12.07.2015 Views

5 optimality theoretic pragmatics and the ... - Reinhard Blutner

5 optimality theoretic pragmatics and the ... - Reinhard Blutner

5 optimality theoretic pragmatics and the ... - Reinhard Blutner

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

70 <strong>pragmatics</strong>2. <strong>the</strong> distinction between explicatures <strong>and</strong> implicaturesIt is not a simple task to give a definition of <strong>the</strong> explicature/implicaturedistinction. The school of ‘radical <strong>pragmatics</strong>’ (cf. Cole 1981) does notmake <strong>the</strong> distinction, <strong>and</strong> from <strong>the</strong> perspective of a basically Griceanmechanism of pragmatic streng<strong>the</strong>ning, <strong>the</strong> distinction appears not tobe significant. As mentioned already, for RT <strong>the</strong> distinction is essentialbecause RT crucially departs from Grice in that it sees linguisticallyencoded semantic information as not sufficient for determining <strong>the</strong>propositional content of an utterance. This relates to RT’s underdeterminacy<strong>the</strong>sis, which gives <strong>the</strong> pragmatic component a much greater rolein deriving communicated assumptions. Hence, it is not surprising thatRT comes up with a new classification of pragmatically communicatedassumptions. In <strong>the</strong> words of Carston (2003):There are two types of communicated assumptions on <strong>the</strong> relevance<strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong>account: explicatures <strong>and</strong> implicatures. An ‘explicature’is a propositional form communicated by an utterance which ispragmatically constructed on <strong>the</strong> basis of <strong>the</strong> propositional schemaor template (logical form) that <strong>the</strong> utterance encodes; its contentis an amalgam of linguistically decoded material <strong>and</strong> pragmaticallyinferred material. An ‘implicature’ is any o<strong>the</strong>r propositional formcommunicated by an utterance; its content consists of whollypragmatically inferred matter (see Sperber <strong>and</strong> Wilson 1986, 182). So<strong>the</strong> explicature/implicature distinction is a derivational distinction<strong>and</strong>, by definition, it arises only for verbal (or, more generally, codebased)ostensive communication. (9)As Burton-Roberts (2005) points out, in RT, an implicature is definednegatively – as a communicated assumption that is not an explicature.This contrasts with <strong>the</strong> original definition of Sperber <strong>and</strong> Wilson (1986)who have a similar description for explicatures like Carston (2003) butsee implicatures as assumptions constructed by ‘developing assumptionschemas retrieved from encyclopaedic memory’ (Sperber <strong>and</strong> Wilson1986, 181). Whereas <strong>the</strong> early Sperber/Wilson definition contrasts <strong>the</strong>development of (underspecified) logical forms with <strong>the</strong> development ofencyclopaedic assumptions, <strong>the</strong> position of Carston (2003) is to contrast<strong>the</strong> development of a linguistically encoded logical form with o<strong>the</strong>r waysof deriving communicated assumptions. The definition of Sperber <strong>and</strong>Wilson (1986) is clearer than <strong>the</strong> definition of Carston (2003) because itcontrasts <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong>al concepts that are simpler to underst<strong>and</strong> than <strong>the</strong>14039_86991_06_chap05 70 5/2/07 13:50:26


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 73In this case <strong>the</strong> implicature (4c) arises; however, it cannot be cancelled.Van Kuppevelt (1996) argues that <strong>the</strong> ‘phenomenon of cancellation’ thatis normally discussed in connection with scalar implicatures has nothingto do with genuine cancellation; instead it has to do with contextualchange. In this sense scalar implicatures are particularized conversationalimplicatures. Obviously, <strong>the</strong> topic-dependency of scalar implicatures is notrestricted to numerals but also holds in connection with <strong>the</strong> Q-implicaturetriggered by ‘or’ (cf. van Rooy 2006). The consequence of thisfinding is that cancellability is ruled out as a criterion that distinguishesexplicatures from implicatures. Hence we are left with EIH as <strong>the</strong> crucialtest. This suggests we should take <strong>the</strong> different projection properties ofexplicatures <strong>and</strong> implicatures as defining <strong>the</strong> distinction.Since explicatures are derived by pragmatic inference as much asimplicatures, RT has to stipulate two distinct cognitive strategies/stylesin order to explain <strong>the</strong> difference between explicatures <strong>and</strong> implicatures.One strategy/style explains <strong>the</strong> cognitive mechanism of developing alogical form, <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r deals with o<strong>the</strong>r forms of deriving pragmaticassumptions. In contrast, <strong>the</strong> present view is neo-Gricean in nature <strong>and</strong>tries to explain pragmatic inferences as <strong>the</strong> product of rational behaviourbetween cooperative interlocutors. I claim that this global account makesit possible to derive <strong>the</strong> different projection behaviour of explicatures <strong>and</strong>implicatures <strong>and</strong> gives, moreover, a detailed description of <strong>the</strong> embeddingcontexts where even implicatures project with nearly universal regularity.However, as already mentioned <strong>the</strong>re is a problem with such a globalaccount because it does not apply to <strong>the</strong> actual, on-line interpretationprocess. Therefore, we propose to add a <strong>the</strong>ory of fossilization for fillingin this gap. We will argue in <strong>the</strong> following section that this approachrelates to a general <strong>the</strong>ory of cultural learning.3. <strong>pragmatics</strong> in <strong>optimality</strong> <strong>the</strong>oryIn this section I will give a concise, but informal introduction into<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong>. For a detailed discussion <strong>the</strong> reader isreferred to original literature (e.g. <strong>Blutner</strong> <strong>and</strong> Zeevat 2004; <strong>Blutner</strong> etal. 2005). Not surprisingly, <strong>the</strong> idea of optimization was present in <strong>the</strong>pragmatic enterprise from <strong>the</strong> very beginning. Much more than in o<strong>the</strong>rlinguistic fields, <strong>optimality</strong> scenarios are present in most lines of thinking:Zipf’s (1949) balancing between effect <strong>and</strong> effort, <strong>the</strong> Gricean conversationalmaxims (Grice 1975), Ducrot’s argumentative view of languageuse (Ducrot 1980), <strong>the</strong> principle of optimal relevance in relevance <strong>the</strong>ory(Sperber <strong>and</strong> Wilson 1986). Interestingly, more than one optimizationprocedure is involved in some of <strong>the</strong>se accounts. For instance, <strong>the</strong> neo-14039_86991_06_chap05 73 5/2/07 13:50:26


74 <strong>pragmatics</strong>Gricean framework assumes two countervailing optimization principlescalled Q <strong>and</strong> I principle (Atlas <strong>and</strong> Levinson 1981; Horn 1984, who writesR instead of I).Optimality Theory (OT) can be seen as a general framework thatsystematizes <strong>the</strong> use of optimization methods in linguistics. Onecomponent of OT is a list of tendencies that hold for observable propertiesof a language. These tendencies take <strong>the</strong> form of violable constraints.Because <strong>the</strong> constraints usually express very general statements, <strong>the</strong>ycan be in conflict. Conflicts among constraints are resolved because<strong>the</strong> constraints differ in strength. Minimal violations of <strong>the</strong> constraints(taking <strong>the</strong>ir strength into account) define optimal conflict resolutions.OT specifies <strong>the</strong> relation between an input <strong>and</strong> an output. This relationis mediated by two formal mechanisms, GEN <strong>and</strong> EVAL. GEN (forGenerator) creates possible output c<strong>and</strong>idates on <strong>the</strong> basis of a giveninput. EVAL (for Evaluator) uses <strong>the</strong> particular constraint ranking of <strong>the</strong>universal set of constraints (CON) to select <strong>the</strong> best c<strong>and</strong>idate for a giveninput from among <strong>the</strong> c<strong>and</strong>idate set produced by GEN. In phonology,<strong>the</strong> input to this process of optimization is an underlying linguistic representation.The output is <strong>the</strong> form as it is expressed. In syntax, <strong>the</strong> inputis an underlying logical form, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> output is <strong>the</strong> surface form as itis expressed. Hence, what is normally used in phonology <strong>and</strong> syntax isunidirectional optimization. Obviously, <strong>the</strong> point of view of <strong>the</strong> speakeris taken. This contrasts with OT semantics where <strong>the</strong> view of <strong>the</strong> heareris taken (Hendriks <strong>and</strong> de Hoop 2001; Hoop <strong>and</strong> de Swart 2000).Bidirectional optimization integrates <strong>the</strong> speaker’s <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> hearer’sperspective into a simultaneous optimization procedure. In <strong>pragmatics</strong>,this bidirectional view is motivated by a reduction of Grice’s maxims ofconversation to two principles: <strong>the</strong> I/R-principle, which can be seen as <strong>the</strong>force of unification minimizing <strong>the</strong> Speaker’s effort, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Q-principle,which can be seen as <strong>the</strong> force of diversification minimizing <strong>the</strong> Auditor’seffort. The Q-principle corresponds to <strong>the</strong> first part of Grice’s quantitymaxim (make your contribution as informative as required), while it can beargued that <strong>the</strong> countervailing I/R-principle collects <strong>the</strong> second part of<strong>the</strong> quantity maxim (do not make your contribution more informative than isrequired), <strong>the</strong> maxim of relation <strong>and</strong> possibly all <strong>the</strong> manner maxims. Ina slightly different formulation, <strong>the</strong> I/R-principle seeks to select <strong>the</strong> mostcoherent interpretation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Q-principle acts as a blocking mechanismwhich blocks all <strong>the</strong> outputs which can be grasped more economicallyby an alternative linguistic input (<strong>Blutner</strong> 1998). This formulationmakes it quite clear that <strong>the</strong> Gricean framework can be conceived of asa bidirectional <strong>optimality</strong> framework which integrates <strong>the</strong> speaker <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> hearer perspective. Whereas <strong>the</strong> I/R-principle compares different14039_86991_06_chap05 74 5/2/07 13:50:26


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 75possible interpretations for <strong>the</strong> same syntactic expression, <strong>the</strong> Q-principlecompares different possible syntactic expressions that <strong>the</strong> speaker couldhave used to communicate <strong>the</strong> same meaning.In <strong>the</strong> so-called strong version of bidirectional OT, a form-interpretationpair is called (strongly) optimal iff (I/R) no o<strong>the</strong>r pair is generated that satisfies <strong>the</strong> constraints better than. I will give a very schematic example in order to illustrate somecharacteristics of <strong>the</strong> bidirectional OT. Assume that we have two forms f 1<strong>and</strong> f 2which are semantically equivalent. This means that GEN associates<strong>the</strong> same set of interpretations with <strong>the</strong>m, say {m 1, m 2}. We stipulatethat <strong>the</strong> form f 1is less complex (marked) than <strong>the</strong> form f 2<strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong>interpretation m 1is less complex (marked) than <strong>the</strong> interpretation m 2.This is expressed by two markedness constraints F <strong>and</strong> M for forms <strong>and</strong>interpretations, respectively – F prefers f 1over f 2<strong>and</strong> M prefers m 1overm 2. This is indicated in Example (5).(5)< f 1, m1 >FM< f 2,m 1> *< f 1,m 2>< f 2,m 2>** *From <strong>the</strong>se differences of markedness <strong>the</strong> following ordering relationbetween form-meaning pairs can be derived as shown in (6). I’m usinga graphical notation to indicate <strong>the</strong> preferences by arrows in a twodimensionaldiagram. Such diagrams give an intuitive visualization for<strong>the</strong> optimal pairs of (strong) bidirectional OT: <strong>the</strong>y are simply <strong>the</strong> meetingpoints of horizontal <strong>and</strong> vertical arrows. The optimal pairs are markedwith <strong>the</strong> symbol A in <strong>the</strong> diagram.(6)f 1Af 2m 1m 2The scenario just mentioned describes <strong>the</strong> case of total blocking wheresome forms (e.g. *furiosity, *fallacity) do not exist because o<strong>the</strong>rs do14039_86991_06_chap05 75 5/2/07 13:50:27


76 <strong>pragmatics</strong>(fury, fallacy). However, blocking is not always total but may be partial.This means that not all <strong>the</strong> interpretations of a form must be blockedif ano<strong>the</strong>r form exists. McCawley (1978) collects a number of fur<strong>the</strong>rexamples demonstrating <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of partial blocking. Forexample, he observes that <strong>the</strong> distribution of productive causatives (inEnglish, Japanese, German, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r languages) is restricted by <strong>the</strong>existence of a corresponding lexical causative. Whereas lexical causatives(e.g. (7a)) tend to be restricted in <strong>the</strong>ir distribution to <strong>the</strong> stereotypicalcausative situation (direct, unmediated causation through physicalaction), productive (periphrastic) causatives tend to pick up more markedsituations of mediated, indirect causation. For example, (7b) could havebeen used appropriately when Black Bart caused <strong>the</strong> sheriff’s gun tobackfire by stuffing it with cotton.(7) a. Black Bart killed <strong>the</strong> sheriff.b. Black Bart caused <strong>the</strong> sheriff to die.To make things concrete we can take f 1to be <strong>the</strong> lexical causative form(7a), f 2<strong>the</strong> periphrastic form (7b), m 1direct (stereotypical) causation <strong>and</strong>m 2indirect causation.Typical cases of total <strong>and</strong> partial blocking are found in morphology,syntax <strong>and</strong> semantics. The general tendency of partial blocking seemsto be that ‘unmarked forms tend to be used for unmarked situations <strong>and</strong>marked forms for marked situations’ (Horn 1984, 26) – a tendency thatHorn (22) terms ‘<strong>the</strong> division of pragmatic labour’.There are two principal possibilities to avoid total blocking within <strong>the</strong>bidirectional OT framework. The first possibility is to formulate so-calledbias constraints (Mattausch 2004) <strong>and</strong> to find <strong>the</strong> appropriate rankingof <strong>the</strong> constraints such that partial blocking comes out. The table in (8)formulates four bias constraints besides <strong>the</strong> two markedness constraints F<strong>and</strong> M: <strong>the</strong> bias constraint F→M says that simple (unmarked) forms expresssimple interpretations (Levinson’s I-constraint), <strong>the</strong> constraint *F→*Msays that complex forms express complex interpretations (Levinson’s(2000) M-constraint), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> two remaining bias constraints express<strong>the</strong> opposite restrictions.(8)F M F→M *F→*M F→*M F*→M * * * * * * * *14039_86991_06_chap05 76 5/2/07 13:50:27


78 <strong>pragmatics</strong>To stress this point again: to get this solution we only have to assume <strong>the</strong>markedness constraints F <strong>and</strong> M (alternately, we can assume that all <strong>the</strong>markedness constraints are higher ranked than <strong>the</strong> bias constraints).The diagrams (9a) <strong>and</strong> (9b) describe <strong>the</strong> same set of solution pairs but<strong>the</strong> calculation of <strong>the</strong> solutions is completely different in <strong>the</strong> two cases.In <strong>the</strong> case of (9a) unidirectional optimization (ei<strong>the</strong>r hearer or speakerperspective) is sufficient to calculate <strong>the</strong> solution pairs. I think this kindof OT system can be used to construct cognitively realistic models of online,incremental interpretation (Fanselow et al., 1999). But what about<strong>the</strong> status of weak bidirection (super-<strong>optimality</strong>) which is illustrated in(9b)? There are implementations of a recursive algorithm to calculate <strong>the</strong>super-optimal solutions (see various contributions in <strong>Blutner</strong> <strong>and</strong> Zeevat2004)). Unfortunately, such a procedure doesn’t even fit <strong>the</strong> simplestrequirements of a psychologically realistic model of on-line, incrementalinterpretation because of its strictly non-local nature (Beaver <strong>and</strong> Lee2004; Zeevat, 2000).Ano<strong>the</strong>r problem is conceptual in nature <strong>and</strong> is raised by <strong>the</strong> existence oftwo notions of bidirectionality. What is <strong>the</strong> proper notion of optimizationin natural language processing? The puzzle can be solved by relatingweak bidirection to an off-line mechanism that is based on bidirectionallearning. Benz (2003) worked out <strong>the</strong> formal details of such a <strong>the</strong>ory. His<strong>the</strong>ory is based on <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong> speaker <strong>and</strong> hearer coordinate onform-meaning pairs which are most preferred from both perspectives. This<strong>the</strong>ory predicts partial blocking as <strong>the</strong> result of an associative learningprocess where speaker <strong>and</strong> hearer preferences are coordinated. O<strong>the</strong>rapproaches ground weak bidirection likewise in repeated processes ofbidirectional learning (cf. van Rooy 2004; Jäger 2004). It is remarkablethat in this research <strong>the</strong> solution concept of weak bidirection is consideredas a principle describing <strong>the</strong> direction of language change: super-optimalpairs are tentatively realized in language change. This relates to <strong>the</strong> viewof Horn (1984) who considers <strong>the</strong> Q-principle <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> I/R-principle asdiametrically opposed forces in inference strategies of language change.The basic idea goes back to Zipf (1949), <strong>and</strong> was reconsidered in van Rooy(2004) <strong>and</strong> <strong>Blutner</strong> et al (2002). It conforms to <strong>the</strong> idea that synchronicstructure is significantly informed by diachronic forces. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, it respectsZeevat’s (2000) acute criticism against super-<strong>optimality</strong> as describing anon-line mechanism.For <strong>the</strong> sake of illustration, let’s go back to <strong>the</strong> illustration in (9). Let’sassume a population of agents who realize speaker <strong>and</strong> hearer strategiesbased exclusively on <strong>the</strong> markedness constraints F <strong>and</strong> M. That is, in thispopulation each content is expressed in <strong>the</strong> simplest way (f 1) <strong>and</strong> each14039_86991_06_chap05 78 5/2/07 13:50:27


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 79expression is understood in <strong>the</strong> simplest way (m 1). Let’s assume fur<strong>the</strong>rthat <strong>the</strong>se agents communicate with each o<strong>the</strong>r. When agent x is in <strong>the</strong>speaker role <strong>and</strong> intends to express m 1, <strong>the</strong>n expressive optimizationyields f 1. Agent y is a hearer who receives f 1<strong>and</strong>, according to interpretiveoptimization, he gets <strong>the</strong> interpretation m 1– hence <strong>the</strong> hearer underst<strong>and</strong>swhat <strong>the</strong> speaker intends: successful communication. Now assume <strong>the</strong>speaker wants to express m 2. With <strong>the</strong> same logic of optimization he willproduce f 1<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> agent y interprets it as m 1. In this case, obviously, <strong>the</strong>communication is not successful. Now assume some kind of adaptationei<strong>the</strong>r by iterated learning or by some mutations of <strong>the</strong> ranked constraintsystem (including <strong>the</strong> bias constraints). According to this adaptationmechanism <strong>the</strong> expected ‘utility’ (how well <strong>the</strong>y underst<strong>and</strong> each o<strong>the</strong>rin <strong>the</strong> statistical mean) is improving in time. In that way a system that isevolving in time can be described including its special attractor dynamics.In each case <strong>the</strong>re is a stabilizing final state that corresponds to <strong>the</strong> system(9a) where <strong>the</strong> two Levinsonian constraints I <strong>and</strong> M outrank <strong>the</strong> rest of<strong>the</strong> constraints. It is precisely this system that reflects Horn’s divisionof pragmatic labour. The only condition we have to assume is that <strong>the</strong>marked contents are less frequently expressed in <strong>the</strong> population than<strong>the</strong> unmarked contents. 4Hence, <strong>the</strong> important insight is that a system that is exclusively basedon markedness constraints such as (9b) is evolutionarily related to asystem based on highly ranked bias constraints such as (9a). We will use<strong>the</strong> term fossilization for describing <strong>the</strong> relevant transfer. The mentionedapproaches of grounding weak bidirection in repeated processes ofbidirectional learning can be seen as concrete realizations of fossilization.Recently, Mattausch (2004) has implemented <strong>the</strong> idea of fossilizationusing stochastic OT. In that way he was able to explain <strong>the</strong> evolution ofreflexive marking strategies in English <strong>and</strong> he was able to show how anoptional <strong>and</strong> infrequent marking strategy like that of Old English couldevolve into a pattern of obligatory structural marking like that attestedin modern English.4. embedded implicaturesThere are two views of analyzing embedding implicatures. FollowingChierchia (2004) I will call <strong>the</strong>m <strong>the</strong> global <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> local view, respectively.According to <strong>the</strong> global view one first computes <strong>the</strong> (plain) meaning ofa complete sentence; <strong>the</strong>n, taking into account <strong>the</strong> relevant alternatives,one streng<strong>the</strong>ns that meaning by adding in all <strong>the</strong> implicatures. Thiscontrasts with <strong>the</strong> local view which first introduces pragmatic assumptions14039_86991_06_chap05 79 5/2/07 13:50:27


80 <strong>pragmatics</strong>locally <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n projects <strong>the</strong>m upwards in a strictly compositional waywhere certain filter conditions apply. Representatives of <strong>the</strong> global vieware Gazdar (1979), Soames (1982), <strong>Blutner</strong> (1998), Sauerl<strong>and</strong> (2004),<strong>and</strong> Russell (2006); <strong>the</strong> local view is taken by Chierchia (2004), vanRooy ( 2006), Levinson (2000), <strong>and</strong> RT. Whereas many globalistsargue against <strong>the</strong> local view <strong>and</strong> many localists against <strong>the</strong> global view, Ithink that proper variants of both views are justified if a different statusis assigned to <strong>the</strong> two views: global <strong>the</strong>ories provide <strong>the</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards ofrational discourse <strong>and</strong> correspond to a diachronic, evolutionary setting;local <strong>the</strong>ories account for <strong>the</strong> shape of actual, on-line (synchronic)processing including <strong>the</strong> features of incremental interpretation. Mysuggestion is to take <strong>the</strong> proposal of fossilization as a mediator between<strong>the</strong> two views. However, at <strong>the</strong> moment this is not much more thanan idea since concrete implementations of fossilization have appliedfor very simple examples only, predominantly in <strong>the</strong> domain of lexical<strong>pragmatics</strong>.In <strong>the</strong> last section I introduced an OT implementation of <strong>the</strong> neo-Gricean view. I will illustrate now how this global <strong>the</strong>ory can account for<strong>the</strong> basic distinction between explicatures <strong>and</strong> implicatures. I will explainthis by considering <strong>the</strong> projection behaviour of pragmatic inferencesin complex sentences. What I will demonstrate is that explicaturesregularly satisfy EIH whereas implicatures systematically violate EIH(predominantly in downward entailing contexts). Interestingly, <strong>the</strong> global<strong>the</strong>ory does more than merely predict <strong>the</strong> basic distinction. Especiallyfor scalar implicatures, it precisely predicts <strong>the</strong> projection behaviour’sdependence on <strong>the</strong> surrounding context.In <strong>Blutner</strong> (1998) I have proposed an approach to ‘scalar implicatures’that has some advantages over <strong>the</strong> traditional approach based on Hornscales(cf. Gazdar 1979). For example, it solves a famous puzzle given byJohn McCawley. In <strong>the</strong> exercise part of his logic book McCawley (1993,324) points out that <strong>the</strong> derivation of <strong>the</strong> exclusive interpretation bymeans of Horn-scales breaks down as soon as we consider disjunctionshaving more than two arguments. For example, from a disjunctivesentence of <strong>the</strong> form John or Paul or Ede is sick we can conclude thatonly one of <strong>the</strong> three is sick. However, <strong>the</strong> traditional approach predictsthat not all <strong>the</strong> disjuncts can be true, which is too weak. The solutionwas to admit a whole lattice of alternative expressions constructed by<strong>the</strong> AND operator in order to block all interpretations with more <strong>the</strong>none individual sick.The global solution also works in cases like (10a) where <strong>the</strong> implicaturesare (10b) <strong>and</strong> (10c):14039_86991_06_chap05 80 5/2/07 13:50:28


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 81(10) a. Someone is sick.b. The speaker does not know who is sick.c. The speaker knows (exactly) one individual is sick (given a setof individuals).Blocking sentences are of <strong>the</strong> form i is sick in this case, where i is <strong>the</strong>name of an individual. As van Rooy points out a Gazdarian analysis ofscalar implicatures predicts that i is not sick, for any individual i. Andthis results in an inconsistency because we can draw that conclusion foreach individual i <strong>and</strong> thus conclude that no one is sick. To resolve thisproblem Soames (1982) has proposed an alternative account based on acareful consideration of <strong>the</strong> quality maxim. He concludes that we shouldweaken <strong>the</strong> force with which <strong>the</strong> implicatures are generated. Instead ofclaiming that <strong>the</strong> speaker knows that <strong>the</strong> stronger proposition i is sick isnot <strong>the</strong> case we should conclude only that <strong>the</strong> speaker does not knowwhe<strong>the</strong>r i is sick is <strong>the</strong> case. In that way we get <strong>the</strong> inference (10b). Inorder to get <strong>the</strong> implicature (10c) we can assume a neg-raising accountof propositional attitudes (e.g. Horn 1989). In <strong>the</strong> present context thisconforms to an I-inference of <strong>the</strong> following kind:(11) ¬Kϕ ~> K¬ϕ, for any proposition ϕ [K is a belief operator]Assuming this inference conforms to a default that is realized as longas no conflicts arise, we can derive <strong>the</strong> expected conclusion (10c). For arelated proposal, see Sauerl<strong>and</strong> (2004) <strong>and</strong> Russell (2006).Now we are ready to analyze <strong>the</strong> projection behaviour of scalarimplicatures in complex sentences. Compare <strong>the</strong> simple sentence (12a)<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> complex sentence (12b):(12) a. Mary lives somewhere in <strong>the</strong> south of France.b. If Mary lives somewhere in <strong>the</strong> south of France, <strong>the</strong>n I do notknow where.c. Speaker does not know where in <strong>the</strong> south of France Maryresides.Obviously, uttering (12a) implicates <strong>the</strong> proposition (12c). The derivationof this implicature is analogous to <strong>the</strong> derivation of (10b). However, <strong>the</strong>implicature does not locally arise in <strong>the</strong> antecedent of a conditional suchas in (12b). Were it to arise, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> whole sentence (12b) would be atautology, but it is not. Carston (2002: 194) uses this example to showthat implicatures can violate EIH. The explanation in our neo-Gricean14039_86991_06_chap05 81 5/2/07 13:50:28


82 <strong>pragmatics</strong>framework is straightforward: <strong>the</strong> expression alternates to (12b) have tobe logically stronger than (12b) itself. Because in (12b) <strong>the</strong> weak quantifiersomewhere in <strong>the</strong> south of France occurs in <strong>the</strong> antecedent of a conditional(i.e. in a downward entailing context), replacing it by concrete locationsresults in a weaker expression. Since a weaker expression is not allowed asan expression alternative <strong>the</strong> implicature does not arise. We can concludethat scalar implicatures are indeed implicatures in <strong>the</strong> sense of RT ra<strong>the</strong>rthan explicatures.Ano<strong>the</strong>r example confirms this conclusion:(13) a. I believe that some students are waiting for me ~> I believe thatsome but not all students are waiting for me.b. I doubt that some students are waiting for me ~> I doubt thatsome but not all students are waiting for me.c. ?Possibly all students are waiting for me. Hence, I doubt thatsome students are waiting for me.d. I doubt that some students are waiting for me ~> I believe thatno students are waiting for me.In <strong>the</strong> upward entailing context (13a) <strong>the</strong> scalar implicature is realized asexpected by <strong>the</strong> global account. In <strong>the</strong> downward entailing context (13b),however, <strong>the</strong> implicature does not project locally. O<strong>the</strong>rwise a discoursesuch as (13c) should be fine, but it is not. Ra<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> implicature is asindicated in (13d), which relates to <strong>the</strong> logical negation of <strong>the</strong> quantifier.For a formal derivation <strong>the</strong> reader is referred to Russell (2006). It is essentialthat no extra stipulation is required besides those mentioned.Chierchia discussed many o<strong>the</strong>r examples with scalar implicatures<strong>and</strong> concluded that only a local view can account for <strong>the</strong> observedphenomena. However, as Sauerl<strong>and</strong> (2004) <strong>and</strong> Russell (2006) haveshown, a Gricean <strong>the</strong>ory can also account for each of <strong>the</strong> implicaturesChierchia identified. Hence, I conclude that a global account is possiblefor <strong>the</strong> treatment of scalar implicatures. The important question thatarises now is whe<strong>the</strong>r a global account can also explain all <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rpragmatic inferences that are discussed in <strong>the</strong> literature. In <strong>the</strong> rest ofthis chapter I will show how this account can explain examples typicallytreated as explicatures in <strong>the</strong> RT literature.(14) a. John had a drink ~> John had an alcoholic drink.b. I believe that John had a drink ~> I believe that John had analcoholic drink.c. I doubt that John had a drink ~> I doubt that John had analcoholic drink.14039_86991_06_chap05 82 5/2/07 13:50:28


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 83This example shows that <strong>the</strong> inference of free enrichment in (14a) satisfiesEIH both in upward entailing contexts (14b) <strong>and</strong> in downward entailingcontexts (14c). Hence, it is an explicature in <strong>the</strong> sense of RT. The sameprojection behaviour is visible in <strong>the</strong> following examples when checking<strong>the</strong> pragmatic inferences given in paren<strong>the</strong>ses:Domain restrictions(15) a. Everyone left early (~> everyone at <strong>the</strong> party left early).b. Ei<strong>the</strong>r everyone left early or <strong>the</strong> ones who stayed on are in <strong>the</strong>garden.Meronomic restrictions(16) a. This apple is red (~> <strong>the</strong> outside of <strong>the</strong> apple is red).b. I doubt that <strong>the</strong> apple is red.Reciprocals <strong>and</strong> plural predication(17) a. The girls saw each o<strong>the</strong>r (~> every girl saw every o<strong>the</strong>r girl).b. I doubt that <strong>the</strong> girls saw each o<strong>the</strong>r. No girl sees girl No. 5.(18) a. The cats see <strong>the</strong> dogs (~> every cat sees every dog).b. I doubt that <strong>the</strong> cats see <strong>the</strong> dogs. No cat sees dog No. 3.(19) a. The cats are sitting in <strong>the</strong> baskets (~> every cat is sitting in oneof <strong>the</strong> baskets).b. I doubt that <strong>the</strong> cats are sitting in <strong>the</strong> baskets. Cat No. 1 is notsitting in any basket.A first observation is that all <strong>the</strong>se examples are based on I/R-implicaturesaccording to <strong>the</strong> neo-Gricean classification. Hence, in order to give anexplanation of <strong>the</strong> projection properties it is essential to have a propermeasure of relevance. Van Rooy (2 006) lists some c<strong>and</strong>idatedefinitions found in <strong>the</strong> linguistic, philosophical <strong>and</strong> statistical literature.For goal-oriented <strong>the</strong>ories of relevance, but also for <strong>the</strong> entropy-basedversion, it is essential that <strong>the</strong> value of relevance can be positive <strong>and</strong>negative. The maxim of optimal relevance <strong>the</strong>n means maximizing <strong>the</strong>absolute amount of relevance.Building on Merin (1997), van Rooy (2 006) identifies twocrucial conditions for a local <strong>the</strong>ory of relevance, i.e. a <strong>the</strong>ory of relevance14039_86991_06_chap05 83 5/2/07 13:50:28


84 <strong>pragmatics</strong>that conforms to a compositional, linear mode of calculating <strong>the</strong> valueof relevance for complex sentences:(20) a. Rel(A&B) = Rel(A) + Rel(B) if propositions A <strong>and</strong> B are independentb. Rel(A) = –Rel(¬A)Using such a local <strong>the</strong>ory of relevance (which hopefully can be extendedto o<strong>the</strong>r complex forms than those constructed by negation <strong>and</strong>conjunction), <strong>the</strong> neo-Gricean approach can explain that <strong>the</strong> givenexamples exhibit explicatures, that is, <strong>the</strong>y satisfy EIH.The explanation runs as follows. First, notice that unidirectionaloptimization (hearer perspective) is sufficient in <strong>the</strong> present case becauseblocking does not take place in <strong>the</strong> examples under discussion. Fur<strong>the</strong>r,let’s assume that GEN generates all possible enrichments of <strong>the</strong> logicalform of <strong>the</strong> (complex) sentence; my favourite realization for such amechanism of ‘developing logical forms’ is abduction (cf. <strong>Blutner</strong> 1998).Finally, we assume that <strong>the</strong> evaluation component maximizes <strong>the</strong> amountof relevance (in absolute terms).Assume an enrichment m of LF. By using a local enrichment mechanismit results that ¬m is an enrichment of ¬LF. Assuming <strong>the</strong> condition (20b),Rel(m) = -Rel(¬m), yields <strong>the</strong> following conclusion: if m is an optimalenrichment of LF <strong>the</strong>n ¬m is an optimal enrichment of ¬LF. Hence, itcan be concluded that EIH is inherited by negation, i.e. if a structure Ssatisfies EIH, <strong>the</strong>n also ¬S satisfies it. Since believe <strong>and</strong> doubt are relatedby negation, it is obvious that in <strong>the</strong>se contexts <strong>the</strong> I/R inferences qualifyas explicatures. Assuming that Merin’s linear <strong>pragmatics</strong> can be extendedto o<strong>the</strong>r complex sentences, <strong>the</strong>n it generally holds that all pragmaticinferences that count as I/R implicatures (neo-Gricean terminology) areexplicatures in <strong>the</strong> terminology of RT. I suggest this claim is empiricallysupported.5. consequences <strong>and</strong> conclusionsThis chapter contains some speculation about <strong>the</strong> possibility of deriving<strong>the</strong> explicature/implicature distinction within a neo-Gricean frameworkof <strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong>. The speculations rest on severalassumptions that I will list here once more:(a) The nature of <strong>the</strong> distinction has to do with <strong>the</strong> embedded implicaturehypo<strong>the</strong>sis EIH: explicatures regularly satisfy EIH, implicaturesregularly violate EIH in a definite class of contexts.14039_86991_06_chap05 84 5/2/07 13:50:28


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 85(b) A neo-Gricean <strong>the</strong>ory of scalar implicatures based on a globalblocking mechanism.(c) Soames’ reconsideration of <strong>the</strong> epistemic status of scalar implicaturespaired with a default mechanism of neg-raising.(d) A local <strong>the</strong>ory of relevance.None of <strong>the</strong>se assumptions has a stipulative character. With <strong>the</strong> exceptionof <strong>the</strong> first assumption <strong>the</strong>y are all motivated by independent evidencethat has nothing to do with <strong>the</strong> explicature/implicature distinction.However, while claiming that a global <strong>the</strong>ory can explain <strong>the</strong> distinction,it is essential to state that a global <strong>the</strong>ory cannot count as an on-linemechanism, since it doesn’t conform to <strong>the</strong> principles of incrementalinterpretation. Ra<strong>the</strong>r, a global account describes <strong>the</strong> general forces thatdirect communication. It has a diachronic dimension. In order to geta synchronic system which describes <strong>the</strong> actual pragmatic inferences,<strong>the</strong> idea of fossilization has been proposed (see section 3). A <strong>the</strong>ory offossilization can be seen as describing how pragmatic inferences becomeautomatized <strong>and</strong> form part of an efficient cognitive system that makesfast on-line processing possible. In this way, <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory conforms to amemory/instance <strong>the</strong>ory of automatization (cf. Logan 1988).From <strong>the</strong> perspective of cultural evolution <strong>the</strong> presumption offossilization relates to a <strong>the</strong>ory that realizes Dawkins’ (1983) idea ofmemic selection. This idea conforms to <strong>the</strong> ‘universal Darwinist’ claimthat <strong>the</strong> methodology of evolutionary <strong>the</strong>ory is applicable whenever anydynamical system exhibits (r<strong>and</strong>om) variation, selection among variants,<strong>and</strong> thus differential inheritance. Related proposals are Steels’ recruitment<strong>the</strong>ory of cultural evolution (e.g. Steels 1998) <strong>and</strong> Kirby’s paradigm ofiterated learning (e.g. Kirby 2000).OT is a system of knowledge representation that invites <strong>the</strong>development of <strong>the</strong> evolutionary perspective because <strong>the</strong> manipulationof <strong>the</strong> different rankings of a given system of constraints is a powerfulbut computationally simple task. It has been applied in <strong>the</strong> area of lexical<strong>pragmatics</strong>, especially in order to explain <strong>the</strong> phenomenon of broadening<strong>and</strong> streng<strong>the</strong>ning in connection with <strong>the</strong> prepositions om <strong>and</strong> rond inDutch (Zwarts 2006).In this chapter I have proposed applying <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory to phenomenaoutside <strong>the</strong> realm of lexical <strong>pragmatics</strong>. Though real simulation resultsare missing at <strong>the</strong> moment, <strong>the</strong>re are some psychological implications of<strong>the</strong> new perspective of fossilization. Recent data presented by Noveck’sexperimental <strong>pragmatics</strong> group (cf. Noveck 2005) suggests that childrenare sometimes more logical than adults. In one of <strong>the</strong>ir experiments14039_86991_06_chap05 85 5/2/07 13:50:28


86 <strong>pragmatics</strong><strong>the</strong>y presented children <strong>and</strong> adults with sentences such as (21) wherea relatively weak term is used in scenarios where a stronger term isjustified.(21) Some elephants have trunks.Surprisingly, younger children are typically more likely than adults tofind <strong>the</strong> utterance acceptable. One way of interpreting this data is toassume that children are pragmatically delayed at young ages. From <strong>the</strong>fossilization perspective, it can be claimed that scalar inferences becomeautomatic with age <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong> experimental results are simply revealinghow such inference-making matures. This contrasts with <strong>the</strong> suggestionmade in RT ‘that children <strong>and</strong> adults use <strong>the</strong> same comprehensionmechanisms but that greater cognitive resources are available for adults,which in turn encourages <strong>the</strong>m to draw out more pragmatic inferences’(Noveck 2005). 5With regard to binding phenomena many researchers found a delayedprinciple B effect in comprehension but not in production. Childrencorrectly interpret reflexives like adults from <strong>the</strong> age of 3;0 but <strong>the</strong>ycontinue to perform poorly on <strong>the</strong> interpretation of pronouns even upto <strong>the</strong> age of 6;6. This contrasts with production data where even <strong>the</strong>youngest children use <strong>the</strong> pronoun to express a disjoint meaning while<strong>the</strong>y use <strong>the</strong> reflexive to express a coreferential interpretation. This ispuzzling because, usually, comprehension of a given form precedesproduction of this form. 6 In unpublished work it has been shown thatfossilization <strong>the</strong>ory can explain <strong>the</strong>se data without fur<strong>the</strong>r stipulation.The pragmatic inferences in this case are explicatures in <strong>the</strong> sense of RT,<strong>and</strong> it would be interesting to see how RT can account for <strong>the</strong>se effects.The present examination sees global <strong>and</strong> local accounts of analyzingembedding implicatures as complementary. (And <strong>the</strong> same holds for global<strong>and</strong> local accounts of lexical <strong>pragmatics</strong>.) Hence, peaceful coexistence<strong>and</strong> even collaboration between globalists <strong>and</strong> localists is possible <strong>and</strong>desirable.acknowledgmentsI am grateful to Jason Mattausch, Robert van Rooy, Torgrim Solstad <strong>and</strong>Henk Zeevat for valuable suggestions <strong>and</strong> comments on earlier versionsof this chapter. Special thanks to Noel Burton-Roberts for valuablecomments, advice <strong>and</strong> painstaking editorial help.14039_86991_06_chap05 86 5/2/07 13:50:28


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 87notes1. I use this term collectively for implicatures <strong>and</strong> explicatures.2. Recanati (1993) rejects <strong>the</strong> test because of metalinguistic negation. Indeed,examples like ‘I am not his daughter; he is my fa<strong>the</strong>r’ suggest that sometimesome property o<strong>the</strong>r than propositional content is falling within <strong>the</strong> scope ofnegation or o<strong>the</strong>r logical operators. However, this does not really undermine<strong>the</strong> usefulness of <strong>the</strong> test because of intonational <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r cues that indicate<strong>the</strong> metalinguistic use.3. Ano<strong>the</strong>r example is due to a classic paper by Sadock (1978) <strong>and</strong> discussedin <strong>Blutner</strong> (1998): ‘Grice states explicitly that generalized conversationalimplicatures, those that have little to do with context, are cancellable. But isit not possible that some conversational implicatures are so little dependenton context that cancellation of <strong>the</strong>m will result in something approachinginvariable infelicity? In a paper in preparation, I argue that sentences of<strong>the</strong> form almost P only conversationally entail not P, contrary to <strong>the</strong> claimmade by Karttunen <strong>and</strong> Peters (1979). The implicature is straightforwardlycalculable <strong>and</strong> highly nondetachable but, unfortunately for my <strong>the</strong>sis, justabout uncancellable. The sentence Gertrude not only almost swam <strong>the</strong> EnglishChannel, in fact she swam it is, I admit, pretty strange’ (Sadock 1978, 293).4. There are examples where this condition is violated. For example, Zwarts (2006)considered <strong>the</strong> case of om <strong>and</strong> rond in Dutch where <strong>the</strong> unmarked term omrefers to some streng<strong>the</strong>ning of <strong>the</strong> frequent de tour interpretation, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>term rond refers to some weakening of <strong>the</strong> less frequent circle interpretation.The principle of strongest interpretation – taken as a markedness principle– however predicts <strong>the</strong> opposite pattern.5. Noveck’s data seem to confirm RT, especially <strong>the</strong> data that show a linkbetween scalar-inference production <strong>and</strong> task complexity. However, <strong>the</strong>present fossilization view does not necessarily conflict with <strong>the</strong>se findingssince memory-based automatization does not mean that <strong>the</strong> task complexitycannot have any influence (cf. <strong>Blutner</strong> <strong>and</strong> Sommer 1988). Also <strong>the</strong> idea thatcontext determines in many cases which pragmatic enrichment to make doesnot necessarily conflict with <strong>the</strong> fossilization view. This view assumes thatfossilized pragmatic implicatures can be context-dependent but <strong>the</strong>y are notcancellable. This contrasts with Levinson’s (2000) view, which clearly is refutedby <strong>the</strong>se data.6. For a summary of <strong>the</strong> data <strong>and</strong> an interesting <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong>al proposal cf. Hendriks<strong>and</strong> Spenader (2004).referencesAtlas, J.D. <strong>and</strong> S.C. Levinson (1981). ‘It-clefts, informativeness <strong>and</strong> logical form’.In P. Cole (ed.). Radical Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press. 1–61.Beaver, D. <strong>and</strong> H. Lee (2004). ‘Input-output mismatches in OT’. In R. <strong>Blutner</strong><strong>and</strong> H. Zeevat (eds). Optimality Theory <strong>and</strong> Pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave/Macmillan.Benz, A. (2003). ‘Partial Blocking, associative learning, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> principle of weak<strong>optimality</strong>’. In J. Spenader, A. Eriksson, <strong>and</strong> Ö. Dahl (eds.). Proceedings of <strong>the</strong>14039_86991_06_chap05 87 5/2/07 13:50:29


88 <strong>pragmatics</strong>Stockholm Workshop on Variation within Optimality Theory. Stockholm: Universityof Stockholm. 150–9.<strong>Blutner</strong>, R. (1998). ‘Lexical <strong>pragmatics</strong>’. Journal of Semantics 15: 115–62.— (2000). ‘Some aspects of <strong>optimality</strong> in natural language interpretation’. Journalof Semantics 17: 189–216.<strong>Blutner</strong>, R., E. Borra, T. Lentz, A. Uijlings <strong>and</strong> R. Zevenhuijzen (2002). ‘Signallinggames: Hoe evolutie optimale strategieen selecteert’. H<strong>and</strong>elingen van de 24steNederl<strong>and</strong>s-Vlaamse Filosofiedag. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam.<strong>Blutner</strong>, R., de Hoop, <strong>and</strong> P. Hendriks (2005). Optimal Communication: Amsterdam:CSLI.<strong>Blutner</strong>, R. <strong>and</strong> R. Sommer (1988). ‘Sentence Processing <strong>and</strong> Lexical Access: TheInfluence of <strong>the</strong> Focus-Identifying Task’. Journal of Memory <strong>and</strong> Language 27:359–67.<strong>Blutner</strong>, R. <strong>and</strong> H. Zeevat (eds) (2004). Optimality Theory <strong>and</strong> Pragmatics. Basingstoke:Palgrave/Macmillan.Burton-Roberts, N. (2005). ‘Robyn Carston on semantics, <strong>pragmatics</strong> <strong>and</strong>“encoding”. Journal of Linguistics 41: 389–407.Carston, R. (2002). Thoughts <strong>and</strong> Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication.Oxford: Blackwell.— (2003). ‘Explicature <strong>and</strong> semantics’. In S. David <strong>and</strong> B. Gillon (eds). Semantics:A Reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 817–45.— (2004). ‘Relevance <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> saying/implicating distinction’. In L. Horn<strong>and</strong> G. Ward (eds). H<strong>and</strong>book of Pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell. 633–56.Chierchia, G. (2004). ‘Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> syntax/<strong>pragmatics</strong> interface’. In A. Belletti (ed.). Structures <strong>and</strong> Beyond. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press. 39–103.Cole, P. (ed.). (1981). Radical <strong>pragmatics</strong>. New York: Academic Press.Dawkins, R. (1983). The extended phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Ducrot, O. (1980). Les Echelles argumentatives. Paris: Minuit.Fanselow, G., M. Schlesewsky, D. Cavar <strong>and</strong> R. Kliegl (1999). ‘Optimal parsing,syntactic parsing preferences, <strong>and</strong> Optimality Theory’. Unpublished ms,University of Potsdam.Gazdar, G. (1979). Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Green, M. (1998). ‘Direct reference <strong>and</strong> implicature’. Philosophical Studies 91:61–90.Grice, P. (1975). ‘Logic <strong>and</strong> conversation’. In P. Cole <strong>and</strong> J.L. Morgan (eds). Syntax<strong>and</strong> Semantics 3: Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press. 41–58.Hendriks, P. <strong>and</strong> H. de Hoop (2001). ‘Optimality <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> semantics’. Linguistics<strong>and</strong> Philosophy 24: 1–32.Hendriks, P. <strong>and</strong> J. Spenader (2004). ‘When production precedes comprehension:An optimization approach to <strong>the</strong> acquisition of pronouns’. Unpublishedmanuscript, Groningen.Hoop, H. de <strong>and</strong> H. de Swart (2000). ‘Temporal adjunct clauses in <strong>optimality</strong><strong>the</strong>ory’. Rivista di Linguistica 12. 1: 107–27.Horn, L. (1984). ‘Towards a new taxonomy of pragmatic inference: Q-based <strong>and</strong>R-based implicature’. In D. Schiffrin (ed.). Meaning, form, <strong>and</strong> use in context:Linguistic applications. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. 11–42— (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.14039_86991_06_chap05 88 5/2/07 13:50:29


<strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> <strong>pragmatics</strong> 89Jäger, G. (2002). ‘Some notes on <strong>the</strong> formal properties of bidirectional <strong>optimality</strong><strong>the</strong>ory’. Journal of Logic, Language <strong>and</strong> Information 11: 427–51.— (2004). ‘Learning constraint sub-hierarchies. The bidirectional gradual learningAlgorithm’. In R. <strong>Blutner</strong> <strong>and</strong> H. Zeevat (eds). Pragmatics <strong>and</strong> Optimality Theory.Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Kirby, S. (2000). ‘Syntax without natural selection: how compositionality emergesfrom vocabulary in a population of learners’. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy,<strong>and</strong> J.R. Hurford (eds). The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Origins of Linguistic Form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.303–23.Levinson, S. (2000). Presumptive meaning: The <strong>the</strong>ory of generalized conversationalimplicature. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Logan, G.D. (1988). ‘Toward an instance <strong>the</strong>ory of automatization’. PsychologicalReview 95: 492–527.Mattausch, J. (2004). ‘On <strong>the</strong> Optimization & Grammaticalization of Anaphora’.Unpublished Ph.D Thesis. Humboldt University, Berlin.McCawley, J.D. (1978). ‘Conversational implicature <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> lexicon’. In P. Cole(ed.). Syntax <strong>and</strong> Semantics 9: Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press. 245–59.— (1993). Everything that Linguists have Always Wanted to Know about Logic. 2ndedition.Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Merin, A. (1997). ‘If all our arguments had to be conclusive, <strong>the</strong>re would be fewof <strong>the</strong>m’. Arbeitspapiere SFB 340. Stuttgart: University of Stuttgart. 101.Noveck, I.A. (2005). ‘Pragmatic inferences related to logical terms’. In I.A.Noveck <strong>and</strong> D. Sperber (eds). Experimental Pragmatics. Basingstoke: PalgraveMacmillan.Recanati, F. (1989). ‘The <strong>pragmatics</strong> of what is said’. Mind <strong>and</strong> Language 4: 294–328.— (1993). Direct Reference: From Language to Thought. Oxford: Blackwell.Russell, B. (2006). ‘Against Grammatical Computation of Scalar Implicatures’.Journal of Semantics 23: 361–82.Sadock, J.M. (1978). ‘On testing for conversational implicature’. In P. Cole (ed.).Syntax <strong>and</strong> Semantics 9: Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press. 281–97.Sauerl<strong>and</strong>, U. (2004). ‘Scalar implicatures in complex sentences’. Linguistics <strong>and</strong>Philosophy 27: 367–91.Soames, S. (1982). ‘How presuppositions are inherited: A solution to <strong>the</strong> projectionproblem’. Linguistic Inquiry 13: 483–545.Sperber, D. <strong>and</strong> D. Wilson (1986). Relevance. Oxford: Blackwell.Steels, L. (1998). ‘The origins of syntax in visually grounded robotic agents’.Artificial Intelligence 103: 133–56.van Kuppevelt, J. (1996). ‘Inferring from Topics: Scalar Implicature as Topic-Dependent Inferences’. Linguistics <strong>and</strong> Philosophy 19: 555–98.van Rooy, R. (2004). ‘Signalling games select Horn strategies’. Linguistics <strong>and</strong>Philosophy 27: 493–527.— (2 006). ‘Relevance of complex sentence’. Mind <strong>and</strong> Matter 4: 195-218.Zeevat, H. (2000). ‘The asymmetry of <strong>optimality</strong> <strong><strong>the</strong>oretic</strong> syntax <strong>and</strong> semantics’.Journal of Semantics 17: 243–62.Zipf, G.K. (1949). Human behavior <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> principle of least effort. Cambridge:Addison-Wesley.Zwarts, J. (2006). ‘Om en rond: een semantische vergelijking’. Nederl<strong>and</strong>se Taalkunde11.2: 101–23.14039_86991_06_chap05 89 5/2/07 13:50:29

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!