 see becauseyeah! We can swim but=[This water is -[=the water is cold.


92 LEO VAN LIERIn this conversation between two ESL learners, <strong>in</strong> contrast to the teacher-student<strong>in</strong>teraction above, no one dom<strong>in</strong>ates or is <strong>in</strong> control: both learners contribute fairly equallyto the talk.The learners understand each other perfectly and are able to express viewpo<strong>in</strong>tsand advance arguments. They do not, at least not <strong>in</strong> this extract, <strong>in</strong>fect each other withl<strong>in</strong>guistic errors or create some form of <strong>in</strong>terl<strong>in</strong>gual pidg<strong>in</strong>, as teachers sometimes fearlearners might do when left to their own devices.But what k<strong>in</strong>ds of opportunities do learners have to learn new language when they talkto each other <strong>in</strong> this way? Are the bl<strong>in</strong>d lead<strong>in</strong>g the bl<strong>in</strong>d here, or can such learner-learnerconversation become a sort of <strong>in</strong>teractional bootstrapp<strong>in</strong>g, where participants assemblelearn<strong>in</strong>g material or contribute learn<strong>in</strong>g material to each other <strong>in</strong> the natural course of theirtalk?The effectiveness of teacher talk and of learner talk as <strong>in</strong>put for learn<strong>in</strong>g has beenextensively discussed and researched (Chaudron 1988; Pica 1987; Ellis 1994).Teacher talkhas been lauded for be<strong>in</strong>g comprehensible and criticized for be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>authentic and notattuned to student needs. Learner talk has been lauded for provid<strong>in</strong>g opportunities fornegotiat<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g and criticized for be<strong>in</strong>g a defective model, riddled with <strong>in</strong>accuracies.On the whole, research has been supportive of learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction more than ofteacher talk, but thc learner-learner talk studied has usually been <strong>in</strong>teractional (e.g., asgroup work; see Long and Porter 19SS), and the teacher talk has tended to be monologic(e.g., <strong>in</strong> the form of lectures or <strong>in</strong>structions; sec Parker and Chaudron 1987). We thereforedo not know if it is the nature of the talk or the nature of the <strong>in</strong>terlocutor or a comb<strong>in</strong>ationof both that makes the difference.Constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resourcesThe British sociologist Anthony Giddcns describes the structure of social systems <strong>in</strong> termsof rules that both enable and constra<strong>in</strong> characteristics. Just as <strong>in</strong> a game, and I <strong>in</strong>clude thespecial sense that Ludwig Wittgenste<strong>in</strong> attaches to “language game,” the social world isgoverned by rules that allow certa<strong>in</strong> moves to be madc while disallow<strong>in</strong>g (or disfavor<strong>in</strong>g)others.’ In a game like chess, these rules and moves are clear and circumscribed, but <strong>in</strong>social sett<strong>in</strong>gs the rules are often tacit and ambiguous, and their precise <strong>in</strong>terpretation ordef<strong>in</strong>ition may have to be negotiated <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tcraction.In the social sett<strong>in</strong>g of the classroom, <strong>in</strong>teraction among participants takes place aga<strong>in</strong>sta backdrop of constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resources that are <strong>in</strong> some ways different, <strong>in</strong> some ways similar,to those that characteri7e other sett<strong>in</strong>gs. The classroom thus can be seen to constitute aspeech exchange system (Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson 1974) that has <strong>its</strong> own rules forturn tak<strong>in</strong>g and gives <strong>its</strong> participants certa<strong>in</strong> rights and duties.The classroom is the primarysett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which talk-for-language-learn<strong>in</strong>g (learn<strong>in</strong>g talk) is carried out, and as such theclassroom demonstrates the norms for proper behavior (what is called “fixity” by Giddens(1 984) or “habitus” by Bourdieu (1 990)) that underlie the <strong>in</strong>stitutional task of languageteach<strong>in</strong>g.People <strong>in</strong> language classrooms, engaged <strong>in</strong> the official bus<strong>in</strong>ess of language learn<strong>in</strong>g,tend to behave and talk <strong>in</strong> ways that ratify that bus<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>in</strong> other words, they behave andtalk “appropriately” (see Fairclough (1 992) for an <strong>in</strong>cisive discussion of this problematicterm). Elements of appropriateness, most prom<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>in</strong>side the classroom, may rema<strong>in</strong>visible also outside the classroom, whcncvcr learn<strong>in</strong>g talk is carried out <strong>in</strong> nondesignatedplaccs and at nonscheduled timcs (<strong>in</strong> cafeterias, around picnic tables, and so on), as whentwo students <strong>in</strong> the extract of learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction given above agree to engage <strong>in</strong> a


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALI< 93conversation at the request of a researcher. But time and place may make a difference <strong>in</strong> theway talk is conducted, and lcarn<strong>in</strong>g talk <strong>in</strong>side lessons may differ structurally from learn<strong>in</strong>gtalk outside lessons. This possibility needs to be taken <strong>in</strong>to account when learners’ andteachers’ <strong>in</strong>teractions are analy7ed.There arc practical consequences of this constra<strong>in</strong>ts-resources view of languagc lcarn<strong>in</strong>gcontexts. In an article entitled “NoTalk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Class,” J. H. Lii (1 994) depicts the traditionalrole of teacher as one of lectur<strong>in</strong>g and that of students as “mostly listen<strong>in</strong>g passively <strong>in</strong> class.”Indeed, a student is quoted as say<strong>in</strong>g that he used to have “trouble concentrat<strong>in</strong>g because hewas so bored by lectures.”These comments fit the known stereotypes of teach<strong>in</strong>g wellenough.The <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g twist here is that <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>novative class described (whch has twentyfivestudents), the problem is solved not by the teacher’s chang<strong>in</strong>g his way of speak<strong>in</strong>g and<strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with the students but by the plac<strong>in</strong>g of a computer between the teacher and thetaught. Thanks to the <strong>in</strong>sertion of the computer, students “now have the opportunity to<strong>in</strong>teract with teachers and receive <strong>in</strong>stant feedback.” A skeptical person might ask, Why do<strong>in</strong>teraction and feedback require an artificial <strong>in</strong>terface? Why can’t professors <strong>in</strong>teract withtheir students without a computer?Pierre Bourdieu and Jean-Claude Passeron, <strong>in</strong> their work on cultural reproduction,suggest that the <strong>in</strong>stitution equips the teacher with certa<strong>in</strong> distanc<strong>in</strong>g techniques; the mostefficient technique is “magisterial discourse,” which condemns the teacher to “theatricalmonologue.” So powerful is this <strong>in</strong>stitutional control over the teacher’s language use,accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bourdieu and Passeron (1 977) that “efforts to set up dialogue immediately turn<strong>in</strong>to fiction or farce”. The possibility that computer use may be able to circumvent these<strong>in</strong>stitutional constra<strong>in</strong>ts is <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g.This characterization of teacher-student <strong>in</strong>teraction may seem overdrawn andunrepresentative of today’s classrooms, many of which are more dynamic and democratic.But there is no doubt that <strong>in</strong> various subtle or overt ways the <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>sthe types of talk that can occur with<strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> doma<strong>in</strong>; and it is an open question whether ateacher is free to ignore such constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terests of pedagogical action. Bourdieuand Passeron are clearly skeptical about the possibility of that freedom, though perhapstransformation-m<strong>in</strong>ded educators may want to see how far they can go, and to what effect.The <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>g, of course, offers resources and facilitates their deployment <strong>in</strong>the tangible form of budgets, materials, equipment, and the likc, but also <strong>in</strong> the form, lesspalpable though perhaps more important, of authority and power: the authority to set theagenda, the power to judge (and grade, test, pass, fail); the authority to speak, the powerto control and evaluate the speech of others. This authority and this power have traditionallydef<strong>in</strong>ed the teacher and the work of teach<strong>in</strong>g, but they are <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly viewed as no longerappropriate <strong>in</strong> today’s learn<strong>in</strong>g environments. John Merrow reports the story of a teacher’snot know<strong>in</strong>g how to cont<strong>in</strong>ue with a multimedia project after a specialized <strong>in</strong>structor waslaid off. It had not occurred to this teacher that she could ask the students to teach her;ask<strong>in</strong>g them did not fit her concept of the teacher’s role. As Merrow (1 995) suggests,“teachers won’t survive, and school will become <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly irrelevant, if teachers don’tchange their style of teach<strong>in</strong>g,” a style he refers to as “the bank deposit approach”.It is with<strong>in</strong> the structure of <strong>in</strong>stitutional constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resources that the teacher’s<strong>in</strong>teraction with learners must take place. When teacher talk and teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teractionare exam<strong>in</strong>ed, particularly when recommendations for changes are made, these structur<strong>in</strong>gforces must be kcpt <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. If <strong>in</strong>teraction is as important for language learn<strong>in</strong>g as currenttheories claim it is, then the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>teraction the classroom perm<strong>its</strong> and the changes theteacher can realistically make to those k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>teraction are of great importance toresearch.


94 LEO VAN LIERTak<strong>in</strong>g a closer look at teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> the language classroom, I ignoresuch common types of teacher talk as the lecture, the story, and various forms of explanationand <strong>in</strong>struction, s<strong>in</strong>ce my focus is on social <strong>in</strong>teraction. But I do not deny the importanceand potential value for learn<strong>in</strong>g of these more monologic forms.The <strong>in</strong>itiation-response-feedback exchangeTeacher: What is this called?Learner: Plastic.Teacher: You called it plastic. Good! It’s plastic. Rut it’s got another name too . . .transparency.. . . aThis exchange between a teacher and a learner is unmistakably classroom talk. Itconta<strong>in</strong>s the follow<strong>in</strong>g steps:1 The teacher, hold<strong>in</strong>g up an overhead transparency, asks a question to which the teacheralready knows the answer.2 The teacher wishes to see if the learner has some particular piece of knowledge andcan display this knowledge.3 The learner responds effectively and efficiently, but also elliptically, us<strong>in</strong>g just oneword.4 The teacher evaluates the learner’s response, approv<strong>in</strong>g of it, but then suggests thatthere might be another, more felicitous, answer.This particular form of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, the teach<strong>in</strong>g exchange, is considered amongthc most frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g types of teacher-student talk <strong>in</strong> the classroom (S<strong>in</strong>clair andCoulthard 1975; Mehan 1979; van Lier 1988; Wells 1993) and is usually called an IRFexchangc, s<strong>in</strong>ce it consists of these three parts (or moves): <strong>in</strong>itiation, response, feedback.In the IRF format, a number of different th<strong>in</strong>gs can be accomplished. At the mostmechanical, rote-learn<strong>in</strong>g end of IRF, the teacher’s questions require the students merelyto recite previously learned items. IRF may also be used by the teacher to see if studentsknow a certa<strong>in</strong> word or l<strong>in</strong>guistic item. IRF can demand more, challeng<strong>in</strong>g students to th<strong>in</strong>k,reason, and make connections. At the most demand<strong>in</strong>g end of IRF, students must bearticulate and precise; they are pushed by successive prob<strong>in</strong>g questions, to clarify,substantiate, or illustrate some po<strong>in</strong>t that they made previously.Teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> the three-turn format of IRF therefore occupies acont<strong>in</strong>uum between mechanical and demand<strong>in</strong>g, as shown <strong>in</strong> the figure below.Given the variety of pedagogical work that the IRF format perm<strong>its</strong>, it would be amistake to dismiss it altogether as bad practice. Every case must be exam<strong>in</strong>ed on <strong>its</strong> mer<strong>its</strong>.As a rule of thumb, the precise nature of the IRF be<strong>in</strong>g employed <strong>in</strong> a particular <strong>in</strong>stance isrevealed <strong>in</strong> the third turn,3 s<strong>in</strong>ce this is where the teacher typically reveals the purposc ofthe question or sequence of questions. After the follow<strong>in</strong>g question-answer pairFigure 5.1 IRF cont<strong>in</strong>uumRecitation-Display Cognition-Precision- Depth of process<strong>in</strong>g


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALK 95Teacher:Learner:What’s the dgerence between “water is heat<strong>in</strong>g”and “water is heated’?Water is heat<strong>in</strong>g, it - it’s the one who’s heat<strong>in</strong>g.a variety of third turns are possible. In each case, a different type of task is revealed to be<strong>in</strong> progress:Teacher: Good. Say the whole sentence: Water is heat<strong>in</strong>g the radiators.(recitation)Teacher: Good. What do we call that construction?(display)Teacher: And can you th<strong>in</strong>k fsome th<strong>in</strong>gs that it might be heat<strong>in</strong>g?(cognition)Teacher: Aha, can you expla<strong>in</strong> that <strong>in</strong> a little more detail?(precision)Adapted from van Lier (1 996a)This cxample shows that the IRF structure cannot be regarded as a s<strong>in</strong>gle type of pedagogicalactivity. All four IRF types of teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction given above can be used to evaluateor control or to <strong>in</strong>vite participation. Know<strong>in</strong>g the purpose of a particular IRF exercise,though this may not always be easy, is crucial <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong> pedagogical value. But thereare some th<strong>in</strong>gs that all IRF sequences have <strong>in</strong> common, and these common fcaturcs mustbe exam<strong>in</strong>ed before IRF can be assessed as a pedagogical tool.Learn<strong>in</strong>g as co-construction: the lim<strong>its</strong> of IRFThe central feature of IRF is that the teacher is unequivocally <strong>in</strong> charge.This be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> chargemanifests <strong>its</strong>elf <strong>in</strong> a number of ways.Every IRF exchange is a step <strong>in</strong> an overall plan designed by the teacher. The plan maybe to check what the students know (as <strong>in</strong> recitation or display), to construct knowledgeor an argument, perhaps along Socratic l<strong>in</strong>es, or to push the students toward clarityof expression. It is important to note that the plan is not coconstructed. To vary<strong>in</strong>gdegrees, students may be aware of the nature of the plan and aware of the direction <strong>in</strong> whichthe discourse is mov<strong>in</strong>g, but usually these matters are revealed only gradually and<strong>in</strong>cidentally.The teacher does all the <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g and clos<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>in</strong> other words, takes all the first andthird turns), and the students’ work is done exclusively <strong>in</strong> the response slot.The IRF formattherefore discourages student <strong>in</strong>itiation and student repair work. As Denis Newman, PcgGriff<strong>in</strong>, and Michael Cole (1 989) note, “the three-part unit has a built-<strong>in</strong> repair procedure<strong>in</strong> the teacher’s last turn so that <strong>in</strong>correct <strong>in</strong>formation can be replaced with the rightanswers”. It is extremely hard, if not impossible, <strong>in</strong> thc IRF format, for the student to askquestions, to disagree, to sclf-correct, and so on. Indeed, I found that such student utterancesoverwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly occur as private turns, side sequences, or <strong>in</strong> other ways outside the IRFformat. Oftcn they arc whispered comments to a fellow learner or questions written down<strong>in</strong> a notebook. The IRF format discourages <strong>in</strong>terruption (or disruption) and can therefore


96 LEO VAN LIERbe called a closed rather than open discourse format, <strong>in</strong> that it structurally and functionallycontrols what takes place. It is like a discursive guided bus tour, but the it<strong>in</strong>erary is oftenunknown to the students.Students’ opportunities to exercise <strong>in</strong>itiative (see van Lier 1988; K<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>ger 1994) orto develop a sense of control and self-regulation (a sense of ownership of the discourse, asense of be<strong>in</strong>g empowered) are extremely restricted <strong>in</strong> the IRF format. Not only are studentutterances often highly elliptical and syntactically reduced, occurr<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>in</strong> the responseslot, sandwiched between two teacher turns (van Lier 1996a), they also prevent the studentfrom do<strong>in</strong>g turn tak<strong>in</strong>g, topic development, and activity structur<strong>in</strong>g work. They do notallow, to any significant extent, negotiation of the direction of <strong>in</strong>struction.Given these basic features, how does IRF relate to current recommendations ofco-construction, responsive teach<strong>in</strong>g (Bowers and Fl<strong>in</strong>ders 1990; Shuy 1991), or the <strong>in</strong>structionalconversation (Tharp and Gallimore 1988), especially if such recommendations arediscussed from the perspective of critical pedagogy (Darder 1991 ; Shor 1992)? I explorethis question from three different though related angles.Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development and the notion of pedagogicalscaffold<strong>in</strong>gLev Vygotsky discusses the range of activities a learner can accomplish with the assistanceof a more capable person, such as a teacher. At any po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> a learner’s development, someactivities (skills, operations, etc.) are with<strong>in</strong> the learner’s competence (this might be calledthe area of self-regulation), others can be accomplished only with special guidance, and yetothers lie entirely outside the learner’s scope.The middle band of activity, which is naturallythe focus of pedagogical action, is referred to by Vygotsky as the zone of proximaldevelopment (1978). Work<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> this 7one (the “construction 7one”<strong>in</strong> Ncwman, Griff<strong>in</strong>,and Cole), a teacher develops strategies for assist<strong>in</strong>g the lcarner. The various k<strong>in</strong>ds ofassistance, which guide a learner <strong>in</strong>to an activity that <strong>in</strong>itially is too complex, are often calledscaffold<strong>in</strong>g (Rruner 1983).The <strong>in</strong>itiation-response-feedback exchange, at least when it moves beyond mererecitation and display, can be regarded as a way of scaffold<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, a way of develop<strong>in</strong>gcognitive structures <strong>in</strong> the zone of proximal development, or a way of assist<strong>in</strong>g learners toexpress themselves with maximum clarity. IRF is frequently used to draw on students’ priorexperiences and current background knowledge to activate mental schemata and to establisha platform of shared knowledge that will facilitate the <strong>in</strong>troduction and <strong>in</strong>tegration ofnew knowledge. IRF used <strong>in</strong> several steps <strong>in</strong> a lesson or dur<strong>in</strong>g one activity among otheractivities (see Wells 1993), contributes to the atta<strong>in</strong>ment of a larger goal. Once it has served<strong>its</strong> purpose, it yields to other ways of structur<strong>in</strong>g participation.Scaffold<strong>in</strong>g, to be of true pedagogical benefit, must be temporary. The scaffold must begradually dismantled as the learner shows signs of be<strong>in</strong>g capable of handl<strong>in</strong>g more of thetask <strong>in</strong> question.This process is called handover (Bruner 1983), and without it scaffold<strong>in</strong>gwould simply breed dependence and helplessness. It is unclear whether IRF has <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>structure the flexibility to effect handover. I suspect that, for handover to be possible, IRFmust be abandoned at some po<strong>in</strong>t to make place for autonomous learner discourse. Thisswitch from IRF to more open discourse structures may be a crucial pedagogical decisionpo<strong>in</strong>t, and research should focus on it closely.


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALI< 97Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation and learner autonomyIntr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation can be def<strong>in</strong>ed as the human response to <strong>in</strong>nate needs for competence,relatedness, and autonomy (Deci and Ryan 1992; Deci,Vallerand, Pelletier, and Ryan 1991).It expresses <strong>its</strong>elf as a here-and-now <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g an activity for <strong>its</strong> own sake, forthe pleasure, stimulation, or challenge the activity provides. Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation is closelyrelated to the perception of be<strong>in</strong>g able to choose and of be<strong>in</strong>g somehow <strong>in</strong> control of one’sactions. Actions that are perceived as be<strong>in</strong>g externally controlled have a tendency to reduce<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, as do extr<strong>in</strong>sic rewards and praise or criticism (see Deci and Ryan 1985,1992 for examples and summaries of research <strong>in</strong>to <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation; see van Lier 1996a).S<strong>in</strong>ce IRF is clearly other-controlled (from the learner’s perspective) and s<strong>in</strong>ce therewards (<strong>in</strong> the form of teacher approval or praise <strong>in</strong> the third turn) are extr<strong>in</strong>sic, prolongeduse of the IRF format may have a negative effect on <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation and cause a decrease<strong>in</strong> levels of attention and <strong>in</strong>volvement. IRF exchanges are like discoursal tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g wheels.In bicycle rid<strong>in</strong>g the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g wheels must eventually come off, and likewise <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractionIRF must be replaced by free social <strong>in</strong>teraction.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to proponents of <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation (see van Lier 1996a), pedagogicalaction must be oriented toward <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g levels of <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation and hence toward<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g self-regulation and autonomy. IRF must break <strong>its</strong> lockstep and yield to otherparticipation patterns, ones that allow student <strong>in</strong>itiative and choice to develop.Transformation; or, chang<strong>in</strong>g educational reality through <strong>in</strong>teractionCritical pedagogy seeks to transform exist<strong>in</strong>g structures of control and <strong>in</strong>equality (Young1992; Darder 199 1) and to allow students to f<strong>in</strong>d voices of their own and become criticaland autonomous learners (Wertsch 1991). This emancipatory process requires truedialogue, which, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Paulo Freire (1 972), can flourish only <strong>in</strong> a climate of equalityamong participants. Freire ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s that dialogue is <strong>in</strong>dispensable for education: “Withoutdialogue there is no communication, and without communication there can be no trueeducation”.Characterized by one-sided control, IRF is only m<strong>in</strong>imally dialogic, and the students’participation <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> construction (and <strong>in</strong> the progression toward the overall goal) is largelypassive. Therefore IRF cannot not be regarded as foster<strong>in</strong>g equality or contribut<strong>in</strong>g to atransformation of educational reality; it embodies the status quo.Yet, as <strong>in</strong>dicated above, itmay bc uscd as a preparatory step toward more emancipatory forms of discourse; it maybe valuable not for what it is but, rather, for what it potentially leads to. For that potentialto be realized, discourse must move from the patterns RobertYoung (1992) aptly callsWDPK (What do pupils know?) and GWTT (Guess what teacher th<strong>in</strong>ks) to more discursivepatterns marked by shared <strong>in</strong>quiry. It thus becomes important to <strong>in</strong>vestigate how IRF <strong>its</strong>elfcan be transformed and how transitions from IRF to other discourse forms can be effected.Equality and symmetryThe IRF structure is clearly a significant advance over the ritual magisterial performancesBourdieu and Passeron rcferred to as “theatrical monologue” (sce above), s<strong>in</strong>ce at least it<strong>in</strong>volves students and asks them to contribute, albeit with<strong>in</strong> someone else’s agenda.Howcvcr, <strong>in</strong> terms of communication, control, <strong>in</strong>itiativc, mean<strong>in</strong>g creation and negotiation,message elaboration, and a number of other features characteristic of social <strong>in</strong>teraction, thelearner’s side of the IRF <strong>in</strong>teraction is seriously curtailed.


98 LEO VAN LIERIt is therefore useful to consider other forms of <strong>in</strong>teraction, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g conversational(such as learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teractions) and see what characteristics they have that might berelevant to language learn<strong>in</strong>g. For a general exam<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>in</strong>teraction, I suggest that thereare two ma<strong>in</strong> groups of issues:Issues of equality and <strong>in</strong>equality, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g control and power. In this context, one th<strong>in</strong>ksprimarily of teacher talk, but more generally the question of equality may play a role <strong>in</strong> any<strong>in</strong>teraction between native and nonnative speakers or between a more proficient and a lessproficient nonnative speaker (van Lier and Matsuo 1995).Issues of negotiation and the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of talk. This relates to shared rights andduties of participation, that is, <strong>in</strong>teractional symmetry. Such symmetry, most clearly visible<strong>in</strong> conversation among equals, may be more difficult to achieve for less proficient speakers.But, as the conversation between two ESL students quoted above demonstrates, it is by nomeans impossible.The phenomena relat<strong>in</strong>g to, on the one hand, control, power, and equality and, on the other,conversational symmetry and negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g are connected: unequal participantstend to have asymmetrical <strong>in</strong>teractions. But a dist<strong>in</strong>ction must be made between <strong>in</strong>teractionsthat are oriented toward achiev<strong>in</strong>g symmetry and those that are not (IRF, lectures,<strong>in</strong>structions, and other common teacher talk belong to the second category).An Orientation toward symmetry does not necessarily <strong>in</strong>volve an assumption of equalityor some sort of abdication of authority. A separation between symmetry and equality iscrucial for the possibility of fruitful communication between teachers and learners and,<strong>in</strong>deed, between native speakers and nonnative speakers. If true communication werepossible only hetween equals, then teachers and learners (and even parents and theirchildren) would be forever condemned to pseudo-communication.This is obviously not so.Hav<strong>in</strong>g postulated that communication, whether between equals or unequals, requiresan orientation toward <strong>in</strong>teractional symmetry, I now show, first, how such an orientationmay be visible and, second, what benef<strong>its</strong> it might have for language learn<strong>in</strong>g.In what ways can utterances be oriented toward symmetry? Basically, the orientationexpresses <strong>its</strong>elf <strong>in</strong> relations of cont<strong>in</strong>gency between an utterance and other entities -primarily other utterances (preced<strong>in</strong>g, concurrent, and follow<strong>in</strong>g), shared knowledge, andrelevant features <strong>in</strong> the world (Gibson (1 979) calls them affordances; see further below).Cont<strong>in</strong>gencyThe term cont<strong>in</strong>deny refers to two dist<strong>in</strong>ct characteristics of <strong>in</strong>teraction: first, the signal<strong>in</strong>gof relations between a current utterance and previous utterances, either directly (utteranceto utterance) or through shared knowledge or shared affordances <strong>in</strong> the environment;second, the rais<strong>in</strong>g of expectations and the craft<strong>in</strong>g of deliberate ambiguities so that futureutterances can f<strong>in</strong>d a conversational home (see van Lier 1992, 1994 and 1996a). The firstcharacteristic has been well studied under the head<strong>in</strong>g of contextualization by John Gumperz(1 992). The ways <strong>in</strong> which utterances are l<strong>in</strong>ked to one another have also been studiedextensively by ethnomethodologists,j who have used related concepts such as conditionalrelevance and reflexive ty<strong>in</strong>g (Garf<strong>in</strong>kel 1967; Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson 1974).My preoccupation with cont<strong>in</strong>gency orig<strong>in</strong>ates <strong>in</strong> the belief that speakers, by us<strong>in</strong>glanguage cont<strong>in</strong>gently, unite structure and function <strong>in</strong> the most fundamental way possible


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALK 99(unite the given and the new, the topic and the comment, the foregrounded and the backgrounded).Cont<strong>in</strong>gent language use encourages, justifies, and motivates grammaticalization.Noncont<strong>in</strong>gent language use - or, rather, less cont<strong>in</strong>gent, s<strong>in</strong>ce the quality ofcont<strong>in</strong>gency exists on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum ~ proceeds more statically and encourages a treatmentof language as either form or function <strong>in</strong>stead of as an organic whole.Cont<strong>in</strong>gent features are most visible <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>d of talk usually referred to asconversational. Of all forms of talk, conversation is perhaps the hardest to def<strong>in</strong>e. It is, <strong>in</strong> asense, a catchall concept that can conta<strong>in</strong> other k<strong>in</strong>ds of talk - such as <strong>in</strong>structions, requests,stories, bus<strong>in</strong>ess deals. A complication is that other k<strong>in</strong>ds of talk can have conversationembedded <strong>in</strong> them. Interviews, lessons, or sales transactions may suddenly become chatty,then after a while switch back to bus<strong>in</strong>ess. So neat boundaries cannot be drawn around thephenomenon of conversation. Yet we usually know when a conversation is tak<strong>in</strong>g place.In conversation, every utterance is connected by many l<strong>in</strong>ks ~ some of them overt,many more of them covert - to previous utterances and through them to the shared (orto-be-shared) world of the participants. Every utterance sets up expectations for what willbe said next. Utterances <strong>in</strong> conversation are thus, at the same time, predicted and predict<strong>in</strong>g;<strong>in</strong> this way the <strong>in</strong>teractants’ mutual engagement (what Rommetveit (1974) calls<strong>in</strong>tersubjectivity) is achieved and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed.When talk is cont<strong>in</strong>gent, utterances are constructed on the spot rather than planned <strong>in</strong>advance. In addition, there is symmetry, that is, equal rights and duties of participation, atleast ideall~.~ I say “ideally” s<strong>in</strong>ce it often happens that one person monopolizes theconversation and does not let the others get a word <strong>in</strong> edgewise. But the orientation towardsymmetry still holds, s<strong>in</strong>ce the participants will note that the conversation was one-sided,that so-and-so monopolized it, and that it was therefore not a “good” conversation.To illustrate what makes an <strong>in</strong>teraction conversational, I quote two extracts fromnonnative speaker <strong>in</strong>teractions. In the first there is a high level of cont<strong>in</strong>gency; <strong>in</strong> the second,a much lower level:Speaker 1:Speaker 2:Speaker 1 :Speaker 2:Speaker 1 :Speaker 2:Speaker 1:Speaker 2:Speaker 1:Speaker 1:Speaker 2:Speaker 1:Speaker 2:Speaker 1:Speakcr 2:Speaker 1 :Speaker 2:Speaker 1:Speaker 2:Speaker 1:From my room 1 can see the ocean viewwowAnd -[And how many room do you have?Two bedroom two full bathroomWhat what whatTwo bedroom=[Two bedroom=and two full bathroomI never askedyou, what didyou do <strong>in</strong> Japan b$oreyou came here?Uhm ~ afterjnish high schoolUhuhI work -for. . . . . . . . . three yearsHmmAnd ~[Where did you work?It - this is very - d@cult for expla<strong>in</strong>fiYI use . . .the computerUhuh


100 LEO VAN LIERSpeaker 2, an ESL learner, is the same person <strong>in</strong> both <strong>in</strong>teractions, but <strong>in</strong> the first her<strong>in</strong>terlocutor is of roughly equal proficiency and <strong>in</strong> the second her <strong>in</strong>terlocutor is a nativelikebil<strong>in</strong>gual speaker. The first extract illustrates symmetry, and all utterances exhibit a highdegree of cont<strong>in</strong>gency. The second extract is more like an <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> which speaker 1encourages speaker 2 to speak. Relations of cont<strong>in</strong>gency are weaker, and symmetry isreduced. If cont<strong>in</strong>gency could be visualized as bundles of str<strong>in</strong>gs connect<strong>in</strong>g utterances,then the str<strong>in</strong>gs would be thicker and more numerous <strong>in</strong> the first conversation and moresparse and sp<strong>in</strong>dly <strong>in</strong> the second.Many sorts of devices can be used to create cont<strong>in</strong>gency: empathy markers (“Wow!”),repetitions of parts of each other’s utterances (“two bedroom ~ two bedroom”), <strong>in</strong>tonationpatterns, gestures, and so on. The devices come from a stock of resources similar toGumperz’s (1992) “contextualization cues” (<strong>in</strong>deed, as I suggested above, the creation ofcont<strong>in</strong>gencies overlaps significantly with the process of contextualization), though any<strong>in</strong>teractional marker that can be used to make a cont<strong>in</strong>gent l<strong>in</strong>k can also be used for otherpurposes, and this makes tabulat<strong>in</strong>g and quantify<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gency impossible.Cont<strong>in</strong>gency, negotiation, and language learn<strong>in</strong>gThe dynamics of <strong>in</strong>teraction have been studied <strong>in</strong> most detail by Teresa Pica and hercolleagues (Pica 1987,1992; Pica and Doughty 1985; Pica,Young, and Doughty 1987).Thisresearch, which focuses on opportunities for learners to carry out repair strategies follow<strong>in</strong>gcommunicative problems, has revealed various conditions that favor or disfavor such<strong>in</strong>teractional modification and has shown how it benef<strong>its</strong> comprehension. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Pica(1 987), “What enables learners to move beyond their current <strong>in</strong>terlanguage receptive andexpressive capacities when they need to understand unfamiliar l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put or whenrequired to produce a comprchcnsiblc message are opportunities to modify and restructuretheir <strong>in</strong>teraction with their <strong>in</strong>terlocutor until mutual comprehension is reached”.By resolv<strong>in</strong>g communicative problems through the use of <strong>in</strong>teractional modifications(requests for clarification or confirmation, comprehension checks, recasts, and other suchrepair<strong>in</strong>g moves), the learner obta<strong>in</strong>s comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put or makes new <strong>in</strong>put availablefor learn<strong>in</strong>g. Research has shown how learners actively work on the language to <strong>in</strong>creasetheir knowledge and proficiency.The follow<strong>in</strong>g observations, based on these analyses of repair <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ter-language talk,might help to place repair<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the overall context of <strong>in</strong>teractional language use.First, as Guy Aston has po<strong>in</strong>ted out, repair work and adjustments of various k<strong>in</strong>ds canbe used to express convergence of perspectives among participants or to “seek closure ona problem” (Rudduck 1991), not necessarily to make someth<strong>in</strong>g comprehensible. GeorgeYule (1 990) found that more-proficient <strong>in</strong>terlocutors sometimes simply decide to give upon certa<strong>in</strong> problematic items <strong>in</strong> a task and move on.Therefore repair may have results otherthan <strong>in</strong>creased comprehension, though <strong>in</strong>crcascd comprehension can reasonably be regardedas <strong>its</strong> chief aim.Second, the preponderance of repair (<strong>in</strong> the highly visible form of <strong>in</strong>teractionalmodifications) may be the result of the type of discourse <strong>in</strong>vestigated. In much of the workof Pica and associatcs (Pica,Young, and Doughty 1987; Pica 1992), the activity types <strong>in</strong>question are communication tasks <strong>in</strong> which participants (often a native speaker and anonnative speaker) need to exchange <strong>in</strong>formation. This need leads to <strong>in</strong>teraction thatis usually both asymmetrical and unequal, an environment <strong>in</strong> which explicit repair,with imbalances of the k<strong>in</strong>d illustrated by Yule, tends to be salient. A similar focus on


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES I N CLASSROOM TALI< 101repair can be seen <strong>in</strong> the analysis by Michael Moerman (1988) of <strong>in</strong>teraction among nativespeakers ofThai. He concludes that “repair is of central importance to the organizationof Conversation”. Moerman’s discussion of repair, however, is based on transcripts oftestimony <strong>in</strong> Thai court cases, where the status of overt repair is probably different fromthat <strong>in</strong> general conversation. Indeed, ethnomethodological analyses of repair and relatedmatters <strong>in</strong> conversation (Schegloff, Jefferson, and Sacks 1977; Heritage 1984; Pomerantz1984) <strong>in</strong>dicate a strong preference for self-repair and an avoidance of overt reactive repair,that is, repair that follows communication problems.Third, and related to the second observation, the <strong>in</strong>teractional activity of repair<strong>in</strong>g mustbe placed <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> social context. Repair<strong>in</strong>g, an attempt to achieve mutual understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>the face of problems, is one set of actions among many that manifest orientation towardmutual engagement (<strong>in</strong>ter-subjectivity) and symmetry. Repair<strong>in</strong>g occurs <strong>in</strong> response to theperception of those troubles. But s<strong>in</strong>ce troubles should be avoided <strong>in</strong> the first place, it makessense to focus attention also on other mechanisms for achiev<strong>in</strong>g mutual understand<strong>in</strong>g and<strong>in</strong>tersubjectivity. It makes no sense, from a discourse-analytical or a pedagogical perspective,to assign special status to an activity that is undertaken only when other, more-preferredactivities have been unsuccessful. To use an analogy, ice skaters are judged more on howthey skate than on how they pick themselves up after fall<strong>in</strong>g on the ice.Success <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction ~ that is, the achievement of mutual understand<strong>in</strong>g, cont<strong>in</strong>gency,and <strong>in</strong>tersubjectivity ~ is dependent on the skillful use of all relevant social and l<strong>in</strong>guisticresources, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those described by Gumperz as contextualization cues and those thatcreate cont<strong>in</strong>gency. These resources can be divided <strong>in</strong>to three categories, as follows (seeAtk<strong>in</strong>son and Hcritagc 1984; Duncan 1972; Kasper 1989; van Lier and Matsuo 1995 foradditional examples) :Proactive (plann<strong>in</strong>g, predict<strong>in</strong>g)Open<strong>in</strong>g sequencesCataphoraGrounders and preparersStrategic moves(By the way; Do you know what?)(Now; Listen to this)(OK, three po<strong>in</strong>ts 1 wanna make)(Let me give you an example)Concurrent (mak<strong>in</strong>g signals dur<strong>in</strong>g one’s own or another person’s turn)Back channelsGazeTurnover signalsEmpathy markers(Uhuh; Hm)(eye contact, look<strong>in</strong>g away)(Let mejnish; What doyou th<strong>in</strong>k?)(Oh; Wow; Really?)Reactive (summariz<strong>in</strong>g, rephras<strong>in</strong>g, wrapp<strong>in</strong>g up)Repair and correctionDemonstrations of understand<strong>in</strong>gGists and upshots(Doyou mean x?;Actually it’sy)(Oh; 1 see)(So; In a nutshell; What you’re say<strong>in</strong>g is)The relations between <strong>in</strong>teraction and learn<strong>in</strong>g are not expla<strong>in</strong>ed by this list or, <strong>in</strong>deed, byany other that might be devised. But at the very least the analysis shows that the concept ofnegotiation may need to be expanded from Pica’s def<strong>in</strong>ition: “When a listener signals to aspeaker that the speaker’s message is not clear, and listener and speaker work <strong>in</strong>teractivelyto resolve this impasse” (1 992). Negotiation <strong>in</strong>cludes the proactive and concurrent resources


102 LEO VAN LIERfor utterance design, as well as reactive resources other than repair. Repair is thus only oneamong many forms of negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g.A fourth and f<strong>in</strong>al consideration goes to the very foundations of learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>its</strong> relationto the environment. Almost all the work <strong>in</strong> applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics that addresses the role of<strong>in</strong>put and <strong>in</strong>teraction (see Ellis ( 1 994) for an overview) assumes an <strong>in</strong>put-output model ofcommunication and learn<strong>in</strong>g. This model is based on a view of language use as the transferof l<strong>in</strong>guistic matter from one person to another and largely ignores issues of reciprocity andcont<strong>in</strong>gency. Be<strong>in</strong>g basically a transmission model (as words like <strong>in</strong>put and output <strong>in</strong>dicate),it does not address learn<strong>in</strong>g as transformation and language learn<strong>in</strong>g as grammaticalization(the development of grammatical complexity <strong>in</strong> the organic sense, outl<strong>in</strong>ed, e.g., byRutherford (1 987)). It is likely that the true role of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and the truesense of whatvygotsky meant by the zone of proximal development can be revealed onlythrough an organic or ecological approach (see Gibson 1979; Bowers and Fl<strong>in</strong>ders 1990).In such an approach, notions like cont<strong>in</strong>gency and symmetry will be central, and overt actsof repair<strong>in</strong>g will be epiphenomena1 (Marcus and Zajonc 1985; Graumann 1990; Platt andBrooks 1994). L<strong>in</strong>guistic matter <strong>in</strong> the environment, to the extent that the learner has accessto it (see van Lier (1996) for a detailed discussion of access), provides affordances to theactive and perceptive learner (Gibson 1979; Dcci and Ryan 1992).’Whether or not suchaffordances are packaged as repair sequences is likely to be a m<strong>in</strong>or issue.A theoretical conclusionI have discussed two different types of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g, teacher-learner<strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> the IRF mode and learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction, to illustrate equality andsymmetry. I have suggested that <strong>in</strong>teraction is particularly beneficial for learn<strong>in</strong>g when it iscont<strong>in</strong>gent. Symmetrical <strong>in</strong>teraction is naturally cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong> a variety of ways, butasymmetrical <strong>in</strong>teraction is deficient <strong>in</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gency. Unequal discourse partners tend tof<strong>in</strong>d it more difficult to orient their <strong>in</strong>teraction toward symmetry; as a result their<strong>in</strong>teractions often look like IRF sequences or <strong>in</strong>terviews where one of the partners takes acontroll<strong>in</strong>g role.Two questions rema<strong>in</strong>: What are some ways <strong>in</strong> which unequal discourse partners - suchas teachers and learners or native speakers and nonnative speakers -- can engage <strong>in</strong>symmetrical and cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>teraction, and how would that engagement benefit learn<strong>in</strong>g?What are the pedagogical benef<strong>its</strong> of various forms of asymmetrical discourse, such aslectures and IRF exchanges?<strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g depends on the access learners have to relevant language material(affordances) <strong>in</strong> the environment and on <strong>in</strong>ternal conditions like motivation. <strong>Social</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractionis the prime external condition to ensure access and Icarncrs’ active engagement.Cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>teraction provides an “<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation for listen<strong>in</strong>g” (Sacks, Schegloff,and Jefferson 1974). Learners’ natural learn<strong>in</strong>g processes, through the desire to understandand be understood, synchroni~e with efficient perception and focus<strong>in</strong>g. Learners will bevigilant toward l<strong>in</strong>guistic features and will make an effort to be pragmatically precise yetambiguous where ambiguity is needed. Grammaticalization is thus a natural by-productof cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>teraction. To put this idea <strong>in</strong> the strongest possible (though of coursehypothetical) terms: the organic, self-regulat<strong>in</strong>g process of cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>teraction is anecessary and sufficient condition for language development to occur. In the absence ofappropriate research, this is of course a speculative hypothesis.But that is only one side of the co<strong>in</strong>. To the extent that the target of language learn<strong>in</strong>g


CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALK 103is a standardized, official code (a set of cultural hab<strong>its</strong>) to which the learner has to or wantsto conform, l<strong>in</strong>guistic affordances marked as appropriate and desirable must be presented<strong>in</strong> the environment, and access to these affordances must be facilitated. Here organiclanguage development and external language demands (socioculturally and <strong>in</strong>stitutionallymandated) meet each other halfway, and Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development is thespace where<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal and external realms (<strong>in</strong>ner resources and outer constra<strong>in</strong>ts) oflanguage are mediated.This mediation takes place under the guidance of parents, teachers, and othercompetent persons, and the different ways they do this can be captured by terms such asBruner’s scafold<strong>in</strong>g. (<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>, didactics, <strong>in</strong>struction, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, drill<strong>in</strong>g, and so on are of coursealso terms that have traditionally been used for such expert-novice activities.)If this view of the relations between language learn<strong>in</strong>g and social <strong>in</strong>teraction has merit,then the dynamic connections between more didactic (asymmetrical, less cont<strong>in</strong>gent) andmore conversational (symmetrical, more cont<strong>in</strong>gent) forms of <strong>in</strong>teraction are of centralimportance <strong>in</strong> the language learn<strong>in</strong>g enterprise.A practical conclusionIn a book on talented teenagers, Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, Kev<strong>in</strong> Rathunde, and SamuelWhalen (1 993) compare current teach<strong>in</strong>g with the traditional role of the master <strong>in</strong> anapprentice system.They observe that the teacher, <strong>in</strong>stead of be<strong>in</strong>g a practitioner <strong>in</strong> a doma<strong>in</strong>,is now a transmitter of <strong>in</strong>formation and thus discourages the development of extended andtransform<strong>in</strong>g relationships such as those between master and apprentice. Relationshipsbetween teachers and students are depersonalized and “kept highly specialized,programmatic, and brief”. Technical terms such as “<strong>in</strong>structional delivery systems” anddetailed specifications of <strong>in</strong>structional objectives corroborate this tendency.Th<strong>in</strong>gs can onlyget worse when, as is currently happen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> many parts of the Western world, class sizesand school sizes keep <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g, as do teachers’ workloads.There are thus physical and <strong>in</strong>stitutional constra<strong>in</strong>ts that tend to m<strong>in</strong>imize the possibilitiesfor mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>in</strong>teraction between teachers and students. In Giddens’s structurationtheory, constra<strong>in</strong>ts ideally direct and guide, facilitat<strong>in</strong>g the deployment of resources. But <strong>in</strong>a defective <strong>in</strong>stitution (def<strong>in</strong>able as one <strong>in</strong> whch constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resources arc out of balance),constra<strong>in</strong>ts may obstruct the very purposes for which they were brought <strong>in</strong>to be<strong>in</strong>g. Aga<strong>in</strong>stconstra<strong>in</strong>ts of this second type, thc teacher must marshal all the resources, meager thoughthey often appear to be, that are available to provide learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities to students. Asthe history of educational reform movements shows, large-scale reforms tend to achievelittle transformation of the status quo. But grassroots, bottom-up <strong>in</strong>novations, usually basedon <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>itiative, can produce dramatic results, albeit at the local level only.Marshal<strong>in</strong>g available resources to promote rich and varied <strong>in</strong>teraction with and amongstudents must be the <strong>in</strong>dividual responsibility of every teacher. For teacher developmentthis responsibility means the promotion of what Max van Manen (1 991 ) calls “pedagogicalthoughtfulness” or “tact,” a m<strong>in</strong>dful, understand<strong>in</strong>g orientation <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>gs with students andan ability to act wisely. Many teachers have responded to calls for more <strong>in</strong>teractive andresponsive ways of teach<strong>in</strong>g by reduc<strong>in</strong>g their teacher-fronted activities and <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>glearner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction through cooperative learn<strong>in</strong>g and task-based learn<strong>in</strong>g. In currentjargon, they have become a “guide on the side” <strong>in</strong>stead of a “sage on the stage”.However, before we sw<strong>in</strong>g the pendulum from teacher-centered entirely to teacherperipheral,it may be worth reflect<strong>in</strong>g on what the optimal roles of a teacher should be.


104 LEO VAN LIERLearners need, <strong>in</strong> addition to peer <strong>in</strong>teraction, direct <strong>in</strong>tcraction with the teacher, providedit is quality <strong>in</strong>teraction. If we ask learners, many will say that they want lectures, explanationsand other forms of cxplicit teachcr guidance. And wc should never neglect the universalpower of stories (Egan 1986).The answer to a disproportionate amount of highly controll<strong>in</strong>g and depersonalizedteacher talk is not to m<strong>in</strong>imize all teacher talk per se but to f<strong>in</strong>d ways to modify it <strong>in</strong> morccont<strong>in</strong>gentdirections. In addition, teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction, such as the IRF, that isdesigned for scaffold<strong>in</strong>g learners’ language use (cognitively or socially) must conta<strong>in</strong> with<strong>in</strong>it the sceds of handover (Bruncr 1983), that is, the teacher must cont<strong>in</strong>ually be on thelookout for signs that learners are ready to be more autonomous languagc users.The classroom must regularly provide lcarners with opportunities to engage <strong>in</strong>symmetrical <strong>in</strong>teractions, s<strong>in</strong>ce such <strong>in</strong>teractions immersc learners <strong>in</strong> contextuali7ed andcont<strong>in</strong>gent talk, and s<strong>in</strong>ce these <strong>in</strong>teractions are <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sically motivat<strong>in</strong>g and attentionfocus<strong>in</strong>g. Symmetrical <strong>in</strong>teractions are most easily achieved when <strong>in</strong>terlocutors are equal<strong>in</strong> status and profcicncy, but equality is not always essential. Research byYule suggests that<strong>in</strong>equality <strong>in</strong> proficiency can be counterhalanced by hav<strong>in</strong>g the less proficient speaker carrythe ma<strong>in</strong> burden of <strong>in</strong>formation transfer.Teachers can also cxpcriment with ways of counterbalanc<strong>in</strong>g thc <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong>equality oftheir talk with learners (though <strong>in</strong> most <strong>in</strong>stitutional and cultural sett<strong>in</strong>gs it would be absurdfor them to pretend that status differences between them and their learners do not exist).In a documentary video, classcs <strong>in</strong> various British schools set up l<strong>in</strong>ks with classes <strong>in</strong>far-flung places like F<strong>in</strong>land, Greece, and Portugal (Twitch<strong>in</strong> 1993). At one po<strong>in</strong>t, a faxcame <strong>in</strong> from a class <strong>in</strong> Greece; it conta<strong>in</strong>ed draw<strong>in</strong>gs and descriptions of weav<strong>in</strong>gtechniques, with labels and cxpressions <strong>in</strong> Greek. The teacher and learners were naturallyat the same level with respect to this text, and <strong>in</strong>teraction among thcm became symmetricaland exploratory. When a parent who knew Greek was found and <strong>in</strong>vited to class to expla<strong>in</strong>the text, the teacher and his students were all learners.Tak<strong>in</strong>g guidance from these and other cxamples, the thoughtful teacher-researcherlooks for ways to make classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction varied and multidirncnsional. In the world oflanguage, we all embody different voiccs on different occasions (Bakht<strong>in</strong> 198 1 ; Wertsch199 1 ; Mayb<strong>in</strong> 1994). It is useful for lcarners to f<strong>in</strong>d that their teachers have various voicesand that the learners themselves can cxperiment with multiple voices <strong>in</strong> the target language.Such experimentation is crucial if they arc to f<strong>in</strong>d their own voice, and this is the truepurpose of language education.I thank Kathi Bailey for <strong>in</strong>sightful comments on an earlier draft.1 I realize I gloss over the problems that are <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> the concept of rulc and that havebeen highlighted <strong>in</strong> much of thc work of Wittgenste<strong>in</strong>, for example, PhilosophicalInvestigations.2 While the problcm of poor teacher-student communication cannot be solved by just anycomputer work, there is certa<strong>in</strong>ly cvidcnce that <strong>in</strong>novative use of computers can enhance<strong>in</strong>teraction, for cxample, through <strong>in</strong>teractive writ<strong>in</strong>g programs and collaborative projectwork (for extensive discussion, see Crook 1994; van Lier 1996).3 Wells dist<strong>in</strong>guishes betwecn third turns that cvaluate or provide follow-up (29-30). Seealso Barnes (1976).4 Symmetry and cont<strong>in</strong>gency are closcly related but not synonymous. Symmetry is a


~ (1CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALI< 1055structural discourse term, the result of <strong>in</strong>teractional work by participants. Cont<strong>in</strong>gencyis a cognitive quality. They usually occur together, but this does not mean that they areidentical. As an analogy, light and heat often occur together, for example, <strong>in</strong> flames,sunlight, and light bulbs, but they are not the same.Gibson describes aJordance as follows: “The affordances of the environment are what itoffers the animal, what it provides or furnishes, either for good or ill . . . someth<strong>in</strong>g thatrcfers both to the environment and the animal. . . . It implies the complementarity ofthe animal and the environment” (1 27). The tcrm afordance specifically refers to thoseaspects of the l<strong>in</strong>guistic environment that become perceivable by the learner as a resultof mean<strong>in</strong>gful activity. Affordance is neither the external language nor the learner’s<strong>in</strong>ternalization of it. It refers to the relations among thc engaged learner, mean<strong>in</strong>gfulsigns, and relevant properties of the real world.ReferencesAston, G. (1986) “Trouble-shoot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Interaction with Learncrs: The More the Merrier?”Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 7: 12343.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, J.M. and Heritage, J. (eds) (1984) Structures of <strong>Social</strong> Action: Studies <strong>in</strong> ConversationAnalysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Bakht<strong>in</strong>, M.M. (1981) The Dialogic Imag<strong>in</strong>ation. Aust<strong>in</strong>: University of Texas Press.Barncs, D. (1976) From Communication to Curriculum. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Bourdieu, P. (1990) The Logic $Practice. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Bourdieu, P. and Passeron, J.C. (1977) Reproduction <strong>in</strong> Education, Society and Culture. London:Sagc.Bowers, C.A., and Fl<strong>in</strong>ders, D. J. (1990) Responsive <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: An Ecological Approach to ClassroomPatterns oflanguage, Culture, and Thought. NewYork: Teachers Coll. Press.Bruner, J. (1983) Child’s Talk: Learn<strong>in</strong>g to Use <strong>Language</strong>. NewYork: Norton.Chaudron, C. (1988) Second <strong>Language</strong> Classrooms: Research on <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Carnbridgc:Cambridge University Press.Crook, C. ( 1994) Computers and the Collaborative Experience of Learn<strong>in</strong>g. London: Routledge.Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K. and Whalen, S. (1993) Talented Teenagers:The Roots $Successand Failure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Darder, A. (1991) Culture and Power <strong>in</strong> the Classroom:A Critical Foundationfor Bicultural Education.NewYork: Berg<strong>in</strong>.Deci, E.L., and Ryan, R.M. (1992) “The Initiation and Regulation of Intr<strong>in</strong>sically MotivatcdLearn<strong>in</strong>g and Achievement” <strong>in</strong> Achievement and Motivation:A <strong>Social</strong>-Development Perspective,Ann K. Boggiano andThane S. Pittman (eds) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.985) Intr<strong>in</strong>sic Motivation and SelfDeterm<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong> Human Behavior. NewYork: Plenum.Deci, E.L.,Vallerand, R.J., Pellcticr, L.G. and Ryan, R.M. (1991) “Motivation and Education:The Self-Determ<strong>in</strong>ation Perspective.” Educational Psychologist 26: 325-46.Duncan, S. (1972) “Some Signals and Rules for Tak<strong>in</strong>g Speak<strong>in</strong>gTurns <strong>in</strong> Conversation.”]ournal$Personality and <strong>Social</strong> Psychology 23: 283-92.Edwards, A.D., andwcstgatc, D.P.G. (1987) Investigat<strong>in</strong>g Classroom Talk. London: Palmer.Egan, K. (1986) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> as Storytell<strong>in</strong>g. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Ellis, R. (1994) The Study $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Fairclough, N. (1992) ‘The Appropriacy of “Appropriateness”’, <strong>in</strong> Critical <strong>Language</strong> Awareness,Norman Fairclough (ed.). London: Longman: 33-56.Freire, P. (1972) Pedagogy $the Oppressed. NewYork: Herder.Garf<strong>in</strong>kel, H. (1967) Studies <strong>in</strong> Ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice.Gass, S.M., and Madden, C.G. (1985) Input <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Rowlcy: Newbury.


~ (106 LEO VAN LIERGibson, J. J. (1979) The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton.Giddens, A. (1984) The Constitution $Society. Berkeley: University of California Press.Graumann, C.F. (1990) “Perspectival Structure and Dynamics <strong>in</strong> Dialogue”, <strong>in</strong> The Dynamics $Dialogue, Markovi, I. and Foppa, K. (eds). NewYork: Harvester.Gumpers, J. J. (1992)“<strong>Context</strong>ualization and Understand<strong>in</strong>g”, <strong>in</strong> Reth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Context</strong>: <strong>Language</strong> asan Interactive Phenomenon, Duranti, A. and Goodw<strong>in</strong>, C. (eds). Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.Heritage, J. (1984) “A Change-of-state Token and Aspects of Its Sequential Placement”, <strong>in</strong>Atk<strong>in</strong>son and Heritage: 299-345Kasper, G. (1 984) “Variation <strong>in</strong> Speech Act Realization”, <strong>in</strong> Variation <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition, Gass, S., Madden, C., Preston, D. and Sel<strong>in</strong>ker, L. (eds) Clevedon, Eng.:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.K<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>ger, C. (1994) “Learner Initiative <strong>in</strong> Conversation Management: An Application of VanLier’s Pilot Cod<strong>in</strong>g Scheme.” Modern <strong>Language</strong> journal 78: 2940.Lii, J.H. “NoTalk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Class.” NewYork Times 10 Apr. 1994, educ. supp 7.Long, M. and Porter, P; (1985) “Group Work, Interlanguage Talk, and Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition.” TESOL Quarterly 19: 207-28.Marcus, H. and Zajonc, R.B. (1985) “The Cognitive Perspective <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology”, <strong>in</strong>Theory and Method, L<strong>in</strong>dzey, G. and Aronson, E. (eds). New York: Random. 137-230.Vol. 1 of Handbook $<strong>Social</strong> Psychology. 3rd ed.Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J. (1 994) “Children’s Voices: Talk, Knowledge, and Identity”, <strong>in</strong> Research<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Language</strong>and Literacy <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>, Graddol, D., Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J. and Sticrer, B. (eds). Clevedon, Eng:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters: 131-50.Mehan, H. (1979) Learn<strong>in</strong>g Lessons: <strong>Social</strong> Organization <strong>in</strong> the Classroom. Cambridge: HarvardUniversity Press.Merrow, J. (1995) “Four Million Computers Can Be Wrong!” Education Week 29 Mar. 1995:52+.Moerman, M. ( 1988) Talk<strong>in</strong>g Culture: Ethnography and Conversotion Analysis. Philadelphia:University of Pennsylvania Press.Newman, D., Griff<strong>in</strong>, P. and Cole, M. (1989) The Construction Zone: Work<strong>in</strong>gfor Cognitive Change<strong>in</strong> School. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Parker, K. and Chaudron, C. (1987) “The Effect of L<strong>in</strong>guistic Simplifications and ElaborativeModifications on L2 Comprehension.” University .f Hawaii Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> as aSecond <strong>Language</strong> 6.2: 107-3 3.Pica, T. (1987) “Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, <strong>Social</strong> Interaction, and the Classroom” AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 7: 125.1992) “The Textual Outcomes of Native Speaker-Non-native Speaker Negotiation:What Do They Reveal about Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g?”, <strong>in</strong> Text and <strong>Context</strong>: Cross-Discipl<strong>in</strong>ary Perspectives on <strong>Language</strong> Study, Kramsch, C. and McConnell-G<strong>in</strong>et, S. (eds).Lex<strong>in</strong>gton: Heath: 198-237.Pica, T., and Doughty, C. (1985) “Non-native Speaker Interaction <strong>in</strong> the ESL Classroom”, <strong>in</strong>Input <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, Gass, S. and Madden, C. (eds). Rowley: Newbury:115-32.Pica, T., Young, R. and Doughty, C. (1987) “The Impact of Interaction on Comprehension.”TESOL Quarter1,21: 737-58.Platt, E., and Brooks, F.B. (1994) “The ‘ Acquisition-Rich Environment’ Revisited.” Modern<strong>Language</strong>journal78: 497-5 1 1.Pomerantz, A. (1984) “Pursu<strong>in</strong>g a Response” <strong>in</strong> Atk<strong>in</strong>son and Heritage: 152-63.Rommetveit, R. (1974) On Message Structure. NewYork: Wiley.Rudduck, J. ( 1997) Innovation and Change: Develop<strong>in</strong>g Involvement and Understand<strong>in</strong>g. MiltonKeynes: Open University Press.


~ (1~ (1996b)CONSTRAINTS AND RESOURCES IN CLASSROOM TALI< 107Rutherford, W. (1987) Second <strong>Language</strong> Grammar: Learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. London: Longman.Sacks, H., Schcgloff, E. and Jefferson, G. (1974) “A Simplest Systematics for the OrganizationofTurnTak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Conversation.” <strong>Language</strong> 50: 696-735.Schegloff, E.A., Jefferson, G. and Sacks, H. (1977) “The Preference for Self-correction <strong>in</strong> theOrganization of Repair <strong>in</strong> Conversation.” <strong>Language</strong> 53: 361-82.Shor, I. (1992) Empower<strong>in</strong>g Education: Critical <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>for <strong>Social</strong> Change. Chicago: University ofChicago Press.Shuy, R. (1991)“Secretary Bennett’s <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: An Argument for Responsive <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>”, <strong>in</strong> TheEnlightened Eye: Qualitative Inquiry and the Enhancement ofEducational Practice, Elliott Eisner(ed.). NewYork: Macmillan.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J.M., and Brazil, D. (1982) Teacher Talk. Oxford: Oxford University Press.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J.M., and Coulthard, M. (1975) Towards an Analysis OJ Discourse. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.Tharp, R. and Gallimorc, R. (1988) Rous<strong>in</strong>g M<strong>in</strong>ds to Ltfe. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.Twitch<strong>in</strong>, J. (1993) European Awareness <strong>in</strong> Primary Schools. Mosaic Films. Central Bureau forEduc. Vis<strong>its</strong> and Exchanges.van Licr, L. (1988) The Classroom and the <strong>Language</strong> Learner: Ethnography and Second-<strong>Language</strong>Classroom Research. London: Longman.992) “Not the N<strong>in</strong>e O’clock L<strong>in</strong>guistics Class: Investigat<strong>in</strong>g Cont<strong>in</strong>gency Grammar.”<strong>Language</strong> Awareness 1.2: 91-108.-~ (1994) “<strong>Language</strong> Awareness, Cont<strong>in</strong>gency, and Interaction”, <strong>in</strong> Consciousness <strong>in</strong> Second<strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g, Hulstijn, J. and Schmidt, R. (eds) AILA Review 1 1 : 69-82.__ (1996a) Interaction <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Language</strong> Curriculum: Awareness, Autonomy and Authenticity.London: Longman.“<strong>Social</strong> Interaction and Cooperative Learn<strong>in</strong>g at, around, with, and throughComputers.” Eleventh World Congress of Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics (AILA 96). University ofJyvaskyla, F<strong>in</strong>land. 5 Aug. 1996.van Lier, L., and Matsuo, N. (1995) “Varieties of Conversational Experience.” Unpublishedessay. Monterey Institute of International Studies.van Manen, M. (1997) TheTact ofreach<strong>in</strong>g:The Mean<strong>in</strong>g of~edaSogicalThough~ulness. Albany: StatcUniversity of NewYork Press.Vygotsky, Lev, S. (I 978) M<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> Society. Cambridge: Harvard university Press.Wells, G. (1993) “Reevaluat<strong>in</strong>g the IRF Sequence: A Proposal for thc Articulation of Theoriesof Activity and Discourse for the Analysis of <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Classroom.”L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Education 5: 1-37.Wcrtsch, J.V. (1991) Voices ofthe M<strong>in</strong>d: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action. Cambridge:Harvard Univcrsity Press.Wittgenste<strong>in</strong>, L. (1958) Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Young, R. (1992) Critical Theory and Classroom Talk. Clcvedon, Eng: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Yule, G. (1990) “Interactive Conflict Resolution <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>.” World <strong>English</strong>es 9: 53-62.


Chapter 6Celia RobertsLANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGESOCIALISATION IN AND THROUGHDISCOURSE?Towavds a Redef<strong>in</strong>ition of the Doma<strong>in</strong>of SLAIntroductionVER THE LAST TWENTY YEARS SLA studies have not ignored issues of0 discoursc and the social context. But often the rcfcrcnces to social or socio-culturalcontext give it only a marg<strong>in</strong>al role <strong>in</strong> the processes of language development. Equally, thereis relatively little concern with the social import of sccond language development. By ‘socialimport’ I mean the effect on social idcntitics, groups and relationships of the multitude of<strong>in</strong>tercultural <strong>in</strong>teractions which take placc every day. 1 also <strong>in</strong>clude the effect of these<strong>in</strong>tercultural encounters on <strong>in</strong>dividuals ~ who are, themselves, part of these wider socialforccs.So, this paper is concerned with second language development and the immediate socialcontext <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals succeed, or fail, to construct local mean<strong>in</strong>g together; with howthey connect it to wider knowledge sets and experiences and the social outcomes of this.It is also concerned with the wider social context. In particular, how social processes areconstituted <strong>in</strong> such <strong>in</strong>teractions and how these processes <strong>in</strong> turn feed back <strong>in</strong>to <strong>in</strong>terculturalencounters and so providc the conditions (or not) for discourse production and<strong>in</strong>terpretation.<strong>Language</strong> socialisation rather than language acquisition better describes how learnerscome to produce and <strong>in</strong>terpret discoursc anti how such learn<strong>in</strong>g is supported (or not) bythe assumptions of society at largc about multil<strong>in</strong>gualism and second language learners.These issues are particularly salient when research<strong>in</strong>g SLA with m<strong>in</strong>ority group workers.And hcrc, Gumperz’s notion of contextualisation illum<strong>in</strong>ates the ways <strong>in</strong> which localunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs and misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs have an effect both on thc immediate context forlearn<strong>in</strong>g and on the wider assumptions and ideologies about l<strong>in</strong>guistic m<strong>in</strong>ority groupswhich also enter <strong>in</strong>to and have an effect on local <strong>in</strong>teractions and conditions for discoursedevelopment.


a<strong>in</strong>toLANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 109The transformation of many cities <strong>in</strong> Western and Northern Europe from monol<strong>in</strong>gualto multil<strong>in</strong>gual environments creates crucial sites for the study of second languagedevelopment. Adult m<strong>in</strong>ority workers who are struggl<strong>in</strong>g to make a new life for themselvesrepresent a particularly significant group when researchers are consider<strong>in</strong>g what constitutesthe doma<strong>in</strong> for second language acquisition studies. For many of them, contact with thcmajority group is <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>gs ~ at work or <strong>in</strong> bureaucratic encounters - andthese become the sites where their competence <strong>in</strong> the new language is put to the test.Thesesett<strong>in</strong>gs provide far from ideal conditions for language learn<strong>in</strong>g and yet they may be the onlyones where the new language is used at all. Chart<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>teractions and relative progressof this group <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>different and often hostile world drives the researcher to conccptualise<strong>in</strong>dividuals not simply as language learners but as social be<strong>in</strong>gs struggl<strong>in</strong>g to manage oftenconflict<strong>in</strong>g goals. After all, the researcher may be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> their language development,but the m<strong>in</strong>ority workers are concerned with gett<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs done. As Bourdieuasserts: “What speaks is not utterance, the language, but the whole social person” (Bourdieu,1977, p. 653). Lookmg at the ‘whole social person’ argues for a more holistic approach tosecond language development than orthodox SLA studies offers, both theoretically andmethodologically.Lim<strong>its</strong> to a social perspective on SLAInteraction and pragmatics <strong>in</strong> SLAThere is of course an extensive literature on <strong>in</strong>teraction studies <strong>in</strong> SLA which exam<strong>in</strong>esthe conversational devices which foster certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic features. In a more dialogicve<strong>in</strong>, recent Vygotskian approaches focus on the negotiation of ‘comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put’ <strong>in</strong>social <strong>in</strong>teraction. But despite the concentration on collaborative dialogue, language is stillconceived of as a product to be acquired rather than as a discourse ~ social process ~which members of a community are socialised. Lcarners are now characterised as ‘sociallyconstituted’, as “responsible agents with dispositions to th<strong>in</strong>k and act <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> waysrooted <strong>in</strong> their discursive histories” (Lantolf and Pavlenko, 1995, p. 1 16) but the goal ofdialogic learn<strong>in</strong>g is still the ability to deploy l<strong>in</strong>guistic phenomena. Methodologically,the analysis tends to focus on a particular feature of language rather than exam<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> depthlocal <strong>in</strong>terpretations and reactions. Unsurpris<strong>in</strong>gly, therefore, there is little or no ethnographicevidence to support conclusions drawn. The relatively new field of <strong>in</strong>terlanguagepragmatics would seem to be a more promis<strong>in</strong>g area for look<strong>in</strong>g at the whole social person.But despite <strong>its</strong> concern with contextual factors, it is the narrow concept of the learner andher capacity to rcalise specific speech acts which generate the key research questions. Theendeavour rema<strong>in</strong>s an essentially cognitive one as the authors’ recognition of the potentialsignificance of sociocultural issues implies:It would be a mistake to view developmental issues <strong>in</strong> ILP (<strong>in</strong>terlanguage pragmatics)<strong>in</strong> purely cognitive terms because the strategies for l<strong>in</strong>guistic action are so closely tiedto self-identity and social identity. (Kasper and Schmidt, 1996, p. 159)To date, however, these issues of social identity and, <strong>in</strong>deed, other social issues outside theimmediate contcxt of utterance, have not figured to any significant extent <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguagcpragmatics.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the <strong>in</strong>teraction and pragmatics studies <strong>in</strong> SLA literature cont<strong>in</strong>ue the tendency<strong>in</strong> SLA more generally to reify language so that French, <strong>English</strong> and so on are treated


unproblematically as homogenised ‘target languages’. This essentialis<strong>in</strong>g of a languageassumes that there is only one variety to be learned and that the language and communicativestyle of the broker’s yard or the baker’s is similar to that of the standard variety.A sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic perspective on SLAFrom a sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic po<strong>in</strong>t of view ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA studies rema<strong>in</strong> asocial - the socialimport of learn<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>teract through language rema<strong>in</strong>s hidden. A sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic perspectivcshifts away from the l<strong>in</strong>guistic system and from a concern with specific items of pragmaticand discourse developmcnt to look<strong>in</strong>g at language as a set of norms, at languagediversity and ideologies. Specifically, this more holistic view is concerned with <strong>in</strong>teractionas communicative practice and how such practice helps us to understand larger social forcesand, <strong>in</strong> turn, their impact on <strong>in</strong>teractions. This connect<strong>in</strong>g up the macro and the micro <strong>in</strong>sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic theory gives due recognition to <strong>in</strong>tcractions as sites where m<strong>in</strong>ority workersare not simply exposed to and able to negotiatc comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put but are social actorsstruggl<strong>in</strong>g to get th<strong>in</strong>gs done with their emergent competence <strong>in</strong> a second language.Rcconstitut<strong>in</strong>g learners as social actors br<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>to focus issues of social identity. Thereis a develop<strong>in</strong>g literature on language and social identity and <strong>its</strong> relation to SLA <strong>in</strong> whichapplied and sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics meet. With<strong>in</strong> this literature, the learner is understood as a personwith multiple identities, many of them contradictory. Identity is dynamic across time andplace and language usc, social identity and ethnicity are <strong>in</strong>extricably l<strong>in</strong>ked and understoodwith<strong>in</strong> larger social proccsses. For example, Pierce (1 995) discusses the personal and social<strong>in</strong>vestmcnts <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> as a sccond languagc among adult ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority women,how these are observable <strong>in</strong> their <strong>in</strong>teractions and the ways <strong>in</strong> which certa<strong>in</strong> social identitiesare foregrounded or backgrounded. Once notions of social identity are called up, thcdom<strong>in</strong>ant tradition of SLA as an asocial phenomcnon is put <strong>in</strong>to qucstion.<strong>Language</strong> socialisationOne response to the critique of the relatively asocial character of SLA is to suggest languagesocialisation as an alternative perspective. Thc concept was orig<strong>in</strong>ally developed with<strong>in</strong>anthropology to describe the process whereby a child becomes an emcrgent member of thecommunity <strong>in</strong> which they are grow<strong>in</strong>g up. More rccently it has becn extended to <strong>in</strong>cludesecond language socialisation (SLS) (Duff, 1996). It <strong>in</strong>cludes both the socialisation requiredto use language <strong>in</strong> specific <strong>in</strong>teractional sequenccs and the process of socialisation throughlanguagc - the <strong>in</strong>direct mcans of develop<strong>in</strong>g socio-cultural knowledge. Where SLA has usedmodell<strong>in</strong>g and experimentation as the dom<strong>in</strong>ant paradigm to research how l<strong>in</strong>guistic featurcsare attended to, stored and accessed, language socialisation studies have used participantobservation. Studies of adult m<strong>in</strong>ority workers based on naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g language useprovide data that more nearly resembles child language socialisation studies. Such data canoffer <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to the SLS process provided that it is also supplemented by ethnographicdata on speech events and local histories and identities of participants.In the follow<strong>in</strong>g example (from Bremcr et al., 1996, pp. 60-61) Marcello, an Italianworker <strong>in</strong> Germany, is be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviewcd by T, a counsellor <strong>in</strong> the Job Centre. Marcello wasone of the <strong>in</strong>formants on the European Sciencc Foundation project on natural secondlanguagc acquisition. He had been <strong>in</strong> Heidelberg for about a year when this <strong>in</strong>terview wastaped, hav<strong>in</strong>g come to Germany as a real beg<strong>in</strong>ner. He was still seek<strong>in</strong>g work and the<strong>in</strong>terview with the counsellor was both an opportunity to f<strong>in</strong>d out about work possibilities


LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 111and to use h s develop<strong>in</strong>g German. As an example of language socialisation, Marcello neededto be socialised <strong>in</strong>to the specific genre of counsell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews and use this <strong>in</strong>teraction asan opportunity to develop his socio-cultural knowledge of how bureaucracies work, howwork is categorised, what the goals of such an <strong>in</strong>terview are likely to be and so on:Data Example 112345678910M: wir muss vergessen T:M: jawe have to forgetja + gut + dann hatten wir die saache fur heutok good so we’re through for todayund wenn sie also <strong>in</strong> zukunft noch fragen haben kommen sie bei mir vorbei jaand $you have any questions <strong>in</strong> futureyou’ll look <strong>in</strong> okYesT: ok give me a call okM: so und jetzt muss ich gehenso and now I must goT: M: < > T: wicdersehenbyeM: wiedersehen dankebye thank youTranscription Conventions+ short pauseadditional comments on way of speahng etc.[ I overlap(XX.1<strong>in</strong>audible or omitted wordAt one level, this could be construed as a simple case of pragmatic failure. Marcellofails to understand the pre-clos<strong>in</strong>g signals of T <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g “ja”,“gut” and “dann hatten wir diesache fur heut” and advice for the future. It is only with the non-verbal cues that Marcellorealises that they are <strong>in</strong> the middle of leave tak<strong>in</strong>g. His <strong>in</strong>terpretive difficulty is not surpris<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong>ce as Scarcella (1982) has argued conversational features such as greet<strong>in</strong>gs are acquiredbefore pre-clos<strong>in</strong>gs. But this sequence is also an unusually explicit moment of languagesocialisation when at l<strong>in</strong>e 6 Marcello topicalises the act of departure.This is more than justa matter of pick<strong>in</strong>g up on some pre-clos<strong>in</strong>g signals, and it is worth mention<strong>in</strong>g here that thecrucial nonverbal signals which are part of the <strong>in</strong>teractive environment arc rarely considered<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic pragmatics.In order for Marcello to manage this type of <strong>in</strong>stitutional discourse and understandwhen, how, and why the encounter closes at a particular po<strong>in</strong>t, he needs to be socialised<strong>in</strong>to the norms, role relationships and goals of ‘gatekecp<strong>in</strong>g’ encounters. Ethnographicevidence from m<strong>in</strong>ority workers’ experience of counsell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews (Bremer et al., 1996;Gumperz, 1982~1, 1982,; Roberts et al., 1992) suggests that issues of speaker rights andresponsibilitics, expectations about specific goals and the boundaries of what constitutes


112 CELIA ROBERTSthc personal may differ markedly from that of the majority gatekeepers. In this <strong>in</strong>stance,onc of the difficulties for Marcello is the relatively <strong>in</strong>conclusive way <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>terviewappears to end. Whereas counsellors see such <strong>in</strong>terviews as an opportunity to discuss workprcferences, m<strong>in</strong>ority workers are more likely to expcct to be given specific <strong>in</strong>formationabout particular jobs. Once this <strong>in</strong>formation has bccn given, they expect the <strong>in</strong>terview tobe term<strong>in</strong>ated. But <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance, the counsellor ends the encounter once some<strong>in</strong>formation has been elicited from the clicnt and some advice given.Another frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g example of difference surrounds the issue of thecategorisation of work experience around skills and responsibilities and often, therefore,around social status. In the next cxample (from Bremer et al., 1996, p. 63), Ilhami, aTurkishworker from Germany, is <strong>in</strong>terviewed for an apprenticeship <strong>in</strong> a garage and is asked whatjob his father does:Data Example 21234567891011T: e was arbeit’ denn dc<strong>in</strong> vater was macht der von bcrufwhat work doesyourfather do what is his jobI: metallberuf [und]metal lobT. [ja] undok andI: (wxxx) schnellprcsse (wxxx) stamp<strong>in</strong>g presT: <strong>in</strong> der schnellprcsse <strong>in</strong> w.<strong>in</strong> the stamp<strong>in</strong>g press <strong>in</strong> w.I: [ja] mhmYesT: (ja] und dort tut er metalland he does metal thereI: metall [und]metal andT: [aha]I: die machen auch das macht auch papicrthey also make it makes paper tooT: mhm ah so ist dasmhm ah <strong>its</strong> like that(For transcription conventions see Data Example 1 .)This question and answer sequence is unsatisfactory because Ilhami is unaware ofthe underly<strong>in</strong>g question which is about the social status of his father’s job and so of hisfather’s class position. The garage owner <strong>in</strong>tcrrupts on scveral occasions to elicit a morespecific reply but nevcr makes explicit what he wants to know. Thcse are examples of‘socio-pragmatic failure’ <strong>in</strong> Thomas’ terms (Thomas, 1983). But this tcrm tends toemphasise the pragmatic difficulties rather than highlight the process of language socialisationwhich <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stancc concerns the discourse around class position <strong>in</strong> a gatekeep<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>terview.


LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 113Some problems with the model of SLSSLS as an apprenticeship modelSLS can be seen as an apprenticeship model. The learner ovcr time participates <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>teractional lifc of the new community and is gradually <strong>in</strong>ducted <strong>in</strong>to what arc taken to bc<strong>its</strong> pre-exist<strong>in</strong>g discourses. Such a model implies a ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g by do<strong>in</strong>g’ approach <strong>in</strong> which,for example, the adult m<strong>in</strong>ority worker learns from her <strong>in</strong>teractions with her supervisorhow to evaluate her role <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>g with compla<strong>in</strong>ts about quality (Clyne, 1995). Thislearn<strong>in</strong>g is part of what Rogoff (1 984) calls “the social orchestration of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g throughcultural <strong>in</strong>stitutions and normative techniques of problem solv<strong>in</strong>g” (p. 5). But socialisationis more than cognitive learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> social contexts. It assumes a process of ‘belong<strong>in</strong>g’, ofbe<strong>in</strong>g part of the ‘new community’. And this is wherc the notion of SLS runs <strong>in</strong>to difficultiess<strong>in</strong>ce it “assumes that groups are sociocultural totalities and that people eventually arrive atan endpo<strong>in</strong>t of expert belong<strong>in</strong>g” (Rampton, 1995b, p. 487).The apprenticeship model of SLS is, therefore, only part of the story. It does not fullytake account of the rclationship between the discourses to which learners arc exposed andthe learners themselves. In other words it is an overly functionalist model. It underplaysthe total role and self identity <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and us<strong>in</strong>g a new discourse and theconstructed nature of <strong>in</strong>tercultural contact <strong>in</strong> plural and fragmented societies.So, it is not possible to talk unproblematically of socialisation through language as themeans of develop<strong>in</strong>g sociocultural knowledge as if thcre is a stable body of such knowledge.The idea of gradually be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ducted <strong>in</strong>to a community’s pre-exist<strong>in</strong>g discourses suggests asimple, functional model which does not accord with our data of naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tercultural encounters. In other words, such events are not simply opportunities for thetransmission, however <strong>in</strong>directly, of the necessary socio-cultural knowledge, but they aresites where social identities are constructed, where the <strong>in</strong>teractants arc positioned andposition themselves. Pcople speak from with<strong>in</strong> a particular discursive formation. In the caseof m<strong>in</strong>ority workers, this <strong>in</strong>cludes the discourses of cthnic and class position, the widerdiscourses of racism, their communicative competence and perceived competence and thclocal position<strong>in</strong>g which emerges from each <strong>in</strong>teraction.Position<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> and through discourseThe detailed ways <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>teractants position themselves and are positioned illum<strong>in</strong>atessome of the problems with an orthodox view of language socialisation. Different m<strong>in</strong>orityworkers <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions and <strong>in</strong> the process of language socialisation <strong>in</strong> different waysand are themselves def<strong>in</strong>cd relatively differently.There are numerous examples of this position<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Second <strong>Language</strong> Ly AdultImmigrants project (Bremer et al., 1996; Perdue, 1993). A contrastive study of two Italian<strong>in</strong>formants <strong>in</strong> Brita<strong>in</strong> who are enquir<strong>in</strong>g about buy<strong>in</strong>g property <strong>in</strong> an estate agents (Robertsand Simonot, 1987) shows how they are positioned differently. One of Santo’s strategieswhich helps to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> conversational <strong>in</strong>volvement is to make general, evaluativecomments :Data Example 31 N: then you might get one for about fifty or sixty + or say forty eight sixty someth<strong>in</strong>glike that


114 CELIA ROBERTS2 S: very expensive area anyway3 N: well this/ this is expensive this is less expensiveBy contrast Andrea’s strategies are reactive and he tends to develop only those themes whichthe estate agent has implicitly sanctioned:1 N: blackstock road er thats a one bedroom flat2 A: yeah3 ON: <strong>its</strong> not two bedrooms4 A: mhm(Roberts and Simonot, 1987)Santo’s socialisation <strong>in</strong>to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g conversational <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> service encountersmeans that he elic<strong>its</strong> more helpful and extended comments from the clerk. Andrea’sencounters are less successful, do not produce opportunities for learn<strong>in</strong>g how to do thistype of conversational <strong>in</strong>volvement and, as ethnographic evidence shows, cumulatively,position Andrea as marg<strong>in</strong>alised discursively and socially (Roberts and Simonot, 1987).For other <strong>in</strong>formants <strong>in</strong> this project, the learn<strong>in</strong>g of socio-cultural knowledge isrefracted through their experience of liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a racist society. For example, Abdelmalck, aMoroccan worker <strong>in</strong> France, talks of his politeness strategies and how he has learnt ways ofbe<strong>in</strong>g particularly polite <strong>in</strong> order to get a favourable response from the most racist of his<strong>in</strong>terlocutors (Bremer et al., 1996).Data from multil<strong>in</strong>gual British factories also shows how m<strong>in</strong>ority workers positionthemselves strategically <strong>in</strong> order to attempt to co-construct an argument <strong>in</strong> their favour. Inthis example (Roberts et a]., 1992, p. 39), the m<strong>in</strong>ority worker, IA, is try<strong>in</strong>g to negotiate ajob for his son <strong>in</strong> the same factory as he works <strong>in</strong>.The problem is that his son is only sixteenyears old and is not allowed to work the regulation 55 hour week:Data Example 41 Mrs B: Can’t help him.2 IA: What for?3 Mrs B: All the men <strong>in</strong> this mill arc on 55 hours4 IA: 55 hours?5 MrsB: All themen6 IA: Oldmen?7 Mrs B: All men8 IA: Young men and just 8 hours every day9 Mrs S: Rut Mrs B says not the OLD men. All the men - everybody - must work 5510 Mrs B:hoursLadies work 40 hours11 IA: This is young boy, the same like lady (laughter)12 They are too young. If not wanted then too long time . . . just 40 hours perweekDespite the misunderstand<strong>in</strong>g at l<strong>in</strong>e 6, IA, at l<strong>in</strong>es 8 and 11-12, beg<strong>in</strong>s to negotiatehis way around the company rule. He does this by capp<strong>in</strong>g Mrs B’s assertion with his ownassertions about young men and prevents this from becom<strong>in</strong>g a distanc<strong>in</strong>g strategy byclaim<strong>in</strong>g solidarity through the joke that young men are similar to ladies. The conditions


LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 115for further<strong>in</strong>g his sociocultural competence are <strong>in</strong> place s<strong>in</strong>ce his assertions are respondedto by Mrs B and the encounter ends with her agrec<strong>in</strong>g to talk to the overlooker about herson.The cont<strong>in</strong>gent nature of such <strong>in</strong>teractional position<strong>in</strong>g means that conditions for theproduction and <strong>in</strong>terpretation of discourse vary from <strong>in</strong>teraction to <strong>in</strong>teraction. But theseconditions are also constra<strong>in</strong>ed by wider socio-political formations ~ such as the <strong>in</strong>equalitiesthat exist <strong>in</strong> a stratified multi-l<strong>in</strong>gual society. So a model of second language socialisationneeds to <strong>in</strong>clude an understand<strong>in</strong>g of the ideologies which feed <strong>in</strong>to and are constructedout of <strong>in</strong>teractions.<strong>Language</strong> practice and ideologyThe notion of language as ‘social practice’ helps us to see the ideological <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions.There has been a lot of discussion around the term ‘practice’ <strong>in</strong> what has been called theNew Literacy Studies <strong>in</strong> Brita<strong>in</strong> and the USA. ‘Practice’ or more usefully ‘practiccs’ aremore than action and events. In the case of literacy practices for example, they <strong>in</strong>clude boththe literacy event and the knowledge and assumptions about what this event is and whatgives it mean<strong>in</strong>g. For example, what counts as literacy <strong>in</strong> a subgroup is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by those<strong>in</strong> dom<strong>in</strong>ant positions <strong>in</strong> a society. Literacy practices, therefore, are profoundly associatedwith identity and social position.The notion of ‘practice’ has also been used and debated <strong>in</strong> critical and anthropologicall<strong>in</strong>guistics as both action and the ideologies which surround it. Fairclough (1 992) makes thepo<strong>in</strong>t that language practices are constructed not only out of sociocultural knowledge butout of the discourses which were produced earlier, are produccd <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>teraction and <strong>in</strong>subsequent discourses. So, for example, with<strong>in</strong> this critical perspective, questions have beenraised about taken for granted notions of what constitutes a speaker of a particular language.what is a non-native speaker, what certa<strong>in</strong> groups count as ‘target language’ and so on.However, this problematis<strong>in</strong>g work, although it has <strong>in</strong>fluenced applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics, has hadlittle <strong>in</strong>fluence with<strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA. For example, the m<strong>in</strong>ority worker will bepositioned, by the l<strong>in</strong>guistic ideologies that circulate, as a ‘non-native’ , ‘second languagespeaker’, ‘poor communicator’ and so on.These feed <strong>in</strong>to the <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>its</strong>elf and feed offit to recirculate <strong>in</strong> the wider discourses around language and cthnicity.With<strong>in</strong> the British tradition there are two compet<strong>in</strong>g sets of discourses around ethnicity.The first has been widely reflected <strong>in</strong> government policy and popular discourse. This tendsto essentialise ethnic groups, equate land, language and ethnicity and cast m<strong>in</strong>ority ethnicgroups as <strong>in</strong>competent <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. (See Gilroy, 1987, for a discussion). In the Netherlands,van Dijk and his associates have traced similar processes <strong>in</strong> the discourses of elite groupswhich showhow cthnic belicfs are strategically expressed, acquired and distributed throughoutthe dom<strong>in</strong>ant group, that is as part of manag<strong>in</strong>g ethnic affairs and reproduc<strong>in</strong>g elitcpower and whitc group dom<strong>in</strong>ance. (Van Dijk et a/. , 1997, p. 165)An extreme example of this first set of discourses is from data gathered <strong>in</strong> multiethnicBritish workplaces dur<strong>in</strong>g the late 1970s (Roberts et a!. , 1992). A supervisor was runn<strong>in</strong>gthrough a rout<strong>in</strong>e list of questions <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> as part of a simple recruitment procedure.TheSouth Asian applicant had answered scveral questions about himself and his previous workexperience when he was asked “DO you speak <strong>English</strong>?” to which he replied, “What do youth<strong>in</strong>k I’m talk<strong>in</strong>g to you <strong>in</strong> now!”The current discourse that was circulat<strong>in</strong>g at the time


of116 CELIA ROBERTSassumed that someone of South Asian background was unlikely to speak <strong>English</strong> and theevidence to the contrary did not appear to dent the supervisor’s certa<strong>in</strong>ty that here wasanother non-<strong>English</strong> speaker. We could speculate on the outcomes of such an encounter andthe possible tensions set up for the <strong>in</strong>dividual m<strong>in</strong>ority worker who both needs to becomea participat<strong>in</strong>g member of a new community but who is <strong>in</strong>sult<strong>in</strong>gly positioned by a memberof that community as a non-<strong>English</strong> speakcr.The second set of discourses stem from the British-based Cultural Studies and, <strong>in</strong>particular, Hall’s (1988) notion of ‘new ethnicities’ and what Hewitt (1986) has called ‘localmultiracial vernaculars’. Recent research has shown the destabilisation of <strong>in</strong>heritedethnicities and the emergence of new ethnol<strong>in</strong>guistic identities which challenge theorthodox essentialist ideas of language and race (Gilroy, 1987; Hewitt, 1986; Rampton,1995a). This second set of discourses suggest that the process of second language socialisationis not a straightforward case of becom<strong>in</strong>g communicatively competent with<strong>in</strong> a fixedsociocultural group. It is rather a hybrid process of both learn<strong>in</strong>g to belong and yet rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gapart ~ hav<strong>in</strong>g several social identities and affiliations to several languages (Pierce, 1995).And this <strong>in</strong> turn has an impact on the wider social formations which themselves determ<strong>in</strong>ewhat socialisation means.<strong>Context</strong>ualisation and wider social processesThe l<strong>in</strong>k between SLS and these wider social processes is well illustrated <strong>in</strong> Gumperz’sstudies and their recent formulation <strong>in</strong> Eermans et al. (1 997). As Lev<strong>in</strong>son (1 997) <strong>in</strong> thesame volume asserts:it is the large-scale sociological effects of multitudes of small-scale <strong>in</strong>teractions thatstill partially fuels his (Gumperz’s) preoccupations with conversations, most evidentperhaps <strong>in</strong> his concern with thc plight of the <strong>in</strong>dividual caught up <strong>in</strong> these large-scaleforces. (p. 24)Lev<strong>in</strong>son captures here many of the elements central to a redef<strong>in</strong>ition of second languageacquisition as a social phenomenon. The focus on the micro - the f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed detail ofconversations ~ is l<strong>in</strong>ked to the macro - the wider social processes where social networks,identities and relationships are structured and restructured. What is significant for aredef<strong>in</strong>ition of SLA as part of this is the fact, as Gumperz asserts, that <strong>in</strong>dividuals are ‘caughtup <strong>in</strong> these large-scale forces’. So every encounter where there are language differences isboth an opportunity for language socialisation but also a site where identities andrelationships are played out through the dom<strong>in</strong>ant discourses of language and ethnicity,albeit with<strong>in</strong> a conventionally respectful <strong>in</strong>terpersonal framework. And this may be whyLev<strong>in</strong>son talks of the ‘plight’ of <strong>in</strong>dividuals s<strong>in</strong>ce the k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>tercultural <strong>in</strong>teractions thatrout<strong>in</strong>ely occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>gs are unequal encounters.Gumperz’s concern with the l<strong>in</strong>guistic dimension of social action shows how aspectsof l<strong>in</strong>guistic signall<strong>in</strong>g and cultural and social background knowledge work together toproduce communicative <strong>in</strong>volvement (or not) and outcomes at both <strong>in</strong>dividual and societallevels. His focus, therefore, <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with the discussion above is on communicative practice.In order to analysc these practices, Gumper7 draws on an eclectic bag of tools and, asLev<strong>in</strong>son ( 1997) suggests, there is none of the theoretical cleanl<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> his approach whichcan be found <strong>in</strong> Conversation Analysis. Gumpcrz draws on pragmatic notions <strong>in</strong> his<strong>in</strong>terpretive procedures but as part of a wider sociological <strong>in</strong>terest. Similarly, he has beenmuch <strong>in</strong>fluenced by Conversation Analysis. Like CA his analysis focuses on members’


LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 117procedures, elucidat<strong>in</strong>g how participants use their <strong>in</strong>teractional resources to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>teraction and create a level of mutual <strong>in</strong>terpretation. But Gumperz suggests CA is limited<strong>in</strong> as far as the participants’ <strong>in</strong>terpretations are seen as depend<strong>in</strong>g on sequential order<strong>in</strong>grather than on active <strong>in</strong>volvement. And this <strong>in</strong>volvement rests on two key terms forGumperz: ‘conversational <strong>in</strong>ference’ and ‘ contextualisation’.The capacity to understand <strong>in</strong>teractions and be socialised <strong>in</strong>to new communities ofpractice depends absolutely on some level of shared <strong>in</strong>ferential processes. This does notmean that <strong>in</strong>terlocutors share <strong>in</strong>terpretive conclusions about the mean<strong>in</strong>g of th<strong>in</strong>gs but thatways of process<strong>in</strong>g are sufficiently shared for them to engage with each other and be ableto undertake some level of ‘repair’. This is <strong>in</strong> no sense an absolute shar<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>ce anyconclusions over mean<strong>in</strong>g have to be accomplished, not taken for granted. And, as I havesuggested above, be<strong>in</strong>g competent is not a simple process of learn<strong>in</strong>g to manage <strong>in</strong>stitutionaldiscourse s<strong>in</strong>ce it is just these <strong>in</strong>stitutional discourses which may position the m<strong>in</strong>orityworker as resistant or at least ambiguous about the majority community.Nevertheless, the process of socialisation, however ambiguous, must rely on negotiat<strong>in</strong>glocal mean<strong>in</strong>gs through conversational <strong>in</strong>ference. The question is: What is the relationshipbetween the l<strong>in</strong>guistic signs that participants must process and conversational <strong>in</strong>ference?Gumperz has proposed the notion of ‘contextualisation cues’ to account for how these signsare taken up by <strong>in</strong>teractants. <strong>Context</strong>ualisation consists of:all activities by participants which make relevant, ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>, revise, cancel, any aspectof context which <strong>in</strong> turn is responsible for the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of an utterance <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>particular locus of occurrence. (Auer, 1992, p. 4)<strong>Context</strong>ualisation cues are def<strong>in</strong>ed as:constellations of surface features of message form . . . The means by which speakerssignal and listeners <strong>in</strong>terpret what the activity is, how semantic content is to beunderstood and how each sentence relates to what precedes or follows. (Gumperz,1982a, p. 13 1)These cues serve to foreground or make salient a particular l<strong>in</strong>guistic feature <strong>in</strong> relation toothers and so call up situated <strong>in</strong>terpretations. So, for example, the job counsellor <strong>in</strong> DataExample 1 signals a preclos<strong>in</strong>g sequence with the words ‘ok’ and ‘good’ both spoken withfall<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tonation. These contextualisation cues rout<strong>in</strong>ely mark the clos<strong>in</strong>g of a particulartopic or ‘activity’ (Gumperz, 1982a) <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>teraction.<strong>Context</strong>ualisation cues call up background knowledge which not only relates totraditional l<strong>in</strong>guistic and pragmatic knowledge but to social relations, rights and obligations,l<strong>in</strong>guistic ideologies and so on. In Ilhami’s case, mentioned above, the question about hisfather’s job with<strong>in</strong> the speech event of an <strong>in</strong>terview and occurr<strong>in</strong>g at that po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> thesequence is expected to cue <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation about social status. (See alsoTyler, 1995, on the<strong>in</strong>teractive negotiation of participant status.)Not only are contextualisation cues heavily charged with social and cultural freight, theways <strong>in</strong> which they <strong>in</strong>voke context mark them as problematic for the m<strong>in</strong>ority speaker.Lev<strong>in</strong>son, <strong>in</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g an analytic framework for contextualisation cues, makes theimportant po<strong>in</strong>t that message and context are not <strong>in</strong> opposition - the message can carrywith it or project the context (Lev<strong>in</strong>son, 1997).This makes the process of com<strong>in</strong>g to a levelof shared understand<strong>in</strong>g, and learn<strong>in</strong>g from this experience, an extremely complex one.Lev<strong>in</strong>son argues that contextualisation cues <strong>in</strong>voke context <strong>in</strong> particular ways. The cue is:


118 CELIA ROBERTSa conventional rem<strong>in</strong>der, like a knot <strong>in</strong> a handkerchief, where the content of the memois <strong>in</strong>fcrcntially determ<strong>in</strong>ed. Thus thc ‘CUC’ cannot bc said to cncode or dircctly <strong>in</strong>vokcthe <strong>in</strong>terpretive background, it’s simply a nudge to the <strong>in</strong>ferential process . . . The<strong>in</strong>terpretive process may be guided by general pragmatic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of a Gricean sort,and thus be <strong>in</strong> many ways universal <strong>in</strong> character: but the ‘cues’ are anyth<strong>in</strong>g butuniversal, <strong>in</strong>deed tend<strong>in</strong>g towards sub-cultural differentiation. (p. 29).There are several problems here for m<strong>in</strong>ority language speakers. Firstly, they have toidentify that thcrc is a CUC (for example a particular prosodic feature may have conventionalsignificance <strong>in</strong> one language or variety and not <strong>in</strong> another). Secondly, as Lev<strong>in</strong>son (1997)suggests, the socio-cultural background is not directly <strong>in</strong>voked by a particular CUC. It setsoff the <strong>in</strong>ferential process but unless <strong>in</strong>teractants share <strong>in</strong>terpretative procedures, there isno know<strong>in</strong>g what particular aspects of background knowledge may be called up. Thirdly,there is the fact that contextualisation cues arc reflexive. <strong>Language</strong> shapes context as muchas contcxt shapes language. So the majority and m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>in</strong>terlocutors may make differ<strong>in</strong>gsituated judgements both l<strong>in</strong>guistically and contextually moment by momcnt <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>teraction: a misread prosodic cue can <strong>in</strong>dex a sct of pre-suppositions about speakerperspcctivc, for example, which creates a new <strong>in</strong>terprctativc contcxt and sets the <strong>in</strong>teractionon a different foot<strong>in</strong>g.These iqsues are central to an understand<strong>in</strong>g of what it might mean to be socialised <strong>in</strong>toa second language.The mean<strong>in</strong>g of contextualisation cues can only be learnt by the l<strong>in</strong>guisticm<strong>in</strong>ority speaker if there is extended cxposurc to thc communicative practices of the groupor nctwork from which the majority languagc spcakcr comes.It is long-term exposurc to . . . communicative experience <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>stitutionalisednctworks of relationship and not language or community membership as suchthat lics at thc root of shared culture and shared <strong>in</strong>ferential practices. (Gumperz, 1997,p. 15)The need for this long cxposurc or immersion is that, as I have said, the relationshipbetween cue and context is <strong>in</strong>dircct. Cues function relationally, that is <strong>in</strong> contrast to whathas not been said, just been said and so on (Gumperz, 1992). Also many of the formalproperties of contextualisation cues are difficult to process, for cxamplc aspects of prosody.F<strong>in</strong>ally, they are about <strong>in</strong>vok<strong>in</strong>g context more than message and yet thc learner is orientatedtowards process<strong>in</strong>g thc message. In sum, contextualisation cues are slippery featurcs.Equally important is thc fact that contextual cues arc <strong>in</strong>dcxical markers of membcrshipof a particular group. Know<strong>in</strong>g how to use and <strong>in</strong>tcrpret a particular cue mcans at least forthat <strong>in</strong>teractional moment that you are a ‘belonger’. And <strong>in</strong> contrast, the failure to pickup on a cue not only creates misunderstand<strong>in</strong>g but scts the m<strong>in</strong>ority l<strong>in</strong>guistic speakerapart. She is not <strong>in</strong> that <strong>in</strong>teractional moment an emergent member of the same communicativecommunity. As a result, small <strong>in</strong>teractive differences can contribute to largesocial consequences both for the <strong>in</strong>dividual, for example, <strong>in</strong> fail<strong>in</strong>g to be allocated a houscor get a job and, <strong>in</strong> terms of the social order, feed<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to the structur<strong>in</strong>g of ethnic relations<strong>in</strong> a multil<strong>in</strong>gual society.<strong>Context</strong>ualisation, therefore, functions at the micro lcvel, both guid<strong>in</strong>g (or not) m<strong>in</strong>uteby m<strong>in</strong>ute <strong>in</strong>terpretative processes and also <strong>in</strong>dexes “those implicit values of relationalidentity and power that . . . go by the namc of culture” (Silverste<strong>in</strong>, 1992, p. 57) at thcmacro level. Local situated mcan<strong>in</strong>g and wider ideological concerns are caught up togcther.It is not simply a case of pragmatic failure or evcn of socialisation <strong>in</strong>to some stable body of


LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 119socio-cultural knowledge. Rather, it is a question of the struggle over mean<strong>in</strong>g at manylevels. Any item produced by either side may lack stability and create new and confus<strong>in</strong>g<strong>Context</strong>s. But there is also the struggle over mean<strong>in</strong>g at a more macro socio-political level.Here it is a question of what counts as mean<strong>in</strong>g. What does the gatekeeper have the right toknow? What counts as adequate and relevant evidence? On what basis will the applicantbe judged? What does ‘understand<strong>in</strong>g’ the other’s <strong>in</strong>tent mean <strong>in</strong> these contexts? Theuncerta<strong>in</strong>ty that <strong>in</strong>hab<strong>its</strong> these <strong>in</strong>teractions is rapidly converted <strong>in</strong>to the certa<strong>in</strong>ty of fixedjudgements and positions after the event s<strong>in</strong>ce it is the gatekeeper who, as representativeof a major social <strong>in</strong>stitution, controls the way <strong>in</strong> which reality is represented and contributesto the dom<strong>in</strong>ant discourses about m<strong>in</strong>ority identities. And despite the respectful <strong>in</strong>tcrpersonalconduct of the gatekeeper, m<strong>in</strong>ority workers, as I <strong>in</strong>dicated above <strong>in</strong> the case ofAbdelmalek, are aware of the racism of the dom<strong>in</strong>ant group and this is likely to affect anyorthodox process of socialisation. Abdelmalek may be develop<strong>in</strong>g a Competence <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g change of topic cues and even <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g the goals of such counsell<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>terviews. But the develop<strong>in</strong>g competence that results from such socio-cultural knowledgemay be matched by ambiguity, anomaly or resistance. <strong>Social</strong>isation assumes a sense of‘belong<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> a new community and yet the <strong>in</strong>stitutions where language socialisation cantake place represent what is different, ‘other’, cven hostile and discrim<strong>in</strong>atory.The <strong>in</strong>stability of mean<strong>in</strong>g and the contestation over mean<strong>in</strong>g create a complex set ofsocial conditions with<strong>in</strong> which there is the potential for communicative and material successor not and the potential for language socialisation and the read<strong>in</strong>ess for it ~ or not. Giventhe wider discourses that circulate about ethnic m<strong>in</strong>orities, each <strong>in</strong>tercultural <strong>in</strong>teractioncan both produce relatively adverse conditions for language learn<strong>in</strong>g and can feed <strong>in</strong>to thescwider discourses each time a misunderstand<strong>in</strong>g rema<strong>in</strong>s unresolved.Some methodological implicationsThe connection between micro and macro <strong>in</strong> redef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the doma<strong>in</strong> of SLA has methodologicalas well as theoretical implications. As several examples <strong>in</strong> this paper have shown,analysis of text, us<strong>in</strong>g a CA and <strong>in</strong>teractional sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic approach, is essential <strong>in</strong>understand<strong>in</strong>g the sequential order<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teraction but it needs to be complemented byethnographic methods. Whereas CA is concerned with the general procedures employedby members <strong>in</strong> accomplish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction, a mcthod that will help analysts draw conclusionsabout onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>ferenc<strong>in</strong>g is also needed. In <strong>in</strong>tercultural communication, the analyst needsto participate <strong>in</strong> the everyday rout<strong>in</strong>es of a particular group <strong>in</strong> order to undcrstandconventionaliscd ways of <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g.Ethnographic methods are also needed to understand <strong>in</strong>teractants’ subjectivity (Bremeret al., 1996; Gumperz, 1982b; Pierce, 1995). Ethnographic <strong>in</strong>terviews and regular participation<strong>in</strong> the lives of a particular subgroup contribute to the analysts’ understand<strong>in</strong>g ofhow m<strong>in</strong>ority workers are positioned <strong>in</strong> encounters with the majority and the long-termeffect of this on <strong>in</strong>dividual motivation, personal and social <strong>in</strong>vestment and the constructionof social identities withn the relations of dom<strong>in</strong>ation that characterise a multil<strong>in</strong>gual society.ConclusionBy look<strong>in</strong>g at the environment with<strong>in</strong> which a particular group of people arc expected todevelop communicative competence ~ m<strong>in</strong>ority workers <strong>in</strong> a stratified multil<strong>in</strong>gual society


~ (ed.)~ (1992)~~~ -120 CELIA ROBERTS- a number of questions have been raised about SLA and <strong>its</strong> relatively asocial perspective.<strong>Language</strong> socialisation better describes the process of be<strong>in</strong>g a social actor <strong>in</strong> a new languagebut <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> orthodox form it does not fully account for the connection between micro<strong>in</strong>teractional processes and the macro social issues. Wider discourses of racism, <strong>in</strong>differenceand stratification feed <strong>in</strong>to and off local <strong>in</strong>teractional differences, misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs andcovert or explicit opposition.The environments created by these social forces, at micro andmacro levels, produce complex and often hostile conditions for the understand<strong>in</strong>g andproduction of discourse <strong>in</strong> a second language. Ry exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g these conditions, it is possibleto beg<strong>in</strong> to redef<strong>in</strong>e the process of second language acquisition as second languagesocialisation but <strong>in</strong> so do<strong>in</strong>g, questions are also raised about any orthodox SLS. Learn<strong>in</strong>g tobelong to a new community may also mean learn<strong>in</strong>g to resist, or at the least take up anambiguous position <strong>in</strong> relation to the socio-cultural knowledge and discourses whichconstitute it. As <strong>in</strong> many other theoretical and practical areas, the transformation of WesternEurope <strong>in</strong>to a multil<strong>in</strong>gual society illum<strong>in</strong>ates the process of second language developmentand redef<strong>in</strong>es <strong>its</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> as centrally concerned with the social.AcknowledgementsMy thanks are due to Mike Baynham, Ben Rampton, Jo Arditty and MarieThkrkscVasscurfor comments on earlier drafts of this paper.ReferencesAuer, P. (1992) ‘Introduction: John Gumperz’s approach to contextualisation’, <strong>in</strong> Auer, P. & diLuzio, A. (eds) The contextualisotion oflanguage (pp. 1-37). Amsterdam: Benjam<strong>in</strong>s.Bourdieu, P. (1977) Outl<strong>in</strong>e .fa theory ofpractice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Rremcr, K., Roberts, C., Vasseur, M., Simonet, M. and Breeder, P. (1996) Achiev<strong>in</strong>gunderstand<strong>in</strong>g: Discourse <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tercultural encounters. London: Longman.Clyne, M. (1994) Intercultural communication at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Duff, P. ( 1996) ‘Different languages, different practices: <strong>Social</strong>ization of discourse competence<strong>in</strong> dual-anguage school classrooms <strong>in</strong> Hungary’, <strong>in</strong> Bailey, K. and Nunan, D. (eds) Voicesfrom the language classroom: Qualitative research <strong>in</strong> second language education research(pp. 407433). New York: Cambridge University Press.Eermans, S., Prevignano, C. and Thibault, P. (eds) (1997) Discuss<strong>in</strong>g Communication Analysis 1:john]. Gumperz (pp. 6-23). Lausanne: Beta Press.Fairclough, N. (1992) Discourse and social change. Cambridge: Polity Press.Gilroy, P. (1987) There a<strong>in</strong>’t no black <strong>in</strong> the UnionJack. London: Hutch<strong>in</strong>son.Gumperz, J. (1982a) Discourse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.(1982b) <strong>Language</strong> and social identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.‘<strong>Context</strong>ualisation and understand<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> Duranti, A. and Goodw<strong>in</strong>, C. (eds)Reth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Context</strong>: <strong>Language</strong> as an <strong>in</strong>teractive phenomenon (pp. 229-252). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.(1997) ‘A discussion with John J. Gumperz’ (discussants: C. Prevignano and A. di Luzio),<strong>in</strong> Eerdmans, S., Prevignano, C. andThibault, P. (eds), pp. 6-23.Hall, S. (1988) ‘New ethnicities’, ICA Documents, 7, 27-31.Hewitt, R. (1986) White talk black talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Kasper, G. and Schmidt, R. (1996) ‘Develop<strong>in</strong>g issues <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage pragmatics’. Studies <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, 18, 149-163.


~ (1LANGUAGE ACQUISITION OR LANGUAGE SOCIALISATION? 121Lantolf, J. and Pavlcnko, A. (1995) ‘Sociocultural theory and second language acquisition’.Annual Review $Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 15, 108-1 24.Lev<strong>in</strong>son, S., (1997) ‘Contcxtualis<strong>in</strong>g“contextualisation cues”’, <strong>in</strong> Eermans, S., Prevignano, C.andThibault, P. (eds), pp. 24-30.Perdue, C. (cd.) (1993) Adult language acquisition. Cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic perspectives (Vols. 1 and 2).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Pierce, B. (1995) ‘<strong>Social</strong> identity, <strong>in</strong>vestment and language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. TESOL Quarterb, 29,9-3 1.Rampton, B. (1995a) Cross<strong>in</strong>g: <strong>Language</strong> and ethnicity among adolescents. Harlow: Longman.995b) ‘Languagc cross<strong>in</strong>g and the problematisation of ethnicity and socialisation’.Pragmatics, 5,485-515.Roberts, C., Davies, E. and Jupp,T. (1 992) <strong>Language</strong> and discrim<strong>in</strong>ati0n.A study ofcommunication<strong>in</strong> multiethnic workplaces. Harlow: Longman.Roberts, C. and Simonot, M. (1987) “‘This is my life”: How languagc acquisition is<strong>in</strong>teractionally accomplished’, <strong>in</strong> Ellis, R. (ed.) Second language acquisition <strong>in</strong> context (pp.133-148). Hemel Hcmpstcad: Prentice-Hall.Rogoff, B. (1984) ‘Introduction: Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> social contcxt’ , <strong>in</strong> Rogoff, B. andLave, J. (eds) Everyday Cognition: The development <strong>in</strong> social context (pp. 1-8). Cambridgc,MA: Harvard University Press.Scarcella, R. (1982) ‘Discourse accent <strong>in</strong> sccond language production’, <strong>in</strong> Sel<strong>in</strong>ker, L. andGass, S. (cds) <strong>Language</strong> transfer <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 306-326). Rowley, MA: NewburyHouse.Silverste<strong>in</strong>, M. (1992) ‘The <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>acy of contextualisation: When is enough enough?’, <strong>in</strong>Auer, P., and di Luzio, A. (eds) The contextualisation oflanguage (pp. 55-76). Amsterdam:Bcnjam<strong>in</strong>s.Thomas, J. (1983) ‘Cross-cultural pragmatic failurc’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 4, 91-1 12.Tyler, A. (1995) ‘The co-construction of cross-cultural miscommunication’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second<strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, 17, 129-1 52.van Dijk, T., T<strong>in</strong>g-Toomey, S., Smitherman, G. andTroutman, D. (1997) ‘Discourse, ethnicity,culturc and racism’, <strong>in</strong> van Dijk, T. (ed.) Discourse as social <strong>in</strong>teraction: Discourse studies 2(pp. 144-180). London: Sage.


Chapter 7Michael P. BreenTHE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGELEARNING: A NEGLECTED SITUATION?IntroductionW I S H T 0 EX P L 0 R E T H E B E L I E F that the classroom will have certa<strong>in</strong> effectsI upon language learn<strong>in</strong>g. The assumption rest<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> what I have to say is thatrelationships can be discovered between the social processes of the classroom group and the<strong>in</strong>dividual psychological process of second language development. Given the present stateof our knowledge about the learn<strong>in</strong>g of foreign languages, this assumption is supportedupon tenuous foundations. As most people at least beg<strong>in</strong> to learn new languages <strong>in</strong>classrooms, the researcher can hardly fail to locate some variable of classroom life that willhave a systematic effect upon language learn<strong>in</strong>g, or some variable of learn<strong>in</strong>g behaviourwhich has correlational potential with <strong>in</strong>structional treatment. The researcher may ask:“What arc the spectf’c contributions of the classroom to the process of language development?”The assumption be<strong>in</strong>g that we may lie able to expla<strong>in</strong> how classroom-based<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>fluences and <strong>in</strong>teracts with learn<strong>in</strong>g if we come to understand the spccialwork<strong>in</strong>gs of the classroom context. The teacher’s priorities - perhaps more urgent anddirect ~ are to build upon those <strong>in</strong>herent features of the classroom situation which mayfacilitate the learn<strong>in</strong>g of a new languagc.Thc teacher’s question may bc: “In what ways mightI exploit the social reality of the classroom as a resource for the teach<strong>in</strong>g of language?”This paper offers particular answers to both thc rcscarchcr’s and the teacher’s questions.It beg<strong>in</strong>s with an exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the approaches of current research towards the languageclass. I offer a particular evaluation of recent developments <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigations devoted tosecond language acquisition and to language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom situation. Thisevaluation, though necessarily brief, has thrcc purposes. First, to identify the possiblecontributions of the language classroom which are pcrccivcd and revealed by currentresearch. Second, to identify what seem to lie significant contriliutions of the classroomwhich current research appears to neglect. And third, to deduce certa<strong>in</strong> implications forfuture research and for language teach<strong>in</strong>g.The researcher and the teacher are confronted by a crucial common problem: how torclatc social activity, to psychological change and how to relate psychological process<strong>in</strong>gto the social dynamics ofa group.Thc researcher must expla<strong>in</strong> these relationships if he is tounderstand adequately language learn<strong>in</strong>g as it is experienced by most people ~ <strong>in</strong> a gather<strong>in</strong>gmade up of other learners and a teacher. The teacher is a direct participant <strong>in</strong> this social


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 123event with the aim of <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g psychological development. The teacher is obligedcont<strong>in</strong>ually to <strong>in</strong>tegrate the learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with the collective andcommunal activities of a group of which, unlike the researcher, he is not an outsider. Theresearcher enters the classroom when a genu<strong>in</strong>e sociocognitive experiment is already wellunder way. In evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of research, because of abstraction from the daily lifeof the class, we need to discover and make clear for ourselves the particular perceptions of aclassroom which we, as researchers, hold either before we enter it or subsequent to thecollection of our data. It is a truism of social anthropology that no human social <strong>in</strong>stitutionsor relationships can be adequately understood unless account is taken of the expectations,values, and beliefs that they engage. This is no less true of the <strong>in</strong>stitution of research. Thedef<strong>in</strong>ition of the classroom situation that we hold will <strong>in</strong>fluence how we perceive theclassroom group and how we might act with<strong>in</strong> it, and this is as unavoidable for the researcheras it is for a teacher or a learner. One of the paradoxes of research is to challenge taken-forgrantedbeliefs whilst, at the same time, cl<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g to beliefs which susta<strong>in</strong> the rescarchendeavour. Belief allows the researcher (and many teachers and learners) to take for grantedthe capacity of a classroom to metamorphose <strong>in</strong>structional <strong>in</strong>puts <strong>in</strong>to learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.Is there psychological proof for this relationship between teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g, or is it abelief susta<strong>in</strong>ed primarily by the social purpose that we <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong> a gather<strong>in</strong>g of teacher andtaught?Can we detect particular def<strong>in</strong>itions of the classroom situation with<strong>in</strong> current languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g research? What metaphors for a classroom are available to us as researchers atpresent? I wish to explore two metaphors for the classroom that emerge from two recentand <strong>in</strong>fluential rcscarch traditions. I am conscious that there may be as many metaphorsfor the classroom as there are researchers <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g. But I have to be brief and Iam encouraged to generalise here by the tendency of researchers to seek security aroundparticular dom<strong>in</strong>ant paradigms or ways of see<strong>in</strong>g. ’ One prevail<strong>in</strong>g metaphor is the classroomas experimental laboratory, and another, more recently emergent, is the classroom asdiscourse. I will briefly explore both.The classroom as experimental laboratoryWe are encouraged to regard the classroom as experimental laboratory by the area of theoryand research known as Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition (SLA). Its tradition can be traced backto studies <strong>in</strong> first language acquisition, through <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the natural order ofacquisition of certa<strong>in</strong> grammatical morphemes, through the comprehensive theoricsof Krashen, and up to the recent flower<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the identification of learner strategies fromretrospective accounts offered by <strong>in</strong>dividual learners - either verbally or with<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gdiaries. The primary function of the language classroom as implied or sometimes directlyrecommended by SLA research is that the learner, by be<strong>in</strong>g placed <strong>in</strong> a classroom, can beexposed to a certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d of l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put which may be shown to correlate with certa<strong>in</strong>desirable learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes. Here, the value and purpose of the classroom is <strong>its</strong> potentialto provide l<strong>in</strong>guistic data that are f<strong>in</strong>ely tuned for the efficient process<strong>in</strong>g of new knowledge;classrooms can wash learners with optimal <strong>in</strong>put. Researchers’ more recent <strong>in</strong>ferences fromlearners’ accounts of their own strategies encourage us to deduce further that the classroomis a place <strong>in</strong> which we might re<strong>in</strong>force good language-learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies so that the <strong>in</strong>putbecomes unavoidably optimal. As thc ma<strong>in</strong>stream of SLA rcsearch rests on the assumptionthat the comprehension of <strong>in</strong>put is the catalyst of language development, it implies arole for the teacher that is delimited yet complex. In essence, either the teacher must


124 MICHAEL P. BREENfacilitate comprehension through the provision of l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put sensitive to <strong>in</strong>dividuallearner <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ations, or the teacher should endeavour to shape <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>g behavioursso that each learner may atta<strong>in</strong> a repertoire of efficient process<strong>in</strong>g strategies. The SLAmetaphor for the classroom implies tcacher as surrogatc experimental psychologist andlearners as subject to particular <strong>in</strong>put treatments or behavioural re<strong>in</strong>forcement.However, this view of the language classroom leaves us with a number of unresolvedproblems that warrant more attention if we seck to understand the relationship between alanguagc class and language learn<strong>in</strong>g. First, the <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g variables of l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>putand the strategic behaviour of learners are not special to classroorns.They were not uncoveredas prevail<strong>in</strong>g features of classroom life at all.’ The second and perhaps more significantproblem is that two crucial <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variables seem to have been bypassed by SLAresearch. Both of these variables are centrally related to the process<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>put. Both willdeterm<strong>in</strong>e what a learner might actually <strong>in</strong>take. SLA research which emphasises l<strong>in</strong>guistic<strong>in</strong>put (provided by <strong>in</strong>struction or exposure) as the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable and some laterlearner output (<strong>in</strong> a test or <strong>in</strong> spontaneous speech) as the dependent variable leaps bl<strong>in</strong>dlyover any active cognition on the part of the learner. With <strong>its</strong> heavy reliance on l<strong>in</strong>guisticperformance criteria for psychological change there is a resultant superficiality <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong>attention to learncrs’ <strong>in</strong>ternal perceptual processes. The rcscarch takcs for grantcd whatthe learner may def<strong>in</strong>e as optimal for him. Morc fundamentally, it does not address thequestion of how a learner selectively perceives parts of l<strong>in</strong>guistic data as mean<strong>in</strong>gful andworth act<strong>in</strong>g upon <strong>in</strong> the first place.Thercfore, thc <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variablc of what the learneractually does to <strong>in</strong>put or with <strong>in</strong>put is neglcctcd. Givcn the importancc attached toComprehension by SLA research it seems paradoxical that the active re<strong>in</strong>terpretation andreconstruction of any <strong>in</strong>put by the learner is not accounted for.The search for correlationsbetween, for example, thc frequency of a grammatical form <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>put and the frequentoccurrence of that form <strong>in</strong> some later learner performance seems motivated by a rathernarrow view of human learn<strong>in</strong>g. The research leads us to a causal condition<strong>in</strong>g as opposedto a cognitivc and <strong>in</strong>tcractivc explanation of language development. We are left unsure howand why lcarncrs do what they do <strong>in</strong> order to <strong>in</strong>take selectively.On the face of it, lcarn<strong>in</strong>g strategy research seems to offer some help here. However,thcsc <strong>in</strong>vestigations primarily confirm that learners are unpredictable, <strong>in</strong>consistent, andsometimes seem<strong>in</strong>gly <strong>in</strong>efficient processors. Thus, the same learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome canbe achicvcd by different strategies while different learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomcs can be achieved bythe same strategy. Investigations <strong>in</strong>to learner strategies have not yet hclpcd us to understandhow or why it is that one th<strong>in</strong>g can be <strong>in</strong>terpreted or learned by any two learnerswith seem<strong>in</strong>gly different profiles of strategies. Until we understand thcsc th<strong>in</strong>gs, thecapacity of <strong>in</strong>struction to cncourage or shape desirable or efficient strategic behaviour oflearners rema<strong>in</strong>s unfounded. ’This problem emerg<strong>in</strong>g from the data we derive from learnersconcern<strong>in</strong>g their strategies leads to the second crucial <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variable which seemsto be neglcctcd <strong>in</strong> SLA research. Learners certa<strong>in</strong>ly are strategic <strong>in</strong> how they go aboutlearn<strong>in</strong>g, but if we ask them what they th<strong>in</strong>k they do, or if they keep a diary of whatthey do, such retrospections, <strong>in</strong>evitably post hoc rationalisations, will exhibit a cohercnccthat bears only metaphorical resemblance to the actual moment of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Someth<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tervenes between a learner’s <strong>in</strong>trospections to a researcher or to a diary reader, just assometh<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervencs between <strong>in</strong>put to a learner and between what a learner has <strong>in</strong>takenand some later test performance. I suggest that one th<strong>in</strong>g which crucially <strong>in</strong>tervenes is thelearner’s def<strong>in</strong>ition of situation: the def<strong>in</strong>ition of bc<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>formant to someone <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, the def<strong>in</strong>ition of be<strong>in</strong>g a language learner <strong>in</strong> a classroom, and the def<strong>in</strong>ition ofdo<strong>in</strong>g a test. If we hope to expla<strong>in</strong> fully the relationship between classroom <strong>in</strong>put and


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 125learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes, or to expla<strong>in</strong> possible relationships between strategic behaviourand language learn<strong>in</strong>g, then we need to locate these relationships sonally. How and whylearners do what they do will be strongly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by their situation, who they are with,and by their perceptions of both.4Given that we wish to understand how the external social situation of a classroom relatesto the <strong>in</strong>ternal psychological states of the learncr, the metaphor of the classroom as providerof optimal <strong>in</strong>put or re<strong>in</strong>forcer of good strategies is <strong>in</strong>adequate. It reduces the act or experienceof learn<strong>in</strong>g a language to l<strong>in</strong>guistic or behavioural condition<strong>in</strong>g somehow <strong>in</strong>dependentof the learner’s social reality. Not only is SLA research currently offer<strong>in</strong>g us a delimitedaccount of language learn<strong>in</strong>g, reduc<strong>in</strong>g active cognition to passive <strong>in</strong>ternalisation andreduc<strong>in</strong>g language to very specific grammatical performance, the ma<strong>in</strong>stream of SLAresearch is also asocial. It neglects the social significance of even those variables which the<strong>in</strong>vestigators regard as central. The priority given to l<strong>in</strong>guistic and mentalistic variables<strong>in</strong> terms of the efficient process<strong>in</strong>g of knowledge as <strong>in</strong>put leads <strong>in</strong>evitably to a partialaccount of the language learn<strong>in</strong>g process.The social context of learn<strong>in</strong>g and the social forceswith<strong>in</strong> it will always shape what is made available to be lcarned and the <strong>in</strong>teraction of<strong>in</strong>dividual m<strong>in</strong>d with external l<strong>in</strong>guistic or communicative knowledge. Even Wundt, thefirst experimental psychologist, bclicvcd that he could not study higher mental processessuch as reason<strong>in</strong>g, belief, thought, and language <strong>in</strong> a laboratory precisely because suchprocesses were rootcd with<strong>in</strong> authentic social activity.’ A more recent research tradition -an offspr<strong>in</strong>g of work <strong>in</strong> SLA - does address <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g social variables. This traditionprovides my second metaphor.The classroom as discourseRecent classroom-bascd or classroom-oriented research explicitly seeks to describe whatactually happens <strong>in</strong> a rather special social situation. This research relies upon methods ofconversational and sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic data collection and analysis, thereby seek<strong>in</strong>g to offer aricher and less prcscriptive account of classroom language lcarn<strong>in</strong>g than earlier <strong>in</strong>vestigationsof the comparative effects of different teach<strong>in</strong>g methodologies.6 Classroom-orientedresearch focuses primarily upon the discourse of classroom communication. It sees teacherand learners as active participants <strong>in</strong> the generation of the discoursc of lessons. Here, theresearcher explores the classroom as a text which reveals such phenomena as variableparticipation by learners, various error treatments by teachers, and specific features ofclassroom talk such as teacher evaluation, teacher-learner negotiation, and prevalent<strong>in</strong>structional speech acts <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g display questions, formulation or explanation, andmessage adjustment. Although much of this research seems to avoid be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tentionallyexplanatory <strong>in</strong> terms of the possible effects of classroom discourse upon language learn<strong>in</strong>g,some <strong>in</strong>vestigators seek to correlate selected features of classroom talk with certa<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gbehaviours or learned outcomes. Classroom-oriented rcscarch rests on the assumptionthat the discourse of a language class will reveal what is special and important about thatlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g situation. It <strong>in</strong>tends no practical implications for the teacher, althoughsome of the more overtly correlational studies may encouragc thc teacher to assume thathe must endeavour to orchestrate his own and the learners’ contributions to the discourseaccord<strong>in</strong>g to Conversational moves or speech acts which exemplify “good” <strong>in</strong>struction and“good” learner participation.Clearly, this focus upon the actual discourse of classroom communication providesa valid location if we wish to beg<strong>in</strong> to understand the experience of learn<strong>in</strong>g a language <strong>in</strong>


126 MICHAEL P. BREENa classroom. However, even with such an ecologically valid po<strong>in</strong>t of departure, currentclassroom-oriented research leaves us with two important areas of uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty. We haveto question the extent to which the surface text of classroom discourse can adequatelyreveal the underly<strong>in</strong>g social psychological forces which generate it (the expectations, beliefsand attitudes of the participants) and also reveal the sociocognitive effects it may have(the specific <strong>in</strong>terpretations and learn<strong>in</strong>g it provokes). This central issue leads us back <strong>in</strong>tothe long-established debate on the possible relationships between communicat<strong>in</strong>g andlearn<strong>in</strong>g, between language and cognition. A number of the correlational studies with<strong>in</strong>classroom-oriented research avoid the complexities of this debate by appear<strong>in</strong>g to assumethat certa<strong>in</strong> phenomena <strong>in</strong> classroom discourse cuuse learn<strong>in</strong>g to occur. Any correlationbetween observable features of discourse and testable learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes ~ a teacher’sformulation of a rule, for example, and a learner’s later use or reformulation of that rule- does not expla<strong>in</strong> how or why a learner actually achieved such th<strong>in</strong>gs. This dependencyon the superficial features of classroom talk can force us to deduce that if other learners <strong>in</strong>the class failed to use the rule correctly or were unable to reformulate it then the teacher’sorig<strong>in</strong>al formulation was <strong>in</strong>adequate. Rut what of the <strong>in</strong>ternal dimensions of classroomCommunication: the learners’ variable perception, re<strong>in</strong>terpretation, and accommodation ofwhatever may be provided through classroom discourse? In these matters, classroomorientedresearch seems to share a psychological naivety with SLA research.The second area of uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty is perhaps more fundamental. Most currentclassroom-oriented research paradoxically reduces the external dimensions of classroomcommunication, the actual social event, to observable features of the talk between teacherand learners. Sixty years ago, Edward Sapir po<strong>in</strong>ted out that we cannot use observable dataalone from social events even if we merely aim to describe them adequately. Nor can we<strong>in</strong>terpret the observable data through our eyes only if we ever seck to expla<strong>in</strong> what thosedata actually mean. Even Del Hymcs, who was foremost <strong>in</strong> propos<strong>in</strong>g the ethnography ofspeak<strong>in</strong>g which now underlies much sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic research, also <strong>in</strong>sisted that if we wishadequately to expla<strong>in</strong> any speech event we need to discover <strong>its</strong> existential and experientialsignificance for those tak<strong>in</strong>g part.7 These proposals imply that the mean<strong>in</strong>gs and values ofclassroom discourse reside beh<strong>in</strong>d and beneath what is said and unsaid. A researcher’s<strong>in</strong>terpretation of the “text” of classroom discourse has to be derived through the participants’<strong>in</strong>terpretations of that discourse. Is the teacher’s treatment of an error taken as errortreatment by a learner? Is a learner’s request for <strong>in</strong>formation even if responded to as suchby the teacher ~ actually a piece of time-wast<strong>in</strong>g or even express<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g else entirely?Is superficial negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g or a learner’s generation of further <strong>in</strong>put evidence ofthe wish to learn more?To beg<strong>in</strong> to understand language learn<strong>in</strong>g experience <strong>in</strong> a classroom the researchermust discover what teacher and taught themselves perceive as <strong>in</strong>herent with<strong>in</strong> the discourseof lessons. More importantly, recent classroom research clearly shows the researcher assomeone who <strong>in</strong>vests <strong>in</strong>to his text of classroom discourse certa<strong>in</strong> patternedness ormean<strong>in</strong>gfulness. Classroom communication, like any text, realizes and carries mean<strong>in</strong>gpotential. Because of this, if we wish to discover what the teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g of a language<strong>in</strong> a classroom is for the people undertak<strong>in</strong>g it, we need to know what orderl<strong>in</strong>ess and sensethey <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong> the overt communication of the class. Put simply, the discourse of the classroomdoes not <strong>its</strong>elf reveal what the teacher and the learners experience from that discourse.Such experience is two-dimensional: <strong>in</strong>dividual-subjective experience and collective<strong>in</strong>tersubjectiveexperience. The subjective experience of teacher and learners <strong>in</strong> a classroomis woven with personal purposes, attitudes, and preferred ways of do<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs. The<strong>in</strong>tersubjective experience derives from and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s teacher and learner shared


~ uponTHE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 127def<strong>in</strong>itions, conventions, and procedures which enable a work<strong>in</strong>g together <strong>in</strong> a crowd. Ofcourse, the discourse of a classroom may provide a w<strong>in</strong>dow onto the surface expressionof the <strong>in</strong>tersubjective experience and even onto momentary expressions of subjectiveexperiences, for these two dimensions of experience must <strong>in</strong>terrelate and <strong>in</strong>fluence oneanother. However, classroom discourse alone allows us a partial view from which we areobliged to dcscribc others’ experiences as if “through a glass darkly.”Classroom-oriented research shares with SLA studies the tendency to reduce or avoidconsideration of certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variables which <strong>in</strong>evitably <strong>in</strong>fluence how and whylearners may <strong>in</strong>ternalise <strong>in</strong>put and how and why learners <strong>in</strong>teract with a teacher <strong>in</strong> the waysthey do.Ths reductionism is characterised by an emphatic focus upon l<strong>in</strong>guistic performanceobservable features of language and discourse. To be fair, neither research traditionmay <strong>in</strong>tend to undcrstand or even expla<strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom situation.However, any researcher who tries to correlate features of l<strong>in</strong>guistic Performance data <strong>in</strong>terms of classroom <strong>in</strong>put with some learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome is, at lcast implicitly, seek<strong>in</strong>g apossible explanation of that learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome. And such an cxplanation can only be causal.Classroom rcscarch is not asocial like SLA research, but it does share a non-cognitive viewof learner comprehension and reconstruction of <strong>in</strong>put despite <strong>its</strong> potentially richer view of<strong>in</strong>put as discourse rather than merely grammatical data. Classroom-oriented researchperceives the learner as actively contribut<strong>in</strong>g to the discourse. But how can we relate suchcontributions or even non contributions to language learn<strong>in</strong>g? Learners and tcachers arenot dualities of social be<strong>in</strong>g and mental be<strong>in</strong>g ~ an idea apparcntly unfortunately supportedby the very separateness of SLA and classroom-oriented research priorities. It is <strong>in</strong>cumbentupon classroom-based <strong>in</strong>vestigations of language learn<strong>in</strong>g to account for those socialpsychological forces which gcnerate classroom discourse and for those socio-cognitiveeffects of the discourse even f<strong>its</strong> objective is primarily to describe social phenomena. If thesubjective and <strong>in</strong>tersubjective experiences of and from classroom discourse are reduced towhat we can f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> the discourse alone, then we are allowed to deduce that classroomlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g results from discoursal condition<strong>in</strong>g ~ no more nor less than socialdeterm<strong>in</strong>ism!It appears that the two metaphors for the classroom which we have availablc to u3 atprcsent offer def<strong>in</strong>itions of the classroom situation which seem to neglect the social realityof language learn<strong>in</strong>g as I t IS experienced and created by teachers and learners. Both metaphorsunfortunately constra<strong>in</strong> our understand<strong>in</strong>g of language learn<strong>in</strong>g because each takes forgranted crucial <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g psychological and social variables which arc the fulcra uponwhich language learn<strong>in</strong>g is balanced. The reconstructive cognition of learners and the socialand psychological forces which permeate the processes of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g must residewith<strong>in</strong> any explanation conccrn<strong>in</strong>g how and why people do what thcy do when they worktogether on a new language. More seriously, perhaps, both contemporary metaphorsimplicitly reduce human action and <strong>in</strong>teraction to classical condition<strong>in</strong>g, where<strong>in</strong> learnersthough superficially participat<strong>in</strong>g are essentially passive respondents to observable l<strong>in</strong>guisticand discoursal stimuli. It thcrefore appears nccessary that research has still to adopt adef<strong>in</strong>ition of the classroom which will encompass both cognitive and socd variables 70 thattheir mutual <strong>in</strong>fluence can be better understood. More precisely, we need a metaphor forthe classroom through which teacher and earners can be viewed as th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g social actorsand not reduced to generators of <strong>in</strong>put-output nor analyzed as dualities of either conceptualor social be<strong>in</strong>gs. Perhaps the metaphor we require can providc a basis for the synthesis ofSLA and classroom-oriented research endeavours whilst necessarily be<strong>in</strong>g more comprehensivethan both. These deductions lead me to propose a third metaphor for theclassroom <strong>in</strong> the hope that it might further facilitate our understand<strong>in</strong>g of classroom


theas128 MICHAEL P. BREENlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. One of the characteristics of my third metaphor is that it is likely to bemore expenentially familiar to most language teachers and learners than it may be to someresearchers.The classroom as coral gardensA proposal that the classroom situation could be perceived as coral gardens may be <strong>in</strong>itiallyreacted to as rather odd. The metaphor derives from Mal<strong>in</strong>owski’s classical studies ofTrobriand island cultures, <strong>in</strong> particular those <strong>in</strong>vestigations he described <strong>in</strong> Coral Gardensand Their Magic. I offer the metaphor because it entails three requirements for researchdevoted to classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g. First, <strong>in</strong> order to understand the process of learn<strong>in</strong>gwith<strong>in</strong> a human group, our <strong>in</strong>vestigations are necessarily an anthropological endeavour.Second, the researcher should approach the classroom with a k<strong>in</strong>d of anthropologicalhumility. We should explore classroom life <strong>in</strong>itially as if we knew noth<strong>in</strong>g about it. And,third, it is more important to discover what people <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong> a social situation than it isto rely on what might be observed as <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> that social situation. Just as gardens ofcoral were granted magical realities by thcTrobriand islanders, a language class ~ outwardlya gather<strong>in</strong>g of people with an assumed common purpose ~ is an arena of subjective and<strong>in</strong>tersubjective realities which are worked out, changed, and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed. And these realitiesme not trivial background to the tasks ofteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g CI language.They locate and def<strong>in</strong>ethe new language <strong>its</strong>elf as if it never existed before, and they cont<strong>in</strong>ually specify and mouldthe activities of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. In essence, the metaphor of classroom as coral gardens<strong>in</strong>sists that we perceive the language class as a genu<strong>in</strong>e culture and worth <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g assuch.XIf we can adopt this def<strong>in</strong>ition of the classroom situation, then research may get closerto the daily lives of teachers and learners. We can approach the raison d’etre of a languageclass ~ work<strong>in</strong>g upon and rediscover<strong>in</strong>g of language knowledge ~ <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g sociocognitiveconstruction and re<strong>in</strong>terpretation. A particular culture, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, entailsparticular relationships between social activities and psychological processes and changes.SLA research asserts comprehension as central, whilst the classroom as culture locatescomprehension with<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tersubjective construction of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness and the subjectivere<strong>in</strong>terpretation of whatever may be rendered comprehensible. In other words, <strong>in</strong>put isnever <strong>in</strong>herently optimal, for any new knowlcdgc is socio-cognitively rendered familiar orunfamiliar by those who participate <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> exploration. The culture of the class generatesknowledges and a focus upon any <strong>in</strong>ternalised l<strong>in</strong>guistic outcomes will tell us little aboutclassroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> action. Classroom-oriented research explores the discourseof lessons, whilst the classroom as culture extends across islands of <strong>in</strong>tersubjective mean<strong>in</strong>gand depths of subjective <strong>in</strong>tentions and <strong>in</strong>terpretations which only rarely touch the surfaceof talk and which the discourse <strong>its</strong>elf often deliberately hides.The discourse of lessons willma<strong>in</strong>ly ymbolise what participants contribute to those lessons and it will not signify whatthey actually <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong> them or derive from them.It is, of course, <strong>in</strong>cumbent upon me to justify my own belief <strong>in</strong> the classroom as genu<strong>in</strong>eculture. In order to meet the charge that such a metaphor may be too idealiscd or abstract,I need to identify some of the essential features of the culture of the language classroom. Iwill briefly describe eight essential features:


~ isTHE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 129The culture of the classroom is <strong>in</strong>teractiveThe language class <strong>in</strong>volves all <strong>its</strong> participants <strong>in</strong> verbal and non-verbal <strong>in</strong>teraction of certa<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>ds. This <strong>in</strong>teraction exists on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum from ritualised, predictable, phaticcommunication to dynamic, unpredictable, diversely <strong>in</strong>terpreted communication. Ofcourse, human <strong>in</strong>teraction will be relatively located on this k<strong>in</strong>d of cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>in</strong> all socialsituations. One special characteristic of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, however, is that it is motivatedby the assumption that people can learn together <strong>in</strong> a group.This mean3 that a high premiumis placed upon consensus whilst misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs, alternative <strong>in</strong>terpretations, andnegotiable mean<strong>in</strong>g will paradoxically be the norm, and from which participants will seekto make their own sense and upon which participants will impose their own purposes.Thisis not to say that the observable <strong>in</strong>teraction will not be patterned or constra<strong>in</strong>ed, but thatit is very likely to be patterned differently <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>in</strong>vested <strong>in</strong> it by each person<strong>in</strong> the class. Therefore the researcher needs to be wary of assum<strong>in</strong>g that the patterns of<strong>in</strong>teraction which wc perceive as significant have the same salience for both teacher andtaught. A special characteristic of the language class is that <strong>in</strong>teraction is further motivatedby the assumption that people can objectify a language and talk about it and analysc it <strong>in</strong>ways they may not naturally do if left alone. The language class implies metal<strong>in</strong>guistic<strong>in</strong>teraction. However, it is often further assumed that the language class can provideopportunities for genu<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>teraction through the new language code. A language classentails <strong>in</strong>teraction about language and <strong>in</strong>teraction through languages <strong>in</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ualjuxtaposition.All these and other characteristics of the <strong>in</strong>teractive process of the language class mayor may not be efficient or optimal for language learn<strong>in</strong>g. However, all represent the <strong>in</strong>herentauthenticity of the <strong>in</strong>teraction with<strong>in</strong> a language class given the external constra<strong>in</strong>ts of space,time, participation, etc., which typify any classroom devoted to any subject matter.A significant paradox for the language teacher ~ a paradox of which teachers are well awarethat the established <strong>in</strong>teraction which is evolved and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed by the culture of theclassroom group often conflicts with efforts towards communication through the newlanguage. Communication <strong>in</strong> the new language requires the temporary suspension of thosecultural conventions govern<strong>in</strong>g the everyday <strong>in</strong>teraction of the particular classroom group.It requires communication which is, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong>authentic to the <strong>in</strong>teractive context <strong>in</strong>which it has to occur. This implies that one of the conventions assumed to be honoured byparticipants <strong>in</strong> the culture of a language class is the will<strong>in</strong>gness and capacity to suspenddisbelief, to participate <strong>in</strong> simulated communication with<strong>in</strong> classroom specific <strong>in</strong>teraction.’The culture of the classroom is diflerentiatedAlthough the language class may be one social situation, it is a different social context for allthose who participate with<strong>in</strong> it.The culture of the classroom is an amalgam and permutationof different social realities.This means that the content of lessons (the language be<strong>in</strong>g taught)and the procedures of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g (the th<strong>in</strong>gs be<strong>in</strong>g done) are both cont<strong>in</strong>ually<strong>in</strong>terpreted differently as the life of that language class unfolds.The classroom is the meet<strong>in</strong>gpo<strong>in</strong>t of various subjective views of language, diverse learn<strong>in</strong>g purposes, and differentpreferences concern<strong>in</strong>g how learn<strong>in</strong>g should be done. Such differentiation br<strong>in</strong>gs with itpotential for disagreement, frustrated expectations, and conflict. The culture of theclassroom docs not erase thcsc differences; it conta<strong>in</strong>s them. A major challenge for teacherand learners is the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of a f<strong>in</strong>e balance between conflict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal social realities(a k<strong>in</strong>d of subjective anarchy!) and an external reality which has to be cont<strong>in</strong>ually negotiated.


130 MICHAEL P. BREENThe outside observer has access to the compromise which results, but we would be naiveto deduce that such a compromise represents what is actually <strong>in</strong>tended or perceived as thesocial reality for any one person <strong>in</strong> the class.The culture of the classroom is collectiveThe culture of the classroom represents a tension between the <strong>in</strong>ternal world of the<strong>in</strong>dividual and the social world of the group, a recurrent juxtaposition of personal learn<strong>in</strong>gexperiences and communal teach<strong>in</strong>g- learn<strong>in</strong>g activities and conventions. The culture of theclass has a psychological reality, a m<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>its</strong> own, which emerges from this juxtaposition.The psyche of the group ~ the group’s values, mean<strong>in</strong>gs, and volitions - is a dist<strong>in</strong>ct entityother than the sum of the <strong>in</strong>dividual psychological orientations of teacher and learners.<strong>Social</strong>ly, the sometimes ritualised and sometimes overtly dynamic behaviour of the groupwill both conta<strong>in</strong> and <strong>in</strong>fluence the behaviour of the <strong>in</strong>dividual just as the overt contributionsof a teacher or a learner will fit, or divert the work<strong>in</strong>gs of the class. But this social frameworkbuilds upon and constructs a particular world which has to be accommodated as a po<strong>in</strong>t ofdeparture for psychological change. A teacher and a learner have to discover that def<strong>in</strong>itionof situation which seems to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the group and <strong>its</strong> activities ~ that def<strong>in</strong>ition of situationwhich will be relatively dist<strong>in</strong>ct from their personal def<strong>in</strong>itions. This <strong>in</strong>volves all membersof the group <strong>in</strong> empathis<strong>in</strong>g with the roles and views of others and cont<strong>in</strong>ually check<strong>in</strong>gsuch external frames of reference. The <strong>in</strong>dividual has to adapt his learn<strong>in</strong>g process to thesocial-psychological resources of the group. So also the group’s psychic and social processwill unfold from the <strong>in</strong>dividual contributions of a learner.This <strong>in</strong>terplay between <strong>in</strong>dividualand collective consciousness (and the values, beliefs, and attitudes it generates) implies thatthe researcher should be wary of credit<strong>in</strong>g the classroom with powers separable from what<strong>in</strong>dividual learners actually make classrooms do for them, and similarly wary of credit<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividual learners with powers separable from what the claysroom group provides. An<strong>in</strong>dividual learner <strong>in</strong> a classroom is engaged <strong>in</strong> both an <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>g process and agroup teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process. Therefore <strong>in</strong>dividual psychological change will cont<strong>in</strong>uallyrelate to group psychological forces.The researcher is obliged to discover these two worldsbecause they are dist<strong>in</strong>ctive. To <strong>in</strong>fir <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>g process from classroom process orvice versa will lead to a partial understand<strong>in</strong>g of classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g. We need toexplore both and how they relate one to the other.The culture of the classroom is high’y normativeOur membership <strong>in</strong> any culture implies that our behaviour will be evaluated aga<strong>in</strong>st certa<strong>in</strong>norms and conventions ~ membership entails show<strong>in</strong>g we belong. However, <strong>in</strong> all our lives,classrooms are very special <strong>in</strong> this regard. Schools and classrooms are among the ma<strong>in</strong>agencies for secondary socialisation and, as the first public <strong>in</strong>stitution most of us enter dur<strong>in</strong>gour lives, our views of classrooms will be significantly colourcd by this <strong>in</strong>itial experience.More importantly, our personal identities as learners with<strong>in</strong> a group derive much from suchexperience. This is due to the fact that our public learn<strong>in</strong>g selves have been moulded by acont<strong>in</strong>ual and explicit evaluation of our worth as learners. When a language learner enters aclassroom, he anticipates that the evaluation of him as a learner is go<strong>in</strong>g to be a crucial partof that experience. This implies that the search for cxternal criteria for success <strong>in</strong> cop<strong>in</strong>gwith language learn<strong>in</strong>g and, less optimistically perhaps, the day-to-day search for ways ofreduc<strong>in</strong>g the potential threat of negative judgements of one’s capabilities will imp<strong>in</strong>ge uponwhatever <strong>in</strong>ternal criteria a learner may evolve regard<strong>in</strong>g his own learn<strong>in</strong>g progress.


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 131Learners <strong>in</strong> a class will obviously vary with regard to their relative dependence upon externaland <strong>in</strong>ternal criteria. However, one of the prevalent features of the culture of the classroomis the establishment of overt and covert criteria aga<strong>in</strong>st which <strong>its</strong> members are cont<strong>in</strong>uallyjudged. In other words, the culture of the classroom re+ the persons who participatewith<strong>in</strong> it <strong>in</strong>to “good” learners and “bad” learners, “good” teachers and “bad” teachers,“beg<strong>in</strong>ners,”“advanced,”“high” participators and “low” participators, etc., etc. Put bluntly,the language class is a highly normative and evaluative environment which engages teacherand taught <strong>in</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ual judgement of each other, less as persons, but as members whoare supposed to learn and a member who is supposed to teach. This highly normativecharacteristic of classroom life implies for the researcher that we need to discover the overtand covert group criteria (and members’ <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>terpretations of these criteria) aga<strong>in</strong>stwhich learn<strong>in</strong>g behaviour and progress are judged. To <strong>in</strong>fer, for example, that a teacher’serror corrections are consistently based upon objective l<strong>in</strong>guistic criteria or are otherwiseapparently random would lead to a superficial analysis of phenomena which, though opaque,are deeply significant for a teacher and learners <strong>in</strong> the particular classroom.The culture of the classroom is asymmetricalBecause teachers are expected to know what learners are expected not to know, certa<strong>in</strong>social and psychological consequences <strong>in</strong>evitably obta<strong>in</strong> for the human relationships <strong>in</strong> theclass. The culture of the classroom <strong>in</strong>sists upon asymmetrical relationships. The duties andrights of teacher and taught are different. More significantly, both teacher and taught maybe equally reluctant to upset the asymmetry of roles and identities to which these duties andrights are assigned. In most societies - perhaps all, despite some relative variation ~ anegalitarian relationship between teacher and taught is a contradiction of what a classroomshould be. Teachers and learners are very familiar with the experience of graduallyestablish<strong>in</strong>g the precise degree of asymmetry which enables them to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a relativelyharmonious work<strong>in</strong>g group. As teachers, we are also familiar with a class which erodes whatthey perceive as be<strong>in</strong>g too democratic or too authoritarian an approach on our part, eventhough we ourselves may perceive our teach<strong>in</strong>g style as consistently someth<strong>in</strong>g else entirely!Here is a paradox. Learners give a teacher the right to adopt a role and identity of teacher.And a teacher has to earn particular rights and duties <strong>in</strong> the eyes of the learn<strong>in</strong>g group.However, the history of the tribe marches beh<strong>in</strong>d the teacher, and a teacher through theunfold<strong>in</strong>g culture of the particular classroom group will similarly allocate rights and dutiesto learners. Indeed, one of the rights and duties of a teacher is to do precisely that! However,asymmetrical relationships do not only exist between teacher and taught. Sub-group<strong>in</strong>gswhch are asymmetrical with the dom<strong>in</strong>ant classroom culture also emerge and prosper, suchas anti-academic peer group<strong>in</strong>gs or certa<strong>in</strong> learners who identify themselves as moresuccessful or less successful and even groups who share a common identity (such asfriendship groups) outside the classroom. Thus, not only is the culture of the classroom<strong>in</strong>dividually differentiated yet collective, it is also made up of subgroups which develop forthemselves ma<strong>in</strong>ly covert, though sometimes overtly expressed, roles and identities whichare potentially asymmetrical with both the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture and with other sub-group<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> the class.Asymmetry of roles and identities, and of the rights and duties they bear, derives fromand further generates conceptual and affective dissonances. Asymmetrical relationships veryoftcn entail disagreement <strong>in</strong> beliefs, <strong>in</strong> attitudes, and <strong>in</strong> values held. The collective natureof the classroom culture and the negotiated compromises which permeate the teach<strong>in</strong>glearn<strong>in</strong>gprocess often hide with<strong>in</strong> themselves ~ sometimes with difficulty and often only


132 MICHAEL P. BREENfor a time ~ different views of what should be happen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a class and what should not.This suggests that, although the nature of <strong>in</strong>terpersonal and <strong>in</strong>tergroup relationships with<strong>in</strong>the language classroom may be complex and chang<strong>in</strong>g, the researcher needs to uncoverwhat these are if we wish to describe what happens <strong>in</strong> the class and further <strong>in</strong>terpret thisas it is experienced by those with<strong>in</strong> the class. As researchers <strong>in</strong> the past, we have tended tobe teacher-centred <strong>in</strong> our assum<strong>in</strong>g that the major asymmetry <strong>in</strong> role and identity, and thelikely location of dissonance <strong>in</strong> perceptions and effects, resides between the teacher andthe rest. We have also perhaps undcrcstimated the possible effects ~ both negative andpositive ~ of asymmetry and dissonance with<strong>in</strong> the classroom upon the language learn<strong>in</strong>gprocess.”The culture of the classroom is <strong>in</strong>herently conservativePerhaps one of the best ways of reveal<strong>in</strong>g the establishcd culture of the classroom group isto try to <strong>in</strong>troduce an <strong>in</strong>novation which the majority neither expects nor def<strong>in</strong>es asappropriate. Most teachers have had direct experience of the effort to be radical <strong>in</strong> theirapproach with a class (be it through different material, tasks, or procedure, etc.) and havesuffered the experience of at least <strong>in</strong>itial rejection. A genu<strong>in</strong>e culture is one <strong>in</strong> which <strong>its</strong>members seek security and relative harmony <strong>in</strong> a self-satisfactory milieu. As such th<strong>in</strong>gstake time to develop, anyth<strong>in</strong>g which the group perceives as change will also take time tobe absorbed or it will be resisted as deviant. (This does not mean that harmony willnecessarily reign <strong>in</strong> the classroom, for even apparent anarchy ~ as long as it is the preferredethos of that group ~ may be quite consistent with a def<strong>in</strong>ition of classroom life for someseem<strong>in</strong>gly unsocialiscd collection of learners!). In essence, a classroom group seeks aparticular social and emotional equilibrium just as soon as it can - even one which mayseem to be antithetical to learn<strong>in</strong>g. It will subsequently resist any threat to the newlyestablished order.The <strong>in</strong>dividual lcarncr risks ostracisation from the group if he does not ~overtly at least ~ conform, and the teacher risks rebellion <strong>in</strong> various forms if he does nothonour the conventions expected by the collective def<strong>in</strong>ition of what a language teachershould be. Although this conservative spirit has <strong>its</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> the prior educationalexperiences of the learners, each new classroom group re<strong>in</strong>vents “the rules of the game”<strong>in</strong>ways which both reflect and form the classroom-culture assumptions of the particularparticipants who are suddenly shar<strong>in</strong>g each others’ company. It has to be said, of course,that a teacher may participate <strong>in</strong> this conservatism and, <strong>in</strong>deed, work through it <strong>in</strong> order tohelp develop group harmony, security and efficient ways of work<strong>in</strong>g. And teachers arecerta<strong>in</strong>ly familiar with the dilemma of wish<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>novate whilst be<strong>in</strong>g cautious ofdisruption. This means that the very presence of a researcher, or even the awareness with<strong>in</strong>the group that they are the focus of apparently objective evaluation and study will mobilisechange. Our personal experience of hav<strong>in</strong>g someone visit our home for the first timc andthen look<strong>in</strong>g at it with them, as if see<strong>in</strong>g it through their eyes, can rem<strong>in</strong>d us of the effectof <strong>in</strong>trusion. In a sense, the classroom changes <strong>in</strong> the eyes of those with<strong>in</strong> it and, therefore,will change <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> ways.This is, of course, the truism of observer effect. But there is alsothe observer’s paradox <strong>in</strong> that the classroom we now sec will be <strong>in</strong> a state of disequilibrium:it will not be the same classroom as yesterday and we will be <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g a classroom groupwhich is newly adapt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a number of subtle ways. This phenomenon can be either badnews or good news for the researcher. It will render short-term, one-shot <strong>in</strong>vestigations<strong>in</strong>to classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g largely <strong>in</strong>valid and unreliable. If, on the other hand, weapproach studies of classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g on a longitud<strong>in</strong>al basis, then we may beable to explore the process of re-establishment of social and emotional cquilibrium which


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 133our <strong>in</strong>itial arrival challenged. In other words, we may uncover more precisely the “rulesof the game” which represent the self-ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g culture of that particular workmg group.The culture of the classroom is jo<strong>in</strong>tk constructedWhilst we may accept the truism that all knowledge is socially constructed ~ most especiallyif we arc work<strong>in</strong>g with the knowledge of a language and how it is used between people ~we need to consider how classrooms re-construct knowledge. In a language class, theclassroom group together not only freshly evolves the new language (the content of lessons),but together also jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructs the lessons (the social procedures of teach<strong>in</strong>g andlearn<strong>in</strong>g). Whether or not the teacher plans a lesson <strong>in</strong> advance, the actual work<strong>in</strong>g out ofthat lesson <strong>in</strong> the class demands jo<strong>in</strong>t endeavour.The lesson-<strong>in</strong>-process is most often differentfrom that which either the teacher or the learners anticipated before the lesson began.Thesocial dynamic of the group <strong>in</strong>sists that lessons evolve, through explicit or implicitnegotiation. In whatever ways the lesson may be perceived by those who participate <strong>in</strong> it,the route it takes will be drawn by the jo<strong>in</strong>t contributions of most, if not all, of the membersof the class. Teachers and learners are well aware that lessons are rarely straightforwardjourneys but are punctuated by hesitant starts, diversions, momentary losses of momentum,<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g side tracks, and unexpected breakdowns. That it may be better to plan classroomlearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> advance has little to do with this entirely normal and creative evolution oflessons.”Several important implications for the researcher result from the fact that the contentand process of language classes are jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructed. First, any teacher-centred (orresearcher-centred) perspective on lessons is partial. Second, the researcher’s backgroundknowledge of the actual language be<strong>in</strong>g worked upon <strong>in</strong> a class can be a serious handicapbecause it potentially bl<strong>in</strong>ds us to the process of re-<strong>in</strong>vention of that language which teacherand taught engage <strong>in</strong> together. (This implication warns us aga<strong>in</strong>st rely<strong>in</strong>g on externall<strong>in</strong>guistic criteria alone <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g the nature of comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put, for example.) Theproblem rem<strong>in</strong>ds us of a similar gap between the teacher’s def<strong>in</strong>ition of the new languageand the different learners’ def<strong>in</strong>itions. There are likely to be as many versions of the newlanguage, and chang<strong>in</strong>g versions of it, as there are people <strong>in</strong> the room.Third, the researcherhas to be cont<strong>in</strong>ually wary of be<strong>in</strong>g dazzled by what seems salient <strong>in</strong> classroom life. Forexample, even the most passive or non-contributory learner <strong>in</strong> a class can be a poltergeiston the proceed<strong>in</strong>gs. Silence, encouraged or not, is a characteristic part of the culture of theclassroom and it has grcat significance. Silence or withdrawal can change a lesson just aspowerfully as their opposites, and not just for thc person who withdraws, but also for allthe others who sense it.The fourth implication of the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of the content andprocess of a language class is particularly significant for researchers who wish to exam<strong>in</strong>ethe effects of classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g. The fact that lessons-<strong>in</strong>-process are communalendeavours means that any learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome, for any member of the class, has been sociallyprocessed. The actual nature of <strong>in</strong>dividual achievements has been communally moulded.The culture of the classroom <strong>in</strong>evitably mediates between a new language and a learner <strong>in</strong>class. The culture of a particular class will shape what is made available for learn<strong>in</strong>g, willwork upon what is made available <strong>in</strong> particular ways, will evolve <strong>its</strong> own criteria for progressand achievement, and will atta<strong>in</strong> specific and various objectives. (It is worth emphasis<strong>in</strong>ghere that l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put is only a part of the first of these classroom-based phenomena.)What someone learns <strong>in</strong> a language class will be a dynamic synthesis of <strong>in</strong>dividualand collective experience. Individual def<strong>in</strong>itions of the new language, of what is to beattended to as worth learn<strong>in</strong>g, of how to learn, and personal def<strong>in</strong>itions of progress will all


<strong>its</strong>will134 MICHAEL P. BREEN<strong>in</strong>teract with the particular classroom culture’s def<strong>in</strong>itions of cach of thcsc th<strong>in</strong>gs. If strictly<strong>in</strong>dividualised or autonomous language lcarn<strong>in</strong>g is desirable or even possible then theclassroom is nccessarily antithetical towards it. The language I learn <strong>in</strong> a classroom is acommunal product derived through a jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructed process.The culture of the classroom is immediately signijkantWhat is overtly done <strong>in</strong> a classroom and what can be described by an observer arcepiphenomena; they are reductions of classroom reality. How th<strong>in</strong>gs are done and why th<strong>in</strong>gsare donc have particular psychological significance for the <strong>in</strong>dividual and for the group. Theparticular culture of a language class will socially act <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> ways, but thcse actions areextensions or manifestations of the psychology of the group, <strong>its</strong> collective consciousness andsubconscious. Individual perceptions and def<strong>in</strong>itions will, of course, feed <strong>in</strong>to and evolvefrom those of the group. However, the socio-cognitive world of the class ~ culture ~be a world other than the sum of the <strong>in</strong>dividual worlds with<strong>in</strong> it. What is sipjcant for learners(and a teacher) <strong>in</strong> a classroom is not only their <strong>in</strong>dividual th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and behaviour nor, for<strong>in</strong>stance, a longer-term mastery of a syllabus, but thc day-to-day <strong>in</strong>terpersonal rationalisationof what is to be done, why, and how. Thc immediate significance of the experience ofclassroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g resides <strong>in</strong> how <strong>in</strong>dividual priorities (teacher and learnerdef<strong>in</strong>itions of what, why, and how) can be given social space here and now. It is precisely this<strong>in</strong>terplay between the <strong>in</strong>dividual, the <strong>in</strong>dividual as group member, and the group whichrepresents and generates the social and psychological nexus which I have proposed as theculture of the language classroom. Most oftcn the flow of classroom life is actually under thesurface. What is observable is the rim of a socio-cognitive coral reef! Classroom life seems torequire that many learners spend surpris<strong>in</strong>g amounts of time do<strong>in</strong>g little, whilst a teacherspends equally surpris<strong>in</strong>g amounts of time try<strong>in</strong>g to do too much. As researchers we candcscribe such ovcrt peculiarities, hut we also need to expla<strong>in</strong> them. We have to ask whetheror not such phenomena arc true, and we must doubt the <strong>in</strong>tegrity of the observable. If wedo, then we are led towards discover<strong>in</strong>g what is, <strong>in</strong> fact, immediately significant for the groupof peoplc we started to observe. The search for the significance which a person, learner orteachcr, <strong>in</strong>vests <strong>in</strong> moments of classroom life (and for the significance granted to thesemomcnts by the classroom culture) is neither trivial nor avoidable, though it may be complexand subtle. Wc will never undcrstand classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g unless we explore <strong>its</strong>Iesson-by-lesson significance for those who undertake it.Review<strong>in</strong>g the classroom as cultureI have offered brief descriptions of eight features of the genu<strong>in</strong>e culture of the languageclassroom <strong>in</strong> order to achicvc two purposcs. First, to illustrate thc potential of classroomlife <strong>its</strong>elf, <strong>its</strong> social and psychological richness. The particular features I havc selected areoffered with no evaluative <strong>in</strong>tent. I would not wish to suggest here that such features are“good” or “bad” aspects of a classroom. They are the <strong>in</strong>cvitable characteristics of the socialevent <strong>in</strong> which most people learn a foreign language. My second purpose has been to drawattcntion to significant social and psychological variables which we seem to be neglect<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> our current research <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g. My ma<strong>in</strong> argument would be that, if wc wishto <strong>in</strong>vestigatc language learn<strong>in</strong>g, these variables must be conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> whatever metaphorwe have for that special social location from which a great deal of language learn<strong>in</strong>g actuallyderives.”


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 135My practical purpose <strong>in</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>g the metaphor of the classroom as culture has beento seek to offer a possible means for relat<strong>in</strong>g social and coptive variables which may <strong>in</strong>fluencelanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g; to suggest a particular frame through which we may come to understandlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a more contextually valid way. The culture of the class resembles as<strong>in</strong>gle person through <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration of psychological and social factors. A teacher or alearner is not either <strong>in</strong>dividual m<strong>in</strong>d or social actor when participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> lessons. Each is atonce cognitive and social, and so are the classroom realities which each perceives. Currentlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g research tends to exam<strong>in</strong>e psychological change <strong>in</strong> an asocial way or socialevents <strong>in</strong> a non-cognitive way. Either approach implies dist<strong>in</strong>ctiveness of psychological andsocial dimensions of learn<strong>in</strong>g and, thereby, risks offer<strong>in</strong>g both a partial account and asimplistic causal explanation of the relations between social phenomena and <strong>in</strong>dividualdevelopment. The metaphor of the classroom as culture allows us to perceive the twodimensions as irrevocably l<strong>in</strong>ked and mutually engaged. The metaphor also captures theclassroom group as a socio-cognitive dynamic which is an extension of the <strong>in</strong>dividual with<strong>in</strong>it. Because the classroom culture is a human enterprise, it provides the researcher with aliv<strong>in</strong>g subject, an <strong>in</strong>formant, not unlike a s<strong>in</strong>gle learner. When <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual'slearn<strong>in</strong>g process, we may endeavour to account for the particular permutation of attributesand activities of that learner which may <strong>in</strong>fluence the learn<strong>in</strong>g. Similarly, the study of alanguage class as culture can provide us with a holistic and <strong>in</strong>tegrated framework which<strong>in</strong>corporates the experimental and discoursal attributes of a classroom, but which alsolocates these attributes with<strong>in</strong> a richer cluster of typical characteristics.The eight features I have described are selective, and there are further features whichreflect and create the socio-cognitive realities of a language class. A classroom group willachieve <strong>in</strong>teraction, collectivism, or significance <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> own ways. But all of the featuresoverlap and <strong>in</strong>terrelate, and a class will evolve particular permutations of features over time.Just as each feature will vary as the life of the class proceeds, there will also be changes <strong>in</strong>the pattern<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>teraction of all the features. Although I would suggest that the classroomas culture and the features which represent <strong>its</strong> cultural nature are universal to languageclassrooms wherever they may be, a particular classroom will evolve both <strong>in</strong>dividual featuresand a synthesis of features <strong>in</strong> particular ways at particular times. And it is the synthesis offeatures which is the specific culture of a classroom group. If such proposals are acceptableand valid, what do they imply for undertak<strong>in</strong>g research with a language class? Also, whatdoes thc metaphor of classroom as culture offer to the languagc tcacher? I wish to concludeby briefly outl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g some major deductions for research<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g.Research<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the classroom as cultureA researcher's sympathies with what I have argued so far may be stra<strong>in</strong>ed by the seem<strong>in</strong>gcomplexity suggested for methods of <strong>in</strong>vestigation. If our goal is to move closer to therealities of language learn<strong>in</strong>g and to understand the experience of discover<strong>in</strong>g a ncwlanguage <strong>in</strong> a classroom group, then such an audacious <strong>in</strong>quiry demands anthropologicalsensitivity. The culture of the language class will resist exposure from a s<strong>in</strong>gle source ~ asampled <strong>in</strong>formant or a special moment perhaps - or through a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>vestigatory lens.Cautious triangulation has to be married with longitud<strong>in</strong>al patience! We are required toenter a cultural world - as if from Mars, perhaps ~ and <strong>in</strong>trude upon a relatively uniquesocio-cognitive process, unavoidably participat<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> as many realities as there arc people<strong>in</strong> the room. In essence, we have to critically reexam<strong>in</strong>e our own assumptions and familiarways of collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation. We will be obliged to employ what Garf<strong>in</strong>kel referred to asmethods ofunderstand~ng.'~ And such methods will lead us <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g directions:


136 MICHAEL P. BREENAn <strong>in</strong>itial question<strong>in</strong>g of our own well-established perceptions of the classroomsituation ~ <strong>its</strong> purposes, <strong>its</strong> subject matter, capacities, and social and psychologicalprocesses. (If we have learned or taught a language, or if we know the language be<strong>in</strong>gtaught for example, we are unlikely to be objectively <strong>in</strong>nocent.)A recurrent reasonable doubt about the <strong>in</strong>tegrity of the observable, and an <strong>in</strong>sistentcuriosity for learner and teacher po<strong>in</strong>ts of view.An uncover<strong>in</strong>g of the <strong>in</strong>tentions and <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>in</strong>vested <strong>in</strong> classroom activitiesand content by <strong>its</strong> participants. A search for what is significant <strong>in</strong> the immediate andexistential (historical) experienccs of the classroom for those with<strong>in</strong> it.A socio-cognitive frame of reference which will give access to mutual relationshipsbetween social activity and psychological changes. An <strong>in</strong>vestigatory template whichcan reveal social bchaviour as mentally motivated and th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g as sociallyshaped.An anthropological exploration of what, how, and why th<strong>in</strong>gs are done with<strong>in</strong> theclassroom from the perspectives of all the members of the group (and <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g theresearcher’s perspective). A discovcry, over time, of the subjective rcalities which thatclassroom conta<strong>in</strong>s and the dist<strong>in</strong>ctive <strong>in</strong>tersubjective world of the group which isevolved by them but which is also other than the sum of <strong>in</strong>dividual def<strong>in</strong>itions of thesituation.An evaluation of change and progress which accounts for <strong>in</strong>dividual antl collectivecontributions, achievements, and failures. Evaluation which seeks the <strong>in</strong>teractionsbetween <strong>in</strong>dividual and collective and which can be based upon criteria deriveddirectly from <strong>in</strong>dividual expectations and the group’s emerg<strong>in</strong>g norms and values.A study of the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal and <strong>in</strong>ter-group relationships, the roles and identitiesgenerated and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed, and the rights and duties which are entailed (and <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gthe researcher’s location <strong>in</strong> these relationships).A description and explanation of thc specific culture of thc classroom group whichaccounts for all the features of classroom life which generate thc language learn<strong>in</strong>gcontext for that group. A profile of features and their dynamic permutations whichavoids the partiality of the isolation antl comparison of a few selectcd variables.A rcscarch approach which honestly grapples with ‘observer effects’ so that we canmove from <strong>in</strong>trusion towards a reciprocity of trust and helpfulness; becom<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong>the classroom culture over time and be<strong>in</strong>g seen as contribut<strong>in</strong>g as much to the groupas we rcceive from it.If the above objectives are seen to be difficult or impossible to atta<strong>in</strong>, then our future<strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>in</strong>to classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g will need to acknowledge more explicitlythose th<strong>in</strong>gs which we have not accounted for.<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> with<strong>in</strong> the classroom as cultureAs direct participants <strong>in</strong> the culture of their language classes, teachers are very likely to behighly sensitive to the nuances ofthe features of classroom life which I have tried to describe.However, the metaphor of the classroom as culture suggests two major implications forthe language teacher. The first relates to the special task of teach<strong>in</strong>g a language, and thesecond relates to the teacher’s direct concern with the process of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> classrooms.1 . How can the culture of thc classroom be exploited as a resource for the developmentof l<strong>in</strong>guistic and communicative knowlcdgc and abilities? Although a classroom is an


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 137apprenticeship for later authentic communication and any use of the new language primarilyserves the learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g of that language, any group of language learners has twosignificant contributions to make to the development of the new language: first, <strong>in</strong>dividualprior def<strong>in</strong>itions and experiences of language and communication, of learn<strong>in</strong>g, and ofwork<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> classrooms: second, the capacity to be metal<strong>in</strong>guistic and metacommunicativc,to talk about, to explore collectively, and to reconstruct jo<strong>in</strong>tly language and <strong>its</strong> use. Thelanguage class has the communicative potential for a dialogue about subjective def<strong>in</strong>itionsof language, how language may be best learned, and how the classroom context may be bestused. The positive and explicit use of the <strong>in</strong>teractive, collective, normative, and jo<strong>in</strong>tlyconstructed nature of lessons can be a means to uncover<strong>in</strong>g and shar<strong>in</strong>g what <strong>in</strong>dividuallearners and the teacher perceive as significant for them <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a language together.And what is revealed can, <strong>in</strong> turn, provide the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>ts for later <strong>in</strong>teraction, collectiveendeavour, agreed evaluation, and the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of subsequent lessons. Put simply,a language class may be a place where the underly<strong>in</strong>g culture of that class can be mobilisedand engaged more overtly. I do not have space here to detail the practicalities of mobilis<strong>in</strong>gthe culture of the classroom for language learn<strong>in</strong>g, but I would suggest two pedagogicmotivations for such a propo~al.'~ First, a gather<strong>in</strong>g of people <strong>in</strong> a classroom provides areservoir of prior knowledge and experience -both reflective or abstract and concrete -of language and communicat<strong>in</strong>g from which any new knowledge and experience must flow.Second, the teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process requires decisions to be made, and decision-mak<strong>in</strong>ghas high communicative potential. The shar<strong>in</strong>g of decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a language class willgenerate communication which has authentic roots <strong>in</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs done here and now.2. How can the culture of the classroom help the teacher to facilitate classroom languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g? The culture of the class has the potential to reveal to the teacher the languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g process as it is actually experienced. In this way, teach<strong>in</strong>g language and <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>glanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g may be seen to be synonymous. Teachers and learners already undertakeresearch <strong>in</strong> classrooms, but their jo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>vestigation tends to focus upon subject matter -the new language and <strong>its</strong> use. An additional focus of <strong>in</strong>vestigation could be the languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g process as it actually unfolds and as it is directly experienced <strong>in</strong> the class. Manyteachers and learners already undertake such action research, but it is sometimes ratherimplicit and accorded little space and significance. I am suggest<strong>in</strong>g her- that genu<strong>in</strong>eclassroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g research may progress to the extent that those people who arcimmediately <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> everyday realities also become explicitly engaged <strong>in</strong> a methodicalreflection upon their own learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g. The pedagogic motivation would be thatteacher-learner research has the potential to facilitate a delicate understand<strong>in</strong>g andref<strong>in</strong>ement of language development with<strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>its</strong>elf. If this pedagogic purposemay be seen as valuable, then the researcher can offer knowledge and skills to a classroomrather than act only as a recipient of <strong>its</strong> riches."Learn<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the classroom as cultureI have briefly argued for the explicit use of shared decision mak<strong>in</strong>g and for teacher-learnerresearch <strong>in</strong> the language class because both seem to me pedagogically appropriate with<strong>in</strong>classrooms devoted to the discovery and development of a new language and <strong>its</strong> use. However,both proposals derive from consider<strong>in</strong>g the potential of the culture of the classroomforlanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g. Both also derive from the wish to br<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>gand the classroom experience of language learn<strong>in</strong>g closer together. The research approachsuggested earlier requires participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigators and longitud<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>volvement (at least),


138 MICHAEL P. BREENand it could lead to a positive erosion of the dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between do<strong>in</strong>g research, do<strong>in</strong>gteach<strong>in</strong>g, and learn<strong>in</strong>g.This paper is not <strong>in</strong>tended as some Rousseauesquc appeal for a return to the primitivcsavagery of classroom life, <strong>in</strong> reaction, perhaps, to a vision of f<strong>in</strong>ely-tuned classroomswhere<strong>in</strong> learners might be discoursally programmed. Nor is it <strong>in</strong>tended as a rejection ofthe metaphors of classroom as experimental laboratory or classroom as discourse.Classrooms are experiments and they are places where the discourse symbolizes significantactions and thoughts of those participat<strong>in</strong>g. And classrooms are specific cultures. All threemetaphors seem to me to be true, but all three are also partial. I have tried to show that theclassroom as culture cmbraccs variables which we may have formerly neglected <strong>in</strong> research.The metaphor can allow us to see the classroom more dist<strong>in</strong>ctly and to re-explore <strong>its</strong>potential more precisely. However, we still need to develop, dur<strong>in</strong>g the research process,sufficiently sensitive methods of <strong>in</strong>vestigation so that the culture of the language class maybe less of a metaphor and more of a revelation.I am pleased to be able to end with one of Edward Sapir’s enlighten<strong>in</strong>g observationsbecause he expressed, sixty years ago, a crucial consideration regard<strong>in</strong>g the relationshipbetween scientific efficiency and genu<strong>in</strong>e culture. Sapir comments on his importantdist<strong>in</strong>ction between human progress and cultural experience:We have no right to demand of higher levels of sophistication that they preserve tothe <strong>in</strong>dividual his manifold function<strong>in</strong>g, but wc may wcll ask whether, as acompensation, the <strong>in</strong>dividual may not reasonably demand an <strong>in</strong>tens$cation <strong>in</strong> culturalvalue, a spiritual hcighten<strong>in</strong>g of such functions as are left him.’6(1 949: 97 [my emphasis])In this paper, I have tried to argue that our professional concern with one of the <strong>in</strong>dividual’smost socially motivated functions - learn<strong>in</strong>g how to communicate with members of anothersocial group, another culture ~ requires us to understand how the <strong>in</strong>dividual may bestachieve this. And if the <strong>in</strong>dividual undcrtakcs the task <strong>in</strong> a classroom, we need to understandthe socio-cognitive expcricnce made available through the meet<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>dividual andclassroom group. The classroom may be a relatively <strong>in</strong>efficient cnvironment for themethodical mastery of a languagc system, just as it is limited <strong>in</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g opportunities forreal world communication <strong>in</strong> a new language. But the classroom has <strong>its</strong> own communicativepotential and <strong>its</strong> own authentic metacommunicative purpose. It can be a particular socialcontext for the <strong>in</strong>tensification of the cultural experience of learn<strong>in</strong>g.Notes1 This tendency has bccn captured by Kuhn’s (1962) analysis of scientific rcscarch.Research exemplify<strong>in</strong>g the first view I wish to cxplorc is represented <strong>in</strong> the cxcellentanthologies of Hatch (1 978), Felix (1 980), Scarcella and Krashen (1 983) and Baily, Long,and Peck (1 984). The second prevalent view is implicd by recent studies of classroomlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g, fairly represented <strong>in</strong> the valuable collections of Larscn-Freeman(1980), Seliger and Long (1983) and Fzrch and Kasper (1983). Of course, muchlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g research makes no rcfercnce to thc classroom and several researchersdo not assume the perspectives discussed <strong>in</strong> this paper. My emphasis is upon currently<strong>in</strong>fluential views of language learn<strong>in</strong>g and what these imply for the functions of thcclassroom.


THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 1392 Paradoxically, the features of optimal <strong>in</strong>put were <strong>in</strong>itially derived from (1) the order ofemergence of certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic features <strong>in</strong> the production of language learners and (2)the characteristics of simple codes used by people other than learners - e.g., motherese,foreigner talk, talk to foreigners, etc. Neither phenomenon has been shown to have anynecessary relationship with learn<strong>in</strong>g language. (On the relationship between mothereseand learn<strong>in</strong>g, for example, see Newport, Gleitman, and Gleitman 1977; Shantz 1982 .)Most work on learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies has tended to be <strong>in</strong>dividual case studies undertakenoutside classrooms or through simulated tasks. These po<strong>in</strong>ts are not <strong>in</strong>tended criticallybut suggest limitations <strong>in</strong> relat<strong>in</strong>g research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on learn<strong>in</strong>g to the language classroom.3 To try to teach learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies seems to me an <strong>in</strong>appropriate <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the<strong>in</strong>vestigations of, <strong>in</strong>ter alia, Naiman, Frohlich, Stern, andTodesco (1978), Rub<strong>in</strong> (198 l),and Cohen and Hosenfeld (1981). Apart from the major problem of the researcherhav<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>fer strategies from retrospections (Mann 1982) or from communicationstrategies (Fzrch and Kasper, 1983), we need to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> clear dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between theact of learn<strong>in</strong>g and the <strong>in</strong>fluences of teach<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g research currently lacksan approach to learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies and styles which accounts for key <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variables- such as the context <strong>in</strong> which the learner works and how the learner strategically reactsto that context. Examples of a more comprehensive analysis can be found <strong>in</strong> Gibson andLev<strong>in</strong> (1979, Mann (1983) and Marton, Hounsell, and Entwistle (1984).4 Although SLA research evolved from work <strong>in</strong> L1 acquisition, it has persisted <strong>in</strong> a narrowfocus upon l<strong>in</strong>guistic and mentalistic variables whilst the last decade of L1 research hasbeen characterised by <strong>its</strong> concern with social, contextual and <strong>in</strong>teractive variables also(Waterson and Snow, 1978; Lock, 1978). The significant theoretical synthesis providedto SLA research by Krashen (1981, 1982) has encouraged this asocial perspective.However, a paradox thrives at present where<strong>in</strong> it is fashionable <strong>in</strong> some quartcrs tobelittle Krashen’s <strong>in</strong>valuable contributions to the SLA paradigm whilst many researchersunquestion<strong>in</strong>gly assume his hypotheses proven as the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t of their own<strong>in</strong>vestigations. Both positions seem equally unjustifed.5 See Mueller’s (1979) historical analysis of the “science” of psychology. In this paper, I willargue for a socio-cognitive perspective on language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Current <strong>in</strong>fluential approachesto the social psychology of language learn<strong>in</strong>g seem to me too narrowly focused uponmotivational and attitud<strong>in</strong>al factors (Gardner, 1979) and, although social psychology grantssignificance to relationships between the <strong>in</strong>dividual and social context, <strong>its</strong> prevail<strong>in</strong>gtradition is non-cognitive and somewhat determ<strong>in</strong>istic <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> evaluation of the effects ofsocial experience. A socio-cognitive perspective allows us to identify variables of learn<strong>in</strong>gboth with<strong>in</strong> the social situation and with<strong>in</strong> the active cognition of the lcarner (Forgas,1981 ). It also encourages seek<strong>in</strong>g relationships between learner cognition and situationsand implies the need to understand, to see through languagc learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ways cogentlyargued by Ochsner (1979).678Allwright (1983), Gaies (1983) and Long (1983) provide excellent reviews ofclassroom-oriented rcsearch.Sapir (1 949) and Hymes (1 972) are, of course, emphasis<strong>in</strong>g collective mean<strong>in</strong>gs andvalues. Other scholars, notably Goffman (1 959) and Cicourel (1 973), would also assertthe significance of personal <strong>in</strong>tentions and <strong>in</strong>terpretations with<strong>in</strong> social events. I will arguethat we need to account for both and their <strong>in</strong>terrelationships.The notion of “genu<strong>in</strong>e culture” derives from Sapir’s discussion of “Culture, Genu<strong>in</strong>e andSpurious” (1 949). In referr<strong>in</strong>g to Mal<strong>in</strong>owski’s (1 935) study, I do not wish to imply thatwe adopt a narrow social anthropological approach to the classroom; rather one whichrelates social experience and psychological change <strong>in</strong> the tradition of Margaret Mead,Ruth Benedict, and Clyde Kluckhohn (see, for example, Beattie’s 1964 overview ofsocial anthropology). Perhaps the study of the classroom group might resemble Oscar


140 MICHAEL P. BREEN9IO11121314Lewis’s <strong>in</strong>vestigations of family life <strong>in</strong> Mexico (1 959) but with a particular focus upon therelationships between classroom life and language dcvclopment.“Interactive” is becom<strong>in</strong>g a much-used term <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g circles and is, thereby,expanded to encompass many assumptions and diverse mean<strong>in</strong>gs (as has been the fate of“functional,” “communicative,” “negotiation ,” and, when applied to pedagogy, “natural”).Ambiguity resides <strong>in</strong> the fact that human <strong>in</strong>teraction can be both <strong>in</strong>terpersonal and <strong>in</strong>trapersonal;both overtly social and covertly mental. Allwright’s (1 982, 1984a) fruitfulidentification of <strong>in</strong>teractive work as a def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g feature of classrooms clearly relates to the<strong>in</strong>terpersonal. However, <strong>in</strong>teractive work also occurs <strong>in</strong> the recreative relat<strong>in</strong>g of m<strong>in</strong>dto external phenomena (Neisser, 1976). Rut <strong>in</strong>teraction is more comprehensive than(1) overt behaviour between people and (2) covert perception and reconstructionof perceptions and experiences. We also need to regard social <strong>in</strong>teraction as hav<strong>in</strong>gpsychological roots and outcomes (Rommetveit 198 1) and mental <strong>in</strong>teraction as be<strong>in</strong>gsubject to social forces (Gauld and Shotter, 1977; Harrk, 1978; Shotter, 1978). Thus,<strong>in</strong>teraction is also (3) a socio-cognitive process which cont<strong>in</strong>ually relates social actionand experience to the content and capabilities of the m<strong>in</strong>d, and vice versa.Over the past twenty years there have been a number of <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g studies of classroomrelationships and roles with<strong>in</strong> the school system. Jackson’s (1 968) sem<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>vestigationis complemented by Hargreaves (1972) and Woods (1979) - the more recent worksecho<strong>in</strong>g Goffman’s (1 96 1 ) revelations of the effects upon the perceptions and activitiesof people <strong>in</strong> situations which ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> asymmetrical relationships. Learner experiencesand judgements have been studied by Taylor (1 962), Nash (1 974), Meighan (1 977), andHargreaves (1 977), whilst teacher perspectives are considered by Morrison andMacIntyre (1 969).A well-established tradition with<strong>in</strong> the sociology of knowledge argues that most of ourlearn<strong>in</strong>g is socially constructed. Berger and Luckmann’s (1 966) justification of such aview is based upon a phenomenological approach to human experience. (Douglas, 1973,and Luckmann, 1978, offer a range of studies whilst Filmcr, Phillipson, Silverman, andWalsh, 1972, provide an overview.) Perhaps the two major <strong>in</strong>fluences upon recentendeavours to relate social experience and knowledge have been Schultz (1 962-66,1967) and Husserl (1 965, 1967). Investigations directly concerned with the jo<strong>in</strong>tconstruction of classroom life arc exemplified with<strong>in</strong> Hargreaves (1977), Nash (1 973),Stubbs and Delamont (1 976), Woods and Hammcrslcy (1 977), and Woods (1 980a, b).The eight essential features which I describe are based on my own experience as ateacher and the shared experiences of many teachers from most countries of the worldwith whom I have worked. The features are also <strong>in</strong>fluenced by my <strong>in</strong>terpretation of anumber of scholars. Willard Waller’s (1 932) evaluation of the teach<strong>in</strong>g process is still themost comprehensive, whilst the studies of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g referred to <strong>in</strong> notes 10and 1 1 provide strong justification for see<strong>in</strong>g the classroom group as a special culture. (Ahelpful overview of classroom research with<strong>in</strong> general education is provided by Cohenand Manion 198 1 .)Garf<strong>in</strong>kel asserts the need for methods of understand<strong>in</strong>g the everyday life of the groupwe may be <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g through an cthnomcthodological approach. (Douglas, 1971,Turner 1974, and Douglas, 1973 provide examples of this approach, whilst Hughes,1980, offers a humanistic <strong>in</strong>terpretation of cthnomethodology.) For a broader criticalconsideration of methods of <strong>in</strong>vestigation, see Taylor (1 971). Interest<strong>in</strong>g examples ofcurrent research <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g which adopt various methods ofUnderstand<strong>in</strong>g are found <strong>in</strong> D<strong>in</strong>gwall (1 982), Wenden (1 983), Murphy-O’Dwycr(1 983), Allwright (1 984b), and Bonamy, Cherchalli, Johnson, Kubrusly, Schwerdtfeger,Soule-Susbielles (all 1984):In Brccn (l982), I exam<strong>in</strong>e the practical realities of classroom language and procedures.


~ (1~ (1984a)~ (1984b)~ (1984)THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 1411516The more explicit <strong>in</strong>volvement of learners is considered <strong>in</strong> Breen (1983), whilst syllabusplann<strong>in</strong>g through shared decision mak<strong>in</strong>g is discussed <strong>in</strong> Breen (1 984).This implies that my proposals for the researcher may also be directly relevant to theteach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>its</strong>elf. If the culture of the group is explicitly mobilised forshar<strong>in</strong>g decisions and for reflective <strong>in</strong>vestigation, then the generalisability of what maybe derived from that classroom may seem to be underm<strong>in</strong>ed. But more may be ga<strong>in</strong>edfrom participatory research than might be lost. We have failed, as yet, to discover actualrelationships between the classroom situation and language learn<strong>in</strong>g. We simply do notknow what the classroom contributes to the developmental process. Research whichimplies that phenomena unique to classrooms must be the contributions to learn<strong>in</strong>gwhich only classrooms can offer is trapped <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> own circularity. Objective <strong>in</strong>vestigations- through discourse analysis or the quantification of selected variables of classroom life,for example - represent little more than a researcher’s <strong>in</strong>ferenc<strong>in</strong>g and, thereby, rema<strong>in</strong>only relativcly objective.Yet we cl<strong>in</strong>g onto a faith <strong>in</strong> the chasteness of neutral impartialitywhich is assumed to be synonymous with non-participant data collection and analysis.Validity of classroom data and <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation demands direct teacher-learner<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> the research process, whilst the researcher can facilitate their explorationby contribut<strong>in</strong>g rigourous and established research methods and criteria.Sapir (1 949: 97), my emphasis.ReferencesAllwright, R.L. (1982) Interactive workfor <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> the language classroom. Keynote paper at theSecond <strong>Language</strong> Research Forum, Los Angeles.983) ‘Classroom-centered research on language teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g: A briefhistorical overview’ TESOL Quarterly 17: 191-204.‘The importance of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g’ Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics5: 156-71.Mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of classroom <strong>in</strong>struction. Presentation at the Symposium onClassroom-centred Research, AILA 7th World Congress, Brussels.Bailey, K., Long, M.H. and Peck, S. (eds) (1984) Second language acquisition studies. Rowley,MA: Newbury House.Beattie, J. (1964) Other cultures: Aims, methods and achievements <strong>in</strong> social anthropology. London:Routledge and Kegan Paul.Berger, P. and Luckmann,T. (1966) The social construction ofreality. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Bonamy, D. (1984) ‘Perceptions of saliency <strong>in</strong> a language classroom’. Unpublished M.A. thesis.University of Lancaster.Breen, M.P. (1982) ‘Authenticity <strong>in</strong> the language classroom’. Bullet<strong>in</strong> ofthe Canadian AssociationofApplied L<strong>in</strong>guistics (ACLA) 4: 7-23.~- (1983) ‘How would we recognise a communicative classroom?’, <strong>in</strong> B. Coffey (ed.) Teachertra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and the curriculum, pp. 132-54. London:The British Council.‘Process syllabuses for the language classroom’, <strong>in</strong> C.J. Brumfit (ed.) General<strong>English</strong> syllabus design, pp. 47-60. Oxford: Pergamon Press/The British Council.Cherchalli, S. (1984) Ask<strong>in</strong>g learners about language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Presentation at the Symposium onClassroom Research, University of Lancaster.Cicourel, A.V. (1973) Cognitive sociology. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Cohen, A.D. and Hosenfeld, C. (1981) ‘Some uses of mentalistic data <strong>in</strong> second languageresearch’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 31 : 285-3 13.Cohen, L. and Manion, L. (1981) Perspectives on classrooms and schools. London: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehartand W<strong>in</strong>ston.


~ (1~ (1970)~ (1982)142 MICHAEL P. BREENCole, M., Gay, J., Click, J.A. and Sharp, D.W. (1971) The culturalcontext oflearn<strong>in</strong>g and th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.London: Methuen.Cole, M. and Scribner, S. (1974) Culture and thought. NewYork: Wiley.D<strong>in</strong>gwall, S.D. (1982) ‘Critical self-reflection and decisions <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g research’, <strong>in</strong> D<strong>in</strong>gwall,S.D., Mann, S.J. and Katamba, F.X. (eds) Methods and problems <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g applied l<strong>in</strong>guisticresearch, pp. 3-26. University of Lancaster.Douglas, J.D. (ed.) (1971) Understand<strong>in</strong>g everyday 1:fi. London: Routledge and Kcgan Paul.Douglas. M. (ed.) (1973) Rules and mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Fzrch, C. and Kasper, G. (eds) (1983) Strategies <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage communicarion. London:Longman.Felix, S. W. (ed.) (1980) Second language development: trends and issues. Tub<strong>in</strong>gen: Gunter Narr.Filmer, P., Phillipson, M., Silverman, D. and Walsh, D (1972) New directions <strong>in</strong> sociologicaltheory. London: Collier Macmillan.Forgas, J.P. (ed.) (1981) <strong>Social</strong> cognition. NewYork: Academic Press.Gaies, S.J. (1983) ‘The <strong>in</strong>vestigation of language classroom processes’, TESOL Quarterly 17:205-17.Gardner, R.C. (1979) ‘<strong>Social</strong> psychological aspects of second language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> Giles, H.and St. Clair, R. (eds) <strong>Language</strong> andsocial psychology, pp. 193-200. London: Basil Blackwcll.Gauld, A. and Shotter, J. (1977) Human action and <strong>its</strong> psychological <strong>in</strong>vestigation. London:Routledge and Kegan Paul.Gibson, E.J. and Lev<strong>in</strong>, H. (1975) The psychology ofread<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Goffman, C.E. (1959) The presentation $ser<strong>in</strong> everyday If.. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.961 ) Asylums. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Hargreaves, D.H. (1972) Interpersonal relations and education. London: Routledge and KeganPaul.( 1977) ‘The proccss of typification <strong>in</strong> classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction’. British journal ofEducationalPsychology 47: 274-84.HarrC, R. (1978) ‘Accounts, actions and mean<strong>in</strong>gs: The practice of participatory psychology’,<strong>in</strong> M. Brenner et al. (eds) The social contexts ofmethod. London: Croom Helm.Hatch, E.M. (ed.) (1978) Second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Hughes, J. (1980) The philosophy ofsocial research. London: Longman.Husserl, E. (1965) Phenomenology and the crisis ofphilosophy. NewYork: Harper.Cartesian meditations.The Hague: Nijhoff.Hymes, D. (1972) ‘Models of the <strong>in</strong>teraction of language and social life’, <strong>in</strong> Gumperz, J.J.and Hymes, D. (eds) Directions <strong>in</strong> sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics. The ethnogrophy of communication,pp. 35-71. NewYork: Holt R<strong>in</strong>ehart and W<strong>in</strong>ston.Jackson, P.W. (1968) Ltfe <strong>in</strong> classrooms. NewYork: Holt R<strong>in</strong>ehart andw<strong>in</strong>ston.Johnson, P. (1984) Oral communication between non-native <strong>English</strong> speakers <strong>in</strong> the ESL practicum class.Paper at the Symposium on Classroom-centred Research, AlLA 7th World Congress,Brusscls.Krashen, S.D. (1981) Second language acquisition and second language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: PergamonPress.Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and practice <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Kubrusly, M.H. (1984) ‘Docs the teacher make a difference?’. Unpublished M.A. thesis,University of Lancaster.Kuhn, T.S. (1962) The structure ofscienttf;c revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Larsen-Freeman, D. (ed.) (1980) Discourse analysis <strong>in</strong> second language research. Rowley, MA:Newbury House.Lewis, 0. (1959) Fivefamilies: Mexican case studies <strong>in</strong> the culture ofpoverty. NewYork: Basic Books.Lock, A. (cd.) (1978) Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence oflanguage. NewYork: AcademicPrcss.


~ (1~ (1THE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 143Long, M.H. (1983) ‘Does second language <strong>in</strong>struction make a difference? A review ofresearch’. TESOL Quarterl, 17; 359-82.Luckmann, T. (cd.) ( 1978) phenomenology and sociology. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Mal<strong>in</strong>owski, B. (1935) Coral gardens and their magic. London: Allen and Unw<strong>in</strong> (2nd edition,1966).Mann, S.J. (1982) ‘Verbal reports as data: A focus on retrospcction’, <strong>in</strong> D<strong>in</strong>gwall, S.D., Mann,S.J. and Katamba, F.X. (eds) Methods and problems <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g applied l<strong>in</strong>guistic research,pp. 87-105. University of Lancaster.983) Problems <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and how thy may be solved by the reader. Paper at the 17th AnnualTESOL Convention, Toronto.Marton, F., Hounsell, D.J. and Entwistlc, N.J. (eds) (1984) The experience .f learn<strong>in</strong>g.Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh: Scottish Academic Press.Meighan, R. (1977) ‘The pupil as client: The learner’s experience of school<strong>in</strong>g’. EducationalReview 29: 123-35.Morrison, A. and MacIntyre, D. (1969) Teachers and teach<strong>in</strong>g. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Mueller, C. (1979) ‘Somc orig<strong>in</strong>s of psychology as a science’. Annual Review ofPsychology 30:9-20.Murphy-O’Dwycr, M. (1983) ‘Teachers <strong>in</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g: A diary study dur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>-scrvice course’.Unpublished M.A. thesis, Univcrsity of Lancaster.Naiman, M., Frolich, M., Stern, H.H. and Todesco, A. (1978) The good language learner.Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies <strong>in</strong> Education.Nash, R. (1973) Classrooms observed. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.__ (1974) ‘Pupil’s expectations for thcir teachers’. Research <strong>in</strong> Education, Novcmber 1974:46-7 1 .Ncisscr, U. (1976) Cognition and reality. NewYork: W.H. Freeman.Newport, E.L., Glcitman, H. and Gleitman, L.R. (1977) ‘Mother I’d rather do it myself: Somccffccts and non-effects of maternal spcech style’, <strong>in</strong> Snow, C.E. and Fcrguson, C.A. (eds)Talk<strong>in</strong>g to children:<strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong>put and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge university Press.Ochsner, R. (1979) A poctics of second language acquisition. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 29: 53-80.Rommetveit. R. (1981) ‘On mean<strong>in</strong>gs of situations and social control of such mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>human communication’, <strong>in</strong> Magnussen, D. (cd.) Toward a psychology of situations: An<strong>in</strong>teractional perspective, pp. 151-67. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Rub<strong>in</strong>, J. (1981) ‘The study of cognitive proccsscs <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 2: 117-31.Sapir, E. (1949) Culture, language and personality: Selected essays. Berkeley: University ofCalifornia Press.Scarcclla, R. and Krashen, S.D. (eds) (1983) Research <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition. Rowley, MA:Newbury House.Schutz,A. (1962, 64, 66) Collected PapersVols I-3.The Hague: Nijhoff.967) The phenomenology ofthe social world. Chicago: Northwestern University Press.Schwerdtfeger, I.C. (1984) Exercises <strong>in</strong> theforeign language c1assroorn:The pupils’po<strong>in</strong>t ofview. Papcrat thc Symposium on Foreign <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g under Classroom Conditions, AILA 7thWorld Congress. Brussels.Shatz, M. (1982) ‘On mechanisms of language acquisition: Can features of the communicativeenvironment account for development?’, <strong>in</strong> Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L.R. (eds)<strong>Language</strong> acquisition: The state .f the art, pp. 102-27. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.Shottcr, J. (1978) ‘Towards a social psychology of cvcryday life: A standpo<strong>in</strong>t “<strong>in</strong> action”’, <strong>in</strong>Brenner, M. et al. (eds), pp. 3343.Soulc-Susbielles, N. (1984) Pupils anabse their own classroom behaviour. Paper at the Symposiumon Classroom-ccntrcd Research, AILA 7thWorld Congress, Brusscls.


~ (1980b)144 MICHAEL P. BREENStubbs, M. and Delamont, S. (eds) (1976) Explorations <strong>in</strong> classroom observation. London: Wilcy.Taylor, C. (1971) ‘Interprctation and thc sciences of Man’. Review $Metaphysics xxv: 3-5 1.Taylor, P.H. (1962) ‘Children’s cvaluations of the characteristics of a good teacher’. BritishJournal ofEducationa1 Psychology 32: 258-66.Turncr, R. (ed.) (1974) Ethnornethodology. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Waller, W. (1932) The sociology ofteach<strong>in</strong>g. NewYork: Wiley.Waterson, N. and Snow, C.E. (cds) (1978) The development ofcommunication. London: Wiley.Wenden, A.L (1983) ‘The process of <strong>in</strong>tcraction’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 33: 103-21.Woods, P. and Hammerslcy, M. (eds) (1977) School experience. London: Croom Helm.Woods, P. (1979) The divided school. London: Routledgc and Kegan Paul.Woods, P. (cd.) (19804 Pupils’strategies. London: Croom Helm.Eachers’strategies. London: Croom Helm.


PART TWOStrategies and goals <strong>in</strong> theclassroom context


Chapter 8Paul KnightTHE DEVELOPMENT OF EFLMETHODOLOGYIntroductionHEN PLANNING THIS CHAPTER I CONSIDERED myownUK-basedtra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g as an EFL teacher and the fact that it conta<strong>in</strong>ed virtually no explanation ofthe practices I was tra<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>. Further tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formed me how Communicative <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> had superseded Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism, but it was not until later that further studiesmade me aware that the field of foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g has a long and rich methodologicaltradition.Ways of tcach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> have been shaped by developments <strong>in</strong> many discipl<strong>in</strong>es<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistics, psychology and education. They havc been <strong>in</strong>formed by empiricalresearch, purely theoretical developments and thc practical hands-on experience ofclassroom teachers. In order to take part <strong>in</strong> the current methodological debates, anunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of these <strong>in</strong>fluences is necessary. I hope this chapter can help foster thatunderstand<strong>in</strong>g by present<strong>in</strong>g an overview of the debates and issues, illustrated by referenceto a variety of approaches, practices and materials.First, it is important to remember that most second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, both <strong>in</strong> the pastand today, has not been <strong>in</strong>fluenced by any of the methodologies that I will review here.Outside of the UK and North America, the prevalence of multil<strong>in</strong>gualism across the globeshows that monol<strong>in</strong>gualism is the exception rather than the norm. Most second languagesarc still learnt <strong>in</strong>formally. Formal methodologies have tried to copy certa<strong>in</strong> features of<strong>in</strong>formal second language learn<strong>in</strong>g and this is someth<strong>in</strong>g to look out for as we proceed.While the term ‘method’ might bc used to describe any practical procedure for teachnga language, the term ‘methodology’ implies the existence of a set of procedurcs related byan underly<strong>in</strong>g rationale or theory of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g language. The approaches I willlook at have all been thought of by their advocates as constitut<strong>in</strong>g a ‘methodology <strong>in</strong> thatsense. I will exam<strong>in</strong>e cach of them by consider<strong>in</strong>g three questions:123What is the desired outcome?What model of language is it bascd on?What model of learn<strong>in</strong>g is it based on?


148 PAUL I


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 149This challenge to grammar-translation <strong>in</strong> the 1 qth century and the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> child language learn<strong>in</strong>g led to the development of natural approaches to language teachng.Sauver’s focus on oral <strong>in</strong>teraction and avoidance of the mother tongue <strong>in</strong> his language school<strong>in</strong> the later part of the century became known as the Natural Method, the theoreticalPr<strong>in</strong>ciples of which were outl<strong>in</strong>ed by Franke <strong>in</strong> 1884. This led to what became known asthe Direct Method, which was <strong>in</strong> turn popularised as the ‘Berlitz Method’ by MaximilianBerlitz.In the first decades of the 20th century, the forerunners of today’s applied l<strong>in</strong>guistsstarted to take the ideas of the Reform Movement further. In the United States thefoundations of Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism were be<strong>in</strong>g laid, while <strong>in</strong> the UK the Oral Approach wasdeveloped by Palmer, Hornby and others. The Oral Approach proposed pr<strong>in</strong>ciples ofselection, gradation and presentation which had been lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Direct Method (Richardsand Rogers 1986: 33). The pr<strong>in</strong>ciple that language should be <strong>in</strong>troduced and practised <strong>in</strong>situations, that is, it should be contextualised, led to the Oral Approach becom<strong>in</strong>g knownas Situational <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>.This did not mean that a situational syllabus was proposed,rather that references should be made to the real world <strong>in</strong> order to teach a structuralsyllabus, e.g. by us<strong>in</strong>g pictures, realia and actions (see Figure 8.1 for example). By the 1950sthis was the standard British approach to language teach<strong>in</strong>g. It shared with Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualismboth a structural view of language and a belief <strong>in</strong> behaviourist models of learn<strong>in</strong>g, but <strong>its</strong>focus on situations made it dist<strong>in</strong>ct.Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualismThe Second World War and <strong>its</strong> aftermath provided a great spur to language teach<strong>in</strong>g,especially <strong>in</strong> the USA. The Army Specialized Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Program (ASTP) was established <strong>in</strong>1942 to provide the large number of foreign language speakers required by the military.This programme <strong>in</strong>fluenced the dcvelopment of what became known as Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualismand was a focus of attention amongst applied l<strong>in</strong>guists long after it was wound up by themilitary.Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism saw <strong>its</strong>elf as the first ‘scientific’ language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology.Charles Fries, when he outl<strong>in</strong>ed the ‘Oral Approach’, a forerunner of Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism,saw the SUCC~SS of teach<strong>in</strong>g as depend<strong>in</strong>g not only on classroom methodology, but also:fundamentally upon hav<strong>in</strong>g satisfactory materials selected and arranged <strong>in</strong> accord withsound l<strong>in</strong>guistic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples. (Fries 1945)The pr<strong>in</strong>ciples he is referr<strong>in</strong>g to here were those of structural l<strong>in</strong>guistics, whosc ma<strong>in</strong> tenetswere that language is primarily oral, and that it is a rule-governed system understandable<strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g levels of complexity. These pr<strong>in</strong>ciples were most famously outl<strong>in</strong>edby Bloomfield <strong>in</strong> a number of works between 1914 and 1942 (Bloomfield 1914, 1933,1 942).The other important strand underly<strong>in</strong>g Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism was that of behaviouristpsychology. Behaviourist models of learn<strong>in</strong>g essentially saw language as a behavioural skillwhere learners receive a stimulus (such as a cue <strong>in</strong> a drill), respond (by provid<strong>in</strong>g the correctutterance) and then have correct responses re<strong>in</strong>forced. Error was not tolerated or<strong>in</strong>vestigated as it was thought that this will lead to the errors be<strong>in</strong>g re<strong>in</strong>forced and ‘badhab<strong>its</strong>’ engendered. <strong>Language</strong> had been viewed <strong>in</strong> terms of habit-formation before; <strong>in</strong> 1921Palmer outl<strong>in</strong>ed a theory based on what would later have been called behaviourist pr<strong>in</strong>ciples


150 PAUL I


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 151(Palmer 1921). However, it is Sk<strong>in</strong>ner who is generally credited as lay<strong>in</strong>g down the mostcomplete theoretical basis for this assumption <strong>in</strong> his Verbal Behavior, where he asserted that:We have no reason to assume . . . that verbal behaviour differs <strong>in</strong> any fundamentalrespect from non-verbal behaviour. (Sk<strong>in</strong>ner 1957: 10)The role of the learner <strong>in</strong> Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism came to be portrayed as that of an ‘empty vessel’who needs do no more than take part <strong>in</strong> the drills organised by his/her teacher to learn thetarget languagc (see Figure 8.2 for example).This is to some degree unfair; it was certa<strong>in</strong>lynot what the exponents of the method had <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. Fries outl<strong>in</strong>es the role of the studentas an active one:The student must be will<strong>in</strong>g to give himself whole-heartedly to the strenuous bus<strong>in</strong>essof learn<strong>in</strong>g the new language. (Fries 1945)What’s your job?Exercise 1Look at 13. Look at 14. Look at 15.What‘s his job? What’s her job? What are their jobs?He’s a manager. She’s a receptionist. They’re waiters.Look at 16.?Look at 17 Look at 18. Look at 19.? 7?Use these words:cleanerscooksecretaryporterFigure 8.2 A typical audio-l<strong>in</strong>gual drill


152 PAUL I


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 153<strong>in</strong>dependence and the development of the learner’s own facility to assess correctness. Thesegoals are typical of modern language methodologies; it is the way they are to be achievedthat is unique. The roles of teachers and learners are the key to this.Teachers, although silent much of the time, should be constantly monitor<strong>in</strong>g thelearners as learners’ errors are used to shape future <strong>in</strong>put. Learners are expected to beresponsible for their own learn<strong>in</strong>g, to make their own generalisations from the languagepresented to them and to self-assess their own output. Peer correction is encouraged, solearners are expected to becomc comfortable with each other. It is also thought that learnerscan ‘learn’ what thcy have been exposed to while they are sleep<strong>in</strong>g.Silent Way lessons are characterised by the use of Cuisenaire rods (coloured woodenrods of different lengths), Fidel charts (colour-coded pronunciation charts), vocabularycharts and the fact that the teacher is silent whenever possible. Typically, the teacher willmodel an utterance us<strong>in</strong>g the rods and charts and elicit student responses to it, which theteacher will accept or ask to be rephrased.The Silent Way takes an essentially traditional structural view of language. It does,however, see the spoken language as paramount. Read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g are not explicitlytaught, but are seen to follow from the spoken language.Community <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gCommunity <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g (CLL) is the name given to a teach<strong>in</strong>g methodologydeveloped by Charles Curran <strong>in</strong> the 1970s based on psychological counsell<strong>in</strong>g techniques(Curran 1972, 1976).The teacher acts as thc ‘counsellor’, and the learners are the ‘clients’.In practice this means that the teacher provides a translation of what the learners wish tosay from their L1 to the target language, thus allow<strong>in</strong>g the learners to <strong>in</strong>teract us<strong>in</strong>g thetarget language. Dialogues developed <strong>in</strong> this way then form the basis for furthcr study.It is a crucial part of the teacher’s job to create an unthreaten<strong>in</strong>g supportive atmospherewith<strong>in</strong> the classroom as this is seen to be crucial for successful learn<strong>in</strong>g. In addition,teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction should not be limited to the exchange of ‘<strong>in</strong>formation’ butshould <strong>in</strong>clude the discussion of the learners’ feel<strong>in</strong>gs about the learn<strong>in</strong>g process. Thisrelationship has been compared to that of a parent help<strong>in</strong>g a child atta<strong>in</strong> greater levels of<strong>in</strong>dependence (Richards and Rogers 1986).The desired outcome of CLL is not only that thc learner should be able to communicate<strong>in</strong> the target language, but also that he/she should learn about his/her own learn<strong>in</strong>g andtake <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g responsibility for it (Larsen-Freeman 1986).Initially CLL was not based on any new theories of language; La Forge, Curran’ssuccessor <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g CLL, saw the learners’ job as be<strong>in</strong>g to master the sound andgrammatical systems of the language (La Forge 1983), which suggests a traditional structuralsyllabus. However, he later went on to suggest a theory of language which sees language asa social process.This seems more consistent with the wider foundations of CLL as it focuseson the <strong>in</strong>teractional nature of language, someth<strong>in</strong>g mentioned earlier by Curran but notexpanded upon.SuggestopediaSuggestopedia, the system espoused by Gcorgi Lozanov, is perhaps the best-knownhumanistic method due to the media <strong>in</strong>terest it attracted and the extent of the claims madeby <strong>its</strong> proponents (Lozanov 1978). It is famous for <strong>its</strong> use of music to create a nonthreaten<strong>in</strong>gatmosphere conducive to learn<strong>in</strong>g.


154 PAUL I


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 155Communicative <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>Communicative <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> (CLT) can be said to be the current dom<strong>in</strong>antmethodology. Even <strong>in</strong> countries where CLT has not been adopted <strong>in</strong> the state sector, mostm<strong>in</strong>istries of education appear to be mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> direction. Many of <strong>its</strong> practitioners,however, would espouse it on <strong>in</strong>tuitive rather than theoretical grounds. It has become anumbrella term which covers a wide range of classroom practices. Many teacher tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gcourses teach the classroom practices without expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the underly<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, whichhas led to a mistrust of theory among many teachers. However, it is the theoretical basisof CLT which is orig<strong>in</strong>al; many of the classroom practices with which it is associated arefound elsewhere (see Figure 8.3 for examplc).If we look at the questions asked at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of this chapter, we can answer thefirst, about the desired outcome, by say<strong>in</strong>g that for CLT the desired outcome is that thelearner can communicate successfully <strong>in</strong> the target language <strong>in</strong> real situations, rather thanhave a conscious understand<strong>in</strong>g of the rules govern<strong>in</strong>g that language. (It should beremembered that this was also the outcome sought by Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism.)Our second question looked at the model of language. For CLT the model of languageis one which considers language as it is used rather than as an abstract system.The conceptof‘communicative competence’ is the key to this (Widdowson 1978, Hymes 1971, Canaleand Swa<strong>in</strong> 1980). A theoretical model of language was developed to <strong>in</strong>clude ideas abouthow language is actually used to communicate <strong>in</strong> real life situations. Chomsky had alreadyproposed a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between ‘competence’ and ‘performance’, the former be<strong>in</strong>g whatthe speaker knows and the latter be<strong>in</strong>g what the speaker actually does, with both seen <strong>in</strong>purely l<strong>in</strong>guistic terms. This idea was developed to <strong>in</strong>clude ideas of appropriacy and thesocial use of language, giv<strong>in</strong>g rise to the concept of ‘communicative competence’. In orderto def<strong>in</strong>e communicative competence, Hymes proposed a four po<strong>in</strong>t model concerned withwhat a speaker both knows and is able to use (Hymes 1971). The po<strong>in</strong>ts of this model areas follows: what is formally possible <strong>in</strong> a language, what is feasible given the means ofimplementation, what is appropriate given the context, and lastly, what is <strong>in</strong> fact done.In an environment where the exist<strong>in</strong>g orthodoxy of Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualism had beendiscredited, the concept of communicative competence helped shape new models oflanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. CLT has been described as:an approach that aims to (a) make communicative competence the goal of languageteach<strong>in</strong>g and (b) develop procedures for the teach<strong>in</strong>g of the four language skills thatacknowledge the <strong>in</strong>terdependence of language and communication. (Richards andRogers 1986: 66)These basic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples have been applied <strong>in</strong> a variety of ways. However, Richards and Rogershave isolated three key elements which they feel characterise CLT classroom practice andthe theory of learn<strong>in</strong>g underly<strong>in</strong>g it:One such element might be described as the communication pr<strong>in</strong>ciple: Activities thatpromote real communication promote learn<strong>in</strong>g. A second element is the taskpr<strong>in</strong>ciple: Activities <strong>in</strong> which language is used for carry<strong>in</strong>g out mean<strong>in</strong>gful taskspromote learn<strong>in</strong>g. A third element is the mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness pr<strong>in</strong>ciple: <strong>Language</strong> thatis mean<strong>in</strong>gful to the learner supports the lcarn<strong>in</strong>g process. (Richards and Rogers1986: 72)


~ do~ iswouldwhat156 PAUL KNIGHTLISTENING AND SPEAKINGLeav<strong>in</strong>g homePre-listen<strong>in</strong>g taskDiscuss the follow<strong>in</strong>g questions <strong>in</strong> groups.1 Do you live <strong>in</strong> the capital city of your country?a. Ifyoudoyou like it? ~b. If you don’tyou like to?- what are <strong>its</strong> attractions? - have you visited your capital city?it safe? ~ attractions does it have thatyour town doesn’t have?2 What is the population of your capital city?What is special about it?3When you go away from home (for a short or along time), do you keep <strong>in</strong> touch? How?Jigsaw listen<strong>in</strong>gDivide <strong>in</strong>to two groupsT.2a Group A You will hear David Snow, who lives <strong>in</strong> the north-west of England, talk<strong>in</strong>gabout his only daughter, Jackie.T.2b Group B You will hear Jackie, David Snow’s daughter, talk<strong>in</strong>g about her life <strong>in</strong> London.Figure 8.3 CLT materials which encouragc groupwork and participation (cont<strong>in</strong>ued opposite)


~ can~ cancan~ canTHE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 157Read and answer the questions below as you listen.(You can’t answer them all!)Comprehension check1 Why did Jackie come to London?2 When did she come?3 Where is she liv<strong>in</strong>g?4 Who is she liv<strong>in</strong>g with?5 What’s she do<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> London?6 What does her boyfriend do?7 What does she do at the weekend?8 What does she th<strong>in</strong>k of liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> London?9 How often docs she keep <strong>in</strong> touch?IO What does she th<strong>in</strong>k of her parcnts?When you have answered your questions, f<strong>in</strong>d a partner from the other group.Compare your answers and swap <strong>in</strong>formation.What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?1 Is Jackie’s father right to be so worried about his daughter? Was Jackie right to leave home ateighteen?2Use your dictionary to f<strong>in</strong>d out what generation gap means. Is there a generation gap betweenyou and your parents? Between you and your children?3 In your country, at what agepeople get married? ~they vote?they smoke?they drive?It has been observed that CLT exists <strong>in</strong> both a ‘weak’ and a ‘strong’ version (Howatt 1984).Howatt suggests that:The weak version, which has become more or less standard practice <strong>in</strong> the lastten years, stresses the importance of provid<strong>in</strong>g learners with opportunities touse their <strong>English</strong> for communicative purposes and, characteristically, attemptsto <strong>in</strong>tegrate such activities <strong>in</strong>to a wider programme of language teach<strong>in</strong>g. (Howatt1984: 297)Whilst the ‘strong’ version:advances the claim that languagc is acquired through communication, so that it isnot merely a question of activat<strong>in</strong>g an exist<strong>in</strong>g but <strong>in</strong>ert knowledge of the language,but of stimulat<strong>in</strong>g the developmcnt of the language system <strong>its</strong>elf. (Howatt 1984:297)He concludes:If the former could be described as ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g to use’ <strong>English</strong>, the latter entails ‘us<strong>in</strong>g<strong>English</strong> to learn it’. (Howatt 1984: 297)


158 PAUL I


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 159Krashen andTerrcl1 saw the Natural Approach as ‘similar to other communicative approachesbe<strong>in</strong>g developed’, and it can be seen as shar<strong>in</strong>g the same goals as CLT (Krashen andTerrell1983: 17).The Natural Approach’s uniqueness lies <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> model of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Krashen drew adist<strong>in</strong>ction between conscious learn<strong>in</strong>g and ‘acquisition’, which parallels L1 development.Only language which is ‘acquired’ is seen as be<strong>in</strong>g available for natural language use.<strong>Language</strong> which has been ‘learnt’ can be used to monitor and correct output based on‘acquired’ learn<strong>in</strong>g, but that is all; a function which has obvious time constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> naturallanguage process<strong>in</strong>g.Learners ‘acquire’ new language by be<strong>in</strong>g exposed to ‘comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put’. Such<strong>in</strong>put is def<strong>in</strong>ed by Krashen as be<strong>in</strong>g comprehensible to the learner but conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g languagejust above the learner’s current level. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Krashen it is only comprehensible <strong>in</strong>putwhich facilitates acquisition, learner output is essentially irrelevant. Also accord<strong>in</strong>g toKrashen learners are only able to acquire new grammatical structures <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> order.This is called the Natural Order Hypothesis and is based on studies of children learn<strong>in</strong>gtheir LI which suggested a certa<strong>in</strong> order of acquisition. This focus on grammaticalstructures, usually <strong>in</strong>dividual morphemes, suggests a grammatical view of language more<strong>in</strong> keep<strong>in</strong>g with the audio-l<strong>in</strong>gual tradition than CLT (Richards and Rogers 1986: 130).Krashen also thought that learn<strong>in</strong>g was <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the learner’s emotional state, anidea shared by humanistic approaches. Krashen argued that an ‘Affective Filter’ existed,which meant that learners who weren’t very motivated, lacked confidence or who wereanxious would not do as well as those who were motivated, confident and relaxed.The breadth of Krashen’s model obviously attracted a lot of attention, and it would notbe unreasonable to say that a lot of the claims on which it was based have been overturned.McLaughl<strong>in</strong> has shown that the acquisition/learn<strong>in</strong>g differentiation is hard to support andthat there is no need to postulate a ‘monitor’ based upon it (McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, 1987).Krashen’s ideas concern<strong>in</strong>g cornprehcnsible <strong>in</strong>put have also led to a great deal of debate.It has bcen clearly argued that comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put is not the only, or even the mostimportant, factor <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g (McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, 1987; White, 1987).The Natural OrderHypothesis and Affective Filter Hypothesis have also been subjected to criticism(McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, 1987). In the case of the former for methodological reasons concern<strong>in</strong>g thecollection of data; <strong>in</strong> the case of the latter because it is unclear exactly how such a filterwould work, and alternative models seem better able to expla<strong>in</strong> the evidence.It would be unfair to leave our discussion of the Natural Approach on such a criticalnote without acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong> role <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g our understand<strong>in</strong>g of the language learn<strong>in</strong>gprocess. Krashen’s model of language learn<strong>in</strong>g was an attempt to f<strong>in</strong>d a broad universalframework and although it is not widely accepted now, it has acted as a spur for a great dealof subsequent th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and debate.Task based learn<strong>in</strong>g (TBL)Task based learn<strong>in</strong>g of languages is currently attract<strong>in</strong>g a lot of attention. However, as withCLT, the def<strong>in</strong>ition of this methodology is not fixed. In general though it can bc said thatTBL methodologies:share a common idea: giv<strong>in</strong>g learners tasks to transact, rather than items to learn,provides an environment which best promotes the natural language learn<strong>in</strong>g process(Foster 1999)


160 PAUL I


1THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 161Pallavan Transport Corporation(Madras City)a Students can buy and use bus tokens for a month, buy<strong>in</strong>g a ticket for each busjourney.b The cost of tokens is as follows:30 tokens Rs 7.5060 tokens Rs 15.0090 tokens Rs 22.50120 tokens Rs 30.00c A student has to buy at least 30 tokens a month. He/she cannot buy more than 120tokens a month.d One token is equal to one bus ticket: the student has to give a token to theconductor of the bus, <strong>in</strong>stead of buy<strong>in</strong>g a ticket from him.e Tokens should be used only for the purpose of travell<strong>in</strong>g between one’s home andthe school or college where one is study<strong>in</strong>g.f Tokens should be bought each month between the 1st and the 15th. They can beused only between the 16th of that month and the 15th of the next month.g No money will be refunded on unused tokens.h Only full-time students of a school, college, or university can buy and use bustokens. They have to produce a certificate from the head of the <strong>in</strong>stitution to showthat they are full-time students.ijTokens cannot be transferred from one person to another.If a student misuses his/her tokens, he/she will not be allowed to buy any moretokens dur<strong>in</strong>g that year.Pre-task After a gloss<strong>in</strong>g, at the students’ request, of some words (for example‘refunded’, ‘misused’) and a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary discussion, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g questions, about thenature of some rules (for example on the po<strong>in</strong>t that tokens can be bought only <strong>in</strong>multiples of thrty and that a direct bus from home to school <strong>in</strong>volves the use of a s<strong>in</strong>gletoken while a change of buses <strong>in</strong>volves us<strong>in</strong>g one token on each bus), the follow<strong>in</strong>gcase is discussed as the pre-task:IRaman is a student of the Government Arts College <strong>in</strong> Nandanam. He lives <strong>in</strong>T. Nagar.He has classes from Monday to Friday each week and eats his lunch at the collegecanteen.There are direct buses fromT. Nagar to Nandanam.1 How many bus tokens does Raman need each week?2 How many tokens does he need for a month (i.e. 4 weeks, by convention)?3 A bus ticket from T. Nagar to Nandanam costs Rs 0.50. How much does Ramansave by buy<strong>in</strong>g tokens?4 How many tokens should he buy each month? Why? How many will he actuallyuse?5 Raman’s brother goes to a High School <strong>in</strong> Saidapet. Can he use Raman’s extratokens? How do you know?


162 PAUL KNIGHT6 Raman goes to see his uncle <strong>in</strong> K. K. Nagar every Sunday. Can he use his tokens togo to K. K. Nagar? How do you know?Task Balan studies at the Higher Secondary School <strong>in</strong> Nungambakkam. His home is <strong>in</strong>Adyar. He has classes only <strong>in</strong> the afternoons, from Monday to Saturday.There are directbuses from Nungambakkam to Adyar and a ticket costs one rupee.1 How many tokens does Balan need each month?2 How many tokens should he buy each month? How much money does he save?3 He bought 60 tokens <strong>in</strong> July. His school had some holidays <strong>in</strong> August, so he usedonly 30 tokens up to 15 August.a Can he go on us<strong>in</strong>g the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 30 tokens? How do you know?b Can he return the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 30 tokens and get back the money? How can youtell?Figure 8.4 A typical Prabhu taskHav<strong>in</strong>g used Long’s and Crookes’ analysis ofTBL, we now come to the model that theypropose, known as task-based language teach<strong>in</strong>g (TBLT) .They argue that this model is soundlybased on SLA research, on classroom-centred research and on pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of syllabus andcourse design (Long and Crookes 1992: 41). A dist<strong>in</strong>ctive feature of this model is that itencourages a ‘focus on form’. This is not a traditional structural syllabus approach, but anacknowledgement that acquisition can be accelerated if learners’ attention is drawn to specificl<strong>in</strong>guistic features of the target language (Long 1991). In develop<strong>in</strong>g the model ofTBLTfurther, Long has outl<strong>in</strong>ed those features which should characterise a ‘task’ and attemptedto provide a solid theoretical framework for an approach based on them (Long 1996, et al.).However, there are still questionsTBLT needs to address. Long and Crookes acknowledgethis when they compare it to otherTBL approaches (Long and Crookes 1992: 46). Itsresearch base is still small and no complete programmes have yet been undertaken to accessit. The question of sequenc<strong>in</strong>g tasks is still an issue, as is the question of produc<strong>in</strong>g ataxonomy of tasks. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the degree of reduced learner autonomy could <strong>in</strong>vite criticism.Long and Crookes’ model has also never actually been realised <strong>in</strong> terms of materialsdevelopment or classroom practice, <strong>in</strong> contrast to Prabhu’s model or Breen and Candl<strong>in</strong>’s.Overal1,TBL looks like a very excit<strong>in</strong>g area and one which is already strongly <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>gth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the field of language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology. It is not just limited to those modelsdescribed here; other models are be<strong>in</strong>g proposed and specific questions of task def<strong>in</strong>itionand design are also be<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ed (Skehan 1996, 1998; Nunan 1989, etc.).Text-based teach<strong>in</strong>gAnother new post-CLT approach to language teach<strong>in</strong>g has been text-based teach<strong>in</strong>g (alsoknown as genre-based). Unlike TBL, which we saw is based on a model of learn<strong>in</strong>g, textbasedlearn<strong>in</strong>g grew out of a model of language, namely Systemic-Functional Grammar. Itis an approach which has been summarised <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g observation:<strong>Language</strong> occurs as whole texts which are embedded <strong>in</strong> the social contexts <strong>in</strong> whichthey are used.People learn language through work<strong>in</strong>g with whole texts. (Feez 1998)


THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 163This approach is perhaps better known and more widely applied <strong>in</strong> Australia, wheremuch of the theory was developed, than elsewhere. Its development there has primarilyoccurred with<strong>in</strong> the provision of <strong>English</strong> as a second language for migrants, as well as moregenerally <strong>in</strong> language and literacy programmes. <strong>English</strong> for Academic Purposes (EAP)programmes havc also been <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>novations.Systemic-Functional Grammar describes language not only <strong>in</strong> terms of l<strong>in</strong>guisticsystems, but relates these to the social <strong>in</strong>teraction they are used to undertake and the widerculture <strong>in</strong> which they are used. This model of language was first proposed by Halliday andalso greatly <strong>in</strong>fluenced CLT (Halliday 1973).The model of learn<strong>in</strong>g upon which this method is based is <strong>in</strong>formed by research <strong>in</strong> firstlanguage acquisition. Learn<strong>in</strong>g is seen as a process of acculturisation <strong>in</strong>to the ‘culture’ of thetarget language with learners perceive as go<strong>in</strong>g through an ‘apprenticeship’ process as theylearn more and <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependence. The degree to which learners are expected todevelop declarative knowledge about the target language has been debated by proponentsof text-based methodologies and, <strong>in</strong> general, some declarative knowledge is seen asdesirable, <strong>in</strong> other words, learners are expected to become, to some degree, languageanalysts (see Figure 8.5 for example). This contrasts with both the ideas of Audio-L<strong>in</strong>gualismand CLT, where declarative knowledge is not seen as a necessary outcome of learn<strong>in</strong>g.This br<strong>in</strong>gs us to the question of learner and teacher roles with<strong>in</strong> this approach. Textbasedapproaches can be seen as more teacher-centred than other current methodologiesas the role of teacher as ‘expert’ is central. Typically, the teacher would lead the <strong>in</strong>itialexploration of a text type, then the teacher and learners jo<strong>in</strong>tly construct a text, followedby sole production by the learners.This model is based on first language acquisition parentchildroles, as well asvygotskian notions of the social <strong>in</strong>teractional nature of communicationand learn<strong>in</strong>g.It will be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to see this methodology develop further as more materials basedon it become available and it becomes taken up more widely.ConclusionHow does one conclude an outl<strong>in</strong>e of a process which has been underway for centuries ~namely the search for better ways to teach languages?This search has probably never beenas <strong>in</strong>tense as it is today, with universities, classroom teachers and publishers all active. Therealisation that this is an ‘on-go<strong>in</strong>g’ process is perhaps the first step. This might make usapproach more critically the claims of researchers and publishers who are try<strong>in</strong>g to promoteparticular solutions. Instcad, with a sense of historical perspective, we should assess eachnew development ourselves. This assessment should draw on the discipl<strong>in</strong>es which <strong>in</strong>formour field, not only second language acquisition theory, but psychology and general educationas well. Our three questions from the <strong>in</strong>troduction that we have used to exam<strong>in</strong>e themethodologies presented here can provide a start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t. We should not ignore our ownexperience either; classroom-centred research has been one of the most important stepsforward <strong>in</strong> recent years. In this way the field of language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology will rema<strong>in</strong>vibrant and excit<strong>in</strong>g.


~ (1~ (1~ (1~ (1933)164 PAUL KNIGHTUNIT OF WORKCASUAL CONVERSATIONGoalTo enable learncrs to participate <strong>in</strong> a casual conversation <strong>in</strong> a workplace.Learner objectivesThe learners will:- understand the purpose of casual conversation <strong>in</strong> Australian workplace culture- know which conversation topics are appropriate <strong>in</strong> Australian workplaces- recognise and use thc key features of a casual conversation, i.e. greet<strong>in</strong>gs andclosures, feedback, clarification, manag<strong>in</strong>g topic shifts- recognise and use conversation chunks such as comments, descriptions or recounts- take turns appropriately with<strong>in</strong> simple exchanges ie question/answer, statement,agreement, statement/disagreement- use language appropriate to casual conversation <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g politeness strategies,<strong>in</strong>formal language, idiom- build pronunciation and paral<strong>in</strong>guistic skills and strategies, specifically <strong>in</strong> the areasof <strong>in</strong>tonation and gestureTeacher objectivesThe teacher will:- provide authentic listen<strong>in</strong>g materials- provide conversation practice through scaffolded rolcplay- record learner language for analysisAchievement assessmentThe unit will enable students to achieve the follow<strong>in</strong>g curriculum outcome, eg CSWEI11 Competency 7.Figure 8.5 An examplc of unit objectives with<strong>in</strong> a text-bascd approachReferencesAnthony, E.M. (1963) ‘Approach, method and technique’. <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: 63-7.Asher, J. (1965) ‘The strategy of the total physical response: an application to learn<strong>in</strong>gRussian’. International Review $Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 3: 291-300.966) ‘The learn<strong>in</strong>g strategy of the total physical response: a review’. Modern <strong>Language</strong>Journal 50: 79-84.969) ‘The total physical response approach to second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. Modern<strong>Language</strong>journal 53: 3-17.977) Learn<strong>in</strong>g Another <strong>Language</strong> Through Actions: The Complete Teacher’s Guide Book. LosGatos, Calif.: Sky Oaks Productions Inc.Bcretta, A. and Davies, A. (1985) ‘Evaluation of the Bangalore Project’. ELT/ournal39/2.Bloomfield, L. (1914) An Introduction to rhe Study oflanguage. NewYork: Holt.<strong>Language</strong>. NewYork: Holt.


~ (1942)~ (1987)~ (1987)~ (1~ (1~ (1THE DEVELOPMENT OF EFL METHODOLOGY 165Outl<strong>in</strong>e Guide for the Practical Study .f Foreign <strong>Language</strong>s. Baltimore: L<strong>in</strong>guisticSociety of America.Breen, M.P. (1984) ‘Process syllabuses for the language classroom’, <strong>in</strong> C.J. Brumfit (ed.),General <strong>English</strong> syllabus design. (ELT Documents No. 118, 47-60). London: PergamonPress &The British Council.‘Learner contributions to task design’, <strong>in</strong> C.N. Candl<strong>in</strong> and D. Murphy (eds)Lancaster Practical Papers <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Education: Vol 7. <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Breen, M.P. and Candl<strong>in</strong>, C.N. (1980) ‘The essentials of a communicative curriculum <strong>in</strong>language teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 1 (2). 89-1 12.Canale, M. and Swa<strong>in</strong>, H. (1980) ‘Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to secondlanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g and test<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics I /l. 1-7Candl<strong>in</strong>, C.N. (1984) ‘Syllabus design as a critical process’, <strong>in</strong> C.J. Brumfit (ed.) General <strong>English</strong>syllabus design. (ELT Documcnts No. 118, 29-46). London: Pergamon Press and TheBritish Council.‘Towards task-based language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> C.N. Candl<strong>in</strong> and D. Murphy (eds)Lancaster Practical Papers <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Education: Vol 7. <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Candl<strong>in</strong>, C.N. and Murphy, D. (eds) (1987) Lancaster Practical Papers <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong>Education:Vol 7. <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Chomsky, N. (1957) Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.Chomsky, N. (1966) Aspects ofthe Theory ofsyntax. Boston: MIT Press.Curran, C.A. (1972) Counsel<strong>in</strong>g-Learn<strong>in</strong>g: A Whole-Person Model for Education. New York: Gruneand Stratton.976) Counsel<strong>in</strong>g-Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong>s. Apple River, 111. : Apple River Press.Ellis, R. (1994) The Study ofsecond <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Feez, S. (1998) Text-based Syllabus Design. Sydney: NCELTR, Macquarie University.Foster, P. (1999) ‘Task-based learn<strong>in</strong>g and pedagogy’. ELTjournal53/1. 69-70.Fries, C. (1945) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> us a Foreign <strong>Language</strong>. Michigan: MichiganUniversity Press.Gattengo, C. (1972) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Schools: The Silent Way (2nd ed.). New York:Educational Solutions.976) The Common Sense of<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong>s. NewYork: Educational Solutions.Halliday, M.A.K. (1973) ‘Towards a sociological semantics’. Explorations <strong>in</strong> the Functions of<strong>Language</strong>. London: Edward ArnoldHartley, B. andv<strong>in</strong>e, P. (1978) Streaml<strong>in</strong>e <strong>English</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Howatt, A.P.R. (1984) A History of<strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Hymes, D.H. (1971) On communicative competence. Philadelphia: University of PhiladelphiaPress.Kle<strong>in</strong>jans, E. (1961) ‘From Mim-Mem to Communication’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Descriptive and AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics: Bullet<strong>in</strong> ofthe Summer lnstitute <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guisticsVol 1 .Krashen, S.D. (1981) Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition and Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford:Pergamon.982) Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and Practices <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford, Pergammon.Krashen, S.D. and Tcrrell, T. (1983) The Natural Approach: Languuge Acquisition <strong>in</strong> the Classroom.Oxford, Pergamon.La Forge, P.G. (1983) Counsel<strong>in</strong>g and Culture <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.Lado, R. (1964) <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>:A scient$c approach. NewYork: McGraw-Hill.Larsen-Freeman, D. (1986) Techniques and Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.Long, M.H. (1991) ‘Focus on form: A dcsign feature <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology’, <strong>in</strong>


~ (1~ (1~~~ (1166 PAUL KNIGHTForeign language research <strong>in</strong> cross-cultural perspective, K. de Bot, D. Coste, R. G<strong>in</strong>sberg andC. Kramsch (eds). Amsterdam: John Benjam<strong>in</strong>s.996) ‘The role of the l<strong>in</strong>guistic environment <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong>Handbook $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition, W.C.B. Ritchie (ed.).T K, Academic Press.Long, M.H. and Crookes, G. (1 992) ‘Three Approaches toTask-Based Syllabus Design’. TESOLQuarterly 26:l: 27-55.Lozanov, G. (1978) Suggestology and Outl<strong>in</strong>es ofSuggestopedy. NewYork: Gordon and Breach.McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, B. (1987) Theories of Second-<strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. London: Edward Arnold.Moskowitz, G. (1978) Car<strong>in</strong>g and Shar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Class. Rowley, Mass. : NewburyHouse.Mueller,T. (1 959) ‘Psychology and the <strong>Language</strong> Arts’. School and Society 87.Munby, J. (1978) Communicative Syllabus Design. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Nunan, D. (1988) The Learner-Centred Curriculum. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.989) Design<strong>in</strong>g Tasksfor the Communicative Classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.O’Connor, J.C. and Twaddell, W.F. (1960) ‘Intensive Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for an Oral Approach <strong>in</strong><strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>’. The Modern <strong>Language</strong> Journal Vol XLIV 2:2.Palmer, H.E. (192 1) The Oral Method <strong>in</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Language</strong>s. Cambridge: Heffer.Palmer, H.E. and Palmer, D. (1925) <strong>English</strong> Through Actions. Toyko: IRET. Republished byLongmans, Green, 1959.Prabhu, N.S. (1987) Second <strong>Language</strong> Pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Richards, J.C. and Rogers,T.S. (1986) Approaches and Methods <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Scovel, T. (1979) ‘Review of “Suggestology and Outl<strong>in</strong>es of Suggestopedy”’ . TESOL Quarterly13. 255-266.Skehan, P. (1996) A framework for the implementation of task-based <strong>in</strong>struction. AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 17: 1 : 38-62.998) A Cognitive Approach to <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, B.F. (1957) Verbal Behavior. NewYork: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Stern, H.H. (1 983) Fundamental Concepts of <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Stern, H.H. (1992) Issues and Options <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.White, L. (1987) ‘Aga<strong>in</strong>st Comprehensible Input’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 8: 95-109.Widdowson, H.G. (1978) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Language</strong> as Communication. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.


Chapter 9Jack C. RichardsBEYOND METHODSETHODOLOGY IN TEACHING IS THE ACTIVITIES, tasks, andM learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences used by the teacher with<strong>in</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g process.Methodology is seen to have a theoretical basis <strong>in</strong> the teacher’s assumptions about (a)language and second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, (b) teacher and learner roles, and (c) learn<strong>in</strong>gactivities and <strong>in</strong>structional materials. These assumptions and beliefs provide the basis forthe conscious or unconscious decision mak<strong>in</strong>g that underlies the moment-to-momentprocesses of teach<strong>in</strong>g. Methodology is not therefore someth<strong>in</strong>g fixed, a set of rigid pr<strong>in</strong>ciplesand procedures that the teacher must conform to. Rather it is a dynamic, creative, andexploratory process that beg<strong>in</strong>s anew each time the teacher encounters a group of learners.<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> as an exploratory process is different from the approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g seen <strong>in</strong> manyteacher preparation programs or language teach<strong>in</strong>g programs, where particular <strong>in</strong>structionalmethods, such as the Silent Way, Total Physical Response, or the Natural Approach, arepresented as models to be imitated and <strong>in</strong>ternalized. In this chapter, these two approachesto teach<strong>in</strong>g will be explored <strong>in</strong> more depth.The use of methods as the basis for <strong>in</strong>structionalprocesses <strong>in</strong> a second language program will be compared with one that moves beyondmethods and focuses on explor<strong>in</strong>g the nature of effective classroom teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g.Approach<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of methodsFor many centuries the goal of language teachers has been to f<strong>in</strong>d the right method (Kelly1969).The history of language teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the last hundred years has done much to supportthe impression that improvements <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g will result from improvements <strong>in</strong>the quality of methods, and that ultimately an effective language teach<strong>in</strong>g method will bedeveloped. Some breakthrough <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic theory or <strong>in</strong> second language acquisitionresearch, it is assumed, will eventually unlock the secrets of second and foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g. These will then be <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to a new supermethod that will solve thelanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g problem once and for all. Some believe that the supermethod has alreadybeen found, and that adoption of a method such as the Silent Way, Suggestopedia, or theNatural Approach will br<strong>in</strong>g about dramatic improvements <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Common to all methods is a set of specifications for how teach<strong>in</strong>g should beaccomplished, derived from a particular theory of the nature of language and secondlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g. Differences <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>structional specifications reflect differences <strong>in</strong> thetheories underly<strong>in</strong>g the methods. Some methods advocate an early emphasis on speak<strong>in</strong>g as


168 JACI< C. RICHARDSa basis for establish<strong>in</strong>g basic language patterns. Others recommend that speak<strong>in</strong>g be delayeduntil the learner has built up a receptive competence <strong>in</strong> the language. Some make use ofmemorized dialogues and texts; others requirc that learners attempt to communicate witheach other as soon as possible us<strong>in</strong>g their own language resources. Common to all methodsis a set of prescriptions on what teachers and learners should do <strong>in</strong> the language classroom.Prescriptions for the teacher <strong>in</strong>clude what material should be presented and when it shouldbe taught and how, and prescriptions for learners <strong>in</strong>clude what approach they should taketoward learn<strong>in</strong>g. Specific roles for teachers, learners, and <strong>in</strong>structional materials are henceestablished (Richards and Rodgers 1986). The teacher’s job is to match his or her teach<strong>in</strong>gstyle as well as the learners’ learn<strong>in</strong>g styles to the method. Special tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g packages andprograms are available for some methods to ensure that teachers do what they are supposedto do and teach accord<strong>in</strong>g to the method.Despite the appeal of methods, their past history is somewhat of an embarrassment.Studies of the effectiveness of specific methods have had a hard time demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g that themethod <strong>its</strong>elf, rather than other factors, such as the teacher’s cnthusiasm or the novelty ofthe new method, was the crucial variable. Likewise, observers of teachers us<strong>in</strong>g specificmethods have reported that teachers seldom conform to the methods they are supposed tobe follow<strong>in</strong>g. Swaffar, Arens, and Morgan (1 982), for example, <strong>in</strong>vestigated differencesbetween what they termed rationalist and empiricist approaches to foreign language<strong>in</strong>struction. By a rationalist approach they refer to process-oriented approaches <strong>in</strong> whichlanguage is seen as an <strong>in</strong>terrelated whole, where language learn<strong>in</strong>g is a function ofcomprehension preced<strong>in</strong>g production, and where it <strong>in</strong>volves critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and the desireto communicate. Empiricist approaches focus on the four discrete language skills. Wouldclassroom practices reflect such differences? “One consistent problem is whether or notteachers <strong>in</strong>volvcd <strong>in</strong> present<strong>in</strong>g materials created for a particular method are actuallyreflect<strong>in</strong>g the underly<strong>in</strong>g philosophies of these methods <strong>in</strong> their classroom practices” (Swaffaret a/. 1982: 25). Swaffar et a/. found that many of the dist<strong>in</strong>ctions used to contrast methods,particularly those based on classroom activities, did not exist <strong>in</strong> actual practice:Methodological labels assigned to teach<strong>in</strong>g activities are, <strong>in</strong> themselves, not<strong>in</strong>formative, because they refer to a pool of classroom practices which are useduniformly.The differenccs among major methodologies are to be found <strong>in</strong> the orderedhierarchy, the priorities assigned to tasks. (1 982: 3 1)Methods hence make assumptions about the nature of teach<strong>in</strong>g that are not based on studyof the process of teach<strong>in</strong>g. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of Swaffar et a/. account for the difficulty teachersupervisors often have <strong>in</strong> recogni~<strong>in</strong>g which method a teacher is follow<strong>in</strong>g. Nevertheless,the future for methods cont<strong>in</strong>ues to look good. Several new ones have appeared <strong>in</strong> recentyears, and at conferences where salespersons for the new methods are present, teachersflock to hear prescntations on the current supermethods. Yet there are serious limitations<strong>in</strong> conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of methods.The basic problem is that methods present a predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed, packaged deal for teachersthat <strong>in</strong>corporates a static view of teach<strong>in</strong>g. In this view specific teacher roles, learner roles,and teach<strong>in</strong>g/learn<strong>in</strong>g activities and processes are imposed on teachers and learners. Studiesof classroom events, however, have demonstrated that teach<strong>in</strong>g is not static or fixed <strong>in</strong> timebut is a dynamic, <strong>in</strong>teractional process <strong>in</strong> which the teacher’s “method” results from theprocesses of <strong>in</strong>teraction between the teacher, the learners, and the <strong>in</strong>structional tasks andactivities over time (Chall 1967; Dunk<strong>in</strong> and Biddle 1974; Swaffar et a/. 1982). Attemptsto f<strong>in</strong>d general methods that are suitable for all teachers and all teach<strong>in</strong>g situations reflect


BEYOND METHODS 169an essentially negative view of teachers, one which implies that s<strong>in</strong>ce the quality of teacherscannot be guaranteed, the contribution of the <strong>in</strong>dividual teacher should be m<strong>in</strong>imized bydesign<strong>in</strong>g teacher-proof methods. The assumption that underlies general, all-purposemethods is hence essentially this:Teachers cannot be trusted to tcach well. Left to their owndevices, teachers will <strong>in</strong>variably make a mess of th<strong>in</strong>gs. A method, because it imposes auniform set of teach<strong>in</strong>g roles, teach<strong>in</strong>g styles, teach<strong>in</strong>g strategies, and teach<strong>in</strong>g techniqueson the teacher, will not be affected by the variations that are found <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual teach<strong>in</strong>gskill and teach<strong>in</strong>g style <strong>in</strong> the real world.Researchers who have <strong>in</strong>vestigated the nature of teach<strong>in</strong>g, however, have proposed adifferent view of teach<strong>in</strong>g (Good 1979; Elliot 1980; Tikunoff 1985). They beg<strong>in</strong> with theassumption that teachers (rather than methods) do make a difference; that teachers work<strong>in</strong> ways that are, to an extent, <strong>in</strong>dependent of methods; and that the characteristics ofeffective teach<strong>in</strong>g can be determ<strong>in</strong>ed. Other researchers have turned their attention tolearners and sought to determ<strong>in</strong>e what characterizes effective learn<strong>in</strong>g. This requires adifferent approach to teachng, one <strong>in</strong> which teachers are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g and reflect<strong>in</strong>gupon their own teach<strong>in</strong>g as well as the learn<strong>in</strong>g behaviors of their students.The nature of effective teach<strong>in</strong>gTeacher strategiesEvery teacher aims to be an effective teacher.The concept of effective teachng is a somewhatelusive one, however. Can it be determ<strong>in</strong>ed from the teacher’s behavior, the learner’sbehavior, classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, or the results of learn<strong>in</strong>g? Researchers have attempted tooperationalize the notion of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g by describ<strong>in</strong>g it as teach<strong>in</strong>g that produceshigher-than-predicted ga<strong>in</strong>s on standardized achievement tests (Good 1979). Studies ofteacher effectiveness have dealt ma<strong>in</strong>ly with first language classrooms and with the teach<strong>in</strong>gof read<strong>in</strong>g and math. One major study has dealt with effective teachers <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual programs(Tikunoff et al. 1980). These studies are characterized by detailed observation of teachersperform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional activities <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>in</strong> an attempt to isolate the qualities andskills of effective teachers.In a comprehensive survey of the research on effective school<strong>in</strong>g, Blum (1984: 3-6)summarizes effective classroom practices as follows:1234S6789101 112Instruction is guided by a preplanned curriculum.There are high expectations for student learn<strong>in</strong>g.Students are carefully oriented to lessons.Instruction is clear and focused.Learn<strong>in</strong>g progress is monitored closely.When students don’t understand, they are retaught.Class time is used for learn<strong>in</strong>g.There are smooth and efficient classroom rout<strong>in</strong>es.Instructional groups formed <strong>in</strong> the classroom fit <strong>in</strong>structional needs.Standards for classroom behavior are high.Personal <strong>in</strong>teractions between teachers and students are positive.Incentives and rewards for students are used to promote excellence.Several dimensions of teach<strong>in</strong>g have been found to account for differences betwecn effectiveand <strong>in</strong>effective <strong>in</strong>struction (Doyle 1977; Good 1979) .These <strong>in</strong>clude classroom management,structur<strong>in</strong>g, tasks, and group<strong>in</strong>g.


170 JACK C. RICHARDSClassroom managementClassroom management refers to the ways <strong>in</strong> which student behavior, movement, and<strong>in</strong>teraction dur<strong>in</strong>g a lesson are organized and controlled by the teacher to enable teach<strong>in</strong>gto take place most effectively. Good managerial skills on the part of the teacher are anessential component of good teach<strong>in</strong>g. In a well-managed class, discipl<strong>in</strong>e problems are few,and learners are actively engaged <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks and activities; this contributes to highmotivation and expectations for success. Evertson, Anderson, and Brophy (1 978) found thatit was possible to identify teachers with managerial problems <strong>in</strong> the first few days of theschool year, that such problems cont<strong>in</strong>ued throughout the year, and that managerial skills<strong>in</strong> the classroom were related to levels of student <strong>in</strong>volvement.Structur<strong>in</strong>gA lesson reflects the concept of structur<strong>in</strong>g when the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>tentions are clear and<strong>in</strong>structional activities are sequenced accord<strong>in</strong>g to a logic that students can perceive.Classroom observations and studies of lesson protocols <strong>in</strong>dicate that sometimes neither theteacher nor the learners understood what the <strong>in</strong>tentions of an activity were, why an activityoccurred when it did, what directions they were supposed to follow, or what the relationshipbetween one activity and another was. Hence, it may not have been clcar what studentsneeded to focus on to complete a task successfully. Fisher et a1. (1 980) conclude that students“pay attention more when the teacher spends time discuss<strong>in</strong>g the goals or structures of thelesson and/or giv<strong>in</strong>g directions about what the students are to do” (p. 26). Berl<strong>in</strong>er (1 984)likewise suggests that “structur<strong>in</strong>g affects attention rate: it is sometimes not done at all,sometimes it is done only m<strong>in</strong>imally, and sometimes it is overdone”(p. 63).Tasks, or activity structures, refer to activities that teachers assign to atta<strong>in</strong> particular learn<strong>in</strong>gobjectives. For any given subject at any given level, a teacher uses a limited repertoire oftasks that essentially def<strong>in</strong>e that teacher’s methodology of teach<strong>in</strong>g. These might <strong>in</strong>cludecomplet<strong>in</strong>g worksheets, read<strong>in</strong>g aloud, dictation, quickwrit<strong>in</strong>g, and practic<strong>in</strong>g dialogues.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Tikunoff (I 985) , class tasks vary accord<strong>in</strong>g to three types of demands theymake on learners: response mode demands (the k<strong>in</strong>d of skills they demand, such as knowledge,comprehcnsion, application, analysis/synthesis, evaluation); <strong>in</strong>teractional mode demands (therules govern<strong>in</strong>g how classroom tasks are accomplished, such as <strong>in</strong>dividually, <strong>in</strong> a group, orwith the help of the teacher); and task complexity demands (how difficult the learner perceivesthe task to be).Teachers have to make decisions not only about the appropriate k<strong>in</strong>ds of tasks to assignto learners, but also about the order of tasks (the sequence <strong>in</strong> which tasks should be<strong>in</strong>troduced); pac<strong>in</strong>g (how much time learners should spend on tasks); products (whether theproduct or result of a task is expected to he the same for all students); learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies(what learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies will be recommended for particular tasks); and materials (whatsources and materials to use <strong>in</strong> complet<strong>in</strong>g a task) (Tikunoff 198s).The concept of tasks has been central to studies of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g. The amount oftime students spend actively engaged on learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks is directly related to learn<strong>in</strong>g (Goodand Beckerman 1978). For example,Teacher A andTeacher B are both teach<strong>in</strong>g the sameread<strong>in</strong>g lesson. InTeacher A’s class, learners are actively engaged <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g tasks for 75%of the lesson, the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g time be<strong>in</strong>g occupied with non<strong>in</strong>structional activities such as


BEYOND METHODS 171tak<strong>in</strong>g breaks, l<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g up, distribut<strong>in</strong>g books, homework, and mak<strong>in</strong>g arrangements for futureevents. Students <strong>in</strong> Teacher B’s class, however, are actively <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g for only55% of the lesson. Not surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, studies of time-on-task have found that the more timestudents spend study<strong>in</strong>g content, the better they learn it. In one study (Stall<strong>in</strong>gs andKaskowitz 1974), the students with the highest levels of achievement <strong>in</strong> a read<strong>in</strong>g programwere spend<strong>in</strong>g about 50% more time actively engaged <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g activities than the childrenwith the lowest achievement ga<strong>in</strong>s. Good teach<strong>in</strong>g is hence said to be task oriented. Effectiveteachers also monitor performance on tasks, provid<strong>in</strong>g feedback on how well tasks havebeen completed.Group<strong>in</strong>gA related dimension of effective teachng is the group<strong>in</strong>j of learners to carry out <strong>in</strong>structionaltasks, and the relation between group<strong>in</strong>g arrangement and achievement. An effective teacherunderstands how different k<strong>in</strong>ds of group<strong>in</strong>g (such as seat work, pair work, discussion,read<strong>in</strong>g circle, or lecture) can impede or promote learn<strong>in</strong>g. Webb (1 980) found that themiddle-ability child suffers a loss of achievement, while the low-ability child shows somega<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> achievement <strong>in</strong> mixed-ability groups, compared with what would be expectedif both were <strong>in</strong> uniform-ability groups. Tikunoff (1985) cites Good and Marshall’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gson group<strong>in</strong>gs.Good and Marshall (1984) found that students <strong>in</strong> low-ability read<strong>in</strong>g groups <strong>in</strong> theearly grades received very little challenge, thus perceiv<strong>in</strong>g of themselves as unable toread. In addition, a long-range result of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g most frequently with only otherstudents of low-ability <strong>in</strong> such groups was an <strong>in</strong>ability to respond to the demands ofmore complex <strong>in</strong>structional activities. Ironically, Good po<strong>in</strong>ted out that the verystrategy used to presumably help low-ability youngsters with their read<strong>in</strong>g problems- pull-out programs <strong>in</strong> which teachers worked with small groups of these studentsoutside the regular classroom ~ exacerbated the problem. Demands <strong>in</strong> the specialread<strong>in</strong>g groups were very different from those <strong>in</strong> the regular classroom and at a muchlower level of complexity, so low-ability students were not learn<strong>in</strong>g to respond tohigh level demands that would help them participate competently <strong>in</strong> their regularclassrooms. (p. 56)The research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest therefore that effective teach<strong>in</strong>g depends on such factorsas time-on-task, feedback, group<strong>in</strong>g and task decisions, classroom management, andstructur<strong>in</strong>g. Although the concept of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g evolved from studies of contentteach<strong>in</strong>g,Tikunoff’s (1 98 3) major study of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual education programshas exam<strong>in</strong>ed the extent to which it also applies to other contexts, such as bil<strong>in</strong>gual andESL classrooms.Ejective teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> hil<strong>in</strong>gual classroomsTikunoff (1983) suggests that three k<strong>in</strong>ds of competence are needed for the studentof limited <strong>English</strong> proficiency (LEP): participative competence, the ability “to respondappropriately to class demands and the procedural rules for accomplish<strong>in</strong>g them” (p. 4);<strong>in</strong>teractional competence, the ability “to respond both to classroom rules of discourse and socialrules of discourse, <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g appropriately with peers and adults while accomplish<strong>in</strong>g classtasks” (p. 4); and academic competence, the ability “to acquire new skills, assimilate new


172 JACK C. RICHARDS<strong>in</strong>formation, and construct new concepts” (p. 4). Furthermore, to be functionally proficient<strong>in</strong> the classroom, the student must be able to utili7e these compctences to perform threemajor functions: (a) to decode and understand both task expectations and new <strong>in</strong>formation;(b) to engage appropriately <strong>in</strong> complet<strong>in</strong>g tasks, with high accuracy; and (c) to obta<strong>in</strong>accurate feedback with relation to complet<strong>in</strong>g tasks accurately (p. 5).In his Significant Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Instructional Features (SBIF) descriptive study, Tikunoff(1 983) collected data to f<strong>in</strong>d out how effective teachers <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual education programsorganize <strong>in</strong>struction, structure teach<strong>in</strong>g activities, and enhance student performance ontasks. Teachers were <strong>in</strong>terviewed to determ<strong>in</strong>e their <strong>in</strong>structional philosophies, goals, andthe demands they would structure <strong>in</strong>to class tasks. Teachers were clearly able to specify classtask demands and <strong>in</strong>tended outcomes and to <strong>in</strong>dicate what LEP students had to do to befunctionally proficient. Case studies of teachers were undertaken <strong>in</strong> which teachers wereobserved dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, with three observers collect<strong>in</strong>g data for the teacher and forfour target LEP students. Teachers were <strong>in</strong>terviewed aga<strong>in</strong> after <strong>in</strong>struction.An analysis of data across the case studies revealed a clear l<strong>in</strong>kage between (1) teachers’ability to clearly specify the <strong>in</strong>tent of <strong>in</strong>struction, and a belief that students couldachieve accuracy <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>structional tasks, (2) the organization and delivery of <strong>in</strong>structionsuch that tasks and <strong>in</strong>stitutional demands reflected this <strong>in</strong>tent, requir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tendedstudent responses, and (3) the fidelity of student eonsequences with <strong>in</strong>tendedoutcomes. In other words, teachers were able to describe clearly what <strong>in</strong>structionwould entail, to operationalize these specifications, and to produce the desired results<strong>in</strong> terms of student performance. (p. 9)This approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g is one <strong>in</strong> which methodological pr<strong>in</strong>ciples are developed fromstudy<strong>in</strong>g the classroom practices and processes actually cmployed by effective teachers.Good teach<strong>in</strong>g is not viewed as someth<strong>in</strong>g that results from us<strong>in</strong>g Method X or MethodY,or someth<strong>in</strong>g that results from the teacher modify<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g behaviors to match someexternal sct of rules and pr<strong>in</strong>ciples. Rather, it results from the teacher’s active control andmanagement of the processes of teach<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g, and communication with<strong>in</strong> the classroomand from an understand<strong>in</strong>g of these processes.The classroom is seen as a place where thereis ongo<strong>in</strong>g and dynamic <strong>in</strong>teraction between the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>structional goals, learners’purposes, classroom tasks and activities, the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>structional activities and behaviors,student behaviors <strong>in</strong> complet<strong>in</strong>g assigned tasks, and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.In the bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms observed <strong>in</strong> Tikunoff’s study, effective teach<strong>in</strong>g was foundto reflect the degree to which the teacher is able to successfully communicate his or her<strong>in</strong>tentions, ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> students’ engagement <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>structional tasks, and monitor students’performance on tasks. In classrooms where different <strong>in</strong>structional goals are present anddifferent aspects of second language proficiency arc be<strong>in</strong>g addressed, the characteristics ofeffective teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> those sett<strong>in</strong>gs cannot be <strong>in</strong>ferred merely from read<strong>in</strong>g about thetheoretical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples underly<strong>in</strong>g the method or approach the teacher is supposed to befollow<strong>in</strong>g. Rather, classroom observation of teachers who arc achiev<strong>in</strong>g higher-thanpredictedlevels of achievement <strong>in</strong> their learners, or who are assessed as perform<strong>in</strong>g at highlevels of effectiveness accord<strong>in</strong>g to other criteria, provides the data from which profiles ofeffective teachers <strong>in</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g, read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, speak<strong>in</strong>g, and other k<strong>in</strong>ds of classes can bedeveloped.


BEYOND METHODS 173Learner strategiesThe approach to teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which methodology is developed from study of classroompractices attributes a primary role to the teacher <strong>in</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g/learn<strong>in</strong>g process. Successfullearn<strong>in</strong>g is viewed as dependent upon the teacher’s control and management of what takesplace <strong>in</strong> the classroom. However, what the teacher does is only half ofthe picturc.The otherhalf conccrns what learners do to achieve successful learn<strong>in</strong>g, or learner strategies. Promptedby the awareness that learners may succeed despite the teacher’s methods and techniquesrather than because of them, researchers as well as teachers have begun to look more closelyat learners themselves <strong>in</strong> an attempt to discover how successful learners acheve their results(O’Malley et al. 1985a, b;Will<strong>in</strong>g 1985).Studies of learner strategies attempt to identify the specific techniques and strategieslearners use to facilitate their own learn<strong>in</strong>g (Oxford 1985b).The focus is on the particularcognitive operations, processes, procedures, and heuristics that learners apply to the taskof learn<strong>in</strong>g a second language. Given any language learn<strong>in</strong>g task, such as understand<strong>in</strong>g alecture, read<strong>in</strong>g a text, writ<strong>in</strong>g a composition, understand<strong>in</strong>g the mean<strong>in</strong>g of a newgrammatical or lexical item, or prepar<strong>in</strong>g a written summary of a text, a number ofstrategies are available to a learner to help carry out the task. But what is the practical valueof know<strong>in</strong>g which particular strategies a learner employed?Just as research on effective teach<strong>in</strong>g has identified the k<strong>in</strong>ds of teach<strong>in</strong>g behaviors thatappear to account for superior teach<strong>in</strong>g, so research on effective learn<strong>in</strong>g seeks to identifythe k<strong>in</strong>ds of learn<strong>in</strong>g behaviors that can best facilitate learn<strong>in</strong>g. Good language learners seemto be successful because they have a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of and control over their ownlearn<strong>in</strong>g than less successful learners. Use of <strong>in</strong>appropriate learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies has beenfound to account for the poor performance of learners on many classroom learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks(Hosenfeld 1979). It should therefore be possible to improve student performance onlearn<strong>in</strong>g tasks by identify<strong>in</strong>g successful approaches to learn<strong>in</strong>g and by direct<strong>in</strong>g learncrstoward these k<strong>in</strong>ds of strategies. Research on learner strategies <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>ghence seeks to identify the strategies employed by successful learners and then to teachthose strategies to other learners <strong>in</strong> order to improve their language learn<strong>in</strong>g capacities(Hosenfeld 1977; Cohen and Aphek 1980; Chamot and O’Malley 1984). The premisesunderly<strong>in</strong>g Cohcn and Aphek’s work, for example, are:Some language learners are more successful than others.Some aspects of the learn<strong>in</strong>g process are conscious and others are not.Less successful learners can usc successful Strategies consciously to accelerate learn<strong>in</strong>g.Teachers can promote the use of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies.Learners can become the best judges of how they learn most effectively, both <strong>in</strong> and out ofclasses.The field of learner strategy research <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g is hence now animportant doma<strong>in</strong> of classroom research, and differs substantially from previous research<strong>in</strong> ths area. Earlier work on learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies lacked a sound theoretical basis and consistedlargely of lists of features that good language learners were assumed to possess. Theselists were developed from <strong>in</strong>terviews with successful language learners (e.g., Rub<strong>in</strong> 1975,1981 ; Stern 1975; Naiman et a/. 1978). Will<strong>in</strong>g (1 987: 275) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that “while suchgeneralizations have their usefulness as a help <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g the process of languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g from the po<strong>in</strong>t of view of the learner, they do not immediately yield prescriptionsfor teach<strong>in</strong>g.”


174 JACK C. RICHARDSMore recent work on learner strategies has attempted to yield more usable results bymak<strong>in</strong>g use of data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a broader range of sources, such as classroom observation,“th<strong>in</strong>k-aloud” procedures (<strong>in</strong> which learners record their thoughts and observations as theyperform different tasks), <strong>in</strong>terviews, self-reports employ<strong>in</strong>g note-tak<strong>in</strong>g and diaries,questionnaires, as well as controlled experimental studies designed to <strong>in</strong>vestigate specificcognitive processes (e.g., Heur<strong>in</strong>g 1984).These k<strong>in</strong>ds of approaches are yield<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formationof greater practical value. For example, Cohen (cited <strong>in</strong> Oxford 1985a) lists six strategiesused by successful language learners:Attention-enhanc<strong>in</strong>g strategies, such as respond<strong>in</strong>g silently to tasks asked of otherstudents <strong>in</strong> classUse of a variety of background sources, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g knowledge of the world, knowledgeof the given topic, awareness of stress and tone of voice of the speaker, perception ofthe speaker’s body language, and cues from earlier parts of the conversation <strong>in</strong> theeffort to decode communicative mean<strong>in</strong>gOral production tricks, such as avoid<strong>in</strong>g unfamiliar topics, paraphras<strong>in</strong>g, and ask<strong>in</strong>gfor helpVocabulary learn<strong>in</strong>g techniques, such as mak<strong>in</strong>g associations, attend<strong>in</strong>g to the mean<strong>in</strong>gof parts of the word, not<strong>in</strong>g the structure of the word, plac<strong>in</strong>g the word <strong>in</strong> a topicalgroup with similar words, visualiz<strong>in</strong>g or contextualiz<strong>in</strong>g it, l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g it to the situation<strong>in</strong> which it appears, creat<strong>in</strong>g a mental image of it, and associat<strong>in</strong>g some physicalsensation to itRead<strong>in</strong>g or text-process<strong>in</strong>g strategies, such as clarify<strong>in</strong>g the communicative purposeof the text, dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g important po<strong>in</strong>ts from trivia, skipp<strong>in</strong>g around to get anoverall conceptual picture, us<strong>in</strong>g substantive and l<strong>in</strong>guistic background knowledge,read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> broad phrases rather than word for word, rely<strong>in</strong>g on contextual clues,mak<strong>in</strong>g ongo<strong>in</strong>g summaries, and look<strong>in</strong>g for emphasis and cohesion markers <strong>in</strong> thctextWrit<strong>in</strong>g tcchniques such as focus<strong>in</strong>g on simply gett<strong>in</strong>g ideas down on paper <strong>in</strong>steadof try<strong>in</strong>g for perfection right away; purposefully us<strong>in</strong>g parallel structures and othermeans of enhanc<strong>in</strong>g cohesion; and writ<strong>in</strong>g multiple drafts.Will<strong>in</strong>g (1 987: 278-9) notes that strategies are csscntially “methods employcd by the pcrsonfor process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>put language <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> such a way as to ga<strong>in</strong> control of it, thus enabl<strong>in</strong>gthe assimilation of that <strong>in</strong>formation by the self.” Strategies are hence viewed as ways ofmanag<strong>in</strong>g the complex <strong>in</strong>formation that the learner is receiv<strong>in</strong>g about the target language.Wenden (1 983) <strong>in</strong>terviewed adult language learners about how they organized theirlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences and found that they asked themselves eight k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions.QuestionDecision1 How does this language work? Learners make judgmcnts about thel<strong>in</strong>guistic and sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic codes.2 What’s it like to learn a language?3 What should I learn and how?Learners make judgments about how tolearn a language and about what languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g is like.Learners decide upon l<strong>in</strong>guistic objectives,resources, and use of resources.


BEYOND METHODS 1754 What should I emphasize?5 How should I change?6 How am I do<strong>in</strong>g?Learners decide to give priority to speciall<strong>in</strong>guistic items.Learners decide to change their approach tolanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g.Learners determ<strong>in</strong>e how well they use thelanguage and diagnose their needs.7 What am I gett<strong>in</strong>g out of this? Learners determ<strong>in</strong>e if an activity or strategyis useful.8 How am I responsible for learn<strong>in</strong>g?How is language learn<strong>in</strong>g affect<strong>in</strong>gme?Learners make judgments about how tolearn a language and about what languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g is like.O’Malley et al. have <strong>in</strong>vestigated the use of strategies by ESL learners both <strong>in</strong> and out ofclassrooms (O’Malley et al. 1985a, b; O’Malley and Chamot 1989). ESL students and theirteachers were <strong>in</strong>terviewed about the strategies learners used on specific language learn<strong>in</strong>gtasks, and the learners were observed <strong>in</strong> ESL classrooms.They were also asked about theiruse of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> communicative situations outside the classroom. A total of twenty-sixdifferent k<strong>in</strong>ds of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies were identified.In a follow-up study, high school ESL students were given tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the use ofparticular strategies <strong>in</strong> order to determ<strong>in</strong>e if it would improve their effectiveness as languagelearners and their performance on vocabulary, listen<strong>in</strong>g, and speak<strong>in</strong>g tasks. Strategies werecompared across proficiency levels and with learners of different language backgrounds.Students were given tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the use of specific strategies for particular language learn<strong>in</strong>gtasks. Results supported the notion that learners can be taught to use more effective learn<strong>in</strong>gstrategies (O’Malley et al. 1985a, b):Strategies tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g was successfully demonstrated <strong>in</strong> a natural teach<strong>in</strong>g environmentwith second language listen<strong>in</strong>g and speak<strong>in</strong>g tasks. This <strong>in</strong>dicates that classrooms<strong>in</strong>struction on learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies with <strong>in</strong>tegrative language skills can facilitate learn<strong>in</strong>g.(O’Malley et a]. 1985a: 577)Phillips ( 1975) <strong>in</strong>vestigated how learners approach read<strong>in</strong>g tasks and identified strategiesemployed by good and poor readers. She employed a “th<strong>in</strong>k-aloud” procedure to <strong>in</strong>vestigatereaders’ strategies <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>g with unknown vocabulary. From her students’ descriptionsPhillips found that strategies used by efficient readers <strong>in</strong>cluded categoriz<strong>in</strong>g wordsgrammatically, <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g grammatical operations, and recogniz<strong>in</strong>g cognates and rootwords. Hosenfeld (1 977, 1984) used similar techniques <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g processes employed byforeign language readers when encounter<strong>in</strong>g unfamiliar words. In one study (Hosenfeld1977), some of the differences between those with high and low scores on a read<strong>in</strong>gproficiency test were these: High scorers tended to keep the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the passage <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d,read <strong>in</strong> broad phrases, skip unessential words, and guess mean<strong>in</strong>gs of unknown wordsfrom context; low scorers tended to lose the mean<strong>in</strong>g of sentences as soon as they decodedthem, read word by word or <strong>in</strong> short phrases, rarely skip words, and turn to the glossarywhen they encountered new words. In addition successful readers tended to identify thcgrammatical categories of words, could detect word-order differences <strong>in</strong> the foreignlanguage, recognized cognates, and used the glossary only as a last resort (Hosenfcld 1984:233). Hosenfeld found that unsuccessful readers could be taught the lexical strategies of


176 JACI< C. RICHARDSsuccessful readers, confirm<strong>in</strong>g Wenden’s observation that “<strong>in</strong>effcctive learners are <strong>in</strong>activelearners. Their apparent <strong>in</strong>ability to learn is, <strong>in</strong> fact, due to their not hav<strong>in</strong>g an appropriaterepertoire of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies’’ (1 985: 7).Studies of how learners approach writ<strong>in</strong>g tasks have also focused on the effectivencssof the processes learners employ (Raimes 1985). Lapp (1 984) summarizes some of theresearch f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on differcnces between skilled and unskilled writers with respect torehears<strong>in</strong>g and prewrit<strong>in</strong>g behaviors (what a writer does before beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g writ<strong>in</strong>g), draft<strong>in</strong>gand writ<strong>in</strong>g processes (how the writer actually composes a piece of writ<strong>in</strong>g), and revis<strong>in</strong>gbchaviors (revisions and corrections the writer makcs).Rescarch f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on learner strategies <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g classes (e.g., Heur<strong>in</strong>g1984) suggest that teachers need to evaluate their teach<strong>in</strong>g strategies on an ongo<strong>in</strong>g basis,to determ<strong>in</strong>e if they are promot<strong>in</strong>g effective or <strong>in</strong>cffectivc learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong> learners.Many commonly employed tcchniqucs <strong>in</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g of writ<strong>in</strong>g, such as outl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g orwrit<strong>in</strong>g from a rhetorical model, might well <strong>in</strong>hibit rather than encourage the developmentof effective writ<strong>in</strong>g skills, because they direct the learner’s attention to the form andmechanics of writ<strong>in</strong>g too early <strong>in</strong> the writ<strong>in</strong>g process.In order to present <strong>in</strong>formation about learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies to students, strategies ncedto bc operationalized <strong>in</strong> the form of specific techniques (see Fraser and Skibicki 1987);however, there is no consensus yet concern<strong>in</strong>g how to approach the teach<strong>in</strong>g of learn<strong>in</strong>gstrategies. As with othcr aspects of language teach<strong>in</strong>g, the issue of whcther strategies arebest “learned” or “acquired” is a central one. Some researchers advocate a direct approach.This <strong>in</strong>volves explicit tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the use of specific strategies and teach<strong>in</strong>g students toconsciously monitor their own strategies (e.g., O’Malley et al. 1985a, b; Russo and Stewner-Manzanares 1985). Others favor a more <strong>in</strong>direct approach <strong>in</strong> which strategies are<strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to other k<strong>in</strong>ds of learn<strong>in</strong>g content. Fraser and Skibicki (1 987) describe thedevelopment of self-directed learn<strong>in</strong>g materials for adult migrant learners <strong>in</strong> Australia,which focus on specific strategies <strong>in</strong> cliffcrent skill areas. A related issue concerns whetherthe focus of teacher <strong>in</strong>tervention should be to provide additional strategies to learners ormerely to help the learner develop a bettcr awarencss of and control over exist<strong>in</strong>g strategies.Will<strong>in</strong>g (1987: 277) observes that despite thc recent amount of attention to learn<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, some serious issucs still await resolution:1234Current notions of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies lack conceptual coherence . . .Learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies as currently described have bccn identified more or less <strong>in</strong> isolationand on a purely empirical and arbitrary basis and have not been rclated to an overallview of learn<strong>in</strong>g . . .Thcre has been little systematic work on plac<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies with<strong>in</strong> a broaderdescription of the nature and mean<strong>in</strong>g of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong>elf. . .Thcre has been little effort to relate the notion of lcarn<strong>in</strong>g strategies (with<strong>in</strong> a generallearn<strong>in</strong>g theory) to current ideas about second language acquisition.In addition, there has been little attempt to relate theorics of learn<strong>in</strong>g stratcgies to moregeneral theories of teach<strong>in</strong>g, such as the one discussed previously.SummaryTwo approaches to language teach<strong>in</strong>g havc been discusscd and contrasted. One conceptualizesteach<strong>in</strong>g as application of a teach<strong>in</strong>g method, <strong>in</strong> which both thc teacher and thc


BEYOND METHODS 177learner are approached on the terms of the method promoter, educational theorist, orapplied l<strong>in</strong>guist. The assumptions or theory underly<strong>in</strong>g the method provide the start<strong>in</strong>gpo<strong>in</strong>t for an <strong>in</strong>structional design that is subsequently imposed on teachers and learners. Anattempt is then made to make the teacher’s and learner’s classroom behaviors match thespecifications of the method. This can be contrasted with an approach that starts with theobservable processes of classroom teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g, from which methodologicalpr<strong>in</strong>ciples and practices <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g are derived. Observation can yield twocategories of <strong>in</strong>formation:1 The study of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g provides <strong>in</strong>formation about how effective teachersorganize and deliver <strong>in</strong>struction. This relates to classroom management skills, and tothe strategies teachers use to present <strong>in</strong>structional goals, structure learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks andactivities, monitor learn<strong>in</strong>g, and provide feedback on it.2 The study of effective learn<strong>in</strong>g provides <strong>in</strong>formation about the learn<strong>in</strong>g strategieseffective learners apply to the process of us<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g a second and foreignlanguage.However, a word of caution is <strong>in</strong> order, s<strong>in</strong>ce the goal of ths approach is not simply to arriveat a set of general pr<strong>in</strong>ciples that can be taught to teachers and 1earners.This of course wouldbe to come full circle, and would simply replace one “method” with another. The approachadvocated here starts with the assumption that the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g andlearn<strong>in</strong>g strategies is a central and ongo<strong>in</strong>g component of the process of teach<strong>in</strong>g.This is thecore of a process-oriented methodology of teach<strong>in</strong>g.This approach implies a redef<strong>in</strong>ition of the role of the teacher. Teachers are not viewedmerely as “performers,” who carry out the role prescribed by the method or apply anexternally derived set of pr<strong>in</strong>ciples to their teach<strong>in</strong>g.Teachers are seen rather as <strong>in</strong>vestigatorsof both their own classroom practices and those of the learners. Much of the effort todeterm<strong>in</strong>e what constitutes effcctive teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>itiated by the teacher.Through regular observation of their own classes and through analysis and reflection,teachers can obta<strong>in</strong> valuable feedback about the effectiveness of their own teach<strong>in</strong>g. At thesame time they can develop a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of the pr<strong>in</strong>ciplcs that account for effectiveteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their own classrooms. In the doma<strong>in</strong> of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies, thcteacher also has an important role to play.The teacher is <strong>in</strong>itially an observer and <strong>in</strong>vestigatorof the learners’ learn<strong>in</strong>g behaviors and subsequently provides feedback on the k<strong>in</strong>d ofstrategies that are most successful for carry<strong>in</strong>g out specific learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks. Relevant concernsfor the teacher thus focus not on the search for the best method, but rather on thecircumstances and conditions under which more effective teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g areaccomplished.ReferencesBerl<strong>in</strong>er, D.C. (1984) ‘The half-full glass: a review of research on teach<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> P.L. Hosford(ed.) Us<strong>in</strong>g What We Know about <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>, pp. 51-77, Alexandria, Va.: Association forSupervision and Curriculum Development.Blum, R.E. (1 984) Efcctive School<strong>in</strong>g Practices: A Research Synthesis. Portland, Ore. : NorthwestRegional Educational LaboratoryChall, J. (1967) Learn<strong>in</strong>g to Read:The Great Debate. NewYork: McGraw-Hill.Chamot, A.U., and O’Mallcy, J.M. (1986) A Cognitive Academic <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Approach:AnESL Content-Rased Curriculum. Rosslyn,Va. : National Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse for Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Education.


~ (1985b)178 JACI< C. RICHARDSCohen, A.D., and Aphek, E. (1980) ‘Retention of second-language vocabulary ovcr time:<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the role of mnemonic associations’. System 8: 22 1-35.Doyle, W. (1977) ‘Paradigms for research on teacher effectiveness’, <strong>in</strong> L.S. Shulman (ed.)Review $Research <strong>in</strong> Teacher Education, Vol. 5, pp. 163-98. Itasca, Ill.: Peacock.Dunk<strong>in</strong>, M., and Biddle, B. (1974) The Study of <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. New York: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart andW<strong>in</strong>ston.Elliot, J. (1 980) ‘Implications of classroom research for professional development’, <strong>in</strong> E. Hoyleand J, Megarry (eds) World Yearbook ofEducation, 1980, pp. 308-24. London: Kogan Page.Evertson, C.M., Anderson, L.M. and Brophy, J.E. (1978) TheTexasjunior high school ,tu+: reportofprocess-product relationships. University ofTexas, Research and Development Center forTeacher Education, Aust<strong>in</strong>.Fisher, C.W., Berl<strong>in</strong>er, D.C., Filhy, N.N., Marliave, R.S., Cahen, L.S. and Dishaw, M.M.(1980) ‘<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> behaviors, academic learn<strong>in</strong>g time and academic achievement: anoverview’, <strong>in</strong> C. Denham and A. Lieherman (cds) Time to Learn, pp. 7-32. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton,D.C. : U.S. Department of Education, National Institute of Education.Fraser, H., and Skihicki, A. (1987) ‘Self-directed learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies for adult Vietnameselearners of ESL’. Prospect 3, 1: 3344.Good,T.L. (1 979) ‘Teacher effectiveness <strong>in</strong> the elementary school’. Journal ofTeacher Education30, 2: 52-64.Good, T.L., and Beckerman, T.M. (1978) ‘Time on task: a naturalistic study <strong>in</strong> sixth gradeclassrooms’. Elementary Schooljournal 78: 193-201 .Good, T.L., and Marshall, S. (1984) ‘Do studcnts lcarn more <strong>in</strong> heterogeneous orhomogeneous groups?’, <strong>in</strong> P. Peterson, L.C. Wilk<strong>in</strong>son and M. Hall<strong>in</strong>an (eds) The<strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong> $ Instruction: Group Organization and Group Processes. New York: AcademicPress.Heur<strong>in</strong>g, D.L. (1984) ‘The revision strategies of skilled and unskilled ESL writers: five casestudies. Master’s thesis. University of Hawaii at Manoa.Hosenfeld, C. (1977) ‘A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the read<strong>in</strong>g strategies of successful andnon-successful second language learners’. System 5: 1 10-23.~- (1979) ‘A learn<strong>in</strong>g-teach<strong>in</strong>g view of second language <strong>in</strong>struction’. Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Annals12, 1: 514.~- (1984) ‘Case studies of n<strong>in</strong>th grade readers’, <strong>in</strong> J.C. Alderson and A.H. Urquhart (eds),Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a Foreign <strong>Language</strong>, pp. 23149. London: Longman.Kelly, L. (1969) Twenty-f;ve Centuries of <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Lapp, R. (1 984) ‘The process approach to writ<strong>in</strong>g: towards a curriculum for <strong>in</strong>ternationalstudents’. Master’s thesis. Work<strong>in</strong>g Paper available from Department of <strong>English</strong> as aSecond <strong>Language</strong>, University of Hawaii.Naiman, N., Frohlich, M., Stern, H.H. and Todesco, A. (1978) The Good <strong>Language</strong> Learner.Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies <strong>in</strong> Education.O’Malley, J., and Chamot, A.U. (1989) Learner Strategies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. NewYork: Cambridge University Press.O’Malley, J., Chamot, A.U., Stcwner-Manzanares, G., Russo, R.P. and Kupper, L. (1985a)‘Learn<strong>in</strong>g strategy applications with students of <strong>English</strong> as a second language’. TESOLQuarterly 19, 3: 557-84.O’Malley, J., Chamot, A.U., Stewner-Manzanarcs, G., Kupper, L. and Russo, R.P. (198511)‘Learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies used by beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>termediate ESL learners’. <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g 35, 1.Oxford, R. (1985a) A New Taxonomy $Second <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Strategies. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.: ICenter for Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics.‘Second language learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies: what the research has to say’. ERIC/CLLNews Bullet<strong>in</strong> 9, 1. 1


~ (1BEYOND METHODS 179Phillips, J. (1975) ‘Second language read<strong>in</strong>g: teach<strong>in</strong>g decod<strong>in</strong>g skills’. Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Annals8: 227-30.Raimes, A. (1985) ‘What unskilled ESL students do as they write: a classroom study ofcompos<strong>in</strong>g’. TESOL Quarterbl9, 2: 229-59.Richards, J. C., and Rodgers, T. (1986) Approaches and Methods <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. NewYork:Cambridge University Press.Rub<strong>in</strong>, J. (1975) ‘What the good language learner can teach us’. TESOL Quarterly 9, 1: 41-5 1.981 ) ‘Study of cognitive processes <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 11,2: 117-31.Russo, R. P., and Stewner-Manzanares, G. (1985) ‘The tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and use of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategiesfor <strong>English</strong> as a second language <strong>in</strong> a military context’. Paper presented at the annualmeet<strong>in</strong>g of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.Stall<strong>in</strong>gs, J. A., and Kaskowitz, D.H. (1974) Follow through Classroom Observation Evaluation,1972-1973. Menlo Park, Cal.: Stanford Research Institute.Stern, H. H. (1975) ‘What can we learn from the good language learner?’ Canadian Modern<strong>Language</strong> Review 31: 30618.Swaffar, J.K., Arens, K. and Morgan, M. (1982) ‘Teacher classroom practices: redef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gmethod as task hierarchy’. Modern <strong>Language</strong>journal 66, 1: 24-33.Tikunoff, W.J. (1983) ‘Utility of the SBIF features for the <strong>in</strong>struction of limitcd <strong>English</strong>proficient students’. Report No. SBIF-83-R. 15/ 16 for NE Contract No. 400-80-0026.San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development.______ (1985) Apply<strong>in</strong>g Sign$cant Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Instructional Features <strong>in</strong> the Classroom. Rosslyn, Va.:National Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse for Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Education.Tikunoff, W.J., Ward, B.A., Fisher, C.A., Armendariz, J.C., Parker, L. Dom<strong>in</strong>guez, V.J.A.,Mercado, C., Romero, M. and Good, R.A. (1980) ‘Rcview of the literature for adescriptive study of significant bil<strong>in</strong>gual <strong>in</strong>structional features’. Report No. SRIF-8 1 -D. 1.1. San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development.Webb, N.M. (1980) ‘A process-outcome of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> group and <strong>in</strong>dividual sett<strong>in</strong>gs’.Educational Psychologist 15: 69-83.Wenden, A. (1983) ‘A literature review: the process of <strong>in</strong>tervention’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 33, 1 :103-21.______ (1985) ‘Learner strategies’. TESOL Newsletter (October).Will<strong>in</strong>g, K. (1985) Help<strong>in</strong>g Adults Develop Their Learn<strong>in</strong>g Strategies. Sydney: Adult MigrantEducation Service.(1987) ‘Learner stratcgies as <strong>in</strong>formation managcment’. Prospect 2, 3: 273-92.


Chapter 10Michael H. LongFOCUS ON FORM: A DESIGN FEATURE INLANGUAGE TEACHING METHODOLOGYAga<strong>in</strong>st methodsANGUAGE TEACHER EDUCATION PROGRAMS PERSIST <strong>in</strong>L present<strong>in</strong>g classroom options to tra<strong>in</strong>ees <strong>in</strong> terms of methods. While many have stoppedpretend<strong>in</strong>g that any one method is a panacea or at least that they know which one is, mostnevertheless cont<strong>in</strong>ue to use method as a unit of analysis <strong>in</strong> their professionally orientedcourses, and some even give college credit for tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular methods taught by theirdevelopers or licensed acolytes. Books on methods sell very well, books survey<strong>in</strong>g methodsdo even better, and expensive one-day “sem<strong>in</strong>ars” offer<strong>in</strong>g tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular methodsarc rarely short of customers.Yet it is no cxaggcration to say that language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodsdo not exist ~ at least, not where they would matter, if they did, <strong>in</strong> the classroom.There arc at least four reasons for avoid<strong>in</strong>g the methods trap. First, even as idealizedby their developers, groups of methods overlap considerably, prescrib<strong>in</strong>g and proscrib<strong>in</strong>gmany of the same classroom practices. For example, while one method may have teachersprovide feedback on error us<strong>in</strong>g hand-signals, and one verbally, both prescribe “errorcorrection”. Almost all methods <strong>in</strong> fact advocate error correction (Krashen and Scliger1975).Second, when third parties analyze lesson transcripts ~ records of what teachers andlearners actually do, as opposed to what methodologists tell them to do ~ brief excerptscan occasionally be identified as the product of this or that method, but the classificationsusually have to be made on the basis of one or two salient but (as far as we know) trivialfeatures, e.g. whether students arc <strong>in</strong>formed of the commission of error verbally or nonverbally.Quite lengthy excerpts arc often impossible to dist<strong>in</strong>guish, especially if taken fromreal classes, as opposed to staged demonstration lessons (D<strong>in</strong>smorc 1985; Nunan 1987).Third, studies that have set out to compare the effectiveness of supposedly quitedifferent methods (e.g. Scherer and Wcrtheimcr 1964; Smith 1970;Von Elck and Oskarsson1975) have typically found little or no advantage for one over another, or only local andusually short-lived advantages. One <strong>in</strong>terpretation of such results is that methods do notmatter. Another is that methods do not exist, among other reasons, because most teacherstend to do much the same th<strong>in</strong>gs (many methods require this, after all), whatever they aresupposed to be do<strong>in</strong>g, especially over time. The abscncc of a systematic observationalcomponent <strong>in</strong> most of the comparative methods studies makes either <strong>in</strong>terpretation


FOCUS ON FORM 181problematic. However, the second view is supported retrospectively by descriptive studieswhich have found the same classroom practices surviv<strong>in</strong>g differences not only <strong>in</strong> “methods”(Nunan 1987), but also <strong>in</strong> professional tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (Long and Sat0 1983), materials (Phillipsand Shettlesworth 1975; Long, Adams McLean and Castanos 1976; Ross, to appear),teach<strong>in</strong>g generations (Hoetker and Ahlbrand 1969) and tcach<strong>in</strong>g experience (Pica and Long1986).Fourth, method may or may not be a useful analytic construct for teachers <strong>in</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g,but it is not a conceptual basis for how they operate <strong>in</strong> practice. Numerous studies of theways content teachers plan lessons and recall them afterwards show that they th<strong>in</strong>k of whattranspires <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>structional activities, or tasks (for review, seeShavelson and Stern 198 1 ; Crookes 1986). The same appears to be true of FL tcachers.Swaffer, Arens and Morgan (1 982) conducted a six-month comparative methods study(“comprehension” and “four skills” approaches) of Gcrman teach<strong>in</strong>g at the UniversityofTexas. Classroom observations and debrief<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews with teachers at the end of thestudy showed that, despite the teachers hav<strong>in</strong>g received explicit tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the methods and(supposedly) hav<strong>in</strong>g each used one or the other for a semester, there was no clear dist<strong>in</strong>ctionbetween them <strong>in</strong> their m<strong>in</strong>ds or <strong>in</strong> the classroom practices used across groups.For these and other reasons, it is clear that “method” is an unverifiable and irrelevantconstruct when attempt<strong>in</strong>g to improve classroom FL <strong>in</strong>struction. Worse, it may actuallydo harm by distract<strong>in</strong>g teachers from genu<strong>in</strong>ely important issues. Say<strong>in</strong>g that methods donot exist and so do not matter at the classroom level does not mean, after all, that what goeson <strong>in</strong> classrooms does not matter. On the contrary, there is grow<strong>in</strong>g evidence of theimportance of classroom processes, of pedagogic tasks, and of qualitativc differences <strong>in</strong>classroom language use for success and failure <strong>in</strong> FLs (for review, see Chaudron 1988).Rather than focus on method as the key, however, we would do better to th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>in</strong> terms ofpsychol<strong>in</strong>guistically relevant design features of learn<strong>in</strong>g environments, preferably featureswhich capture important characteristics of a wide range of syllabus types, methods,materials, tasks, and tests. It is to one of these,focus onform, that we now turn.Focus on form <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>gMany developments <strong>in</strong> foreign language syllabus design, materials writ<strong>in</strong>g, methodologyand test<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g the past 30 years reflect the tension between the desirability ofcommunicative use of the FL <strong>in</strong> the classroom, on the one hand, and the felt necd for al<strong>in</strong>guistic focus <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g, on the other. However, while discussion has occurred<strong>in</strong> staff-rooms and journals alike, it has generally concerned how best to achieve such afocus, not whether or not to have one. Most applied l<strong>in</strong>guists and pedagogues cont<strong>in</strong>ue toadvocate teach<strong>in</strong>g and test<strong>in</strong>g isolated l<strong>in</strong>guistic un<strong>its</strong> of one k<strong>in</strong>d or another <strong>in</strong> one way oranother.Thus, whle procedural, process and task-based alternatives are available (see Prabhu1987; Breen 1987; Long and Crookes 1989), the overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g majority of syllabi are stillstructural, notional-functional or a hybrid, and superficially different “mcthods”, like ALM,TPR and the Silent Way, all teach one l<strong>in</strong>guistic item at a time (or assume they do), <strong>in</strong>build<strong>in</strong>g-block fashion. Pervasive classroom practices, such as grammar and vocabularyexplanations, display questions, fill-<strong>in</strong>-the-blanks exercises, dialog memorization, drills anderror correction, all entail treatment of the language as object, and so do discrete-po<strong>in</strong>tlanguage tests.There have always been a few dissent<strong>in</strong>g voices. Newmark (1 966), Ncwmark and Reibel(1 968), Corder (1 967) and Allwright (1 976), among others, have argued strongly aga<strong>in</strong>st


182 MICHAEL H. LONG“<strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>g” with language learn<strong>in</strong>g. While differ<strong>in</strong>g considerably both <strong>in</strong> the detail of theirown proposals and <strong>in</strong> the rationales offered for them, each has claimed that the best way tolearn a language, <strong>in</strong>side or outside a classroom, is not by treat<strong>in</strong>g it as an object of study,but by experienc<strong>in</strong>g it as a medium of communication.More recently, some non-<strong>in</strong>terventionist positions have been espoused on the basis ofsecond language acquisition (SLA) theory and research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (see e.g. Dulay and Burt1973; Ellis 1984; Felix 1981; Krashcn andTerrel1 1983; Prabhu 1987;Wode 1981). Mostoften cited <strong>in</strong> this context are the well attested developmental sequences <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage (IL),such as those for Swedish negation, <strong>English</strong> relative clauses and German word order.Thesesequences are fixed series of overlapp<strong>in</strong>g stages, each characterizable by the relativefrequency of IL structures, which learners apparently have to traverse on the way to masteryof the target language system. (For the most comprehensive study of this phenomenon, seeJohnston 1985 .)Numerous studies show, for <strong>in</strong>stance, that ESL negation has a four-stage sequence (forreview, see Schumann 1979):Stage1 NofX2 no/not/don’t V3 aux. -ncg.4 analyzed don’tSample utterancesNo is happy/No you pay itThey not work<strong>in</strong>g/He don’t have jobI can’t play/You mustn’t do that1 didn’t see her/She doesn’t live thereAt stages 1 and 2, not just Spanish speakers, whose L1 has pre-verbal negation, but alsoJapanese learners, whose native system is post-verbal, <strong>in</strong>itially produce pre-verbally negatedutterances <strong>in</strong> ESL (Gillis and Weber 1976; Stauble 198 l), although the Japanese abandonthe 3tratcgy sooner (Zobl 1982). Pre-verbal negator placement appears to reflect strong<strong>in</strong>ternal pressures, for it is widely observed <strong>in</strong> studies of both naturalistic and <strong>in</strong>structedSLA. Turkish speakers receiv<strong>in</strong>g formal <strong>in</strong>struction, for example, start with pre-verbalnegation <strong>in</strong> Swcdish, even though both L1 and L2 have post-verbal systems (Hyltcnstam1977).With m<strong>in</strong>or variations, the evidence to date suggests that the same developmentalsequences are observed <strong>in</strong> the ILs of children and adults, of naturalistic, <strong>in</strong>structed andmixed lcarncrs, of learners from different L1 backgrounds, and of lcarncrs perform<strong>in</strong>g ondifferent tasks. L1 differences occasionally result <strong>in</strong> additional sub-stages and swifter orslower passage through stages, but not <strong>in</strong> disruption of the basic sequence by skipp<strong>in</strong>g stages(for review, see Ellis 1985; Larsen-Freeman and Long, <strong>in</strong> press; Zobl 1982).Passage through each stage, <strong>in</strong> order, appears to be unavoidable, and obligator<strong>in</strong>ess hasbeen <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to the def<strong>in</strong>ition of “stage” <strong>in</strong> SLA (Meisel, Clahsen and Pienemann1981 ;Johnston 1985).As would be predicted if this def<strong>in</strong>ition is accurate, it also seems thatdevelopmental sequences are impervious to <strong>in</strong>struction. It has repeatedly been demonstratedthat morpheme accuracy orders and developmental sequences do not reflect <strong>in</strong>structionalsequences (Lightbown 1983; Ellis 1989), and tuition <strong>in</strong> a German SL word order structurebeyond students’ current process<strong>in</strong>g abilities has been shown not to result <strong>in</strong> lcarn<strong>in</strong>g(Pienemann 1984).The results for developmental sequences, together with related f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of common(although not <strong>in</strong>variant) naturalistic and <strong>in</strong>structed morpheme accuracy orders, show thatlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g is obviously at least partly governed by forces beyond a teacher’s ortextbook writer’s control. This realization has <strong>in</strong> turn led some theorists to conclude that


FOCUS ON FORM 183classrooms are useful to the extent that they provide sheltered l<strong>in</strong>guistic environmcnts forbeg<strong>in</strong>ners, but that it does not help for teachers to focus on l<strong>in</strong>guistic form. An <strong>in</strong>ferencethat could easily be drawn from such <strong>in</strong>terpretations is that there are only two options <strong>in</strong>this area of coursc design: either (1 ) a l<strong>in</strong>ear, additive syllabus and methodology whosecontent and focus is a series of isolated l<strong>in</strong>guistic forms (sound contrasts, lexical items,structures, speech acts, notions, etc.), or (2) a program with no overt focus on l<strong>in</strong>guisticforms at all. While this turns out to be a false dichotomy,fcus onform is a potentiallyimportant design feature for dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional methodologies and sett<strong>in</strong>gs.Focus onform is a feature which reveals an underly<strong>in</strong>g similarity among a variety of(a) teach<strong>in</strong>g “methods”, e.g. ALM, TPR, Grammar Translation and Silent Way, (b) syllabustypes, e.g. structural, notional-functional, lexical, and (c) program types, e.g. submersion,imrncrsion, sheltered subject-matter, which on the surface appear to differ greatly. Groups(a) and (b) all utilize an overt focus on form; Group (c) does not. It also allowsgeneralizations across traditional boundaries, identify<strong>in</strong>g a l<strong>in</strong>k between the program types<strong>in</strong> group (c) and <strong>in</strong> theory, at least, a l<strong>in</strong>guistically non-isolat<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g “method”, suchas the Natural Approach (Krashen and Terrell 1983). At the classroom process level,techniques, procedures, exercises and pedagogic tasks can also be categorized as to whetheror not they either permit or require a focus on form. Display questions, repetition drillsand error correction, for example, all overtly focus students on form; referential questions,true/false exercises and two-way tasks do not. F<strong>in</strong>ally, while many potentially relevantdesign features will dist<strong>in</strong>guish some methods, syllabi, tasks and tests from others, few havethe valency of focus on form. It appears to be a parameter one value or another of whichcharacterizes almost all language teach<strong>in</strong>g options.Five caveats are <strong>in</strong> order. First, it is not be<strong>in</strong>g suggested that whether or not a programtype, syllabus, method, task or test focuses on form is the only relevant design characteristicor that important differences will not exist among members of groups which share the feature,and vice versa. Second, while most programs, syllabi, methods, tasks and tests either do or donot overtly focus on form, some with<strong>in</strong> the former group differ <strong>in</strong> the degree to which theyisolate l<strong>in</strong>guistic structures, not to mention as to how they do so; there are, <strong>in</strong> other words,relative as well as absolute, with<strong>in</strong>-group as well as <strong>in</strong>ter-group, differences.Third, it is likelythat students will often focus on form when teachers or materials designers <strong>in</strong>tcnd them notto, and ignore form whcn they are supposed to concentrate on it. Fourth, some degree ofawarencss of form and a focus on mean<strong>in</strong>g may not be mutually exclusive on some tasks (forreview, see Schmidt 1990). Fifth, the fact that the dist<strong>in</strong>ction can be made does not mean thatit should; whether it is important is a theoretical and/or an empirical matter.Focus on form: a psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic rationaleThc practice of isolat<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic items, teach<strong>in</strong>g and test<strong>in</strong>g them one at a time, wasorig<strong>in</strong>ally motivated by advances <strong>in</strong> behaviorist psychology and structuralist l<strong>in</strong>guistics.Comb<strong>in</strong>ed with the advcnt of a world war and a sudden need for fluent foreign languagespeakers, these events led to the growth of ALM and <strong>its</strong> many progeny. As dist<strong>in</strong>ct from afocus onform, to which we return below, structural syllabi, ALM, and variants thereof<strong>in</strong>volve a focus onjrrns. That is to say, the content of the syllabus and of lessons based on itis the l<strong>in</strong>guistic items themselves (structures, notions, lexical items, etc.); a lesson isdesigned to teach “the past cont<strong>in</strong>uous”, “request<strong>in</strong>g” and so on, noth<strong>in</strong>g else.Arguments abound aga<strong>in</strong>st mak<strong>in</strong>g isolated l<strong>in</strong>guistic structures the content of a FLcourse, that is, aga<strong>in</strong>st a focus onjrms. Of the hundreds of studies of <strong>in</strong>terlanguage (IL)


184 MICHAEL H. LONGdcvelopmcnt now completed, not one shows either tutored or naturalistic learnersdevelop<strong>in</strong>g proficiency one l<strong>in</strong>guistic item at a time. On the contrary, all reveal complex,gradual and <strong>in</strong>ter-related developmental paths for grammatical subsystems, such as auxiliaryand negation <strong>in</strong> ESL (Stauble 1981 ; Kelley 1983), and copula and word ordcr <strong>in</strong> GSL(Meisel, Clahsen and Pienemann 1981). Moreover, development is not unidirectional;omission/suppliance of forms fluctuates, as does accuracy of suppliance.Although most syllabi and methods assume the opposite, learncrs do not move fromignorance of a form to mastery of it <strong>in</strong> one step, as is attested by the very existence ofdevelopmental scqucnces like that for ESL negation. Typically, when a form first appears <strong>in</strong>a learner’s IL, it is used <strong>in</strong> a non-target-like manner, and only gradually improves <strong>in</strong> accuracyof use. It sometimes shifts <strong>in</strong> function over time as other new (target-like and non-targetlike)forms enter (Huebner 1983). It quite often decl<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> accuracy or even temporarilydisappears altogether due to a change elsewhere <strong>in</strong> the IL (see, e.g. Meisel, Clahsen andPienemann 1981 ; Huebner 1983; Lightbown 1983; Neumann 1977), a phenomenonsometimes describable as U-shaped behavior (Kellcrman 1985). Further, attempts to teachisolated items one at a timc fail unless the structure happens to be one the learner can processand so is psychol<strong>in</strong>guistically ready to acquire. In Pienemann’s (1984) term<strong>in</strong>ology,learnability determ<strong>in</strong>es teachability. F<strong>in</strong>ally, as language teachers, employers and learnersalike will attest, there is a great difference between structural knowledge of a language,when that is achieved, and ability to usc that knowledge to communicative effect.As noted earlier, facts about IL development like these have led some to advocate thatteachers abandon not just a focus onforms, but a focus onform, i.e. any attention to languageas object, as well. Flaws <strong>in</strong> this reason<strong>in</strong>g are obvious. Further, reviews of studies of the effectsof <strong>in</strong>struction on IL dcvclopment (Harley 1988; Long 1988) f<strong>in</strong>d clear evidence of somebeneficial effects of a focus onform, and suggestive evidence of others. Briefly, while it is truethat <strong>in</strong>struction does not seem capable of altcr<strong>in</strong>g sequences of developmcnt, it does appcarto offer three other advantages over either naturalistic SLA or classroom <strong>in</strong>struction with nofocus on form. (1) It speeds up the rate of learn<strong>in</strong>g (for review, see Long 1983). (2) It affectsacquisition processes <strong>in</strong> ways possibly beneficial to long-term accuracy (Lightbown 1983; Pica1983). And most crucially, on the basis of prelim<strong>in</strong>ary data, (3) it appears to raise the ultimatelevel ofatta<strong>in</strong>ment. Further, as White (1987, 1989) has argued, <strong>in</strong>comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put anddraw<strong>in</strong>g learners’ attention to <strong>in</strong>admissable constructions <strong>in</strong> thc L2 (two k<strong>in</strong>ds of negativeevidence) may be necessary when learn<strong>in</strong>g from positive evidence alone will be <strong>in</strong>adequate.To illustrate, an L1 may allow placement of adverbs of manner more flexibly than an L2. “Hedr<strong>in</strong>ks every day coffee” and “He dr<strong>in</strong>ks coffee every day” arc both acceptable <strong>in</strong> French, forexample, but not <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. Both will be communicatively effective <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, however,with the result that the French learner of <strong>English</strong> (hut not the <strong>English</strong> learner of French) willneed negative <strong>in</strong>put (e.g. error correction) on this po<strong>in</strong>t.Whereas the content of lessons with a focus onforms is theforms themselves, a syllabuswith a focus onform teaches someth<strong>in</strong>g else ~ biology, mathematics, workshop practice,automobile repair, the geography of a country where the foreign language is spoken, thecultures of <strong>its</strong> speakers, and so on ~ and overtly draws students’ attention to l<strong>in</strong>guisticelements as they arise <strong>in</strong>cidentally <strong>in</strong> lessons whose overrid<strong>in</strong>g focus is on mean<strong>in</strong>g, orcommunication. Views about how to achieve this vary. One proposal is for lessons to bebriefly “<strong>in</strong>terrupted” by teachers when they notice students mak<strong>in</strong>g errors which are (1)systematic, (2) pervasive and (3) remediable. The l<strong>in</strong>guistic feature is brought to learners’attention <strong>in</strong> any way appropriate to the students’ age, proficiency level, etc. before the classreturns to whatever pedagogic task they were work<strong>in</strong>g on when the <strong>in</strong>terruption occurred.(For details and a rationalc, see Crookes and Long 1987; Long, <strong>in</strong> press).


FOCUS ON FORM 185An example of the probable effect of <strong>in</strong>struction on ultimate atta<strong>in</strong>ment comes fromwork on the acquisition of relative clauses <strong>in</strong> a SL. Several studies (e.g., for <strong>English</strong>: Gass1982; Gass and Ard 1980; Pavesi 1986; Eckman, Bell and Nelson 1988; for Swedish:Hyltenstam 1984) have shown that both naturalistic and <strong>in</strong>structed acquirers develop relativeclauses <strong>in</strong> the order predictable from the noun phrase accessibility hierarchy (Keenan andComrie 1977; Comrie and Keenan 1979; see Figure lO.l), although with occasionalreversals of levels 5 and 6.least marked1. subject (The man that stole the car . . .)2. direct object (The man that the police arrested . . .)3. <strong>in</strong>direct object (The car that he paid noth<strong>in</strong>g for . . .)4. object of a preposition (The man that he spoke to . . .)5. possessive/genitive (The man whose . . .)6. object of a comparative (The man that Joe is older than . . .)most markedFigure 10.1 Noun phrase accessibility hierarchyOf particular <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the present context, Pavesi (1 986) compared relative clauseformation by <strong>in</strong>structed and naturalistic acquirers. The former were 48 Italian high schoolstudents, ages 14-18, who had received from 2 to 7 years (an average of 4 years) ofgrammar-based EFL <strong>in</strong>struction and who had had m<strong>in</strong>imal or (<strong>in</strong> 45 of 48 cases) no <strong>in</strong>formalexposure to <strong>English</strong>. The untutored learners were 38 Italian workers (mostly restaurantwaiters), ages 19-50, who had lived <strong>in</strong> Scotland anywhere from 3 months to 25 years (anaverage of 6 years), with considerable exposure to <strong>English</strong> at home and at work, but whohad received m<strong>in</strong>imal (usually no) formal <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction.Relative clause constructions were elicited us<strong>in</strong>g a set of numbered pictures andquestion prompts: (“Number 7 is the girl who is runn<strong>in</strong>g”, and so on). Implicational scal<strong>in</strong>gshowed that both groups’ developmental sequences correlated significantly with the nounphrase accessibility hierarchy. There were two other k<strong>in</strong>ds of differences, however. First,naturalistic learners produced statistically significantly more full nom<strong>in</strong>al copies than the<strong>in</strong>structed learners (e.g. “Numbcr 4 is the woman who the cat is look<strong>in</strong>g at the woman”),whereas <strong>in</strong>structed learners produced more pronom<strong>in</strong>al copies (“Number 4 is the womanwho the cat is look<strong>in</strong>g at her”). Given that neither <strong>English</strong> nor Italian allow copies of eitherk<strong>in</strong>d, this is further evidence of the at least partial autonomy of IL syntax, a claim alsosupported by the developmental sequence <strong>its</strong>elf, of course. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, the relativefrequencies of the diffcrent k<strong>in</strong>ds of copies suggest that the <strong>in</strong>structed learners had“grammaticized” more, even <strong>in</strong> the errors they made, a result consistent with f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs byPica (1 983) and Lightbown (1 983). Second, more <strong>in</strong>structed learners reached 80 percentcriterion on all of the five lowest NP categories <strong>in</strong> the hierarchy, with differences atta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gstatistical significance at the second lowest (genitive) level and fall<strong>in</strong>g just short (p. 06) atthe lowest (object of a comparative) level. More <strong>in</strong>structed learncrs (and very fewnaturalistic acquirers) were able to relativize out of the more marked NPs <strong>in</strong> the hierarchy.In considerably less average time, that is, <strong>in</strong>structed learners had reached higher levels ofatta<strong>in</strong>ment.


186 MICHAEL H. LONGPavcsi’s study is a non-equivalent control groups design, so causal claims are precluded.There are also no data on whether or not the high school students were ever actually taughtrelative clauses, or if so, which ones. We know simply that they received someth<strong>in</strong>g like agrammar-translation course. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are nonetheless suggestive of the k<strong>in</strong>d of effectsa focus on form may have on ultimate SL atta<strong>in</strong>ment.Two other studies, furthermore, haveshown that structurally focused teach<strong>in</strong>g of relative clause formation can accelerate learn<strong>in</strong>g,also that, at least as far down as level 4 (object of a preposition) <strong>in</strong> the hierarchy, <strong>in</strong>struction<strong>in</strong> a more marked structure will generalize hack up the implicational scale to less markedstructures (Gass 1982; Eckman et al. 1988; and see also Zobl 1985).SLA research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs like those briefly described here would seem to supporttwo conclusions. (1) Instruction built around afocus on forms is counter-productive.(2) Instruction which encourages a systematic, non-<strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>&focus onform produces a fasterrate of learn<strong>in</strong>g and (probably) higher levels of ultimate SL atta<strong>in</strong>ment than <strong>in</strong>struction withno focus onform. If correct, this would make I+ focus on form] a desirable design featureof FL <strong>in</strong>struction. Programs cxist which have this feature, alternat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> some pr<strong>in</strong>cipledway between a focus on mean<strong>in</strong>g and a focus on form. (One example is task-based languageteach<strong>in</strong>g. See Long 1985; Crookes and Long 1987; Long and Crookes 1989; Long, <strong>in</strong> press).Programs with a focus on form need to be compared <strong>in</strong> carefully controlled studies withprograms with a focus on forms and with (c.g. Natural Approach) programs with no overtfocus on form.Further researchTrue experiments are needed which compare rate of learn<strong>in</strong>g and ultimate level ofatta<strong>in</strong>ment after one of three programs:focus on forms, focus on form, and focus on communication.Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary research <strong>in</strong> this area has produced mixed results, two studics f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g positiverelationships between the amount of class time given to a focus on forms and variousproficiency measures (McDonald, Stone andYates 1977, for ESL; Mitchell, Park<strong>in</strong>son andJohnstone 198 1 , for French FL), and a third study of ESL (Spada 1986, 1987) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g nosuch effects. (For detailed review, see Chaudron 1988.) All three studies were comparisonsof <strong>in</strong>tact groups which differed <strong>in</strong> degree of focus onforms, it should be noted. Research hasyet to be conducted compar<strong>in</strong>g the unique program types.Studies of this k<strong>in</strong>d should be true experiments, employ<strong>in</strong>g a pretcst/post-test controlgroup design, and should also <strong>in</strong>clude a proccss component to monitor implementation ofthe three dist<strong>in</strong>ct treatments. They should utilize multiple outcomc measures, some focus<strong>in</strong>gon accuracy, some on communicative ability or fluency, thereby avoid<strong>in</strong>g (supposed) bias <strong>in</strong>favour of one program or another. The post-tests should <strong>in</strong>clude immediate and delayedmeasures, s<strong>in</strong>ce at least one study (Harley 1989) has found a short-term advantage forstudents receiv<strong>in</strong>g form-focused <strong>in</strong>struction disappeared (three months) later. Some of themeasures should further reflect known developmental sequences and patterns of variation<strong>in</strong> ILs, appropriate for the developmental stages of the subjects as revealed on the pretests.A dist<strong>in</strong>ction should be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed between constructions which arc <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple learnablefrom positive <strong>in</strong>stantiation <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>put and constructions which <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple require negativeevidence. (For further details and desirable characteristics of such studies, see Long 1984,forthcom<strong>in</strong>g; Larsen-Freeman and Long 1989.)Several additional issues need to be addressed, either as separate studies of thefcus onform design feature or as sub-parts of the basic study outl<strong>in</strong>ed above. Many <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gquestions rema<strong>in</strong> unanswered, after all. It will be useful to ascerta<strong>in</strong> which structures require


~ (1985)~ (1FOCUS ON FORM 187focus and/or negative evidence, and which can be left to the care of “natural processes”(White 1987). Other possibilities <strong>in</strong>clude studies motivated by implicational markednessrelationships designed to determ<strong>in</strong>e the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples govern<strong>in</strong>g maximal generalizability of<strong>in</strong>struction (see, e.g. Eckman et ul. 1988). Similarly, one can envisage studies <strong>in</strong>spired bycurrent models of UG designed to test the claimed potential of certa<strong>in</strong> structures to trigger<strong>in</strong>stantaneous (re-)sett<strong>in</strong>g of a parameter. An example would be Chomsky’s (1 98 1) workon the pro-drop parameter, and the claimed trigger<strong>in</strong>g effects of expletives with it and thereas dummy subjects (Hyams 1983; Hilles 1986). F<strong>in</strong>ally, further theoretically motivatedwork, like that of Pienemann (1 984) and Pienemann and Johnston (1 987), is clearly neededon the tim<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>struction. Research of these and other k<strong>in</strong>ds will establish the validity andscope of focus on form as a design feature <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g methodology.ReferencesAllwright, R. L. (1977) “<strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g through communication practice.” EL7 Docs76/ 3.2-14.Breen, M.P. (1987) “Contemporary paradigms <strong>in</strong> syllabus design.” <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>20/2.8 1-92, and 20/3.157-174.Chaudron, C. (1988) Second <strong>Language</strong> Classrooms. Research on <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Chomsky, N. (1981) Lectures on Government and B<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g. Dordrecht: Foris.Comrie, B. and Keenan, E.L. (1979) “Noun phrase accessibility revisited”. <strong>Language</strong>55.649-664.Corder, S.P. (1967) “The significance of learners’ errors.” International Review .f AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 5.161-170.Crookes, G. (1986) Task classtf;cation: a cross-discipl<strong>in</strong>ary review (Technical Report 4.) Honolulu:Center for Second <strong>Language</strong> Classroom Research, <strong>Social</strong> Science Research Institute,University of Hawaii at Manoa.Crookes, G. and Long, M.H. (1987) “Task-based language teach<strong>in</strong>g. A brief report”. Modern<strong>English</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> (Part 1) 8.26-28 + 61, and (Part 2) 9.20-23.D<strong>in</strong>smore, D. (1985) “Wait<strong>in</strong>g for Godot <strong>in</strong> the EFL classroom.” ELTJournal 39.225-234.Dulay, M. and Bert, H. (1973) ‘Should we teach children syntax?’ Languuge Learn<strong>in</strong>g24/2.245-258.Eckman, F.R., Bell, L. and Nelson, D. (1988) “On the generalization of relative clause<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> the acquisition of <strong>English</strong> as a second language.” Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics9/ 1.1-20.Ellis, R. (1984) “The role of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition.” <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>Formal and Informal <strong>Context</strong>s ed. by D.M. S<strong>in</strong>gleton and D.G. Little, 19-37. Dubl<strong>in</strong>:IRAAL.Understand<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.989) “Are classroom and naturalistic acquisition the same? A study of the classroomacquisition of German word order rules.” Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition11 /3.305-328.Felix, S. W. (198 1) “The effect of formal <strong>in</strong>struction on second language acquisition.” <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g 31 / 1.87-1 12.Gass, S.M. (1982)“From theory to practice.” On TESOL ’81 ed. by M. H<strong>in</strong>es andW. Rutherford,129-139. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: TESOL.Gass, S.M. and Ard, J. (1980) “L2 data: their relevance for language universals.” TESOLQuarterly 14 /4.44345 2.


~ (1989)~ (1~ (1984)~ (1~ (1~ (1188 MICHAEL H. LONGGillis, M. and Weber, R. (1976) “The emergence of sentence modalities <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong> ofjapanese-speak<strong>in</strong>g children.” <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 26/ 1.77-94.Harley, B. (1 988) “Effects of <strong>in</strong>struction on SLA: issues and evidence.” Annual Review $AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 9.165-178.“Functional grammar <strong>in</strong> French immersion: a classroom experiment.” AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 1013.331-359.Hilles, S. (1 986). “Intcrlanguage and the pro-drop parameter.” Second <strong>Language</strong> Research2/1.33-52.Hoetker, J. and Ahlbrand, W.P. (1969) “The persistence of the recitation.” American EducationalResearch Journal 6/1/ 145-167.Hyams, N. (1983) “The pro-drop parameter <strong>in</strong> child grammars.’’ Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs $ the West CoastConference on Formal L<strong>in</strong>guistics ed. by M. Barlow, D. Flick<strong>in</strong>ger and M. Westcoat. Stanford,CA: Stanford University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.Hyltenstam, K. (1977) “Implicational patterns <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage syntax variation.” <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g 27/2.383411.984) “The use of typological markedness conditions as predictors <strong>in</strong> second languageacquisition: the case of pronom<strong>in</strong>al copies <strong>in</strong> relative clauses.” Second <strong>Language</strong>s. A Cross-L<strong>in</strong>guistic Perspective ed. by R.W. Anderscn, 39-58. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Johnston, M. (1985) Syntactic and morphological progressions <strong>in</strong> learner <strong>English</strong>. Canberra,Australia: Commonwealth Department of Immigration and Ethnic Affairs.Keenan, E. and Comrie, B. (1977) “Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar.”L<strong>in</strong>guisticInquiry 8.63-99.Kellerman, E. (1985) “If at first you do succeed . . . ” Input <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition ed. byS. Gass and C. Madden, 345-353. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Krashen, S.D. and Seliger, H.W. (1975) “The essential contributions of formal <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong>adult second language learn<strong>in</strong>g.” TESOL Quarter!,9/ 12.173-1 83.Krashen, S.D. andTerrel1,T. (1983) The Natural Approach. NewYork: Pergamon Press.Larscn-Freeman, D. and Long, M.H. (1989) Research Priorities <strong>in</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gand <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: Johns Hopk<strong>in</strong>s University, National Foreign <strong>Language</strong>Center.Larsen-Freeman, D. and Long, M.H. (1991) An Introduction to Second <strong>Language</strong> AcquisitionResearch. London: Longman.Lightbown, P.M. (1983) “Explor<strong>in</strong>g relationships between developmental and <strong>in</strong>structionalsequences.” Classroom-Oriented Research on Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition ed. by H. W. Seligerand M.H. Long, 217-243. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Long, M.H. (1983) “DOCS <strong>in</strong>struction make a difference? A review of research.” TESOLQuarter!, 17 / 3.359-3 82.“Process and product <strong>in</strong> ESL program evaluation.” TESOL Quarter!,18 / 3.409425.985) “A role for <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition: task-based languageteach<strong>in</strong>g.” Modell<strong>in</strong>g and Assess<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition ed. by K. Hyltenstam and M.Pienemann, 77-99. Clevedon, Avon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.988) “Instructed <strong>in</strong>terlanguage development.” Issues <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition.Multiple Perspectives ed by L.M. Beebe, 115-141. NewYork: Newbury House.991) “The design and psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic motivation of research on foreign languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g.’’ Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research and the Classroom ed. by B. Freed. Boston:D.C. Heak<strong>in</strong>...- (2001) Task-Based <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Long, M.H.,Adams, L., McLean, M. and Castanos, F. (1976) “Do<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs with words: verbal<strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> lockstep and small group classroom situations.” On TESOL. ‘76 ed. byJ.F. Fanselow and R. Crymes, 137-153. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC:TESOL.


FOCUS ON FORM 189Long, M.H. and Crookes, G. (1 989) Un<strong>its</strong> ofanalysis <strong>in</strong> syllabus design. Ms. Department of ESL,University of Hawaii at Manoa.Long, M.H. and Sato, C.J. (1983) “Classroom foreigner talk discourse: forms and functionsof teachers’ questions.” Classroom-Oriented Research <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition ed. byH.W. Seligcr and M.H. Long, 268-285. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.McDonald, F.J., Stone, M.K. andYates, A. (1 977) The efects ofclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction patterns andstudent characteristics on the acquisition .f projcieny <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> as a second language.Pr<strong>in</strong>ceton, NJ: Educational Test<strong>in</strong>g Service.Meisel, J.M., Clahsen, H. and Pienemann, M. (198 1) “On determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g developmental stages<strong>in</strong> natural second language acquisitions.” Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition3/2.109-135.Mitchell, R., Park<strong>in</strong>son, B. and Johnstone, R. (1981) The foreign language classroom: anobservational study. (Stirl<strong>in</strong>g Educational Monographs 9.) Stirl<strong>in</strong>g: Department of Education,University of Stirl<strong>in</strong>g.Neumann, R. (1977) An attempt to d$ne through error anaksis an <strong>in</strong>termediate ESL level at UCLA.M.A. <strong>in</strong>TESL thesis. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA.Newmark, L. (1 966) “How not to <strong>in</strong>terfere with language learn<strong>in</strong>g.” lnternational lournu1 .fAmerican L<strong>in</strong>guistics 32/ 1.77-83.Newmark, L. and Reibel, D.A. (1968) “Necessity and sufficiency <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g.”lnternational Review ofApplied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 6.145-1 64.Nunan, D. (1 987) “Communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g: mak<strong>in</strong>g it work.” ELT Journal41 /2.136-145.Pavesi, M. (1986) “Markedness, discoursal modes, and relative clause formation <strong>in</strong> a formaland an <strong>in</strong>formal context.” Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 8.138-55.Phillips, D. and Shettlesworth, C. (1 975). “Questions <strong>in</strong> the design and implementation ofcourses <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> for specialized purposes.” Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs ofthe 4th lnternational Congress ofApplied L<strong>in</strong>guistics (Volume 1) ed. by G. Nickel, 249-264. Stuttgart: HochschuleVerlag.Pica,T. (1 983) “Adult acquisition of <strong>English</strong> as a second language under different conditions ofexposure.” <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 33 /4.465497.Pica, T. and Long, M.H. (1 986) “Thc l<strong>in</strong>guistic and conversation performance of experiencedand <strong>in</strong>experienced teachers.”“Talk<strong>in</strong>g to learn”: Conversation <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisitioned. by R.R. Day, 85-98. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Piencmann, M. (1 984) “Psychological constra<strong>in</strong>ts on the teachability of languages’ .” Studies <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition 6/2.186-214.Pienemann, M. and Johnston M. (1987) “Factors <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the dcvclopment of languageproficiency.” Apply<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research ed. by D. Nunan, 45-141.Adelaide. SA: National Curriculum Resource Centre.Prabhu, N.S. (1987) Second <strong>Language</strong> Pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Ross, S. (1992) “Program-def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g evaluation <strong>in</strong> a decade of cclecticism.” In Alderson C. andBcrctta, A. (eds). Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Education. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Prcss.Scherer, G. and Wertheimer, M. (1 964) A Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic Experiment <strong>in</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>.NewYork: McGraw-Hill.Schmidt, R. W. (1 990) “The role of consciousness <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g” AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics 1 1 /2.1745.Schumann, J.H. (1 979) “The acquisition of <strong>English</strong> negation by speakers of Spanish: a review ofthe literature.” The Acquisition and Use of Spanish and <strong>English</strong> as First and Second <strong>Language</strong>sed. by R.W. Andersen, 3-32. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC:TESOL.Smith, P. (1 970) A Comparison of the Cognitive and Audio1<strong>in</strong>gual Approaches to Foreign <strong>Language</strong>Instruction: The Pennsylvania Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Project. Philadelphia: Center for CurriculumDevelopmcnt.


~ (1~ (1190 MICHAEL H. LONGSpada, N. (1986) “The <strong>in</strong>teraction between types of content and types of <strong>in</strong>struction: someeffects on the L2 proficicncy of adult lcarncrs.” Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition8 /2.18 1-199.987) “Relationships between <strong>in</strong>structional differences and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes: aprocess-product study of communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g.” Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics8.137-1 61 .Stauble, A.-M. (1981) A comparative study $a Spanish-<strong>English</strong> undjapanese-<strong>English</strong> second languagecont<strong>in</strong>uum: verb phrase morphology. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA.Swaffer, J.K., Arens, K. and Morgan, M. (1982) “Teacher classroom practices: redef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gmethod as task hierarchy.” Modern <strong>Language</strong> journal 66.24-33.Von Elek, T. and Oskarsson, M. (1975) Comparative Methods Experiments <strong>in</strong> Foreign <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Department of Educational Research. Gothenburg, Sweden: Molnda School ofEducation.White, L. (1987) “Aga<strong>in</strong>st comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put: the Input Hypothesis and the development ofsecond-language competence.” Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 8/2.95-110.White, L. (1989) ‘The pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of adjacency <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition: do learnersobserve the subset pr<strong>in</strong>ciple?’ Paper presented at the Child <strong>Language</strong> Conference BostonMA. March.Wode, H. (1 981) “<strong>Language</strong>-acquisitional universals: a unified view of language acquisition.”Native <strong>Language</strong> and Foreign Languuge Acquisition. (Annuls ofthe New York Academy ofsciences379) ed. by H. W<strong>in</strong>itz, 2 18-234. NewYork: NewYork Academy of Sciences.Zobl, H. (1982) “A direction for contrastive analysis: the comparative study of developmentalsequences.” TESOL Quarterly 16. 169-183.985) “Grammars <strong>in</strong> search of <strong>in</strong>put and <strong>in</strong>take.” Input <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition cd.by S.M. Gass and C. Madden, 329-344. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.


Chaptev 11David NunanTEACHING GRAMMAR IN CONTEXTIntroductionROM A GRAMMATICAL PERSPECTIVE, MANY foreign languageF programmes and teach<strong>in</strong>g materials are based on a l<strong>in</strong>ear model of language acquisition.This model operates on the premise that learners acquire one target language item at a time,<strong>in</strong> a sequential, step-by-step fashion. However, such a model is <strong>in</strong>consistent with what isobserved as learners go about the process of acquir<strong>in</strong>g another language. In this chapter Iargue for an alternative to the l<strong>in</strong>ear model which I call, for want of a better term, an organicapproach to second language pedagogy. In the first part of the chapter I shall contrast bothapproaches, and look at evidence from second language acquisition and discourse analysiswhich supports the organic view. In the second part I shall outl<strong>in</strong>e some of the pedagogicalimplications of thc organic approach, illustrat<strong>in</strong>g them with practical ideas for the classroom.Metaphors for second language acquisitionA strictly l<strong>in</strong>ear approach to language learn<strong>in</strong>g is based on the premise that learners acquireone grammatical item at a time, and that they should demonstrate their mastery of one th<strong>in</strong>gbefore mov<strong>in</strong>g on to the next. For example, <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>, a student should masterone tense form, such as the simple present, before be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>troduced to other forms, suchas the present cont<strong>in</strong>uous or the simple past. Metaphorically, learn<strong>in</strong>g another language bythis method is like construct<strong>in</strong>g a wall.The language wall is erected one l<strong>in</strong>guistic ‘brick’ ata time.The easy grammatical bricks are laid at the bottom of the wall, provid<strong>in</strong>g a foundationfor the more difficult ones.The task for the learner is to get the l<strong>in</strong>guistic bricks <strong>in</strong> the rightorder: first the word bricks, and then the sentence bricks. If the bricks are not <strong>in</strong> the correctorder, the wall will collapse under <strong>its</strong> own ungrammaticality.When we observe learners as they go about the process of learn<strong>in</strong>g another language,we see that, by and large, they do not acquire language <strong>in</strong> the step-by-step, build<strong>in</strong>g blockfashion suggested by the l<strong>in</strong>ear model. It is simply not the case that language learners acquiretarget items perfectly, one at a time. Kellerman (1983), for example, notes the ‘u-shapedbehavior’ of certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic items <strong>in</strong> learners’ <strong>in</strong>terlanguage devclopment. Accuracy doesnot <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> a l<strong>in</strong>ear fashion, from 20% to 40% to 100%; at times, it actually decreases.It appears that, rather than be<strong>in</strong>g isolated bricks, the various elements of language <strong>in</strong>teractwith, and are affected by, othcr elements to which they are closely related <strong>in</strong> a functional


e<strong>in</strong>gmore192 DAVID NUNANsense. This <strong>in</strong>terrelationship accounts for the fact that a learner’s mastery of a particularlanguage item is unstable, appear<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>crease and decrease at different times dur<strong>in</strong>g thelearn<strong>in</strong>g process. For example, mastery of the simple present deteriorates (temporarily) atthe po<strong>in</strong>t when learners are beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g to acquire the present cont<strong>in</strong>uous. Rutherford (1 987)describes this process as a k<strong>in</strong>d of l<strong>in</strong>guistic metamorphosis.The adoption of an ‘organic’ perspective can greatly enrich our understand<strong>in</strong>g oflanguage acquisition and use. Without this perspective, our understand<strong>in</strong>g of otherdimensions of language such as the notion of ‘grammaticality’ will be piecemeal and<strong>in</strong>complete, as will any attempt at understand<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g utterances <strong>in</strong> isolationfrom the contexts <strong>in</strong> which they occur. The organic metaphor sees second languageacquisition more like grow<strong>in</strong>g a garden than build<strong>in</strong>g a wall. From such a perspective,learners do not learn one th<strong>in</strong>g perfectly, one item at a time, but numerous th<strong>in</strong>gssimultaneously (and imperfectly). The l<strong>in</strong>guistic flowers do not all appear at the same time,nor do they all grow at the same rate. Some even appear to wilt, for a time, before renew<strong>in</strong>gtheir growth.The rate of growth is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by a complex <strong>in</strong>terplay of factors related tospeech process<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>ts (Pienemann and Johnston 1987), pedagogical <strong>in</strong>terventions(Pica 1985), acquisitional processes (Johnston 1987), and the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the discoursalenvironment <strong>in</strong> which the items occur (Nunan 1993).<strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> contextIn textbooks, grammar is very often presented out of context. Learners are given isolatedsentences, which they are expected to <strong>in</strong>ternalke through exercises <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g repetition,manipulation, and grammatical transformation. These exercises are designed to providelearners with formal, declarative mastery, but unless they provide opportunities for learnersto explore grammatical structures <strong>in</strong> context, they make the task of develop<strong>in</strong>g proceduralskill ~ able to use the language for communication ~ difficult than it needs tobe, hecause learners are denied the opportunity of see<strong>in</strong>g the systematic relationships thatexist between form, mean<strong>in</strong>g, and use.As teachers, we need to help learners see that effective communication <strong>in</strong>volves achiev<strong>in</strong>gharmony between functional <strong>in</strong>terpretation and formal appropriacy (Halliday 1985) by giv<strong>in</strong>gthem tasks that dramatize the relationship between grammatical items and the discoursalcontexts <strong>in</strong> which they occur. In genu<strong>in</strong>e communication beyond the classroom, grammarand context are often so closely related that appropriate grammatical choices can only bemade with reference to the context and purpose of the communication.This, by the way, isone of the reasons why it is often difficult to answer learners’ questions about grammaticalappropriacy : <strong>in</strong> many <strong>in</strong>stances, the answer is that it depends on the attitude or orientationthat the speaker wants to take towards the events he or she wishes to report.If learners are not given opportunities to explore grammar <strong>in</strong> context, it will be difficultfor them to see how and why alternative forms exist to express different communicativemean<strong>in</strong>gs. For example, gett<strong>in</strong>g learners to read a set of sentences <strong>in</strong> the active voice, andthen transform these <strong>in</strong>to passives follow<strong>in</strong>g a model, is a standard way of <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g thepassive voice. However, it needs to be supplemented by tasks which give learnersopportunities to explore when it is communicatively appropriate to use the passive ratherthan the active voice. (One of my favourite textbook <strong>in</strong>structions is an <strong>in</strong>junction to students,<strong>in</strong> a book which shall rema<strong>in</strong> nameless, that ‘the passive should be avoided if at all possible’.)We need to supplement form-focused exercises with an approach that dramatizes forlearners the fact that different forms enable them to express different mean<strong>in</strong>gs; that


TEACHING GRAMMAR IN CONTEXT 193grammar allows them to make mean<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly sophisticated k<strong>in</strong>ds, to escape fromthe tyranny of the here and now, not only to report events and states of affairs, but toeditorialize, and to communicate their own attitudes towards these events and affairs.Unfortunately, many courses fail to make clear the relationship between form and function.Learners are taught about the forms, but not how to use them to communicate mean<strong>in</strong>gFor example, through exercises such as the one referred to <strong>in</strong> the preced<strong>in</strong>g paragraph, theyare taught how to transform sentences from the active voice <strong>in</strong>to the passive, and back <strong>in</strong>tothe active voicc; however, they are not shown that passive forms have evolved to achievecerta<strong>in</strong> communicative ends -to enable the speaker or writer to place the communicativefocus on the action rather than on the performer of the action, to avoid referr<strong>in</strong>g to theperformer of the action. If the communicative value of alternative grammatical forms is notmade clear to learners, they come away from the classroom with the impression that thealternative forms exist merely to make th<strong>in</strong>gs difficult for them. We need an approachthrough which they learn how to form structures correctly, and also how to use them tocommunicate mean<strong>in</strong>g. Such a methodology will show learners how to use grammar to getth<strong>in</strong>gs done, socialize, obta<strong>in</strong> goods and services, and express their personality throughlanguage. In other words, it will show them how to achieve their communicative endsthrough the appropriate deployment of grammatical resources.Some practical implicationsIn the rest of this chapter I shall focus on the implications of an organic approach to languageteach<strong>in</strong>g. Such an approach offers excit<strong>in</strong>g opportunities for teachers and students to lookat language <strong>in</strong> a new way ~ as a vehicle for tak<strong>in</strong>g voyages of pedagogical exploration <strong>in</strong> theclassroom and beyond.There are many different ways of activat<strong>in</strong>g organic learn<strong>in</strong>g, and many ‘traditional’exercise types can, with a slight twist, be brought <strong>in</strong>to harmony with this approach,particularly if they are <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong>to the classroom as exploratory and collaborative tasks.(For examples, see Wajnryb’s (1990) ‘grammar dictation’ tasks, and Woods’ (1995) gap andcloze exercises.)In my own classroom, I try to activate an organic approach by:teach<strong>in</strong>g language as a set of choices;provid<strong>in</strong>g opportunities for learners to cxplore grammatical and discoursalrelationships <strong>in</strong> authentic data;teach<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> ways that make form/function relationships transparent;encourag<strong>in</strong>g learners to become active explorers of language;encourag<strong>in</strong>g learners to cxplore relationships between grammar and discourse.<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> language as a set of choicesAs <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> the preced<strong>in</strong>g section, one of the reasons why it is difficult to give learnershard-and-fast grammatical rules is that, <strong>in</strong> many <strong>in</strong>stances, once grammar is pressed <strong>in</strong>tocommunicative service, decisions about which forms to use will be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by themean<strong>in</strong>gs learners themselves wish to make. For example, if learners wish to give equalweight to two pieces of <strong>in</strong>formation, they can present the <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle sentence,us<strong>in</strong>g co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation. If they wish to give one of these pieces of <strong>in</strong>formation greater weight,they can use subord<strong>in</strong>ation.In order to help learners see that alternative grammatical realizations exist <strong>in</strong> order to


194 DAVID NUNANenable them to make different k<strong>in</strong>ds of mean<strong>in</strong>gs, and that ultimately it is up to them todecide exactly what they wish to convey, I often beg<strong>in</strong> my language courses with ‘icebreaker’tasks such as Example 1. In complet<strong>in</strong>g this task, learners come to fashion theirown understand<strong>in</strong>g of the functional dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between contrast<strong>in</strong>g forms.They also cometo appreciate the fact that <strong>in</strong> many <strong>in</strong>stances it is only the speaker or writer who can decidewhich of the contrast<strong>in</strong>g forms is the appropriate one.Example 1In groups of 3 or 4, study the follow<strong>in</strong>g conversational extracts. Focus <strong>in</strong> particular on theparts of the conversation <strong>in</strong> italics. What is the difference between what Person A says andwhat Person B says? When would you use one form, and when would you use the other?1 A:B:2 A:B:A:3 A:B:4 A:B:5 A:B:6 A:B:7 A:B:8 A:B:9 A:B:A:I’ve seen Romeo and Juliet twice.Me too. I saw it last Tuesday, and aga<strong>in</strong> on the weekend.Want to go to the movies?No. I’m go<strong>in</strong>g to study tonight. We have an exam tomorrow, you know.Oh, <strong>in</strong> that case, I’ll study as well.Looks wet outside. I’m supposed to go to Central, but I don’t have an umbrella. vI went out without one, I’dget wet.Yes, I went out a while ago. YI’d gone out without an umbrella, I’d have got wet.Ijnished my essay just before the deadl<strong>in</strong>e for submission.Yes, m<strong>in</strong>e wasjnished just <strong>in</strong> time as well.My brother, who lives <strong>in</strong> NewYork, is visit<strong>in</strong>g me here <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong.What a co<strong>in</strong>cidence! My brother, who is visit<strong>in</strong>g me <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong, lives <strong>in</strong> NewYork, too.I need you to look after the kids.You’l1 bc homc carly tonight, won’tyou?Oh, you’ll be late tonight, willyou?I won a prize <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g competition.Yeah? I won the prize <strong>in</strong> the poetry competition.The baby was sleep<strong>in</strong>g when I got homc.So, he’ll be sleep<strong>in</strong>g when I get home, then?Are you hungry?No, I’ve already eaten.Well, I’ll have already eaten by the time you get home.Compare explanations with another group. What similarities and differences are there <strong>in</strong>your explanations?Provid<strong>in</strong>g opportunities for learners to explore grammatical and discoursalrelationships <strong>in</strong> authentic dataNon-authentic texts are meant to make language easier to comprehend but an unvary<strong>in</strong>gdiet of such texts can make language learn<strong>in</strong>g more, not less, difficult for learners. Authenticlanguage shows how grammatical forms operate <strong>in</strong> the ‘real world’, rather than <strong>in</strong> the m<strong>in</strong>dof a textbook writer; it allows learners to encounter target language items - such as thecomparative adjectives and adverbs <strong>in</strong> Example 2 ~ <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with other closely relatedgrammatical and discoursal elements. What learners need is a balanced diet of both typesof text.


TEACHING GRAMMAR IN CONTEXT 195Example 2Study the follow<strong>in</strong>g extracts. One is a piece of genu<strong>in</strong>e conversation, the other is taken froma language teach<strong>in</strong>g textbook. Which is which? What differences can you see between thetwo extracts? What language do you th<strong>in</strong>k the non-authentic conversation is try<strong>in</strong>g to teach?What grammar would you need <strong>in</strong> order to take part <strong>in</strong> the authentic conversation?Text A‘A: Excuse me, please. Do you knowwhere the nearest bank is?B: Well, the City Bank isn’t far fromhere. Do you know where the ma<strong>in</strong>post office is?A: No, not really. I’m just pass<strong>in</strong>g through.B: Well, first go down this street to thetraffic light.A: OK.B: Then turn left and go west on SunsetBoulevard for about two blocks. Thebank is on your right, just past the postoffice.A: All right. Thanks!B: You’re welcome.Text B2A:B:A:B:A:B:A:B:A:B:A:How do I get to Kens<strong>in</strong>gton Road?Well you go down Fullarton Road , . .. . . what, down Old Belair, and around. . .. ?Yeah. And then you go straight . . .. . . past the hospital?Yeah, keep go<strong>in</strong>g straight, past theracecourse to the roundabout.Youknow the big roundabout?Yeah.And Kens<strong>in</strong>gton Road’s off to the right.What, off the roundabout?YeahRight.<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> language <strong>in</strong> ways that make form /function relationships transparentThis pr<strong>in</strong>ciple can be activated by creat<strong>in</strong>g pedagogical tasks <strong>in</strong> which learners structureand restructure their own understand<strong>in</strong>g of form/function relationships through <strong>in</strong>ductiveand deductive tasks. Example 3, taken from Badalamenti and Henner-Stanch<strong>in</strong>a (1 993:lOS), is useful for explor<strong>in</strong>g a range of structures, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g ‘there + be’, articles, yes/noquestions, and conjunctions.The teacher can determ<strong>in</strong>e which form/function relationshipsare focused on by giv<strong>in</strong>g the learners certa<strong>in</strong> types of prompts, for example: Whoseapartment is this? How much can you tell about the person who lives here? Is the personpoor? Why is the person fit?Encourag<strong>in</strong>g learners to become active explorers of languageBy exploit<strong>in</strong>g this pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, teachers can encourage their students to take greaterresponsibility for their own learn<strong>in</strong>g. (A strik<strong>in</strong>g example of this pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, <strong>in</strong> an ESL sett<strong>in</strong>g,can be found <strong>in</strong> Heath (1 992).) Students can br<strong>in</strong>g samples of language <strong>in</strong>to class, and worktogether to formulate their own hypotheses about language structures and functions. Isometimes give my students a Polaroid camera, and get them to walk around the campustak<strong>in</strong>g photographs, either of signs and public notices which they believe are ungrammatical,or of signs which they th<strong>in</strong>k are <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, or puzzl<strong>in</strong>g, or which conta<strong>in</strong> language theywould like to know more about. The photographs then become the raw material for ournext language lesson. In fact, the last time I did ths, the lesson culm<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> the studentswrit<strong>in</strong>g a letter to the university estates office po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out the errors and suggest<strong>in</strong>gamendments.


196 DAVID NUNANExample 3Look at the picture. Whose apartment is this? Make guesses about the person who liveshere. Circle your guesses and then expla<strong>in</strong> them by circl<strong>in</strong>g the clues <strong>in</strong> the picture.3 '5-1 I1. The person is2. Theperson3. The person4. The person is5. The person is6. The person is7. The person is8. The person is9. The person is10. The person isa man / a womanhas a baby / doesn't have a babyhas a pet / doesn't have a petathletic / not athletica coffee dr<strong>in</strong>ker / not a coffee dr<strong>in</strong>kerwell-educated / not well-educateda smoker / not a smokermiddle class I poora music lover / not a music loveron a diet / not on a dietClassrooms where the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of active exploration has been activated will becharacterized by an <strong>in</strong>ductive approach to learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which learners are given access todata and provided with structured opportunities to work out rules, pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, andapplications for themselves.The idea here is that <strong>in</strong>formation will be more deeply processedand stored if learners are given an opportunity to work th<strong>in</strong>gs out for themselves, ratherthan simply be<strong>in</strong>g given the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple or rule.Encourag<strong>in</strong>g learners to explore relationships between grammar and discourseTasks exploit<strong>in</strong>g this pr<strong>in</strong>ciple show learners that grammar and discourse are <strong>in</strong>extricably<strong>in</strong>terl<strong>in</strong>ked, and that grammatical choices (for example, whether to comb<strong>in</strong>e two pieces of<strong>in</strong>formation us<strong>in</strong>g co-ord<strong>in</strong>ation or subord<strong>in</strong>ation) will be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by considerations ofcontext and purpose. Such tasks help learners to explore the function<strong>in</strong>g of grammar <strong>in</strong>


TEACHING GRAMMAR IN CONTEXT 197context, and assist them <strong>in</strong> deploy<strong>in</strong>g their develop<strong>in</strong>g grammatical competence <strong>in</strong> thecreation of coherent discourse.Example 4Consider the follow<strong>in</strong>g pieces of <strong>in</strong>formation about nurs<strong>in</strong>g.The nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is a systematic method.The nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is a rational method.The method <strong>in</strong>volves plann<strong>in</strong>g nurs<strong>in</strong>g care.The method <strong>in</strong>volves provid<strong>in</strong>g nurs<strong>in</strong>g care.These can be ‘packaged’ <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle sentence by us<strong>in</strong>g grammatical resources of variousk<strong>in</strong>ds:The nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is a systematic and rational method of plann<strong>in</strong>g and provid<strong>in</strong>gnurs<strong>in</strong>g care.Tusk 1 Us<strong>in</strong>g the above sentence as the topic sentence <strong>in</strong> a paragraph, produce a coherentparagraph <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation. (You can rearrange the order <strong>in</strong> whichthe <strong>in</strong>formation is presented.)The goal of the nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is to identify a client’s health status.The goal of the nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is to identify a client’s health care problems.A client’s health care problems may be actual or potential.The goal of the nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is to establish plans to meet a client’s health careneeds.The goal of the nurs<strong>in</strong>g process is to deliver specific nurs<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventions.Nurs<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventions are designed to meet a client’s health care needs.The nurse must collaborate with the client to carry out the nurs<strong>in</strong>g process effcctively.The nurse must collaborate with the client to <strong>in</strong>dividualize approaches to each person’sparticular needs.The nurse must collaborate with other members of the health care team to carry out thenurs<strong>in</strong>g process effectively.The nurse must collaborate with other members of the health care team to <strong>in</strong>dividualizeapproaches to each person’s particular needs.Tusk 2 Compare your text with that written by another student. Make a note of similaritiesand differences. Can you expla<strong>in</strong> the differences? Do different ways of comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation lead to differences of mean<strong>in</strong>g?Esk 3 Now revise your text and compare it with the orig<strong>in</strong>al. [This is supplied separatelyto the students.](Adapted from Nunan 1996)


198 DAVID NUNANConclusionIn this chapter, I have argued that we need to go beyond l<strong>in</strong>ear approaches and traditionalform-focused methodological practices <strong>in</strong> the grammar class, and that while such practicesmight be necessary, they do not go far enough <strong>in</strong> prepar<strong>in</strong>g learners to press theirgrammatical resources <strong>in</strong>to communicative use. I have suggested that grammar <strong>in</strong>structionwill be more effective <strong>in</strong> classrooms where:learners are exposed to authentic samples of language so that the grammatical featuresbe<strong>in</strong>g taught are encountered <strong>in</strong> a range of different l<strong>in</strong>guistic and experientialcontexts:it is not assumed that once learners have been drilled <strong>in</strong> a particular form they haveacquired it, and drill<strong>in</strong>g is seen only as a first step towards eventual mastery:there are opportunities for recycl<strong>in</strong>g of language forms, and learners are engaged <strong>in</strong>tasks designed to make transparent the l<strong>in</strong>ks between form, mean<strong>in</strong>g, and use:learners are given Opportunities to develop their own understand<strong>in</strong>gs of thegrammatical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>English</strong> by progressively structur<strong>in</strong>g and restructur<strong>in</strong>g thelanguage through <strong>in</strong>ductive learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences which encourage them to explorethe function<strong>in</strong>g of grammar <strong>in</strong> context:over time, learners encounter target language items <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly diverse andcomplex range of l<strong>in</strong>guistic and experiential environments.In mak<strong>in</strong>g a case for a more organic approach to grammar teach<strong>in</strong>g, I hope that I have notgiven the impression that specially written texts and dialogues, drills, and deductivepresentations by the teacher, have no place <strong>in</strong> the grammar class. What we need is anappropriate balance between exercises that help learners come to grips with grammaticalforms, and tasks for explor<strong>in</strong>g the use of those forms to communicate effectively.In seek<strong>in</strong>g to explore alternative ways of achiev<strong>in</strong>g our pedagogical goals, it is importantnot to overstate the case for one viewpo<strong>in</strong>t rather than another, or to discount factors suchas cognitive style, learn<strong>in</strong>g strategy preferences, prior learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences, and the culturalcontexts <strong>in</strong> which the language is be<strong>in</strong>g taught and learnt. However, while there arc somegrammatical structures that may be acquired <strong>in</strong> a l<strong>in</strong>ear way, it seems clear from a rapidlygrow<strong>in</strong>g body of research that the majority of structures are acquired <strong>in</strong> complex, nonl<strong>in</strong>earways.Notes1 I have not acknowledged the source of this extract, because I do not wish to appear to becriticiz<strong>in</strong>g the text from which it was taken. It is cited here for contrastive purposes only.2 Source: D. Nunan (1993).AcknowledgementThe author and the publisher would like to thank He<strong>in</strong>le and He<strong>in</strong>le for their k<strong>in</strong>dpermission to reproduce copyright material from Badalamenti and Henner-Stanch<strong>in</strong>a(1993).


~ (1~ (1996)TEACHING GRAMMAR IN CONTEXT 199ReferencesBadalamenti, V. and Henner-Stanch<strong>in</strong>a, C. (1993) Grammar Dimensions One. Boston: He<strong>in</strong>le andHe<strong>in</strong>le.Ellis, R. (1994) The Study $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Halliday, M.A.K. (1985) An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Arnold.Heath, S.B. (1992) ‘Literary skills or literate skills? Considerations for ESL/EFL learners’ <strong>in</strong>Nunan (1992).Johnston, M. (1987) ‘Understand<strong>in</strong>g learner language’ <strong>in</strong> Nunan (1987).Kellerman, E. (1 983) ‘If at first you do succeed . . . , <strong>in</strong> S. Gass and C. Madden (eds) Input <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Rowley, Mass. : Newbury House.Larsen-Freeman, D. and Long, M. (1991) An Introduction to Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research.London: Longman.Nunan, D. (ed.) (1987) Apply<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition Research. Adelaide: NCRC.992) Collaborative <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Cambridge: Cambridgc UniversityPress.Nunan, D. (1993) Introduc<strong>in</strong>g Discourse Analysis. London: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Academic Writ<strong>in</strong>gfor Nurs<strong>in</strong>g Students. Hong Kong: The <strong>English</strong> Centre. Universityof Hong Kong.Pica,T. (1985) ‘The selective impact of classroom <strong>in</strong>struction on second language acquisition’.Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 6 / 3 : 2 14-2 2.Pienemann, M. and Johnston, M. (1987) ‘Factors <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the development of languageproficiency’ <strong>in</strong> Nunan (1987).Rutherford, W. (1987) Second <strong>Language</strong> Grammar: <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Learn<strong>in</strong>g. London: Longman.Wajnryb, R. (1990) Grammar Dictation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Woods, E. (1995) Introduc<strong>in</strong>g Grammar. London: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.


Chapter 12Anne BurnsGENRE-BASED APPROACHES TO WRITINGAND BEGINNING ADULT ESL LEARNERSIntroductionOMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING (CLT) HAS played <strong>its</strong> partC <strong>in</strong> revolutionis<strong>in</strong>g narrowly conceived theories of language learn<strong>in</strong>g and most languageteachers would say they no longer equate the learn<strong>in</strong>g of a second language with the learn<strong>in</strong>gof traditional grammar. At the same time, CLT has given rise to a sometimes confus<strong>in</strong>g arrayof methodologies, some of which claim to be ‘the method’ by which second languages willbe acquired and all of which call themselves ‘communicative’. This has often led to a stateof affairs <strong>in</strong> the language classroom which seems to derive much of <strong>its</strong> pedagogical base from<strong>in</strong>tuition.More and more, researchers and educators have bcgun to question somc of theassumptions implicit <strong>in</strong> communicative approaches to secontl-language teach<strong>in</strong>g which havefailed to take <strong>in</strong>to account a well-formulated theory of language. Copc (1989) has arguedthat what is needed is an ‘authoritative’ pedagogy for the 1990s which will replace what heterms the ‘progressive’ curriculum which has existed s<strong>in</strong>ce the mi& 1970s. Because of <strong>its</strong>discovery learn<strong>in</strong>g, ego-centred base, progressive ESL pedagogy has failed to make explicitto learners the knowledge they need to ga<strong>in</strong> access to socially powerful forms of language.It has emphasised <strong>in</strong>quiry learn<strong>in</strong>g, process and naturalism but has neglected to offer learnerssystematic explanations of how language functions <strong>in</strong> various social contexts.In recent years much attention has been given to socially based theories of languageand <strong>in</strong> Australia work draw<strong>in</strong>g on systemic l<strong>in</strong>guistics and notions of genre and registerdeveloped by Michael Halliday (e.g. Halliday 1985; Halliday and Hasan 1985) has provideda model for expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> relation to the context <strong>in</strong> which it is used, while atthe same time tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to account language at the levels of whole text. I would also arguethat systemic-functional approaches to language learn<strong>in</strong>g and teach<strong>in</strong>g fit well withCommunicative <strong>Language</strong><strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>, as they provide teachers and learners with a means ofexplor<strong>in</strong>g language use with<strong>in</strong> a framework of cultural and social purpoTe.Although genre-based language theories have application to both spoken and writtenlanguage, much of the work done <strong>in</strong> educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs has related to literacy development<strong>in</strong> the schools context (Mart<strong>in</strong> and Rothcry 1980, 1981 ; Mart<strong>in</strong> 1985). Thc Adult MigrantEducation Program (AMEP) Literacy Project organised throughout the National Centre for<strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Research (NCELTR) described by Hammond (1989) has


GENRE-BASED APPROACHES TO WRITING 201drawn on this work as well as on work done by the Sydncy Metropolitan East DisadvantagedSchools Program (Callaghan and Rothery 1988).The NCELTR Literacy Project: a genre-based approachAs one of the teachers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the Project, I was particularly <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>ghow gcnre-based approaches could be applicd to adult second-language learners at the earlystages of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Typically <strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g ESL classes, read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g are consigned tosecond place and the focus is on the development of speak<strong>in</strong>g and listen<strong>in</strong>g. In addition,assumptions are frequently made that bcg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g learners are unable or not rcady to copewith the development of read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, even though there is a frequentreliance on written materials to support spoken language development.Teachei-s sometimesma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that learners do not have well-developed skills <strong>in</strong> first-language literacy andthcrefore it will be difficult to provide <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> a second language where oral skills arealmost non-existent also. This may be true, but many beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g learners do have welldevelopedlitcracy skills <strong>in</strong> first language and those who do not will generally wish to acquirethem <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>.I would argue that these beliefs prevent learners from ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g access to opportunitiesto develop their literacy skills <strong>in</strong> second language and from understand<strong>in</strong>g and respond<strong>in</strong>gto the written texts which will be of value to them <strong>in</strong> further<strong>in</strong>g their learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>extend<strong>in</strong>g their ability to cope with a range of tasks common <strong>in</strong> the wider community, manyof which depend on the ability to read and writc.In the schools context the range of genres dealt with <strong>in</strong> the classroom is fairly rcstrictcd,as they will be those which are pedagogical <strong>in</strong> their purpose and powerful withn the contextof the school curriculum. In the adult context the choice is more open-ended, as texts willbe drawn from a larger number of social, vocational and work-related genres. At present,teachers work<strong>in</strong>g with beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g adult ESL learners have few guidel<strong>in</strong>es to direct them toappropriate texts. This has meant that teachers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the NCELTR Literacy Projecthave, to a certa<strong>in</strong> extent, become classroom researchers try<strong>in</strong>g out a variety of genres basedon needs expressed by their learners, to discover which are appropriate and relevant atdifferent stages of learn<strong>in</strong>g.Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g learners and a genre-based approachWith<strong>in</strong> the group of Literacy Project participants was one teacher who was workmg on aclass for beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g learners. Because part of the participants’ <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> the projectwas the record<strong>in</strong>g of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction and the document<strong>in</strong>g of any written texts used,she agreed that I would work collaboratively with her, collect<strong>in</strong>g and record<strong>in</strong>g the classroomdata as she taught thc class. The 19 lcarncrs were all with<strong>in</strong> their first year of settlement aspermanent immigrants to Australia and had all been rated as less than 1 .O on a seven-po<strong>in</strong>toral rat<strong>in</strong>g scalc (AMES, Speak<strong>in</strong>g Proficiency Descriptions, Br<strong>in</strong>dlcy 1979) .Twelve of themhad completed high school and, of these, six had some post-high school education. Of theothers, two had primary school education only, while six had received vary<strong>in</strong>g levels of highschool education. They came from a wide variety of first-language backgrounds, some ofwhich used non-Roman script.One of the genres identified as important by the learners, <strong>in</strong> consultation with theteacher, was job applications, and the writ<strong>in</strong>g of a lettcr of application was used by the


202 ANNE BURNSteacher to structure a unit of work. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the theoretical <strong>in</strong>put sessions at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the project, Jennifer Hammond had proposed a teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g cycle (Callaghan andRothery 1988), an adaptation of which (Hammond 1990) is presented <strong>in</strong> Figure 1 2.1 below,which could be used to <strong>in</strong>form the plann<strong>in</strong>g of classroom activities.read examples of genre;discuss and analyse textteacher and learnersconstruct text; ongo<strong>in</strong>gstandard ,,‘DEVELOPINGJ’conferenc<strong>in</strong>g between ‘,,I’ teacher and learner ‘\,own textiINDEPENDENT CONSTRUCTION OF TEXTFigure 1 2.1 The teach<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g cycleSource: Hammond (1990)This cycle <strong>in</strong>corporates different classroom activities which move the learners throughvarious spoken and written tasks related to the genre be<strong>in</strong>g taught. The teacher can beg<strong>in</strong>the cycle at any po<strong>in</strong>t, but for genres be<strong>in</strong>g taught for the first time it is preferable for theteacher to work through all stages. For this particular class, the teacher decided to work <strong>in</strong>the follow<strong>in</strong>g sequence of stages:1 Modell<strong>in</strong>g2 Jo<strong>in</strong>t Negotiation ofText3 Independent Construction.Modell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volved discussion of the cultural and social purpose of the genre and theshar<strong>in</strong>g of experience with<strong>in</strong> that context, followed by examples of a model text. At thispo<strong>in</strong>t the teacher and learners discussed the stag<strong>in</strong>g of the text and the dist<strong>in</strong>ctive languagefeatures which realised the text.The stage Jo<strong>in</strong>t Negotiation <strong>in</strong>volved the teacher and learners<strong>in</strong> a jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of a text <strong>in</strong> the same genre, followed by the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction ofanother text by the learners work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> groups. The f<strong>in</strong>al stage, Independent Construction,comprised the teacher and learners work<strong>in</strong>g together to discuss and rcvise the group’s jo<strong>in</strong>tlyconstructed texts and the learners’ construction of their own <strong>in</strong>dependent texts.


GENRE-BASED APPROACHES TO WRITING 203Putt<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>in</strong>to practiceThe rest of this paper describes part of a lesson which focuses on the stage where the teachergave a presentation of a model and the learners followed up with jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of asimilar text. At the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the lesson, the teacher and learners aga<strong>in</strong> discussed a modeljob-application letter which had been presented to them the previous day:Text 1: Model job application letter11 Cotten AvenueKens<strong>in</strong>gtonNSW 20337th December 1989The Personnel OfficerElfex LtdHigh StreetNorth KydeNSW 2113Dear Sir or MadamHe: Receptionist’s JobI am writ<strong>in</strong>g for the job of receptionist advertised <strong>in</strong> The Sydney Morn<strong>in</strong>g Heraldtoday.I have worked as a receptionist for three years <strong>in</strong> a dentist’s consultancy and I amvery experienced <strong>in</strong> answer<strong>in</strong>g the telephone, writ<strong>in</strong>g letters and prepar<strong>in</strong>g accounts.I am 20 years old and I have my Higher School Certificate. I speak and write fluent<strong>English</strong> and Greek and consider myself a most suitable candidate for the job.I have enclosed a reference from my last job. Please contact me a t home on 3702915 any time <strong>in</strong> the even<strong>in</strong>g.Yours s<strong>in</strong>cerely(5 ig natu re)The teacher’s aim was to help the learners develop a metalanguage to describc theschematic structure of this text, which would assist them dur<strong>in</strong>g the jo<strong>in</strong>t constructionactivity which was to follow.The follow<strong>in</strong>g extract from the classroom illustrates how thiswas done:Classroom transcript 1T: All right, have a look at thc letter we wrote together yesterday. In fact I’m go<strong>in</strong>g toread it to you so that we can recall what we did. At the top right hand corner we putthe. . .?LL: Address . . . address. . . date.T: Address . . . OK and date.Then on the left underneath we put . . .?L: Who. . . and address.


204ANNE BURNST:L:T:L:T:L:T:LL:T:L:LL:T:L:T:L:LL:T:OK . . . to whom and the address.Then ‘Dear Sir or Madam’. Why did we put ‘Sir orMadam’?Because I don’t know man or woman.You don’t know if it is a man or a woman, ‘Re: Receptionist’s job’. What does ‘re’mean?About. . . about. . .‘I am writ<strong>in</strong>g to apply for the job of the reccptionist advertised <strong>in</strong>The Sydney Morn<strong>in</strong>gHerald today.’ So the first th<strong>in</strong>g you should say <strong>in</strong> the letter is what the letter is about.‘I’m writ<strong>in</strong>g to apply for the job. I have worked as a receptionist for three years <strong>in</strong> adentist’s consultancy and I am very experienced <strong>in</strong> answer<strong>in</strong>g the phone, writ<strong>in</strong>g lettersand prepar<strong>in</strong>g accounts. So, the second part, what is that . . .?Experience. . .Right. Two is the experience (writ<strong>in</strong>g on board next to number 2). What was one?What would you put for one?(Umntelligible)What is the first th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the letter? ‘I am writ<strong>in</strong>g . . .’?Address?No ... no.. .‘I am writ<strong>in</strong>g to apply . . .’What could we put there?The problem. . .Not a problem . . .No . . . <strong>in</strong>formationNo. . . about me. . .The ma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> the letter . . . OK? (writes on board next to number 1).As can be seen from this extract, the teacher builds up a description of the schematicstag<strong>in</strong>g of the text by elicit<strong>in</strong>g from the learncrs metal<strong>in</strong>guistic labels, which they can drawup to guide their own construction of a similar letter and to <strong>in</strong>crease their awareness of thetext as an object of study. Although the dcscriptions of the stag<strong>in</strong>g may not be very sophisticatedat this level of language proficiency, they provide a guid<strong>in</strong>g framework which is accessible tothe learners. The result was the follow<strong>in</strong>g description of the schematic structure:Text 2: Schematic structure of job application letter12345678910111213AddressDateWho to and addressDearRe (about)Ma<strong>in</strong> InformationExperienceAbout me (relevant to job)End<strong>in</strong>gReferenceContactYours s<strong>in</strong>cerelySignature.The teacher followcd this by discuss<strong>in</strong>g with the learners some of the dist<strong>in</strong>ctive features


5GENRE-BASED APPROACHES TO WRITING 205of the text, such as the predom<strong>in</strong>ant use of the present tense, the focus on an <strong>in</strong>dividualparticipant and the use of primarily ‘be<strong>in</strong>g’ and ‘hav<strong>in</strong>g’ clauses. At the end of this segment ofthe lesson, the learners were asked to construct their own letters <strong>in</strong> response toCommonwealth Employment Service (CES) advertisements, which had also been read anddiscussed <strong>in</strong> a previous lesson. Each group was given a sample advertisement (secText 3 below)and asked to choose a scribe who would record the text as it was produced.The account whichfollows described how one group of three learners went about construct<strong>in</strong>g their text.The group was composed of three female learners; Katia, who was Chilean; Zorka, whowasYugoslav; and Susanna, a Czech. All three were <strong>in</strong> their 20s and had post-high schooleducation, two hav<strong>in</strong>g been nurses and one a teacher <strong>in</strong> her own country.They had all <strong>in</strong>dicatedthat job-seek<strong>in</strong>g was a priority for them and were highly motivated to improve their languageskills so that they could eventually f<strong>in</strong>d employment. Susanna was nom<strong>in</strong>ated to scribe thejo<strong>in</strong>tly negotiated text and what she wrote was closely monitored by the two other membersof the group. The letter they wrote was <strong>in</strong> response to the follow<strong>in</strong>g advertisement:Text 3: CES job advertisementMechanicWoolloomoolooGeneral Repair Work on Jaguars8 a.m. ~ p.m.$Award ~ NegotiableAge: 25+Tradesman MechanicExp. On Jaguars6052-a/ 531 kcsMotor repairsDespite their limited proficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, the group employed a wide range ofstrategies dur<strong>in</strong>g the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction activity. The follow<strong>in</strong>g extract illustrates how theycollaborated to produce their text:Classroom transcript 2Su: I am writ<strong>in</strong>g to apply . . . (writ<strong>in</strong>g)Ka: Ofthe job . . .Zo: Excuse meSu: About the job . . .Zo: Of? Of the job?Su: AboutKa: About. . . about. . . of. . . of. . .Zo: No . . . for the . . . for the jobSu: For the job . . . (writ<strong>in</strong>g)zo: Of. . .Ka: Motor mechanic(Susanna writes, then reads aloud)Su: I am writ<strong>in</strong>g to apply for the job . . . (compares with model) <strong>in</strong> the CES today . . .Ka: In the service station . . . <strong>in</strong> the newspaper . . . Spanish . . . newspaper


206 ANNE BURNSSu: Oh (laughs) . . . (looks at job advertisement) . . . In the CES . . . C . . . (writes)Ka: (watches as Susanna writes) C . . . E . . . S . . .. This extract illustrates how the learners:referred to the modeltransferred language from the model to the situational context of their owntextoffered suggestions for construct<strong>in</strong>g thc textcollaborated to produce what they believed to be correct versions of the textmonitored what was be<strong>in</strong>g writtenused the model to check their version of thc textread aloud to 'try out' the text on each otherused each other as resources for writ<strong>in</strong>gexchanged cross-cultural <strong>in</strong>formation about the social context of the genre.The f<strong>in</strong>al version of the letter by this group of learners is reproduced below and isrepresentative of similar texts completed by other groups <strong>in</strong> the class dur<strong>in</strong>g this activity.Text 4: Jo<strong>in</strong>tly negotiated letter of application


~ (1990)~ (198GENRE-BASED APPROACHES TO WRITING 207It can be observed that the learners have drawn upon the model provided by the teacherto structure the text appropriately <strong>in</strong> terms of layout and overall presentation. In additionthey have used appropriate schematic stag<strong>in</strong>g, with the head<strong>in</strong>g and statement of generalpurpose com<strong>in</strong>g as an <strong>in</strong>troduction, followed by accounts of previous experience,qualifications and personal details.They have also written a suitable conclusion which refersto the reference and <strong>in</strong>cludes a contact number.Although these learners are at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g stage <strong>in</strong> their second-languagedevelopment, they have been able to produce a fairly effective text approximat<strong>in</strong>g the genre,‘letter of job application’. I believe that this was made possible because the approach takenhere, based on systemic l<strong>in</strong>guistics and the notions of genre and register, provides an explicitaccount of the schematic structure, organisation and language features of the genre uponwhich they were focus<strong>in</strong>g.Even at early stages <strong>in</strong> second-language learn<strong>in</strong>g, learners can, and must, be assisted tobeg<strong>in</strong> the process of acquir<strong>in</strong>g and extend<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g. As there is noreason to suppose that written language acquisition <strong>in</strong> a second language cannot bedevelopmental <strong>in</strong> the samc way that spoken language acquisition is generally agreed to be,it is vital that <strong>in</strong> a technologically oriented and highly literate society, adult learners aregiven <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> written language as early as possible and <strong>in</strong> a pr<strong>in</strong>cipled way. A genrebasedapproach provides them with learn<strong>in</strong>g activities presented with<strong>in</strong> a social contextualframework, which encourage them to focus on language and which assist them to becomemore <strong>in</strong>dependent and analytical learners.NoteI am grateful to the other participants <strong>in</strong> the NCELTR Literacy Project and <strong>in</strong> particular toJenny Hammond and Eileen Lustig for their advice and contributions to the writ<strong>in</strong>g of thispaper.ReferencesBr<strong>in</strong>dley, G. (1979) The Assessment of Speak<strong>in</strong>g Projciency Through the Oral Interview. Sydney:AMES.Callaghan, M. and Rothery, J. (1988) <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Factua1Writ<strong>in</strong>g:A Genre Based Approach. Report ofthe DSP Literacy Project, Metropolitan East Region. Sydney: NSW School EducationDepartment.Cope, W. (1989) A historical background to current curriculum changes and the shft to genre.Presentation at the First LERN Conference, Sydney.Halliday, M.A.K. (1985) An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Edward Arnold.Halliday, M.A.K. and Hasan, R. (1985) <strong>Language</strong>, context and text: Aspects oflanguage <strong>in</strong> a socialsemiotic perspective. Geelong, Victoria: Deak<strong>in</strong> University Press.Hammond, J. (1989) ‘The NCELTR Literacy Project.’ Prospect 5, 1: 23-30.Collaborat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Literacy <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and Research. Paper presented at 26th AnnualTESOL Convention, San Francisco.Mart<strong>in</strong>, J.R. (1985) Factual Writjng: Explor<strong>in</strong>g and Challeng<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Social</strong> Reality. Geelong, Victoria:Deak<strong>in</strong> University Press.Mart<strong>in</strong>, J.R. and Rothery, J. (1980) Writ<strong>in</strong>g Project, Report No. 1, Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics No.1. University of Sydney: Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.1) Writ<strong>in</strong>g Project, Report No. 2,Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics No. 2. University of Sydney:Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.


Chapter 13A. Suresh CanagarajahCRITICAL ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRILANKAN CLASSROOM: AMBIGUITIES INSTUDENT OPPOSITION TOREPRODUCTION THROUGH ESOLIntroductionH I S C H A PT E R A R G U E S T H AT T H E way <strong>in</strong> which dom<strong>in</strong>ation is experiencedT and oppositional tendencies are formed <strong>in</strong> classroom life has to be observed closelyrather than conceived abstractly.This ethnographic study of 22 tertiary-levelTamil studentsfollow<strong>in</strong>g a mandatory <strong>English</strong> for general purposes (EGP) course reveals that whereas thelived culture displays opposition to the alienat<strong>in</strong>g discourscs <strong>in</strong>scribed <strong>in</strong> a U.S. textbook,the students affirm <strong>in</strong> their more conscious statements before and after the course theirstrong motivation to study ESOL. Interpret<strong>in</strong>g this contradiction as reflect<strong>in</strong>g the conflictstudents face between cultural <strong>in</strong>tegrity, on the one hand, and socioeconomic mobility, onthe other, the study cxpla<strong>in</strong>s how students’ dcsire for learn<strong>in</strong>g only grammar <strong>in</strong> a productorientedmanner enables them to be somewhat detached from cultural alienation whilebe<strong>in</strong>g sufficiently exam<strong>in</strong>ation oriented to pass the course and fulfill a socioeconomicnecessity. However, this two-pronged strategy is an ideologically limit<strong>in</strong>g oppositionalbehavior that conta<strong>in</strong>s elements of accommodation as well as resistance and unwitt<strong>in</strong>glyleads students to participate <strong>in</strong> their own dom<strong>in</strong>ation.The recent <strong>in</strong>troduction of poststructuralist perspectives on language and radicaltheories of school<strong>in</strong>g that view language teach<strong>in</strong>g as a political act is a long-awaiteddevelopment <strong>in</strong>TESOL. Such theories enjoy much currency <strong>in</strong> L1 circles, almost becom<strong>in</strong>gthe orthodoxy <strong>in</strong> areas like composition teach<strong>in</strong>g, with words like discourse and empowermentbecom<strong>in</strong>g clichkd and pos<strong>in</strong>g the danger that they might have lost their critical edge.TESOL,on the other hand, while be<strong>in</strong>g a far more controversial activity, has managed to see <strong>its</strong>elfas safely “apolitical” due to <strong>its</strong> positivistic preoccupation with methods and techniques.In recent issues of the TESOL Quarterly, scholars such as Pcnnycook (1 989) and Pcirce(1 989) have deconstructetl dom<strong>in</strong>ant methods and the idea of method <strong>its</strong>elf <strong>in</strong> order toexpose the ideologies that <strong>in</strong>form TESOL. Though their papers perform a pioneer<strong>in</strong>gfunction, the force with which they are compelled to present their theses also <strong>in</strong>volves somesimplification. Whereas Penny cook’s del<strong>in</strong>eation of ideological dom<strong>in</strong>ation throughTESOLappears overdeterm<strong>in</strong>ed and pessimistic, Pcirce’s characteri/ation of the possibilities of


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


ofTESOL-related macrostructures, and only assume implications for language classroomsrather than report<strong>in</strong>g empirical observations of the classroom <strong>its</strong>elf for how dom<strong>in</strong>ation isexperienced and oppositional tendencies are formed there. We can understand the“ambiguous areas” (Giroux, 1983, p. 109) of student response, where a confus<strong>in</strong>g range ofaccommodative and oppositional tendencies are displayed, only if we take a closer look atthe day-to-day function<strong>in</strong>g of the classroom and the lived culture of the students. It is bydo<strong>in</strong>g so that we can atta<strong>in</strong> a realistic understand<strong>in</strong>g of the challenges as well as thepossibilities for a pedagogy of resistance <strong>in</strong> TESOL. The objective of this chapter is not tooutl<strong>in</strong>e one more pedagogy of resistance, but to <strong>in</strong>terrogate the range of behaviors studentsdisplay <strong>in</strong> the face of dom<strong>in</strong>ation ~ the awareness of which should precede and <strong>in</strong>form anydevelopment of such pedagogies. The ethnographic study below of an ESOL classroom <strong>in</strong>Sri Lanka creatively complicates the pcrspectivcs on dom<strong>in</strong>ation and resistance presentedby Pennycook and Peirce.<strong>Context</strong>ualiz<strong>in</strong>g the studyEver s<strong>in</strong>ce the British colonial power brought the whole island of (then) Ceylon under <strong>its</strong>control <strong>in</strong> 1796 and <strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>English</strong> education to create a supportive lower adm<strong>in</strong>istrativework force, <strong>English</strong> has functioned as a valued l<strong>in</strong>guistic capital over the local S<strong>in</strong>hala andTamil languages to provide socioeconomic advantages for native Lankans. Although s<strong>in</strong>ce1956 (8 years after <strong>in</strong>dependence), “leftist” governments have professed to raise the statusof S<strong>in</strong>hala (and, to a limited extent, Tamil), it is the <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g bil<strong>in</strong>guals who havedom<strong>in</strong>ated the professions and social hierarchy. On the other hand, the democratization orpopularization of <strong>English</strong> promised by “rightist” governments has only amounted toprovid<strong>in</strong>g limited mobility <strong>in</strong>to lower-middle-class rungs for aspirants whosc newly acquired<strong>English</strong> is marked as a nonprestige “sub-standard Sri Lankan <strong>English</strong>” (see Kandiah, 1979).These developments have historically disgruntled the monol<strong>in</strong>gual majority to make themperceive <strong>English</strong> as a double-edged weapon that frustrates both those who desire it as wellas those who neglect it (Kandiah, 1984). Similarly, <strong>in</strong> theTamil society, whereas the emergentmilitant nationalism has unleashed a Tamil-only and even “pure Tamil” movement, suchparallel developments as the exodus to the West or the cosmopolitan capital as economicand political refuges have bolstered <strong>English</strong> to assure the dom<strong>in</strong>ance of <strong>English</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gualsand to attract monol<strong>in</strong>guals.As for <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g, the teachers, adm<strong>in</strong>istrators, and general public <strong>in</strong>Sri Lanka agree that <strong>English</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g is a“colossa1 failure” (de Souza, 1969, p. 18)consider<strong>in</strong>g the vast resources expended on this enterprise by the state and Western culturalagencies. Though all identify the problem as one of student motivation, they differ as towhy students are unmotivated. Hanson-Smith (1984), a U.S. TESOL consultant, andGoonetilleke (1983), a local professor of <strong>English</strong>, fault the educational system. In theuniversity, for <strong>in</strong>stance, they perceive that the requirements for <strong>English</strong> are not str<strong>in</strong>gentenough to motivate students to take the subject as seriously as other subjects. Both, however,are <strong>in</strong> agreement that <strong>English</strong> does a world of good for Sri Lankan students: “<strong>English</strong> islearned not primarily to communicate with other Lankans . . . but to converse with theworld at large ~ and not just the world of technology and mach<strong>in</strong>es, but also of dreams,aspirations and idcals”(Hanson-Smith, 1984, p. 30). Because Kandiah (1984), on the otherhand, is of the view that the dreams encouraged by <strong>English</strong> are illusory (as <strong>English</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gdoes not challengc but <strong>in</strong> fact perpetuates <strong>in</strong>equality) and <strong>its</strong> ideals are suspected by studentsof result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> cultural derac<strong>in</strong>ation, he sees the problem of motivation differently: “[The]


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


212 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHI enjoyed natural access to the daily exercises and notes of the students and the record oftheir attendance without hav<strong>in</strong>g to foreground my role as researcher. As the teach<strong>in</strong>gprogressed, I stumblcd <strong>in</strong>to other naturalistic data that provided <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to students’ ownpo<strong>in</strong>t of view of the coursc, such as the comments students had scribbled dur<strong>in</strong>g class time<strong>in</strong> the marg<strong>in</strong>s of the textbook (which, due to frequent losses, was distributed before eachclass and collected at the end).To add a chronological dimension to the study, I situated the other methods of datacollection at significant po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the progression of the course. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the first week ofclasses, I conducted a free recall procedure, ask<strong>in</strong>g the students to jot down their impressionsof <strong>English</strong>. I also gave a detailed qucstionnaire cover<strong>in</strong>g their social and l<strong>in</strong>guistic backgroundto be completed at home. At the end of the course, but beforc their f<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ation,I conducted an oral <strong>in</strong>terview with the students <strong>in</strong> my office to analyze their responses tothe course, textbook, and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> general. Though I <strong>in</strong>vited the students for a15-m<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview, eventually each <strong>in</strong>terview ranged from 70 to 90 m<strong>in</strong>. Because somestudents preferred to converse with me <strong>in</strong> the company of another classmate, I permittedthem to meet me <strong>in</strong> pairs. Evcn then, 7 students, all females, failcd to turn up - probablyreflect<strong>in</strong>g the taboo on close <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relations between the sexes <strong>in</strong>Tamil society.The<strong>in</strong>terview, like the questionnaire, was <strong>in</strong> Tamil so that students could express themselvesfreely. (Such data is presented below, <strong>in</strong> translation, unless otherwise stated. The orig<strong>in</strong>alTamil is cited only when discursively significant.)The questionnaire and the <strong>in</strong>terview modules were constructed <strong>in</strong> such manner as toenable cross-check<strong>in</strong>g of students’ op<strong>in</strong>ions. In the questionnaire, the first part surveyedstudents’ educational backgrounds and exposure to <strong>English</strong>. The second part surveyed theeducational and socioeconomic background of the parents. The third part provided a set oftrue/false statcments to test morc obliquely students’ attitudcs toward the use of <strong>English</strong>.The f<strong>in</strong>al part conta<strong>in</strong>ed open-ended questions that further sampled their attitudes, allow<strong>in</strong>gcomparison of thcsc with their prcviou? statemcnts. Though the f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>terview wasprcstructured, I shifted topic freely accord<strong>in</strong>g to the flow of conversation. Questions 1-3queried the attitude of the students towards <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> relation to their other courses;Questions 4-7 checked their response to the organimtion and cultural content of thetextbook; 8 and 9 sampled the effects of <strong>English</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g on their th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and identity;10-1 2 <strong>in</strong>vited a critique of the pedagogy and curriculum; 13-1 5 explored their use of<strong>English</strong> outside the class; and 16-1 8 solicited their recommendations for the improvementof the course. Some of the similar questions <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview then enabled me to comparethe motivation and attitudes of the students with their op<strong>in</strong>ions stated <strong>in</strong> the questionnaire<strong>in</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the coursc. The other modes of data collection, too, enabled me toauthenticate the data more effectively through triangulation (see Denz<strong>in</strong>, 1970). For<strong>in</strong>stance, the lived culture of the students (as recorded <strong>in</strong> my field notes and students’comments <strong>in</strong> the textbook) was at odds with thcir stated op<strong>in</strong>ions <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview andqucstionnaire, compell<strong>in</strong>g me to reconstruct morc complex hypotheses to expla<strong>in</strong> thcirattitudes.The courseThe class that I observed consisted of 22 first-year students <strong>in</strong> the arts and humanities at theUniversity of Jaffna. The ESOL course is mandatory for all students of the faculty of arts.A pass is required <strong>in</strong> ESOL to qualify for admission to the second ycar. For eligibilityto specialize <strong>in</strong> a specific subject from the second year onwards, students are required toscore at least a B on thc ESOL exam <strong>in</strong> the first sitt<strong>in</strong>g. It is from the second year that <strong>English</strong>


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANKAN CLASSROOM 213teach<strong>in</strong>g is structured <strong>in</strong>to <strong>English</strong> for specific purposes (ESP), cater<strong>in</strong>g to the diffcrentsubject specialties. The first-year coursc is based on <strong>English</strong> for general purposes (EGP),provid<strong>in</strong>g practicc <strong>in</strong> all four skills.Because the course is structured around a core text, it is necessary to discuss theorganization of American Kernel Lessons (AKL): Intermediate (O’Ncill, K<strong>in</strong>gbury, Yeadon, andCornelius, 1978). We have to remember that such prepackaged material, which comes witha teachers’ manual, test<strong>in</strong>g lat, and audiotapes for listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension, represents “adirect assault on the traditional role of thc teacher as an <strong>in</strong>tellectual whose function is toconceptualize, design and implement learn<strong>in</strong>g experiences suited to the specificity and needsof a particular classroom cxperiencc” (Aronowitz and Giroux, 1985, p. 149). Althoughteachers <strong>in</strong> thc University of Jaffna realize these problems, the limitations of time, funds,stationery, and pr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g facilities <strong>in</strong> war-torn Jaffna eventually drive thcm to use tcxts such asAKL which have been amply gifted by Western agencies such as the Asia Foundation. If exist<strong>in</strong>gbooks become dated, teachers have to simply wait for the next consignment of material.As thc title implics, the text is targeted towards <strong>in</strong>termediatc-level students and focuseson the tenses, us<strong>in</strong>g eclectic methods organized around a predom<strong>in</strong>antly situational approach(see Richards and Rodgcrs, 1986). Each unit conta<strong>in</strong>s five parts. Part A <strong>in</strong>troduces thegrammatical item for that unit through a set of“situations,” accompanied by visuals. Part B,labeled Formation and Manipulation, <strong>in</strong>troduces the grammatical item more overtly andprovides pattern practice. Part C is a serialized detective story that <strong>in</strong>troduces newvocabulary <strong>in</strong> addition to provid<strong>in</strong>g practice <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g/listen<strong>in</strong>g comprehension. Part Dpresents a conversation for role play<strong>in</strong>g, whereas the f<strong>in</strong>al part conta<strong>in</strong>s guided composition.The last two parts also provide grammar revision exercises. Though grammar is presentedovcrtly <strong>in</strong> some sections, <strong>in</strong> most others, students are encouraged to formulatc their ownhypotheses <strong>in</strong>ductively through activc use of the language <strong>in</strong> specific skills.It is also necessary to analyze the ideologies that structure the text <strong>in</strong> order to place <strong>in</strong>context the attitudes and responscs of the students to the course. What stands out <strong>in</strong> thenote, “To the Student andTeacher,”<strong>in</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the text is the concern with provid<strong>in</strong>gadequate “practice” so that studcnts will “progress” <strong>in</strong> the “fundamentals of <strong>English</strong>” which<strong>in</strong>termediate students “still cannot seem to use correctly, easily and as automatically as theywould like” (O’Neill et a/. , 1978, p. vi). The language echoes behaviorism and assumes thatwith sufficient drill, students can be made to display habit-oriented automatic responses.Furthermore, the fundamcntals of <strong>English</strong> are considered autonomous, value-frecgrammatical structurcs (<strong>in</strong> thc fashion of U. S. structuralism), ignor<strong>in</strong>g the culture andideologies that <strong>in</strong>form the language or the textbook. Thc students themselves are isolatedfrom their social contcxt, and there is no consitlcration of how their own l<strong>in</strong>guistic andcultural backgrounds can affect or enhance their Icarn<strong>in</strong>g. In <strong>its</strong> concern with correctness(which, of coursc, is based on standard U.S. <strong>English</strong> rather than on the <strong>English</strong>es studentsbr<strong>in</strong>g with them), the textbook empowers the teacher as the sole authority <strong>in</strong> the classroomto regulate, discipl<strong>in</strong>e, and arbitrate the learn<strong>in</strong>g process. Such assumptions amount to whatGiroux (1 983) has identified as <strong>in</strong>strumental ideology (p. 209). Though AKL acknowledgesthe need to make learn<strong>in</strong>g an “enjoyable experience” and also provides opportunities forcollaborative pair work, these attempts provide only occasional relief from the largelypositivistic pedagogy.In fairncss to AKL, we have to note that certa<strong>in</strong> scctions are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the notion ofcommunicative compctcnce with advice to students that “thc situations themsclves arc mor?important than isolated words” (O’Neill et a/., 1978, p. v). However, the <strong>in</strong>teractions andthe discourse employed <strong>in</strong> such situations assume an urbanized, technocratic, Westernculture that is alien to the students. Even such simple spcech activities as conversations arc


214 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHconducted <strong>in</strong> a strictly goal-oriented manner (see Unit 2d), whereasTamil discourse valuesthe “digression” and <strong>in</strong>direction typical of oral communities.The values that emerge throughthe situations arc not hard to decipher, such as upward social mobility and consumerism(4d).The work ethic (1 2a) and rout<strong>in</strong>e of factory life (1 3a) are prcscnted positively, whereasstrikes and demonstrations (Sa) and the lifestyle of blacks (<strong>in</strong> the story of Jane and herboyfriends) are not. The potential of the textbook to <strong>in</strong>fluence students with certa<strong>in</strong>dom<strong>in</strong>ant values of U.S. society is subtly effective because AKZ disarms <strong>its</strong> users bypresent<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>g as a value-free, <strong>in</strong>strumental activity.The classThe class consisted of 13 female and 9 male nativeTamil students, of whom 3 were RomanCatholics and the rest H<strong>in</strong>dus. These students had failed the <strong>in</strong>itial placement test <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>and fared among the worst among the new entrants for that academic year. They wcrcenrolled <strong>in</strong> a range of subjects related to the humanities and social sciences besides themandatory ESOL. A majority of these students were from rural communities and from thepoorest economic groups. Except for 4 students whose parents were <strong>in</strong> clerical or teach<strong>in</strong>gprofessions (thus earn<strong>in</strong>g the relatively decent sum of 1000 rupees, or USS2S a month!),the other parents did not have steady jobs or salaries. In the latter group, some were tenantfarmers, and others wcrc seasonal casual laborcrs.The families of the students had also hadlimited education. Only one student’s parents had proceeded beyond Grade 1 O.The parentsof 5 others had not completed an elementary school education.Furthermore, the students came from backgrounds <strong>in</strong> which <strong>English</strong> held limitedcurrency. Only 8 students said their parents had managcd to study some elementary <strong>English</strong><strong>in</strong> school. Of these, 3 reported that their parents might listen to <strong>English</strong> programs on themultil<strong>in</strong>gual television or radio. Five reported that their parents could be expected to uttersome <strong>English</strong> words if they encountered foreigners or if need arose <strong>in</strong> their workplace. Noneof them could read or write <strong>English</strong>. Consider<strong>in</strong>g the students themselves, although 18 hadsat for the Grade 10 <strong>English</strong> language test, only 10 had managed to score a simple pass (i.e.,a grade of 40%). Three students reported that they had read <strong>English</strong> newspapershooks orseen <strong>English</strong> films ~ although they could not remember the titles of any. Fourteen reportedthat they might occasionally switch on some <strong>English</strong> programs on radio or television. Thesame number said they might code-mix <strong>English</strong> with friends or when they needed a l<strong>in</strong>klanguage.<strong>Context</strong>ualiz<strong>in</strong>g classroom lifePrecourse determ<strong>in</strong>ationWhen the university reopened belatedly for the academic year, it was after much doubt asto whether it would cont<strong>in</strong>ue to function at all because renewed hostilities between theS<strong>in</strong>hala govcrnmcnt andTamil nationalists had brought life to a standstill <strong>in</strong> theTamil region.Yet students trickled <strong>in</strong> from jungles where they had taken refuge from the fight<strong>in</strong>g ~ <strong>in</strong>some cases, trckk<strong>in</strong>g hundreds of miles by foot. In a country where only a small percentageof all those who annually qualify for tertiary education do get admission, the students valuedtheir university degrees sufficiently to turn up for classes. As a grim rem<strong>in</strong>der of the violenceand tension that would cont<strong>in</strong>ue to loom beh<strong>in</strong>d thcir studies, government fighter jetsscreamed overhead and bombed the vic<strong>in</strong>ity ofthc university while the students were tak<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>English</strong> placement test dur<strong>in</strong>g the open<strong>in</strong>g week of classes.


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


216 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHtranscended the specific cultures and ideologies of different nations. So Gnani stated,“Although it is the language of a particular nation, it is a common language for all peopleand nations.”Although the relatively more spontaneous impressions of the students <strong>in</strong> the free recallprocedure largely confirm their positive attitudes toward <strong>English</strong>, they are also t<strong>in</strong>ged withfears and <strong>in</strong>hibitions. Hence, though a majority of the students associated <strong>English</strong> withdevelopment, progress, learn<strong>in</strong>g, civil<strong>in</strong>tion, literacy, culture, social respect, andpersonality, one can also detect other comments which suggest that students are not unawareof the sociopsychological damage anti politics of the language. Shanthi wrote:British mother tongue. We were forced to study it because of colonialism. If we havea knowledge of this language we can live <strong>in</strong> whichever country we want. Br<strong>in</strong>gs tom<strong>in</strong>d the developed life of the white people. A language that everybody should know.Though conflict<strong>in</strong>g impressions are mixed <strong>in</strong> Shanthi’s stream of consciousness, whatis remarkable is that she rema<strong>in</strong>s detached from the negative features and fails to take aperspective on them. The fact that students are probably consciously rationaliz<strong>in</strong>g their fearsor suppress<strong>in</strong>g their <strong>in</strong>hibitions is evident from Ratnam’s comments. He argued, “S<strong>in</strong>ce thedom<strong>in</strong>ance of <strong>English</strong> is uncontestable, the best strategy is to exploit <strong>its</strong> resources to developour own language and culture.”Midcourse resistanceThe <strong>in</strong>hibitions towards <strong>English</strong> which lay partly suppressed dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>itial period of thecourse <strong>in</strong> the conscious responses of the students, came <strong>in</strong>to relief <strong>in</strong> their largelyunconscious lived culture as the course proceeded. It is evident from the record of dailyattendance that students faced problems <strong>in</strong> the course. Although students recordcd animpressive 94% daily turn out for most of the first 2 months, at the end of the second month,attendance fell to 50%. Students began to miss classes for the slightest reason: to writetutorials for another subject, to prepare for a test, to attend funerals of friends’ relatives.At times <strong>in</strong>tense fight<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the district or the imposition of curfew also affected attendance.Rut none of this deterred 90% of the students from attend<strong>in</strong>g from the eighth month as thef<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ation was approach<strong>in</strong>g, demand<strong>in</strong>g that past test papers be done and revisionundertaken.The comments, draw<strong>in</strong>gs, and pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>gs students had penned <strong>in</strong> the textbook are moresubtle evidence of the flagg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest of students. Because students had written thesedur<strong>in</strong>g class time, this activity suggested that topics other than <strong>English</strong> grammar hadpreoccupied them while teach<strong>in</strong>g was go<strong>in</strong>g on. Although students had appeared to bepassively observ<strong>in</strong>g or listen<strong>in</strong>g to the teacher, as required by the <strong>in</strong>strumental pedagogy <strong>in</strong>the class, the glosses <strong>in</strong> the text suggest a very active underlife. Unknown to the teacher,students were communicat<strong>in</strong>g with each other or sometimes with themselves through theseglosses. The glosses suggest the discourses and themes that seem to have <strong>in</strong>terested thestudents more than those <strong>in</strong> the textbook. In one sense, these are the discourses whichmediate for the students the situations, grammar, and language taught by the textbook. Inanother sense, these are students’ countertiiscourses that challenge the textual language,values, and ideology. Hence, they deserve close exam<strong>in</strong>ation.Many of the glosses arc <strong>in</strong>spired by the ongo<strong>in</strong>g nationalist struggle for a separateTamilstate. For this reason, <strong>in</strong> Unit IC, the picture of Fletcher (the protagonist <strong>in</strong> the detectivestory) as he is seated <strong>in</strong> a prison cell is modified <strong>in</strong> a couple of textbooks. He has been pa<strong>in</strong>ted


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


218 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAH(c)(d)(e)(f)(g)(h)(i)(j)Teacher: Army? What makes you say that?Shanthi: He is wear<strong>in</strong>g a uniform.Teacher: Well. . . Indran?Indran: He is <strong>in</strong> the hospital. . . . He is seated on a bed.Teacher: But what about the bars? . . . Don’t you see the bars? He is actually <strong>in</strong>prison.Shanthi: Okay, but he is wear<strong>in</strong>g good clothes. He is wear<strong>in</strong>g shoes.Indran: And he is said to bc go<strong>in</strong>g to the library and hav<strong>in</strong>g regular meals. . . . Andhe is seated alone <strong>in</strong> the room.Teacher: (Expla<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> detail the difference between prison life <strong>in</strong> Sri Lanka and theUS.)The students’ image of prison life as overcrowded, dirty, and more repressive (basedon Sri Lankan conditions) <strong>in</strong>terferes with their <strong>in</strong>terpretation. The other situations visuallyrepresented, such as an orchestra play<strong>in</strong>g, air travel, department store shopp<strong>in</strong>g, andapartment liv<strong>in</strong>g, also confused the students. Such cultural estrangement created anadditional layer of problems to the l<strong>in</strong>guistic ones students were already confronted with.Other tensions <strong>in</strong> the course resulted from the styles of learn<strong>in</strong>g desired by the students.The students seemed uncomfortable with a collaborative approach to learn<strong>in</strong>g whenever itwas encouraged. Because the textbook specified pairwork occasionally, and I myself wantedto create more l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>teraction among students, I <strong>in</strong>sisted that the desks be arranged <strong>in</strong>a circle. But before each class, the students rearranged the desks <strong>in</strong>to a traditional lecturcroomformat, with the teacher’s desk <strong>in</strong> front of the room and their own <strong>in</strong> hori7ontal rows.Thus, students m<strong>in</strong>imi7ed <strong>in</strong>teraction among themselves and failed to take <strong>in</strong>itiative <strong>in</strong> theflow of classroom discourse. As the conversation cited above suggests, typical <strong>in</strong>teractionsfollow the features of traditional teacher-centered classroom discourse (see Mehan, 1985;Stubbs, 1976), <strong>in</strong> which the teacher regulates and dom<strong>in</strong>ates talk. Turn tak<strong>in</strong>g follows thetripartite Structure of Question (see Turn a above), Answer (Turn b), and Evaluation(Turn c); such sequences follow <strong>in</strong> c-tlx, e-f-g.Turns for students arc assigned by the teacher(seeTurns a and e); for each s<strong>in</strong>gle turn by the student, the teacher takes two, thus dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>gthe quantity of talk. The questions asked are display questions for which the teacher alreadyknows the answer. In a quite atypical move here, Shanthi and Indran attempt to contradictthe teacher’s explanation; significantly, these were not framed as questions but simply as casualasides. It was only Supendran who asked for clarifications or challenged my explanations moreexplicitly. For most of the time, the rest preferred to sit, pen <strong>in</strong> hand, and write down whateverwas on the board or simply listen to the teacher’s lccture (as <strong>in</strong>Turn j). Ironically, one of theglosses above an <strong>in</strong>teractive pair-work exercise said, “This is a job for the jobless.”Accompany<strong>in</strong>g this desire for teacher-centered learn<strong>in</strong>g, students made learn<strong>in</strong>g aproduct rather than process. Students expected to be provided with the abstract forms andrules of language deductively or prescriptively for them to store <strong>in</strong> memory rather than to<strong>in</strong>ductively formulate the rules for themselves through active use of the language <strong>in</strong>communicative <strong>in</strong>teractions. Disregard<strong>in</strong>g activities, students demanded notes. Whenevercharts or grammatical paradigms were presented, the students eagerly wrote them down.They demanded more written work rather than speech or listen<strong>in</strong>g exercises because theyfelt that they could reta<strong>in</strong> it for personal study and revision before tests. My diary recordsmuch time taken <strong>in</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>g the importance of“use rather than rules.”But the slogan failedto create changes <strong>in</strong> their attitude. Gradually students noted my practice of reserv<strong>in</strong>g the2-hr classes for activities and 1 -hr slots for the more overtly grammar-oriented sectionsof the textbook and attended the latter while cutt<strong>in</strong>g the former.


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANKAN CLASSROOM 219Students also resisted the active use of <strong>English</strong> as a medium for <strong>in</strong>struction or <strong>in</strong>teraction<strong>in</strong> the classroom. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the first week when I asked students to <strong>in</strong>troduce themselves <strong>in</strong><strong>English</strong> by mak<strong>in</strong>g use of simple syntactic structures 1 had written on the board, they simplygiggled and found it embarrass<strong>in</strong>g to do so. Students responded <strong>in</strong>Tamil even though I used<strong>English</strong> for questions, commands, and explanations, whether <strong>in</strong> formal or <strong>in</strong>formalsituations. Thiru displayed the most paralyz<strong>in</strong>g sense of <strong>in</strong>hibition. It was simply impossiblefor him to produce a s<strong>in</strong>gle word of <strong>English</strong> from the textbook or by himself. The longmoments of silence would become embarrass<strong>in</strong>g as the class waited patiently for Thiru toopen his mouth when his turn came to do an exercise or read a passage orally. AlthoughThiru was very voluble <strong>in</strong> class <strong>in</strong> Tamil about matters related to university policies andregulations, <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> he was simply tongue-tied.Much of the stress seemed to result from the implications of <strong>English</strong> for the identityand group solidarity of the students. A particularly try<strong>in</strong>g time was the correction ofpronunciation as required by the textbook. BecauseTamil lacks syllable-<strong>in</strong>itial fricatives, thestudents pronounced he and she as /ki/ and /si/. The discomfort of the students <strong>in</strong> myrepeated attempts to correct such pronunciation was expla<strong>in</strong>ed by their later commentsthat revealed their awareness of such pronunciation be<strong>in</strong>g identified as “nonstandard” SriLankan <strong>English</strong>. These students had been the target of <strong>in</strong>sults by middle-class speakers of“educated” Sri Lankan <strong>English</strong>. Not only pronunciation but the very language was a classmarker. Supendran said that he simply avoided contexts <strong>in</strong> which students (from “betterbackgrounds”) used <strong>English</strong> with him because he felt that they were flaunt<strong>in</strong>g theirknowledge of the language <strong>in</strong> order to make him look ignorant. <strong>English</strong> then providedunfavorable subject positions to such students, mak<strong>in</strong>g them feel disadvantaged, helpless,<strong>in</strong>ferior, and uneducated. Students also felt that the use of <strong>English</strong> for <strong>in</strong>teractions wouldbe <strong>in</strong>terpreted by their peers as an attempt to discard their local rural identity and pass offas an anglicized bourgeois or even a foreigner. It was probably for this reason that <strong>in</strong> thequestionnaire, although 50% stated that they would use <strong>English</strong> “with a foreigner who alsoknew Tamil,” all except one rejected the possibility of us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> “with aTamil who alsoknew <strong>English</strong>.”The conflicts <strong>English</strong> created for the representation of their identity become moreexplicit <strong>in</strong> the conversation pieccs students had to role-play <strong>in</strong> each unit. Students typicallyuttered their parts <strong>in</strong> a flat read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tonation when they were asked to dramatize thedialogue <strong>in</strong> front of the class. My model renditions with an eye for realism only <strong>in</strong>creasedtheir <strong>in</strong>hibition. Students said that it was “funny” or “unbecom<strong>in</strong>g of themselves” to speak<strong>in</strong> such manner. It soon became apparent that the discourse beh<strong>in</strong>d these dialogues was <strong>its</strong>elfso alien to these students that they had difficulty enter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to the roles specified. One suchconversation was between Joe and Susan <strong>in</strong> Unit 4d while they budgeted their weeklyexpenses: Joe’s casual remark that he has to hold a party soon for 35 people <strong>in</strong> his office tocelebrate h s promotion irks Susan because of <strong>in</strong>sufficient notice and the amount of additionalexpenses <strong>in</strong>volved when they have just purchased a new house. When, as usual, studentsfound it difficult to imag<strong>in</strong>atively enter <strong>in</strong>to the situation, I tried to construct local situationswhere such dialogue could be expected to occur. Students however po<strong>in</strong>ted out that thegenre of “money talk” or “budget<strong>in</strong>g conversation” was alien to their peasant background.“We spend as we earn,”accord<strong>in</strong>g to one student, was their lifestyle. Even the consumerism,thrift, delayed gratification, and drive for social mobility assumed by the conversation turnedout to be alien. It was not surpris<strong>in</strong>g then that such role-play<strong>in</strong>g exercises were purely ofacademic <strong>in</strong>terest to them and, therefore, noth<strong>in</strong>g better could be employed for these otherthan the read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tonation for descriptive prose. Indran’s notes <strong>in</strong> his notebook at the endof the class were a tell<strong>in</strong>g comment on his attitude to the exercise. He had simply jotted


220 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHdownTamil synonyms for new lexical items like add<strong>in</strong>g, tradition, and promotion and identifiedsome examples of count/noncount structures which the unit was supposed to teach: “Howmany employees are at the bank? How much money did you spend last week?” Indran hadsimply filtered out the necessary grammatical and vocabulary items from the supposedly<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g conversation.What the lived culture of the students suggests is a dual oppositional trend. On the onehand, they oppose the alien discourses beh<strong>in</strong>d the language and textbook. On the otherhand, they oppose a process-oriented pedagogy and desire a product oriented one. Indran’snotebook suggests that both trends could be connected: See<strong>in</strong>g little possibility of relat<strong>in</strong>gwhat they learned to their sociocultural background, students saw little mean<strong>in</strong>g for thecourse other than the formal, academic one of act<strong>in</strong>g through the exam<strong>in</strong>ation and satisfy<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>English</strong> requirements of the <strong>in</strong>stitution.Postcourse contradictionAlthough the f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>terview with the students solicit<strong>in</strong>g their own impressions of the contentand organization of the course confirmed some of the observations on their lived culture,it also contradicted many f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs - at least at face value. Asked which subjects they hadenjoyed most and which they had worked hardest <strong>in</strong>, students mentioned their differentsubjects of specialization for the former but unanimously cited <strong>English</strong> for the latter. WhenI po<strong>in</strong>ted out the flagg<strong>in</strong>g attendance <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> and Contradicted their claim, I wasconfronted with a surpris<strong>in</strong>g piece of evidence.The majority of the students <strong>in</strong> the class hadbeen go<strong>in</strong>g for private <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> outside the university. As lndran put it conclusively,“For no other subject <strong>in</strong> the University do we go for tutor<strong>in</strong>g, thus spend<strong>in</strong>g additionaltime and money on it. The fact that we do this only for <strong>English</strong> proves our motivation tomaster the languagc.”The students cont<strong>in</strong>ued to affirm, as they had done at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the course, thc nced for <strong>English</strong> and the priority they had given to it.The admission that students had sought help outside the class was potentially an<strong>in</strong>dictment of the university ESOL course. 1 then began explor<strong>in</strong>g what it was that thestudents were gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their private <strong>in</strong>struction that they were not gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the university.It appeared that the tutors were us<strong>in</strong>g Sri Lankan or Indian textbooks - if they used any atall. But it was not the cultural relevance that students seemed to value <strong>in</strong> thec;e courses asmuch as the grammar <strong>in</strong>struction. In fact, the texts and pedagogy were overtly grammaroriented and were rarely contextuali7ed. Tharma praised his tutor (us<strong>in</strong>g lexical borrow<strong>in</strong>gsfrom <strong>English</strong>): “He ‘cleared’ the ‘grammar.”’Other questions <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview confirm the desire of the students for grammaroriented<strong>in</strong>struction. When asked which section of the textbook they had enjoyed and whichthey had found useful (1 3 out of the 15 <strong>in</strong>terviewed) replied that they found the grammartables and exercises (Sections b and e) useful although they had variously enjoyed theserialized story, conversation, and listen<strong>in</strong>g sections. Some conflated these dist<strong>in</strong>ctions:Jeyanthi said that she enjoyed the grammar section “because it is useful for the test.”Statements such as Jayanthi’s revealed that the desire of the students to learn the rules ofgrammar prescriptively was related to an exam<strong>in</strong>ation-oriented motivation. In fact, thef<strong>in</strong>al 3-hr written test featured mody discrete-item questions on formal aspects. Later,asked specifically what the students had <strong>in</strong>itially hoped to achieve through this course andthe extent to which the course had fulfilled their expectations, Siva said, ‘‘I expected thatthe course would prepare me for the test. . . that is, cover the necessary grammarcomprehensively.” It was not surpris<strong>in</strong>g, then, when all eventually agreed that the coursehad failed to satisfy their expectations.


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


222 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHearly OS), male, “progressive,” Christian, culturally Westerniied, middle class, nativeTamil,bil<strong>in</strong>gual, director of <strong>English</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g at the university are the identities that I believe weremost salient for the students. So students’ <strong>in</strong>sistence on the use ofTamil <strong>in</strong> the classroom,for example, is motivated by my be<strong>in</strong>g a bil<strong>in</strong>gualTamil. If there had been a native-<strong>English</strong>speak<strong>in</strong>gteacher, students would have been compelled to use <strong>English</strong>. Additionally, use of<strong>English</strong> with me would havc been perceived to violate our Tamil <strong>in</strong>-group solidarity.(However, my class and cultural identities separate me from the rural poor and would likelyhave <strong>in</strong>creased students’ <strong>in</strong>hibitions <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g their marked <strong>English</strong>.) Our common Tamilidentity would likely have also forced students to sound more nationalistic, especially as thepresent communalist mood tends not to tolerate neutrality. In this context, however, theiraffirmation of <strong>English</strong> is dar<strong>in</strong>g. On the other hand, because I was <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>stitutionallypowerful role, <strong>in</strong>stances of opposition to <strong>English</strong> (as their fall<strong>in</strong>g attendance) are significant.The same identity, however, would have motivated students to affirm the language, textbook,and the course. (In a sense, then, my multiple subject positions seem to qualify each other.)Although the uniqueness of each teacher/researcher-student <strong>in</strong>teraction should not beslighted <strong>in</strong> favor of the gcncralizability of this study, we have to note that almost all SriLankan ESOL teachers are Westernized, middlc-class, bil<strong>in</strong>gual, native Lankans like me.<strong>Context</strong>ualiz<strong>in</strong>g student oppositionAt face value, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the study seem <strong>in</strong>conclusive, if not contradictory. On the onehand, students seemed to gradually lose motivation <strong>in</strong> the course, as it was most objectivelydisplayed <strong>in</strong> their record of attcndance.There is reason to believc that this drop <strong>in</strong> motivationwas related to an oppositional response to the threats posed by the discourse <strong>in</strong>scribed <strong>in</strong>the language, pedagogy, and the textbook. At the very least, students were experienc<strong>in</strong>g atension or discomfort <strong>in</strong> the confrontation between the discoursc they preferred and thediscourses <strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g the ESOL course. But, on the other hand, students <strong>in</strong>sisted that theyworked hardest <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> compared to all the other subjects (which is true because theyhad been attend<strong>in</strong>g private classes as well). They ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed, as they did <strong>in</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the course, the importance of <strong>English</strong> and the high priority given to learn<strong>in</strong>g the language.They went further to <strong>in</strong>sist that they enjoyed learn<strong>in</strong>g Western culture and us<strong>in</strong>g the U.S.textbook (although they did not f<strong>in</strong>d them useful from the exam<strong>in</strong>ation po<strong>in</strong>t of view). Ingeneral, the oppositional attitude was manifested <strong>in</strong> the largely unrcflected, untheorizedlived culture of the students emerg<strong>in</strong>g from their glosses <strong>in</strong> the textbooks and my field notes;the receptive attitude emerges from the more conscious expression of their views <strong>in</strong> thequestionnaires and <strong>in</strong>terviews.As a way of reconcil<strong>in</strong>g this tension, we have several options: We can suppress one setof data <strong>in</strong> favor of the other; we can judge the students as confused and contradict<strong>in</strong>gthemselves; or we can simply fault the methodology. Not see<strong>in</strong>g valid reasons to do any ofthis, 1 f<strong>in</strong>d it challeng<strong>in</strong>g to preserve both sets of data and consider how both attitudesof the students display a complex response to the learn<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong>. It appears that thesedual attitudes simply dramatize the conflict students faced <strong>in</strong> the course between the threatsof cultural alienation experienced <strong>in</strong>tuitively or <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctively and the promises of asocioeconomic necessity acknowledged at a more conscious level. The students experienceddiscomfort <strong>in</strong> the face of the alien discourses, although they do not theorize about it. Butthis experience has to be juxtaposed with their awareness of the powerful discourses whichglorify the role of <strong>English</strong> (such as those of policymakers Goonctilleke, 1983, and Hanson-Smith, 1984), the pressure from the educational system to display proficiency <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>,


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANKAN CLASSROOM 223the promise of social and economic advancement <strong>English</strong> holds, and (especially for Tamilstudents today) thc uses of <strong>English</strong> as a buffer aga<strong>in</strong>st S<strong>in</strong>hala nationalism and passport forexodus as political or economic refugees abroad.The grammar-based, product-oriented learn<strong>in</strong>g which students alternatively desired(as exemplified <strong>in</strong> the lived culture as well as their statements) is one way for them toreconcile this conflict. That is, grammar learn<strong>in</strong>g enabled the students to be detached fromthe language and the course, avoid active use of the language which could <strong>in</strong>volve<strong>in</strong>ternalization of <strong>its</strong> discourses, and thereby cont<strong>in</strong>ue their opposition to the reproductivetendencies of the course. At the same time, this strategy enabled them to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> them<strong>in</strong>imal contact necessary with the language <strong>in</strong> order to acquire the rules of grammar --which <strong>in</strong> their view was the most efficient preparation for gett<strong>in</strong>g through the exam<strong>in</strong>ation.This strategy while enabl<strong>in</strong>g them to preserve their cultural <strong>in</strong>tegrity (however tenuously)also enabled them to accommodate the <strong>in</strong>stitutional requirement of hav<strong>in</strong>g to pass <strong>English</strong>and thus bid for the socioeconomic advantages associated with the language.Although not<strong>in</strong>g that grammar learn<strong>in</strong>g functions as a possible strategy to negotiate theconflicts studcnts face <strong>in</strong> the ESOL classroom, we have to realize that there are significanthistorical and cultural reasons which motivate them to adopt this strategy. The populardemand for grammar among all Sri Lankan university students is attested to by thechairperson for <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Centres <strong>in</strong> the country (R. Raheem, personalcommunication, September 28th, 1991). Students’ desire to be simply given the abstractrules of the language by the teacher could be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by traditional styles of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>Tamil society (or, for that matter, Sri Lankan society), which have been largely productoriented and teacher centered. Although it is hard to generali~e about the different<strong>in</strong>stitutions of learn<strong>in</strong>g that have existed historically (such as th<strong>in</strong>nai, or “house front,” andtemple schools), it can be said that typically the teacher (always male) passed on his stockof received knowledge orally to the disciple at his feet (see Jeyasuriya, no date; Sirisena,1969; Somasegaram, 1969). The disciples had to cultivate the art of listen<strong>in</strong>g meditativelyand memoriz<strong>in</strong>g accurately the huge stock of <strong>in</strong>formation to be preserved withoutcorruption.Thc reverence paid to the guru, as to the knowledge he transmitted, was almostreligious <strong>in</strong> character. This tradition is directly <strong>in</strong>herited by private <strong>in</strong>stitutes <strong>in</strong>contemporary Tamil society, enjoy<strong>in</strong>g immense popularity among parents and students (andpitted by my own students as a corrective to the university ESOL course), which <strong>in</strong>tensivelyprepare passive students for competitive exam<strong>in</strong>ations.Moreover, traditional descriptions of language and pedagogies of language teach<strong>in</strong>gdisplay a penchant for prescriptive, deductive, and formalistic methods. Although thewell-known Dravidian scholar Emeneau (1 955) outl<strong>in</strong>es the fundamental <strong>in</strong>fluence ofH<strong>in</strong>du l<strong>in</strong>guistic tradition on Western descriptive l<strong>in</strong>guistics, he also notes: “Intellectualthoroughness and an urge toward ratioc<strong>in</strong>ation, <strong>in</strong>tellection, and learned classification fortheir own sakes should surely be recognized as characteristic of the H<strong>in</strong>du higher culture.. . . They become grammarians, it would seem, for grammar’s sake” (pp. 145-146).Similarly, as late as the colonial period, the teach<strong>in</strong>g of local languages to Europeanadm<strong>in</strong>istrators was primarily based on study<strong>in</strong>g and memoriz<strong>in</strong>g learned grammaticaltreatises (see Wickramasuriya, 198 1).Anthropological approaches based on a narrowly conceived egalitarianism wouldencourage us to fashion a method of language teach<strong>in</strong>g that resembles the native traditionof a community (see, e.g., a description of the KEEP project <strong>in</strong> Watson-Gegeo, 1988).However, the grammar focused tradition of Tamils - which resembles the now disreputedgrammar-translation method <strong>in</strong>TESOL - drives to a reductio ad absurdum such attempts.Critical ethnography would posit that native learn<strong>in</strong>g traditions have to be <strong>in</strong>terrogated for


224 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHthe <strong>in</strong>terests they serve because m<strong>in</strong>ority culturcs are steeped <strong>in</strong> traditions of dom<strong>in</strong>ationas well as resistance. Without delv<strong>in</strong>g too much <strong>in</strong>to how this favored pedagogy ofTamilstraditionally bolstered their caste structure and religious hierarchy, we can proceed to <strong>its</strong>contemporary implications for the students discussed <strong>in</strong> this study. We must remember thatsuch a pedagogy encourages a teacher-controlled, nondialogic, “bank<strong>in</strong>g” style of learn<strong>in</strong>gthat is known to reproduce the dom<strong>in</strong>ant values and social relations of an oppressivelystratified society (see Freire, 1970; Giroux, 1983).Furthermore, though a formalistic approach to the abstract rules of “standard <strong>English</strong>”might appear to preserve students from the more obvious cultural content associated withthe communicative orientation of the course, it <strong>in</strong> no way saves them from other forms ofdom<strong>in</strong>ation: It disconfirms the <strong>English</strong>es students br<strong>in</strong>g with them; it prevents students from<strong>in</strong>terrogat<strong>in</strong>g their own culture and socicty through literacy; it fails to alter the unfavorablesubject positions belong<strong>in</strong>g to monol<strong>in</strong>gual and <strong>English</strong>-<strong>in</strong>competent Lankans. Nor doesthe formalistic approach enable students to effectively <strong>in</strong>ternalize the rules of the languageor progress rapidly <strong>in</strong> fluent language use. In the <strong>in</strong>-course assessments carried out tomonitor their progress, the majority of the students cont<strong>in</strong>ued to score below the pass<strong>in</strong>ggrade. They rema<strong>in</strong>ed with the smatter<strong>in</strong>g of “marked” <strong>English</strong> they brought with them.What all this implies is that these students will cont<strong>in</strong>ue to occupy the marg<strong>in</strong>alizcd positionaccorded to the monol<strong>in</strong>gual, poorly educated, rural poor <strong>in</strong> a social system dom<strong>in</strong>ated bythe <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g, bil<strong>in</strong>gual, urban middle class (see Kantliah, 1984). Ironically, the desirefor grammar-oriented learn<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>in</strong>fluences students to accept these limitations moreuncritically and give <strong>in</strong> to social reproduction.Hence, although on one level the grammatical approach ~ which is a culturallymandated, <strong>in</strong>digenous form of learn<strong>in</strong>g ~ enables students to somewhat resist the ideologicalthrusts of the foreign language and textbook, it is doubtful whether we can glorify this as aform of radical “resistance” as Kandiah (1984) implies. This is not to deny that the studysympathizes with Kandiah’s explanation of lack of motivation <strong>in</strong> ESOL students as be<strong>in</strong>ga result of the sociopolitical implications of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> Sri Lanka; the study also refutesthe alternative explanations of Goonetilleke (1983) and Hanson-Smith (1984) that this issimply a consequence of the educational policy which makes students give more time torival subjects even though students are conv<strong>in</strong>ced of the benef<strong>its</strong> of <strong>English</strong>. Yet Kandiahfails to grapple with the complexity of students’ opposition which has to be qualified by theirbelief <strong>in</strong> the benef<strong>its</strong> of <strong>English</strong>, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ation-oriented motivation. This tensionresults eventually <strong>in</strong> their giv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> to social and ideological reproduction through <strong>English</strong>.It becomes important therefore to unravel the ambiguous strands of students’ behaviorwith the help of Giroux (I 983) who warns that the concept of resistance must not be allowedto become a category <strong>in</strong>discrim<strong>in</strong>ately hung over every expression of “oppositional behavior”(p. 109). Thus, Giroux dist<strong>in</strong>guishes between resistance, which he sees as display<strong>in</strong>gideological clarity and commitmcnt to collective action for social transformation from mereopposition, which is unclear, ambivalent, and passive. Hav<strong>in</strong>g analyzed the effects of classroombehavior <strong>in</strong> the larger historical and social contexts, we can say that the responses andattitudes of the students do not fall untlcr Giroux’s def<strong>in</strong>ition of radical resistance. Studentsfail to susta<strong>in</strong> consciousness-rais<strong>in</strong>g or collective critical action. Theirs is largely a vague,<strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctive oppositional behavior which, due to <strong>its</strong> lack of ideological clarity, ironicallyaccommodates to their reproductive forces. It is perhaps <strong>in</strong> Supendran we see any signs ofconscious resistance that display potential for the development of a radical pedagogy for theLankan context.The behavior of most other students <strong>in</strong> the class is an ambivalent state whichconta<strong>in</strong>s elements of accommodation as well as opposition <strong>in</strong> response to the conflict<strong>in</strong>gpulls of socioeconomic mobility, on the one hand, and cultural <strong>in</strong>tegrity on the other.


ETHNOGRAPHY OF A SRI LANI


~ (1226 A. SURESH CANAGARAJAHO’Neill, R., K<strong>in</strong>gbury, R., Yeadon, T., and Cornelius, E.T. (1978) American kernel lessons:Intermediate. NewYork: Longman.Peirce, B.N. (1989) ‘Toward a pedagogy of possibility <strong>in</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternationally:People’s <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> South Africa’. TESOL Quarterly, 23(3), 401420.Pennycook, A. (1 989) ‘The concept of method, <strong>in</strong>terested knowledge, and the politics oflanguage teach<strong>in</strong>g’. TESOL Quarterly, 23(4), 589-618.Richards, J. C., and Rodgers,T.S. (1986) Approaches and methods <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g:A descriptionand analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Sirisena, U.D.I. (1969) Editorial <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>in</strong> Education <strong>in</strong> Ceylon (Pt. 1, pp. xxv-xvii).Colombo, Sri Lanka: M<strong>in</strong>istry of Education and Cultural Affairs.Somasegaram, S.W. (1969) ‘The H<strong>in</strong>du tradition’, <strong>in</strong> U.D.I. Sirisena (ed.) Education <strong>in</strong> Ceylon(Pt. 3, pp. 1131-1144). Colombo, Sri Lanka: M<strong>in</strong>istry of Education and Cultural Affairs.Stubbs, M. (1976) <strong>Language</strong>, schools and classrooms. London: Methuen.Watson-Gegeo, K. A. (1988) ‘Ethnography <strong>in</strong> ESL: Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the essentials’. TESOL Quarterh,22(4), 575-592.Weis, L. (1985) Between two worlds: Black students <strong>in</strong> an urban community college. Boston:Routledge.Wickramasuriya, S.(1981) ‘Jamcs de Alwis and second language teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Sri Lanka’.Navasilu, 4, 11-29.Willis, P. (1977) Learn<strong>in</strong>g to labour: How work<strong>in</strong>g class kids get work<strong>in</strong>g class jobs. Manchester,England: Saxon House.978) Profane cultures. London: Routledge.


Chapter 14J. Keith ChickSAFE-TALK: COLLUSION INAPARTHEID EDUCATIONIntroductionBackground to the studyHERE IS WIDESPREAD AGREEMENT AMONGST observers about whatT were the essential characteristics of <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong> schools for black people <strong>in</strong> SouthAfrica under the former apartheid system: highly centralised, with teachers adopt<strong>in</strong>gauthoritarian roles and do<strong>in</strong>g most of the talk<strong>in</strong>g, with few pupil <strong>in</strong>itiations, and with mostof the pupil responses tak<strong>in</strong>g the form of group chorus<strong>in</strong>g. Schlemmer and Bot (1 986: 80)report a senior African school <strong>in</strong>spector as stat<strong>in</strong>g that black pupils were discouraged fromask<strong>in</strong>g questions or participat<strong>in</strong>g actively <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and expla<strong>in</strong> that it was regarded asimpolite and even <strong>in</strong>subord<strong>in</strong>ate to ask questions or make suggestions <strong>in</strong> class. Thembela(1986: 41) refers to classroom practice be<strong>in</strong>g characterised by rote learn<strong>in</strong>g and teachercentred<strong>in</strong>struction.Most observers, moreover, agree that the educational consequences of such <strong>in</strong>teractionstyles were unfortunate. Schlemmer and Bot (1986) and Thembela (1 986), for example,argue that the use of such styles oppressed creativity, <strong>in</strong>itiative and assertiveness. MacDonald(1 988) claims that there are aspects of metacognition and disembedded th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g crucial toadvanced learn<strong>in</strong>g and to effective function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a technological society which these stylesof <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g did not promote.I became very aware of the possible negative educational consequences of theoverwhelm<strong>in</strong>g preference for such styles of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> schools for black people <strong>in</strong> SouthAfrica, through my <strong>in</strong>volvement with <strong>in</strong>-service teacher education projects which had, asone of their primary objectives, the foster<strong>in</strong>g of communicative approaches to the teach<strong>in</strong>gof <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu schools. (KwaZulu was a patchwork of geographical areas on theeastern seaboard of South Africa which, <strong>in</strong> terms of apartheid policy, was designated a‘homeland’ for Zulu people. At the time of the study reported here, the total population ofnative speakers of Zulu was almost seven million; they thus constituted the largest languagegroup <strong>in</strong> South Africa. Zulu speakers live <strong>in</strong> many parts of South Africa, but at that timeapproximately five million of them lived <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu.)A number of the implementors of the <strong>in</strong>-service teacher education projects compla<strong>in</strong>edabout the reluctance of many of thc teachers, and even some of the students, to adopt the


228 J. KEITH CHICI


SAFE-TALI< 229Organisation of the studyMost research reports imply that the research which they are report<strong>in</strong>g on proceeded <strong>in</strong>very orderly and logical ways, and that the researchers, from the outset, were moreknowledgeable and <strong>in</strong>sightful than they actually were. The false starts, the partialunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs and the dead ends do not feature. In this chapter I will be depart<strong>in</strong>g fromthis tradition, and shar<strong>in</strong>g with my readers the oftcn tortuous paths I followed <strong>in</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>gthc significance of <strong>in</strong>teractional styles widely employed <strong>in</strong> schools for black people <strong>in</strong> SouthAfrica.To beg<strong>in</strong> with, I report on my micro-ethnographic analysis of an episode <strong>in</strong> a lesson <strong>in</strong>a KwaZulu classroom. The general goal of micro-ethnographic analysis is to provide adescription of how <strong>in</strong>terlocutors set up or constitute contexts that allow them to make senseof one another’s messages. My spccific purpose was to try to establish why teachers andstudents <strong>in</strong> such classrooms found it difficult to transfer to styles compatible with communicativelanguagc teach<strong>in</strong>g. The analysis reveals <strong>in</strong>teractional behaviour consistent withthe putative Zulu-<strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractional styles idcntified <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terethnic encountersreferred to abovc. More significantly, it reveals that such styles servcd valuable socialfunctions for students and teachers alike. This could account for why teachers and studentswere reluctant to abandon such styles, despite the fact that the academic consequences ofsuch preference were probably unfortunate.I then expla<strong>in</strong> how my grow<strong>in</strong>g awareness of the limitations of micro-ethnographicresearch <strong>in</strong> general, and explanations of pervasive school failure amongst dom<strong>in</strong>ated groups<strong>in</strong> terms of culturally-specific <strong>in</strong>teractional styles <strong>in</strong> particular, prompted me to re-exam<strong>in</strong>emy classroom <strong>in</strong>teractional data. Critics have po<strong>in</strong>ted out that micro-ethnographic studiesoftcn take <strong>in</strong>sufficient account of how pervasive values, ideologies and structures <strong>in</strong> thewider socicty (macro context) constra<strong>in</strong> what takes place at a micro level. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, Igivc an account of the historical, structural circumstances which Contributed to mak<strong>in</strong>gprimary school education for most teachers and students <strong>in</strong> so-called black education <strong>in</strong>apartheid South Africa such a traumatic experience. F<strong>in</strong>ally I offer a re<strong>in</strong>terpretation of theanalysed data. I suggest that what is most significantly displayed <strong>in</strong> this episode is notculturally-specific Zulu <strong>in</strong>teractional styles, but styles consistent with <strong>in</strong>teractional normswhich teachers and students <strong>in</strong>teractionally constitutcd as a means of avoid<strong>in</strong>g the oppressiveand demean<strong>in</strong>g effects of apartheid ideology and structures. Follow<strong>in</strong>g McDermott andTylbor (1 987) I see the teacher and her students as collud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> preserv<strong>in</strong>g their dignity byhid<strong>in</strong>g the fact that little or no learn<strong>in</strong>g is tak<strong>in</strong>g placc. While serv<strong>in</strong>g the short-term <strong>in</strong>terestsof teachers and students, such strategies, I suggest, contributed to the widely documentedhigh failure rate <strong>in</strong> black education <strong>in</strong> apartheid South Africa, and made teachers and studentsresistant to educational <strong>in</strong>novation. The strategies thus servcd to re<strong>in</strong>force and reproducethe <strong>in</strong>equalities between the various population groups which characterised apartheidsociety.Culturally-specific <strong>in</strong>teractional styles as barriers to <strong>in</strong>novationand learn<strong>in</strong>gWith the goal, then, of try<strong>in</strong>g to establish why many teachers and students <strong>in</strong> KwaZuluschools resisted the adoption of egalitarian, decentralised ways of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g, I carried outa f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed micro-ethnographic analysis of an episode <strong>in</strong> a video-recorded mathematicslesson, <strong>in</strong>itially with the help of Marianne Claude (who had observed the lesson while itwas tak<strong>in</strong>g placc) and, later, <strong>in</strong>dependently. I selected this episode from the corpus collected


230 J. KEITH CHICI


SAFE-TALI< 23114151617181920212223242526272829303132333435363738394041424344454647the elements of the union setStudentA: (two[ threeMrs Gumbi: [ that isStudentA:Mrs Gumbi:StudentA:StudentA: 1-Mrs Gumbi:IfourMrs Gumbi: Ifoui-StudentA: lfive1fii.eMrs Gumbi:StudentA: Is&Mrs Gumbi:I&StudentA:Mrs Gumbi: leipht and eight . . .what type of set is this now . . . it is i [ union sctStudents: [ union sctlu aMrs Gumbi:union sct because we have been k<strong>in</strong>g now at the elementsof set B togher with the elements of &t [ CStudents: [ CMrs Gumbi: [to form oncset which called what . . . a union [ setStudents: [ setMrs Gumbi: rememberwhen you j&t the union set the elements for for the union setdo not rcpeat those elements which are written twice do you get thatStudents: I=Mrs Gumbi: I& not repeat them list them once OKStudents: 1-Mrs Gumbi: I& you understand thisStudents: IpMrs Gumbi: I& you understand thisStudents: 19What is immcdiatcly strik<strong>in</strong>g about this cpisodc (as also the lesson as a wholc) is theco<strong>in</strong>cidencc of tcachcr volubility and student (particularly <strong>in</strong>dividual student) taciturnity,charactcristics of <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong> the formerly segregated schools for black people <strong>in</strong> SouthAfrica, which, as I noted above, have been commented upon by many observers. Mrs Gumbi<strong>in</strong> this extract, as elsewhere <strong>in</strong> the lesson, does most of the tallung. Indeed, of thc total 19m<strong>in</strong>utes duration of thc lesson as a wholc, fivc seconds short of 16 m<strong>in</strong>utes consists ofteacher talk. Also the students’ opportunities to talk (with one or two cxccptions) arcreduced to group chorus<strong>in</strong>g.Volubility on the part of the teacher, which Scollon and Scollon (1 983) regard as asolidarity strategy, and taciturnity on the part of the students, which they regard as adeference strategy, is consistent with the culturally-specific <strong>in</strong>tcractional stylcs I had foundevidence for <strong>in</strong> my analysis of <strong>in</strong>terethnic encounters between Zulu-<strong>English</strong> spcakcrs andSouth African (white) <strong>English</strong> speakers (Chck 1985).This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g might, therefore, be seenas lend<strong>in</strong>g credence to the notion that the <strong>in</strong>teractional styles employed <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu


232 J. KEITH CHICI


SAFE-TALI< 233function is to signal participation rather than level of understand<strong>in</strong>g, i.e. it is aga<strong>in</strong> socialrather than academic <strong>in</strong> purpose.The social function of chorus<strong>in</strong>g became even more clearly evident when Iexam<strong>in</strong>ed the lesson as a whole. I discovered that the students are required, <strong>in</strong> response toboth k<strong>in</strong>ds of cue, to provide ma<strong>in</strong>ly confirmative onc- or two-word responses, or responseswhich repeat <strong>in</strong>formation on the board or <strong>in</strong>formation which has been recycled aga<strong>in</strong> andaga<strong>in</strong> by Mrs Gumbi. This suggests that chorus<strong>in</strong>g gives the students opportunities toparticipate <strong>in</strong> ways that reducc the possibility of the loss of face associated with provid<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>correct responses to tcacher elicitations, or not be<strong>in</strong>g able to provide responses at all. Itis <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to note that the chorus<strong>in</strong>g is more evident at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the lesson thanlater on. Once responses have been well rehearsed, so that the chance of be<strong>in</strong>g wrongpublicly is reduced, more <strong>in</strong>dividual responses are elicited, and at the end students are even<strong>in</strong>vited to leave their desks and carry out the very public act of writ<strong>in</strong>g their responseson the board.There is, of course, noth<strong>in</strong>g unusual about teachers need<strong>in</strong>g to resort to face-sav<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, s<strong>in</strong>ce the asymmetrical role relations between teachers and students to be found<strong>in</strong> most parts of the world ensure that the risk of face-threat is great. As Cwden (1 979: 147)expla<strong>in</strong>s, ‘teachers, by the very nature of their professional role, are cont<strong>in</strong>uously threaten<strong>in</strong>gboth aspects of their students’ face constra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g their freedom of action; evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, oftennegatively, a high proportion of student acts and utterances; and often <strong>in</strong>terrupt<strong>in</strong>g studentwork and student talk’. To reduce this risk, teachers employ face-sav<strong>in</strong>g strategies such asexpress<strong>in</strong>g directives <strong>in</strong>directly by means of <strong>in</strong>terrogatives, e.g. ‘Can you open your books,please?’ This strategy reduces the sense of imposition associated with the directiveby suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the students are free to decide whether or not to comply. However, theneed to resort to face-sav<strong>in</strong>g strategies is particularly great <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu classrooms becausethe asymmetry <strong>in</strong> the relative status of teachers and students is marked. This reflects themarked asymmetry <strong>in</strong> the relativc status of adults and childrcn <strong>in</strong> the wider community.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Mariannc Claude’s <strong>in</strong>formants (see Chick and Claude 1985), an adult <strong>in</strong> thatcommunity has the right to ask any child, who may well be a stranger, to do errands forthem (i.e. take a message to someone; buy someth<strong>in</strong>g at the shop) and may even chastise achild not their own.Another strik<strong>in</strong>g feature of this episode is the remarkably rhythmic manner <strong>in</strong> whichteacher and students synchronise their verbal and prosodic behaviours, particularly <strong>in</strong>accomplish<strong>in</strong>g the chorus<strong>in</strong>g sequences. <strong>Context</strong> analysts (e.g. Schefl<strong>in</strong> 1973; Condon 1977;Kendon 1973, 1979; McDermott, Gospod<strong>in</strong>off and Aaron 1978) have demonstrated thatparticipants <strong>in</strong> conversations organise their behaviours <strong>in</strong> co-operative, reciprocal,rhythmically co-ord<strong>in</strong>ated ways <strong>in</strong> signall<strong>in</strong>g to one another and negotiat<strong>in</strong>g the context oftheir talk.This enables them to make sense of what it is that they are do<strong>in</strong>g together. In theepisode such <strong>in</strong>teractional synchrony is possible, presumably, because the teacher and herstudents are able to draw on their shared, implicit knowledge of the discourse conventionsassociated with conventional <strong>in</strong>teractional styles. I suggest that this synchrony contributesto the perception that purposeful activity and learn<strong>in</strong>g are tak<strong>in</strong>g place.To sum up, the micro-ethnographic analysis of this episode reveals <strong>in</strong>teractionalbehaviour consistent with Zulu-<strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractional styles identified <strong>in</strong> a study of <strong>in</strong>terethnicencounters (see Chick 1985). Particularly noteworthy features of the discourse are thechorus<strong>in</strong>g behaviour and the remarkably rhythmic manner <strong>in</strong> which the participantssynchronise their <strong>in</strong>teractional behaviours <strong>in</strong> accomplish<strong>in</strong>g the chorus<strong>in</strong>g sequences.Analysis revealed that thcse putative styles serve social rather than academic functions. Forexample, they help the students to avoid the loss of face associated with bc<strong>in</strong>g wrong <strong>in</strong> a


234 J. I


SAFE-TALI< 235Ogbu (198 l), too, while not deny<strong>in</strong>g that micro-ethnographic studies have a role <strong>in</strong>expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how <strong>in</strong>teraction acts as an immediate cause of a particular child’s failure, arguesthat it is essential also to study how these classroom events are built up by forces emanat<strong>in</strong>gfrom outside these micro sett<strong>in</strong>gs.Influenced by such th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, I concluded that my micro-ethnographic analysis of theepisode from the mathematics lesson needed to be <strong>in</strong>formed by a macro-ethnographicaccount of the school<strong>in</strong>g provided for black students <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu.This account, along l<strong>in</strong>essuggested by Ogbu (1981), would be one that showed how the school system was relatedto social organisation, economy, political organisation, belief system and values, change andso on.In the section which follows, I provide <strong>in</strong>formation about the macro context ofschool<strong>in</strong>g for blacks <strong>in</strong> South Africa dur<strong>in</strong>g the apartheid era, which I identified as potentiallyrelevant to the re<strong>in</strong>terpretation of this episode. S<strong>in</strong>ce the lesson occurred <strong>in</strong> a Senior Primaryschool (fourth to eighth years of school<strong>in</strong>g) I focus on this phase of the school<strong>in</strong>g system. Ifocus also on the role of <strong>English</strong> as medium of <strong>in</strong>struction, s<strong>in</strong>ce research suggests (see, forexample, MacDonald 1990) that difficulties associated with the transfer from mother tongueto <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the first year of this phase constra<strong>in</strong> classroom behaviour <strong>in</strong> powerful ways.The macro context of school<strong>in</strong>g for black people <strong>in</strong> apartheidSouth AfricaAs most people are aware, apartheid, an Afrikaans word mean<strong>in</strong>g literally ‘apartness’ orseparateness, refers to the policy of the Nationalist Party, which, subsequent to <strong>its</strong> com<strong>in</strong>gto power <strong>in</strong> 1948, was implemented as a massive programme of social eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g. Racialsegregation had been a feature of South African society ever s<strong>in</strong>ce the arrival of whites <strong>in</strong>the 17th century. However, after 1948, segregation on racial and even, with<strong>in</strong> racial groups,on ethnic l<strong>in</strong>es, <strong>in</strong> every sphere of life, was implemented on a scale unprecedented <strong>in</strong> humanhistory. Not merely were separate <strong>in</strong>stitutions such as educational <strong>in</strong>stitutions establishedfor different race and ethnic groups, but geographical separation was attempted throughthe creation of ethnic ‘homelands’, of which KwaZulu was one.Exemplify<strong>in</strong>g as it does the classic divide-and-rule strategy, the apartheid policyadmirably served the goal of the Nationalist Party of consolidat<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the newlywonhegemony of Afrikanerdom. Segregation also served to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> and <strong>in</strong>crease theprivileged status that whites had enjoyed s<strong>in</strong>ce the 17th century, by facilitat<strong>in</strong>g the systematicdiscrim<strong>in</strong>ation aga<strong>in</strong>st people of colour.In education, systematic discrim<strong>in</strong>ation was evident <strong>in</strong> the differential per capitaexpenditure on education for the various population groups. Towards the end of theapartheid era, there were attempts by the government to narrow the gaps between theprovision for the various groups. However, as recently as the f<strong>in</strong>ancial year 1986/7, the percapita expenditure on education for whites was R2508.That for blacks (i.e. Africans ratherthan Asians or so-called ‘coloureds’) was only R476, whilst that for blacks <strong>in</strong> the homelandswas still lower; for example, <strong>in</strong> KwaZulu it was only R359 (South African Institute of RaceRelations (SAIRR) Survey 1987/88).One ofthc consequences of this differential expenditure, which probably played a role<strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g what styles of <strong>in</strong>teraction were possible, was differential teacher-studentratios. In 1987, whereas the student-teacher ratio for whites was 16 to 1, that for blacks <strong>in</strong>so-called white areas was 41 to 1, and for KwaZulu primary schools 53 to 1 and KwaZulusecondary schools 37 to 1 (SAIRR Survey 1987/88). It is very difficult for teachers, who


236 J. KEITH CHICKare responsible for large numbers of studcnts and who usually have to cope withovcrcrowded classrooms, to facilitate more egalitarian, dccentralised ways of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g.The more long-term discrim<strong>in</strong>atory effects of segregated education were evident, also,<strong>in</strong> thc differential levels of professional qualification of teachcrs <strong>in</strong> schools for the variouspopulation groups. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Du Plessis, Du Pisani and Plekker (1 989) whereas, <strong>in</strong> 1989,100% of teachers <strong>in</strong> schools for whites were professionally qualificd <strong>in</strong> the sense of hav<strong>in</strong>gat least matriculation or higher academic qualifications, as well as a tcachcrs’ certificate ordiploma, only 20% of teachers <strong>in</strong> black primary schools and 10% <strong>in</strong> black secondary schoolswere professionally qualified.Of particular rclevance to the constra<strong>in</strong>ts of macro factors upon classroom discourseis another factor, namely, how apartheid idcology was translated <strong>in</strong>to language mediumpolicy <strong>in</strong> black cducation. Hartshornc (1 987) reports that, until the Nationalists camc topower, the position of <strong>English</strong> as sole medium of <strong>in</strong>struction after thc first few years ofschool<strong>in</strong>g was unchallenged. He reports, further, that the Nationalists:made of Afrikaans a symbol of cxclusiveness and scparatcness, and the struggle forAfrikaans became part of the ‘mission’ to control and rule South Africa. In educationthis exprcssed <strong>its</strong>elf <strong>in</strong> a commitment to separate schools and rigid mother-tonguceducation policy. (Hartshornc 1987: 88)This commitment eventually translated <strong>in</strong>to mother-tongue <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> primaryeducation with <strong>English</strong> and Afrikaans as compulsory subjects from the first year of school<strong>in</strong>g,and with both Afrikaans and <strong>English</strong> as media of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> secondary cducation (halfthe subjects through <strong>English</strong> and half through Afrikaans). It was the <strong>in</strong>flcxible and doctr<strong>in</strong>aireimplementation of this policy, and the deafness to the protcsts of the black community, thatsparked the Soweto upris<strong>in</strong>g of 1976.This spread to the rest of the country, almost assum<strong>in</strong>gthe proportions of a full-scale civil war. As a conscqucnce of the conflict, the govcrnmcntwas forced to concede to the black community the right to choose either <strong>English</strong> or Afrikaansas medium <strong>in</strong> the high schools. In response to further pressure from the community, thisright to choose was extended to thc higher primary phase. <strong>English</strong> became overwhelm<strong>in</strong>glythe chosen medium <strong>in</strong> black cducation after the first three ycars of school<strong>in</strong>g. In 1988, forexample, only 20 primary schools (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g some very small farm schools) and no highschools used Afrikaans as mcdium (SAIRR 1988/89).Though the choice of <strong>English</strong> as medium represented thc will of the people, asMacDonald (1990) expla<strong>in</strong>s, <strong>in</strong> primary education at Icast, it added to the burdens ofteachers and students. She po<strong>in</strong>ts out (1 990: 39) that the apartheid systcm ensured thatmost of the teachers <strong>in</strong> so-called black education did not speak <strong>English</strong> with confidence orfluency, used outmoded materials, and had almost no contact with <strong>English</strong> speakers. Also,follow<strong>in</strong>g the major shift to <strong>English</strong> as medium <strong>in</strong> primary education from 1979 onwards,no changes were made to the syllabus for <strong>English</strong> to prepare the ground l<strong>in</strong>guistically andconceptually for <strong>its</strong> use across the curriculum. As a consequence, black primary schoolstudents were not adequately prepared for the suddcn transition to <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the fourthyear of school<strong>in</strong>g concurrently with the curriculum broaden<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to ten subjects. Nor weremost of the tcachcrs equipped to expla<strong>in</strong> effectively <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> the new concepts <strong>in</strong> thevarious contcnt subjects such as mathematics.MacDonald and her fellow rcscarchcrs found that there was a considerable gap betwecnthe <strong>English</strong> competence rcquircd for the read<strong>in</strong>g of contcnt subject textbooks <strong>in</strong> the fourthyear of school<strong>in</strong>g, and thc <strong>English</strong> competence that might have been expected if a studenthad bcncfited optimally from <strong>English</strong> as a second language teach<strong>in</strong>g materials then used <strong>in</strong>


SAFE-TALI< 237junior primary schools. They also found that there was also a very large gap between thishypothesised optimal competence and the level of competence students actually reached.They estimated, for example, that the vocabulary requirements <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased by1000% <strong>in</strong> the fourth year of school<strong>in</strong>g. They calculated that a student who had learntoptimally from the ESL materials <strong>in</strong> the junior primary phase might have encountered notmore than half the vocabulary, and might have been unfamiliar with syntactic elements <strong>in</strong>up to 60% of sentences <strong>in</strong> science textbooks used <strong>in</strong> the fourth year of school<strong>in</strong>g. Moreoverthey might have been so ignorant of the conventions of expository writ<strong>in</strong>g as to experiencewhat is referred to as ‘register shock’ when read<strong>in</strong>g those texts.As a consequence, the fourth year of school<strong>in</strong>g was a time of trauma for both teachersand students; a trauma reflected <strong>in</strong> the high drop-out rate <strong>in</strong> black schools at the end of thatyear (64, 100 or 8.9% of the total outflow <strong>in</strong> 1987 accord<strong>in</strong>g to the SAIRR Report1988 /89). The researchers found that the effect of those conditions was what they termed‘the loss of mean<strong>in</strong>g’. ‘The children are likely to be alienated by what they have to learn,and only dimly perceive the implications and l<strong>in</strong>kages between the concepts they arepresented with’ (MacDonald 1990: 141). Faced with these odds, teachers tended to resortto provid<strong>in</strong>g notes that the students were required to memorise. This gave the impressionof real learn<strong>in</strong>g tak<strong>in</strong>g place, but as MacDonald (1 990: 143) po<strong>in</strong>ts out, the students oftenlearnt what they did not understand, and were usually unable to use what they had learntbecause this mode of education did not allow the <strong>in</strong>tegration of new <strong>in</strong>formation with whathad been learnt before.A re<strong>in</strong>terpretation: safe-talk as the outcome of collusion betweenteachers and studentsReexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g my micro-ethnographic analysis of the episode <strong>in</strong> a mathematics lesson <strong>in</strong> aKwaZulu classroom, I was struck by the similarity between MacDonald’s account of theteachers’ response to the trauma experienced <strong>in</strong> the early years of senior primary school<strong>in</strong>gand my <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the <strong>in</strong>teractional behaviour <strong>in</strong> the episode as ‘safe-talk’.My th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g was also strongly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by two studies that attempt to trace therelationship between the structure of classroom discourse and the macro context <strong>in</strong> whichit occurs, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the ideologies that are promoted <strong>in</strong> them. In the first of these studies,Coll<strong>in</strong>s (1 987) argues that the ideology of ability group<strong>in</strong>g promoted <strong>in</strong> school systems <strong>in</strong>the United States leads students <strong>in</strong> low ability groups and their teachers to socialise oneanother <strong>in</strong>to systematic departures from the norms of classroom discourse. Behaviourconsistent with these ‘emergent’ norms (see Mchan 1979: 90) <strong>in</strong>terferes with the read<strong>in</strong>gpractice which members of these groups so badly need. Coll<strong>in</strong>s argues, further, that theideology of prescriptivism also promoted <strong>in</strong> the United States school system results <strong>in</strong>evaluation be<strong>in</strong>g made on the basis of cultural background rathcr than on academic aptitude.This leads to the systematic exclusion of m<strong>in</strong>ority students from opportunities to learn andpractise forms of literary discourse.In the second of these studies, McDcrmott and Tylbor (1 987) analyse an episode <strong>in</strong>which teachers and students do <strong>in</strong>teractional work to make the illiteracy of one of thestudents, Rosa, not noticeable. In the process Rosa does not get a turn to practise herread<strong>in</strong>g. They show that while evaluation is constantly tak<strong>in</strong>g place, teachers and studentscollude <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g overtly only when the evaluation is positive, while, at the same time,mak<strong>in</strong>g covert, unspoken, negative evaluations. Such collusion hides the unpleasant fact thatschool<strong>in</strong>g is structured <strong>in</strong> such a way as to provide access to opportunities for learn<strong>in</strong>g forsome students and to denv it to others.


238 J. KEITH CHICI


~ (1976)~ (1982b)~~~~~ (1979)SAFE-TALI< 239dismantl<strong>in</strong>g of apartheid structures and the assembl<strong>in</strong>g of alternative structures. Hopefully,the latter will makc it less necessary for teachers and students to engage <strong>in</strong> ‘safe-talk’.AcknowledgementI wish to acknowledge the contribution of Marianne Claude, who recorded the <strong>in</strong>teractionaldata and assisted <strong>in</strong> the analysis of it, and that of my colleagues Ralph Adcndorff and NicolcGesl<strong>in</strong> for their <strong>in</strong>sightful comments and suggestions.ReferencesCazden, C. (1 979) ‘<strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> education: variation <strong>in</strong> the teacher-talk register’, <strong>in</strong> J.E. Alatisand G.R. Tucker (eds) <strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> Public Ltfe, 144-62. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D.C.: GeorgetownUniversity Press.Chick, J.K. (1 985) ‘The <strong>in</strong>teractional accomplishment of discrim<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong> South Africa.<strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> Society 14(3): 229-326.Chick, J.K., and Claude, M. (1985) ‘The Valley Trust <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> Project: research <strong>in</strong>progress’. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of the Fourth National Conference of the Southern African AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics Association. Johannesburg: University of the Witwatersrand.Coll<strong>in</strong>s, J. (1 987) ‘Conversation and knowledge <strong>in</strong> bureaucratic sett<strong>in</strong>gs’. Discourse Processes 10:303-19.Condon, W. (1 977) ‘The relation of <strong>in</strong>teractional synchrony to cognitive and emotionalprocesses’, <strong>in</strong> M. Key (cd .) The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication, 50--65.The Hague: Mouton.Du Plessis, A,, Du Pisani, T. and Plekker, S. (1989) Education and Manpower Development.Bloemfonte<strong>in</strong>: Research Institute for Education Plann<strong>in</strong>g.Ellis, R. (1987) ‘Us<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>English</strong> medium <strong>in</strong> African Schools’, <strong>in</strong> D.Young (ed.) Bridgjng theGap between Theory and Practice <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>, 82-99. Cape Town:Maskew Miller Longman.Erickson, F. (1975) ‘ Gatekeep<strong>in</strong>g and the melt<strong>in</strong>g pot: <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> counsell<strong>in</strong>g encounters’.Harvard Educational Review 45 (1): 44-70.‘Gatekeep<strong>in</strong>g encounters: A social selection process’, <strong>in</strong> P.R. Sanday (cd.)Anthropology and the Public Interest: Fieldwork andTheory, 111-45. NewYork: Academic Press.Gumperz, J. (19824 Discourse Strategies (Studies <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractional sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics 1).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<strong>Language</strong> and <strong>Social</strong> Identity (Studies <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractional sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics 2).cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Hartshorne, K. ( 1987) ‘<strong>Language</strong> policy <strong>in</strong> African education <strong>in</strong> South Africa 1910--1985, withparticular reference to the issue of medium of <strong>in</strong>struction’, <strong>in</strong> D. Young (ed.) <strong>Language</strong>:Plann<strong>in</strong>g and Medium .f Education. Rondebosch: <strong>Language</strong> Education Unit and SAALA.Karabel, J. anti Halsey, A.H. (Eds) (1977) Power and Ideology <strong>in</strong> Education. New York: OxfordUniversity Press.Kendon, A. (1973) ‘Thc role of visible behaviour <strong>in</strong> the organization of social <strong>in</strong>teraction’, <strong>in</strong>M. Von Cranach and I. V<strong>in</strong>e (cds) <strong>Social</strong> Communication and Movement, 3-74. New York:Academic Press.‘Some theoretical and methodological aspects of the use of film <strong>in</strong> thc study ofsocial <strong>in</strong>teraction’, <strong>in</strong> G. G<strong>in</strong>sberg (ed.) Emerg<strong>in</strong>g Strategies <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Pychological Research,67- -91. NewYork: John Wiley.


~ (1240 J. I


PART THREEAnalys<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g


Chapter 15Neil MercerLANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGEIntroductionHIS CHAPTER IS ABOUT THE use of language as amcdium for teach<strong>in</strong>g andT learn<strong>in</strong>g, with special relevance to the teach<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>English</strong>. However, many of the issuesI will deal with, especially those <strong>in</strong> the early parts of the chapter, are not specific to the useof any particular language <strong>in</strong> the classroom, or the teach<strong>in</strong>g of any particular curriculumsubject. Of course, languages of <strong>in</strong>struction and curricula vary from country to country,region to region and even from school to school.Teachers differ <strong>in</strong> their style and approach,and their classes are made up of <strong>in</strong>dividuals of various personal characteristics and culturalbackgrounds, who differ <strong>in</strong> the ways they respond to teachers and particular styles ofteach<strong>in</strong>g. But, as I will expla<strong>in</strong>, observational research suggests that some ways that languageis used <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions between teachers and students are common features of classroom lifethroughout the world. I will illustrate some of these features of classroom language withreal-life examples, and discuss their possible educational functions. In the latter part of thechapter, I will use the theoretical perspective of socio-cultural psychology to relate theearlier analysis of classroom language to a consideration of the nature and quality ofclassroom education. In thesc ways, I hope to demonstrate the practical educational valueof a careful analysis of the <strong>in</strong>teractive process of teach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Language</strong> and teach<strong>in</strong>gWherever they are and whatever they are teach<strong>in</strong>g, teachers <strong>in</strong> schools and other educational<strong>in</strong>stitutions are likely to face some similar practical tasks. They have to organize activities tooccupy classes of disparate <strong>in</strong>dividuals, learners who may vary considerably <strong>in</strong> their aims,abilities and motivations. They have to control unruly behaviour. They are expected to teacha specific curriculum, a body of knowledge and skills which their students would notnormally encounter <strong>in</strong> their out-of-school lives. And they have to monitor and assess thceducational progress the students make. All thesc aspects of teachers’ responsibilities arereflected <strong>in</strong> thcir use of language as the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal tool of their responsibilities. As examplesof this, I would like you now to consider two transcribed sequences of classroom talk,Sequences 1 and 2 overlcaf. For each <strong>in</strong> turn, consider:1 Can you identify any recurr<strong>in</strong>g patterns of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> the talk between teacher andpupils?


244 NEIL MERCER2 What would you say wcrc the ma<strong>in</strong> functions of the teacher’s questions <strong>in</strong> each of thesequences? Do the sequences differ at all <strong>in</strong> this respect?I have made my own comments after both the sequences.(Note: <strong>in</strong> the transcriptions words spoken particularly emphatically are underl<strong>in</strong>ed. Wordswhich were unclear dur<strong>in</strong>g transcription are <strong>in</strong> curled brackets { }. The onset ofsimultaneous speech is marked with a square bracket [ .)Sequence 1 : Toy animalsThis sequence was recorded <strong>in</strong> an <strong>English</strong> lesson <strong>in</strong> a Russian primary school.The teachcrhas just set up a collection of soft toy animals <strong>in</strong> front of the class.T:S:T:Have you got any toy animals at home? Be quick. Raise your hand (she raises her own hand)and show me. Have you got any toy animals? S- { Name of child}(Stand<strong>in</strong>g up) I have got a cat, aNo, sit down, <strong>in</strong> your place.S: Yes, I have.T: 1 have got many?S: Toys at home.T: Toy animals at home.Sequence 2: Personal qualitiesThis next sequence comes from aTESOL class for young adults <strong>in</strong> a college <strong>in</strong> London. Alittle earlier, the teacher had asked each of the students to list their own personal qualities,both positivc and negative.T:D:T:D:T:D:T:D:T:D:T:D:T:D:T:Who would like to tell the class about their personal qualities? Dalia?I am polite, friendly, organized, trustworthy, responsible but sometimes I am impatientand unpunctual. Sometimes (laughs).Good, isn’t it? (Address<strong>in</strong>g the c1ass)Thank you, Dalia.That was good. Now can you tell methe positive qualities you have just said.Yeah?That is, friendly, um, organized.{Right}How is it help<strong>in</strong>g you . . .Yeah?. . . with your friends [<strong>in</strong> the class?[It help me to get along with people and to understand them andhelp them.That’s good. And what about the, the not very positive ones [like punctual[SometimesWhat happens then?Sometimes I lose my friend basically of that because I lose my temper very quickly.And what happens with me? I don’t smile at you that much do 1 ?


LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 245Comments on Sequences 1 and 2Sequence 1 illustrates some patterns which typify most classroom talk. First, the teachertook longer turns at speak<strong>in</strong>g than any students. Second, she asked all the questions.Observational rcscarch has shown that <strong>in</strong> classroom conversations teachers usually ask thegreat majority of questions, usually - as <strong>in</strong> this case ~ to elicit some k<strong>in</strong>d of participatoryresponse from the students. She then evaluates the replies they give. She is also us<strong>in</strong>g questionsto direct the topic or content of the talk towards issues that she wishes to focus attentionon. Look<strong>in</strong>g more carefully at Sequence 1, we can see that there is a structural pattern tothe talk: a teacher’s question is followed by a student response, followed <strong>in</strong> turn by some teacherfeedback or evaluation. This structural element of classroom talk was first described by thel<strong>in</strong>guists S<strong>in</strong>clair and Coulthard (1975; see also Mehan, 1979; Van Lier, Chapter 5 of thisbook) and usually known as an Initiation-Response-Feedback (IRF) exchange. For cxample:T: . . . Have you got any toy animals? S- {Name of child} IS: (Stand<strong>in</strong>g up) I have got a cat, aRT: No, sit down, <strong>in</strong> your place.FIRF exchangcs can be thought of as the archetypal form of <strong>in</strong>teraction between a teacherand a pupil ~ a basic unit of classroom talk as a cont<strong>in</strong>uous stretch of language or ‘text’.They do not typify the pattern of talk <strong>in</strong> all classroom activities; other k<strong>in</strong>ds of talk <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gdifferent patterns of exchanges (e.g. <strong>in</strong> which students ask questions of teachers, or of otherstudents) may happen too. And outside the most formal and traditional of classrooms, theymay not often be found <strong>in</strong> their classic, simple form. But IRFs have been observed as acommon feature <strong>in</strong> classrooms the world over, and <strong>in</strong> other languages bcsides <strong>English</strong>.In Sequence 1, the IRF exchanges are be<strong>in</strong>g used to perform a common function <strong>in</strong>classrooms, one that is almost certa<strong>in</strong>ly familiar to you from your own schooldays: a teacheris elicit<strong>in</strong>g from learners their knowledge of the relevant curriculum subject (<strong>in</strong> this case,<strong>English</strong>). Rcsearch shows that this particular hnd of use of question-and-answer by a teacherask<strong>in</strong>g questions to which thc teacher knows exactly what answers she seeks ~ is the mostcommon function of IRFs <strong>in</strong> classrooms. Here students are essentially try<strong>in</strong>g to provide the<strong>in</strong>formation that the teacher expects them to know. As the classroom researchers Edwardsand Wcstgate say:Most classroom talk which has been recorded displays a clear boundary bctweenknowledge and ignorance . . . To bc asked a question by someone who wants to knowis to be given the <strong>in</strong>itiative <strong>in</strong> decid<strong>in</strong>g the amount of <strong>in</strong>formation to be offered andthe manner of tell<strong>in</strong>g. But to be asked by someone who already knows, and wants toknow if you know, is to have your answer accepted, rejected or otherwise evaluatedaccord<strong>in</strong>g to thc questioner’s beliefs about what is relevant and true. (1 994, p 48)Teachers need to check students’ understand<strong>in</strong>g of procedural, factual matters, and that iscommonly the function of IRF cxchangcs. Sequence 1 illustrates also how ‘feedback’ froma teacher may also be used to control students’ behaviour. These are quite legitimatefunctions of teacher-talk, and all teachers might expect to use language <strong>in</strong> this way quitefrequcntly. But the danger of rcly<strong>in</strong>g hcavily and cont<strong>in</strong>uously on traditional, formalquestion-and-answer reviews for guid<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g is that students then get little opportunityfor us<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> more creative ways ~ such as experiment<strong>in</strong>g with ncw types of languageconstructions.


246 NEIL MERCERAs <strong>in</strong> much classroom talk, <strong>in</strong> Sequence 2 we can also see IRF exchanges occurr<strong>in</strong>g,though here as slightly more complex, l<strong>in</strong>ked structures, <strong>in</strong> which the student <strong>in</strong>terjectsdur<strong>in</strong>g the teacher’s elicitations, perhaps seek<strong>in</strong>g clarification which the teacher provides.And if we consider the content and function of the question-and-answer exchanges <strong>in</strong> thetwo sequences, we can see that someth<strong>in</strong>g rather different is go<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>in</strong> each of them. InSequence 1, the teacher is ask<strong>in</strong>g her primary school pupils to produce <strong>English</strong> sentenceswhich conform to the models she has <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. The children respond by try<strong>in</strong>g to providethese ‘right answers’. The teacher <strong>in</strong> Sequence 2 is not do<strong>in</strong>g that. Instead, she is ask<strong>in</strong>gquestions to encourage the students to elaborate, <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, on what they have written. Inthis way, the teacher is not so much try<strong>in</strong>g to elicit particular forms or structures of <strong>English</strong>,but rather encourag<strong>in</strong>g the student to use <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> a practical, communicative manner. Iam not suggest<strong>in</strong>g that either teacher is us<strong>in</strong>g their question<strong>in</strong>g techniques to better or worseeffect, but simply illustrat<strong>in</strong>g the fact that IRF exchanges can be made to serve a variety ofpragmatic, educational functions.Techniques for teach<strong>in</strong>gHav<strong>in</strong>g identified the archetypal structure of teacher-student talk, I will next describe somespecific ways of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with students which are commonly used by teachers. I call these‘techniques’ , because I believe that they represent teachers attempt<strong>in</strong>g to shape language<strong>in</strong>to a set of suitable tools for pursu<strong>in</strong>g their professional goals. I will illustrate each techniqueand consider how they can contribute to the process of teach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g. Thetechniques are summarised <strong>in</strong> Table 1 5.1 below.Toble 15.1 Some techniques that tcachera use. . . to elicit knowledge from learnersDirect elicitationsCued elicitations. . . to respond to what learners sayConfirmationsRejectionsRepetitionsReformulationsElaborations. . . to describe significant aspects of shared experienceamplificationsexplanations‘we’ statementsrecapsElicit<strong>in</strong>g knowledgefrom learnersWe have seen that when a teacher <strong>in</strong>itiates an IRF sequence, this usually has the function ofelicit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation from a student. If this is simply a straightforward request, we candescribe the teacher’s verbal act as a direct elicitation. But teachers also often engage <strong>in</strong> whatcan be called cued elicitation, which is a way of draw<strong>in</strong>g out from learners the <strong>in</strong>formation


thecanLANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 247they are seek<strong>in</strong>g ~ the ‘right’ answers to their questions - by provid<strong>in</strong>g visual clues andverbal h<strong>in</strong>ts as to what answer is required. Here is an example recorded <strong>in</strong> an <strong>English</strong> lesson<strong>in</strong> a Zimbabwean primary school. The teacher has set up a number of objccts on her desk,and also has a set of cards on which various consonants (‘b’, ‘f, ‘j’ etc.) are written. Thechildren have to come to the front of the class and match the consonants to the name of anobject.Sequence 3: say the soundTeacher: (to child): Say the sound.Child: b-I-bTeacher: b-b-b is for?(Child does not answer. Teacher waves her hand over the nearest objects, one ofwhich is a book)Child: b-b-b is for book.Teacher: Well done!The use of cued elicitation as a teach<strong>in</strong>g technique is widespread. It can be traced to theSocratic dialogues constructcd by Plato (Edwards, 1988). By us<strong>in</strong>g this technique, theteacher avoids simply giv<strong>in</strong>g the child the right answer. Sequence 3 also illustrates hownon-verbal communication ~ use of gestures and other signs ~ be an importantcomponent of classroom talk.Respond<strong>in</strong>g to what learners sayAs illustrated by the sequences above, one of the ways that teachers susta<strong>in</strong> dialogues withtheir students is to use what students say as the basis for what they say next. In this way, thelearners’ own rcmarks are <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to the teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process. The mostobvious way of do<strong>in</strong>g this is through conjrmation (as, for example, a tcacher’s ‘Yes, that’sright’ to a pupil’s answer). Repetitions of th<strong>in</strong>gs learners say are another way, one whichallows the teacher to draw to the attention of a whole class an answer or other remark whichis judged by the teacher to have educational significance.Teachers often paraphrase or reformulate a pupil’s remark, usually so as to offer the classa revised, tidied-up version of what was said which f<strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong> better with the po<strong>in</strong>t that theteacher wishes to make or the form of response be<strong>in</strong>g sought. For example, <strong>in</strong> this extractfrom Sequence 1 :S: Yes, I have.T: I have got many?S: Toys at home.T: Toy animals at home.There are also elaborations, when a teacher picks up on a cryptic statement made by a pupiland expands and/or expla<strong>in</strong>s <strong>its</strong> significance to the rest of the class. Wrong answers orunsuitable contributions may be explicitly rejected by a teacher. But we should also note apopular technique that teachers have for deal<strong>in</strong>g with wrong answers - simply ignor<strong>in</strong>gthem.


248 NEIL MERCERDescrib<strong>in</strong>g shared experienceClassroom activities often rely on students read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions, whether <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t or on acomputer screen. It is important that thcy understand properly what is expected of them,if the activity is to succeed.Teachers therefore often amp1~<strong>in</strong>structions with the <strong>in</strong>tentionof mak<strong>in</strong>g them clearer and less ambiguous. Other texts may also conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation whichstudents necd to make sense of before they cont<strong>in</strong>ue any further. In classrooms it is commonto hear tcachcrs expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g these texts to students as either a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary to activities or ifsome confusion about them seems to arise. For example, <strong>in</strong> this cxtract from a Spanishlesson for adult students:Sequence 4: Ser and EstarTeacher: It says (read<strong>in</strong>gfrom text) ‘ This is one of the ma<strong>in</strong> difficulties for <strong>English</strong> speak<strong>in</strong>glearners’ mean<strong>in</strong>g the two verbs ser and estar which both, uh, translate as ‘to be’ <strong>in</strong><strong>English</strong>. (Read<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong>) ‘Ser means to exist while estar means to be situated’. Thatsounds horribly complicated, I th<strong>in</strong>k to start by th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of ser as be<strong>in</strong>g aboutpermanent th<strong>in</strong>gs and estar as temporary ways of be<strong>in</strong>g. Vamos u ver . . . (He cont<strong>in</strong>ues<strong>in</strong> Spanish)An important task for a teacher is to help learners see how the various activities they do,over time, contribute to the development of their understand<strong>in</strong>g. Education cannot bemerely the experience of a series of consecutive cvcnts, it must bc a developmental process<strong>in</strong> which earlier experiences provide the foundations for mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of later ones. Forthose <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g and Icarn<strong>in</strong>g, cont<strong>in</strong>uous sharcd cxpericncc is one of the mostprecious resources available. There are many ways that teachers try to create cont<strong>in</strong>uitics<strong>in</strong> the experience of lcarncrs ~ by sequenc<strong>in</strong>g activities <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> ways, by deal<strong>in</strong>g withtopics <strong>in</strong> order of difficulty, and so on.Tcachcrs can hclp lcarncrs perceive cont<strong>in</strong>uity <strong>in</strong> whatthey are do<strong>in</strong>g. Through language there is the possibility of rcpcatcdly rcvisit<strong>in</strong>g andre<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g that experience, and of us<strong>in</strong>g it as the basis for future talk, activity andlearn<strong>in</strong>g.‘We’ statements (as <strong>in</strong> a teacher say<strong>in</strong>g to a class ‘last week we learned how to measureangles’) are often used when teachers are try<strong>in</strong>g to rcprcsent past experience as relevant topresent activity. They show how teachcrs hclp learners see that they have significant pastcxperience <strong>in</strong> common and so have ga<strong>in</strong>ed sharcd knowledge and collective understand<strong>in</strong>gwhich can be drawn upon to progress further. Teachers also often recap shared classroomcxpericnce from earlier <strong>in</strong> a lesson, and from previous Icssons, usually emphasis<strong>in</strong>g thepo<strong>in</strong>ts or events they consider of most cducational significance.I have described and illustrated cach of the techniques as separate items, each with anobvious function; but this is a simplification, for the sake of clarity of exposition, of therclationship between language form, function and context. An analyst of classroom discoursehas to recognize that (a) any particular utterance can perform more than one function (sothat, as <strong>in</strong> the first part of Scqucncc 3, a repetition can also be an elicitation); (b) any particulartechnique can serve more than one pedagogic purposc, and be used effectively or otherwise;and (c) the functional mean<strong>in</strong>g of any <strong>in</strong>teraction for participants may bc shapcd bycontextual factors not available to the analyst (such as <strong>in</strong>formation ga<strong>in</strong>cd from their sharedpast expcricncc of <strong>in</strong>teraction; see Brccn, Chapter 7, for further discussion of such matters).However, despite these caveats, I have found the identification of thcsc techniques a useful,practical aid to analysis.


LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 249Interaction <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual and multil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gsIn the next part of the chapter I will consider some aspects of teacher-student <strong>in</strong>teraction<strong>in</strong> classrooms where <strong>English</strong> is be<strong>in</strong>g used as a classroom language, but is not the firstlanguage of the children. I hope to show through these examples some of the qualities thcscbil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs have <strong>in</strong> common with monol<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms, while also po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g outsome of the special <strong>in</strong>teractional features they may generate. There are two ma<strong>in</strong> sorts ofsituation which can be <strong>in</strong>cluded here.The first occurs <strong>in</strong> countries where <strong>English</strong> is not theusual everyday language and the mother tongue of most of the children is not <strong>English</strong>. Thesecond is where pupils whose mother tongue is not <strong>English</strong> enter schools <strong>in</strong> a predom<strong>in</strong>antly<strong>English</strong> speak<strong>in</strong>g country. I will provide examples from both of these types of situation.In any situation where <strong>English</strong> is used as a classroom language but is not the ma<strong>in</strong>language of children’s home or community, teachers may have thc multiple task of teach<strong>in</strong>g(a) the <strong>English</strong> language, (b) the educational ground rules for us<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong> the classroom, and(c) any specific subject content. Jo Arthur (1992) carried out observational research onteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> primary school classrooms <strong>in</strong> Botswana. <strong>English</strong> was used as themedium of education, but it was not the ma<strong>in</strong> language of the pupils’ local community. Sheobserved that when teachers were teach<strong>in</strong>g mathematics, they commonly used questionand-answersessions as opportunities for school<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>in</strong> the use of appropriate‘classroom <strong>English</strong>’ as well as maths. For example, one primary teacher commonly <strong>in</strong>sistedthat pupils reply to questions ‘<strong>in</strong> full sentences’, as shown below:Sequence 5: How many parts?Teacher:First pupil:Teacher:Pupil:Teacher:Second pupil:Tcacher:Second pupil:Teachcr:How many parts are left here (first pupil’s name)?Seven parts.Answer fully. How many parts are there?There are . . . there are seven parts.How many parts are left? Sit down my boy. You have tried. Yes (second pupil’sname)?We are left with seven parts.We are left with seven parts. Say that (second pupil’s name).We are left with seven parts.Good boy. We are left with seven parts.(Arthur, 1992, pp. 6--7)Sequence 5 is made up of a l<strong>in</strong>ked series of IRF exchanges. For example:How many parts are left here? [Initiation]Seven parts [Response]Answer fully [Feedback/ Evaluation]The Botswanan students therefore needed to understand that their teacher was us<strong>in</strong>g thesecxchanges not only to evaluate their mathematical understand<strong>in</strong>g, but also to test theirfluency <strong>in</strong> spoken <strong>English</strong> and their ability to conform to a ‘ground rule’ that she enforccd<strong>in</strong> her classroom - ‘answer <strong>in</strong> full sentences’. Arthur comments that for pupils <strong>in</strong> this k<strong>in</strong>dof situation, the demands of classroom communication are complicated because their teacheris attcmpt<strong>in</strong>g to get them to focus on both the medium (<strong>English</strong>) and the message (maths).


250 NEIL MERCERArthur reports that such dual focus is common <strong>in</strong> Botswanan classrooms, as the follow<strong>in</strong>gsequence from another lesson shows:Sequence 6: the cont<strong>in</strong>ent of AfricaT:P1:T:P2 :T:P3:T:Ps:In which cont<strong>in</strong>ent is your country? In which cont<strong>in</strong>ent is your country? Give an answerIn Africa is my countryHe says <strong>in</strong> Africa is my country. Who could frame her sentence? In Africa is my countryAfrica is my Cont<strong>in</strong>entMy question was <strong>in</strong> which cont<strong>in</strong>ent is your country?Its cont<strong>in</strong>ent is <strong>in</strong> AfricaIt is <strong>in</strong> the cont<strong>in</strong>ent ofAfrica. everybodyIt is <strong>in</strong> the cont<strong>in</strong>ent of Africa(Arthur, 1992, p. 13)Bil<strong>in</strong>gual code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroomIn circumstances where one language is be<strong>in</strong>g used as a classroom language, but where thepupils’ first language is a different one, a teacher may sometimes ‘code-switch’ to the firstlanguage if they judge it necessary. (We saw this k<strong>in</strong>d of switch tak<strong>in</strong>g place between Spanishand <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> Scqucncc 4 above). Somctimcs thc first language may be used only for asides,for control purposes or to make personal comments. However, when code-switch<strong>in</strong>gamounts to translation by the teacher of the curriculum content be<strong>in</strong>g taught, <strong>its</strong> use as anexplanatory teach<strong>in</strong>g strategy is somewhat controversial. On the one hand, thcrc arc thosewho argue that it is a sensible, common-sense response by a teacher to the specific k<strong>in</strong>d ofteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g situation. Thus <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong> use <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>-medium classrooms <strong>in</strong>Hong Kong,Angel L<strong>in</strong> (Chapter 17 ofthis book) expla<strong>in</strong>s a particular teacher’s use of codeswitch<strong>in</strong>gas follows:by always start<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> L1, Teacher D always starts from where the student is ~what the student can fully understand and is familiar with. (p. 282)fromResearchers of bil<strong>in</strong>gual code-switch<strong>in</strong>g (as reviewed by Martyn-Jones, 1995) have oftenconcluded that it is of dubious value as a teach<strong>in</strong>g strategy, if one of the aims of the teach<strong>in</strong>gis to improve students’ competence <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. Thus Jacobson comments:the translation <strong>in</strong>to the child’s vernacular of everyth<strong>in</strong>g that is be<strong>in</strong>g taught may preventhim/her from ever develop<strong>in</strong>g thc k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>English</strong> language proficicncy that must beone of the objcctivcs ofa sound bil<strong>in</strong>gual programme (Jacobson, 1990, p. 6.)It seems, however, that teachers often use code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> more complex ways than simplytranslat<strong>in</strong>g content directly <strong>in</strong>to another language. On observ<strong>in</strong>g classrooms <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong,Johnson and Lee (1 987) ohserved that the switch<strong>in</strong>g strategy most commonly employed byteachers had a three-part structure as follows:1 ‘Key statement’ of topic <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>2 Amplification, clarification or explanation <strong>in</strong> Cantonese3 Restatement <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>


LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 251They comment that ‘direct translation was comparatively rare; the general effect was of aspirall<strong>in</strong>g and apparently haphazard recycl<strong>in</strong>g of content, which on closer exam<strong>in</strong>ationproved to be more organised than it appeared.’ (1 987, p 106). The implication here is thatsuch teachers are pursu<strong>in</strong>g the familiar task of guid<strong>in</strong>g children’s understand<strong>in</strong>g ofcurriculum content through language, but us<strong>in</strong>g special bil<strong>in</strong>gual techniques to do so.An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g study of code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms <strong>in</strong> Malta was carried outby Anto<strong>in</strong>ette Camilleri (1 994). She showed that code-switch<strong>in</strong>g was used as a teach<strong>in</strong>gtechnique by teachers <strong>in</strong> a variety of ways. Look for example at these two extracts from thetalk of a teacher <strong>in</strong> a secondary school lesson about the production and use of wool, andbased on a textbook written <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. The teacher beg<strong>in</strong>s by read<strong>in</strong>g part of the text (Atranslation oftalk <strong>in</strong> Maltese is given <strong>in</strong> the right hand column)Sequence 7:WoolExtract 1England Australia New Zealand andArgent<strong>in</strong>a are the best producers of wooldawk l-aktar li gfiandhom farms li jrabbu n-nagfiaggfios-suf 0. K. Englandtgfiiduli m<strong>in</strong>n licma post Englandgfiandhom Scotland magfiruJ<strong>in</strong> tantgfiall-wool u gersijict tagfihom O.K.Extract 2wool issa it does not crease but it has to bewashed with care issa d<strong>in</strong> importantima gfiidtilkomx illi jekk ikolli nara xagfira jewsufa wafida under the microscope ghandhaqisha fiaJno scales tal. fiuta issa jekk ma nafislux sewwa dawk 1-iscales jitgfiaqqdu go xulx<strong>in</strong>u <strong>in</strong>dafifi gersi daqshekk 801- wash<strong>in</strong>g’mach<strong>in</strong>e u nofiorgu daqshekk gfiax jixxr<strong>in</strong>kjaliu jitgfiaqqad kolluthey have the largest number of farmsand the largest number of sheep for woolO.K. England where <strong>in</strong> England we reallymean Scotland they are very well-knownfor their woollen productsnow this is important didn’t I tell you thatif I had a look at a s<strong>in</strong>gle hair or fibreit has many scales which if not washedproperly get entangled and I put a jerseythis size <strong>in</strong>to the wash<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>e and itcomes out this size because it shr<strong>in</strong>ks andgcts entangled(Adapted from Camilleri, 1994)Camilleri notes that the first extract shows the teacher us<strong>in</strong>g the switch from <strong>English</strong> toMaltese to expand or ampltfv the po<strong>in</strong>t be<strong>in</strong>g made, rather than simply repeat it <strong>in</strong>translation. In the second extract, she expla<strong>in</strong>s the <strong>English</strong> statement <strong>in</strong> Maltese, aga<strong>in</strong>avoid<strong>in</strong>g direct translation. Camilleri comments that the lesson therefore is a particular k<strong>in</strong>dof literacy event, <strong>in</strong> which these are ‘two parallel discourses ~ the written one <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>,the spoken one <strong>in</strong> Maltese’ (p 12).Studies of code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> classrooms have revealed a variety of patterns of bil<strong>in</strong>gualuse (Martyn-Jones, 1995). For example, Zentella (1981) observed and recorded events <strong>in</strong>two bil<strong>in</strong>gual classes <strong>in</strong> NewYork schools, one a first grade class (<strong>in</strong> which the children wereabout six years old) and the other a sixth grade (<strong>in</strong> which the average age would be about12). The pupils and tcachers were all native Spanish speakers, of Puerto Rican orig<strong>in</strong>, butthe official medium for classroom education was <strong>English</strong>. One of the focuses of her analysisof teacher-pupil <strong>in</strong>teractions was IRF sequences. Both Spanish and <strong>English</strong> were actually


crucially(d)252 NEIL MERCERused by teachers and pupils <strong>in</strong> the classes, and Zcntclla was able to show that there werethree recurr<strong>in</strong>g patterns of language-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> IRF sequences, which seem to representthe use of certa<strong>in</strong> ‘ground rules’ govern<strong>in</strong>g language choice. These are summarized below:Rules govern<strong>in</strong>g teacher <strong>in</strong>itiation student reply teacher feedbacklanguage choice1. Teacher and <strong>English</strong>student: ‘follow Spanishthe leader’2. Teacher: ‘follow <strong>English</strong>the child’ SpanishSpanishSpanishSpanish<strong>English</strong><strong>English</strong>SpanishSpanish<strong>English</strong>3. Teacher: ‘<strong>in</strong>clude <strong>English</strong> Spanish both languagesthe child’s choice Spanish <strong>English</strong> both languagesnot yours’(Adapted from Zentella, 1981)From this example, we can see that dist<strong>in</strong>ctive patterns of language use emerge <strong>in</strong>bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms, but these can be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as adaptations of the common IRFstructure and language strategies used by teachers <strong>in</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs. What is more,the dist<strong>in</strong>ctive patterns of switch<strong>in</strong>g which emerge <strong>in</strong> teacher-talk can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> termsof the special communicative resources that arise <strong>in</strong> a modern language classroom and theways that teachers decide to respond to these special circumstances. The extent to whichcode-switch<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>English</strong> and another language occurs <strong>in</strong> a particular sett<strong>in</strong>g willtherefore be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by factors such as (a) the degree of fluency <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> that membersof a particular class have achieved; (b) the bil<strong>in</strong>gual competence of teachers (c) the specificteach<strong>in</strong>g goals of teachers; and ~ ~ the attitudes of both children and teachersto the practice of code-switch<strong>in</strong>g and to the languages <strong>in</strong>volved.What learners have to understand about classroom languageWhen students enter an <strong>English</strong> medium or EFL classroom hav<strong>in</strong>g grown up speak<strong>in</strong>ganother language, it may be difficult for both teachers and children to dist<strong>in</strong>guish betweentwo ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g tasks’ ~ acquir<strong>in</strong>g a basic fluency <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> and learn<strong>in</strong>g the social conventionsof us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> as a classroom language. Some patterns of classroom language - such asIRF sequences ~ are likely to be familiar to any student who has had experience of school,even if they had encountered them <strong>in</strong> another language. As I noted earlier, however (<strong>in</strong> thecomparison of Sequences 1 and 2), IRFs can be used for different purposes, some of whichmay not be familiar to students from their previous educational experience (say, if they havearrived as immigrants <strong>in</strong> an <strong>English</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g country hav<strong>in</strong>g been educated elsewhere <strong>in</strong>another language). Depend<strong>in</strong>g on their experiences with<strong>in</strong> their own language communities,students might also be unfamiliar with some other Conventions or ‘ground-rules’ for us<strong>in</strong>g<strong>English</strong> that are associated with particular social sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>side and outside school.For these reasons, it can be difficult for a teacher to tell whether a new pupil who isnot fluent <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, and who appears to be hav<strong>in</strong>g difficulties with us<strong>in</strong>g the language <strong>in</strong>the classroom, is struggl<strong>in</strong>g with general aspects of us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> or hav<strong>in</strong>g difficulties withgrasp<strong>in</strong>g the ‘local’ ground rules for classroom language use.This k<strong>in</strong>d of difficulty may arise


~ orwhatassignments,LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 253<strong>in</strong> relation to the learn<strong>in</strong>g of written as well as spoken <strong>English</strong>, and is well illustrated by theresearch ofAlex Moore (1 995) who studied the progress of children of non-<strong>English</strong> speak<strong>in</strong>gimmigrant families enter<strong>in</strong>g secondary schools <strong>in</strong> Brita<strong>in</strong>.Because of his close and cont<strong>in</strong>uous <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> classroom events as a k<strong>in</strong>d of ‘actionresearcher’ (Elliot, 1991), Moore was able to observe, describe and analyse teach<strong>in</strong>g andlearn<strong>in</strong>g over several weeks or months <strong>in</strong> one class. One of his special ‘case studies’ was ofthe progress of a Sylheti boy of 15 who had been <strong>in</strong> Brita<strong>in</strong> one year s<strong>in</strong>ce com<strong>in</strong>g fromBangladesh (where he had been educated <strong>in</strong> Bengali). Moore focused on Mashud’s classroomeducation <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>. Mashud had quite a few problems with ‘surface features’ of<strong>English</strong> such as handwrit<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g and grammatical structures, but was an enthusiasticwriter. However, Moore and Mashud’s teacher (Mrs Montgomery) both noticed that:his work had a particular idiosyncrasy <strong>in</strong> that whenever he was set creative writ<strong>in</strong>geven discursive writ<strong>in</strong>g ~ he produced heavily formulaic fairy-storystylemoral tales which were apparently ~ accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>formation volunteered byother Sylheti pupils <strong>in</strong> the class - translations of stories he had learnt <strong>in</strong> his nativetongue. (Moore, 1995: 362)Despite be<strong>in</strong>g a will<strong>in</strong>g pupil, Mashud seemed unable to transcend this traditional style ofgenre, and write <strong>in</strong> the genres that his teachers knew would be required of him <strong>in</strong> the Britisheducation system and <strong>in</strong> wider society. Further consideration led Moore and MrsMontgomery to some hypotheses about why this was so:It has to be said that neither Mrs Montgomery or I knew enough about Bangladeshior Sylheti story-tell<strong>in</strong>g traditions to be able to expound with any degree of confidenceon the cause of Mashud’s particular way of go<strong>in</strong>g about th<strong>in</strong>gs. The key to our futurepedagogy, however [. . .] lay <strong>in</strong> Mrs Montgomery’s very wise recognition that ‘‘therecould be the most enormous difference between what Mashud has been brought upto value <strong>in</strong> narratives and what we’re tell<strong>in</strong>g him he should be valu<strong>in</strong>g”. (Moore, 1995:366)Ths <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to Mashud’s difficulties with genres of writ<strong>in</strong>g was supported by a more carefulanalysis of Mashud’s texts, which had a l<strong>in</strong>ear, additive, chronological structure associatedwith oral, rather than literate cultural traditions (Ong, 1982).The outcome was the teacherdesign<strong>in</strong>g activities for Mashud which would support or ‘scaffold’ (Bruner, 1986; Mayb<strong>in</strong>,Mercer and Stierer, 1992) his development as a writer of <strong>English</strong>:If we responded appropriately, Mashud would, we hoped, learn someth<strong>in</strong>g of whatwas valued <strong>in</strong> expressive writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> his new school, and how that was different from- though no better than ~ he may have learned to value at school <strong>in</strong> Bangladesh.(Moore 1995: 368)This approach proved successful, as dur<strong>in</strong>g the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g period of Moore’s researchMashud showed clear progress <strong>in</strong> com<strong>in</strong>g to understand and cope with the demands ofwrit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the genres of <strong>English</strong> required <strong>in</strong> the British school system. Describ<strong>in</strong>g researchwith children <strong>in</strong> a Spanish-<strong>English</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual program <strong>in</strong> Californian schools, Moll andDwor<strong>in</strong> (1 996) also highlight the important role of a teacher <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g learners make thebest educational use of their bi-cultural language experience <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g their literacyskills <strong>in</strong> the second language.


254 NEIL MERCERA socio-cultural perspective on classroom <strong>in</strong>teractionI now wish to relate the above discussion of language as the medium of teach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>gto a consideration of the quality of education.To do this, I will draw on a particular approachto human learn<strong>in</strong>g and devclopment which is known as sociocultural psycholou.This approachhas emerged dur<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>al dccadcs of the twentieth century from a belated appreciationof the pioneer<strong>in</strong>g research on the relationship between language and cognitive developmentcarried out by the Russian psychologist Lcv Vygotsky (for example, Vygotsky, 1962).Vygotsky worked <strong>in</strong> Moscow <strong>in</strong> the 1920s and 30s, <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>stitution for children who hadspecial educational needs, but his ideas on the process of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g have muchbroader educational relevance than the specific <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> which he put them<strong>in</strong>to practice. Vygotsky gave language a special, important role <strong>in</strong> human cognitivedevelopment, describ<strong>in</strong>g human <strong>in</strong>dividuals and their societies as be<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ked by language<strong>in</strong>to a historical, cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g, dynamic, <strong>in</strong>teractive, spiral of change. Led by the example ofJerome Bruner (1 985,1986), a considerablc body of research has now emerged which USCSa ‘neo-Vygotskian’ , socio-cultural perspective <strong>in</strong> the analysis of educational processes. Someof the most significant and dist<strong>in</strong>ctive implications of adopt<strong>in</strong>g a socio-cultural perspectiveon classroom education are, I belicvc, as follows:<strong>Language</strong> is our most important pedagogic tool. Although they do not necessarily make thisexplicit, I suggest that the most <strong>in</strong>fluential socio-cultural theorists of cognitivedevelopment (as represented by such as Bruner, 1986; Wertsch, 1991 ; Rogoff, 1990)ascribe three important functions to language: (a) as a cognitive tool whose acquisitionenables children to ga<strong>in</strong>, process, organize and evaluate knowledge; (b) as a culturaltool, by which knowlcdge is shared, stored and made available to successivegenerations; (c) as a pedagogic tool by which <strong>in</strong>tellectual guidance is provided tochildren by other peoplc.Thcsc roles are <strong>in</strong>extricably <strong>in</strong>tertw<strong>in</strong>ed. To this specificationof the roles of language we might add the comment: learn<strong>in</strong>g how to use languageeffectively as a cultural tool is an important educational goal for native speakers aswell as second language learners. So language is both the tool for carry<strong>in</strong>g outteach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g and also that which is meant to be learnt and taught.Education is a dialogical, cultural process. The development of students’ knowledge andunderstand<strong>in</strong>g is shaped by their relationships with teachers and other students, andby the culture <strong>in</strong> which those relationships are located. (Newman, Griff<strong>in</strong> and Cole,1989; Gee, 1996).The educational success students achieve is only partly under theirown control, and only partly under the control of their teachers. This is where thesociocultural concept of ‘scaffold<strong>in</strong>g’, which I mentioned briefly earlier, is useful .Theessence of this concept, as developed by Bruner (1986), Wood (1988) and others, isthat an effective teacher provides the k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>tellectual support which enableslearners to make <strong>in</strong>tellectual achievements they would never accomplish alone; andone way they do $0 is by us<strong>in</strong>g dialogue to guide and support the development ofunderstand<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Language</strong> carries the history .f classroom activity <strong>in</strong>to <strong>its</strong> future. The socio-culturalperspective suggests that if we want to understand the proccss of learn<strong>in</strong>g, we muststudy not only what a learner does but also the activities of parents, teachers, peerswho create ~ <strong>in</strong>deed, constitute - the dynamic context of their learn<strong>in</strong>g experience(Edwards and Mercer, 1987; Hicks, 1996). Rogoff (1990) talks of children be<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a process of ‘guided participation’ <strong>in</strong> thc <strong>in</strong>tellectual life of theircommunitieq, which implies the neccssary <strong>in</strong>volvement of others. For similar reasons,


LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 2554I have described the process of tcach<strong>in</strong>g-and-learn<strong>in</strong>g as ‘the guided construction ofknowledge’ (Mercer, 1995).This is a process which is carried on over time, so that,as the language researcher Janet Mayb<strong>in</strong> (1 994) has put it, the talk on any occasionbetween a teachcr and their regular class of students can be considered part of the‘long conversation’ of their relationship. <strong>Language</strong> is a tool for build<strong>in</strong>g the future outof the past: the mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness of current and future jo<strong>in</strong>t activitics of teachers andlearners depends on the foundations of their common knowledge (Merccr, 2000).Classroom <strong>in</strong>teruction follows implicit ‘ground rules’. The socio-cultural perspectiveemphasises that evcryday human activity depends heavily on participants be<strong>in</strong>g ableto draw on a considerable body of shared knowledge and understand<strong>in</strong>g, based ontheir past shared experience or similar histories of experience. The conventions or‘ground rules’ which ensure that speakers and listeners, writers and readers areoperat<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the same genres of language are rarely made explicit, but so long asparticipants can safely assume shared knowledge, the language of everyday <strong>in</strong>teractionfollows <strong>its</strong> conventional patterns. If the contextual foundations of shared knowledgeare lack<strong>in</strong>g - such as when students’ home backgrounds have not prepared them wellfor mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of the language and culture of the classroom - misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gsmay easily arise and persist unresolved (Heath, 1983; LoCastro, 1997). Mak<strong>in</strong>g the‘ground rules’ of classroom activity explicit can help overcome misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gsand mis<strong>in</strong>terpretations, and there is grow<strong>in</strong>g evidence that students’ progress issignificantly enhanced if teachers do so (Christie, 1990; Mercer, Wegerif and Dawcs1999).ConclusionRecord<strong>in</strong>gs and transcriptions of classroom talk, analysed from a socio-cultural perspective,offer us glimpses of the social, cultural, communicative process of education be<strong>in</strong>g pursuedand, with vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of success, accomplished. They may capture illustrations of thebest practice, <strong>in</strong> which teachers enable students to achieve levels of understand<strong>in</strong>g whichmight never, or at least not nearly so quickly, have been achieved without a ‘scaffold<strong>in</strong>g’guidance; they as often reveal misunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs be<strong>in</strong>g generated, and opportunities forguided development be<strong>in</strong>g squandered. As teachers, as well as researchers, we can learnmuch from what they reveal. It is of course unrealistic to expect any busy teacher to monitorand evaluate every <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> their classroom; but recent research (<strong>in</strong> arcas of thecurriculum other than language teach<strong>in</strong>g) has shown that through a better understand<strong>in</strong>gof the use of language as a pedagogic tool, teachers can help students improve theircurriculum-related learn<strong>in</strong>g and their use of language as a tool for construct<strong>in</strong>g knowledge.(Brown and Pal<strong>in</strong>csar, 1989; Wcgerif, Rojas-Drummond and Mercer, 1999; Mercer, Wegerifand Dawes, 1999.) A socio-cultural perspcctive has only quitc recently been brought tobear on teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the modern language classroom (sec Chapters 5, 16 and19 of this book, by Van Lier, Gibbons and Breen), but I am conv<strong>in</strong>ced that <strong>its</strong> applicationwill have significant practical implications for this field of educational endeavour.ReferencesArthur, J. (1992) ‘<strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> Botswana classrooms: functions and constra<strong>in</strong>ts’. Centre f.r<strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Lfe Work<strong>in</strong>g %pus No.46. University of Lancaster, U.K.


~ (2000)256 NEIL MERCERBrown, A. and Pal<strong>in</strong>csar A.S. (1989) ‘Guided, cooperative lcarn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>dividual knowledgeacquisition’, <strong>in</strong> L. Resnick (ed.) Know<strong>in</strong>g, Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Instruction. Ncw York: LawrenceErlbaum.Bruner, J.S. (1985) ‘Vygotsky: a historical and conccptual perspective’, <strong>in</strong> J.V. Wertsch (ed.)Culture, Communication and Cognition: Qgotskian perspectives. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.Bruner, J.S. (1986) Actual M<strong>in</strong>ds, Possible Worlds. London: Harvard university Press.Camilleri, A. (1994) ‘Talk<strong>in</strong>g bil<strong>in</strong>gually, writ<strong>in</strong>g monol<strong>in</strong>gually’ . Paper prcscntcd at theSociol<strong>in</strong>guistics Symposium, Lancaster University, March 1994.Christie, F. (1990) Literayfr a Chang<strong>in</strong>g World. Mclbourne: Australian Council for EducationalResearch.Edwards, A.D. and Westgate, D. (1994) Investigat<strong>in</strong>g Classroom Talk (Second Edition). London: TheFalmer Press.Edwards, D. (1988) ‘The Meno’, <strong>in</strong> Billig, M., Condor, S., Edwards, D., Cane, M., Middleton,D. and Radley, A. (eds) Ideological Dilemmas: a social psychology .f everyday th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.London: Sage.Edwards, D. and Mercer, N. (1987) Common Knowledge: the development .f understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> theclassroom. London: Methuen / Routledge.Elliot, J. (1991) Action Researchfor Educational Change. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.Gee, J.P. (1996) ‘Vygotsky and current debates <strong>in</strong> education: some dilemmas as afterthoughtsto Discourse, Learn<strong>in</strong>g and School<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> D. Hicks (cd.) Discourse, Learn<strong>in</strong>g and School<strong>in</strong>g.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Heath, S.B. (1983) Ways with Words: language, lfe and work <strong>in</strong> communities and classrooms.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Hicks, D. (1996) ‘<strong>Context</strong>ual enquiries: a discourse-oriented study of classroom learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong>D. Hicks (ed.) Discourse, Learn<strong>in</strong>g and School<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.Jacobson, R. (1990) ‘Allocat<strong>in</strong>g two languagcs as a key feature of a bil<strong>in</strong>gual methodology’, <strong>in</strong>R. Jacobson and C. Faltis (eds) <strong>Language</strong> Distribution Issues <strong>in</strong> Bil<strong>in</strong>gual School<strong>in</strong>g.Clevedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Johnson, R.K. and Lee, P.L.M. (1987) ‘Modes of <strong>in</strong>struction: tcach<strong>in</strong>g strategies and studentsresponses’, <strong>in</strong> R. Lord and H. Cheng (eds) <strong>Language</strong> Education <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong. Hong Kong:The Ch<strong>in</strong>ese University Press.LoCastro, V. (1997) ‘Politeness and pragmatic competence <strong>in</strong> foreign language education’.<strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Research,Vol. 1, No.3, 239-268.Martyn-Jones, M. (1995) ‘Code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> thc classroom’, <strong>in</strong> L. Milroy and P. Muysken (cds)One Speaker, two languages: cross discipl<strong>in</strong>ary perspectives on code-switch<strong>in</strong>g. Carnbridgc:Cambridge University Press.Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J. (1994) ‘Children’s voices: talk, knowledge and idcntity’, <strong>in</strong> Graddol, D, Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J.and Stiercr, B. (eds) Research<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Language</strong> and Literacy <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>. Clevedon:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J., Merccr, N. and Stierer, B. (1992) ‘“Scaffold<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom’, <strong>in</strong>Norman, K. (ed.) Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g Voices. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Mehan, H. (1979) Learn<strong>in</strong>g Lessons:social organization <strong>in</strong> the classroom. Carnbridgc, Mass: HarvardUniversity Press.Mercer, N. (1995) The Guided Construction .f Knowledge: talk amongst teachers and learners.Clevcdon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Mattcrs.Words and M<strong>in</strong>ds: how we use language to th<strong>in</strong>k together. London: Routledge.Mercer, N., Wegerif, R. and Dawes, L. (1999) ‘Children’s talk and the development ofreason<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom’. British Educational Research Journal, 25, 1, 95-1 13.Moll, L. and Dwor<strong>in</strong>, J. (1996) ‘Biliteracy developrncnt <strong>in</strong> classrooms: social dynamics and


LANGUAGE FOR TEACHING A LANGUAGE 257cultural possibilities’, <strong>in</strong> D. Hicks (ed.) Discourse, Learn<strong>in</strong>g and School<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Moore, A. (1 995) The Academic, L<strong>in</strong>guistic and <strong>Social</strong> Development .f Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Pupils <strong>in</strong> SecondaryEducation: issues .f diagnosis, pedagogy and culture. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, The OpenUniversity.Newman, D., Griff<strong>in</strong>, P. and Colc, M. (1989) The Construction Zone. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.Ong, W. (1 982) Orality and Literacy. London: Methuen.Rogoff, B. (1990) Apprenticeship <strong>in</strong> Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: Oxford University Press.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J. and Coulthard, M. (1 975) Towards an analysis ofdiscourse: the <strong>English</strong> used by teachers andpupils. London: Oxford Univcrsity Press.Vygotsky, L.S. (1 962) Thought and <strong>Language</strong>. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. (Orig<strong>in</strong>allypublished <strong>in</strong> Russian <strong>in</strong> 1934.)Wegerif, R., Rojas-Drummond, S. and Mercer, N. (1999) ‘<strong>Language</strong> for the socialconstruction of knowlcdge: compar<strong>in</strong>g classroom talk <strong>in</strong> Mexican pre-schools’ , <strong>Language</strong>andEducation,Vol. 13, No. 2, pp. 133-150.Wertsch, J. (1991) Voices of the M<strong>in</strong>d: a socio-cultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge,Mass. : Harvard University Press.Wood, D. (1988) How children th<strong>in</strong>k and learn. Oxford: Basil Blackwcll.Zentella, A.C. (1981) ‘Tu bien, you could answer me <strong>in</strong> cualquier idioma: Puerto Rican codeswitch<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms’, <strong>in</strong> R. Duran (ed.) Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>Language</strong> and CommunicativeBehavior, pp 109-1 32. Norwood, N. J. : Ablex Publish<strong>in</strong>g Corporation.


Chapter 16Paul<strong>in</strong>e GibbonsLEARNING A NEW REGISTER IN ASECOND LANGUAGEIntroductionOR STUDENTS WHO ARE LEARNING <strong>English</strong> as a second language <strong>in</strong> anF <strong>English</strong> medium school, <strong>English</strong> is both a target and medium of education: they are notonly learn<strong>in</strong>g the dom<strong>in</strong>ant language but they are lcarn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> it and through it as well. Forthcse learners, the construction of curriculum knowledge must go hand <strong>in</strong> hand with thedevelopment of the second language.This chapter illustrates how such <strong>in</strong>tegration can be achieved. In it I argue that learners’current understand<strong>in</strong>gs of a curriculum topic, and their use of familiar ‘everyday’ languageto express these understand<strong>in</strong>gs, should be seen as the basis for the development of themore unfamiliar and academic registers of the school. I show how teacher-student talk,based on shared common experiences, leads to the development of new ways of mean<strong>in</strong>g.I also suggest the usefulness of br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g together, for the purposes of classroom-basedresearch, bodies of knowledge which have rarely overlapped; second language acquisition(SLA) research, neo-Vygotskian socio-cultural approaches to teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g, andsystemic functional l<strong>in</strong>guistics (SFL)The context for the studyThe classroom from which the data derive is <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner city school <strong>in</strong> Sydney. At the timeof the study, twenty three languages were spoken by the children <strong>in</strong> the school. The classconsisted of 30 children aged between 8-10, with all but two children <strong>in</strong> the class com<strong>in</strong>gfrom homes where a language other than <strong>English</strong> was spoken. Many children had been born<strong>in</strong> Australia but entered school with little <strong>English</strong>, others were first generation migrants,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g two children who had arrived <strong>in</strong> Australia with<strong>in</strong> the last year. Generally, suchchildren very quickly bccome adept at us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> face-to-face contexts, where theconversation relates to what is occurr<strong>in</strong>g around them. However, as Cumm<strong>in</strong>s (1 996),Collier (1989) and McKay et al. (1 997) have shown, children who appear ‘fluent’ <strong>in</strong> suchcontexts may still have difficulty <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g the more written-like and subject specificregisters of school, because these more academic registers usually require a much longertimc for developmcnt.The focus of this paper is on thc learn<strong>in</strong>g of a more academic registerby students who are largely fluent <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> face-to-face, everyday communication.


~ note,LEARNING A NEW REGISTER 259The language modelWhere the teach<strong>in</strong>g of a new language is to be <strong>in</strong>tegrated with the teach<strong>in</strong>g of subjectcontent, then program plann<strong>in</strong>g needs to be <strong>in</strong>formed by a model of language which relateslanguage to mean<strong>in</strong>g, and to the context <strong>in</strong> which it is used. This study draws on systemicfunctional grammar (Halliday, 1985) and related descriptions of register theory (Hallidayand Hasan, 1985).A major organis<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of the teach<strong>in</strong>g program described was the construct ofmode (which refers to the channel of the text, whether it is spoken or written) and thenotion of a mode cont<strong>in</strong>uum (Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1984), because it offers a l<strong>in</strong>guistic frameworkaga<strong>in</strong>st which teach<strong>in</strong>g activities can be sequenced from most situationally-dependent (andthus for ESL learners the most easily understood), to least situationally-dependent. Thefollow<strong>in</strong>g four texts illustrate this mode cont<strong>in</strong>uum, and show how certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic featureschange as language becomes <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly closer to written forms.Text 1 : (spoken by three 1 O-year-old students and accompany<strong>in</strong>g action)this . . . no it doesn’t go . . . it doesn’t move . . . try that . . . yes it does . . . a bit . . . thatwon’t . . . won’t work it’s not metal . . . these are the best . . . go<strong>in</strong>g really fast.Text 2: (spoken ly one student about the action, $ter the event)we tried a p<strong>in</strong> . . . a pencil sharpener . . . some iron fil<strong>in</strong>gs and a piece of plastic . . . themagnet didn’t attract the p<strong>in</strong>.Text 3: (written by the same student)Our experiment was to f<strong>in</strong>d out what a magnet attracted. We discovered that a magnetattracts some k<strong>in</strong>ds of metal. It attracted the iron fil<strong>in</strong>gs, but not the p<strong>in</strong>.Text 4: (taken from a child’s encyclopedia)A magnet . . . is able to pick up, or attract, a piece of steel or iron because <strong>its</strong> magnetic fieldflows <strong>in</strong>to the magnet, turn<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong>to a temporary magnet. Magnetic attraction occurs onlybetween ferrous materials.Text 1 is typical of the k<strong>in</strong>d of situationally-dependent language produced <strong>in</strong> face-to-facecontexts. Because the visual context obviates the need to name the referent, exophoricreference is used (this, these, that), and there is a relatively low lexical density, or number of‘content’ words per clause. In Text 2 the context changes, because the student is tell<strong>in</strong>gothers what she learned, and no longer has the science equipment <strong>in</strong> front of her. She mustnow reconstruct the experience through language alone, and so makes explicit theparticipants (we, p<strong>in</strong>, pencil sharpener, ironjl<strong>in</strong>gs, piece of plastic) and process (attract) she isreferr<strong>in</strong>g to. Text 3 is a written text and, s<strong>in</strong>ce the audience is now unseen, it cannot relyon shared assumptions, and so the writer must recreate experience through language alonefor example, the orientation which is needed to provide the context for whatfollows: Our experiment was to . . . InText 4 the major participant (a magnet) is generic: <strong>its</strong>properties are those of all magnets. There is a further <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> lexical density, and thetext <strong>in</strong>cludes a nom<strong>in</strong>alisation, the cod<strong>in</strong>g of a process term as a noun (attraction) which istypical of much written text.While spoken and written language obviously have dist<strong>in</strong>ctive characteristics, thiscont<strong>in</strong>uum of texts illustrates that there is no absolute boundary between them.Technology


andregisters260 PAULINE GIBBONS<strong>in</strong>creases this blurr<strong>in</strong>g. Leav<strong>in</strong>g a detailed message on an answer<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>e, for example,may be quite l<strong>in</strong>guistically demand<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>ce, <strong>in</strong> the absence of two-way contact, and without(<strong>in</strong>itially at least) the shared understand<strong>in</strong>gs and expectations which are implicit <strong>in</strong> twoway,face-to-face communication, we are required to ‘speak aloud’ the lund of language thatwould more usually be written. Thus <strong>in</strong> terms of the mode cont<strong>in</strong>uum it is perhaps moreappropriate to describe texts as ‘more spoken-like’ or ‘more written-like’ , and these arethe terms which will be used here.In many ways the cont<strong>in</strong>uum reflects the process of formal education <strong>its</strong>elf, as studentsare required to move from personal everyday ways of mak<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs towards the sociallyshared discourses of specific discipl<strong>in</strong>es. A second language learner is likely to have fewerdifficulties with produc<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g like text 1, where the situational context <strong>its</strong>elfprovides a support for mean<strong>in</strong>g and there are thus fewer l<strong>in</strong>guistic demands, than with morewritten-like texts, where more lexico-grammatical resources are required. It is worthnot<strong>in</strong>g, too, that when children are expected to write simply on the basis of personalexperiences, they are be<strong>in</strong>g asked to take a very large l<strong>in</strong>guistic step (as can be seen bycompar<strong>in</strong>g text 1 and 3), and one which is beyond the current l<strong>in</strong>guistic resources of somesecond language learners.In the classroom described here, a major focus is on students us<strong>in</strong>g spoken language <strong>in</strong>the way that text 2 illustrates, that is, language which, while spoken, is not embedded <strong>in</strong>the immediate situational context <strong>in</strong> which it occurs. This more ‘written-like’ spokenlanguage can be seen as a bridge between the language associated with experiential activitiesand the more formal ~ often written ~ of the curriculum.The role of talk <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gWhile the importance of talk <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g has long bccn rccognised (Barnes 1976; Bruner1978; Mart<strong>in</strong> et al. 1976), a more recent focus, largely <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the work ofvygotsky,has been on the social and cultural basis for learn<strong>in</strong>g (Mercer 1994, 1995 and Chapter 15of this book; Mayb<strong>in</strong>, Mercer and Stierer 1992; Wells 1992, 1999). A socio-cultural or ‘neo-Vygotskian’ perspective places <strong>in</strong>teractions and the broad social context of learn<strong>in</strong>g at theheart of the learn<strong>in</strong>g process; the classroom is viewed as a place where understand<strong>in</strong>g andknowledge are jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructed, and where learners are guided or ‘apprenticed’ <strong>in</strong>to thebroader understand<strong>in</strong>gs and language of the curriculum and the particular subject discipl<strong>in</strong>e.The notion of apprenticeship <strong>in</strong>to a culture is particularly relevant <strong>in</strong> an ESL school context,where, <strong>in</strong> order to participate <strong>in</strong> society, students must learn to control the dom<strong>in</strong>ant genresthrough which that culture is constructed (Mart<strong>in</strong> 1986; Dclpit 1988; Kalantzis, Cope,Noble and Poynt<strong>in</strong>g 199 1 ).SLA researchers have also shown the significance of <strong>in</strong>teraction for second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g (see for example, Ellis 1985, 199 1, 1994; van Licr 1988, 1996 and Chapter 5 ofthis book; Swa<strong>in</strong> 1995; Swa<strong>in</strong> 2000). Of particular importance are the k<strong>in</strong>ds of on-go<strong>in</strong>gmodifications which occur as mean<strong>in</strong>g is negotiated or clarified (Long 1983; Pica,Youngand Doughty 1986; Pica 1994). Swa<strong>in</strong> (1985, 1995) also argues for the need for‘comprehensible output’, whereby learners pay attention to their own talk, and as a resultproduce more comprehensible, coherent, and syntactically improved discourse. Thisattention to output ‘stretches’ the learner, <strong>in</strong> that s/he is pushed to attend to syntactic aswell as to semantic process<strong>in</strong>g. The classroom implication for this, I suggest, is not thatlanguage ‘form’ per sc should become a major teach<strong>in</strong>g focus, but that it is important, attimes, for learners to have opportunities to use stretches of discourse <strong>in</strong> contexts where


LEARNING A NEW REGISTER 261there is a ‘press’ on their l<strong>in</strong>guistic resources, and where, for the benefit of their listeners,they must focus not only on what they wish to say but on how they are say<strong>in</strong>g it.One clear teach<strong>in</strong>g implication of these various studies is that the degree to which aclassroom is facilitative of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g depends largely on how classroomdiscourse is constructed. Traditional classroom <strong>in</strong>teractions consist<strong>in</strong>g of sequences of<strong>in</strong>itiation, response, and feedback moves (S<strong>in</strong>clair and Coulthard 1975; Edwards and Mercer1987) may, <strong>in</strong> fact, deprive learners of just those <strong>in</strong>teractional features and <strong>in</strong>teractiveconditions which SLA research suggests are enabl<strong>in</strong>g factors <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Whenteacher <strong>in</strong>itiations lead to s<strong>in</strong>gle word or s<strong>in</strong>gle clause responses, there is little opportunityfor learner language to he ‘stretched’, or for the production of comprehensible output. Aclassroom program which is supportive of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g must therefore createopportunities for more dialogic <strong>in</strong>teractional patterns to occur (see van Lier 1996, fordetailed discussion of these issues).The dataThe classroom contextBased on the science topic of magnetism, teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g activities were planned toreflect po<strong>in</strong>ts along the mode cont<strong>in</strong>uum, the assumption be<strong>in</strong>g that this would offer a logicaldevelopment <strong>in</strong> terms of language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Thus students <strong>in</strong>itially participated <strong>in</strong> smallgrouplearn<strong>in</strong>g experiences where the language used was clearly situationally-embedded.This was followed by a teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g session, where, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with theteacher, each group shared their learn<strong>in</strong>g with the whole class. Talk<strong>in</strong>g with the teacherabout what had been learned, s<strong>in</strong>ce ths did not <strong>in</strong>volve the use of the concrete materials,led to a mode shift towards more written-like language, and provided a bridge <strong>in</strong>to thewrit<strong>in</strong>g, which was the f<strong>in</strong>al activity of the cycle and l<strong>in</strong>guistically the most demand<strong>in</strong>g.Thisthree-part cycle was repeated several times dur<strong>in</strong>g the course of the development of theunit of work. The three stages are described below, together with representative texts fromeach stage. Taken as a sequence, they illustrate how language development can evolvethrough jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructed discourse.Stage 1In many primary schools it is usual for students to rotate through a number of activities overthe course of one or two lessons. Howcvcr, such an organisational structure may negate anyauthentic purpose for report<strong>in</strong>g back to others, s<strong>in</strong>ce children are likely to share very similarexperiences. Here, an attempt was made to set up a genu<strong>in</strong>e communicative situation byhav<strong>in</strong>g each group of children work at dgerent (though related) science experiments; thusthey held different <strong>in</strong>formation from other class members. In <strong>its</strong> communicative structurethe classroom organisation was based on an important pr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>in</strong> second language taskdesign: the notion of an <strong>in</strong>formation ‘gap’ and the need for <strong>in</strong>formation exchange (Long1989).One experiment consisted of a small polystyrene block <strong>in</strong>to which a number of paddlepop(ice-lolly) sticks had been <strong>in</strong>serted to enclose a bar magnet. The students were askedto test the effect ofa second magnet. (When the second magnet is placed above the first <strong>in</strong>a position <strong>in</strong> which they are repell<strong>in</strong>g, repulsion causes the second magnet to be suspended<strong>in</strong> mid-air.)The texts bclow (1.1 and 1.2) occurred as students wcrc engaged <strong>in</strong> this activity.Prior to beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g the activity, they were told that they would later describe and attemptto expla<strong>in</strong> what happened to thc rest of the class ([. . .] marks an obvious pause).


262 PAULINE GIBBONSText 1.1Hannah: try . . . the other wayPatrick: like thatHannah: north pole fac<strong>in</strong>g downJoanna: we tried thatPeter: oh!Hannah: it stays up!Patrick: magic!Peter: let’s show thc othersJoanna: mad!Peter:Patrick:Peter:I’ll put north pole fac<strong>in</strong>g north pole . . . see what happenthat’s what we just didyeah . . . like this . . . lookThe dialogue cont<strong>in</strong>ues for several m<strong>in</strong>utes longer as the students ty clgerent positions for the magnet,and then they beg<strong>in</strong> to formulate an explanation.Text 1.2Hannah: can I try that? . . . I know why . . . I know why . . . that’s like . . . because the northPeter:pole is on this side and that north pole’s there . . . so they don’t stick togetherwhat like this? yeahHannah: yeah see because the north pole on this side . but turn it on the other . . . this sidelike that . . . turn it that way . . . yeahPeter: and it will stickHannah: and it will stick because. look . . . the north pole’s on that side because . . .Peter: the north pole’s on that side yeahStage 2The overall aim of the teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g was to extend children’s l<strong>in</strong>guistic resourcesand focus on aspects of the specific discourse of science. As the teacher expressed it tothe children: we’re ty<strong>in</strong>g to talk like scientists. It was anticipated that the report<strong>in</strong>g stagewould create a context for students to ‘rehearse’ language structures which were closerto written discourse. Before the report<strong>in</strong>g began, there had been a short teacher-leddiscussion focus<strong>in</strong>g on the specific lcxis the children would need to use, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the lexicalitem repel.In the text below (Text 2), Hannah is expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g what she learned.Text 2STUDENTS1TEACHERtry to tell them what you learned . . .OK . . . (to Hannah) yes?


LEARNING A NEW REGISTER 2632 when I put/ when you put . . . when youput a magnet . . . on top of a magnet andthe north pole poles are. . .(7 second pause, Hannah is clearly hav<strong>in</strong>gd@culty In express<strong>in</strong>g what she wants to say)3yes yes you’re do<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>e . . . you put onemagnet on top of another . . .4 and and the north poles are together erem the magnet . . . repels the magneter . . . the magnet and the other magnet. . . sort of floats <strong>in</strong> the air?5(The teacher <strong>in</strong>vites other contributions, andthen asks Hannah to expla<strong>in</strong> it aga<strong>in</strong>.)I th<strong>in</strong>k that was very well told . . . verywell told . . . do you have anyth<strong>in</strong>g to addto that Charlene?6now listen . . . now Hannah expla<strong>in</strong> oncemore. . . alright Hannah . . . excuse mceverybody (rega<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g classes attention) . . .listen aga<strong>in</strong> to her explanation7 the two north poles are lean<strong>in</strong>g togcthcrand the magnet on the bottom is repell<strong>in</strong>gthe magnet on top so that the magnet onthe top is sort of. . . float<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the air8so that these two magnets are repell<strong>in</strong>g(said with emphasis) each other and . . .(demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g) look at the force of it.Stage 3After the students had taken part <strong>in</strong> the report<strong>in</strong>g session, they wrote a response <strong>in</strong> theirjournals to the question ‘what have you learned?’ These were later used as a source of<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> the writ<strong>in</strong>g of more formal reports about magnets. The <strong>in</strong>terest of thejournals here, however, is that they provide some evidence of ‘uptake’, <strong>in</strong> that they reflectword<strong>in</strong>gs which occurred <strong>in</strong> the process of jo<strong>in</strong>tly-produced student-teacher discourse. Thetexts below <strong>in</strong>clude Hannah’s own entry, and an entry from another student who had listenedto Hannah’s talk with her teacher.Text 3.1 (Hannah’s journal entry)I found it very <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g that when you stuck at least 8 paddle pop sticks <strong>in</strong> a piece ofpolystyrene, and then put a magnet with the North and South pole <strong>in</strong> the oval and put


264 PAULINE GIBBONSanother magnet with the north and south pole on top, the magnet on the bottom will repelthe magnet on the top and the magnet on the top would look like it is float<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the air.Text 3.2 (another student’s journal entry)The th<strong>in</strong>g made out of polystyrene with paddle pop sticks, one group put one magnet fac<strong>in</strong>gnorth and another magnet on top fac<strong>in</strong>g north as well and they repelled each other. It lookedlike the top magnet was float<strong>in</strong>g up <strong>in</strong> the air.DiscussionStage 1 textsThe small group activities produced situationally embedded, ‘here-and now’ language. Note,for example, the exophoric references: like that; like thls; that way <strong>in</strong> text 1.1 (Thesereferences, of course, carry mean<strong>in</strong>gs which, <strong>in</strong> the absence of a visual context, must bercalised <strong>in</strong> a different way, and it is precisely this aspect of discourse which causes Hannah,and many of the other students, difficulty <strong>in</strong> the later report<strong>in</strong>g session.)Talk at this stage also foregrounds the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal aspects of language. Students areconcerned with direct<strong>in</strong>g each other’s actions, rather than exchang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation. Text1.1 is about social <strong>in</strong>teraction as much as it is about magnets: subject specific language issimply not necessary for communication between the <strong>in</strong>teractants because of the visual faceto-facecontext <strong>in</strong> which the discourse occurs. There arc also personal comments <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gaffect, such as the expression of attitude and feel<strong>in</strong>gs: <strong>in</strong> this text, magic! mad! Participantsarc generally human and frequently thematised, and they relate to thc <strong>in</strong>teractantsthemselves: We tried that; I’ll put north pole fac<strong>in</strong>g north pole.What is important about the activities, however, is that they allowed children to exploreand develop together certa<strong>in</strong> scientific understand<strong>in</strong>gs (the position of the poles is significantto the movement of the magnets). As the discourse progresses (text 1.2), <strong>in</strong>dividualutterances become longer and more explicit, and this occurs as the students beg<strong>in</strong> toformulate explanations for what they see (notc the logical connectives so, because).Interpersonal elements are reduced; there is now a non-human participant (the north pole)and this, rather than the <strong>in</strong>teractants themselves, becomes the topic of conversation. Thecognitive challenge <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>struction to ‘try to expla<strong>in</strong> what you see’ mayhave been significant here, s<strong>in</strong>ce it extended the task from simply ‘do<strong>in</strong>g’ to ‘do<strong>in</strong>g andth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g’. This explicit focus on th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g is an important one <strong>in</strong> the light of this type ofteach<strong>in</strong>g context, where a teacher must balance the need for suitably high levels of cognitivelearn<strong>in</strong>g with learners’ relatively low levels of <strong>English</strong>, and where learn<strong>in</strong>g activities aimedat development of the second language must also be l<strong>in</strong>ked to cognitive growth. Clearlywith<strong>in</strong> these texts there is evidence of children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g of science: the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs of anunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of why the magnets are behav<strong>in</strong>g as they are, and attempts to hypothesiseabout the causal relations <strong>in</strong>volved. Through the k<strong>in</strong>d of exploratory talk which beg<strong>in</strong>s tobe ebident here <strong>in</strong> the small group work, “knowledge is made more publicly accountableand reason<strong>in</strong>g is more visible” (Wegerif and Mercer, 1996: 51).From the po<strong>in</strong>t of view of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g, it is also important to note thatthe children developed some understand<strong>in</strong>g? about magnets before they were expected tounderstand and use more scientific discourse. For example, at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the report<strong>in</strong>g


LEARNING A NEW REGISTER 265session, the teacher <strong>in</strong>troduces the term repel at a time when students had already cxprcsscdthis mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> familiar everyday language, us<strong>in</strong>g terms such as it pushes away; it feels like astrong w<strong>in</strong>d. There is some parallel here to the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple with<strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual programs whichsuggests that learn<strong>in</strong>g should occur first <strong>in</strong> L1 as a basis to learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> L2, but here the issueis one of register rather than language.Stage 2 textsDriver makes the important po<strong>in</strong>t about science education that ‘activity by <strong>its</strong>elf is notenough. It is the sense that is made of it that matters’ (Driver, 1983: 49). In Stage 2 textswe see the teacher work<strong>in</strong>g with the children to ‘make sense’ of the activities <strong>in</strong> which theyhave been engaged, by help<strong>in</strong>g them reconstruct their experiences and develop sharedunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs through language. Wegerif and Mercer suggest that it is through be<strong>in</strong>gencouraged and enabled ‘to clearly describc cvents, to account for outcomes and consolidatewhat they have learned <strong>in</strong> words’ that children are helped to ‘understand and ga<strong>in</strong> access toeducated discourse’ (Wcgerif and Mercer, 1996: 53).Text 2 illustrates one type of situation<strong>in</strong> which this process can occur.The teacher’s role <strong>in</strong> these episodes was crucial; the texts show how her <strong>in</strong>teractionswith <strong>in</strong>dividual students provided a ‘scaffold’ for their attempts, allow<strong>in</strong>g for communicationto proceed while giv<strong>in</strong>g the learner access to new l<strong>in</strong>guistic data. InText 2, the <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween teacher and students is different <strong>in</strong> several small but important respects from thetraditional IRF pattern, but these modifications appear to have significant effects on the<strong>in</strong>teraction as a whole. Typically, the IRF pattern is realised <strong>in</strong> fairly predictable ways,frequently <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a teacher known-answer question, followed by a student answer (oftenbrief), and followed by a teacher evaluation relat<strong>in</strong>g to the correctness or otherwise of theanswer. InText 2, the <strong>in</strong>teractions approximate more closely what occurs <strong>in</strong> L1 adult-child<strong>in</strong>teractions outside of the formal teach<strong>in</strong>g context (see for example, Halliday 1975; Wells1981 ; Pa<strong>in</strong>ter 1985). The teacher beg<strong>in</strong>s the exchange with <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g students to relate whatthey have learned, rather than with a ‘known answer’ or display question. While teachers’questions are often framed <strong>in</strong> ways which do not allow for students to make extendedresponses (Dillon, 1990), here, by contrast, the teacher sets up a context where it is thestudents who <strong>in</strong>itiate the specific topic of the exchange. As Ellis (1996) shows, when learner?<strong>in</strong>itiate what they wish to talk about, language learn<strong>in</strong>g is facilitated because they enter thediscourse on their own terms, rather than respond<strong>in</strong>g to a specific request for <strong>in</strong>formationfrom the teacher. In &us text, the student takes on the role of what Berry refers to as ‘primaryknower’ (Berry 1981). Although of course it is the teacher who is <strong>in</strong> control of theknowledge associated with the overall thematic development of the unit of work, the<strong>in</strong>dividual exchanges locate that control <strong>in</strong> the student.The reciprocity and mutuality <strong>in</strong> thespeaker roles leads to Hannah produc<strong>in</strong>g longer stretches of discourse than often occurs <strong>in</strong>classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction. As is typical <strong>in</strong> these rcport<strong>in</strong>g sessions, the teacher ‘leads frombeh<strong>in</strong>d’, and whilc follow<strong>in</strong>g Hannah’s lead and accept<strong>in</strong>g as a valid contribution the<strong>in</strong>formation she gives, the teacher also recasts it, provid<strong>in</strong>g alternative l<strong>in</strong>guistic forms toencode student mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> more context-appropriate ways.It is also clear that teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g encourages learner language to be ‘pushed’.(As one student commented as she struggled to expla<strong>in</strong> what she had done: I can’t suy itMISS!). Hannah is go<strong>in</strong>g beyond what is unproblcmatic for her but, because she is allowed asecond attempt, she has an opportunity to produce more comprehensible output. Hannah’ssecond attempt at her explanation is considerably less hesitant and syntactically morecomplete than her first, and is produced th~s time without the help of the teacher.Vygotsky’s


266 PAULINE GIBBONSnotion of the ‘ZPD’ is significant here.Vygotsky suggests that learn<strong>in</strong>g occurs, with supportfrom those more expert, <strong>in</strong> the learner’s ‘zone of proximal development’ (Vygotsky 1978),that is, at the ‘outer edges’ of a learner’s current abilities. In 1.2, Hannah appears to havereached her own zone of proximal development for this task, s<strong>in</strong>ce she hesitates for aconsiderable time, and can presumably go no further alone. The recast<strong>in</strong>g and support shereceives from the teacher (1.3) then appears to be precisely timed for learn<strong>in</strong>g to occur andto assist Hannah to cont<strong>in</strong>ue.As Text 2 illustrates, the report<strong>in</strong>g context also gives students opportunities to producelonger stretches of discourse which are more written-like than those which occurred <strong>in</strong> thesmall group work. Often this required the teacher to <strong>in</strong>crease ‘wait time’, on occasions foras long as eight seconds. Research suggests that when teachers ask questions of students,they typically wait one second or less for the students to beg<strong>in</strong> a reply, but that when teacherswait for three or more seconds, there are significant changes <strong>in</strong> student use of language and<strong>in</strong> the attitudes and expectations of both students and teachers (Rowe, 1986). We cansurmise that the importance of wait time is <strong>in</strong>creased for students who are formulat<strong>in</strong>gresponses <strong>in</strong> a language they do not fully control. Perhaps equally important, students wereable to complete what they wanted to say and as a result were positioned as successful<strong>in</strong>teractants and learners. In addition, s<strong>in</strong>ce it is the immediate need of the learner whichis <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g to a large extent the teacher’s choice of actual word<strong>in</strong>g, it would seem likelythat this word<strong>in</strong>g will be more salient to the learner - more likely to be noticed ~ than if ithad occurred <strong>in</strong> a context which was less immediate. (For discussion of the significance of‘notic<strong>in</strong>g’ <strong>in</strong> second language development, see Ellis, 1994).Another significant mode shift occurred towards the end of most report<strong>in</strong>g sessions,where the teacher used children’s personal knowledge to show how generalisations mightbe generated. Her questions at this po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>cluded, for example: can you see someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>common with all these experiences? what’s the same about all these experiments?Such questions rcquirc the students to do more than simply produce a personal recountof what they did; they must now recontextualise this <strong>in</strong> terms of the teacher’s question.What they say is now characterised by a shift towards generalisation, an <strong>in</strong>creased use offield specific lexis, and the thematisation of field-related participants; the children themselvesare no longer the ‘actors’ <strong>in</strong> the text:the north pole .f the magnet sticks . . . attracts . . . the second magnet . . . the south pole .fthe second magnet.$you put the south and north together then t hy will . . . attract but $you put north and northor south and south . . . together . . . t hy won’t stick . . . attract.Thus the teacher aga<strong>in</strong> mediates between children’s <strong>in</strong>dividual experiences and the broaderknowledge and discourse <strong>in</strong>to which they are be<strong>in</strong>g apprenticed, locat<strong>in</strong>g these experienceswith<strong>in</strong> a larger framework of mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Stage 2 texts, then, both <strong>in</strong> the way language isused, and <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of knowledge which is constructed, serve to create a ‘bridge’ forlearners between personal experiential ways of know<strong>in</strong>g and the public discourse of sharedand socially constructed knowledge.Stop 3 textsMany of the journals reflected what had been said <strong>in</strong> the teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g sessions.Students <strong>in</strong>cluded word<strong>in</strong>g which they had used <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with the teacher, or which


awhichLEARNING A NEW REGISTER 267had been part of the teacher’s recast<strong>in</strong>g, and this was particularly evident when the studentsthemselves had reformulated their own talk. Compare, for example, Hannah’s written text(3.1) with what she says <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with the teacher. There is also evidence that thereport<strong>in</strong>g back sessions <strong>in</strong>fluenced not only the <strong>in</strong>teractants themselves but also those wholistened to the <strong>in</strong>teractions as part of the larger group: Text 3.2 was written by a studentwho had not taken part <strong>in</strong> this particular experiment herself.ConclusionsWhile the research I have described illustrates the value of ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g by do<strong>in</strong>g’ (especiallyfor second language learners where concrete experiences help to make languagecomprehensible), it also illustrates the critical role of teacher-learner talk <strong>in</strong> children’slearn<strong>in</strong>g and language development, and the way that such scaffolded <strong>in</strong>teractions can beg<strong>in</strong>to co-construct a new register. Teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular appears to offer arich potential for second language dcvclopment.The research also suggests that <strong>in</strong> analys<strong>in</strong>g how <strong>in</strong>teractions are made comprehensibleto ESL students <strong>in</strong> the classroom context, we need to look further than the l<strong>in</strong>guistic featuresof the <strong>in</strong>teractions themselves (for example the simplicity or otherwise of syntacticstructures), and exam<strong>in</strong>e the on-go<strong>in</strong>g context <strong>in</strong> which those <strong>in</strong>teractions are situated. Ofparticular significance with<strong>in</strong> the sequence of lessons was the scaffold<strong>in</strong>g of new language.Occurr<strong>in</strong>g as this did gter students had already developed some understand<strong>in</strong>g of keyconcepts through the small group work, it allowed the teacher to use new word<strong>in</strong>gs andways of mean<strong>in</strong>g ~ new register ~ were then more readily <strong>in</strong>terpretable by thestudents.The broader pr<strong>in</strong>ciple is that language which would normally be beyond students’comprehension is likely to be understood when students can br<strong>in</strong>g their experiences andunderstand<strong>in</strong>gs as a basis for <strong>in</strong>terpretation. The degree to which <strong>in</strong>teractions arecomprehensible for ESL students should therefore be related not only to the <strong>in</strong>teractionalfeatures themselves, and to the immediate situational context <strong>in</strong> which they occur, but alsoto what has preceded them ~ <strong>in</strong> this case the learn<strong>in</strong>g which the students had ga<strong>in</strong>ed throughparticipation <strong>in</strong> the small group work. For second language learners, the ‘long conversation’(Mayb<strong>in</strong> 1994; Mercer 1995) is an important part of the total teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g context,because students and teacher ‘relate discourse to context, and build through time a jo<strong>in</strong>tframe of rcfcrcncc’ (Edwards and Mercer, 1995). As Wong-Fillmore states <strong>in</strong> her study ofan ESL k<strong>in</strong>dergarten class, “the prior experience becomes a context for <strong>in</strong>tcrprct<strong>in</strong>g thcnew cxpcriencc . . . prior experiences serve as the contexts with<strong>in</strong> which the languagebc<strong>in</strong>g used is to be understood” (Wong-Fillmore 1985).The overall sequence of activities also presents a challenge to more traditional ways ofsequenc<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g and lcarn<strong>in</strong>g activities <strong>in</strong> the second language classroom, where a unitvery often beg<strong>in</strong>s with the pre-teach<strong>in</strong>g of vocabulary or a grammatical structure. While thisapproach may be appropriate <strong>in</strong> some teach<strong>in</strong>g contexts, it is underp<strong>in</strong>ned by the notionthat learners must first ‘learn’ language before they can ‘use’ it. Aside from questions aboutthe nature of language and language lcarn<strong>in</strong>g which this sets up, it is also clear that it is anapproach which cannot be easily applied to the school ESL context, where children mustfrom the outset use their target language <strong>in</strong> specific social contexts and for specific purposes.In this class, students used their current language resources at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the unit whilethe focus on new language occurred at later stages, a scqucnce which allowed for studentsto build on their exist<strong>in</strong>g understand<strong>in</strong>gs and language, and to l<strong>in</strong>k old learn<strong>in</strong>g with new;<strong>in</strong> effect to movc successfully towards target texts, rather than beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with them.


~ (1991)~ (1994)~ (1985)268 PAULINE GIBBONSThe research I have described also <strong>in</strong>dicates the significance for language learn<strong>in</strong>g ofthe <strong>in</strong>tertextual nature of classroom language: how one text is understood or produced <strong>in</strong>relation to another. A wide range of <strong>in</strong>tertextual relationships exist <strong>in</strong> all classrooms,between, for example, what a teacher says and what students are expected to read; whatstudents listen to and what they are expected to write; the discourse of the lesson and thetexts students are expected to work with for homework; and the familiar language or dialectof the home and the less familiar language of the school. A consideration of how these l<strong>in</strong>ksare made <strong>in</strong>tertextually - and recognis<strong>in</strong>g where l<strong>in</strong>guistic ‘bridges’ are miss<strong>in</strong>g - mightoffer <strong>in</strong>sights for the plann<strong>in</strong>g of school programs for all learners, and help to suggest thek<strong>in</strong>d of l<strong>in</strong>guistic support most relevant for students less familiar with the language of theclassroom.A f<strong>in</strong>al po<strong>in</strong>t concerns the model of language drawn on <strong>in</strong> my research. A languagemodel which addresses the relationship between context and mean<strong>in</strong>g, and which isconcerned therefore with more than grammatical competence, provides a significantdimension to the plann<strong>in</strong>g of ESL program3 and dcsign and sequenc<strong>in</strong>g of learn<strong>in</strong>gactivities.Further classroom-based studies are needed <strong>in</strong>to the language learn<strong>in</strong>g processes ofschool-aged ESL learners, if educators are to develop more theoretically <strong>in</strong>formed andequitable curricula and pedagogy. This task requires researchers to take a more<strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary approach to research <strong>in</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms, one which draws onseveral theoretical and methodological l<strong>in</strong>es of enquiry and which is underp<strong>in</strong>ned by a socialview of learn<strong>in</strong>g and a model of language-<strong>in</strong>-context.ReferencesBarnes, D. (1976) From Communication to Curriculum. Harmondsworth: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Berry, M. (198 1) ‘Systemic l<strong>in</strong>guistics and discourse analysis: a multi-layered approach toexchange structure’, <strong>in</strong> M. Coulthard and M. Montgomery (eds) Studies <strong>in</strong> DiscourseAnalysis. London: Routlcdgc and Kegan.Bruner, J. (1978) ‘The role of dialogue <strong>in</strong> language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> A. S<strong>in</strong>clair, R. Jarvella, andW. Levclt (cds) The Child’s Conception oflanguage. New York: Spr<strong>in</strong>gcr-Verlag.Collier, V. (1989) ‘How long? A synthesis of rcscarch <strong>in</strong> academic achicvcmcnt <strong>in</strong> a secondlanguage’. TESOL Quarterly, 23, 509-531.Cumm<strong>in</strong>s, J. (1996) Negotiat<strong>in</strong>g Identities: Educationfor Empowerment <strong>in</strong> a Diverse Society. OntarioCA: California Association for Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Education.Dclpit, L. (1988) ‘The silenced dialogue: power and pedagogy <strong>in</strong> cducat<strong>in</strong>g other people’schildren’. Harvard Educational Review, 58(3), 280-298.Dillon, J. (1990) The Practice $Question<strong>in</strong>g. London: Routledge.Driver, R. (1983) The Pupil as Scientist? Milton Keynes: Open University Press.Edwards, D., and Mercer, N. (1987) Common Knowledge: The Development .f Understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> theClassroom. London: Methuen.Ellis, R. (1 985) Understand<strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.‘The <strong>in</strong>tcraction hypothesis: a critical evaluation’, <strong>in</strong> E. Sadtono (ed.), <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition and the Second/Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Classroom. S<strong>in</strong>gapore: Anthology Series 28,SEAMEO Regional Languagc Centre.The Study $Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Prcss.Halliday, M. (1975) Learn<strong>in</strong>g How to Mean: Explorations <strong>in</strong> the Development ofLanguqe. London:Arnold.An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Edward Arnold.


~ (1~ (1LEARNING A NEW REGISTER 269Halliday, M., and Hasan, R. (1985) <strong>Language</strong>, <strong>Context</strong> and Text. Geelong Victoria: Deak<strong>in</strong>University Press.Kalantzis, M., Cope, B., Noble, G., and Poynt<strong>in</strong>g, S. (1991) Cultures of School<strong>in</strong>g: PedagogiesforCultural Dflerence and <strong>Social</strong> Access. London: Falmer Press.Long, M. (1983) ‘Native speaker/non native speaker conversations and the negotiation ofcomprehensible <strong>in</strong>put’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 4, 126-141.Mart<strong>in</strong>, J. (1984) ‘<strong>Language</strong>, register and genre’, <strong>in</strong> F. Christie (ed.) Children Writ<strong>in</strong>g, StudyGuide. Geelong, Victoria: Deak<strong>in</strong> University Press.986) Secret <strong>English</strong>: discourse technology <strong>in</strong> a junior secondary school. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs from theWork<strong>in</strong>g Conference on <strong>Language</strong> <strong>in</strong> Education, Macquarie University.Mart<strong>in</strong>, N., Williams, P., Wild<strong>in</strong>g, J., Hemm<strong>in</strong>gs, S., and Medway, P. (1976). Understand<strong>in</strong>gChildren Talk<strong>in</strong>g. London: Pengu<strong>in</strong>.Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J. (1994) ‘Children’s voices: talk, knowledge and identity’, <strong>in</strong> D. Graddol, J. Mayb<strong>in</strong>and B. Stierer (eds) Research<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Language</strong> and Literacy <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>s. Clevedon:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Mayb<strong>in</strong>, J., Mercer, N., and Stierer, B. (1992) ‘Scaffold<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom’, <strong>in</strong>K. Norman (ed.) Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g Voices: The Work ofthe National Oracy Project. London: Hodderand Stoughton.McKay, P., Davies, A., Devl<strong>in</strong>, B., Clayton, J., Oliver, R., and Zammit, S. (1997) The Bil<strong>in</strong>gualInterface Project Report. Canberra: Department of Employment, Education, Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g andYouth Affairs.Mercer, N. (1994) “eo-Vygotskian theory and classroom education’, <strong>in</strong> B. Sticrer andJ. Mayb<strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong>, Literacy and Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Educational Practice. Clevedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gualMatters..~ (1995) The Guided Construction $Knowledge: Talk Amongst Teachers and Learners. Clevedon:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Pa<strong>in</strong>ter, C. (1985) Learn<strong>in</strong>g the Mother Tongue. Geelong, Victoria: Deak<strong>in</strong> University Press.Pica, T. (1994) ‘Research on negotiation: what does it reveal about second language learn<strong>in</strong>gconditions, processes and outcomes?’ <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g, 44,493-527.Pica, T., Young, R., and Doughty, C. (1987) ‘The impact of <strong>in</strong>teraction on comprehension’.TESOL Quarterly, 21(4), 737-758.Rowe, M. (1986) ‘Wait time: slow<strong>in</strong>g down may bc a way of speed<strong>in</strong>g up’. Journal qfEacherEducation, 37,43-50.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J., and Coulthard, R. (1975) Towards an Analysis ofDiscourse: the <strong>English</strong> Used by Teachersand Pupils. London: Oxford University Press.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1985) ‘Communicative competence: some roles of comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put andcomprehensible output <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> development’, <strong>in</strong> S. Gass and C. Madden (cds) Input <strong>in</strong>Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Newbury House.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1995) ‘Three functions of output <strong>in</strong> second language learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> G. Cook andB. Seidlehofer (eds) Pr<strong>in</strong>ciple and Practice <strong>in</strong> Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics: Studies <strong>in</strong> Honour .f H.G.Widdowson. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (2000) ‘The output hypothesis and beyond: rncdiat<strong>in</strong>g acquisition throughcollaborative dialogue’, <strong>in</strong> J. Lantolf (ed.) Sociocultural Theory and Second <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: Oxford university Press.van Lier, L. (1988) The Classroom and the <strong>Language</strong> Learner. Harlow: Longman.996) Interaction <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Language</strong> Curriculum: Awareness, Autonomy and Authenticity. London:Longman.Vygotsky, L. (I 978) M<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> Society: The Development .f Higher Pychological Processes. London:Harvard University Press.Wegerif, R., and Mercer, N. (1996) ‘Computers and reason<strong>in</strong>g through talk <strong>in</strong> the classroom’.<strong>Language</strong> and Education, 10(1), 47-64.


~ (1992)~ (1270 PAULINE GIBBONSWells, G. (1 981) Learn<strong>in</strong>g through Interaction: the Study of <strong>Language</strong> Development. Cambridge:Cambridge university Press.‘The centrality of talk <strong>in</strong> education’, <strong>in</strong> K. Norman (ed.) Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gVoices:The Workofthe National Oracy Project. London: Hodder and Stoughton.999) Dialogic Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Wong-Fillmore, L. (1985) ‘When does teacher talk work as <strong>in</strong>put?’, <strong>in</strong> S. Gass and C. Madden(eds) Input <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition. Rowley MA: Newbury House.


Chapter 17Angel M. Y. L<strong>in</strong>DOING-ENGLISH-LESSONS IN THEREPRODUCTION OR TRANSFORMATION OFSOCIAL WORLDS?1 IntroductionHIS ARTICLE TELLS A STORY of four classrooms, situated <strong>in</strong> differentT socioeconomic backgrounds. Draw<strong>in</strong>g on the theoretical notions of cultural capital,habitus, symbolic violence, and creative, discursive agency as analytic tools, the story unfoldswitness<strong>in</strong>g the classroom dilemmas <strong>in</strong> which students and teachers found themselves, aswell as the creative, discursive strategies which they used to cope with these dilemmas.Theimplications of their strategies are discussed with reference to the question of whether do<strong>in</strong>g<strong>English</strong> lessons contributes to the reproduction or <strong>in</strong> the transformation of the students’social worlds.Statements about the global spread of <strong>English</strong> and <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g socioeconomicimportance <strong>in</strong> the world have almost become c1ichi.s. On colorful banners celebrat<strong>in</strong>g theTESOLAnnual Convention <strong>in</strong> Chicago streets <strong>in</strong> 1996 was written the eye-catch<strong>in</strong>g missionslogan, “<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>English</strong> to the World”. Indeed, <strong>English</strong> seems to have become a preciouscommodity <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly demanded by the world, andTESOL practitioners and researchersseem to be striv<strong>in</strong>g to meet the demand of the world market with all our professionalism.InTESOL journals and annual conventions, practitioners and researchers share their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsabout methods, approachcs, material designs that are effective.However, apart from the technical concern of efficiency <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>its</strong>eems that a far morc diverse range of questions needs to be addressed which <strong>in</strong>cludesquestions such as whether, and if yes, how, <strong>English</strong> is implicated <strong>in</strong> the reproduction of social<strong>in</strong>equalities <strong>in</strong> different contexts <strong>in</strong> the world. As regards the global <strong>in</strong>fluence of <strong>English</strong>,Pennycook (1994) po<strong>in</strong>ts out both the global dom<strong>in</strong>ant position of <strong>English</strong> and thesocioeconomic, cultural and political embeddedness of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the world. Access (or lackof it) to <strong>English</strong> often affects the social mobility and life chances of many children and adultsnot spealung <strong>English</strong> as their first or second language. The classroom <strong>in</strong> many places <strong>in</strong> theworld is a key site for the reproduction of social identities and unequal relations of power(Martyn-Jones and Hcller, 1996). It is also likely that many students <strong>in</strong> the world hold anambivalent, want-hate relationship with <strong>English</strong> and the classroom becomes a site forstudents’ struggles and oppositional practices which, however, often lead students to


272 ANGEL M. Y. LINparticipate <strong>in</strong> their own dom<strong>in</strong>ation (c.g. see Canagarajah, Chapter 13 of this book). Thischapter is written forTESOL practitioners and researchers who want to listen to more ofthe lived stories of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the world and who share a similar concern <strong>in</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>g waysof do<strong>in</strong>g TESOL that do not participate <strong>in</strong> the reproduction of student disadvantage.2 A theoretical preamble: cultural capital, symbolic violence, andcreative, discursive agencySome theoretical notions that can serve as analytical tools for achiev<strong>in</strong>g a greaterunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of social phenomena of reproduction are discussed <strong>in</strong> this section. Givenlimited space, what goes below must be treated as a highly synoptic characterization andthe <strong>in</strong>terested reader is urged to consult the references themselves for a more detailedaccount.Cultural capitalThis is a concept from Bourdieu (Bourdieu, 1973; Bourdieu and Passeron, 1977; Bourdieu,1977; Bourdieu, 199 1) referr<strong>in</strong>g to language use, skills, and orientations/dispositionsattitudedschemes of perception (also called “habitus”) that a child is endowed with by virtueof socialization <strong>in</strong> her/his family and community. Bourdieu’s argument is that their familialsocialization bestows on children of the socioeconomic elite the right k<strong>in</strong>d of cultural capitalfor school success (i.e., their habitus becomes their cultural capital). A recurrent theme <strong>in</strong>Bourdieu’s works is that children from disadvantaged groups, with a habitus <strong>in</strong>compatiblewith that presupposed <strong>in</strong> school, are not compet<strong>in</strong>g with equal start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>ts with childrenof the socioeconomic elite; hence the reproduction of social stratification. The notion ofcultural capital has been used by educationists (e.g., Delpit, 1988; Luke, 1996) to describethe disadvantaged position of ethnic and l<strong>in</strong>guistic m<strong>in</strong>orities and to problematize the notionthat state education <strong>in</strong> modern societies is built on meritocracy and equal opportunity.Symbolic violenceAnother recurrent theme <strong>in</strong> Bourdicu’s works concerns how the disadvantag<strong>in</strong>g effect ofthe school<strong>in</strong>g system is masked or legitimized <strong>in</strong> people’s consciousness. School failure canbe conveniently attributed to <strong>in</strong>dividual cognitive deficit or lack of effort and not to theunequal <strong>in</strong>itial shares of the cultural capital both valued and legitimized <strong>in</strong> school:the dom<strong>in</strong>ated classes allow (the struggle) to be imposed on them when they acceptthe stakes offered by the dom<strong>in</strong>ant classes. It is an <strong>in</strong>tegrative struggle and, by virtueof the <strong>in</strong>itial handicaps, a reproductive struggle, s<strong>in</strong>ce those who enter this chase, <strong>in</strong>which they are beaten before they start, as the constancy of the gaps testifies, implicitlyrecognize the legitimacy of the goals pursued by those whom they pursue, by the merefact of tak<strong>in</strong>g part. (Bourdieu, 1984: 165)Symbolic violence, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bourdieu, is the imposition of representations of the worldand social mean<strong>in</strong>gs upon groups <strong>in</strong> such a way that they are experienced as legitimate.Thisis achieved through a process of misrecognition. For <strong>in</strong>stance, thc recent “<strong>English</strong> Only”campaigns <strong>in</strong> the United States provide illustrations of the political struggles required tocreate and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a unified l<strong>in</strong>guistic market <strong>in</strong> which only one language is recognized as


DO1 N G-E NG LIS H - LESS0 N S 273legitimate and appropriate for discourse <strong>in</strong> official sett<strong>in</strong>gs, and this “<strong>English</strong> = American”symbolic representation has numerous consequences for school<strong>in</strong>g and jobs (Coll<strong>in</strong>s, 1993).For another <strong>in</strong>stance, many Hong Kong parents <strong>in</strong>sist on fight<strong>in</strong>g for a place for their children<strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> medium schools (often despite the fact that their children speak and understandlittle <strong>English</strong>) because of the “<strong>English</strong> medium schools = good schools” symbolicrepresentation that they have steadfastly accepted even <strong>in</strong> a largely Ch<strong>in</strong>ese society and apost- 1997 era (for some background to the symbolic dom<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong,see L<strong>in</strong>, 1996, 1998; and more on this <strong>in</strong> section 3 below).Creative, discursive agenyBourdieu has often been accused of be<strong>in</strong>g overly determ<strong>in</strong>istic and a theorist more ofreproduction than transformation (e.g., Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, 1992; Canagarajah <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13). Lcmke,however, po<strong>in</strong>ts out that Bourdieu is not limited to reproduction; what he does limit is theeffectiveness of s<strong>in</strong>gle agents <strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g whole fields of valuation (Jay Lemke, personalcommunication). For <strong>in</strong>stance, the legitimate prestige and value attached to <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> HongKong cannot be changed by s<strong>in</strong>gle agents unless there are systematic changes <strong>in</strong> the socialselcction mechanism (e.g., the medium of the universities and the professions; the languageof the job market; see section 3 below). While the above seems true, an area <strong>in</strong> whichBourdieu offers few analyses is the creative, discursive agency of social actors who f<strong>in</strong>dthemselves caught <strong>in</strong> dilemmas. As Coll<strong>in</strong>s po<strong>in</strong>ts out:we need to allow for dilemmas and <strong>in</strong>tractable oppositions; for divided consciousness,not just dom<strong>in</strong>ated m<strong>in</strong>ds; . . . for creative, discursive agency <strong>in</strong> conditionsprestructured, to be sure, but also fissured <strong>in</strong> unpredictable and dynamic ways.(Coll<strong>in</strong>s, 1993: 134)In section 4 below, we shall see some cxamples, and discuss the consequences, of teachers’and students’ different creative discursive strategies <strong>in</strong> response to the classroom dilemmasposed by the larger social structures. However, before look<strong>in</strong>g at the classrooms, let us firstlook at the larger social context of the classrooms.3 Hong Kong: the sett<strong>in</strong>g of the storyDespite <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternational cosmopolitan appearance Hong Kong is ethnically ratherhomogcneous. About 97% of <strong>its</strong> population is ethnic Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, and Cantonese is the mothertongue of the majority. <strong>English</strong> native speakers account for not more than 3% of the entircpopulation.They constituted the privileged class ofthe society until July 1, 1997 when HongKong’s sovereignty was returned to Ch<strong>in</strong>a and Hong Kong became a Special Adm<strong>in</strong>istrativcRegion (SAR) of Ch<strong>in</strong>a.The <strong>English</strong>-conversant bil<strong>in</strong>gual Ch<strong>in</strong>ese middle class has, howcvcr,rema<strong>in</strong>ed the socioeconomically dom<strong>in</strong>ant group <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong.Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g the mother tongue of only a m<strong>in</strong>ority, <strong>English</strong> has been thclanguage of educational and socioeconomic advancement; that is, the dom<strong>in</strong>ant symbolicresource <strong>in</strong> the symbolic market (Bourdieu, 1991) <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong. Even <strong>in</strong> the post-1997/colonial era, <strong>English</strong> has rema<strong>in</strong>ed a socioeconomically dom<strong>in</strong>ant language <strong>in</strong> HongKong society. For <strong>in</strong>stance, a 1998 survey on bus<strong>in</strong>ess corporations <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong foundthat the majority of bus<strong>in</strong>ess corporations said they would prefer employees with a goodcommand of <strong>English</strong> to cmployees with a good command of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (S<strong>in</strong>g Tao Jih Puo, May


274 ANGEL M. Y. LIN21, 1998). Besides, <strong>English</strong> rema<strong>in</strong>s the mcdium of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> most universities andprofessional tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programmes.It can be seen that the symbolic market is embodied and enacted <strong>in</strong> the many keysituations (e.g., educational and job sett<strong>in</strong>gs) <strong>in</strong> which symbolic resources (e.g., certa<strong>in</strong> typesof l<strong>in</strong>guistic skills, cultural knowledge, specialized knowledge and skills) are demanded ofsocial actors if they want to ga<strong>in</strong> access to valuablc social, educational and eventually materialresources (Bourdieu, 199 1). For <strong>in</strong>stance, a Hong Kong student must have adequate <strong>English</strong>resources to cnter and succeed <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong>-mcdium professional tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programmesand <strong>in</strong> order to earn the qualifications to entcr high-<strong>in</strong>come professions.To see how the larger social context can pose local dilemmas on teachers and studentsand how they can exercise their creative discursive agency <strong>in</strong> dcal<strong>in</strong>g with their dilemmas,let us compare and contrast four different classrooms.4 A story of four classroomsTaken from the database of the author’s ethnographic and classroom discourse study of eightclassrooms <strong>in</strong> scven schools from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong, thefollow<strong>in</strong>g four classroom scenarios arc meant to give the reader a sense of the diversity ofdiscursive practices that can bc found across even similarly constra<strong>in</strong>ed classrooms (e.g.,Classrooms R, C, and D). To protect the anonymity of the schools and the participants, allnames are pseudo-names and all identify<strong>in</strong>g details of the schools and teachcrs are left out.In listen<strong>in</strong>g to these very different stories, however, you will sense a prcoccupation with arecurrent question: To what extent arc classroom participants shaped by thc larger socialstructures such as sociocultural and familial background and to what extent arc they freeto transform their lot (and habitus)? We shall return to this question <strong>in</strong> section 5. For cachclassroom I shall first describe the background, with <strong>in</strong>formation based on questionnairesurveys and <strong>in</strong>terviews ofthe students, and then an <strong>English</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g lesson. All four teachersare Hong Kong Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, shar<strong>in</strong>g the samc mother-tongue with their students.Classroom A: a scenario ?fcompatihle habitusBackgroundThis is a form 3 (grade 9) class of thirty-three students, aged from fourteen to fifteen, <strong>in</strong> aprestigious girls’ school. The majority of the students came from families <strong>in</strong> the expensiveresidential area <strong>in</strong> which the school is located. Their parents were professionals, bus<strong>in</strong>essexecutives, or university professors, whose education level ranged from secondary,university, to postgraduate. They spoke mostly Cantonese at home, but sometimes also<strong>English</strong>, for example, when speak<strong>in</strong>g to their Filip<strong>in</strong>o domestic hclpcrs.They read a varietyof extra-curricular materials, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g both <strong>English</strong> and Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, both serious and nonseriousmaterials; for example, comics, Ch<strong>in</strong>esc newspapers, <strong>English</strong> ncwspapers, <strong>English</strong>fashion magaz<strong>in</strong>es, <strong>English</strong> detective stories, science fiction, pop youth magaz<strong>in</strong>es,TV news,Rcadcr’s Digest (both <strong>English</strong> and Ch<strong>in</strong>esc editions), and Ch<strong>in</strong>ese translations of foreignclassics (c.g., Gone with the W<strong>in</strong>d). The students were fluent <strong>in</strong> their responses to theteacher’s questions and could claboratc their answers with the teacher’s prompts.Teacher A’s <strong>English</strong> was the best among the eight teachers who participated <strong>in</strong> my study.<strong>English</strong> seemed to be a tool she readily used <strong>in</strong> her daily life and not just <strong>in</strong> academiccontexts. She spoke to her students about her daughter, her shopp<strong>in</strong>g hab<strong>its</strong>, Mother’s Day,


DO1 NG-E N G L IS H-L E S SO N S 275and her feel<strong>in</strong>gs naturally and comfortably <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. She was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> both Ch<strong>in</strong>cscand <strong>English</strong> literature, and she read for lcisure <strong>English</strong> magai<strong>in</strong>es. Sometimes, she wouldbr<strong>in</strong>g her old magaz<strong>in</strong>es from home to the class library and share them with her students.The read<strong>in</strong>g lesson described below was run smoothly and the teacher cngaged students<strong>in</strong> high-level (e.g., beyond factual) questions about the story they had read. All through thelesson <strong>English</strong> was consistently used by both teacher and students and the classroomatmosphere was <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gly both relaxed and seriously on-task.A read<strong>in</strong>g lesson <strong>in</strong> Classroom AThe teacher began the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson with the follow<strong>in</strong>g extended <strong>in</strong>troduction:T: Okay. . . now. . . have you brought back. . . Flowers for Mrs. Harris?. . . Now. . .I’d like to discuss one th<strong>in</strong>g with you . . . for this lesson for this book. Have you everwondered WHY this book is called Flowers for Mrs. Harris . . . and not a Dior dress forMrs. Harris?. . . Now the whole book we are talk<strong>in</strong>g about HOW Mrs. Harris . . .saved . . . how she worked extra hard to save up the money . . . so that she could go toParis to buy the dress. And after that . . . aa . . . aga<strong>in</strong> she went through a lot of troubles<strong>in</strong> order to get the dress back. . . and at the end it was ru<strong>in</strong>ed. So all along we weretalk<strong>in</strong>g about a drcsr . . . and Mrs Harris . . . but why . . . why Flowers for Mrs. Harris?. . . Alright now . . . I want to spend . . . aa . . . the next five to ten m<strong>in</strong>utes or so . . .and try to discuss <strong>in</strong> groups, okay? aam . . . you can probably f<strong>in</strong>d some h<strong>in</strong>ts . . .towards the end of this book, <strong>in</strong> the last chapter.The students swiftly formed groups and discussed.The teacher walked to a group and startedto engage students <strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g deeper about the story by ask<strong>in</strong>g them some guid<strong>in</strong>gquestions, e.g., “What did Mrs. Harris see <strong>in</strong> those flowers?” or, “Besides the flowers, howelse can she feel that friends are very important?”. After spend<strong>in</strong>g some time with one groupshe moved onto another group and did the same.After about fifteen m<strong>in</strong>utes she addressed the whole class aga<strong>in</strong> and asked morequestions about the story. The students readily gave her answers and she built on theiranswers to br<strong>in</strong>g out the themes of the story: friendship, hard work and couragc.Then shetalked about the class’s upcom<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ation and encouraged her students to emulate Mrs.Harris, to work hard and not to lose heart when faced with difficulties. Most of the timedur<strong>in</strong>g the lesson, the students seemed to be attentive to their teacher or on-task.Classroom R: a scenario f<strong>in</strong>compatible habitusBackgroundThis is a form 2 (grade 8) class of forty-two students, twenty boys and twenty-two girls,aged bctween thirteen to fourteen.The school is located <strong>in</strong> a government-subsidi7ed publichous<strong>in</strong>g estate. The students largely came from families who lived <strong>in</strong> the nearby publichous<strong>in</strong>g estates. Their parents were manual or service workers and their education lcvelranged from primary to secondary school.They spoke only Cantonese at homc. Most of thcboys read comics, newspapers,TV news, and pop youth magaz<strong>in</strong>es. Most of the girls readTV news, love stories, ghost stories, newspapers, and pop youth magaz<strong>in</strong>es. They did notrcad any <strong>English</strong> cxtra-curricular materials.I <strong>in</strong>formally <strong>in</strong>terviewed a group of boys who were observctl to be the most resistantto the teacher <strong>in</strong> the classroom.They were playful and test<strong>in</strong>g, as if check<strong>in</strong>g out whether I


276 ANGEL M. Y. LINcould understand their <strong>in</strong>sider jokes. When I asked them questions such as whether theyliked <strong>English</strong> or thcir <strong>English</strong> lessons, they replied <strong>in</strong> the affirmative, but <strong>in</strong> an exaggcratcdand jok<strong>in</strong>g way. I sensed that they were try<strong>in</strong>g to give me what they thought I was after, soI said aga<strong>in</strong> that I would like to hear what they really thought and that I would not tellanyth<strong>in</strong>g they said to the school authorities. Then they seemed to be more will<strong>in</strong>g to voicetheir feel<strong>in</strong>gs.They said they found their <strong>English</strong> lessons bor<strong>in</strong>g and they did not know a lotof the th<strong>in</strong>gs the teacher said as the teacher would only speak <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. I asked why theydid not tell the teacher and request her to expla<strong>in</strong> the th<strong>in</strong>gs they did not understand.Theysaid the teacher would only expla<strong>in</strong> aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, and they would still not understand.They said they chatted and played <strong>in</strong> the classroom because the lesson was too bor<strong>in</strong>g butthey were also afraid of be<strong>in</strong>g asked by the teacher to answer questions. They said they feltvery “yyu” (“without face”) stand<strong>in</strong>g up there <strong>in</strong> the class and be<strong>in</strong>g unable to answer theteacher’s questions.They had a very cynical view about school life and about their future. They said theydid not like learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> but they knew they could not f<strong>in</strong>d a job without <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> thissociety. They also stated that they did not consider they would be able get <strong>in</strong>to university.Teacher B’s relationship with some of the boys appeared to be stressful at times. For example,sometimes she had to chide the boys angrily for not pay<strong>in</strong>g attention or chatt<strong>in</strong>g with theirneighbours. The follow<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g lesson will give the reader a sense of the atmosphere <strong>in</strong>her classroom.A rcad<strong>in</strong>g lesson <strong>in</strong> Classroom BThe teacher started by say<strong>in</strong>g they were go<strong>in</strong>g to read chapter 30 of the storybook,Adventures of Tom Sawyer, <strong>in</strong> groups of four or five and each group would send arepresentative to retell the story <strong>in</strong> 50 to 60 words to the whole class. Each group was towrite down a summary on a piece of paper first and the summary should cover the ma<strong>in</strong>po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> that chapter. As thc teacher was say<strong>in</strong>g these <strong>in</strong>structions, the class was noisy andsome students said loudly <strong>in</strong> Cantonese that they did not know what to do. The teacherrepeated her <strong>in</strong>structions and walked around to help students to form groups and to expla<strong>in</strong>aga<strong>in</strong> what they were expected to do. Most of the students were off-task, chatt<strong>in</strong>g and jok<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> Cantonese. A girl at the back was writ<strong>in</strong>g the lyrics of a popular Cantonese love song ona piece of paper. There seemed to lie a lot of non-teacher-approved activities go<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>in</strong>the classroom and a lot of noise. The teacher seemed exhausted circulat<strong>in</strong>g around theclassroom try<strong>in</strong>g to get her students to do the task. All through the lesson <strong>English</strong> wasconsistently spoken by the teacher while, <strong>in</strong> contrast, Cantonese was <strong>in</strong>variably spoken bythe students except when they were called upon to do the story-retell<strong>in</strong>g. When they didthat, they read mechanically from a series of sentences they wrote on a piecc of paper whilemost other students cont<strong>in</strong>ued to chat noisily on their own. After a student had f<strong>in</strong>ishedrcad<strong>in</strong>g from the paper, the teacher would say “Very nice, their report <strong>in</strong>cludes all the po<strong>in</strong>ts”or “Quite nice, they have covered some of the po<strong>in</strong>ts” and then immediately called anothergroup’s representative to do the retell<strong>in</strong>g. She seemed to he runn<strong>in</strong>g out of time and had toget all the retell<strong>in</strong>gs done with<strong>in</strong> the lesson.This might expla<strong>in</strong> the brevity of her feedbackto the students.


DO1 NG-E NG LIS H- L E SSON S 277Classroom C: a scenario of <strong>in</strong>compatible habitusBackgroundThis is a form 2 (grade 8) class of thirty-n<strong>in</strong>e students, n<strong>in</strong>eteen male and twenty female,aged from thirteen to fourteen.The school is located <strong>in</strong> a town close to an <strong>in</strong>dustrial area.The socioeconomic backgrounds of the students and their sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic and extracurricularliteracy hab<strong>its</strong> are like those of their counterparts <strong>in</strong> Classroom B.Their <strong>English</strong> fluency, ascan be seen from how and what they spoke <strong>in</strong> the classroom, seemed to be rather limitedfor their grade level. There were many words <strong>in</strong> the textbook that they did not understandor did not know how to pronounce.When I <strong>in</strong>formally <strong>in</strong>terviewed a group of boys after class, they expressed that theyfound <strong>English</strong> “bor<strong>in</strong>g” and “difficult” but they also said they knew it was very important tolearn <strong>English</strong> well.They found school work generally bor<strong>in</strong>g but said they still preferred togo to school because they said they could at least meet and play with friends at school.Theysaid it would be even more bor<strong>in</strong>g to stay all day at home. “Bor<strong>in</strong>g” was a word these boysused frequently to describe their life and school.The reader can get a sense of the atmosphere<strong>in</strong> their classroom by look<strong>in</strong>g at the follow<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g lesson.A read<strong>in</strong>g lesson <strong>in</strong> Classroom CThe read<strong>in</strong>g lesson can be divided <strong>in</strong>to three stages. In the pre-read<strong>in</strong>g stage, the teacherasked some pre-read<strong>in</strong>g questions about the topic of the story - Heaven-Queen Festival,us<strong>in</strong>g the Initiation-Response-Feedback (IRF) discourse format (S<strong>in</strong>clair and Coulthard,1975; Mehan, 1979; Heap, 1985). Then the teacher wrote ten numbered read<strong>in</strong>gcomprehension questions on the blackboard and the class was given fifteen m<strong>in</strong>utes to readsilently and f<strong>in</strong>d answers from the text to the ten questions by underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g relevant parts <strong>in</strong>the text. This constitutes the read<strong>in</strong>g stage. The f<strong>in</strong>al stage is an answer-check<strong>in</strong>g stage. Theteacher elicited answers from the class us<strong>in</strong>g the IRF discourse format. The teacher oftenhad to re-ask or elaborate her <strong>English</strong> questions <strong>in</strong> Cantonese to get responses from studentsand then the teacher rephrased the students’ Cantonese response <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> (L2).In the follow<strong>in</strong>g excerpt taken from the answer-check<strong>in</strong>g stagc, we f<strong>in</strong>d the creativityof the students burst<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>in</strong> a niche that they capitalize on <strong>in</strong> an otherwise ratherun<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g IRF discourse. The teacher had been ask<strong>in</strong>g factual read<strong>in</strong>g comprehensionquestions about the Heaven-Queen story that they have just read. She came to question 9(What happened when she answered her mother?) and first asked the question <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>.No response was forthcom<strong>in</strong>g and so she was now elaborat<strong>in</strong>g the question <strong>in</strong> Cantonese<strong>in</strong> pursuit of a response from her students:Lesson Excerpt(To facilitate read<strong>in</strong>g, Cantonese utterances have been translated <strong>in</strong>to <strong>English</strong>; they arebolded and placed <strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ted brackets. See appendix for other notes on transcription.)870T: ?872 Leih: . { chuckl<strong>in</strong>g towards the end ofhis sentence }


278 ANGEL M. Y. LIN872.5 Ss:872.8T:873.2 Chan:873.5 S1:873.8 S2:874 T:874.5 L:874.8 //T:875 L:875.2T:875.5 S1:875.8 S2:876 T:876 //S3:876.5 ==T:=Haha! haha! haha! hahahaha! {other Ss laugh<strong>in</strong>g hilariously}! {aga<strong>in</strong>st a background of Ss’ laughter}! {chuckl<strong>in</strong>g } =ZHihihihik! ! = { laugh<strong>in</strong>g }=?? {T <strong>in</strong> an amused tone; some students laugh}==? {quite amus<strong>in</strong>gly}....// Right? (1) Her father dropped <strong>in</strong>to the SEA!==Hckhck! {laugh<strong>in</strong>g}Right? (2) . . . SHH! (1) . . . . . . SHH! number ten . . .The need to base one’s answer (or to “f<strong>in</strong>d the answer”) <strong>in</strong> the text has been a recurrentconcern of the teacher voiced <strong>in</strong> her recurrent prompts and follow-up questions such as”“Where can you f<strong>in</strong>d it?”, “Does the book really say so?”, “Look at paragraph -, l<strong>in</strong>efound <strong>in</strong> other parts of the lesson transcript. However, there arc times when a bookishanswer is bor<strong>in</strong>g to the students.The factual nature of the set of questions has left little roomfor imag<strong>in</strong>ation for these lively thirteen-year-olds. In the above lesson excerpt we see howa student has exploited the response slot to do someth<strong>in</strong>g playful, to illegitimately putforward a contribution that will turn the whole story <strong>in</strong>to a comic-strip type of story, whichthey enjoy read<strong>in</strong>g outside school. In their most favourite comic strips, the characters usuallydo funny, impossible th<strong>in</strong>gs and amusement and enjoyment come from the superimpos<strong>in</strong>gof impossible and unpredictable fantasy with the familiar, predictable, and bor<strong>in</strong>g mundaneworld. It seems that the boy who provides this funny answer (turns [872],[873.2]) is a skillfulstory-teller with a ready audience, and this is reflected <strong>in</strong> the hilarious laughter ofhis fellowstudents.Classroom D: a scenario of transform<strong>in</strong>g habitusBackgroundThis is a form 1 (grade 7) remedial <strong>English</strong> class of thirty students, twenty boys, ten girls,aged between twelve to thirteen. The students came from families who lived <strong>in</strong> the nearbypublic hous<strong>in</strong>g estates. The socioeconomic backgrounds of the students and theirsociol<strong>in</strong>guistic and extra-curricular literacy hab<strong>its</strong> are like those of their counterparts <strong>in</strong>Classrooms B and C.The classroom atmosphere was very lively. Most students were attentive to the teacherand focused on their lesson tasks most of the timc.They seemed to enjoy their <strong>English</strong> lessonsand were both eager and often able to answer the teacher’s questions.When I asked the students <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews after class whether they liked <strong>English</strong>and their <strong>English</strong> lessons, they said yes, and they especially liked their <strong>English</strong> teacher.Theysaid that they liked to hear her tell stories from their <strong>English</strong> reader book, and that she couldalso expla<strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs clearly to them. They liked the way she expla<strong>in</strong>ed some grammaticalpo<strong>in</strong>ts. For example, when expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the difference between ‘‘little’’ and “few”, the teacher


DO1 NG-E NG LIS H - LESS0 N S 279helped them to remember the difference by say<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘little’’ has more letters than “few” andso is uncountable and “few” has not so many letters and so is “countable”. The students saidthey found this mnemonic tip very helpful to them. They also sounded positive about theirstudies and their future. They said that they thought they could learn <strong>English</strong> well becausethey could see themselves do<strong>in</strong>g better and better <strong>in</strong> their <strong>English</strong> dictations, exercises andtests.The teacher had kept a personal progress chart for the students so that they knew howthey were do<strong>in</strong>g over time, and the teacher would give prizes for the best-perform<strong>in</strong>gstudents. They felt that they could succeed <strong>in</strong> their studies and would have a good chanceof further<strong>in</strong>g their studies (e.g., enter<strong>in</strong>g university) <strong>in</strong> the future.Teacher D used Cantonese to expla<strong>in</strong> vocabulary, give directions, make the <strong>English</strong>story texts come alive, expla<strong>in</strong> grammatical po<strong>in</strong>ts, and <strong>in</strong>teract with students most ofthe time. She was the teacher who used the most Cantonese among the eight teachers<strong>in</strong> my study. She believed that s<strong>in</strong>ce the students were still Form 1 students and werenot up to a level for us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> all the time, us<strong>in</strong>g Cantonese could help them becomemore <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the lessons and understand the lessons better. She also found that herstudents had made good progress over the academic year, for <strong>in</strong>stance, as reflected <strong>in</strong> their<strong>in</strong>creased motivation to learn <strong>English</strong>, and their improved scores <strong>in</strong> school tests andexam<strong>in</strong>ations.Teacher D was the form teacher of this class. She spent most of her recess, lunch, andafter-school hours talk<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>dividual students who had various problems, for example,forgett<strong>in</strong>g to br<strong>in</strong>g books to school, noisy <strong>in</strong> other teachers’ lessons, scor<strong>in</strong>g poorly <strong>in</strong>dictations or tests. I got a sense that the good relationships she had with her students (ascould be reflected <strong>in</strong> their eager responses to her questions, and their co-operative responsesto her directives) might have someth<strong>in</strong>g to do with the amount of <strong>in</strong>dividual attention shegave to each student <strong>in</strong> her class. Every day, she had her lunch with a student together. Inthis way, she ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed both a classroom and a personal relationship with her students.However, that also seemed to make her school days fully packed and busy from earlymorn<strong>in</strong>g till late <strong>in</strong>to the afternoon. She seemed to be an energetic teacher who did notm<strong>in</strong>d do<strong>in</strong>g extra work and spend<strong>in</strong>g extra time with her students. The reader can get asense of the atmosphere <strong>in</strong> her classroom by look<strong>in</strong>g at the follow<strong>in</strong>g lesson excerpt.A read<strong>in</strong>g lesson <strong>in</strong> Classroom DThe lcsson excerpt below is taken from the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson. The teacherannounces that she is go<strong>in</strong>g to ask them questions about the part of the <strong>English</strong> storybookthat they have read <strong>in</strong> a previous lesson:46947 8478.5 T:T: ?{A girl raises her hand;T turns to her and says} Yes,Girl 1 {stands up and speaks}: !?! No:::,


280 ANGEL M. Y. LIN478.8479 T:479.5479.8 T:481.5483T:483.8484T:487488 T:492492.5T:492.8493 T:494.3494.5 T:495495.5T:498498.3T:498.5498.8T:499499.5 T:500 L500.5 T:Some Ss {speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their scats} : Baa-Gaak-Daaht!No, not ! (many students raisc their hands now andT po<strong>in</strong>ts to a boy}Boy 1 {stands up and speaks}: !, how to spell . . . ? <strong>English</strong> , <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> . . .. {Girl 1 raises her hand aga<strong>in</strong>;T turns to her and gestures her tospeak} Yes,Girl 1 {stands up and speaks}: b-a-g-h . . . &a-d{ T writes it on thcblackboard as the girl spells it }Yes! ?Some Ss {speak<strong>in</strong>g up <strong>in</strong> their seats}: ! !No, Baghdad, Baghdad, Baghdad . . .Ss {speak<strong>in</strong>g up <strong>in</strong> their seats }: Monkeys!Ycs! {T writes the word “monkey” on the blackboard} , what is the na::mc of this island? Can you spell theword? { Another girl raises her hand } Yes,Girl 2 {stands up and spcaks} : Z-u-g . . .z-u-g . . .Girl 2 {stand<strong>in</strong>g up} : (d)No, b, b for boy. { T writes the word “Zugb” on the board } Some Ss {speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their seats } : Zugb!Z :: ugb ::Ss {repeat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their seats }: ZUGB!!! Zugb!! Au ugly place for the ugly men. ?Boy {speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> his seat}: ! { Another boy raiscs hishand}YCS,Boy 2: ?Another boy {speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> his scat}: !!{stands up and speaks} : Giant.Giant! Very good! Yes! { T writes the word “giant” on board }In the excerpt above, the tcachcr dramatizes, with <strong>in</strong>tonations and gestures, the part of thestory about S<strong>in</strong>bad sail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a boat.The teacher then asks the students where S<strong>in</strong>bad is sail<strong>in</strong>gback to (last three l<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> turn [469]).The teacher gives negative feedback to a student’s answer <strong>in</strong> turn [478.5]. Some otherstudents immediately speak out their answers from their seats (turn [478 .SI). The teachersignals to a boy to speak.The boy stands up from his scat and givcs his answer (turn [479.5]:Baa-Gaak-Daaht). We see that <strong>in</strong> this way, the teacher ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s the practice of hav<strong>in</strong>g a“student-bids-and-teacher-accepts” pre-sequcncc to a student rcsponse.This time the student’s answcr is corrcct (turn I479.51: Baa-Gaak-Daaht).The teacherrepeats it and immcdiatcly <strong>in</strong>itiates another question <strong>in</strong> the feedback-cum-<strong>in</strong>itiation slot(turn [479.8]).This question is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g. It scems to belong to a different type of questionfrom the first question she asks (see last l<strong>in</strong>c <strong>in</strong> turn [469]: S<strong>in</strong>bad . . . sail<strong>in</strong>g back


DOING-ENGLISH-LESSONS 281to where?>). Instead of follow<strong>in</strong>g the storyl<strong>in</strong>e and ask<strong>in</strong>g about what happens to S<strong>in</strong>badnext, the second question requires the students to give the spell<strong>in</strong>g of the <strong>English</strong> versionof the name of the place, “Baa-Gaak-Daaht”, which has been offered by a student as aresponse and acknowledged and repeated by the teacher (turns (479.51, [479.8]). It seemsto be a question that requircs thc students to focus on the l<strong>in</strong>guistic aspects of the story.They have read the <strong>English</strong> text (pp. 4042 of their storybook), and the <strong>English</strong> text is nowlaid out on their desks before them. The question requires them to shift their focus fromthe content of the storyfor a while to concentrate on the language <strong>in</strong> which this content iscouched. It seems that the place name <strong>in</strong> Cantonese (“Baa-Gaak-Daaht”) cannot be acceptedby the teacher as an acceptablejnal answer.The teacher’s follow-up question on the elicitedanswer would have the effect of gett<strong>in</strong>g the students to reformulate the answer <strong>in</strong>to anultimately acceptable format - “<strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>” (the words the teacher uses <strong>in</strong> her follow-up<strong>in</strong>itiation; see l<strong>in</strong>e 2 <strong>in</strong> turn [479.8]).We see <strong>in</strong> turns [48 1.51 and [483] that the teacher ultimately gets the L2 formulationof the answer -“Baghdad”, and she writes it on the blackboard. Only L2 answers are writtenon the blackboard. It seems that the teacher’s act of writ<strong>in</strong>g the student’s response on theblackboard has the effect of conferr<strong>in</strong>g a f<strong>in</strong>al-answer status on the response of the student(Heyman, 1983).UnlikeTeacher C, who often does her <strong>in</strong>itiations <strong>in</strong> an L2 (Question) - L1 (Annotationof Question) sequence,Teacher D often starts with L1 to <strong>in</strong>itiate a question about the story.Teacher D seems to be us<strong>in</strong>g a couplet of IRF formats to do consecutively two differentk<strong>in</strong>ds of th<strong>in</strong>gs. The first IRF format is always used to engage the students <strong>in</strong> co-tell<strong>in</strong>g thestory (e.g., turns [469]-[479.8]).The focus is on the content of the story and the questionsasked <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itiation slots follow naturally from the storyl<strong>in</strong>e.The second IRF format (e.g.,turns [479.8]-[483]) is used to get the students to reformulate <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> their Cantoneseanswer that has been acknowledged <strong>in</strong> the first IRF format. The second IRF format may berepeated to get the students to focus on the l<strong>in</strong>guistic aspects of the f<strong>in</strong>al L2 answer. Forexample, the second IRF format is repeated <strong>in</strong> turns [483], [483 .SI, [484] to get the studentsto say “Baghdad” <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>.With the paired use of the story-focus-IRF format immediately followed by thelanguage-focus IRF format, the teacher can get the students to reformulate their earlier L1responses <strong>in</strong>to the language that they are supposed to be learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the lesson: <strong>English</strong>.Thisspecial use of the IRF formats <strong>in</strong> Teacher D’s classroom stands <strong>in</strong> contrast with the use ofthe IRF format <strong>in</strong> Teacher C’s class. For <strong>in</strong>stance, Teacher C always starts with L2 texts orquestions <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itiation slot of the IRF format. She then uses the L2-L1 Annotation format<strong>in</strong> the same <strong>in</strong>itiation slot to annotate the L2 text or question. Students usually respond <strong>in</strong>L1 .Then the teacher herself reformulates the students’ L1 response <strong>in</strong>to L2 and confers onit the f<strong>in</strong>al-answer status. This k<strong>in</strong>d of discourse practice has the effect of allow<strong>in</strong>g thestudents to get away with L1 responses only. The students are not required to do anyreformulation of their L1 responses <strong>in</strong>to L2.The teacher does it all for them <strong>in</strong> the feedbackslot of the IRF format. The discourse structure of Teacher C <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson can berepresented as follows:Teacher-Initiation [L2-L1]Student-Response [Ll]Teacher-Feedback [ (Ll-)L2]In contrast,Teacher D uses two different IRF formats <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g cycle <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>glesson:


culturalto282 ANGEL M. Y. LIN(1) Story-Focus-IRF:Teacher-Initiation [ L1 ]Student-Response [ L1 ]Teacher-Feedback [ L1 ](2) <strong>Language</strong>-Focus-IRF:Teacher-Initiation [ L1 /L2 ] (L1/L2L1 or L2)Student-Response [ L1 /L2 ]Teacher-Feedback [ L2 1, or use (2) aga<strong>in</strong> until Student-Response is <strong>in</strong> L2(3) Start (2) aga<strong>in</strong> to focus on another l<strong>in</strong>guistic aspect of the L2 response elicited<strong>in</strong> (2); or return to ( 1) to focus on the story aga<strong>in</strong>.This k<strong>in</strong>d of discourse practice allows the tcachcr to <strong>in</strong>terlock a story focus with a languagefocus <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson. There can be enjoyment of the story, via the use of the storyfocusIRF, <strong>in</strong>tertw<strong>in</strong>ed with a language-learn<strong>in</strong>g focus, via the use of the language-focus IRF.We have noted above that the teacher never starts an <strong>in</strong>itiation <strong>in</strong> L2. She always starts <strong>in</strong>L1 .This stands <strong>in</strong> sharp contrast with the discourse practices ofTcacher C who always startswith L2 texts or questions <strong>in</strong> her <strong>in</strong>itiations. It appears to me that by always start<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> L1,Teacher D always starts from where the student is ~ from what the student can fullyunderstand and is familiar with. On the other hand, by us<strong>in</strong>g the language-focus IRF formatimmediately after the story-focu.; IRF format, she can also push the students to move fromwhat they are familiar with (e.g., L1 expressions) to what they need to become more familiarwith (e.g., L2 counterparts of the L1 cxpressions).5 Do<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>English</strong>-lessons <strong>in</strong> the reproduction or transformationof habitus?You want to know why I don’t pay attention <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> Icssons?You rcally want toknow? Okay, here’s the reason: NO INTEREST!! It’s so bor<strong>in</strong>g and difficult and I canncvcr master it. But the society wants you to learn <strong>English</strong>! If you’re no good <strong>in</strong><strong>English</strong>, you’re no good <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g a job!The above was said by a 14-year-old boy from Classroom B to the author <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>formal<strong>in</strong>terview after class (orig<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong> Cantonese). In section 2 above we mentioned Rourdieu’snotion of habitus referr<strong>in</strong>g to language use, skills, and orientations/attitudes/dispositions/schemes of perception that a child is endowed with by virtue of socialization <strong>in</strong> her/hisfamily and community. The four classroom scenarios outl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> section 4 above canrepresent situations where there are vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of compatibility hctwccn the habitusof the students and what is required of thcm <strong>in</strong> the school <strong>English</strong> lesson. In Classroom A,thc middle class students br<strong>in</strong>g with thcm the right k<strong>in</strong>d of habitus ~ capital ~the school lesson: they have both thc right k<strong>in</strong>d of attitudcs/<strong>in</strong>tcrest and l<strong>in</strong>guisticskills/confidence to participate <strong>in</strong> high-lcvcl discussions on the themes of the story <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>with onc another and the teacher. Do<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>English</strong>-lrssons <strong>in</strong> Classroom A reproduces, andre<strong>in</strong>forces, the students’ cultural capital and both their subjective expectations and objectiveprobabilities of succeed<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> school and the society. Both tcachcr and students are not <strong>in</strong>any dilemmas caused by <strong>in</strong>compatibility of habitus, and thus the atmosphere of relaxedharmony <strong>in</strong> her classroom.


DO IN G - E N G L IS H - L E S SO N S 283In Classroom B, however, we witness a situation of <strong>in</strong>compatibility between students’habitus and what is required of them <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong> lesson. The 14-year-old schoolboy’svoice quoted above expresses vividly what Bourdieu would call a work<strong>in</strong>g class child’ssublective expectations ofoblective probabilities:social class, understood as a system of objective determ<strong>in</strong>ations, must be brought <strong>in</strong>torelation not with the <strong>in</strong>dividual or with the “class” as a population, . . . but with theclass habitus, the system of dispositions (partially) common to all products of the samestructures.Though it is impossible for all members of the same class (or even two ofthem) to have had the same experiences, <strong>in</strong> the same order, it is certa<strong>in</strong> that eachmember of the same class is more likely than any member of another class to havebeen confronted with the situations most frequent for the members of that class.Theobjective structures which science apprehends <strong>in</strong> the form of statistical regularities(e.g. employment rates, <strong>in</strong>come curves, probabilities of access to secondary education,frequency of holidays, etc.) <strong>in</strong>culcate, through the direct or <strong>in</strong>direct but alwaysconvergent experiences which give a social environment <strong>its</strong> physiognomy, with <strong>its</strong>“closed doors”. “dead ends”. and limited “DrosDects”, . . . <strong>in</strong> short, the sense of realityor realities which is perhaps the best-concealed pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of their efficacy. (Bourdieu,1977, pp. 85-86; underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g added)In Classroom B, we witness students who seem to f<strong>in</strong>d themselves confronted with alanguage <strong>in</strong> which they have neither <strong>in</strong>terest nor competence/confidence, and yet a languagethey recognile, though angrily, as a key to success <strong>in</strong> their society. Their conclusion forthemselves seems to be that they can never master the language and that they are excludedfrom any chances of social success. Their behaviour <strong>in</strong> the classroom seems to stem fromtheir contradictory feel<strong>in</strong>gs about both their seIJrecognition of <strong>in</strong>ability to change, and angryprotests of, their fate: they engage <strong>in</strong> classroom practices oppositional to the curriculumand the teacher, fully expect<strong>in</strong>g themselves to be never able to master the “difficult”, foreignlanguage anyway (e.g., by ignor<strong>in</strong>g the lesson task or the teacher altogether and engag<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> peer talk <strong>in</strong> their mother tongue most of the time). Their resistance seems to resemblethat of marg<strong>in</strong>alized ethnic m<strong>in</strong>orities <strong>in</strong> North American <strong>in</strong>ner city schools (e.g., Solomon,1992).We also witness a teacher <strong>in</strong> dilemma <strong>in</strong> Classroom B.The dilemma is one of hav<strong>in</strong>g toteach <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> only, as this is her school’s policy and, <strong>in</strong> general, a methodologicalprescription dom<strong>in</strong>ant <strong>in</strong> ELT (<strong>English</strong> language teacher) education <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong, and atthe same time hav<strong>in</strong>g to get her limited-<strong>English</strong>-proficiency and apparently uncooperativestudents to understand her <strong>in</strong>structions and explanations as well as to complete the lessontask with<strong>in</strong> the time limit of the lesson. We witness a teacher runn<strong>in</strong>g around the classroomto get her large class of 42 students on-task. She was exhausted and frustrated, andapparently fail<strong>in</strong>g to get connected <strong>in</strong> any mean<strong>in</strong>gful way to her students despite her pa<strong>in</strong>fulefforts.Let us turn to Classroom C, where we witness a slightly different picture. The lessonis perceived as equally “bor<strong>in</strong>g”, a word used by the students describ<strong>in</strong>g their lesson andtheir view of <strong>English</strong> to the researcher <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>formal after-class <strong>in</strong>terview. However, theteacher seems to be (partially) successful <strong>in</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g her students to collaborate <strong>in</strong> extract<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation from the story text to answer pre-given read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension questions, thek<strong>in</strong>d of questions typically found <strong>in</strong> school tests and exam<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong. She seemsto be impart<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ation skills albeit <strong>in</strong> ways that students might f<strong>in</strong>d unengag<strong>in</strong>g. Themother tongue is a tool she uses to get her limited-<strong>English</strong>-proficiency students to


284 ANGEL M. Y. LINcollaborate <strong>in</strong> this text-<strong>in</strong>formation extraction process. She seems to be connected to herstudents at some level, e.g., shar<strong>in</strong>g their joke (she smiles and appears to be amused by thestudent’s fun answer), though she also seems to be eager to socialize students <strong>in</strong>to the tcxt<strong>in</strong>formationextraction m<strong>in</strong>dsct. In this respect there is some <strong>in</strong>compatibility between thestudents’ habitus and what the teacher requires of them <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson. Us<strong>in</strong>g themother tongue (Lt) as a bridg<strong>in</strong>g tool, the teacher seems to be partly <strong>in</strong>duc<strong>in</strong>g and partlycoerc<strong>in</strong>g her students <strong>in</strong>to a specific school mode of orientations to text, albeit with vary<strong>in</strong>gdegrees of success across her students.It seems that as a result of the teacher’s efforts, the students may become better versed<strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ation skills although their basic habitus orientation towards <strong>English</strong> ~ f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g itbor<strong>in</strong>g and irrelevant to their daily life -rema<strong>in</strong>s unchanged.The teacher’s use of L1 seemsto reflect her discursive strategy to deal with her dilemma: how to get her students tocollaborate <strong>in</strong> a task perceived as uncngag<strong>in</strong>g by her students.Now let us turn to Classroom D. The students come from a similarly disadvantagedsocioeconomic background as their counterparts <strong>in</strong> Classrooms B and C. Like thcircounterparts, their habitus does not equip them with the right k<strong>in</strong>d of attitudes and <strong>in</strong>terest,as well as skills and confidence <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>. However, we witness some sign of theirhabitus be<strong>in</strong>g transformed through the creative discursive agency and efforts of their teacher.For <strong>in</strong>stance, she uses L1 <strong>in</strong> a strategic way to <strong>in</strong>tertw<strong>in</strong>e an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g story focus and alanguage-learn<strong>in</strong>g focus <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>g lesson. She helps her students to experience a senseof achievement and confidence <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> (e.g., by chart<strong>in</strong>g their progress so thatthey can see their own improvement; by giv<strong>in</strong>g them mnemonic strategics regard<strong>in</strong>gvocabulary usage). She also spends most of hcr school spare time with her students toestablish a personal relationship with each of them. With all these extra personal creativeefforts, she succeeds <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g her students to develop <strong>in</strong>terest, skills as well as confidcnce<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g a language that is otherwise pcrccived as “difficult”, “bor<strong>in</strong>g” and basicallyirrelevant <strong>in</strong> the daily lives of these students com<strong>in</strong>g from a Cantonese-dom<strong>in</strong>ant work<strong>in</strong>gclass habitus.Search<strong>in</strong>g for the appropriate methodology for different k<strong>in</strong>ds of students com<strong>in</strong>g fromdifferent cultural and social backgrounds with different habituses becomes an importanttask and possibility for TESOL practitioners work<strong>in</strong>g with students from backgrounds thatdo not give them the right k<strong>in</strong>d of cultural capital. It seems thatTESOL practitioners willbenefit more from their own reflective action-research <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g their own appropriatemethodology for their studcnts rather than from merely follow<strong>in</strong>g ELT prescriptions(Holliday, 1994). For <strong>in</strong>stance, while the prescription of us<strong>in</strong>g only the target language <strong>in</strong>teach<strong>in</strong>g the target language is widely held, it becomes clear from observ<strong>in</strong>g the above fourclassrooms that it is not whether L1 or L2 is used that matters, but rather, bow L1 or L2 canbe used to connect with studcnts and to help them transform their attitudes/dispositions/skills/self-image ~ their habitus or social worlds. For <strong>in</strong>stance, unlike the self-defeat<strong>in</strong>gsound<strong>in</strong>gstudents <strong>in</strong> Classroom B (see quotation of a boy’s voice above), students <strong>in</strong>Classroom D are not pessimistic about their life chances: “I want to further my studies.”, “Ifeel confident about learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>.” .~ these are what the students <strong>in</strong> Classroom C toldthe researcher. Their school results confirm their newly-found confidence and expectations.The question then is not one of whether to use L1 or not but one of search<strong>in</strong>g for appropriatecreative discursivc practices with one’s own students. In this respect, we confirm Coll<strong>in</strong>s’(1993) observation that <strong>in</strong>dividual creativc, discursive agency can make transformation ofonc’s social world possible dcspitc the larger constra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, reproduc<strong>in</strong>g social structuresoutl<strong>in</strong>ed by Bourdieu (1977).


DO1 N G - E N G L IS H - L E SSO N S 2856 Interrogat<strong>in</strong>g symbolic violenceAlthough we can see a glimpse of hope <strong>in</strong> creative, discursive agency <strong>in</strong> transform<strong>in</strong>g ourhabitus and life chances, we cannot neglect the need for the cont<strong>in</strong>ual <strong>in</strong>terrogation of powerand fields of valuation <strong>in</strong> the larger society (Pennycook, 1994; Luke, 1996). For <strong>in</strong>stance,students <strong>in</strong> Classroom D might have found a bit of the cultural capital that they need forschool and social success through their teacher’s and their own extra creative efforts, butthey are still <strong>in</strong> a race the rules of which are laid down by the privileged classes, who arealready way ahead of them <strong>in</strong> the race (e.g., Classroom A students). These rules are, however,often taken for granted and perceived as legitimate by all parties: teachers, students,curriculum designers, and parents ~ a case of symbolic violence exercised on them (seesection 2 above). It seems that TESOL practitioners need to cont<strong>in</strong>ue to encourage the<strong>in</strong>terrogation, together with their students, of the role of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> their society and <strong>in</strong>their life chances ~ to develop a critical social theory of practice (Luke, 1996). As Pennycookpo<strong>in</strong>ts out,In some senses, then, the <strong>English</strong> language classroom, along with other sites of culturalproduction and political opposition, could become a key site for the renewal of bothlocal and global forms of knowledge. (Pennycook, 1994, p. 326)Understand<strong>in</strong>g exist<strong>in</strong>g practices and the sociocultural and <strong>in</strong>stitutional situatedness ofclassroom practices is a first step towards explor<strong>in</strong>g the possibility of alternative creative,discursive practices that might hold promise of contribut<strong>in</strong>g to the transformation of thestudents’ habitus. More of these stories await another opportunity to be told. It is my hopethat through tell<strong>in</strong>g these lived stories of classroom participants, TESOL practitioners andresearchers can ga<strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to how our role as teachers of <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> the world canbe reassessed, reconceived, and ultimately, repractised.Appendix: notes on transcriptionThe numeral preced<strong>in</strong>g each turn is the transcrib<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>e counter no.; a speak<strong>in</strong>gturn is referred to as: turn [counter no.]Simultaneous utterances: The po<strong>in</strong>t at which another utterance jo<strong>in</strong>s an ongo<strong>in</strong>g oneis <strong>in</strong>dicated by the <strong>in</strong>sertion of two slashes <strong>in</strong> the ongo<strong>in</strong>g turn. The second speakerand her/his utterance(s) are placed below the ongo<strong>in</strong>g turn and are preceded by twoslashes. The latch<strong>in</strong>g of a second speak<strong>in</strong>g turn to a preced<strong>in</strong>g one is <strong>in</strong>dicated by as<strong>in</strong>gle equal sign, “=”.<strong>Context</strong>ual <strong>in</strong>formation: Significant contextual <strong>in</strong>formation is given <strong>in</strong> curly brackets:e.g., { Ss laugh }Transcriptionist doubt: Un<strong>in</strong>telligible items or items <strong>in</strong> doubt are <strong>in</strong>dicated byquestion marks <strong>in</strong> parentheses or the words <strong>in</strong> doubt <strong>in</strong> parentheses.ReferencesBourdieu, P. (1973) ‘Cultural rcproduction and social reproduction’, <strong>in</strong> Brown, R. (cd.)Knowledge, education and cultural change. London: Tavistock.Bourdieu, P. (1977) Outl<strong>in</strong>e . fa theory of practice (translated by Richard Nice). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.


~ (1~ (1286 ANGEL M. Y. LIN(1984) Dist<strong>in</strong>ction:A social critique ofthe judgement oftaste. London: Routledge and KeganPaul.991) <strong>Language</strong> and symbolic power. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Bourdieu, P., and Passeron, J.-C. (1 977) Reproduction <strong>in</strong> education, society and culture. London:Sage.Coll<strong>in</strong>s, (1993) ‘Determ<strong>in</strong>ation and contradiction: An appreciation and critique of the work ofPierre Bourdieu on language and education’, <strong>in</strong> Calhoun, C., LiPuma, E., and Postone,M. (cds) Bourdieu: Critical perspectives, pp. 116-1 38. Cambridge: Polity Press.Dclpit, L. D. (1988) ‘The silenced dialogue: Power and pedagogy <strong>in</strong> educat<strong>in</strong>g other people’schildren’. Harvard Educational Review, 58( 3), 280-298.Heap, J. L. (1985) ‘Discourse <strong>in</strong> the production of classroom knowledge: Read<strong>in</strong>g lessons’.Curriculum Inquiry, 15(3), 245-279.Heyman, R. D. (1983) ‘Clarify<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g through classroom talk. Curriculum Inquiry, 13( l),2342’.Holliday, A. (1994) Appropriated methodology and social context. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, R. (1992) Pierre Bourdieu. London: Routledge.L<strong>in</strong>, A. M. Y. (1996) ‘Bil<strong>in</strong>gualism or l<strong>in</strong>guistic segregation? Symbolic dom<strong>in</strong>ation, resistance,and code-switch<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Hong Kong schools’. L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Education, 8(1), 49-84.997) Hong Kong children’s rights to a culturally compatible <strong>English</strong> education. HongKongJournal ofApplied L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 2(2), 2 348.Luke, A. (1996) ‘Genres of power? Literacy education and the production of capital’, <strong>in</strong> Hasan,R., and Williams, G. (eds) Literacy <strong>in</strong> society, pp. 308-338. London: Longman.Martyn-Jones, M., and Heller, M. (1996) ‘Education <strong>in</strong> multil<strong>in</strong>gual sett<strong>in</strong>gs: Discourse,identities and power’. L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Education, 8( l), 3-1 6.Mehan, H. (1979) Learn<strong>in</strong>g lessons: <strong>Social</strong> organization <strong>in</strong> the classroom. Cambridge, Mass.:Harvard University Press.Pcnnycook, A. (1994) The cultural politics .f <strong>English</strong> as an <strong>in</strong>ternational language. London:Longman.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J. M., and Coulthard, R.M. (1975) Towards an analysis ofdiscourser The <strong>English</strong> used byteachers and pupils. London: Oxford University Press.S<strong>in</strong>g Taojih Pao, May 2 1 , 1998. <strong>English</strong> important for job promotion: blow to mother-tongueeducation [<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese].Solomon, R. P. (1992) Black resistance <strong>in</strong> high school. NewYork: State University of NewYorkPress.


Chapter 18Assia SlimaniEVALUATION OF CLASSROOMINTERACTIONNTIL RELATIVELY RECENTLY, THE TRADITION <strong>in</strong> the field ofU language teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g has been to expect a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of thetcach<strong>in</strong>g/learn<strong>in</strong>g phenomenon by mak<strong>in</strong>g a broad comparison between the learn<strong>in</strong>goutcomes and the teacher’s plan. The focus was set on the extreme poles of the situationunder <strong>in</strong>vestigation: those of methods and outcomes. What happened dur<strong>in</strong>g theimplementation of the method was largely ignored when it camc to the evaluation of thelearn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes. This approach is illustrated by the large-scale projects conducted byScherer and Wertheimer (1964) and Smith (1970), who focused on outcomes and paidrelatively little attention to process.This chapter proposes to analysc and evaluate what is claimed to be learned fromclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction. The method, which will be described later, allows a detailed studyof the classroom <strong>in</strong>teractive processes <strong>in</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g to uncover and evaluate the quality of<strong>in</strong>teraction which leads to learners’ claims of uptake. (Uptake is def<strong>in</strong>ed as what learnersclaim to have learned from a particular lesson.)Importance of the study of classroom <strong>in</strong>teractionAllwright (1 984a) suggests that a hgh proportion of apparent mismatches betwccn tcachngand learn<strong>in</strong>g could be expla<strong>in</strong>ed if <strong>in</strong>struction is perceived as be<strong>in</strong>g the product of bothteachers’ and learners’ contributions. Learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes are not necessarily the reflectionof the teacher’s plan s<strong>in</strong>ce, <strong>in</strong> the process of accomplish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional objectives,<strong>in</strong>teractive work takes place among the participants and leads to the creation of a wholerange of learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities, many of which are perhaps unexpected.The observation of language classes typically shows that the discourse is not someth<strong>in</strong>gprepared beforehand by the teacher and simply implemented with the students. Instead, itis jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructcd by contributions from both parties so that learners are not justpassively fed from the <strong>in</strong>structor’s plan. They can have preoccupations or goals on thcirpersonal agendas that they attempt to clarify dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractive work. Teachers know fromexperience that a lesson does not often take the direction it was planned to take, or, if itdoes, it might nevertheless <strong>in</strong>clude or excludc aspects that neither the teacher nor thelearners have anticipated. Problems, queries, perhaps various unexpected teacher’s andlearner’s comments, <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the teacher’s as well as the learners’ psychological and


288 ASSIA SLIMANIemotional dispositions, arise <strong>in</strong> the course of the ‘planned’ lesson and create the learn<strong>in</strong>gopportunities from which learners presumably grasp whatever gets learned. Hence,considered from this po<strong>in</strong>t of view, lessons are ‘co-productions’ and ‘socially constructedevents’ brought to existence through the ‘co-operative enterprise’ (Corder 1977: 2) of bothparties.The learners’ role <strong>in</strong> the creation of the co-production is not to be underestimated<strong>in</strong> comparison with the role played by the <strong>in</strong>structor. No matter how powerful the latter’s<strong>in</strong>fluence, ‘no teacher teaches without consent’ (Corder 1977:66).The perspective of view<strong>in</strong>g discourse as a co-production adds a new dimension whichties the teacher, <strong>in</strong> his/her attempts to make <strong>in</strong>struction relevant and comprehensible, withthe learners, <strong>in</strong> their attempts to understand <strong>in</strong>struction and manage their own learn<strong>in</strong>g. Ifthe classroom negotiation process is disregarded then what learners get might be differentfrom what the <strong>in</strong>structor or the researcher had <strong>in</strong>tended (see also Allwright 1984b, 1984cfor a fuller discussion of these ideas). In fact, teachers’ exclamations of surprise, such as,‘But I taught them that last week!’, are only too common <strong>in</strong> staff rooms.They bear witnessto the fact that much more than the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the teacher’s plan is needed to providefuller explanations of the learners’ reactions.Seen from this po<strong>in</strong>t of view, it appears quite mislead<strong>in</strong>g to predict which l<strong>in</strong>guisticitems will be ‘uptaken’ by learners even before the lesson has taken place. As argued byAllwright (1 984a), each lesson is a different lesson for each <strong>in</strong>dividual learner as differentth<strong>in</strong>gs arc likely to be drawn by different learners from the same event.Some researchers (Lightbown, 1983; Ellis 1984; Ellis and Rathbone 1987) make priorassumptions about what learners might see as optimal <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>put. Hence, choos<strong>in</strong>g toexam<strong>in</strong>e the teach<strong>in</strong>g effect on the learners’ accuracy of use of the -s morphemes(Lightbown 1983), ofWH-questions (Ellis 1984), and German word order and verb end<strong>in</strong>gs(Ellis and Rathbone 1987) might provide the <strong>in</strong>vestigators with the advantage of hav<strong>in</strong>g arich description of the developmental stages of such features <strong>in</strong> first and second languagedevelopment. However, by predict<strong>in</strong>g the subjects’ learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes, such <strong>in</strong>vestigatorsmight be miss<strong>in</strong>g out on what has actually attracted the learners’ attention <strong>in</strong> discourse.Therefore, Allwright ( 1 984a) suggested the study of the notion of ‘uptake’, that is, the<strong>in</strong>vestigation of what <strong>in</strong>dividual learners claim to have learned from the <strong>in</strong>teractive classroomevents which have just preceded. What follows is a discussion of uptake, and of <strong>its</strong>contribution to a better understand<strong>in</strong>g and evaluation of what gets claimed to be learneddur<strong>in</strong>g classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction.UptakeLearn<strong>in</strong>g a language is def<strong>in</strong>ed by some proponents of communicative curricula ‘as learn<strong>in</strong>ghow to communicate as a member of a socio-cultural group’ (Breen and Candl<strong>in</strong> 1980:91).Hence, it is amply acknowledged that learn<strong>in</strong>g a language is not merely a matter of recall<strong>in</strong>gbeads of items but rather of com<strong>in</strong>g to grips with the ideational, <strong>in</strong>terpersonal and textualknowledge which is realised through effective communication <strong>in</strong> the target language.Therefore, one might argue that attempt<strong>in</strong>g to measure learn<strong>in</strong>g at the end of a lessonimplies a narrow def<strong>in</strong>ition of what language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volves. In this chapter, it isconsidered to be the realisation of communicative competence as well as performance <strong>in</strong>relevant situations.However, s<strong>in</strong>ce we are concerned with relat<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes to their immediateand potentially determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g environment, it appears rather difficult to th<strong>in</strong>k of ways ofgett<strong>in</strong>g at learn<strong>in</strong>g evidence through test<strong>in</strong>g and elicitation procedures as traditionally


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 289understood. The <strong>in</strong>teractive process lends <strong>its</strong>elf to the creation of an <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite set of learn<strong>in</strong>gopportunities which are not pre-established by the teacher’s plan. In such circumstances,it appears to be practically impossible to undertake the complicated task of design<strong>in</strong>g a testto assess the effects of <strong>in</strong>teraction as it occurs, especially s<strong>in</strong>ce the test has to be adm<strong>in</strong>isteredat the end of the lesson. However, the major problem encountered when attempt<strong>in</strong>g toresearch the issue of the direct impact of <strong>in</strong>teraction on the subjects’ claims is that of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>ga way to identify and collect the learners’ performance data or ‘uptake’. Once identified,uptake needs to be related to the classroom environment which might subsequently expla<strong>in</strong><strong>its</strong> emergence.To do this, uptake has to be captured some time after the <strong>in</strong>teractive eventtook place, but before too much could happen to the <strong>in</strong>formants that would obscure thedirect impact of the event on the learners’ claims.The problem is not restricted to formal test-based evaluation procedures. SLAelicitation techniques would also fail to meet the objectives of gett<strong>in</strong>g unmediated learnerdata. Elicitation procedures, similar to those used by Lightbown (1983), provide the<strong>in</strong>formants with an obligatory context of use; this enables the researcher to evaluate, underexperimental conditions, the <strong>in</strong>formants’ accuracy when us<strong>in</strong>g the features which are be<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>vestigated. By their nature, these procedures assume that one is look<strong>in</strong>g for particularfeatures which are predicted from the teacher’s plan. However, what is needed is a way ofidentify<strong>in</strong>g what learners have got from their experience of be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a particular class session.The solution eventually adopted to the problem of ‘uptake’ identification must seemsomewhat naive at first sight: simply ask<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formants to tell the researcher what theybelieved they had learned <strong>in</strong> the lesson they had just attended. It was felt that the advantagesof the procedure outweighed <strong>its</strong> obvious shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs.The great advantage of this approach is that it offers an operational way of gett<strong>in</strong>g atwhat learners perceive they have learned. It makes it possible to relate learn<strong>in</strong>g claims tothe immediate environment from which they emerged <strong>in</strong> order to see if it is possible toestablish a relationship. The idea of requir<strong>in</strong>g learners to tell us what they thought they hadlearned would supply the researcher with manageable amounts of data, directly referableto the classroom data. For <strong>in</strong>stance, if some learners claimed that they had learned thedifference between ‘list’ and ‘least’, the <strong>in</strong>vestigator could trace the words back <strong>in</strong> thetranscripts and study the opportunities where ‘list’ and ‘least’ arose and scrut<strong>in</strong>ise also thecircumstances which might have made those items particularly outstand<strong>in</strong>g to the po<strong>in</strong>t ofprompt<strong>in</strong>g learners to claim them as learned.It should be acknowledged at this stage that I am deal<strong>in</strong>g here with the learners’perceptions of what they believed they have uptaken rather than with ‘facts’. However, <strong>in</strong>the absence of a satisfactory means of gett<strong>in</strong>g at learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> such a way as to relate it to <strong>its</strong>potentially determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g environment, a qualitative approach based on the study of uptakeseems to be an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g phenomenon to guide <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong>to a possible relationshipbetween <strong>in</strong>teraction and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.Prior to mov<strong>in</strong>g to the description of the method, it is relevant to provide brief<strong>in</strong>formation about the participants <strong>in</strong> the study. They were thirteen Algerian male first yearuniversity students at 1’Institut National d’Electricit6 et d’Electronique (INELEC). Theywere aged between eighteen and twenty. They all spoke Arabic as their mother tongue andFrench as a second or foreign language. They were on a six-month <strong>in</strong>tensive languageprogramme (24 hours per week) to prepare them to undertake their eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g studies<strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. To benefit from their language tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, the students were put <strong>in</strong> small groups(<strong>in</strong> this case thirteen) accord<strong>in</strong>g to the results of a placement tcst.Their exposure to <strong>English</strong>outside their classes was limited to their classroom work and occasionally to listen<strong>in</strong>g tofolk music. Their <strong>in</strong>structor was a tra<strong>in</strong>ed Algerian male teacher.


290 ASSIA SLIMANIMethodUptakeThe procedure developed to collect the learners’ claims about uptake was to distributc aquestionnaire or ‘Uptake Recall Chart’ at the end of every observed lesson, ask<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formants to relate, <strong>in</strong> terms of grammar, words and expressions, pronunciation andspell<strong>in</strong>g, and <strong>in</strong> as much detail as possible what po<strong>in</strong>ts they recalled <strong>in</strong> the events that hadjust preceded (see Appendix 1 <strong>in</strong> this chapter for the orig<strong>in</strong>al layout of the Uptake RecallChart). After approximately three hours (before too much had happened to them, but afterenough had happened to counter immediate recency and primacy effects), each learner waspresented with his own uptake recall chart accompanied this timc with an ‘UptakeIdentification Probe’ (see Appendix 2 <strong>in</strong> this chapter for the Uptake Identification Probe).This is another questionnaire ask<strong>in</strong>g the participants to annotate their uptake recall chartsby clearly dissociat<strong>in</strong>g the items they believed they had actually learned <strong>in</strong> that particularlesson from those they had already seen with other teachers or the same teacher on previousoccasions. In this way, I gave thc data the strongest possible chance of be<strong>in</strong>g relatable tospecific <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong> the lesson by ask<strong>in</strong>g learners to commit themselves to the th<strong>in</strong>gs theybelieved they had encountered and learned for the first time from the preced<strong>in</strong>g events. Thethree-hour delay allowed the participants to add, if possible, to their first list of items, butabove all, it was estimated that the delay allowed time for the learners to absorb what theythought they had learned from today’s lesson.Both <strong>in</strong>struments, the Uptake Recall Charts and the Uptakc Identification Probes, werepresented <strong>in</strong> French, a language with which the researcher and all the learners were familiar.Learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunitiesOnce uptaken items have been identified, it is necessary to locate them <strong>in</strong> the relevant<strong>in</strong>teractive events of the lesson <strong>in</strong> which they occurred. Learners were observed two hoursa week dur<strong>in</strong>g the first six weeks of the term. To carry out the classroom observationprocedure a high quality audio-record<strong>in</strong>g of class sessions was crucial to allow the trac<strong>in</strong>gof uptake <strong>in</strong> the learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities which arose <strong>in</strong> the 1essons.The latter needed to havea good number of <strong>in</strong>stances of <strong>in</strong>teractive work which could be closely studied <strong>in</strong> an attcmptto understand what made learners claim uptake <strong>in</strong> those particular <strong>in</strong>stances. A monologuewhere the teacher would be hold<strong>in</strong>g the floor dur<strong>in</strong>g the entire lesson would not have suitedthe needs of the study. However, a relative lack of <strong>in</strong>teraction seems to be a characteristicof lectures rather than language classes where a fair amount of <strong>in</strong>teractive work generallytakes place.It was felt that the amount of <strong>in</strong>teraction occurr<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g lessons depended also onthe learncrs’ ability level and the subjcct studied. To produce the right conditions for theproject, it was assumed that the teach<strong>in</strong>g of grammar to low <strong>in</strong>termediate or advancedbeg<strong>in</strong>ners would offer the most suitable atmosphere. A weak as opposed to a strong groupof students might tend to seek more learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities and pay extra attention to whatgoes on <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>in</strong> order to improve their languagc command. It is noted that thesubjects of this study were particularly motivated to master the second IanguagcThey wereexpected to take their tcchnical subjects <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> at the end of an <strong>in</strong>tensive languageprogramme which served as the sett<strong>in</strong>g for this data collection.Grammar lessons were chosen because discrete po<strong>in</strong>ts are frequcntly dealt with <strong>in</strong> suchlessons and it is relatively easy to f<strong>in</strong>d out what has become of items <strong>in</strong> the learners’ uptakelist. Moreover, it was assumed that it was simpler for thc learner to pick up discrete po<strong>in</strong>ts,


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 291such as one might expect to occur dur<strong>in</strong>g grammar lessons, remember them, and afterwardslist them on the charts which would be distributed at the end of the record<strong>in</strong>g.To <strong>in</strong>vestigate the learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities fully, I exhaustively collected all classroomtextual materials <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g all visual and audio aids. I also took notes of what went on theblackboard to help account later for the claims of uptaken items.InterviewTo provide the study with corroborative data, it was felt necessary to <strong>in</strong>terview the subjectstwice over the six-week period: once <strong>in</strong> the middle and once at the end of the data gather<strong>in</strong>g.The idea was to give the researcher a further chance to probe the <strong>in</strong>formants about thepossible reasons which made them claim the particular items they reported on their uptakecharts. The <strong>in</strong>terview was also believed to allow learners to express other ideas they feltwere miss<strong>in</strong>g from their uptake charts. As the number of learners was rather small, allthirteen could be <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong> about one hour, the same day, after the third lessonrecord<strong>in</strong>g. The subjects were <strong>in</strong>dividually asked to answer the researcher’s queries while theother learners were outside the room, wait<strong>in</strong>g for their turn to be <strong>in</strong>terviewed.The <strong>in</strong>terview, conducted <strong>in</strong> French or <strong>in</strong> Arabic accord<strong>in</strong>g to the learners’ wishes, wasan adaptive structured <strong>in</strong>terview where respondents were free to give details on the fiveissues which were followed up with all learners dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terview session. The issuescould be summarised as follows:12345Clarifications (if necessary) of self-reported data on the charts distributed at the endof every observed lesson.Rationale for claim<strong>in</strong>g those specific items on today’s uptake chart or, if possible, onthe uptake charts distributed at the end of the two previous observed lessons.Possibilities for the learners to extend their perceptions of those items.Reactions to the benef<strong>its</strong> or otherwise of complet<strong>in</strong>g the charts at the end of everytaped grammar lesson.Feel<strong>in</strong>gs about the researcher’s presence and the tape-recorder <strong>in</strong> the back of theclassroom dur<strong>in</strong>g the lesson.The second question, about the reasons for claim<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> items <strong>in</strong>stead of others,was found to be most problematic to the respondents as some rema<strong>in</strong>ed evasive whle othersproduced overgeneralised statements as to what made them claim those items. They wereunable to tell the researcher the reasons which made any particular item outstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>their m<strong>in</strong>ds.The fact that many of them reacted as if the question was irrelevant or irrationaldiscouraged the researcher from <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g a second time as this question was the focusof the <strong>in</strong>terview.The respondents produced responses that were <strong>in</strong>sufficiently precise to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted<strong>in</strong> relation to what might account for their claims. Because I was observ<strong>in</strong>g the same groupfor the period of six weeks I could have tra<strong>in</strong>ed the <strong>in</strong>formants by ask<strong>in</strong>g perhaps moredetailed and specific questions about what most attracted their attention <strong>in</strong> classroomdiscourse. However, as I had never even conducted an <strong>in</strong>terview before, I was afraid to putwords <strong>in</strong> the learners’ mouths. Moreover, be<strong>in</strong>g miles away from any professional consultant,I did not dare meddle with the procedure and run the risk of underm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the data gather<strong>in</strong>g.The <strong>in</strong>terview had to be given with<strong>in</strong> the six observational weeks as the learners’ responseshad to relate to these precisely observed events.


292 ASSIA SLIMANIMethod effectI am aware of the fact that the methodological procedure used to collect the data can stronglyraise the subjects’ consciousness of the learn<strong>in</strong>g process and might, by the same token,pollute the data.This would have been the case if the class observation had lasted over a longperiod of time. I was however, only th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of observ<strong>in</strong>g two hours a week dur<strong>in</strong>g six weeksof the <strong>in</strong>formants’ timetable, which amounted to twenty-four (24) hours of <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>English</strong>lessons per week. It seemed rather unlikely that the methodological procedure would haveany major effect on the subjects’ behaviour.However, to confirm this supposition, the results of the Michigan Test were used. Thistest was already be<strong>in</strong>g used, at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the programme, as a placement test todeterm<strong>in</strong>e the learners’ ability levels. This procedure produced four groups, one of whichwas the group under study.The other three were, for the purpose of the project, consideredas control groups. All four groups were follow<strong>in</strong>g the same programme, though at theirown pace. Without tell<strong>in</strong>g the learners <strong>in</strong> advance, the same test was aga<strong>in</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istered tothe experimental group, as well as to the three control groups, after the six observationalperiods. The pre and post-test results were <strong>in</strong>spected to see whether the study groups’progress had been significantly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the effects of the design.Table 18.1 summarises the results of the pre- and post-MichiganTests results (T1 andT2 on the table).The table shows the average score obta<strong>in</strong>ed by the participants <strong>in</strong> the studyto be slightly higher (74.76) than the one achieved by group 2 (72.66). In comparison, theaverage score of group 2 does not overtake that of group 1, and neither does group 4 overgroup 3. It seems rather unreasonable however to attribute this slight improvement whollyto the procedure <strong>its</strong>elf as it was applied on only two hours of <strong>in</strong>struction out of 24 hours aweek.The merit I can see the procedure objectively deriv<strong>in</strong>g from this slight <strong>in</strong>crease is thatit did not h<strong>in</strong>der the group <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> activities. My presence and the tape-recorder <strong>in</strong> the backof the room did not seem to have negatively affected the group.The total percentage <strong>in</strong>crease for each group is a representation, with<strong>in</strong> the wholeprogramme, of the students’ language tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g development <strong>in</strong> the first six weeks. It appearsto happen <strong>in</strong> an expected way: the lower groups show more progress than group 1 (20.65%)and 2 (37.5 3%). This <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> language development is quite comprehensible s<strong>in</strong>ceknow<strong>in</strong>g much less at the outset of the programme, groups 3 (67.88%) and 4 (103.1 2%)have more room for improvement. The total percentage <strong>in</strong>crease therefore does not displayany conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g sign <strong>in</strong> favour of an <strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>g methodological design.The learners <strong>in</strong> group3, <strong>in</strong> spite of my demands on them at the end of each of the observed sessions, do not achieve<strong>in</strong> any markedly different manner than what would be expected from them if one thoughtthat the procedure could have <strong>in</strong>fluenced the quantity of their learn<strong>in</strong>g.In summary, two types of data were gathered for the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the issue: learners’specific claims collected through uptake charts and detailed accounts of the learn<strong>in</strong>gopportunities obta<strong>in</strong>ed through systematic Observation of audio-recorded, naturallyoccurr<strong>in</strong>g classroom data. These were supplemented with field notes taken by the author.The <strong>in</strong>terview which was <strong>in</strong>tended to provide corroborative data did not produce responsesthat were sufficiently precise to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> relation to what might account for theirclaims. In the end, the bulk of what might help us f<strong>in</strong>d out about the learners’ selectiveattention mechanism would have to arise from a consideration of classroom transcripts <strong>in</strong>relation to uptake charts as the learners themselves did not seem to be aware of whatdirected their attention while attend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction.Both the teacher and the learners under study were <strong>in</strong>formed <strong>in</strong> general terms of thegoals of the research. Both parties were told that the project was seek<strong>in</strong>g a relationship


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 293Table 18.1 Average scores and percentage <strong>in</strong>crease for each groupGroup 1 I Group2 I Group 3* I Group4SS T1 T2 I SS T1 T2SS T1 T2SS T1 T21 80 882 75 833 74 844 67 835 62 766 61 787 60 758 60 649 58 7510 58 7511 57 7811121 63 702 57 733 54 794 53 785 53 686 48 757 52 638 52 769 51 7010 51 6811 50 7850 74 1250 74 12131 60 852 50 723 48 684 47 835 45 826 44 747 43 668 42 849 41 7710 41 7111 41 6839 7239 7238 701 49 732 36 813 35 634 34 715 32 786 30 377 29 638 11 54Average Scores For Each GroupT1 T2 I T1 T2 I T2 T264.72 78.09 I 52.82 72.66 I 44.53 74.76 I 32 65Group 120.65%Group 2 Group 3 Group 437.53% 67.88% 1 03.1 2 Yo*group under studybetween what the <strong>in</strong>formants report as ‘uptake’ and the <strong>in</strong>teractivc process <strong>in</strong> which theclass participates. However, I did not go <strong>in</strong>to any further detail with them, not want<strong>in</strong>g theteacher to give undue emphasis to l<strong>in</strong>guistic items <strong>in</strong> order for learners to remember asmany as possiblc. It was hoped that the usual teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g situation would not be<strong>in</strong>fluenced by alert<strong>in</strong>g the participants’ attention to the researcher’s focus of <strong>in</strong>terest.In fact, when fill<strong>in</strong>g out the ‘uptake charts’ at the end of the first observational lesson,it was noticed that some learners tried to peep at their peers’ charts to enable them toreport more items than they actually could. At this po<strong>in</strong>t it was emphasised to the subjectsthat they should look upon the author as an outsider, a researcher rather than as a teacher,and that whatever reports and comments they made would be entirely confidential. Theirreports were not to be shown to the <strong>in</strong>structor, nor would they have any bear<strong>in</strong>g on theirgrades.As it was planned to observe the same teacher with thc same group for two hours aweek for six weeks, the procedure became rout<strong>in</strong>e and my presence was accepted with ease


294 ASSIA SLIMANIby the learners. The <strong>in</strong>structor also appeared much more rclaxed after the first hour ofobservation. Prior to decid<strong>in</strong>g which teacher was to be part of the project, I felt someresistance and avoidancc on the part of the staff members who alluded to the fact that ‘really,not much is go<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>in</strong> our classes right now’. The procedure discussed <strong>in</strong> this chaptcr waspart of a doctoral project and this made the teachers particularly apprehensive at hav<strong>in</strong>gtheir lessons ‘dissected’ and looked at through ‘magnify<strong>in</strong>g’ lenses for research purposes.However, I persisted <strong>in</strong> spite of their anxiety as their refusal could mean the end of my plans.Therefore, I rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>debted to the ‘chosen’ teacher who, know<strong>in</strong>g that he could not openlyrefuse me without los<strong>in</strong>g face, gracefully adjusted to my persistent presence <strong>in</strong> the back ofhis classes.The rcst of the chapter will describe some of the tentative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (see Slimani 1987for a fuller report) which might help us understand the relationship betwcen the classroom<strong>in</strong>teractive processes and uptake, and their conscquences for evaluation studies. Two<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g characteristics of uptake emerged <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of thc lcarncrs’ uptakecharts. The first characteristic is that most of the learners’ claims were topicalised dur<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>struction. The second is that learners’ uptake is strongly idiosyncratic. Both aspccts willbe discussed <strong>in</strong> detail bclow.Importance of topicalisation on uptakeA thorough study of the <strong>in</strong>formants’ Uptakc Charts and Uptake Identification Probes showedthat a total of 126 items were claimed to have been learned.These items were vcrbs, nouns,adjectives, adverbs, connectors, auxiliaries, models and some set phrases. Almost all (1 12items or 89 per cent) of what the respondents claimed to have seen and learned for the firsttime <strong>in</strong> those six observed lessons, had, <strong>in</strong> one way or another, been focused upon dur<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>struction. 1 12 out of 126 were given some sort of prom<strong>in</strong>ence by be<strong>in</strong>g the topic ofconversation while the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g fourteen items or 11 pcr cent happened as part ofclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction with no particular cmphasis brought upon them. The follow<strong>in</strong>gexcerpts illustrate the various means used to focus upon or topicalise those items claimedto have been learned: ‘least’, ‘list’, ‘like’, ‘look after’, ‘look like’, ‘match’, ‘<strong>in</strong> order to’.T:T:L1:L2 :L3 :T:L4 :L5 :T:L6:T:T:r..What’s the dflerence between least and list?[po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g at both items written on the board].The mother looks after her son at home. Canyou use another word or expression <strong>in</strong>steadoflook after?Don’t worry.Not worried but uh the same uh.Uh, take care.OK. When I say uh this car is like that one, what does ‘like’mean?Similar.Almost the same.OK. Now, John’s new car looks almost the same. What is ‘looks’?To see . . .To see, uhuh. So, canyou replace ‘to look’here by ‘to see’and say yohn’s new car sees almostthe same?Let’s see the <strong>in</strong>structions given here and see if they match. To match, that’s a newword, I th<strong>in</strong>k [writes it on board]. To match. [A long explanation with attempts tojndsynonyms follows.]OK, <strong>in</strong> order to. What does that mcan!


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 295In the above cases thc uptaken items have themselves, however briefly, become the ostensibletopic of the conversation rather than be<strong>in</strong>g simply a part of classroom discourse.The episodesdeal<strong>in</strong>g with these particular features are also seen to be term<strong>in</strong>ated by some feedback fromthe teacher which might be expected to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted by learners as <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that an itemis worth pay<strong>in</strong>g attention to.The difference between the fourteen (1 1 per cent) and 1 12 (89 per cent) items claimedto have been learned dur<strong>in</strong>g the sessions under study is that the latter had, to a greater orlesser extent, been the specific topic of <strong>in</strong>struction by hav<strong>in</strong>g their mean<strong>in</strong>g, their spell<strong>in</strong>g,their pronunciation and sometimes two or all three aspects treated by the teacher and/orby the learners. In the case where learners provide their pcers with guidance <strong>in</strong> one or otherof the aspects, the teacher is seen to <strong>in</strong>tervene by approv<strong>in</strong>g the provision of <strong>in</strong>formation.It must be emphasised however that this does not necessarily mean that the claimeditems were <strong>in</strong>tended to be taught prior to the lessons. Many of them, as the follow<strong>in</strong>gexamples show, arose <strong>in</strong>cidentally <strong>in</strong> the course of events and became topics <strong>in</strong> discourseterms.. . . Bob/bought/five books and George did too.6: L:T:L:Bob? What did he do? [Teacher <strong>in</strong>terrupts]Five booksT: What did he do?L: /bought/LL: Bought [correct pronunciation]T:L:Bought. Which verb is that?To buy.T: To buy, bought bought7 T: . . . OK. Did you like it?L: Yes, yes, I likc it.T: Yes, I?L: Yes, I likcd it.T: Yes, I liked it or I did.It appears, then, that with<strong>in</strong> the lim<strong>its</strong> of the analysis so far of the uptaken items, <strong>in</strong>structionhas exercised a rather positive impact on the subjects s<strong>in</strong>ce 1 12 out of the 126 items claimedto have been learned for the first time dur<strong>in</strong>g those observed lessons have become, howevermomentarily, teach<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>ts. However, a close exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the data suggests that theabove statement alone is far from establish<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>structor’s supremacy as a learn<strong>in</strong>gfacilitator. A further <strong>in</strong>vestigation was necessary to f<strong>in</strong>d out the proportion of the topicaliseditems that are claimed as new acquisitions <strong>in</strong> relation to those which have apparently bcenthe subject of similar <strong>in</strong>tentions and treatment but which failed to lead to any claims on thepart of the subjects.To evaluate the proportion of what has been claimed to be learned from what has beenpedagogically focused upon <strong>in</strong> some way dur<strong>in</strong>g those six <strong>in</strong>structional sessions, the sumtotal of the topicalised items was counted <strong>in</strong>dependently of whether they had been claimedas new or otherwise on the learners’ uptake charts. The results are summarised <strong>in</strong> Table18.2 where column 1 <strong>in</strong>dicates the total number of items topicalised <strong>in</strong> each lesson. Column2 presents the total number of items which are both focused upon and also claimed by atleast one learner to have been learned. Column 3 <strong>in</strong>troduces those which have not led toany positive assertion on the part of the subjects despite the attention paid to them, andcolumn 4 displays the total number of items which have been claimed to be partly orcompletely familiar already and therefore ‘<strong>in</strong>eligible’ for learn<strong>in</strong>g claims <strong>in</strong> the context of


296 ASSIA SLIMANIFTable 18.2 Effect of topicalisation’ILESSONS Total No oftopicalised items4Topicalised andclaimedTopicalised butnot claimedTopicalised butknownI TOTAL I 256405631603732172116311116162312151907100%112I 9243.75% 35.93%0712031407095220.3 1 Yothis study.The data of the last column were derived from the answers to questions b, c, andd on Uptake Identification Probes which were distributed to help learners dissociate theitems they believed they had learned dur<strong>in</strong>g the observed lessons from those they had alreadyencountered <strong>in</strong> different circumstances. The observed lessons <strong>in</strong> which these items occurredaga<strong>in</strong> could not fully justify their ‘uptak<strong>in</strong>g’ as they have already happened <strong>in</strong> situations whichmight have facilitated their learn<strong>in</strong>g.Table 18.2 shows that out of 256 topicalised cases provid<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities forthe class, 92 failed to attract the learners’ attention and 52 were claimed to be somewhatknown as they hat1 already encountered them <strong>in</strong> earlier events unrelated to this study. Inother words, 43.75 per cent focused episodes have ‘reached the target’, while 35.93 percent went completely unnoticed and 20.31 per cent were already to some cxtcnt familiarto the subjects.The above figures provide us with a picture of the ‘syllabus as reality’ as opposed to the‘syllabus as plan’. The former represents what actually happens <strong>in</strong> the midst of <strong>in</strong>teractivework done by the participants. The on-go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction leads to the creation of a wholerange of learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities, some of which are the results of the teachcr’s plan; othersarise as a by-product of the plan, but some others arise <strong>in</strong>dependently of any <strong>in</strong>tentions,pcrhaps as a by-product of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction.No precise comparison can be made with the ‘syllabus as plan’ which is def<strong>in</strong>ed as asyllabus which attempts to predict what is likcly to be learned from a planned learn<strong>in</strong>gevent. I was not, despite my request, providcd with very many details about the teacher’sobjectivcs. I was given the title of the structure to be taught and thc series of exercises <strong>in</strong>the textbook to practise the grammatical features to be <strong>in</strong>troduced to the group.Hence, the detailed study of the classroom discourse has revealed that about 44 percent only of what has been pedagogically topicalised was claimed by the learners. Evcnthough the teacher’s objectivcs were gcarcd toward the teach<strong>in</strong>g of some particularstructural features, most of the 44 per cent were lexical items claimed to be seen and learnedfor the first time <strong>in</strong> those observed events. Nevertheless it would be mislead<strong>in</strong>g to concludethat the lessons were not successful because learners did not claim many of the structuralobjectives the teacher had on his plan. Although it might be suggested that the shortage ofgrammatical claims is due to thc possibility that it is much easier to report lexis because


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 297this does not require the use of metalanguage, <strong>in</strong> fact, a close perusal of the learners’ uptakecharts demonstrates that the <strong>in</strong>formants were perfectly capable of report<strong>in</strong>g what went ondur<strong>in</strong>g the course of the lessons <strong>in</strong> terms of grammar. By and large, learners succeeded <strong>in</strong>account<strong>in</strong>g for the teacher’s structural <strong>in</strong>tentions by report<strong>in</strong>g the title if not writ<strong>in</strong>g thema<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts of the sessions. Some even illustrated the teacher’s focus of <strong>in</strong>struction byprovid<strong>in</strong>g examples of sentences to show their comprehension or at least familiarity withwhat was taught. This suggests that the <strong>in</strong>formants did not lack the means of express<strong>in</strong>g thestructural objectives.It is believed that one of the reasons why learners did not report as many structuralfeatures as lexical ones is that several of these features were already familiar to the class. Infact, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that most of the structural features emphasised dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structionwere not reported as newly learned because most of them, if not all of them, were part ofthe syllabus <strong>in</strong> high school. For <strong>in</strong>stance, only one <strong>in</strong>formant claimed to have seen andlearned the passive and active voices for the first time dur<strong>in</strong>g the observed events. In fact,these affirmations are confirmed by the 20 per cent of topicalised episodes <strong>in</strong> the lessonwhich were claimed to be part of the learners’ prior knowledge. One could add that aftera few hours of teach<strong>in</strong>g, second language <strong>in</strong>struction becomes very much remedial asstructural features are presented and represented for a review.It looks as if the learners’ claims are somewhat different from what the teacher hasplanned for them. His <strong>in</strong>tentions might have helped learners to rehearse already encountered(if not mastered) structural features. However, <strong>in</strong> the process of carry<strong>in</strong>g out the plan, the<strong>in</strong>teractive work has lent <strong>its</strong>elf to the creation of a whole range of perhaps unexpected andbeneficial events (at least, to some learners if not to all).The learners’ claims (44 per centonTable 18.2) rema<strong>in</strong> a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of the teacher’s objectives but also their by-productas well as the by-product of the classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction. For these reasons, therefore, attemptsto evaluate the learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes aga<strong>in</strong>st the teacher’s plan can be mislead<strong>in</strong>g if one doesnot take <strong>in</strong>to account the mediat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractive processes which characterise classroom<strong>in</strong>teraction.In view of the data expressed <strong>in</strong> the table, therefore, the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>fluence over thesubjects’ learn<strong>in</strong>g did not reveal <strong>its</strong>elf to be as strong as suggested earlier s<strong>in</strong>ce approximately56 per cent of what has been focused upon did not apparently bear any immediate fruit: 20per cent were claimed to be already familiar and 36 per cent were not, <strong>in</strong> any way, mentionedby the learners.It should lie po<strong>in</strong>ted out that about 77.45 per cent of the topicalisation was effected bythe teacher. This is not particularly surpris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> view of the fact that the discourse wasunidirectionally controlled by the teacher, who did 45 per cent of the talk<strong>in</strong>g. What appearsto be strik<strong>in</strong>gly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g though is that a further analysis of the effect of the teacher’sversus the learners’ scarce opportunities (22.54 per cent) for topicalisation showed that thelatter offered much higher chances for items to be uptaken. Learners benefited much morefrom their peers’ rare <strong>in</strong>stances of topicalisation than from the teacher’s.A close scrut<strong>in</strong>y of the theme of topicalisation reveals that topics <strong>in</strong>itiated by learnersattracted more claims from the learners than the ones <strong>in</strong>itiated by the teacher. The analysi sshows that out of 46 items <strong>in</strong>itiated by the learners, 34 (73.9 per cent) were claimed,whereas only 78 (49.4 per cent) out of 158 were claimed when topicalised by the <strong>in</strong>structor.Thus, the chances for claims are much higher when items are triggered by classmates. Afurther emphasis on the profitability of the learners’ <strong>in</strong>itiation is that it attracts morereporters than when topics are brought up by the teacher.By limit<strong>in</strong>g to himself the <strong>in</strong>itiative of topicalis<strong>in</strong>g most items for <strong>in</strong>struction, the teacherdoes not give the learners much opportunity to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between items which are


298 ASSIA SLIMANIimportant and those which are not.To this particular teacher everyth<strong>in</strong>g was relevant. It istherefore possible that the reason why the participants of this study were not affectedby the teacher’s efforts is that <strong>in</strong> his attempts to focus their attention on everyth<strong>in</strong>g, nospecific aspect appeared as particularly prom<strong>in</strong>cnt <strong>in</strong> his discourse. Hav<strong>in</strong>g little opportunityto raise topics for <strong>in</strong>struction, learners might have made some features outstand<strong>in</strong>g to theirpeers if only for the reason that, com<strong>in</strong>g from learners, topicalisation appeared as amemorable event rather than the rout<strong>in</strong>e procedure of the teacher (see Slimani 1989 forfurther details).F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong> this discussion it is worth mention<strong>in</strong>g that the majority of the unnoticed or‘lost’ items (36 per cent) are <strong>in</strong>stances of error treatment provided most often by theteacher. Their analysis has allowed the identification of a limited number of features whichdifferentiate their treatment from that allocated to the topicalised and claimed items (112,or 44 per cent). As the illustrations below show, it appears that absence of metalanguage <strong>in</strong>the teacher’s talk and straight provision, most often by the teacher, of the correct form ofthe item under focus, without further <strong>in</strong>volvement from the teacher or the learners,characterise the strategies used to deal with these items (see examples 8, 9, 10 below).Cue<strong>in</strong>g by the teacher is another common corrective strategy sometimes followed by theimmediate provision of the expected forms by the speaker himself, if he swiftly manages tospot the error (example 1 l), by his peers (example 12) but less often by thc <strong>in</strong>structor.8 L: . . . and uh sometimes uh on Wednesday.T: And sometimes on Wednesdays. Why on Wednesdays?9101112L:T:. . . I look<strong>in</strong>g for my pen.You are look<strong>in</strong>g for your penL1: . . . [Read<strong>in</strong>g from the book] Bob dr<strong>in</strong>k a glass.L2: Dr<strong>in</strong>ks [Interrupts the speakcr].L2: Bob dr<strong>in</strong>ks a glass of milk every day and Gcorgc does too.L: Pencils have been sharpT: Sharp?L: SharpenedT: Sharpened, yes.L: . . . Thc simplest method is by swimm<strong>in</strong>g on one side. The rcscucr pulls the victimby the /hair/LS: Hair, hair [correct pronunciation]T: Yes, hair, by the hair. All right . . .Nearly a third of the lost items consists of corrections of tenses and -s morphemes.Informants can, however, be assumed to be already familiar with these features as they havebeen the explicit content of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> other lessons or <strong>in</strong> high school. Despite previousexposure to explicit explanation of the rules and recurrent repetitions of the correct formsof these features, the subjects of this study persisted <strong>in</strong> misus<strong>in</strong>g them when us<strong>in</strong>g the targetlanguage. It is possible that the <strong>in</strong>formants are not ready to learn these structures as part oftheir <strong>in</strong>terlanguage system and consequently their cont<strong>in</strong>ued treatment rema<strong>in</strong>s po<strong>in</strong>tless,at least, at this stage of their tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. It is widely accepted that features such as the use ofarticles by Arab speakers and some of the -s morphemes, for many <strong>English</strong> as a secondlanguage speakers, rema<strong>in</strong> unmastered <strong>in</strong> oral production till an extremely advanced stageof their tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g even if these features are explicitly known to the tra<strong>in</strong>ees. This situationmakes us question the necessity or otherwise of attempt<strong>in</strong>g to keep on correct<strong>in</strong>g features


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 299which have been persistently dealt with but still rema<strong>in</strong> largely ignored by some learnersdur<strong>in</strong>g verbal <strong>in</strong>teraction (see Slimani 1987 for further quantitative and qualitative analysisof error treatment <strong>in</strong> this sett<strong>in</strong>g).Learners’ idiosyncraciesThe second characteristic which emerged from the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the learners’ claims isthat uptake is highly idiosyncratic. The fcaturc is particularly revcal<strong>in</strong>g for evaluation whichgenerally assumes the effect of <strong>in</strong>struction is somehow uniform for most members of theclass. Such evaluation takes as <strong>its</strong> start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t the teacher’s plan which is expected tocontrol what learners would sec as optimal <strong>in</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g. Even though the teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> thisparticular sett<strong>in</strong>g was not differentiated <strong>in</strong> any obvious way, i.e., <strong>in</strong> the sense that differentlearners were given different tasks, it appears that typically only very few learners at anyone time happened to take the <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong>. Table 18.3 illustrates the extreme<strong>in</strong>dividuality with which learners react to <strong>in</strong>struction. It prcsents thc total number (N) ofitcms or l<strong>in</strong>guistic features (1 26) reported to have been learned dur<strong>in</strong>g the observed sessionsas well as the percentage of claims associated with them and the number of reporters thateach case has attracted.Tohle 18.3 Percentage of claims madc by reporters on each l<strong>in</strong>guistic featureN of items (1 26) I % of claimsN of reporters47202775103312137.30%15.872 1.42Total 74.59%5.553.967.932.382.38I Total 22.20%0.791.580.791523491011Total 3.16%The results po<strong>in</strong>t to thc fact that as many as 74.59 per cent of the total number of claimsare reported by no morc than three learners at a time, and no fewer than 37.30 per cent of thetotal are reportcd by only one person at any one time. A negligible percentage (3.16 per cent)of claims is simultaneously made by n<strong>in</strong>e, ten or eleven subjects.These figures express the highlevel of ‘<strong>in</strong>dividuality’ and ‘autonomy’ with which some subjects might face <strong>in</strong>struction. The


300 ASSIA SLIMANIfigures are particularly strik<strong>in</strong>g as the teach<strong>in</strong>g style was not <strong>in</strong>dividualised <strong>in</strong> any sense. Itwas unidirectionally addressed to the class as a whole. One, therefore, might expect thesame items or l<strong>in</strong>guistic features to be claimed by many learners. What happened howeveris that <strong>in</strong>dividual learners reacted <strong>in</strong>dividually despite the centrality of the teach<strong>in</strong>g style.Further evidence that learners show autonomy when undergo<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions is alsoclearly illustrated <strong>in</strong> the 1 1 per cent or fourteen uptaken items that were mentioned earlierunder the head<strong>in</strong>g of the importance of topicalisation. While 1 12 l<strong>in</strong>guistic features claimedto be learned were the focus of <strong>in</strong>struction, fourteen happened as a part of the classroomdiscourse without any specific attention drawn to them. Despite a teach<strong>in</strong>g situation wherethe classroom discourse is highly controlled by the teacher and does not <strong>in</strong>volve any groupwork activity, learners have shown considerable <strong>in</strong>dividual reaction by claim<strong>in</strong>g items whichdid not receive any k<strong>in</strong>d of attention <strong>in</strong> terms of topicalisation, as def<strong>in</strong>ed earlier.The aboveproportion might have been even higher if the teacher had allowed more room for learnersto express themselves.While some of the 1 1 per cent of the claims were traced back as part of the discourseto deal with classroom rout<strong>in</strong>es, some were not found at all <strong>in</strong> the transcripts. To expla<strong>in</strong>their presence on the learners’ uptake charts, one can only assume that what went on dur<strong>in</strong>gthe lessons possibly re<strong>in</strong>forced some previous learn<strong>in</strong>g and brought those particular wordsback to the learners’ m<strong>in</strong>ds.The word ‘slippers’, for <strong>in</strong>stance, rema<strong>in</strong>ed a complete mysteryas I did not even recall the teacher hav<strong>in</strong>g dealt, however remotely, with a situation whichmight have led to such a claim on the part of the learner. Moreover, the exam<strong>in</strong>ation of thelearners’ charts revealed also the presence of a few examples of appropriate generalisation.For <strong>in</strong>stance, when the words ‘thick’, ‘thickness’, and ‘th<strong>in</strong>’ were expla<strong>in</strong>ed, one of the mostable learners reported hav<strong>in</strong>g learned the word ‘th<strong>in</strong>ness’ even though the latter was notuttered <strong>in</strong> class. The word ‘narrow’ was also claimed to have been learned by the samelearner <strong>in</strong> relation to ‘thick’ and ‘th<strong>in</strong>’.It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to notice here this learner’s tcndcncy to generalise so successfully froma lesson event that he can believe the generalisation was taught. In this respect, it has beensuggested that one of the good language learner’s attributes is to be able to organise thediscrete and disparate <strong>in</strong>formation they receive about the target language <strong>in</strong>to coherent andordered patterns (Rub<strong>in</strong> 1975; Stern 1975).ConclusionThe problem of mak<strong>in</strong>g sense of <strong>in</strong>struction seems to lie <strong>in</strong> the difficulty of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gappropriate research techniques capable of evaluat<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes <strong>in</strong> relation to <strong>in</strong>put.In this paper, <strong>in</strong>put is seen as a co-production by the participants <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>structional sett<strong>in</strong>gand therefore renders the task of us<strong>in</strong>g traditional test<strong>in</strong>g measures rather difficult. Weattempted to f<strong>in</strong>d a way of relat<strong>in</strong>g the learners’ claims to their immediate <strong>in</strong>teractiveenvironment.The technique used proved to lie a useful means of shedd<strong>in</strong>g light on what is claimedto be lcarncd from the on-go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractive work which takes place <strong>in</strong> the classroom. Byask<strong>in</strong>g learners to reflect on their perceptions of what they have uptaken, one could see, byexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>teractive work, some of the factors which characterise the emergence ofthese particular uptaken features.Most of the learners’ claims were topicalised. In this sense, White’s (1987)recommendations seem to broadly match the present teacher’s bchaviour <strong>in</strong> this particularcontext. She suggests that


~ (1983)~ (1EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 301We should not be afraid occasionally to provide <strong>in</strong>put which is explicitly geared toward. . . the form of grammatical teach<strong>in</strong>g, of correction, or otherforms Ofemphasis onparticular structures [my emphasis]; at worst, it will be ignored and at best, it may trigerchange <strong>in</strong> the acquisition system. (White 1987: 108)Br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g particular l<strong>in</strong>guistic features to the class’s attention appears to be a rather valuablecharacteristic of uptake as most of the uptaken items were focused upon dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction.The fact that most of the ‘lost’ items were error correction does not necessarily contradictthe effect of topicalisation. Learners may not be ready to <strong>in</strong>ternalise particular structuralfeatures despite their persistent explanation and correction. Correction is often seen, <strong>in</strong>this study, to be provided <strong>in</strong> an erratic and confus<strong>in</strong>g manner. The study revealed that whilesome uptaken featurcs were products of the teacher’s plan, others were by-products of theplan or perhaps of the classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction.These uptaken items, which represent 44 per cent of the participants’ <strong>in</strong>teractiveefforts, are revealed to be highly idiosyncratic. The detailed analysis of the <strong>in</strong>teractiveprocesses has shown that different features of the same event have been uptaken by differentlearners. Very few items were claimed by all or even most learners. Moreover, while manyof the claims could be traced <strong>in</strong> the transcripts as hav<strong>in</strong>g received some k<strong>in</strong>d of emphasison the part of the participants, mostly of the teacher, others merely occurred as part of theclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction or did not feature at all <strong>in</strong> the text, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that learners reactedwith some autonomy to what went on dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>teractive event.View<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>put as co-produced by the participants has highlighted idiosyncrasy andtopicalisation as particularly relevant to evaluation studies which generally tend to assesslearn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes on the basis of the teacher’s objectives: these objectives are subsequentlyassumed to be learned by most learners <strong>in</strong> the class. A test based on the teacher’s objectiveswould have taken <strong>in</strong>to consideration the features which the teacher planned to treat. Sucha test would, by <strong>its</strong> nature, ignore the very many other features which <strong>in</strong>cidentally arosedur<strong>in</strong>g the actual classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, some of which learners claimed to have bcnefitcdfrom.Because of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that what actually gets topicalised dur<strong>in</strong>g the claw-oom<strong>in</strong>teractive work is different from the teacher’s plan, and because uptake is stronglyidiosyncratic, it is therefore not helpful to use the teacher’s plan as a measur<strong>in</strong>g rod for whathas been uptaken from the lesson. In fact, a consideration of the actual classroom <strong>in</strong>teractivework which characterises second language <strong>in</strong>struction and a study of learner idiosyncrasymight help us ga<strong>in</strong> a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of the complexities of second language teachngand learn<strong>in</strong>g. This understand<strong>in</strong>g might subsequently <strong>in</strong>form the improvement of evaluationsof what actually gets learned from language programmes.ReferencesAllwright, R.L. (ed.) (1975a) ‘Work<strong>in</strong>g papers: languagc teach<strong>in</strong>g classroom rcscarch’ .Dcpartment of <strong>Language</strong> and L<strong>in</strong>guistics, University of Essex, England.Allwright, R.L. (1975b) ‘Problems <strong>in</strong> the study of the teacher’s treatment of learner error’, <strong>in</strong>Burt and Dulay: 96-109.‘The nature and function of the syllabus <strong>in</strong> languagc teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g’.Unpublished mimeograph. Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Modern <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong>,Lancaster University.984a) ‘Why don’t learners learn what teachers teach? The <strong>in</strong>teraction hypothesis’, <strong>in</strong>


~ (1~ (1~ (1~ (1~ (1982)~ (1989)The302 ASSIA SLIMANID. M. S<strong>in</strong>gleton and D. G. Little (eds) <strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formal and <strong>in</strong>formal contexts, pp.3-18. Dubl<strong>in</strong>: IRAAL.984b) ‘The importance of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. AppliedL<strong>in</strong>guistics, 52 : 156-7 1 .984c) ‘The analysis of discourse <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage studies: the pedagogical evidence’, <strong>in</strong>Davies, Criper and Howatt.988) ‘Autonomy and <strong>in</strong>dividualisation <strong>in</strong> whole-class <strong>in</strong>struction’, <strong>in</strong> A. Brookes and P.Grundy (eds) Individualization and autonomy <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g, pp. 3544. ELTDocuments 131. London: Modern <strong>English</strong> Publications, British Council.Brecn, M.P. and Candl<strong>in</strong>, C.N. (1980) ‘The essentials of a communicative curriculum <strong>in</strong>language tcach<strong>in</strong>g’, Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics l(2): 89-1 12.Burt, M. and Dulay, H.C. (1975) On TESOL ‘75. New directions <strong>in</strong> second language, learn<strong>in</strong>g,teach<strong>in</strong>g and bil<strong>in</strong>gual education. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C. : TESOL.Corder, S.P. (1977) ‘<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> as a second 1anguage:Trcnds <strong>in</strong> research andpractice’, <strong>in</strong> H.D. Brown, C.A.Yorio, and R.H. Crymes (eds) On TESOL ‘77. <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> andlearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong> as a second language: trends <strong>in</strong> research and practice. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.:TESOL.Davies, A., Criper, C. and Howatt, A.P.R. (cds) (1984) Interlanguage. Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh: Ed<strong>in</strong>burghUniversity Press.Ellis, R. (1984) ‘Can syntax be taught?: a study of the effects of formal <strong>in</strong>struction on theacquisition ofWH questions by children’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics S(2): 138-55.Ellis, R. and Rathbone, M. (1987) ‘The acquisition of German <strong>in</strong> a classroom context’.Unpublished report. Eal<strong>in</strong>g College of Higher Education.Fanselow, J. (1977) ‘The treatment of learner error <strong>in</strong> oral work’. Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Annals 10:583-93.Krashen, S.D. (1980) ‘The theoretical and practical relevance of simple codes <strong>in</strong> secondlanguage acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> R. Scarcella and S.D. Krashen (eds) Research <strong>in</strong> second languageacqusition, pp. 7-1 8. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury Housc.981) Second language acquisition and second language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and practice <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition. NewYork: Pergamon.Lightbown, P.M. (1983) ‘Exploratory relationships between developmental and <strong>in</strong>structionalsequences <strong>in</strong> L2 acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> H.W. Scligcr and M.H. Long (eds) Classroom orientedresearch <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, pp. 2 1745. Rowlcy, Mass. : Newbury House.MacFarlanc, J.M. (1975) ‘Some types of psychological discussion that help to establish theteacher’s treatment of error as a fruitful variable for <strong>in</strong>vestigation’, <strong>in</strong> Allwright (1975a)pp. 4-63.Morray, M. (1976) ‘INELEC: Teamwork <strong>in</strong> an EST program’, <strong>in</strong> British Council Team teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ESP’. ELT Document 106. ETIC Publications.Rub<strong>in</strong>, J. (1975) ‘What the “good language learner” can teach us’. TESOL Quarterly 9( 1): 41-5 1.Schercr, A. and Wcrthcimer, M. (1964) A psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic experiment <strong>in</strong>.foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g.NewYork: McGraw Hill.Slimani, A. (1987) ‘The teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g relationships: Learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities and learn<strong>in</strong>goutcomes. An Algerian case study’. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, LancasterUniversity, England.role of topicalisation <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g. .’stem 17: 223-34.Smith, P.D. (1970) A comparison .f the cognitive and audiol<strong>in</strong>gual approaches to foreign language<strong>in</strong>struction: The fennyslvania Foreign <strong>Language</strong> Project. Philadelphia, Pcnn. : The Center forCurriculum Development.Spada, N.M. (1987) ‘Relationships between <strong>in</strong>structional differences and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes: aprocess-product study of communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 8( 2):137-61.


EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 303Stern, H.H. (1975) ‘What can wc learn from the good language learner?’ Canadian Modern<strong>Language</strong> Review 31: 304-18; also <strong>in</strong> K. Croft (ed.) (1980) Read<strong>in</strong>gs on <strong>English</strong> as a secondlanguage: for teachers and teacher tra<strong>in</strong>ees (2nd edition). Cambridge, Mass. : W<strong>in</strong>throp.Whitc, L. (1 987) ‘Aga<strong>in</strong>st comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put: the <strong>in</strong>put hypothesis and the development ofsecond language competence’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 8(2): 95-1 10.


APPENDIX 1: UPTAKE RECALL CHARTQUESTION: WHAT POINTS HAVE COME UP IN TODAY'S LESSON?Please answer FULLY and <strong>in</strong> DETAIL. Try to remember EVERYTHING.DATE:NAME:1. GRAMMAR:5. PUNCTUATION:6. WAYS OF USING THE LANGUAGE:2. WORDS AND PHRASES:----7 '. SUGGESTIONS ABOUT MORE EFFECT1 IVE INSTRUCTION: -3. SPELLING:8. OTHER(S) . . . (Please specify):PRONUNCIATION:Thank you for your cooperation


APPENDIX 2: UPTAKE IDENTIFICATION PROBEREAD CAREFULLY THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS, MARKYOUR ANSWERSAS INDICATED ON THE ‘UPTAKE RECALL CHART’.1. Of all the th<strong>in</strong>gs you wrote on your ‘Uptake Recall Chart’, which do you th<strong>in</strong>kyou learned today?(a) Did you learn anyth<strong>in</strong>g that was really new to you? If yes, circle it.(b) Did you learn anyth<strong>in</strong>g that was not really completely new, that you knewpartly already? If yes, underl<strong>in</strong>e it.(c) Was there anyth<strong>in</strong>g that you did not learn at all because you knew it already?If yes, mark it with a zigzag l<strong>in</strong>e.2. Of all the th<strong>in</strong>gs you wrote, which do you th<strong>in</strong>k the teacher most wanted you tolearn? Mark them with aT.Thank you for your cooperation.


Chapter 19Michael P. BreenNAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE:ON WHAT IS LEARNED IN THELANGUAGE CLASSROOMIntroductionCENTRAL CONCERN FOR LANGUAGE teachers is what learners can learnA from language lessons. Allwright, <strong>in</strong> a somewhat startl<strong>in</strong>g paper some years ago,deduced that, regardless of what a teacher taught <strong>in</strong> a lesson, the learners will <strong>in</strong>evitablylearn different th<strong>in</strong>gs from the same lesson (Allwright 1984). He expla<strong>in</strong>ed thisunpredictable trend with reference to the overt spoken <strong>in</strong>teraction that takes place betweenteachers and learners and the covert <strong>in</strong>teraction that takes place between the learner andthe various sources of <strong>in</strong>put dur<strong>in</strong>g a lesson, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the text of the lesson and otherwritten texts available to the learners. Such <strong>in</strong>teraction, he argued, mediates between whatthe teachers teach as “<strong>in</strong>put” and what learners actually “uptake” from the lesson. In otherwords, the <strong>in</strong>teractive process of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the particular context of theclassroom ensures variation <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.In this chapter, I wish to explore this phenomenon further by focus<strong>in</strong>g upon thediscourse of language lessons as revealed by current research. I want to suggest that one ofthe crucial th<strong>in</strong>gs which learners learn <strong>in</strong> the classroom is how to navigate the opportunitiesand constra<strong>in</strong>ts provided by classroom discourse. A central argument will be that relativesuccess or failure <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g can be at least partly expla<strong>in</strong>ed withreference to how learners choose or are obliged to undertake such navigation. Of course,the particular features of the classroom context which I describe can not provide a fullyadequate explanation of variation <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g. The <strong>in</strong>fluences of the context oflearn<strong>in</strong>g are only one set of variables <strong>in</strong> the broader picture. However, I wish to assert thatan account of such <strong>in</strong>fluences can enrich Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition research and theoryand usefully <strong>in</strong>form the practical concerns of language pedagogy.Expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Second <strong>Language</strong> AcquisitionAny adequate theory of Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition (SLA) has to account for three keyfactors and, crucially, their <strong>in</strong>terrelationship. These are: (1) what the learner br<strong>in</strong>gs orcontributes to the process, from <strong>in</strong>nate predispositions, through the activation of certa<strong>in</strong>


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 307psychological processes such as attention or memory, and through affective <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong>the process, to strategic behaviour which may rcnder the process more manageable andunthreaten<strong>in</strong>g; (2) the nature of the actual language learn<strong>in</strong>g process; and (3) the outcomesfrom the process <strong>in</strong> terms of l<strong>in</strong>guistic or, more broadly, communicative competence <strong>in</strong> thetarget language.In explor<strong>in</strong>g this rclationship, SLA research to date has primarily focused upon the<strong>in</strong>teraction between what learners contribute, particularly their <strong>in</strong>nate template for languageor their cognitive processes, and the language data made available to them. In a recent reviewof SLA research, I argued that the research appears to favour particular paradigms of learn<strong>in</strong>gand, thereby, constructs the learner <strong>in</strong> particular ways (Brcen, 1996). Summaris<strong>in</strong>g verybriefly, SLA research tells us a great deal about the learner as be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpretative,accommodat<strong>in</strong>g, and strategic. That is, the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>in</strong>put and thc effortto express mean<strong>in</strong>g appear to be thc catalysts for language 1earn<strong>in</strong>g.The accommodation bythe lcarner of language data is typified by the learner’s creative construction of<strong>in</strong>tcrlanguages which represent gradual approximations to the target language. And bothlearn<strong>in</strong>g strategies and communicative strategies are adopted by learners <strong>in</strong> order to maketheir <strong>in</strong>terpretative and accommodat<strong>in</strong>g work much more manageable. These threeconstructs of the learner which we can deduce from the research contribute significantlyto an explanation of how language is learned.However, this explanation will rema<strong>in</strong> partial if much of SLA research persists <strong>in</strong>decontextualis<strong>in</strong>g learner contributions, the learn<strong>in</strong>g process, and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes fromthe location <strong>in</strong> which these three factors are realised. Ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA research, <strong>in</strong> focus<strong>in</strong>gupon the relationship between the learner and language data, is conducted and reported on<strong>in</strong> ways that appear to overlook the social reality <strong>in</strong> which the research is actually conducted.Dyadic encounters between caretakers and young learners or between native spcakerresearchers and non-native speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formants, experimental situations us<strong>in</strong>g elicitationtechniques, quasi-experimental negotiation tasks undertaken by non-native speakers, orobserved <strong>in</strong>teractions dur<strong>in</strong>g lessons are never socially neutral activities. To reduce the datafrom such events to a psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic objectivity of <strong>in</strong>puts and outputs is to dislocate themfrom their <strong>in</strong>tersubjectivc nature. The evidence we obta<strong>in</strong> from any learn<strong>in</strong>g evcnt, even <strong>in</strong>a quasi experimental sett<strong>in</strong>g, is significantly shaped by thc social situation and the socialrelations with<strong>in</strong> that event.If we used Ellis’s recent very comprehensive review of SLA research (Ellis, 1994) as an<strong>in</strong>dicator of the major focus of SLA researchers to the present time, we f<strong>in</strong>d that more thantwo thirds of the chapters <strong>in</strong> his account refer to work which assumes that the <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween the learner’s mental resources and features of l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put will provide asufficiently adequate explanation for language learn<strong>in</strong>g Ellis fairly reflects current SLAresearch <strong>in</strong> devot<strong>in</strong>g just over a quarter of his review to more recent studies which locatethe <strong>in</strong>teraction between learner and language <strong>in</strong> the context of <strong>in</strong>terpersonal or socialsituations. His account reveals that context has been def<strong>in</strong>ed or framed <strong>in</strong> particular waysby SLA research. It is addressed <strong>in</strong> a fragmentary way as a diversity of“socia1 factors”- fromidentification by the learncr with the target language group to the possible effects of differenttypes of language programs ~ or as the specific features of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, or as thepossible impact of formal <strong>in</strong>struction. Ellis himself concludes that “the relationship betweensocial factors and L2 achievemcnt is an <strong>in</strong>direct rather than a direct one” (1994: 239). Inreferr<strong>in</strong>g to classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction studies, he concludes that they have “contributed littleto our understand<strong>in</strong>g of how <strong>in</strong>teraction affects acquisition” (1994: 607). And he deducesthat formal <strong>in</strong>ytruction can, at most, be credited with “facilitat<strong>in</strong>g natural languagedeve1opment”<strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>creased accuracy and accelerated progress (1994: 659).


308 MICHAEL P. BREENThe apparent assumption <strong>in</strong> thcse deductions is that “L2 achievement”, “acquisition” or“natural language development” can somehow occur almost regardless of contextualvariables. In this chapter, however, I want to suggest that, if we look morc closely at theclassroom as context, such a focus will reveal that the <strong>in</strong>tcraction between learner and dataand the differential outcomes from this <strong>in</strong>teraction will be significantly moulded andcircumscribed by that context. If we discovered and could implement <strong>in</strong> the classroom allthosc ideal conditions which we may deduce from currcnt SLA research as optimal forlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g, learners will still differcntially achieve. They will cont<strong>in</strong>ue to learn mostlydifferent th<strong>in</strong>gs, at different rates, and to different levels of proficiency. Clearly a part ofthis variation <strong>in</strong> outcomes will bc duc to diversity <strong>in</strong> the contributions of the lcarncrs tothe process. But variation will also have to lie expla<strong>in</strong>ed with rcfcrcncc to the particularcontext <strong>in</strong> which the learn<strong>in</strong>g occurred so that <strong>in</strong>put, process, and outcomes are seen asfunctions of how thc learners variously def<strong>in</strong>ed that context and acted <strong>in</strong> it. If we areconcerned with try<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood of success <strong>in</strong> languagc learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> theclassroom, then we nced to take a socially situated perspcctivc on the <strong>in</strong>teraction betweenlearner and data. In order to justify such a claim, I will beg<strong>in</strong> by offer<strong>in</strong>g my <strong>in</strong>terpretationof the context of Icarn<strong>in</strong>g.Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition <strong>in</strong> contextThere is little doubt that the history of SLA not only grew out of the roots put down bystudies of first languagc acquisition and has, over the last twenty years or so, sent up <strong>its</strong> ownshoots and branches <strong>in</strong> the shadow of this area of research. Build<strong>in</strong>g on the <strong>in</strong>fluences ofsociol<strong>in</strong>guistics, discourse analysis and the work ofvygotsky (1986), there is a significantbody of first language acquisition research which explicitly recognises the <strong>in</strong>terpersonalcontext of learn<strong>in</strong>g as the crucible of the whole process of language development(Donaldson 1978, Bruner 198 I , Lock 1980, Schiefelbusch and Pickar 1984, Wells 198 1and 1 985, Foster 1990, <strong>in</strong>ter alia). Evelyn Hatch brought this k<strong>in</strong>d of perspective <strong>in</strong>to SLAresearch <strong>in</strong> revcal<strong>in</strong>g how learners extend their grammatical repertoires on the basis of the“scaffold<strong>in</strong>g” providcd for them by proficicnt speakers dur<strong>in</strong>g conversations (Hatch 1978and 1992, Hatch et al. 1990). Her work has had an <strong>in</strong>direct <strong>in</strong>fluence upon those <strong>in</strong> SLAresearch who claim a “social <strong>in</strong>teractionist” perspective <strong>in</strong> see<strong>in</strong>g speech modifications dur<strong>in</strong>gcommunication between learners or learners and their teachers as central to thc acquisitionprocess (Long 1981, 1985 and 1996, Lightbown 1985, Pica et al. 1986 and 1987).Only very recently have a number of SLA researchers returned tovygotsky’s complexideas which <strong>in</strong>sist on learn<strong>in</strong>g as embedded with<strong>in</strong>, and <strong>in</strong>separable from social activity.These researchers propose an extension of <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>in</strong>to SLA to <strong>in</strong>clude a“sociocultural” perspective (Lantolf 1994, Lantolf and Appel 1994). Such a perspective isfairly rcpresented by Leont’cv who, like Vygotsky, saw lcarn<strong>in</strong>g as an <strong>in</strong>terpsychologicalundertak<strong>in</strong>g between thosc <strong>in</strong> society who have mastered knowledge or capability and thosewho are discovcr<strong>in</strong>g such knowledge or develop<strong>in</strong>g such capabilities. Leont’ev idcntifiedlearn<strong>in</strong>g as directly equivalent to othcr social activities <strong>in</strong> the wider world such as work, orfamily life, or participation <strong>in</strong> various everyday situations and <strong>in</strong>stitutional sett<strong>in</strong>gs. ForLeont’cv, when we read a text, listen to music, or pa<strong>in</strong>t a picture, even when not <strong>in</strong> thepresence of othcrs, wc are participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a process that is socially constructed:if we removed human activity from the system of social relationships and social life,it would not exist and would have no structure. With all <strong>its</strong> varicd forms, the human


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 309<strong>in</strong>dividual’s activity is a system <strong>in</strong> the system of social relations. It docs not existwithout these relations.The specific form <strong>in</strong> whch it exists is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the formsand means of material and mental social <strong>in</strong>teraction.(Leont’ev 198 1 : 47)Leont’ev is suggest<strong>in</strong>g that an activity like learn<strong>in</strong>g a language is a mental process<strong>in</strong>evitably <strong>in</strong>terwoven <strong>in</strong> our social identity and our social relationships. But he goes furtherthan this. He is also assert<strong>in</strong>g that the object or content on which we focus <strong>in</strong> our learn<strong>in</strong>gis, by <strong>its</strong> nature, a social and cultural construct. And l<strong>in</strong>guists such as Halliday support sucha claim <strong>in</strong> reveal<strong>in</strong>g that social structure and system may be seen to permeate the wholetexture of a language (Halliday 1978). This perspective implies that the <strong>in</strong>terpretative,accommodat<strong>in</strong>g, and strategic work of learners as revealed by ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA research isnot merely an act of cognition but that it is simultaneously social action.If we learn a language <strong>in</strong> the company of others <strong>in</strong> a classroom, then the nature of thissocial action is not merely a superficial frame for our work on language data. <strong>Social</strong>relationships <strong>in</strong> the classroom orchestrate what is made available for learn<strong>in</strong>g, how learn<strong>in</strong>gis done, and what we achieve.These relationships and the purposeful social action of teach<strong>in</strong>gand learn<strong>in</strong>g are directly realiscd through the discourse <strong>in</strong> which we participate dur<strong>in</strong>glessons. The data made available to learners are socially filtered through the particulardiscourse of the classroom and, thereby, rendered dist<strong>in</strong>ctive from what we might describeas naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g language data <strong>in</strong> a different context. Furthermore, because the datamade available to learners <strong>in</strong> a classroom arc a collective product with which teacher andlearners <strong>in</strong>teract actively as both creators and <strong>in</strong>terpreters, because what learners actuallylearn from the classroom is socially rather than <strong>in</strong>dividually constructed, any explanationof how language is learned must locate the process withm the discourse of language lessons.This implies that language learners need not only be <strong>in</strong>terpretative, accommodat<strong>in</strong>g,and strategic as SLA research suggests, but also active practitioners with<strong>in</strong> the discourse ofthe learn<strong>in</strong>g context <strong>in</strong> which they f<strong>in</strong>d themselves. If the context happens to be a classroom,it will provide very particular opportunities for and specific constra<strong>in</strong>ts upon languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g. These opportunities and constra<strong>in</strong>ts can be identified <strong>in</strong> the discourse of languagelessons and a crucial variable which can contribute to our understand<strong>in</strong>g of the relativesuccess or failure of learners is how they themselves are obliged to navigate with<strong>in</strong> it.We can express this central issue <strong>in</strong> terms of a question: Does a learner’s success <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong> a classroom depend upon the learner’s successful navigation of theopportunities and constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> discourse of lessons? This is a difficult questionthat needs further elaboration and I will offer this by look<strong>in</strong>g more closely at some of theprevail<strong>in</strong>g features of classroom discourse. I will address the question with reference to anumber of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from SLA research.Dimensions of discourseDiscourse is a difficult concept because, like SLA research, discourse analysis is a relativelyyoung discipl<strong>in</strong>e and there are several conflict<strong>in</strong>g and overlapp<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>itions deriv<strong>in</strong>g froma rangc of theoretical and analytical positions (van Dijk 1985, Macdonnell 1986). Early work<strong>in</strong> discourse analysis sought to uncover pattern and system at a higher level of organisationthan the sentence and to analyse the properties of dialogue such as speech acts, turn tak<strong>in</strong>g,topicalisation, and so on. Descriptive discourse analysis was also undertaken <strong>in</strong> relation towhat were seen as dist<strong>in</strong>ctive discourses such as media discourse, medical discourse, or legal


310 MICHAEL P. BREENdiscourse. More recently, the ideas of social thkrists such as lwucault


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 311are not directed at particular <strong>in</strong>dividuals but serve as a k<strong>in</strong>d of communal monologuedirected by the teacher at the whole class where<strong>in</strong> learner contributions are woven by theteacher <strong>in</strong>to his or her own text.Chaudron’s (1988) review of research on teacher talk <strong>in</strong> the language class furtherreveals that a good proportion of teacher <strong>in</strong>put made available to learners has very specificcharacteristics.Tcachers appear to have two-thirds more practice <strong>in</strong> the target language thanall the learners put together. They also modify their speech <strong>in</strong> ways similar to thecharacteristics of caretaker speech to young chddren or native speaker speech to non-nativespeakers. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, such teacher modification appears more emphatic when address<strong>in</strong>glearners whom they regard as hav<strong>in</strong>g lower proficiency (Dah1 1981, Griffiths 1991, HamayanandTucker 1980, Henzel 1979, Kliefgen 1985, Ellis 1985, Wong-Filmore 1982). In otherwords, the degree of modification <strong>in</strong> a teacher’s direct <strong>in</strong>teraction with an <strong>in</strong>dividual learnermay signal to that learner the tcacher’s judgement of his or her capabilities.A crucial feature of the text of lessons is teacher feedback on learner utterances. Becauseof the fast flow of lessons, teachers are understandably <strong>in</strong>consistent <strong>in</strong> their reactions tolearner errors with the result that different learners may either fail to dist<strong>in</strong>guish a teacher’scorrection from other k<strong>in</strong>ds of teacher utterance or assume that almost all teacher responsesto what they say are some form of judgement or correction (Allwright and Bailey 1991,Edmondson 1985, Nystrom 1983,Van Lier 1988). Underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gVan Lier’s observations aboutthe teacher’s discursive control of the text of lessons, research reveals that a remarkablyhigh proportion of teacher utterances are <strong>in</strong>terrogatives (Johnston 1990, Long and Sat01983). And a very high proportion of these arc closed display qucstions <strong>in</strong> which learnersare required to provide <strong>in</strong>formation which the teacher already knows rather than openreferential questions which genu<strong>in</strong>ely seek <strong>in</strong>formation from the lcarners (Long and Satoop. ut.).Although acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g the centrality of the teacher <strong>in</strong> the orchestration of classroomdiscourse, Van Lier (1988) suggests that the text of language lessons constantly shifts dueto <strong>its</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g generated by four types of <strong>in</strong>teraction: teacher <strong>in</strong>structions, teacher’s highlystructured elicitations of student responses, and procedurally structured learner activitiessuch as small group or dyadic tasks, all of which arc occasionally punctuated by small talkor student asides. Van Lier suggests that these different types of talk reflect different degreesof teacher control over topics or activities. From this we may also deduce that each of thefour types of <strong>in</strong>teraction will facilitate or delimit particular discursive practices on the partof learners.There appear to be features of the text of language lessons that may be dist<strong>in</strong>ctive ascompared with other types of lessons. We might describe this as thc <strong>in</strong>ter-textual nature oflanguage <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> classroom talk. Allwright (1980) analyses classroom talk <strong>in</strong>to three types:‘samples’ or <strong>in</strong>stances of the target language, ‘guidance’ where communication occurs aboutthe target language, and ‘management’ where<strong>in</strong> procedural talk facilitates the optimaloccurrence of samples and guidance. It seems, therefore, that the data made available to thelearner <strong>in</strong> the classroom is an on-go<strong>in</strong>g amalgam of three dom<strong>in</strong>ant and <strong>in</strong>ter-weav<strong>in</strong>gdiscursive practices: communication through the target language, metacommunication aboutthe target language, and communication about the teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process, <strong>its</strong> proceduresand classroom rout<strong>in</strong>es. And, as participants <strong>in</strong> the discourse, learners have to navigatethrough <strong>its</strong> <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality identify<strong>in</strong>g the textual cues which signal a transition from onek<strong>in</strong>d of talk to another. It is vcry likely that different learncrs will be more or less skilled <strong>in</strong>such navigation.We might concludc from these general patterns <strong>in</strong> the contributions of teachers to the<strong>in</strong>teractive text of language lessons that learners are not actually required to do much overt


312 MICHAEL P. BREENor explicit discursive work while devot<strong>in</strong>g their discursive energies to keep<strong>in</strong>g track of theteacher’s text and be<strong>in</strong>g alert to the moments when they have to contribute to it and to theteacher’s reactions to their contributions.Learners’ discursive practices <strong>in</strong> the classroomSo far, on the basis of language classroom research, I have suggested that the discourse oflessons is significantly shapcd by the teacher, that learners are positioned <strong>in</strong> particular waysby this, that the discourse manifests a shift<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality, and that learners are obligedto undertake pragmatic navigation with<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality if they arc to f<strong>in</strong>d their waythrough it <strong>in</strong> order to make scnse of it. For a fuller picture, however, we need to focus uponvariations <strong>in</strong> the overt participation of learners <strong>in</strong> the discourse which may be sccn as furthercontributory factors <strong>in</strong> their differential achievement <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.There arc fcw studics of learner <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> the classroom apart from the body of workon controversial modifications dur<strong>in</strong>g group or dyad work on tasks, some of which hav<strong>in</strong>gbeen undertaken <strong>in</strong> classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gs. Perhaps this is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g when, if we exam<strong>in</strong>ethe research on learner participation and, by implication, their contributions to the text oflessons as discursive practitioners, we f<strong>in</strong>d that learners are most often positioned by thediscourse <strong>in</strong> a responsive role (Polit7er et al. 1981). Generally, it seems that, through theircontrol of the discursive practices of lessons, through their use of questions, explanations,procedural <strong>in</strong>structions, and, crucially, their evaluation of much of the language producedby learners immediately after it is uttcred, teachers construct learners as primarilyresponsive and scem<strong>in</strong>gly fairly passive participants <strong>in</strong> the discourse. In offer<strong>in</strong>g anexplanation for the failure of French immersion students to fully atta<strong>in</strong> native-speaker likelevels <strong>in</strong> their own speech despite years of exposure to content-based and comprehensiblelanguage <strong>in</strong>put result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> very high levels of receptive understand<strong>in</strong>g, Swa<strong>in</strong> (1 985)suggests that this failure may bc partially due to the relative lack of opportunities for themto participate overtly <strong>in</strong> classroom discourse through their own speech production.Howcvcr, cvcn responsive discursivc practices appear to lead to variation <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.In <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g whether greater learner participation had an cffcct upon lcarn<strong>in</strong>g, Strong(1 983 and 1984) discovered that a high response rate among certa<strong>in</strong> learners corrclatcdwith thcir achievement <strong>in</strong> tests based upon the grammar, pronunciation and vocabulary ofclassroom speech. Scligcr (1977) suggcstcd that those learners which he identified as “high<strong>in</strong>put gcncrators” performed better on an aural comprehension task than did lessparticipat<strong>in</strong>g learners. In their classic study of the good language learner, Naiman et al.(1 978) found that lcarners who raised their hands more and more often responded toteacher elicitations did better on tests than other learners.Studies by Larsen-Freeman (1 976a and 1976b), Hamayan and Tucker (1 980),Lightbown (1 983), and Long (1 980), all suggest that the frequency of occurrence of certa<strong>in</strong>l<strong>in</strong>guistic forms <strong>in</strong> classroom text is likely to correlate with thc accurate production of theseforms by learners. More significantly, studics by Lightbown (1980 and 1991), Snow andHoefnagel Hohle (1 982), and White et al. (1991) not only confirm this but also show highretention rates of question forms. Given the regular occurrcncc of questions <strong>in</strong> the text oflessons, this may not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g. Learners are obliged to be alert to questions <strong>in</strong> case theyare directed to them <strong>in</strong>dividually.These studies also found that, not only questions, but otherk<strong>in</strong>ds of utterances directed specifically at <strong>in</strong>dividual learners correlated with higher ga<strong>in</strong>scores <strong>in</strong> tests taken by those <strong>in</strong>dividuals. It appears that, while it may not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g thatfrequent occurrence of certa<strong>in</strong> features <strong>in</strong> the text of lessons render them more accessible,


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 313types of teacher utterances which place what we may describe as discursive pressure uponlearners, such as question<strong>in</strong>g or nom<strong>in</strong>ated terms, demand overt discursive work on thepart of learners that may, <strong>in</strong> turn, <strong>in</strong>fluence their learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.The recent research on the k<strong>in</strong>ds of classroom tasks which most facilitate <strong>in</strong>teractionamong learners confirms the significance of discursive pressure. A task that entails an<strong>in</strong>formation gap between <strong>in</strong>terlocutors, that is unfamiliar to them, that engages learners <strong>in</strong>social exchanges about shared goals and problems, that is undertaken by learners of differentlevels of proficiency, and that demands a s<strong>in</strong>gle, closed solution for successful completionis found to encourage learners to have longer turns, produce more complex language, anddevote more time to explicit negotiation for mean<strong>in</strong>g than any other k<strong>in</strong>ds of task (Berwick1990, Long 1989 and 1996. Plough and Gass, 1993). Furthermore,Tanaka (1991) andYamazaki (1 991 ) have suggested that learner work on modify<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic data throughtheir own <strong>in</strong>teraction provides for greater ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g than provid<strong>in</strong>g them with eitherunmodified or premodified <strong>in</strong>put. In addition, Lightbown’s studies of corrective feedback(Lightbown 1991) and Swa<strong>in</strong>’s exploration of the functions of learner output (Swa<strong>in</strong> 1985and 1995) confirm that feedback is most likely to have an impact on the learner’s<strong>in</strong>terlanguage if it occurs at times when the learner is work<strong>in</strong>g hard to convey a particularmessage. In sum, the struggle to negotiate for mean<strong>in</strong>g through overt discursive workrcnders relatively complex text comprehensible and, consistent with a major assumption<strong>in</strong> SLA research, thereby facilitates learn<strong>in</strong>g.However, different learners will navigate through the discourse of lessons <strong>in</strong> differentways depend<strong>in</strong>g upon their own def<strong>in</strong>itions of the situation, their previous experiences ofclassrooms, and their particular understand<strong>in</strong>g of the dynamic social practices or culture ofthe classroom group (Breen op. ut.). Learners will therefore place different values andsignificance upon their role as a participant <strong>in</strong> the class. Overt discursive pressure uponparticular learners or even spontaneous participation do not alone account for differences<strong>in</strong> what learners learn from a lesson. Day’s (1 984) replication of Seliger’s study of “high<strong>in</strong>put generators” (Seliger op. at.) and Ely’s (1 986) <strong>in</strong>vestigation of learner <strong>in</strong>itiatedutterances found no relationship between overt learner participation and later testatta<strong>in</strong>ment. In trac<strong>in</strong>g learners’ immediate “uptake” from lessons of previously unknownvocabulary, Slimani (1 989 and 1992; Chapter 18 of this book) confirmed Allwright’shypothesis that different learners will learn different th<strong>in</strong>gs even from the same lesson(Allwright op. at.). Slimani made the <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g discovery that low-participat<strong>in</strong>g and evennon-participat<strong>in</strong>g learners often recalled as much from lessons as did high-participat<strong>in</strong>glearners. And, significantly, learners recalled more items from lessons if they weretopicalised or <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong>to the text of the lesson by learners rather than those topicalisedby the teacher. Slimani deduced that low-participat<strong>in</strong>g learners were directly benefit<strong>in</strong>gfrom their high-participat<strong>in</strong>g colleagues. Allwright <strong>in</strong>terpreted these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs as suggest<strong>in</strong>gthat the more proficient learners <strong>in</strong> a class who appeared to be those more will<strong>in</strong>g toparticipate were tak<strong>in</strong>g on the burden of discursive work but without seem<strong>in</strong>gly ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gfrom it. In other words, proficiency <strong>in</strong> the language may enable greater participation ratherthan participation lead<strong>in</strong>g to ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> proficiency. Slimani’s study also cast doubt on theclaims of ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA researchers that conversational modifications lead to greatercomprehensibility and, thereby, <strong>in</strong>creased likelihood of acquisition. In fact she found norelationship between the number of conversational adjustments occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the text oflessons around specific l<strong>in</strong>guistic items and the “uptake” of these items by learners.A recent replication of Slimani’s study by Dob<strong>in</strong>son (1 996) largely confirmed thesef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and suggested that differences between learners <strong>in</strong> what they recalled from lessonswere due to a whole range of factors and that some of the previously unknown vocabulary


314 MICHAEL P. BREENwhich they not only recalled but also reta<strong>in</strong>ed over a longer period were never overtlynegotiated about <strong>in</strong> the text of the lesson. Only 27% of reta<strong>in</strong>ed vocabulary items had beenovertly topicalised <strong>in</strong> the lesson, whilst 56% of reta<strong>in</strong>ed vocabulary could be traced to the<strong>in</strong>dividual learner’s personal work upon items occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the spoken or written texts ofthe lesson which triggered efforts to seek items <strong>in</strong> a dictionary, to make associations withwhat they knew already, to write the word down to f<strong>in</strong>d out <strong>its</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g later, and so on.In essence, a key characteristic of items which <strong>in</strong>dividual learners learned from the lessonswas the relative <strong>in</strong>comprehensibility of that item to an <strong>in</strong>dividual learner and this resulted<strong>in</strong> covert <strong>in</strong>dividual work towards understand<strong>in</strong>g and, thereby, remember<strong>in</strong>g it. In fact,Dob<strong>in</strong>son discovered that there was a converse relationship between the amount of overtnegotiation about new vocabulary items and their retention by 1earners.The more an itemwas focused upon <strong>in</strong> the text of the lesson, the lcss likely it was to be reta<strong>in</strong>ed. She concludedthat there must be an optimal degree of overt negotiation which facilitates learn<strong>in</strong>g. Infocus<strong>in</strong>g upon learner participation, Dob<strong>in</strong>son also discovered that learners who did notparticipate at all recalled equal or greater numbers of previously unknown words from thelessons as did higher participat<strong>in</strong>g learners.From Slimani’s and Dob<strong>in</strong>son’s research it appears that we can deduce that <strong>in</strong>dividuallearners appear to be capable of navigat<strong>in</strong>g the discourse <strong>in</strong> ways that reflect their <strong>in</strong>dividualpurposes and agendas. In certa<strong>in</strong> circumstances, discursive pressure to respond or tonegotiate with the teacher or other learners facilitates acquisition only for some learners.However, as with all deductions from classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g research, these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gshave to be seen <strong>in</strong> the light of thc context from which the data were obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Slimani andDob<strong>in</strong>son located their studies <strong>in</strong> classrooms that were conventionally teacher-fronted withstrong teacher control over the text of the lessons. It appears, therefore, that there may bea difference <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes based upon overt negotiation for mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this k<strong>in</strong>d ofclassroom context as compared with dyads or small groups of learners ncgotiat<strong>in</strong>g formean<strong>in</strong>g without the <strong>in</strong>tervention of the teacher. This discovery, of course, would supportthe argument that context makes a difference. Overt participation <strong>in</strong> classroom discourseappears to serve other purposes <strong>in</strong> addition to the purpose of learn<strong>in</strong>g. In thesecircumstances, some learners will deliberately avoid discursive prcssure so that they candevote their attention to their own learn<strong>in</strong>g agendas. And the Slimani and Dob<strong>in</strong>son studiesconfirm that it is likely that learners will differentially ga<strong>in</strong> from such practices.<strong>Social</strong> practices <strong>in</strong> the classroomLearners selectively work through the discourse of the classroom not only as discursivepractitioners with<strong>in</strong> the immediate lesson but also on the basis of how they judge whichsocial practices are appropriate <strong>in</strong> the particular classroom group. Their selectiveparticipation and the judgements on which they base it are derived from their def<strong>in</strong>ition ofthe particular teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g situation and from their experience with other realms ofdiscourse beyond the classroom. Learners therefore navigate the discourse <strong>in</strong> two constantly<strong>in</strong>ter-weav<strong>in</strong>g ways; for learn<strong>in</strong>g purposes and for social purposes. Differential outcomesfrom lessons may reflect the fact that learners will differ <strong>in</strong> their abilities to balance thesetwo priorities and, crucially, <strong>in</strong> their relative allocation of attention to them.Classroom discourse is, for the learner, a voyage of discovery <strong>in</strong> the close company ofothers with a teacher who leads the expedition or, at least, carries the map. On the onehand, learners navigate classroom discourse <strong>in</strong> order to discover here and now what countsas valid <strong>in</strong>terpretation, what counts as knowledge worth accommodat<strong>in</strong>g, and what counts


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 315as appropriate stratcgic behaviour for learn<strong>in</strong>g be it overt or covert. On the other hand,they navigate the discourse anticipat<strong>in</strong>g that the social practices with<strong>in</strong> the classroom willconstruct knowledge and the role identities of, and relationships between teacher andlearners <strong>in</strong> very specific ways.They are therefore obliged to work <strong>in</strong> ordcr to maximise thelearn<strong>in</strong>g and social benef<strong>its</strong> they may ga<strong>in</strong> from the discourse while m<strong>in</strong>imis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>its</strong> potentialpsychological and social costs. Their selective work therefore reflects their understand<strong>in</strong>gof, and contributions to, the emerg<strong>in</strong>g culture of the particular classroom group and theirown location with<strong>in</strong> it. In an carlier paper, I suggested that this culture is not onlyasymmetrical <strong>in</strong> terms of who controls the discourse, or normative <strong>in</strong> terms of the teacher’sjudgements of correctness or appropriacy, but that learners jo<strong>in</strong>tly conspire with teachers <strong>in</strong>crcat<strong>in</strong>g and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a managcable work<strong>in</strong>g harmony through the particular rout<strong>in</strong>esand procedurcs of the surface text of lessons (Brcen op. cit.). From SLA research, we knowthat different types of classroom-based activities and tasks will permit different outcomesfor different learners (Larsen-Freeman 1976a,Tarone 1988, Schmidt 1980, Bahns andWode1980, Hyltenstam 1984, Lightbown 1991). But wc also know that different types ofclassrooms <strong>in</strong> terms of their overt rout<strong>in</strong>es and procedures or, more broadly, their socialpractices will generate different learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes as well (Wong-Filmorc 1982, Enright1984, Spada 1987, Allen et al. 1990).Allwright (1 989) has suggested that data from classroom <strong>in</strong>tcraction often reveal teacherand learncrs hav<strong>in</strong>g to solve a recurr<strong>in</strong>g discoursal dilemma.The dilemma confront<strong>in</strong>g bothteacher and lcarners is that of ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g social harmony or avoid<strong>in</strong>g what he calls “socialproblems” whilst, at the same time, preserv<strong>in</strong>g what he regards as “pedagogic possibilitics”or genu<strong>in</strong>e opportunitics for learn<strong>in</strong>g. For Allwright, such social problems <strong>in</strong>cludeunexpected topics that arise as side issues but become an extended focus of the <strong>in</strong>tcraction,or dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g or highly reluctant learners, or procedural confusions that appear to detractfrom the teacher’s plans or lesson rnanagemcnt. Allwright suggests that the resolution of“social trouble” is an <strong>in</strong>evitable part of classroom discourse and that, paradoxically, “goodpedagogy” based upon approaches to languagc teach<strong>in</strong>g which encourage overt learnerparticipation necessarily risks creat<strong>in</strong>g social problems. However, the culture of mostclassrooms is often built upon and preserved by a shared and unspoken assumption thatcooperation to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> relative harmony on the surface of lessons between compet<strong>in</strong>gagendas is ultimately easier for both teacher and learners. The costs of social trouble areconstantly <strong>in</strong> balance with the benef<strong>its</strong> of fairly predictable and stable rout<strong>in</strong>cs andprocedures and the teacher and most learners work hard <strong>in</strong> order to resolvc or avoid suchtroubles. At different times, it is very likely that somc learners will perceive some socialtroubles as learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunitics just as thcy may <strong>in</strong>terpret what the teacher regards as apedagogic possibility as socially thrcatcn<strong>in</strong>g. However, the very salience of social trouble <strong>in</strong>the discourse will alert learners’ attention to it while possibly <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g teacher and learners<strong>in</strong> exactly the k<strong>in</strong>d of resolution work that may be directly beneficial to language learn<strong>in</strong>g.However, learners also navigate through classroom discourse <strong>in</strong> ways that will enablcthem to avoid <strong>in</strong>dividual trouble for themselves, <strong>in</strong> particular avoid<strong>in</strong>g to appear foolish <strong>in</strong>public. The <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g studies of Beebe and Zuengler (Beebe 1977, Beebe and Zuengler1983) and ofYoung (1988 and 1991) reveal that the learners will actually vary the style oftheir production depend<strong>in</strong>g upon whom they are address<strong>in</strong>g and, <strong>in</strong> particular theirperception of the relative status and l<strong>in</strong>guistic competence of their <strong>in</strong>terlocutors. Of directrelevance to the classroom,Takahashi’s research suggests that learners will be more hesitantand briefer <strong>in</strong> their utterances when address<strong>in</strong>g someone whom they perceive as highlycompetent <strong>in</strong> the target language such as their teacher (Takahashi 1989). And Rampton(1 987) reveals that learners, while actually capable of more complex language, may revert


316 MICHAEL P. BREENto earlier features of their <strong>in</strong>terlanguage precisely <strong>in</strong> order to signal that they me learners,Learners may undertake a k<strong>in</strong>d of impression management <strong>in</strong> their discursive practiceswhich publicly expresses their own construction of themselves as learners and theirconstruction of whom they <strong>in</strong>teract with. Therefore, variations <strong>in</strong> how learners participate<strong>in</strong> the text of lessons will also be a reflection of their self assessment and their assessmentof both the teacher’s language and the teacher’s likely reactions to their own production. Itseems that some learners’ perceptions of the established social relationships <strong>in</strong> someclassrooms may actually encourage them to underachieve.What learners learn from the discourse of lessonsThe forego<strong>in</strong>g review of classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g research has illustrated some of theways <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>teraction between the lcarner and the target language data is situatedwith<strong>in</strong> social action. In order to summarise what we know of the discursive practices oflearners <strong>in</strong> the language classroom, we can see that learners are obliged to participate overtlyand covertly <strong>in</strong> the discourse of lessons <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g ways:Adopt a responsive role <strong>in</strong> relation to the teacher’s management of the discoursethrough his/her control over the text of lessons.Be alert to and adapt to the vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality of lessons.Act <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong> responsc to discursive pressure with<strong>in</strong> teacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teractionand with<strong>in</strong> tasks and activities dur<strong>in</strong>g lessons.Covertly exploit others’ participation <strong>in</strong> classroom discourse as Opportunities to serveown purposes and learn<strong>in</strong>g agenda.Navigate the discourse of the classroom <strong>its</strong> specific text, discursive requirements,and particular social practices with direct referencc to personal costs and benef<strong>its</strong>.Def<strong>in</strong>e the situation on the basis of past experience and present understand<strong>in</strong>g of theemerg<strong>in</strong>g culture of the classroom group antl act <strong>in</strong> ways that are seen as appropriateto that culture.Participate with the teacher antl other learners <strong>in</strong> the ongo<strong>in</strong>g construction of lessonsand the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of fairly predictable classroom rout<strong>in</strong>es and procedures.Manage the presentation of self through the discourse accord<strong>in</strong>g to one’s owndef<strong>in</strong>ition of both self identity and the demands of the situation.In general, therefore, a learner who is a successful discursive practitioner <strong>in</strong> theclassroom appears to be someone who avoids risks to self identity <strong>in</strong> the group andcontributes <strong>in</strong> ways that seem appropriate to the group culture whilst exploit<strong>in</strong>g discoursalopportunities for their own learn<strong>in</strong>g. The question I raised at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of this chapterwas: Does a learner’s success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom depend upon the learner’ssuccessful navigation of the opportunities and constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> the discourse oflessons? Clearly learners will differ <strong>in</strong> their responses to the k<strong>in</strong>ds of demands that are placedupon them by such discourse and they will differ <strong>in</strong> terms of their own priorities andcapabilities as discursive practitioners <strong>in</strong> the specific context of a classroom. I have suggestedthat learners <strong>in</strong> classrooms will differentially <strong>in</strong>terpret, accommodate, and adopt strategieslargely on the basis of what classroom discourse provides as text, what practices it requiresof teacher and learners, and how it constructs both the knowledge to be learned and theunfold<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g process through social practice. Learners’ cognitions are framedwith<strong>in</strong> the prevail<strong>in</strong>g discourse through which they learn and there is good evidence that


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 317learners navigate that discourse <strong>in</strong> different ways. It is <strong>in</strong>evitable that different learners willdifferentially achieve <strong>in</strong> such circumstances. In fact, the variables to which I have referred<strong>in</strong> review<strong>in</strong>g second language classroom research are an important explanation for suchdifferentiation.Implications for classroom pedagogyWe might deduce from the evidence that there is only a very tenuous relationship betweensuccessful participation by learners <strong>in</strong> the discourse of lessons and their actual progress <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g the language. At least it seems that overt participation <strong>in</strong> lessons has little impacton actual learn<strong>in</strong>g whilst overt negotiation for mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> small group and dyadic tasks isseen by SLA researchers as pivotal for learn<strong>in</strong>g. But a crucial issue is that overt participationseems to be relatively rare for <strong>in</strong>dividual learners <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of lessons from which mostdata for second language classroom research are obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Navigat<strong>in</strong>g the discourse <strong>in</strong> manylanguage classrooms, whilst result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> different learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes for most learners, isnot a difficult th<strong>in</strong>g for most of them to do. S<strong>in</strong>ce their early years at school, languagelearners have gradually discovered what is expected of them as discursive practitioners <strong>in</strong>a classroom. In many cases, they have had years of practice at <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the texts oflessons, learn<strong>in</strong>g and adopt<strong>in</strong>g appropriate discursive practices, and understand<strong>in</strong>g andcontribut<strong>in</strong>g to the social practices of classrooms. As we have seen, the data from secondlanguage classroom research primarily reveals that teachers orchestrate the discourse whilelearners play their parts as a k<strong>in</strong>d of counterpo<strong>in</strong>t to their <strong>in</strong>dividual learn<strong>in</strong>g agendas. Thediscourse may momentarily harmonise with these agendas while at other times, there isdiscordance between the discourse and genu<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g. To be provocative, we mightconclude that some learners’ highly attentive efforts to avoid trouble by successfullynavigat<strong>in</strong>g the prevail<strong>in</strong>g discourse of language lessons might actually distract their attentionfrom actually learn<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g.There is a grow<strong>in</strong>g body of evidence which suggests that the discourse of the languageclassroom is dist<strong>in</strong>ctive. And it is dist<strong>in</strong>ctive <strong>in</strong> many ways from the discourse <strong>in</strong> which weparticipate <strong>in</strong> other contexts (Riley 1977, Gremmo et al. 1978, Edmondson 1985, Kramsch1985, Glahn and Holman 1985, Kasper 1986, Ellis 1992). If, for most learners perhaps,language learn<strong>in</strong>g is embedded <strong>in</strong> the discourse of the classroom, if they learn how to becomemembers of a new language community through the discursive practices which they adoptor are obliged to adopt <strong>in</strong> the classroom, how will these practices prepare them forparticipation <strong>in</strong> discourse beyond the classroom? In other words, how are learners totranscend what they have learned <strong>in</strong> manag<strong>in</strong>g classroom discourse <strong>in</strong> order to participateas speakers of the new language <strong>in</strong> other realms of discourse?A paradoxical but central issue for language pedagogy is how it may facilitate the gradualduembeddmg of language learn<strong>in</strong>g from what appears to be the prevail<strong>in</strong>g discourse of lessons.In rais<strong>in</strong>g this issue I am not <strong>in</strong>tend<strong>in</strong>g to imply that all the features of such discourse <strong>in</strong>hibitthe learner’s capacity to participate <strong>in</strong> other k<strong>in</strong>ds of discourse. However, I believe it doesimply that we need to consider how we might identify and mobilise the discursive work of‘learners which actually benef<strong>its</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g while also identify<strong>in</strong>g and reduc<strong>in</strong>g those mutableconstra<strong>in</strong>ts with<strong>in</strong> the current discursive and social practices of language classrooms whichmay <strong>in</strong>hibit it.If a lcarncr is positioned <strong>in</strong> a largely responsive role with<strong>in</strong> the discourse, the rcscarchsuggests that the learner has to fall back upon covert and unguided ways of mak<strong>in</strong>g sensebecause the opportunities for overt negotiation which entail the added benef<strong>its</strong> of <strong>in</strong>dividual


318 MICHAEL P. BREENoutput and directly formative feedback are significantly curtailed (Slimani; Dob<strong>in</strong>son; Swa<strong>in</strong>op. ut.). If the discourse of a language lesson constra<strong>in</strong>ts the k<strong>in</strong>ds of participation whichSLA research identifies as genu<strong>in</strong>e negotiation for mean<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> what ways can we vary thetext, discursive practices and social practices of the classroom so that genu<strong>in</strong>e negotiationfor mean<strong>in</strong>g becomes more possible? If there is a jo<strong>in</strong>t conspiracy between a teacher andlearners that predictable and trouble free discourse is preferable to hav<strong>in</strong>g to work harderwith<strong>in</strong> it, <strong>in</strong> what ways might we more overtly mobilise learner efforts to make sense of theunpredictable and to participate directly <strong>in</strong> resolv<strong>in</strong>g both learn<strong>in</strong>g and social confusions?The <strong>in</strong>dividual effort to confront and reduce complcxity <strong>in</strong> text through discursivenegotiation with and about that text is the catalyst for understand<strong>in</strong>g and, thereby, anopportunity for further learn<strong>in</strong>g (Long 1996).All these considerations directly imply that we should be facilitat<strong>in</strong>g the k<strong>in</strong>d ofdiscourse <strong>in</strong> a language class which is more challeng<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>its</strong> participants than it often is.Such a discourse will positively support the k<strong>in</strong>d of risk-tak<strong>in</strong>g among learners that cancontribute to deeper and more resilient level5 of learn<strong>in</strong>g. This means focus<strong>in</strong>g upon thepotential <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> those discursive practices which learncrs are currently obliged to adopt.Given that teachers have the major responsibility <strong>in</strong> manag<strong>in</strong>g the discourse of lessons, howcan we manage it <strong>in</strong> ways that may maximise such opportunities? Recall<strong>in</strong>g the discursivepractices of learners that I summariscd earlier, alternative ways of manag<strong>in</strong>g the discoursemay <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Expect<strong>in</strong>g learners to adopt an active and creative role <strong>in</strong> construct<strong>in</strong>g the tcxt oflessons so that at least two-thirds of it is generated by learners rather than the teacher.Build<strong>in</strong>g on the learners’ alertness and adaptability to the <strong>in</strong>ter textuality of lessonsand familiarity with <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality <strong>in</strong> the first language by encourag<strong>in</strong>g theunderstand<strong>in</strong>g and creation of <strong>in</strong>ventive and diverse comb<strong>in</strong>ations of written andspoken texts <strong>in</strong> the new language.Positively encourag<strong>in</strong>g learner risk-tak<strong>in</strong>g so that discursive pressure is seen bylearners as genu<strong>in</strong>e opportunities for creative use of emerg<strong>in</strong>g knowledge and skillsrather than requir<strong>in</strong>g responses which may bc judged <strong>in</strong> personally threaten<strong>in</strong>g ways.Enabl<strong>in</strong>g learners to make overt and to develop their own on-go<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g agcndasso that these may be personally reflected upon and ref<strong>in</strong>ed and also acted upon <strong>in</strong> acollective way.Enabl<strong>in</strong>g learners to recognise that the <strong>in</strong>evitable and on-go<strong>in</strong>g costs <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gthrough a more challeng<strong>in</strong>g discourse of lessons are outweighed by both immediateand long term benef<strong>its</strong>.Explor<strong>in</strong>g with learners ways <strong>in</strong> which the emerg<strong>in</strong>g culture of the classroom groupcan be adapted and constructed <strong>in</strong> an on-go<strong>in</strong>g way <strong>in</strong> order to facilitate their ownlearn<strong>in</strong>g as a participant <strong>in</strong> that group.Accept<strong>in</strong>g that lcssons are jo<strong>in</strong>tly constructed by teacher and learners together, seekways of engag<strong>in</strong>g learner responsibility for them so that rout<strong>in</strong>es and procedures arechoscn and adapted on the basis of overt teacher-learner and learner -teachernegotiation about such th<strong>in</strong>gs.Appreciat<strong>in</strong>g the social risk of do<strong>in</strong>g all these th<strong>in</strong>gs, facilitate cooperative andsupportive ways of work<strong>in</strong>g as a classroom group that respects the identity, difficulties,and relative autonomy of the <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those of the teacher.Each of these ways of work<strong>in</strong>g is, of course, related to one another and, therefore,complementary. The effort to implement one makes it more possible to implement any of


NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 319the others. Of coursc, lcarncrs <strong>in</strong> such a context will be confronted by the challenge ofhav<strong>in</strong>g to navigate a discourse that may be different from the lund of classroom discoursewith which they arc more at ease. However, if we accept the implications of current SLAresearch, it is possible that a more positive relationship between success <strong>in</strong> navigat<strong>in</strong>g suchdiscourse and success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g will emerge.ReferencesAllen, P., Swa<strong>in</strong>, M., Harley, B., and Cumm<strong>in</strong>s, J. (1990) ‘Aspects of classroom treatment:toward a more comprehensive view of second language education’, <strong>in</strong> The development ojsecond language projciency, B. Harley et al. (eds). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Allwright, R. (1980) ‘Turns, topics and tasks: patterns of participation <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g andlearn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> Discourse analysis <strong>in</strong> second language research, D. Larsen-Freeman (ed.).Rowley Mass: Newbury House.____ (1984) ‘Why don’t learners learn what teachers teach? The <strong>in</strong>teraction hypothesis’, <strong>in</strong><strong>Language</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formal and <strong>in</strong>formal contexts, D. S<strong>in</strong>glcton and D. Little (eds). Dubl<strong>in</strong>:IRAL.(1989) Interaction <strong>in</strong> the language classroom: social problems and pedagogic possibilities. Paperpresented at Les Etats Generaux des Langues, Paris, April 1989.Allwright, R. and Bailey, K. (1991) Focus on the language classroom: an <strong>in</strong>troduction to classroomresearch for language teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Bahns, J. and Wodc, H. (1980) ‘Form and function <strong>in</strong> L2 acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> Second languagedevelopment: trends and issues, S. Felix (ed.). Tub<strong>in</strong>gen: Gunter Narr.Beebe, L. (1977) ‘The <strong>in</strong>fluence of the listener on code-switch<strong>in</strong>g’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 27,331-9.Beebe, L. and Zuengler, J. (1983) ‘Accommodation theory: an explanation for style shift<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>second language dialects’, <strong>in</strong> Sociol<strong>in</strong>guistics and second language acquisition, N. Wolfson andE. Judd (eds). Rowley Mass: Newbury House.Bcrwick, R. (1990) Task variation and repair <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong> as a foreign language. Kobe University ofCommcrcc: Institute of Economic Research.Rourdicu, P. (1991) <strong>Language</strong> and symbolic power. Cambridge: Polity Press.Breen, M.P. (1985) ‘The social context for languagc lcarn<strong>in</strong>g: a neglected situation?’ Studies <strong>in</strong>second language acquisition 7. 135-58.____ (1996) ‘Constructions of the teacher <strong>in</strong> SLA research’, <strong>in</strong> Georgetown University RoundTable on <strong>Language</strong>s and L<strong>in</strong>guistics 1996, J.E. Alatis, C.A. Straehle, M. Ronk<strong>in</strong>, and B.Gellenberger (eds). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D. C. : Georgetown Univcrsity Press.Bruner, J. (1981) ‘The pragmatics of acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> The child’s construction .f language, W.Deutsch (cd.). NewYork: Academic Press.Chaudron, C. (1988) Second language classrooms: research on teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Dahl, D. (1981) ‘The role of experience <strong>in</strong> speech modifications for second language lcarncrs’.M<strong>in</strong>nesota Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics and Philosophy of <strong>Language</strong> 7. 78-93.Day, R. (1984) ‘Student participation <strong>in</strong> the ESL classroom, or some imperfections <strong>in</strong>practice’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 34. 69-102.Dob<strong>in</strong>son, T. (1996) The recall and retention .f new vocabulary from second language classrooms.Unpublished M. A. dissertation, Edith Cowan University, Western Australia.Donaldson, M. (1978) Children’s m<strong>in</strong>ds. London: Fontana.Edmondson, W. (1985) ‘Discourse worlds <strong>in</strong> the classroom and <strong>in</strong> foreign language learn<strong>in</strong>g’.Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition 7. 159-68.


~ (1~ (1~ (1984)~ (1320 MICHAEL P. BREENEllis, R. (1 985) ‘Teacher-pupil <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> second language development’. Lnput <strong>in</strong> secondlanguage acquisition, S. Gass and C. Madden (eds). Rowley Mass.: Newbury House.992) ‘Learn<strong>in</strong>g to communicate <strong>in</strong> the classroom’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong> Acquisition14. 1-23.994) The study ofsecond language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Enright, D. (1984) ‘The organisation of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> elementary classrooms’, <strong>in</strong> On TESOL ‘83:the question ofcontrol, J. Handscombc et al. (eds). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton D.C.: TESOL.Ely, C. (1986) ‘An analysis of discomfort, risktak<strong>in</strong>g, sociability and motivation <strong>in</strong> the L2classroom’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 36. 1-25.Fairclough, N. (1989) <strong>Language</strong> and power. London: Longman..~ (1992) Discourse and social change. Cambridge: Polity Press.Foster, S. (1990) The communicative competence $young children. London: Longman.Foucault, M. (1972) The archeology ofknowledge. London: Tavistock Publications.‘The order of discourse’, <strong>in</strong> <strong>Language</strong> and politics, M. Shapiro (cd). Oxford: BasilBlackwell.Glahn, E. and Holman, A. (cds) (1985) ‘Anglica et Americana 22’. Learner discourse.Copenhagen: University of Copenhagen.Grcmmo, M., Holec, H., and Riley, P. (1978) Mdanges Pedagogiques. Tak<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>itiativersornepedagogical applications ofdiscourse analysis. University of Nancy: CRAPEL.Griffiths, R. (1991) ‘Pausological research <strong>in</strong> an L2 context: a rationale and review of selectedstudies’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 12. 276-94.Halliday, M.A.K. (1978) <strong>Language</strong> as social semiotic. London: Edward Arnold.Hamayan, E. andTucker, R. (1980) ‘Languagc <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> the bil<strong>in</strong>gual classroom and <strong>its</strong> relationsto second language achievement’. TESOL Quarterly 14.453-68.Hatch, E. (1978) ‘Discoursc analysis and second language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> Second languageacquisition, E. Hatch (cd.). Rowley Mass: Newbury house.992) Discourse and language education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Hatch, E., Shirai,Y., and Fantuxxi, C. (1990) ‘The necd for an <strong>in</strong>tegrated theory: connect<strong>in</strong>gmodules’. TESOL Quarterr, 24. 697-716.Hcnzel,V. (1979) ‘Foreigner talk <strong>in</strong> the classroom’. Lnternational Review $Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 17.159-65.Hyltcnstam, K. (1984) ‘The use of typological markcdncss conditions as predictors <strong>in</strong> secondlanguage acquisition: the case of pronom<strong>in</strong>al copies <strong>in</strong> rclativc clauses’, <strong>in</strong> Second language;a crossl<strong>in</strong>guistic perspective, R. Anderson (cd.). Rowley Mass: Newbury House.Hyltenstam, K. and Pienemann, M. (cds) (1985) Modell<strong>in</strong>g anti assess<strong>in</strong>g second languageacquisition. Clevedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Johnston, S. (1990) Teacher questions <strong>in</strong> the academic language-content classroom. Unpublishedpaper. Tokyo: Temple University Japan.Johnston, M. and Picncmann, M. (1986) Second language acquisition: a classroom perspective. NewSouth Wales Migrant Education Servicc.Kaspcr, G. (cd.) ( 1986) Learn<strong>in</strong>g, teach<strong>in</strong>g and communication <strong>in</strong> the-foreign language classroom.Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Kliefgen, J. (1985) ‘Skilled variation <strong>in</strong> a k<strong>in</strong>dergarten teacher’s usc of foreigner talk’. Input <strong>in</strong>second language acquisition, S. Gass and C. Madden (etls). Rowley Mass.: NewburyHouse.Kramsch, C. (1985) ‘Classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction and discourse options’. Studies <strong>in</strong> Second <strong>Language</strong>Acquisition 7. 169-83.Lantolf, J. (1994) ‘Sociocultural theory and second language Icarn<strong>in</strong>g: an <strong>in</strong>troduction to thespecial issue’. Modern <strong>Language</strong>journal 78.418420.Lantolf, J. and Appel, G. (1994) (cds) l!ygotskyan approaches to second language research.Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publish<strong>in</strong>g Corporation.


~ (1976b)NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 321Larsen-Freeman, D. (1976a) ‘Teacher speech as <strong>in</strong>put to the ESL learner’. University ofCalfornia Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> TESL 10. 4549.‘An exploration of the morpheme acquisition order of second languagelearners’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 26. 125-134.-~ (1983) ‘The importance of <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> Pidg<strong>in</strong>ization andcreolization and language acquisition, R. Anderson (ed.). Rowley, Mass: Newbury House.Leont’ev, A.N. (1981) ‘The problem of activity <strong>in</strong> psychology’, <strong>in</strong> pgotsky and the socialformation ofm<strong>in</strong>d, J. Wertsch (ed.). Cambridge, Mass. : Harvard University Press.Lightbown, P. (1980) ‘The acquisition and use of questions by French L2 learners of <strong>English</strong>’,<strong>in</strong> Second language development: trends and issues, S. Felix (ed.). Tub<strong>in</strong>gen: Gunter Narr.______ (1983) ‘Explor<strong>in</strong>g relationships between developmental and <strong>in</strong>structional sequences <strong>in</strong>L2 acquisition’, <strong>in</strong> Classroom oriented research <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, H. Seligcr andM. Long (eds). Rowley Mass: Newbury House.______ (1985) Can language acquisition be altered ly <strong>in</strong>struction?, Hyltenstam and Pienemann (eds).______ (1991) ‘What have we here? Some observations on the effect of <strong>in</strong>struction on L2learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> Foreign /second language Pedagogy Research, R. Phillipson et al. (eds).Clevedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.~- (1992) ‘Gett<strong>in</strong>g quality <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> the second/foreign language classroom’, <strong>in</strong> Text andcontext: cross-discipl<strong>in</strong>ary perspectives on language study, C. Kramsch and S. McConnel-G<strong>in</strong>ct(eds). Lex<strong>in</strong>gton Mass: D.C. Heath and Co.Lock, A. (1980) The guided re<strong>in</strong>vention oflanguage. NewYork: Academic Press.Long, M. (1980) Input, <strong>in</strong>teraction and second language acquisition. Unpublished Ph.D.dissertation. University of California at Los Angeles.(198 1) ‘Input, <strong>in</strong>teraction and second language acquisition’. Native language andforeignlanguage acquisition, H. W<strong>in</strong>itz (ed). Annals of the NewYork Academy of Sciences, 379.______ (1985) ‘A role for <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition: task-based languageteach<strong>in</strong>g’. Modell<strong>in</strong>g and assess<strong>in</strong>g second language acquisition, K. Hyltcnstam and M.Pienemann (cds). Clevedon: Mulitl<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.______ (1989) ‘ Task, group, and task-group <strong>in</strong>teractions’. University ofHawai’i Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong>ESL 8. 251-86.______ (1996) ‘The role of the l<strong>in</strong>guistic environment <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition’, <strong>in</strong>Handbook ofsecond language acquisition, W. Ritchie andT. Rhatia (eds). NewYork: AcademicPress.Long, M. and Sato, C. (1983) ‘Classroom foreigner talk discourse: forms and functions ofteachers’ questions’, <strong>in</strong> Classroom oriented research <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, H. Seligerand M. Long (eds). Rowley Mass: Newbury house.Macdonnel, D. (1986) Theories $discourse. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.McTear, M. (1975) ‘Structure and categories of foreign language teach<strong>in</strong>g sequences’, <strong>in</strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers: <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Classroom Research, R. Allwright (ed.). University ofEssex, Department of <strong>Language</strong> and L<strong>in</strong>guistics.Naiman, N., Frohlich, M., Stern, H., andTodcsco. A. (1978) The good language learner. Research<strong>in</strong> Education Series 7. Toronto: The Ontario Institute for Studies <strong>in</strong> Education.Nystrom, N. (1983) ‘Teacher-student <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms: four approaches toerror feedback’, <strong>in</strong> Classroom oriented research <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, H. Scliger andM. Long (eds). Rowley Mass: Newbury House.Pica,T. (1983) ‘Adult acquisition of <strong>English</strong> as a second language under diffcrent conditions ofexposure’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 33.465~97.Pica, T., Doughty, C., and Young, D. (1986) ‘Mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>put comprehensible: do <strong>in</strong>teractionalmodifications hclp? International Review $Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 72. 1 ~ 25.Pica, T., Young, R., and Doughty, C. (1987) ‘The impact of <strong>in</strong>teraction on comprehension’.TESOL Quarterh21. 737-58.


~ (1322 MICHAEL P. BREENPlough, I. and Gass, S. (1993) ‘Intcrlocutor and task familiarity: effects on <strong>in</strong>tcractionalstructure’, <strong>in</strong> Tasks and language learn<strong>in</strong>g: <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g theory and practice, G. Crookes and S.Gass (eds). Clcvedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Politzcr, R., Ramirez, A., and Lcwis, S. (1981) ‘<strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> standard <strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong> thc third grade:classroom functions of language’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 31. 171-93.Rampton, B. (1987) ‘Stylistic variability and not spcak<strong>in</strong>g “normal” <strong>English</strong>: some post-Labovian approachcs and thcir implications for the study of <strong>in</strong>terlanguage’, <strong>in</strong> Secondlanguage acquisition <strong>in</strong> context, R. Ellis (ed.). London: Prentice Hall International.Riley, P. (1977) Discourse networks <strong>in</strong> classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction: some problems <strong>in</strong> communicative languageteach<strong>in</strong>g. Mdangcs Pedagogiques. University of Nancy: CRAPEL.Schiefclbusch, R. and Pickar, J. (eds) (1 984) The acquisition .f communicative competence.Baltimore MD.: University Park Press.Schmidt, M. (1980) ‘Coord<strong>in</strong>atc structurcs and language universals <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terlanguage’. <strong>Language</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g 30. 397416.Seligcr, H. (1977) ‘Does practice makc perfect? A study of <strong>in</strong>teraction patterns and L2competence’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 27. 263-78.S<strong>in</strong>clair, J. and Coulthard, M. (1975) Towards an analysis ofdiscourse. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.Slimani, A. (1989) ‘The rolc of topicalisation <strong>in</strong> classroom language learn<strong>in</strong>g’. System 17.223-34.992) ‘Evaluation of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction’, <strong>in</strong> Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g second language acquisition,J. Alderson and A. Berctta (eds). Cambridgc: Cambridge Univcrsity Press.Snow, C. and Hoefnagcl-Hiihlc, M. (1982) ‘School age second language learncrs’ access tosimplified l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put’. <strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 32.411-30.Spada, N. (1987) ‘Relationships hctween <strong>in</strong>structional differcnces and lcarn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes: aprocess-product study of comrnunicativc language teach<strong>in</strong>g’. Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics 8.181-99.Strong, M. (1983) ‘<strong>Social</strong> stylcs and sccond language acquisition of Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>gk<strong>in</strong>dergartcners’ . TESOL Quarterr, 17. 241-5 8.Strong, M. (1984) ‘Intcgrative motivation: cause or rcsult of succcssful language acquisition’.<strong>Language</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g 34. 1-14.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1985) ‘Communicative compctcnce: somc roles of comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put andcomprehensiblc output <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> developmcnt’, <strong>in</strong> Input <strong>in</strong> second language acquisition, S. Gassand C. Madden (cds). Rowlcy Mass: Ncwhury Housc.Swa<strong>in</strong>, M. (1995) ‘Threc functions of output <strong>in</strong> second language Icarn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>in</strong> For H.G.Widdowson: pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and practice <strong>in</strong> the study oflanguage, G. Cook and B. Seidlhofer (eds).Oxford: Oxford University Press.Takahashi, T. (1989) ‘The <strong>in</strong>flucncc of listcncr on L2 spccch’, <strong>in</strong> Variation <strong>in</strong> second languugeacquisition, vol II: Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic issues, S. Gass., C. Maddcn, D. Prcston, and L. Sel<strong>in</strong>ker(eds). Clcvedon: Multil<strong>in</strong>gual Matters.Tanaka, N. (1991)‘Politcncss: some problcrns for Japanese learncrs of <strong>English</strong>’./ALT/ournal9.81-102.Tarone, E. (1988) Variation <strong>in</strong> Interlanguage. London: Edward Arnold.Van Dijk, T. (1985) Handbook ofdiscourse analysis,Vols 1-4. London: Academic Press.Van Lier, L. (1988) The classroom and the language learner. London: Longman.Vygotsky, L. (1986) Thought and language (new edition, A. Kozul<strong>in</strong> (ed)). Cambridge, Mass:MIT Press.Wells, G. (1981) Learn<strong>in</strong>g through <strong>in</strong>teraction: the study of language development. <strong>Language</strong> at homeand school: 1. Cambridgc: Cambridgc University Press.~- 1985. <strong>Language</strong> development <strong>in</strong> the preschoolyears. <strong>Language</strong> at home and school: 2. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.t


Chapter 20Joan SwannRECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALKIN EDUCATIONAL SETTINGSIntroductionHIS CHAPTER PROVIDES GUIDANCE FOR those who wish to carry outT an <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong>to aspects of spoken language. It is designed ma<strong>in</strong>ly for use <strong>in</strong>educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs, and will probably be particularly appropriate for teachers and othereducationists engaged on small-scale research projects. Many of the techniques andpr<strong>in</strong>ciples it discusses, however, apply equally well to <strong>in</strong>vestigations of spoken language <strong>in</strong>non-cducational contexts.I shall discuss factors to take <strong>in</strong>to account when mak<strong>in</strong>g audio and video record<strong>in</strong>gs ofspoken language, then look at different ways of mak<strong>in</strong>g a written transcript from thcserecord<strong>in</strong>gs. The article does not provide detailed guidance on analysis, but I shall refer toother chapters <strong>in</strong> this volume that serve as examples of different ways of analys<strong>in</strong>g talk.Prelim<strong>in</strong>aries: decid<strong>in</strong>g what <strong>in</strong>formation you need and how tocollect thisI am assum<strong>in</strong>g that, as a reader of this chapter, you will alrcady have <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d a clear purposefor record<strong>in</strong>g and analys<strong>in</strong>g spoken language - that you will have identified certa<strong>in</strong> issuesto focus on, perhaps specified, <strong>in</strong> a formal project, as a set of research questions. Thesequestions will affect the sett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which you carry out your research, the people and eventsyou decide to observe and record, the stance you adopt towards others <strong>in</strong>volvcd <strong>in</strong> yourrcsearch, the particular types of record<strong>in</strong>g you make and how you transcribe and analysethese.Select<strong>in</strong>g a sample of people and eventsS<strong>in</strong>ce you cannot, and will not wish to record everyth<strong>in</strong>g that is go<strong>in</strong>g on you will need toselect people and events to focus on. If your <strong>in</strong>terest is <strong>in</strong> aspects of classroom talk, you maywish to focus on talk betwcen the teacher (yoursclf or a colleague) and pupils, or betwcendifferent pupils, or both. You may be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> wholc-class discussion or small-grouptalk.You may wish to compare contributions from a small number of pupils <strong>in</strong> differentcontexts, or to monitor one child closely <strong>in</strong> a range of activities.


324 JOAN SWANNYou will also need to th<strong>in</strong>k about the representativeness of the types of talk you wishto exam<strong>in</strong>e. For <strong>in</strong>stance, how are you select<strong>in</strong>g the types of activity that you wish to recordand analyse? Do these cover the full range of activities normally encountered? Or are youcontrast<strong>in</strong>g contexts you th<strong>in</strong>k are dist<strong>in</strong>ctive <strong>in</strong> some way?If you are carry<strong>in</strong>g out a small-scale <strong>in</strong>vestigation focus<strong>in</strong>g on talk <strong>in</strong> one or twocontexts, there are two important po<strong>in</strong>ts to bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d about the samples of talk youeventually come up with:Your observations may provide great <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to peoples’ conversational strategies,the way they manage certa<strong>in</strong> activities or their understand<strong>in</strong>g of certa<strong>in</strong> concepts -but you cannot make broad generalizations on the basis of a small number ofobservations. For <strong>in</strong>stance, observations of peoples’ behaviour <strong>in</strong> one set of contextsdo not provide evidence of how they ‘generally’ behave.A related po<strong>in</strong>t is that there are problems <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferences about people’s abilitiesor understand<strong>in</strong>g on the basis ofwhat they happen to do when you are record<strong>in</strong>g them.For <strong>in</strong>stance, just because students do not produce certa<strong>in</strong> types of talk does not meanthey cannot. On the other hand, students may develop cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies that make itappear they understand more than they do.Adopt<strong>in</strong>g a researcher stanceA dist<strong>in</strong>ction is commonly made <strong>in</strong> research between participant and non-participantobservation. A participant observer is someone who takes part <strong>in</strong> the event she or he isobserv<strong>in</strong>g; a non-participant observer does not take part. There are practical difficultieswith this dist<strong>in</strong>ction: for <strong>in</strong>stance, by virtue of be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a classroom (or meet<strong>in</strong>g, etc.), orby sett<strong>in</strong>g up record<strong>in</strong>g equipment, you are to some extent a participant - and you are likelyto have an effect on people’s language behaviour. The l<strong>in</strong>guist Labov identified what hetermed ‘the observer’s paradox’ (Labov, 1970) ~ that the mere act of observ<strong>in</strong>g people’slanguage behaviour (or, for that matter, other aspects of their behaviour) is <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to changethat behaviour. Different effccts arc likely to be produced by different observers (it maymatter whether an observer is female or male, or perceived as relatively senior or junior).Many l<strong>in</strong>guistic researchers (such as Labov himself) have attempted, <strong>in</strong> various ways, tom<strong>in</strong>imise the <strong>in</strong>trusion of their observations <strong>in</strong> order to obta<strong>in</strong> more ‘authentic’ data. Othershave argued that such detachment is not a reasonable research goal:We <strong>in</strong>evitably br<strong>in</strong>g our biographies and our subjectivities to every stage ofthe research process, and this <strong>in</strong>fluences the questions we ask and the ways <strong>in</strong> whichwe try to f<strong>in</strong>d answers. Our view is that the subjectivity of the observer should notbe seen as a regrettable disturbance but as one element <strong>in</strong> the human <strong>in</strong>teractionsthat comprise our object of study. Similarly, research subjects themselves are activeand reflexive be<strong>in</strong>gs who have <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to their situations and experiences.They cannot be observed as if they were asteroids, <strong>in</strong>animate lumps of matter: theyhave to be <strong>in</strong>teracted with. (Cameron, Frazer, Harvey, Rampton and Richardson,1992, p. 5)For educationists research<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their own <strong>in</strong>stitutions, or <strong>in</strong>stitutions with which they havea close association, it will probably be impossible to act as a completely detached observer.It will be impossible, for <strong>in</strong>stancc, to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a strict separation between your role as anobserver and your usual role as a teacher or a colleague. When <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the talk you


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALI< 325collect you will need to take account of the effect your own presence, and the way youcarried out the observations, may have had on your data.It is also important to consider, more generally, the relationship you have, or that youenter <strong>in</strong>to, with those who participate <strong>in</strong> your research and allow you to observe thcirlanguage behaviour. I have used the term researcher stance to refer to this more generalrelationship - the way a researcher behaves towards the people and events she or hc isobserv<strong>in</strong>g. Cameron, Frazer, Harvey, Rampton and Richardson (1 992) dist<strong>in</strong>guish betweenthree k<strong>in</strong>ds of relationship, or researcher stance:‘ethical research’, <strong>in</strong> which a researcher bears <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d the <strong>in</strong>terests of researchparticipants - e.g. m<strong>in</strong>imis<strong>in</strong>g any <strong>in</strong>convenience caused, protect<strong>in</strong>g privacy - but stillcarries out research on participants: <strong>in</strong> this case, it is the researcher who sets theagenda, not other research participants;‘advocacy’, <strong>in</strong> which researchers carry out research on and for participants - e.g.regard<strong>in</strong>g themselves as accountable to participants and be<strong>in</strong>g will<strong>in</strong>g to use theirexpert knowledge on participants’ behalf (when required by participants to do so);‘empower<strong>in</strong>g research’, <strong>in</strong> which researchers carry out research on, for and with otherparticipants - e.g. be<strong>in</strong>g completely open about the aims and methods of the research,recognis<strong>in</strong>g the importance of participants’ own agendas, empower<strong>in</strong>g participantsby giv<strong>in</strong>g them direct access to expert knowledge.The k<strong>in</strong>d of researcher stance you feel able to adopt will affect the overall conduct of yourresearch - what you research, the specific methods you adopt, how you <strong>in</strong>terpretyour results, the forms <strong>in</strong> which you dissem<strong>in</strong>ate research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Po<strong>in</strong>ts to consider<strong>in</strong>clude:What k<strong>in</strong>d oftalk is it reasonable to record? Only ‘public’ talk or also casual, or ‘private’conversation?Do you always need permission to record talk? Researchers would usually ga<strong>in</strong> permissionto make record<strong>in</strong>gs (perhaps from parents <strong>in</strong> the case of young children), whereas talkmay be recorded by teachers as a part of ‘normal’ teach<strong>in</strong>g activity that docs notrequire permission. But what if the teacher is also a researcher, or if s/he wishes tomake use of ‘rout<strong>in</strong>e’ record<strong>in</strong>gs for research purposes?How open shouldyou be about the purposes $your record<strong>in</strong>gs? Bound up with this questionis the notion of the observer’s paradox: it is likely that the more you tell people aboutyour research the more their behaviour will be affected. Some researcherscompromise: they are rather vague about the precise purposes of their research,though they may say more after complet<strong>in</strong>g their record<strong>in</strong>g. ‘Empower<strong>in</strong>g’ rescarchwould require greater openness and consultation.You may also feel that, if you areobserv<strong>in</strong>g as a colleague or a teacher, it is important to rcta<strong>in</strong> an atmosphere of trustbetween yourself and thosc you work with.To what extent should you discuss your record<strong>in</strong>gs with research participants? This has to dopartly with the researcher stance you adopt. Discuss<strong>in</strong>g record<strong>in</strong>gs with others alsolets you check your <strong>in</strong>terpretations aga<strong>in</strong>st theirs, and may give you a differentunderstand<strong>in</strong>g of your data.How shouldyou identfy thoseyou have recorded? In writ<strong>in</strong>g reports, researchers often givepseudonyms to <strong>in</strong>stitutions <strong>in</strong> which they have carried out research, or people whoscwords they quote. If you have worked more collaboratively with participants, however,they may wish to be identified by name. If you do wish to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> confidentiality it


326 JOAN SWANNmay be hard to do this where you are observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> your own <strong>in</strong>stitution ~ the identityof those you refer to may be apparent to other colleagues. One solution is to tliscusswith colleagues or students how much confidentiality they feel is necessary and howthis may be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed.In what ways should you consult those you have recorded about the dissem<strong>in</strong>ation and furtheruse fyour work? People may give permission to be recorded for a certa<strong>in</strong> purpose, butwhat if' your purposcs change? E.g. you may wish to dissem<strong>in</strong>ate your work to a wideraudience, or to use a video obta<strong>in</strong>ed for your research <strong>in</strong> a professional developmentsession with local teachers.For those <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the relationships between researchers and 'the rescarched' , Cameronet a]. (1992) is a useful source. Professional organisations also provide research guidel<strong>in</strong>es- see for <strong>in</strong>stance the British Association for Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics (1994) Recommendations onGood Practice <strong>in</strong> Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics.The sections that follow provide practical guidance on mak<strong>in</strong>g audio and videorecord<strong>in</strong>gs, mak<strong>in</strong>g fieldnotes to supplement these record<strong>in</strong>gs, and transcrib<strong>in</strong>g talk fordetailed analysis.Mak<strong>in</strong>g audio and video record<strong>in</strong>gsWhen plann<strong>in</strong>g to record talk <strong>in</strong> classrooms or othcr educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs, it is importantto allow adequate time for this. Unlers record<strong>in</strong>g equipment is rout<strong>in</strong>ely used, you will needto allow time to collect, set up and check equipment.You will also need to pilot your datacollection methods to ensure that it is possible to record clearly the k<strong>in</strong>ds of data you are<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>. When you have made your record<strong>in</strong>gs you will need time to play and replaythese to become familiar with your data and to make transcriptions.An <strong>in</strong>itial decision concerns whether to make audro or video record<strong>in</strong>gs. Videos areparticularly useful for those with an <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> non-verbal behaviour; they are also usefulfor show<strong>in</strong>g how certa<strong>in</strong> activities are carried out, or certa<strong>in</strong> equipment used. On the otherhand, video cameras are likely to be more <strong>in</strong>trusive then audio rccorders, and you may alsof<strong>in</strong>d it harder to obta<strong>in</strong> a clear record<strong>in</strong>g of speech.I have set out bclow some practical po<strong>in</strong>ts to bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d when mak<strong>in</strong>g a choicebetween audio and video record<strong>in</strong>gs.After you have made record<strong>in</strong>gs, it is useful to make a separate note of the date, timeand context of each sequence, and then summarize the content (use the cassette playercounter to make an <strong>in</strong>dex of your tape and help you locate extracts aga<strong>in</strong>).Audio or video record<strong>in</strong>gs?A udio-record<strong>in</strong>gsAn audio-cassette recorder can be <strong>in</strong>trusive - though this is less likely to be thecase <strong>in</strong> classrooms where pupils are used to be<strong>in</strong>g recorded, or record<strong>in</strong>gthemselves. Intrusiveness is more of a problem if' cassette recorders are uscd<strong>in</strong> contexts where talk is not normally recorded, and where there is not theopportunity for record<strong>in</strong>g to become rout<strong>in</strong>e (e.g. staff or other meet<strong>in</strong>gs).


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 327Intrusiveness can be lessened by keep<strong>in</strong>g the technology simple and unobtrusive,for example by us<strong>in</strong>g a small, battery-operated cassette recorder with a built-<strong>in</strong>microphone. This also avoids the danger of trail<strong>in</strong>g wires, and the problem off<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g appropriate sockets.It is also better to use a fairly simple cassette recorder if pupils are record<strong>in</strong>gthemselves. In this case, go for a mach<strong>in</strong>e with a small number of controls, andcheck that young pupils can operate the buttons easily.There is a trade-off between lack of <strong>in</strong>trusiveness/ease of use and quality ofrecord<strong>in</strong>g: more sophisticated mach<strong>in</strong>es, used with separate microphones,will produce a better quality record<strong>in</strong>g. This is a consideration if you <strong>in</strong>tend touse the record<strong>in</strong>gs with others, for example <strong>in</strong> a professional developmentsession.A s<strong>in</strong>gle cassette recorder is not suitable for record<strong>in</strong>g whole-class discussion,unless you focus on the teacher’s talk. The recorder will pick up loud voices, orvoices that are near to it, and probably lose the rest beh<strong>in</strong>d background noise(scrap<strong>in</strong>g chairs and so on). Even when record<strong>in</strong>g a small group, backgroundnoise is a problem. It is worth check<strong>in</strong>g this by pilot<strong>in</strong>g your record<strong>in</strong>garrangements: speakers may need to be located <strong>in</strong> a quieter area outside theclassroom.With audio-record<strong>in</strong>gs you lose important nonverbal and contextual <strong>in</strong>formation.Unless you are familiar with the speakers you may also f<strong>in</strong>d it difficult todist<strong>in</strong>guish between different voices. Wherever possible, supplement audiorecord<strong>in</strong>gswith field-notes or a diary provid<strong>in</strong>g contextual <strong>in</strong>formation.Video-record<strong>in</strong>gsVideo cameras are more <strong>in</strong>trusive than audio-cassette recorders. In contexts suchas classrooms, <strong>in</strong>trusiveness can be lessened by leav<strong>in</strong>g the recorder around fora while (switched off).A video camera is highly selective ~ it cannot pick up everyth<strong>in</strong>g that is go<strong>in</strong>g on<strong>in</strong> a large room such as a classroom. If you move it around the classroom you willget an impression of what is go<strong>in</strong>g on, but will not pick up much data you canactually use for analysis. A video camera may be used to focus on the teacher’sbehaviour. When used to record pupils, it is best to select a small group, carry<strong>in</strong>gout an activity <strong>in</strong> which they don’t need to move around too much.As with audio-record<strong>in</strong>gs, it is best to have the group <strong>in</strong> a quiet area where theirwork will not be disrupted by onlookers.The record<strong>in</strong>g will be more useable if you check that the camera has all that youwant <strong>in</strong> view and then leave it runn<strong>in</strong>g. If you move the camera around you maylose important <strong>in</strong>formation, and you may <strong>in</strong>troduce bias (by focus<strong>in</strong>g selectivelyon certa<strong>in</strong> pupils or actions).Video cameras with built-<strong>in</strong> microphones don’t always produce good soundrecord<strong>in</strong>gs.You will need to check this. A common problem is that you may needto locate a camera a long way from the group you are observ<strong>in</strong>g both to obta<strong>in</strong>a suitable angle of view, and to keep the apparatus unobtrusive. If it is importantthat you hear precisely what each person says, you may need to make a separateaudio-record<strong>in</strong>g or use an external microphone plugged <strong>in</strong>to the video camera.


328 JOAN SWANNMak<strong>in</strong>g field-notesFicld-notes allow you to jot down, <strong>in</strong> a systematic way, your observations on activitiesand events. Thcy provide useful contextual support for audio and video record<strong>in</strong>gs, andmay also be an important source of <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> their own right. For <strong>in</strong>stance, if yourfocus is on students <strong>in</strong> a particular lesson, you may wish to make notes on a (related)discussion between teachers; on other lessons you are unable to record; or on the lessonyou are focus<strong>in</strong>g on, to supplement your audio/video record<strong>in</strong>gs.You may also wish to makenotes on the audio/video record<strong>in</strong>gs themselves, as a prelude to (and a context for)transcription.If you are tak<strong>in</strong>g notcs of a discussion or lesson on the spot, you will f<strong>in</strong>d that the talkflows very rapidly. This is likely to be the case particularly <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal talk, such as talkbetween students <strong>in</strong> a group. More formal talk is often easier to observe on the spot. Inwhole-class discussion led by a teacher, or <strong>in</strong> formal meet<strong>in</strong>gs, usually only one person talksat a time, and participants may wait to talk until nom<strong>in</strong>ated by the teacher or chair. Theteacher or chair may rephrasc or summarirle what others speakers have said. The slightlymore ordered nature of such talk gives an observer more breath<strong>in</strong>g space to take notes.It is usual to date notes and to provide brief contextual <strong>in</strong>formation.The format adoptcdis highly variable - depend<strong>in</strong>g on particular research <strong>in</strong>terests and personal preferences.Figure 20.1 shows extracts from field-notes made by my Open University colleague JanetMayb<strong>in</strong> while watch<strong>in</strong>g an assembly <strong>in</strong> a school <strong>in</strong> the south-cast of England. Janet Mayb<strong>in</strong>’sobservations form part of a larger study of 10-1 2 year old children’s collaborative languagepractices <strong>in</strong> school. In this extract, she was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g the values laid down <strong>in</strong>school assemblies. She wanted to see whcthcr, and how, these might resurface later <strong>in</strong>children’s talk <strong>in</strong> other contexts.Janet Mayb<strong>in</strong> was not tak<strong>in</strong>g an active part <strong>in</strong> the assembly, so she could jot downobservations and briefcomments at the time. She also audio-recorded the assembly for lateranalysis (she occasionally jots down counter numbers <strong>in</strong> her field-notes). After school, shewrote up her ficld-notes, separat<strong>in</strong>g observations (what actually happened) from acommentary (her questions, reflections, <strong>in</strong>terpretations, ideas for th<strong>in</strong>gs to follow up later).Separat<strong>in</strong>g ‘observation’ from ‘commentary’ is useful <strong>in</strong> that it encourages the observerto th<strong>in</strong>k carefully about what they have observed, and to try out different <strong>in</strong>terpretations.Bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, however, that no obscrvation is entirely free from <strong>in</strong>terpretation: what youfocus on and how you describe events will already depend on an implicit <strong>in</strong>terpretiveframework.Mak<strong>in</strong>g a transcriptIn order to analyse spoken language at any level of detail, you will need to make a writtentranscript. Transcription is, however, very time-consum<strong>in</strong>g. Edwards and Westgate (1 994)suggest that every hour’s record<strong>in</strong>g may require 15 hours for transcription. I f<strong>in</strong>d that I canmake a rough transcript more quickly than this, but a detailed transcript may take far longer,particularly if a lot of nonvcrbal or contextual <strong>in</strong>formation is <strong>in</strong>cluded.In small-scale research, transcripts may be used selectively. For <strong>in</strong>stance, you couldtranscribe (timed) extracts - say 10 m<strong>in</strong>utes from a longer <strong>in</strong>teraction.You could use fieldnotesto identify certa<strong>in</strong> extracts for transcription; or you could makc a rough transcript ofan <strong>in</strong>teraction to identify general po<strong>in</strong>ts of <strong>in</strong>terest, then more detailed transcripts of relevantextracts.


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 329Figure 20. I Field-notcs of an assembly <strong>in</strong> a school <strong>in</strong> south-east England


330 JOAN SWANNWhile transcripts allow a relatively detailed exam<strong>in</strong>ation of spoken language, they onlyprovide a partial record: thcy cannot faithfully reproduce evcry aspect of talk. Transcriberswill tend to pay attention to different aspects depend<strong>in</strong>g upon their <strong>in</strong>terests, which meansthat a transcript is already an <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the event it seeks to record. El<strong>in</strong>or Ochs, <strong>in</strong>a now classic account of ‘Transcription as theory’, suggests that ‘transcription is a selectiveprocess reflect<strong>in</strong>g theoretical goals and def<strong>in</strong>itions’ (1 979, p. 44). This po<strong>in</strong>t is illustratedby the sample layouts and transcription conventions discussed below.Transcription conventionsMany published transcripts, such as those cited elsewhere <strong>in</strong> this volume, use conventionsof written language such as punctuation <strong>in</strong> represent<strong>in</strong>g speech. But because written downspeech is not the same as writ<strong>in</strong>g it can be quite hard to punctuate.If you do wish to punctuate a transcript bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that <strong>in</strong> so do<strong>in</strong>g you are giv<strong>in</strong>gthe speech a particular <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Compare the follow<strong>in</strong>g two methods of punctuat<strong>in</strong>ga teacher’s question(s):Now, th<strong>in</strong>k very carefully. What would happen if we cut one of those hollow balls <strong>in</strong>half? What would we f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>side?Now, th<strong>in</strong>k very carefully what would happen if we cut one of those hollow balls <strong>in</strong>half. What would we f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>side?Use of punctuation represents a trade-off between legibility and accessibility of the transcriptand what might be a premature and impressionistic analysis of the data. It is probably bestat least <strong>in</strong>itially to use as little conventional punctuation as possible. Several sets oftranscription conventions are available to <strong>in</strong>dicate features of spoken language. Some ofthese are highly detailed, allow<strong>in</strong>g transcribers to record <strong>in</strong>takes of breath, <strong>in</strong>creased volume,stress, syllable lengthen<strong>in</strong>g etc. (see, for <strong>in</strong>stance, Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, 1974;Ochs, 1979). Such conventions are designed to produce accurate transcriptions, but thereis a danger that they will lend a mislead<strong>in</strong>g sense of scientific objectivity to the exercise.Rather than be<strong>in</strong>g ‘objectively identified’ such features of speech are likely to correspondto the transcriber’s <strong>in</strong>itial <strong>in</strong>terpretations of their data.Bear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d this caveat, Figure 20.2 illustrates a simple set of conventions fortranscrib<strong>in</strong>g spoken language.Further transcription conventions may be added if need be. Alternatively, as <strong>in</strong> Figure20.2, you can leave a wide marg<strong>in</strong> to comment on features such as loudness, whisper<strong>in</strong>g,or other noises that add to the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the talk (as with other aspects of transcriptionthese will necessarily be selective).In Figure 20.2 I have used an extract from field-notes to contextualise the transcript.In the transcript <strong>its</strong>elf, I have followed the frequently-uscd convention of referr<strong>in</strong>g to thespeakers simply as teacher and students. An alternative is to give speakers pseudonyms (seethe discussion of confidentiality under ‘Adopt<strong>in</strong>g a researcher stance’ above).The sequence<strong>in</strong> Figure 20.2 comes from an <strong>English</strong> lesson carried out with seven-year-old students <strong>in</strong> aschool <strong>in</strong> Moscow, <strong>in</strong> Russia. The students are be<strong>in</strong>g encouraged to rehearse certa<strong>in</strong>vocabulary and structures. The teacher addresses each student directly to ensure theycontribute and uses features such as humour (‘I’m like a tiger’) to further encourage thestudents. In this extract Student 2 seems unsure of how to respond to the teacher’s question(as <strong>in</strong>dicated by his hesitation). In an attempt to help, the teacher offers him suggestions for


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 331Teacher beg<strong>in</strong>s by tell<strong>in</strong>g class the lesson is to be about toy animals. She arranges some stufidtoy animals on her desk, then asks the class ‘Have you got any toy animals at home?’ Studentsare selected <strong>in</strong>dividually to respond. Teacherfirst asks a girl, and makes her repeat careJully ‘Ihave got many toy animals at home.’Then turns to a boy, S1.rTranscription1 T:23 51:4 T:5 51:6 T:7891011 ss:12 T:13 S2:14 T:1516 S2:1718 T:19 52:You (student’s name] have you gotmany toy animals at homeYes I have { (.) I have a got{ mmhmany toy animals at homeThat’s good that’s right what toyanimals have you got at home (.)what name for animals (.)[student’s name] what toy animalshave you got at home (.) I’m like a tigerWhat yesI have { I have got { (.) I{ mmh {mmh a (.)or maybe two or { maybe three{ I have got a manytoy animalsmmh I have got { many toy animals{ many toy animalsNoteslow voiceT nods; lowers S2’shand and places ondeskKeyTSstudent’s name= Tcacher= Student (S1 = Student 1, ctc)underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dicates any feature you wish to comment on(.) brief pause(I SCC) timed pause{ maybebrackets <strong>in</strong>dicate the start of overlapp<strong>in</strong>g specch{ I have gottranscription of a sound etc that forms part of the utteranceFigure 20.2 Transcription of teacher-studcnt talkthe next word <strong>in</strong> his sentence (a, two, or three ~ presumably toy animals).This may be whatleads to the student’s error (a many toy animals) which is subsequently corrccted by theteachcr.Lay<strong>in</strong>g out a transcriptThe most commonly used layout, which I shall call a ‘standard’ layout, is set out rather likea dialogue <strong>in</strong> a play, with speak<strong>in</strong>g turns follow<strong>in</strong>g one another <strong>in</strong> sequence.This is the layout


someimpossible332 JOAN SWANNadopted <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.2, and <strong>in</strong> several chapters <strong>in</strong> this volume. One of the better knownalternatives to this layout is a ‘column’ layout, <strong>in</strong> which each speaker is allocated a separatecolumn for their speak<strong>in</strong>g turns.Figures 20.3 and 20.4 illustrate respectively ‘standard’ and ‘column’ layouts applied tothe same brief extract of talk. This comes from one of a series of <strong>English</strong> lessons <strong>in</strong> asecondary school <strong>in</strong> Denmark, near Copenhagen (Dam and Lentz, 1998). The class of 15-year-old mixed-ability students were carry<strong>in</strong>g out a project on ‘England and the <strong>English</strong>’.The extract shows a group of students, two girls and two boys, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g to plan what todo for their homework. The students are seated round a table, the girls opposite the boys.TranscriptionG 1 :BI:What are we go<strong>in</strong>g to do at home(.) any ideasYes(.)Itakeh(.) Itakethis yes yesI take it mmh antl 1 see and Isee if there’s someth<strong>in</strong>g I canuse (.)GI?: We can use9 BI: We canuseIO B2: So what (would) we do ( )1112read it at home (.) thequestionnaire13{ (.) read it at home1 4 B 1 {(I. . .I15 G2: Maybe 1 can get some materials16 for this17 G 1 : From ( mother)18 G2: Yes19 RI?: fromwhere20 G2 from my mother (.) from thc21 travel agencyNotesaddresses groupdirectlyrefers to book whichhe holds upquestion towardsgirls?KeyAs <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.2 with, <strong>in</strong> addition:G, B = Girl, Boy(would)transcription uncerta<strong>in</strong>: a guess( ) unclear speech ~[, . , ] excision ~to transcribedata excludedFigure 20.3 Transcription of small group talk: standard layoutIn group talk it’s often <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to look at the role taken by different students. In this case,the group seemed to collaborate fairly well antl to be generally supportive of one another.Girl 1 seemed to play an organis<strong>in</strong>g or chair<strong>in</strong>g role - e.g. by ask<strong>in</strong>g for ideas from the restof the group; by ‘correct<strong>in</strong>g’ Boy 1, rem<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g him that his work is for the group as a whole(l<strong>in</strong>e 8 of the standard layout); and by complet<strong>in</strong>g Girl 2’s turn (l<strong>in</strong>e 17 of the standard


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 333G1G2B1-B2Notes1234567891011121314151617181920212223242526272829What are we go<strong>in</strong>gto do at home (.)any ideasWe can use (?)[..'IFrom ( mother )Maybe I can getsome materialsfor thisYesfrom my mother(.) from thetravel agencyYes (.) I take- this (.) I takethis yes yesI take it mmh andI see and I seeif there'ssometh<strong>in</strong>g I canuse (.)We can use( 1So what (would)we do ( ) readit at home (.)the questionnaire(.) read it athomefrom where (?)addresbes groupdirect1 yrefers to bookwhich he holdsUPquestiontowards girls?As <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.3 with, <strong>in</strong> addition:(?) Guess at speakerFigure 20.4 Transcription of small group talk: column layoutlayout). I would be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> look<strong>in</strong>g further at this group's work to see if Girl 1ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed this role or if it was also taken on by other students.The way transcription is laid out may highlight certa<strong>in</strong> features of the talk, for <strong>in</strong>stance:The standard layout suggests a connected sequence, <strong>in</strong> which one turn follows onfrom the preced<strong>in</strong>g one.This does seem to happen <strong>in</strong> the extract transcribed <strong>in</strong> Figures20.3 and 20.4 but it is not always the case. In young children's speech, for <strong>in</strong>stance,speak<strong>in</strong>g turns may not follow on directly from a preced<strong>in</strong>g turn. I shall also give anexample of more <strong>in</strong>formal talk below <strong>in</strong> which it is harder to dist<strong>in</strong>guish a series ofscquential turns.Column transcripts allow you to track one speaker's contributions: you can look atthe number and typcs of contribution madc by a speaker (e.g. Girl 1's 'organis<strong>in</strong>g'contributions), or track the topics they focus on - or whatever else is of <strong>in</strong>terest.


334 JOAN SWANNIn a column transcript, it’s important to bcar <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d which column you allocate toeach speaker. Because of factors such as the left-right orientation <strong>in</strong> European scripts,and associated conventions of page layout, we may give priority to <strong>in</strong>formation locatedon the left hand side. Ochs (1 979) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that, <strong>in</strong> column transcripts of adult-childtalk, the adult is nearly always allocated thc left-hand column, suggest<strong>in</strong>g they are the<strong>in</strong>itiator of thc conversation. In Figure 20.4 1 began with Girl 1, probably because shespoke first, but I also groupcd thc girls and thcn the boys togethcr.This may be usefulif your <strong>in</strong>terest is, say, <strong>in</strong> gender issucs, but it’s important to considcr why you arcadopt<strong>in</strong>g a particular order and not to regard this as, somehow, ‘natural’.Accounts of convcrsational turn-tak<strong>in</strong>g havc oftcn assumcd that onc person talks at a timc(e.g. Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, 1974). As I suggested above, however, this is not alwaysthe case, particularly <strong>in</strong> young children’s talk, or <strong>in</strong> more <strong>in</strong>formal discussion where thereis lots of ovcrlapp<strong>in</strong>g talk and whcrc spcakcrs frcqucntly complctc one another’s turns. Inher analysis of <strong>in</strong>formal talk amongst women fricnds, Jcnnifcr Coatcs dcvcloped a methodof transcription <strong>in</strong> which she used a ‘stave’ layout (by analogy with musical staves) torepresent the jo<strong>in</strong>t construction of speak<strong>in</strong>g turns (see, for <strong>in</strong>stance, Coates, 1996). Stavctranscription has not been used frequently <strong>in</strong> educational contexts but may be adopted toillustrate highly collaborative talk <strong>in</strong> small groups. Figure 20.5 comes from a study madeby Julia Davies (2000) of <strong>English</strong> lcssons <strong>in</strong> thrcc sccondary schools <strong>in</strong> Sheffield, <strong>in</strong> the northof England. Davics was particularly <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> gender issues ~ <strong>in</strong> how girls and boysworked together <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-sex and mixed-sex groups. Figurc 20.5 shows a group of fourteenage girls reflect<strong>in</strong>g on their earlier cxpcricnccs of school. Davies found (like Coates)that the girls’ talk was particularly collaborative (e.g. it conta<strong>in</strong>ed overlapp<strong>in</strong>g specch, jo<strong>in</strong>tconstruction of turns and several <strong>in</strong>dicators of conversational support).Thc layout you choose for a transcript will depend on what you arc transcrib<strong>in</strong>g andwhy. Here I have tried to show how different layouts highlight certa<strong>in</strong> aspects of talk andplay down others.You will need to try out, and probably adapt, layouts till you f<strong>in</strong>d one thatsu<strong>its</strong> your purposes ~ bear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, as ever, that such dccisions are already lead<strong>in</strong>g youtowards a particular <strong>in</strong>terpretation of your data.Includ<strong>in</strong>g nonverbal and contextual <strong>in</strong>formationTranscriptions tend to highlight verbal <strong>in</strong>formation, though I have <strong>in</strong>dicated above hownonverbal <strong>in</strong>formation can be shown <strong>in</strong> a ‘notes’ column, or by typographical conventionssuch as capital letters for emphasis or loudncss. In some chapters of this book authors usediffcrcnt conventions. Paul<strong>in</strong>e Gibbons and Angel L<strong>in</strong>, for <strong>in</strong>stance (Chapters 16 and 17respectively) <strong>in</strong>clude some nonverbal <strong>in</strong>formation with<strong>in</strong> brackets <strong>in</strong> the dialogue. If youare particularly <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> nonverbal <strong>in</strong>formation you may wish to adopt transcriptionconventions that highlight this <strong>in</strong> somc way. As examples, Figure 20.6 shows how a storytelleruses a number of nonverbal features <strong>in</strong> her performance of a Nigerian story (‘A manamongst men’); and Figure 20.7 shows how a teacher uses gaze to nom<strong>in</strong>ate female or malestudents to respond to her questions.Represent<strong>in</strong>g d<strong>in</strong>eren t language varietiesThe transcripts of classroom talk I have illustrated so far come from contexts <strong>in</strong> which<strong>English</strong> is be<strong>in</strong>g used as a medium of <strong>in</strong>struction. In many contexts, however, even whcrc<strong>English</strong> is used as a classroom language, teachers and students may also usc another language,


~andRECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 3351 Bel Right/anyth<strong>in</strong>g else? / everyone {have a th<strong>in</strong>k/right/Jan{everyone have a th<strong>in</strong>kLouRosa2 BelJanLouRosaabout their important memories /3 ncl I’ve got one (.) /right I rcmember (.)JanLouRosa4 Re1 { Jan AGAIN/JanLouRosaI’ve got this important {memory of school was-/I got5 BelJan {this effort trophy at mid& school (.) /Lou {Jan aga<strong>in</strong>/ yeah?/Rosa6 BelJanLouRosaand I-/oh and I were-/and I was dead chuffed/I thought it were great/7 nciJanan cffort trophy?/ it were grcat weren’t it?/Lou I got one of them/ yeah/Rosa8 BelJanLouRosa{it werc great/{at the fourth year of juniors/KCYAs abovc with, <strong>in</strong> addition:Yeah/ A slash represents the end of a tone group, or chunk of talkYeah?/ A question mark <strong>in</strong>dicates the end of a chunk analyscd as a questionAGAIN Capital lctters <strong>in</strong>dicate a word uttered with emphasisStaves are numbered and separated by horizontal l<strong>in</strong>es; all the talk with<strong>in</strong> a stave is to be read together,sequentially from left to right.Figure 20.5 Transcription of group talk: stavc layoutSource: adapted from Davies (2000): 290Note: Davies follows Coates <strong>in</strong> represent<strong>in</strong>g, with<strong>in</strong> a stave, only those students who arc speak<strong>in</strong>g. Here I hare<strong>in</strong>cluded all students throughout the transcription which illustrates, for <strong>in</strong>stance, that one student, Rosa, doesnot speak at all <strong>in</strong> this sequence. Rosa may have been contribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> other ways ~ e.g. nonverbally ~ shedoes speak later <strong>in</strong> the discussion.


336 JOAN SWANNTranscriptNotcs1 [Once upon a time] a I longspread<strong>in</strong>g gesture to start story; downwardI long I long I long I longtimeago there was a I hunter a [verywcll-known and respected hunter]*every day he would go I out <strong>in</strong>to theI bush he would catch whatevermeat he needed for the village, hewould carrv it on his back he wouldbr<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong>to the villape he wouldthrow it down on the floor the peoplethey would see him *they would startclapp<strong>in</strong>g their hands body orientation.*hands out to A; A also <strong>in</strong>vited hy directgaze, head movcmcnt, generalKeyI[Once upon a time]Ilong*every daycatch whatever meatASquare brackets <strong>in</strong>dicate beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g and endof large spread<strong>in</strong>g gestureVcrtical slash <strong>in</strong>dicates downward gestureaccompany<strong>in</strong>g a wordAsterisk <strong>in</strong>dicates someth<strong>in</strong>g that iscommented on <strong>in</strong> the ‘Notes’ columnUnderl<strong>in</strong>ed speech <strong>in</strong>dicates that thestoryteller also mimes the actions shedescribesAs <strong>in</strong> transcripts above, <strong>in</strong>dicatessound/action that forms part of theutteranceAudienceFigure 20.6 Representation of nonverbal features <strong>in</strong> an oral narrativesuch as the students’ first or ma<strong>in</strong> language, for certa<strong>in</strong> purposes. In this case, it may be<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to see when a teacher or student uses each language.There are many diffcrent ways of represent<strong>in</strong>g the alternation between differentlanguage varieties. In Chapter 17 for <strong>in</strong>stance, Angel L<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicates ‘codeswitch<strong>in</strong>g’ betweenCantonese and <strong>English</strong>, represent<strong>in</strong>g Cantonese <strong>in</strong> translation and <strong>in</strong> bold type. Figures 20.8and 20.9 show how researchers have represented languages <strong>in</strong> their orig<strong>in</strong>al form whilstalso offer<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>English</strong> translation. Figure 20.8, from research carried out by Anto<strong>in</strong>etteCamillcri <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms <strong>in</strong> Malta, shows a teacher alternat<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>English</strong> andMaltese, where Maltese is used to amplify or expla<strong>in</strong> (rather than simply translate) an <strong>English</strong>sentence read from a textbook. In this case, an <strong>English</strong> translation of the Maltese utterancesi.; given <strong>in</strong> a separate column. Figure 20.9, from research carried out by G.D. Jayalakshmi<strong>in</strong> Bihar, <strong>in</strong> northern India, shows how a teacher uses Sanskrit partly to demonstrate hisknowledge and also ‘because he believes that his function is to <strong>in</strong>struct students not only <strong>in</strong>language but also, more generally, <strong>in</strong> life’ (Jayalakshmi, 1996, p. 145). In this case, an <strong>English</strong>translation is given <strong>in</strong> brackets beneath the Sanskrit.In Figure 20.9, Jayalakshmi represents Sanskrit <strong>in</strong> Devanagari script. It would alsohave been possible to represent it <strong>in</strong> transliteration, <strong>in</strong> Roman script. It is, however, moredifficult to decide how to represent language varieties closely related to <strong>English</strong>, or differentvarieties of <strong>English</strong>, that do not have a conventional orthography. Figure 20.5 represented


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALI< 33Teacher:Mathew:Teacher:Mathew:Teacher:Boy:Teacher:Boy:Boy:-If you have a pendulum (.) which we established last week was a weight a mass (.) suspendedfrom a str<strong>in</strong>g or whatever....................... ----------I-------------- --------------- ---------------(.) and watch I’m hold<strong>in</strong>g it with my hand so it’s at rest at the moment (.) what is it thatI (.)just watchit1 gravity11111-111-----What is it Mathew? [2]-----Gravityc-:)IYes (.) now we mentioned gravity when we were............................... -------------------actually do<strong>in</strong>g the experiments but we didn’t discuss it too much (.) OK so it’s gravity then that---11111111------------pulls it down (.) what causes it to go up aga<strong>in</strong> at the other side? [3]---------I-Force the forceThe str<strong>in</strong>g Missmakes the pendulum sw<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a downward direction for <strong>in</strong>stance till it gets to there? [ 1 I?-----{ } overlap(.)pause( )unclearmeans gaze to boysmeans gaze to girlsFigure 20.7 Representation of teacher’s gaze towards female and male studentsSource: Swann and Graddol (1 989/ 1994): 157-9Note: The full transcript from which Figure 20.7 is extracted shows that the teacher’s gaze is morefrequently directed towards the boys at critical po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>teraction, such as when a question isto be answered.England Australia New Zealand andArgent<strong>in</strong>a are the best producers ofwool dawk 1-aktar h gfiandhom farms11 Irobbu n-nagfiag gfios-sUfO.K. Englandtgfiiduli m<strong>in</strong>n Iicma post Englandgfiondhom Scotland magfiruFntant gfia11-wool u gersipcttagfihom O.K.they have the largest number of farms andthe largest number of sheep for wool O.K.England where <strong>in</strong> England we really meanScotland they are very well-known fortheir woollen productsFigure 20.8 Transcript illustrat<strong>in</strong>g alternation between <strong>English</strong> and MalteseSource: Camilleri (1994) cited <strong>in</strong> Mercer (1996): 134, and Chapter 15 of this volumenonstandard grammar (‘it were great’) but did not attempt to represent the girls’ accent.Some transcribers resort to ‘eye dialect’ (as <strong>in</strong> we wuzjus’go<strong>in</strong>”orne) to give an <strong>in</strong>dication ofpronunciation but there is a danger here of represent<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> speakers (work<strong>in</strong>g classspeakers, children, non-native speakers) as somehow deviant or <strong>in</strong>competent.


sometimes338 JOAN SWANNDr Kcval:[Good company produces had qualities](SK)You might have come across this very say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Sanskrit.[Good company produces had qualities](sic)There I mean, we cultivate qualities by virtue of‘ what (?) company. If weare <strong>in</strong> good company, we’ll cultivate good th<strong>in</strong>gs, good hab<strong>its</strong>. If we are <strong>in</strong>bad company we’ll he cultivat<strong>in</strong>g had hab<strong>its</strong>. So this will be our attempt tohe <strong>in</strong> good company. Always have control over yourself. Try your bestalways for keep<strong>in</strong>g good company.(Lesson I, 6 July 1987)Figure 20.9 Transcript illustrat<strong>in</strong>g alternation bctween Sanskrit and <strong>English</strong>Source: Jayalakshmi (1 996): 145Note: In this ease there is an error <strong>in</strong> Dr Keval’s Sanskrit. Jayalakshmi comments that he may have learntquotations such as this by rote.Mark Sebba used a mixed system <strong>in</strong> his transcription of the speech of young Blackspeakers <strong>in</strong> London, who alternate between Creole (derived from Jamaican Creole) andLondon <strong>English</strong>. Creole utterances were underl<strong>in</strong>ed, London <strong>English</strong> utterances were not.Underl<strong>in</strong>ed utterances were, then, to be ‘pronounced as if Creole’ (1993, p. 163). Sebbaalso used some ‘eye dialect’ features to <strong>in</strong>dicate the pronunciation of specific words orsounds; and certa<strong>in</strong> ‘one-off’ conventions, such as the use of ‘Yo’ to represent a glottal stop(the sound used as a variant of /t/ <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic contexts, and <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> varieties of<strong>English</strong> ~ represented as an apostrophe as <strong>in</strong> bu’er for butter). Figure 20.10illustrates this. One po<strong>in</strong>t of <strong>in</strong>terest is that the glottal stop, a feature of London <strong>English</strong> butnot (usually) of Jamaican Creole, is here used with<strong>in</strong> a Creole uttcrancc (mvi%e, l<strong>in</strong>e 4).Sets of symbols such as the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) are used byphoneticians to give a systematic representation of the sounds of <strong>English</strong> and other languages.Such alphabets are hard for the non-expert to read and arc not usually suitable fortranscrib<strong>in</strong>g long conversational sequences. However if you are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> learners’pronunciations of <strong>English</strong>, and you are familiar with the IPA or a similar alphabet, you coulduse phonetic symbols selectively for certa<strong>in</strong> words, or to rcprcscnt certa<strong>in</strong> sounds.Figure 20.1 1 below illustrates the use of phonetic symbols to represent a young Russianstudent’s pronunciation of the word bushy (this is taken from the same lesson as thattranscribed <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.2 above).


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALK 3391 J: did you go to Jackie’s paryoy?(1 .O)C: who JackieJ: yeahC:5 J:C: is it?J: yeahJ: noC:no one never <strong>in</strong>viyoe me1 heard that she had a really nice par%y an’ Cheryl said there was a lo% of boys there (0.6)you know and they (were) play<strong>in</strong>’ pass the parcel an’ that10 C: she <strong>in</strong>vite YOU?she never <strong>in</strong>vite me neither an Leonie lave one as well never <strong>in</strong>vite never tellme not’<strong>in</strong>’ (0.4) me no bus<strong>in</strong>ess too!Figure 20. I0 Transcription of a conversation us<strong>in</strong>g Creole and London <strong>English</strong>Source: Sebba (1993): 19-20TranscriptionNotes1 S: Its tail is short and [b~ji] pronounced to rhyme withjsby2 T: Bushy ([buJi])more conventional pronunciation3 S: Rushy ( [buJi])more conventional pronunciationFigure 20.1 1 Representation of pronunciation us<strong>in</strong>g phonetic symbolsTowards an analysis: quantitative and qualitative approachesDiscussions of research methodology often make a dist<strong>in</strong>ction betwecn quantitative andqualitative approaches to research. Broadly, quantitative approaches allow you to identifyand count the distribution of certa<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic features, or certa<strong>in</strong> types of utterance. Youcan then draw a numerical comparison between, for <strong>in</strong>stance, the types of talk produced <strong>in</strong>different contexts or by different students, or groups of students. Some forms ofquantification can be carried out ‘on the spot’. For <strong>in</strong>stance, while observ<strong>in</strong>g a lcsson youcould count the number of times each student responded to a teachcr’s question. Morecomplex patterns can be identified from scrut<strong>in</strong>y of audio or video record<strong>in</strong>gs, or from atranscript. G.D. Jayalakshmi, for <strong>in</strong>stance, whose research <strong>in</strong> Indian classrooms I referredto above, noticed that students participated less <strong>in</strong> ‘traditional’ teacher-directed lessons(draw<strong>in</strong>g on textbooks) than <strong>in</strong> lessons based on videos which she had <strong>in</strong>troduced.To checkher impressions, she analysed record<strong>in</strong>gs of a random sample of lessons, count<strong>in</strong>g up thenumber of times a student <strong>in</strong>itiated talk; and what types of talk this <strong>in</strong>volved (whether thestudent was seek<strong>in</strong>g clarification, ask<strong>in</strong>g about the mean<strong>in</strong>g of a word, mak<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle wordcontribution, or mak<strong>in</strong>g a longer contribution to discussion). She displayed her results <strong>in</strong> atable (cited asTable 20.1 below). Table 20.1 shows that, <strong>in</strong> the contexts analysed, studcnts<strong>in</strong>itiated more talk <strong>in</strong> video than traditional lessons, and they also made a large number oflonger contributions.


340 JOAN SWANNTable 20. INumber and type of student-<strong>in</strong>itiated moves <strong>in</strong> two types of lessonType of class Number of Clarification Mean<strong>in</strong>g of S<strong>in</strong>gle word Longerstudent-<strong>in</strong>itiated seek<strong>in</strong>g words contributions contributionsmovesTraditional 11 3 2 5 0Video Led 38 2 3 0 33Source: Jayalakshmi (1993): 287Chapter 18 <strong>in</strong> this volume provides a more formal and detailed example of quantification.Assia Slimani was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the relationship between students’ claims about whatl<strong>in</strong>guistic features they had learnt, and the direct teach<strong>in</strong>g of such features. Table 18.2(p. 296) illustrates this, show<strong>in</strong>g the number of l<strong>in</strong>guistic features that had been explicitlydealt with <strong>in</strong> lessons (identified from audio record<strong>in</strong>gs), and the proportion of these thatwere recalled by students, those that were not recalled, and those that were said to havebeen learned on a previous occasion.By contrast, a qualitative approach tends to be used if the questions that are asked of apiece of data are more open-ended: if you wanted to know, for <strong>in</strong>stance, what happeneddur<strong>in</strong>g a meet<strong>in</strong>g; how students worked together <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g situations; howrelationships were established and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed; or how students achieved an understand<strong>in</strong>gof certa<strong>in</strong> concepts. Most of the chapters <strong>in</strong> this volume that look at classroom languageadopt a qualitative approach to the analysis of talk. In Chapter 15, for <strong>in</strong>stance, Neil Mercerdiscusses how teachers use language to guide students’ learn<strong>in</strong>g. While Mercer identifiescerta<strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g techniques, these are not systematically coded and quantified. Mercer ismore concerned with analys<strong>in</strong>g the function of the techniques teachers use than withcount<strong>in</strong>g the frequency with which techniques are used, and illustrates this by quot<strong>in</strong>gextracts from transcripts. In Chapter 16 Paul<strong>in</strong>e Gibbons exam<strong>in</strong>es children’s progressionfrom ‘everyday’ language to the use of scientific discourse, focus<strong>in</strong>g on the experiences ofone student. The language used at different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a series of lessons is illustrated bytranscripts along with a close l<strong>in</strong>guistic commentary. Angel L<strong>in</strong>, <strong>in</strong> Chapter 17, also usesextracts from transcripts of classroom talk to illustrate the extent to which differentstudents’ ‘habitus’ is compatible with what is required of them <strong>in</strong> school <strong>English</strong> lessons.There have been several debates with<strong>in</strong> educational research about the relative mer<strong>its</strong>of quantitative and qualitative approaches. Features of each approach, and some advantagesand disadvantages that have traditionally been associated with them, are summarised <strong>in</strong> thebox opposite.While some researchers argue for an <strong>in</strong>tegration of quantitative and qualitativeapproaches, it has also been suggested that they embody fundamentally different views ofthe mean<strong>in</strong>g of spoken language (cod<strong>in</strong>g language <strong>in</strong>to discrete categories, for <strong>in</strong>stance,suggests that mean<strong>in</strong>gs are relatively fixed and unambiguous, whereas qualitative approachesoften emphasise ambiguity <strong>in</strong> language and argue that utterances need to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong>context). For an overview of this debate see, for <strong>in</strong>stance, Edwards and Westgate (1 994).Wegerif and Mercer (1 997) suggest that it is possible to progress beyond this apparentdivide by draw<strong>in</strong>g on corpus, or computer-based forms of analysis. Corpus-based analysesallow researchers to process huge amounts of spoken or written language and establishquantitative patterns of language use. They have frequently been used to identify mean<strong>in</strong>gsof words and phrases and to aid the compilation of dictionary entries.They may also be used


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALI< 341Quantitative and qualitative approaches to the analysis of spokenlanguageA quantitative approach allows you to represent your data <strong>in</strong> terms of numbers.You canmake a numerical comparison between talk produced by different people or dur<strong>in</strong>gdifferent events.When represent<strong>in</strong>g data that has been analysed us<strong>in</strong>g quantitative methods it is usualto display this <strong>in</strong> a table. Alternative forms of representation such as histograms or barcharts may be used to po<strong>in</strong>t up comparisons between people or events.Data may be analysed us<strong>in</strong>g prespecified categories of talk. Alternatively, as <strong>in</strong>Jayalakshmi’s research, categories may emerge from close scrut<strong>in</strong>y of data, e.g. fromplay<strong>in</strong>g, and replay<strong>in</strong>g, an audio or video record<strong>in</strong>g, or work<strong>in</strong>g slowly through atranscript. Such categories are not ‘naturally’ present <strong>in</strong> the data, but will depend uponyour own research <strong>in</strong>terests.Represent<strong>in</strong>g talk <strong>in</strong> terms of numbers has the disadvantage that it is necessarily areductive exercise: talk is reduced to a set of categories; it is abstracted from <strong>its</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>alcontext; it is unambiguously pigeon-holed, mask<strong>in</strong>g the rather fluid, uncerta<strong>in</strong> andnegotiated mean<strong>in</strong>gs that are evident when talk is exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> context.Talk may be recorded and analysed <strong>in</strong> a more open-ended way. Researchers adopt<strong>in</strong>ga qualitative approach to record<strong>in</strong>g can note down and explore any <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspectsof their data. What count as <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspects will depend upon the questions theresearcher is concerned to <strong>in</strong>vestigate, but sometimes po<strong>in</strong>ts emerge that are quiteunexpected.Aspects of the data may only beg<strong>in</strong> to make sense when mulled over and comparedwith other <strong>in</strong>formation, or perhaps discussed with speakers. Sometimes <strong>in</strong>terpretationsmay change, or you may want to allow for a number of different <strong>in</strong>terpretations.When present<strong>in</strong>g and discuss<strong>in</strong>g data that has been recorded and analysed us<strong>in</strong>g aqualitative approach, researchers frequently quote selectively from field-notes ortranscripts to support po<strong>in</strong>ts they wish to make. Transcripts may be supported by adetailed commentary, as <strong>in</strong> Chapters 18 and 19.Such ways of analys<strong>in</strong>g and present<strong>in</strong>g data allow the researcher to preserveimportant contextual <strong>in</strong>formation that affects the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of utterances, and also topreserve the ambiguity and fluidity of these mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The approach is selective <strong>in</strong> thattwo researchers may (legitimately) notice different th<strong>in</strong>gs about a stretch of talk orprovide different <strong>in</strong>terpretations of utterances. There is also a danger of un<strong>in</strong>tended bias,<strong>in</strong> that researchers may notice features of talk that support a po<strong>in</strong>t they wish to makeand ignore counter evidence.to identify stylistic differences between different (literary) authors or different types of text.Wegerif and Mercer illustrate how corpus-based methods may be used with smaller amountsof data, and <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with a qualitative exploration of language.Wegerif and Mercer drew on this comb<strong>in</strong>ation of methods as part of an ongo<strong>in</strong>g studyof exploratory talk <strong>in</strong> the classroom. They found that primary school children performedbetter on a standardised test of reason<strong>in</strong>g after they had been ‘coached’ <strong>in</strong> the use ofexploratory talk. They also looked at transcript evidence of the quality of children’s talkdur<strong>in</strong>g problem solv<strong>in</strong>g activities carried out before and after the coach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention.Extracts from transcripts are used to show that, after the <strong>in</strong>tervention children spent moretime discuss<strong>in</strong>g problems, considered alternative solutions and eventually reached


342 JOAN SWANNFocal Group 1 pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention task use of "cos' or 'because'Ela<strong>in</strong>e: It isn't 'cos look that's a squareGraham: No 'cos look watch there all down there and thcy arc all at the side and they areall up thereEla<strong>in</strong>e: Wait wait wait <strong>its</strong> that one 'cos look it's them two and them two ( ) and themtwoJohn: 'Cos look that goes out like that ~Ela<strong>in</strong>e: 'Cos look that goes <strong>in</strong>John: 'Cos look that goes too far outGraham: Look 'cos that's got 4Ela<strong>in</strong>e: No . . . not that one not that one because it's got a little bit like that it's that onelook ~ it goes <strong>in</strong> and then it goes outJohn: No it's isn't because it's thereEla<strong>in</strong>e: No because it will come along like thatEla<strong>in</strong>e: Could be that one because look stops at the bottom and lookEla<strong>in</strong>e: It isn't it isn't because look(12)Focal Group 1 post-<strong>in</strong>tervention task use of "cos' or 'because'Graham: Number 6 'cos 6 stops <strong>in</strong> there 'cos look if youEla<strong>in</strong>e: It can't be there 'cos look if you done thatEla<strong>in</strong>e: It is look if that goes like that and then it has another one 'cos those two makeEla<strong>in</strong>e: He doesn't say what thcy arc 'cos he might he wrongGraham: Yeh 'cos lookEla<strong>in</strong>e:John:Ela<strong>in</strong>e:'Cos it would go roundIt is 'cos it goes away 'cos look that one goes like thatNo it can't be 'cos look . . . with the squarc with the triangle you take away thetriangle so you're left with the square so if you do just this and then aga<strong>in</strong> takethat away it's go<strong>in</strong>g to end up, like that isn't it?Graham: Actually 'cos that's got a square and a circle round itJohn: Yeh 'cos it goes like that and then it takes that one away and does thatEla<strong>in</strong>e: No 'cos lookEla<strong>in</strong>e: Probably one <strong>in</strong> the circle 'cos there are only two circlesGraham: 'Cos if they are l<strong>in</strong>es and then they are go<strong>in</strong>g like that it is because they arewonky isn't itGraham: No actually it a<strong>in</strong>'t 'cos thenEla<strong>in</strong>e: Yeh it's number 8 because those ones ~ those two came that those two make thatJohn: No because 1, 2, 3 1, 2, 3John: No because that goes that way and that goes that wayGraham: No because it's that one(21)Figure 20.12 Incidence of 'cos and hecouse <strong>in</strong> primary school children's talk


RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALI< 343agreement on the correct answer. Wegerif and Mercer po<strong>in</strong>t out, however, that suchevidence may not be seen as conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g because it consists only of one or two brief extractsfrom transcripts.As a way of complement<strong>in</strong>g their <strong>in</strong>itial qualitative approach, Wegerif and Mercer useda computerised concordanc<strong>in</strong>g program. This identifies all <strong>in</strong>stances of a word or expressionused <strong>in</strong> a particular set of data, and displays these <strong>in</strong> their immediate l<strong>in</strong>guistic context. InFigure 20.12 above, for <strong>in</strong>stance, the words 'cos and because are displayed <strong>in</strong> each speak<strong>in</strong>gturn <strong>in</strong> which they occurred <strong>in</strong> one group's <strong>in</strong>teraction before and after the <strong>in</strong>tervention.Wegerif and Mercer suggest that 'cos and because are used differently <strong>in</strong> the pre- and post<strong>in</strong>tervention<strong>in</strong>teraction: <strong>in</strong> the post-<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>teractions they are more frequently usedto l<strong>in</strong>k reasons to claims. Wegerif and Mercer carried out similar analyses of other termsthat might be seen as <strong>in</strong>dicative of reason<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. fand so used to l<strong>in</strong>k a reason to anassertion).This form of analysis provides quantifiable data (i.e. it is possible to calculate thefrequency with which 'cos and because are used <strong>in</strong> different contexts). It is also possible tosee each <strong>in</strong>stance of 'cos and because <strong>in</strong> a limited l<strong>in</strong>guistic context, which provides further<strong>in</strong>formation about their use <strong>in</strong> each case (as <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.12). And it is possible, for any one<strong>in</strong>stance, to display further l<strong>in</strong>guistic context (any number of preced<strong>in</strong>g and follow<strong>in</strong>gspeak<strong>in</strong>g turns) to allow a qualitative exploration of the data.If this form of analysis <strong>in</strong>terests you, it is possible to purchase concordanc<strong>in</strong>g software(or, <strong>in</strong> some cases, to download this from the Internet).' You will need, however, to beprepared to spend time explor<strong>in</strong>g the software to see how it can be made to work mosteffectively for your own purposes. For further discussion and examples of corpus-bascdanalysis see, for <strong>in</strong>stance, Stubbs (1 996).ConclusionIn this chapter I have discussed various techniques you can use to record and transcribespoken language. There is no 'ideal' way to do this, and I have tried to <strong>in</strong>dicate the strengthsand weaknesses of different approaches so that you can select the most appropriate method,or comb<strong>in</strong>ation of methods, for your own purposes. It is beyond the scope of this chapterto consider, at any level of detail, ways of analys<strong>in</strong>g spoken language, though I have suggestedsome <strong>in</strong>itial considerations to bear <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. Other chapters <strong>in</strong> this volume <strong>in</strong>clude examplesof research on spoken language, and illustrations of different forms of analysis: these mayprovide ideas for your own research.AcknowledgementI am grateful to Rupert Wegerif for suggestions on computer-based methods of analys<strong>in</strong>gspoken language.


~ (1996)344 JOAN SWANNNote1 See, for <strong>in</strong>stance, the examples of software listed at http: / /<strong>in</strong>fo.ox.ac.uk/ctitext/resguide/resources/<strong>in</strong>dcx.html#tat It is often possible to obta<strong>in</strong> demo versions of textanalysis tools - see, for <strong>in</strong>stance, ‘Wordsmith’, available from http: / /wwwl.oup.co.uk/cite/oup/elt/software/wsmith/ReferencesBritish Association for Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics (BAAL) (1994) Recommendations on Good Practice <strong>in</strong>Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics. BAAL.Cameron, D., Fraser, E., Harvey, P., Rampton, M.B.H. and Richardson, K. (1992) Research<strong>in</strong>g<strong>Language</strong>: Issues ofpower and Method. London: Routledge.Camillcri, A. (1994) ‘Talk<strong>in</strong>g bil<strong>in</strong>gually, writ<strong>in</strong>g monol<strong>in</strong>gually’ . Paper presented at theSociol<strong>in</strong>guistics Symposium, University of Lancaster, March.Coates, J. (1996) Women Talk. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Dam, L. and Lentz, J. (1 998) It’s up toyourseCftfyou want to learn: autonomous language learn<strong>in</strong>g at<strong>in</strong>termediate level (Video and pr<strong>in</strong>t). Copenhagen: Danmarks Laererhngskole.Davics, J.A. (2000) Expressions of Gender: An Enquiry <strong>in</strong>to the way Gender Impacts on the DiscourseStyles of Pupils <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> Small Group Talk dur<strong>in</strong>g a GCSE <strong>English</strong> lesson, with particularreference to the under-achievement ofboys. Unpublished PhD Thesis. Sheffield: University ofSheffield.Edwards, A.D. and Westgate, D.P. G. (1994) Investigat<strong>in</strong>g Classroom Talk. London: Falmer Press(2nd edn).Jayalakshmi, G.D. (1993) ‘Video <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong> curriculum of an Indian secondary school’.Unpublished PhD thesis. Milton Keynes: The Open University.‘One cup of newspaper and one cup of tea’, <strong>in</strong> N. Mercer and J. Swann (eds)Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>: Development and Diversity. London, The Open University/Routledge.Labov, W. (i 970) ‘The study of language <strong>in</strong> <strong>its</strong> social context’, <strong>in</strong> W. Labov (1972) Sociol<strong>in</strong>guisticPatterns. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Mercer, N.M. (with contributions from Douglas Barnes) (1996) ‘<strong>English</strong> as a classroomlanguage’, <strong>in</strong> N. Mercer and J. Swann (eds) Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>English</strong>: Development and Diversity.London: The Open University/Routledge.Ochs, E. (1979) ‘Transcription as theory’, <strong>in</strong> E. Ochs and B.B. Schieffel<strong>in</strong> (eds) DevelopmentalPragmatics. London: Academic Press.Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. and Jcffcrson, G. (1974) ‘A simplest systematics for the organization ofturn-tak<strong>in</strong>g for conversation. <strong>Language</strong> 50 (4), pp. 696-735.Sebba, M. (1993) Londonjamaican: <strong>Language</strong> Systems <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction. London: Longman.Stubbs, M. (1996) Text and Corpus Analysis: Computer-assisted Studies of <strong>Language</strong> and Culture.Oxford: Blackwell.Swann, J. and Graddol, D. (1994) ‘Gender Inequalities <strong>in</strong> ClassroomTalk’, <strong>in</strong> D. Graddol., J.Mayb<strong>in</strong> and B. Stierer (eds) Research<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Language</strong> and Literacy <strong>in</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Context</strong>. Clevedon:Multil<strong>in</strong>gual MatterdThe Open UniversityWegerif, R. and Mercer, N. (1997) ‘Us<strong>in</strong>g computer-based text analysis to <strong>in</strong>tegrate qualitativeand quantitative methods <strong>in</strong> research on collaborative learn<strong>in</strong>g’, <strong>Language</strong> and Education,Vol. 11, No. 4, pp. 271-86.


Indexabsolute <strong>in</strong>novation 6 1academic competence 171-2academic register 258-70acquisition see second language acquisitionaction research 57-60, 137active exploration of language 195-6adolescent learners 40-- 1adopters 61adult learners 40-1, 174-5; genre-basedapproaches 5-6, 200-7Adult Migrant Education Program (AMEP)Literacy Project 200-7advocacy 325affective factors 24-5Affective Filter Hypothesis 159age 23, 3642agency: crcative, discursive 273, 278-82, 284Allwright, R. 287, 288, 306, 31 1, 315alternation, transcrib<strong>in</strong>g 336, 337, 338American Kernel Lessons (AKL): Intermediate 2 13. 14amplifications 248Anderson, A. 77-8Anderson, J.K. 17anti-grammar stance 148anxiety 24 5apartheid 22740; macro context of school<strong>in</strong>gfor black people 235-7Aphek, E. 173application 65, 66applied l<strong>in</strong>guistics 64 8applied science, education as 54-5appraisal 65, 66apprenticeship 1 13; <strong>in</strong>to a culture 260aptitude 24, 31-2Arens, K. 168Army Specialized Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Program (ASTP) 149Arthur, J. 249-50Asher, J. 154Aston, G. 82asymmetry 131-2attitudes: to <strong>English</strong> as a language 2 15-1 6,222-3; role <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g 24, 334audio-l<strong>in</strong>gualism 44, 149-52audio record<strong>in</strong>gs 326 7auditory discrim<strong>in</strong>ation test 39Auer, P. 117authenticity 129; us<strong>in</strong>g authentic data 194-5automaticity 80-1autonomy: learner 97, 299-300; school’srelative autonomy 209Rahns, J. 52balanced divergent factor 64Bangalore/Madras Communicational <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>Project (CTP) 634, 160Reckerman, T.M. 171behaviourist psychology 149-5 1beliefs, learner 35-6belong<strong>in</strong>g 1 18-1 9Beretta, A. 25, 50, 63-4Rerlitz, M. 149bil<strong>in</strong>gual classrooms 1 7 1-2bil<strong>in</strong>gual code-switch<strong>in</strong>g 250-2black South Africans 22740Bley-Vroman, R. 75Bloomfield, L. 149Blum, R.E. 169Bot, M. 227Bourdieu, P. 93, 272-3, 283Breen, M.P. 158, 160Rr<strong>in</strong>dley, G. 65Brumfit, C. 59-60, 68Bruner, J. 96, 254burcaucratic structures 235 7, 237-8Cameron, D. 324, 325Camilleri, A. 251Canale, M. 83, 84Candl<strong>in</strong>, C.N. 158, 160Carr, W. 54, 55


346 INDEXCazden, C. 233Chaudron, C. 31 1child learners 40-1choices, set of 193-4Chomsky,N. 14, 15, 16, 17,45, 152chorus<strong>in</strong>g 231, 232-3Clark, E. 76-7Clark, H.H. 76-7classroom: as coral gardens see classroom asculture metaphor; as discourse 125-8; asexperimental laboratory 123-5classroom context I ; motivation 34; strategiesand goals <strong>in</strong> 4 -6classroom as culture metaphor 128- 34; learn<strong>in</strong>gwith<strong>in</strong> 137-8; research<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> 135 6;review<strong>in</strong>g 134-5; teach<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> 136 7classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction see <strong>in</strong>teractionclassroom management 170classroom research 51-2, 67, 125 8Claude, M. 229-30Clifford, R. 84-5Coates, J. 334COBUILD team 15coconstruction 95-7, 1334code-switch<strong>in</strong>g 250-2cognition 94-5cognitive factors 24Cohen, A.D. 173, 174collective culture 130Coll<strong>in</strong>s, J, 237, 238, 273, 284collusion 6, 22740column transcript layouts 3324communication strategies 824; problems with847Communicational <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong> Project (CTP)634, 160communicative competence 83, 84, 155communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g (CLT) 155-8,200, 227-8community language learn<strong>in</strong>g (CLL) 153compatible habitus 274- 5, 282competence: communicative 83, 84, 155;participative, <strong>in</strong>teractional and academic171-2; and performance 14-15; strategic82 4comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put 21, 75-6, 79, 159comprehensible output 21, 79, 260-1comprehension 3, 75--89; place of <strong>in</strong> languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g 75-6; strategies 76 8computer-based analysis 340 2conceptual evaluation 65, 66concordanc<strong>in</strong>g 342, 343concurrent resources 101 -2confidentiality 325-6confirmations 247confirmatory research tradition 53-6Conrad, J. 36conservatism 132 -3constra<strong>in</strong>ts 92 4, 103consultants, researchers as 52-3contextual knowledge 78contextualisation 116- 19cont<strong>in</strong>gency 98-1 02, 102; negotiation, languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g and 100-2cont<strong>in</strong>uity 248control 98conversation analysis (CA) 116-1 7, 119conversational <strong>in</strong>ference 1 17conversational <strong>in</strong>teraction 99-100co-operative learn<strong>in</strong>g activities 34Cope, W. 200co-production 288coral gardens, classroom as 128-34; see a/.soclassroom as culture metaphorCorder, S.P. 48, 49Cornelius, E.T. 2 13corpus l<strong>in</strong>guistics 15, 340-2correction of errors 180, 298-9creativity 19; creative, discursive agency 273,27% 82, 284critical action research 59critical ethnography 2 11Critical Period Hypothesis 36, 37 9Crookes, G. 34, 58, 59, 60, 160 2cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>fluences 20Csikszentmihalyi, M. I03cued elicitations 246-7cultural capital 272, 274 -5, 282culturally-specific <strong>in</strong>teraction styles 22740;barriers to <strong>in</strong>novation and learn<strong>in</strong>g 229 34;limitations of explanations of school failure234-5culture 252~ 3, 254; classroom as see classroomas culture metaphor; and student opposition216-18, 221-2, 223-4Curran, C. 153Day, K. 313decision-driven modcl of research use 47deference politeness 228determ<strong>in</strong>ation 2 I4 16dcvclopmental sequences 18, 182dialogue 97, 254differences between learners 23~5, 299-300,301differentiation 129 30direct elicitations 246direct method 149discourse 59; classroom as 125 8; dimensions of309-16; grammar and 196 7; languageacquisition or language socialisation 4,108-2 1 ; manag<strong>in</strong>g 318; navigat<strong>in</strong>g 8,306-22; position<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> and through 113-15;what learners learn from the discourse oflessons 316-1 7; see also <strong>in</strong>teractiondiscourse skills 81


INDEX 347discursive practices 3 10, 3 1 2 14display 94-5Dob<strong>in</strong>son, T. 313-14dom<strong>in</strong>ation 208-26Driver. R. 265ecological approach see organic approacheducational perspectives 53-60Edwards, A.D. 245effective teach<strong>in</strong>g 169-76elaborations 247elicitation 246-7, 289elliptical conversation 86Ellis, R. 18, 307Ely, C. 313Emeneau, M.B. 223empirical evaluation 65, 66empiricist approaches 168empower<strong>in</strong>g research 325<strong>English</strong>: global <strong>in</strong>fluence 271 ; as medium for<strong>in</strong>struction 2 19, 236--7; number of speakers23cquality 97-8Eraut, M. 47error correction 180, 298 9ethical research 325ethnicity 1 15-16ethnographic methods 1 19; Sri Lankanclassroom study 6, 208 26evaluation of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction 8,287 ~305experiential learn<strong>in</strong>g 113, 261-2, 264-5, 267experimental laboratory, classroom as 123-5explanations 248exploration, active 195 6extroversion 32‘eye dialect’ 337, 338face-sav<strong>in</strong>g strategies 233 4Farch, C. 83Fairclough, N. 115, 310falsification of theories 50Fccz, S. 162field <strong>in</strong>dependence/depmdence 35field-notes 328, 329Firth, J.R. 14, 15focus on form 5, 180-90; <strong>in</strong> language teach<strong>in</strong>g181-3; psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic rationale 183-6;research issues 186-7Foreign Service Institute (FSI) 84-5form, focus on see focus on formform/function rclationships I95forms, focus on 5, 183-5fossilization 19-20Frazer, E. 324, 325Freidson, E. 46Freire, P. 97Fries, C. 149, 151Gardner, R.C. 23-5, 33Garf<strong>in</strong>kel, H. 135Gass, S. 53Gattengo, C. 152general model of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g13 14genre-based approaches 5-6, 162-3, 164,200~ 7Giroux, H. 209, 224-5global <strong>in</strong>fluence of <strong>English</strong> 271glosses 2 16-18glottal stop 338goals 4-6Good, T.L. 171‘good language learner’, characteristics of 28-9Goonetilleke, D.C.R.A. 210, 224grammar 5, 191-9; language <strong>in</strong> context192-3; metaphors for second languageacquisition 191-2; organic approach193-7; Tamil students’ orientation to220--1, 223~ 4grammar-translation method 44, 148grammaticality 39Greek 148Grice, H.P. 86‘ground rules’ 252 3, 255group learn<strong>in</strong>g: collective culture of theclassroom 130; small group learn<strong>in</strong>g and newacademic registers 261-2, 264-5group<strong>in</strong>g 171Guiora, A. 32-3guided construction of knowledge 254 -5Gumperz, J. 116 -18habitus 272; compatible 274 5, 282;<strong>in</strong>compatible 272, 275-8, 2834;reproduction or transformation of 2824;transform<strong>in</strong>g 278-82, 284Hall, S. 1 I6Halliday, M.A.K. 163Halsey, A.H. 234Hammond, J. 200, 202handover 96Hanson-Smith, E. 210, 224Harley, R. 76Hartshorne, K. 236Harvey, P. 324, 325Hatch, E. 48-9, 308Havelock, R. 62Hawk<strong>in</strong>s, R. 18Hew-itt, R. 116Higgs, T. 84 5Hirst, P. 57Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. 3941Hong Kong 7, 273~ 85Hopk<strong>in</strong>s, D. 59Hosenfeltl, C. 175-6Howatt, A.P.R. 148, 157


348 INDEXhumanistic methodologies 152 4Hymes, D. 126, 155hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g 80ice-breaker tasks 194identity 2 19; social 1 10ideology: language practice and 1 15-1 6; macrocontext of lcarn<strong>in</strong>g and 235-7, 237-8idiosyncracies, learners’ 23-5, 299-300, 301immersion 76, I 18; programmes 158-9<strong>in</strong>compatible habitus 272, 275-8, 283-4<strong>in</strong>complete success 19 -20<strong>in</strong>dependent construction 202<strong>in</strong>equalities, social 7, 271-86<strong>in</strong>hibition 32- 3<strong>in</strong>itiation-response-feedback (IRF) exchange94 6, 1014, 245-6, 261-3, 281<strong>in</strong>novation: conservatism of culture of theclassroom 132; culturally-specific<strong>in</strong>teractional styles as barriers to 229- 34;SLA and 60 4<strong>in</strong>put 45; comprehensible 21, 75-6,79, 159;generat<strong>in</strong>g better <strong>in</strong>put 79- 80<strong>in</strong>put-output model 102<strong>in</strong>sider/outsider problem 52-3<strong>in</strong>stitutional constra<strong>in</strong>ts and resources 92 ~4<strong>in</strong>stitutional ideologies 235-7, 237 8<strong>in</strong>strumental motivation 33<strong>in</strong>tegrative motivation 33<strong>in</strong>telligence 24, 3 1<strong>in</strong>teraction 4, 7-8; <strong>in</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual and multil<strong>in</strong>gualsett<strong>in</strong>gs 249-52; constra<strong>in</strong>ts, resources,equality and symmetry 4, 90 107; culture ofthe classroom 129; evaluation of8,287-305; importance of study of 287 8;importance of talk <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g 260-1 ;language learn<strong>in</strong>g process 2 1-2; learn<strong>in</strong>ga new register 258- 70; and pragmatics<strong>in</strong> SLA 109-1 0; record<strong>in</strong>g andtranscrib<strong>in</strong>g 8, 32344; reproduction/transformation of social worlds 271 -86;use of language as a medium 243-57;see also discourse<strong>in</strong>teractional competence 17 1-2<strong>in</strong>teractional symmetry 98<strong>in</strong>teractive model of research use 47-8International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) 338<strong>in</strong>terpretation 65, 66<strong>in</strong>terpretative research tradition 53 6<strong>in</strong>tersubjective experience 126-7, 248, 254-5<strong>in</strong>tertextual relationships 268<strong>in</strong>terviews 291<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation 97Jarvis, G. 51Jayalakshmi, G.D. 336, 338, 339, 340job applications genre 201 7Johnson, J. 39Johnson, R.K. 250 1Johnston, M. 51jo<strong>in</strong>t construction 95-7, 1334jo<strong>in</strong>t negotiation of text 202, 205-7journals 2634, 266-7Kandiah, T. 210 1 I , 224Karabel, J. 234Kasper, G. 83, 109Kellerman, E. 191-2Kemmis, S. 54, 55Kennedy, G. 61K<strong>in</strong>gbury, R. 213knowledge: guided construction of 254-5;personal 56; sources of and comprehension77 -8; technical and practical 46 8knowledge-driven model of research use 47Krashen, S. 12, 17, 61, 158-9; comprehensible<strong>in</strong>put 2 1, 75-6, 79, 159; theory 49 50Kuper, A. 234KwaZulu schools 227 40La Forge, P.G. 153laboratory: experimental 123-5; language 152Labov, W. 324Lambert, W.E. 33language: carry<strong>in</strong>g the history of classroomactivity <strong>in</strong>to <strong>its</strong> future 254-5; and context 7;cultural and cognitive tool 6-7; importantpedagogic tool 254; levels of 14; medium forteach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g 243 --57; and teach<strong>in</strong>g243G6; views on the nature of 14-1 5language acquisition see second languageacquisitionlanguage anxiety 24&5language aptitude 24, 31-2language-focus-IRF 28 1-2language laboratory 152language learn<strong>in</strong>g 2 4 , 11-27; cont<strong>in</strong>gency,negotiation and 100-2; factors affect<strong>in</strong>g2843; and language use 21-2; l<strong>in</strong>ks withsocial practice 25; process 16-22; theory and12-14; views of the language learner 22 ~5;views on the nature of language 14--15language medium policy 236; see alto medium of<strong>in</strong>structionlanguage process<strong>in</strong>g 23language socialisation see second languagesocialisationlanguage transfer 20language varictics 334-9Lat<strong>in</strong> 148layouts, transcript 331 4, 335learner-learner <strong>in</strong>teraction 91-2, 98-1 02,1034learners: active explorers of language 195-6;autonomy 97, 299 -300; characteristics of‘good languagc Irarner’ 28 9; collusion with


INDEX 349teachers 6, 22740; constructs of 307;difficulties with ‘ground rules’ for classroomlanguage use 252-3; discursive practices <strong>in</strong>the classroom 312-14; exploration ofrelationships between grammar anddiscourse 196-7; idiosyncracies 23 5,299-300, 301; as language processors 23;learn<strong>in</strong>g from discourse of lessons 316-1 7;motivation see motivation; research onlearner characteristics 3042; resistance 6,208--26, 283; as social be<strong>in</strong>gs 25; socialpractices 310, 314 ~ 16; strategies 173-6;taciturnity 23 I 2; uptake see uptake; viewsof 22-5learn<strong>in</strong>g 6-8; and acquisition 12; with<strong>in</strong>classroom as culture 137-8;culturally-specific <strong>in</strong>teractional styles asbarriers to 229-34; experiential 113,261-2, 264-5, 267; role oftalk <strong>in</strong> 260~ I;SLA theory and 306-9learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities 290-1, 292learn<strong>in</strong>g styles 35; Tamil students and 2 18,223Lee, P.L.M. 250 1Leont’ev, A.N. 308-9levels of language 14Lev<strong>in</strong>son, S. 116, 117 18Lew<strong>in</strong>, K. 57-8Lightbown, P. 49Lii, J.H. 93l<strong>in</strong>ear model of language acquisition 191 -2literacy practices 115local multiracial vernaculars 116Long, M. 50, 51, 79, 160L2‘long conversation’ 255, 267Lozanov, G. 153 4Lynch, T. 77 8MacDonald, C. 227, 236-7Maclntyre, P.D. 23~5macro factors 234-7magisterial discourse 93Mal<strong>in</strong>owski, B. 128mastery 37-8McDermott, R. 237McTear, M. 310mean<strong>in</strong>g 126; loss of 237; negotiation of21-2,79-80, 82medical practitioners 46medium for <strong>in</strong>struction: <strong>English</strong> as 219, 236-7;use of language as 243 57Mercer, N. 265, 340-2Merrow, J. 93metaphors 4; for second language acquisition191-2methodology 4-5, 147-66; audio-l<strong>in</strong>gualism149-52; communicative language teach<strong>in</strong>g155 -8; historical/pre-World War I1 148--9;humanistic methodologies 1524; immersionprogrammes and the natural approach158-9; task-based learn<strong>in</strong>g 159-62;text-based teach<strong>in</strong>g 162-3, 164; see alsofocus on formmethods 4 5, 167 79, 180~~1 ; approach<strong>in</strong>gteach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of 167-9; learner strategies173-6; nature of effective teach<strong>in</strong>g 169-76;teacher strategies 169 72Michigan Test 292m<strong>in</strong>ority ethnic groups 115--16misrecognition 272-3, 2734Mitchell, R. 59-60mode 259mode cont<strong>in</strong>uum 259-60model of second language learn<strong>in</strong>g 13-1 4modell<strong>in</strong>g 202, 203-5Modern <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Test (MLAT) 24,31 --2modularity 1 617; and second language learn<strong>in</strong>g17Moerman, M. 101Moore, A. 253Morgan, M. 168morphology 39motivation 24, 30 1 ; and attitudes 334; <strong>in</strong> theclassroom sctt<strong>in</strong>g 34; Tamil students 2 15Naiman, N. 312National Centre for <strong>English</strong> <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Teach<strong>in</strong>g</strong>and Research (NCELTR) Literacy Project200 7natural approach 61, 158-9natural method 149Natural Order Hypothesis 159nature and nurturc 16navigation of discourse 8, 306-22needs analysis 158negation 18, 182negative evidence 22negotiation 98, 100-2; jo<strong>in</strong>t negotiation of text202, 205-7; ofmean<strong>in</strong>g 21 2, 79-80, 82‘new ethnicities’ 116Newport, E. 39non-participant observation 324-5nonverbal <strong>in</strong>formation 334, 336, 337normative culture 130-1noun phrase accessibility hierarchy 185Nunan, D. 59nurture and nature 16Obler, L. 31observable data 126observation 324-5observer’s paradox 324Ogbu, J. 235O’Malley, J. 175O’Neill, R. 2 13


350 INDEXoperation 65, 66opposition, student 6, 208 26; contextualiz<strong>in</strong>g222-5oral approach 149organic approach 102, 191-3; pedagogical<strong>in</strong>plications for grammar teach<strong>in</strong>g 193 8output: comprehensible 21, 79, 260 1 ; see alsoperformance, productionoutsider/<strong>in</strong>sider problem 52 3participant observation 324-5participation 313-1 4participative competence 171 -2Passeron, J.C. 93Patkowski, M. 37-8Pavesi, M. 185-6Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 40pedagogy see teach<strong>in</strong>gPeirce, B.N. 208-10Pennycook, A. 208- IO, 285perceived <strong>in</strong>novations 6 1performance 127; competence and 14 15; useand learn<strong>in</strong>g 2 I -2personal knowledge 56personal voice 81personality 32-3Phillips, J. 175phonetic symbols 338, 339Piaget, J. 17Pica, T. 52, 100, 101Pierce, B. 1 IOPimsleur <strong>Language</strong> Aptitude Battery (PLAK)31-2plan, syllabus as 296Polanyi, M. 56position<strong>in</strong>g 1 13- 15power 34, 98Prabhu, N.S. 61, 63, 160, 161-2practical action research 58practical knowledge 46-8practice see social practicespragmatics 109-10praxis (theory of action) 51, 66~-7precision 94-5preference 228; learner preferences 35prescriptions 168private <strong>in</strong>struction 220proactive resources 101L2problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g model 62production 3, 75-89; importance of output81-4; problems with communicationstrategies 84-7; roles for output 79 8 1professional context 1proficiency test<strong>in</strong>g 30progressive approach 200pronunciation 39, 2 19prosodic cues 2 32provisional specifications 60proximal development, zone of 96, 266pure research 5 1qualitative analysis 33942quantitative analysis 33942radical resistance 224-5Raheem, R. 223Rampton, B. 25Rampton, M.H.H. 324, 325Rathunde, K. 103rationalist approach I68reactive resources 101 -2read<strong>in</strong>g 175 6reality, syllabus as 296recall, learner 313-1 4rccaps 248recitation 94~5record<strong>in</strong>g spoken language 326 7Reform Movement 148reformulations 247register 7, 258 70Reid, J. 35rejections 247relative autonomy 209relative clause formation 185-6rcpair 100-- 2rcpctitions 247report<strong>in</strong>g, teacher-guided 261, 262-3, 265-6reproduction of social worlds 7, 271-86rcsearch 3,44 -74; action research 57-60, 137;applied l<strong>in</strong>guists’ perspective 64-8;classroom as culture mctaphor 135-6, 137;classroom research 5 1-2, 67, 125S8; cultureof 53 6; educational perspcctivcs 53-60;<strong>in</strong>novationist perspective 60-4; on learnercharacteristics 30-1 ; SLA researchers’perspective 48-5 3; technical and practicalknowledge 46 8research, development and diffusion model 62researcher stance 324--6resistance: conservativc culturc of classroomand resistance to change 132; learners’ 6,208 26, 283; opposition and 224-5resources: constra<strong>in</strong>ts and 924, 103; cultureof classroom as resource 136-7retention 313-14Richards, J.C. 155Richardson, K. 324, 325Rogers, K. 61Rogers, T.S. 155role play 2 19-20rout<strong>in</strong>es 19rules: ‘ground rules’ for classroom languageuse 252-3, 255; <strong>in</strong>stitutional contra<strong>in</strong>ts92~ 4safe-talk 227-40


3INDEX 351sampl<strong>in</strong>g 323 4Sapir, E. 126, 138scaffold<strong>in</strong>g 96, 103, 254, 265, 267, 308schematic knowledge 77-8schematic structure 204-5Schlemmer, L. 227Schmidt, R. 34, 84, 109Schon, D. 56school failure 234- 5Schumann, J. 50Sebba, M. 338, 339second language acquisition (SLA) 12; appliedl<strong>in</strong>guists’ perspective 64-8 ; classroom asexperimental laboratory 123 5; <strong>in</strong> context308 9; educational perspectives 53~ 60;expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 306 8; <strong>in</strong>novationist pcrspective604; and language socialisation 4, 108-21 ;lim<strong>its</strong> to a social perspective 109-10;metaphors for 191 2; research and languagepedagogy 44-74; researchers’ perspectiveand pedagogy 48-53; sociol<strong>in</strong>guisticperspective on 1 10second language learn<strong>in</strong>g see language learn<strong>in</strong>gsecond language socialisation (SLS) 4,108-21 ; contextualisation and wider socialprocesses 1 16-19; methodologicalimplications 1 19; problems with the model113-16segregated education 235 -6; see also apartheidSeliger, H. 31 2semantic strategies 77sentence judgement 40sequenc<strong>in</strong>g 267shared experience 126-7, 248, 254-5significance 134Significant Bil<strong>in</strong>gual Instructional Features (SRIF)study 172silent way 152 ~S<strong>in</strong>clair, J. 15situational language teach<strong>in</strong>g 149, 150Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, B.F. 16, 151Slimani, A. 313, 314small-group learn<strong>in</strong>g 26 1-2, 264-5Snow, C. 3941social be<strong>in</strong>g, learner as 25social context I, 4, 122-44; classroom as coralgardens see classroom as culture metaphor;classroom as discoursc 125-8; classroom asexperimental laboratory 123-5social identity 110social <strong>in</strong>equalities 7, 271 86social <strong>in</strong>teraction model of <strong>in</strong>novation 62social practiccs: language practice and ideology1 15-- 16; navigat<strong>in</strong>g discourse <strong>in</strong> theclassroom 310, 314-16; second languagelearn<strong>in</strong>g theory and 2 5‘social troublc’ 315socialisation see second language socialisationsocio-cultural context 1-2socio-cultural perspective 254 5, 260sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic perspective 1 10socio-pragmatic failure 1 12solidarity politeness 228South Africa 22740; macro context ofschool<strong>in</strong>g for black people 235- 7specifications 60spoken language see discourse, <strong>in</strong>teractionSpolsky, R. 13-14Sri Lankan classroom 6, 208-26standard layout transcripts 331-3stave layout transcripts 334, 335Stenhouse, L. 51-2, 60Stern, H. 57Stoller, F. 64strategic competence 824strategies 4-6, 24, 75 89; classroom asexperimental laboratory 124-5;comprehension strategies 76-8; learner173-6; problems with communicationstrategies 84 7; teacher 169-72Strong, M. 312structural l<strong>in</strong>guistics 149structures, bureaucratic 235 7, 237 8structur<strong>in</strong>g 170Stubbs, M. 14, 15students see learnerssubjective experience 126~-7Suggcstopedia 153 4summaries of research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs 66Swaffar, J.K. 168Swa<strong>in</strong>,M. 76, 80, 83, 84, 312; cornprehensiblcoutput 21, 79, 260Swcct, H. 148syllabus: as reality and as plan 296symbolic violence 272-3, 273 4; <strong>in</strong>terrogat<strong>in</strong>g285symmetry 97 8synchrony 2 33syntactic process<strong>in</strong>g 80syntactic strategies 76-7systematic knowledge 78systematicity 18 19systemic-functional grammar 162 3taciturnity 228; student 231 2talk see discourse, <strong>in</strong>teractionTamil students 208- 26; midcourseresistance 2 16 20; postcoursecontradiction 220 2; precoursedeterm<strong>in</strong>ation 2 14 16Tarone, E. 48-9task-based language teach<strong>in</strong>g (TBLT) 162task based learn<strong>in</strong>g (TBL) 159-62tasks 170-1teacher education 57teacher-guided report<strong>in</strong>g 261, 262-3, 265-6


352 INDEXteacher-learner <strong>in</strong>teractions 91, 93 8, 103-4;see also <strong>in</strong>teractionteacher research (practical action research) 58teachers: collaboration with researchers 52-3;collusion with students <strong>in</strong> apartheid SouthAfrica 6, 227 40; qualifications <strong>in</strong> apartheidSouth Africa 236; as researchers 57 -60,67 8; respond<strong>in</strong>g to what learners say 247;role 1034, 177; strategies 169-72; talk andtext of language lessons 310-1 2; volubility231-2teach<strong>in</strong>g 6-8; classroom as culture metaphor and136-7; conceptions of 56; culture of 56--60;language and 243-6; methodology seemethodology; methods see methods; natureof effective teach<strong>in</strong>g 169 76; research andpedagogy 3,44-74; techniques 246 8teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g cycle 202technical action research 58technical knowledge 46 8, 50-1technology of teach<strong>in</strong>g 51Terrell, T. 61, 158 9test<strong>in</strong>g 288-9text 310, 310 12text-based (genre-based) teach<strong>in</strong>g 5-6, 162 3,164, 200-7theatrical monologue 93Thembela, A. 227theory: of language learn<strong>in</strong>g 12-14; SLAresearch and pedagogy 49-5 1, 66- 7Tikunoff, W.J. 170, 171 -2topicalisation 294 -9, 300-1total physical response (TPK) 154Towell, R. 18transcriptions of spoken language 323 44;conventions 330-1; layout 3314, 335;nonverbal and contextual <strong>in</strong>formation 334;represent<strong>in</strong>g different language varieties334-9transfer, language 20transformation 97; of social worlds 7, 271 86transform<strong>in</strong>g habitus 278--82, 284Tylbor, H. 237u-shaped behaviour 184, 191-2United States 237Universal Grammar 16, 17uptake 287 30.5; and evaluation of learn<strong>in</strong>g288-9; importance of topicalisation 294- 9;learnen’ idiosyncracies 299-300; uptakeidentification probe 290, 305; uptake recallchart 290. 304Van Dijk, T. 1 15Van Lier, L. 25, 55-6, 310 11variability 18-1 9varieties, language 334-9video record<strong>in</strong>gs 326-7volubilityh28; teacher 23 1-2Vygotsky, L. 6-7, 96, 254, 265- 6, 308wait time 266‘we’ statements 248Webbe, 148Wegerif, R. 265, 340-2Weiss, C. 47Wcnden, A. 174-5, 176Wesche, M.B. 32Westgate, D. 245Whalen, S. 103White, L. 300-1Widdowson, H. 64,654, 67 8Will<strong>in</strong>g, K. 173, 174, 176Willis, P. 21 1Wright, T. 51writ<strong>in</strong>g 176; genre-based approaches 5-6,200 7; learn<strong>in</strong>g a new register 261, 263~~4,266- 7Wundt. W.M. 125Yeadon, T. 21 3Yorio, C. 35Zahorik, J. 56Zcntella, A.C. 251-2zone of proximal development 96, 266Zulu-<strong>English</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractional stylrs 227740

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!