12.07.2015 Views

Culture shock due to contact with unfamiliar cultures.

Culture shock due to contact with unfamiliar cultures.

Culture shock due to contact with unfamiliar cultures.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmBochner, S. (2003). <strong>Culture</strong> <strong>shock</strong> <strong>due</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>contact</strong> <strong>with</strong> <strong>unfamiliar</strong> <strong>cultures</strong>. In W. J. Lonner, D. L.Dinnel, S. A. Hayes, & D. N. Sattler (Eds.), Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong> (Unit 8,Chapter 7), (http://www.wwu.edu/~culture), Center for Cross-Cultural Research, Western Washing<strong>to</strong>nUniversity, Bellingham, Washing<strong>to</strong>n USA.This material is copyrighted by the author(s), who have kindly extended <strong>to</strong> the Center the right <strong>to</strong> usethe material as described in the Introduction <strong>to</strong> this collection and the form entitled "Agreement <strong>to</strong>Extend License <strong>to</strong> Use Work."UNIT 8, CHAPTER 7CULTURE SHOCK DUE TO CONTACT WITH UNFAMILIAR CULTURESStephen BochnerSchool of PsychologyUniversity of New South WalesSydney, AustraliaABSTRACTThe <strong>to</strong>pic of this chapter is the social psychology of cross-cultural interaction. We discuss thepsychological processes that take place during and after meetings between individuals and groups whodiffer in their cultural backgrounds. We identify two types of cross-cultural <strong>contact</strong>: a) meetings tha<strong>to</strong>ccur between two societies when individuals travel from their place of origin <strong>to</strong> another country for aspecific purpose and a limited amount of time, such people being called sojourners in the literature; andb) meetings <strong>with</strong>in multi-cultural societies among its ethnically diverse permanent residents. Contact<strong>with</strong> culturally <strong>unfamiliar</strong> people and places can be unsettling, and the term "culture <strong>shock</strong>" is frequentlyused <strong>to</strong> describe how people react <strong>to</strong> novel or unaccus<strong>to</strong>med situations. Although the unknown can beterrifying, we nevertheless argue that "culture <strong>shock</strong>" is not inevitable, or for that matter as widespreadas is often suggested. Indeed, in many circumstances culture <strong>contact</strong> can be a satisfying experience. Wedraw on the ABC model of culture <strong>contact</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide a framework for the discussion, that is, wedistinguish between the Affective, Behavioural and Cognitive components of cross-cultural interaction.In the chapter we describe the conditions that determine whether the <strong>contact</strong> will have positive oradverse consequences, and the psychological techniques that can be deployed <strong>to</strong> increase cross-culturalunderstanding among the individuals, groups and societies in <strong>contact</strong>.INTRODUCTIONMost of us live and work in familiar surroundings, usually in places where we grew up or like thosewhere we were raised. And by and large the people whom we meet at work, school or play tend <strong>to</strong> besimilar <strong>to</strong> ourselves, in the sense of having comparable ethnic backgrounds, matching beliefs, sharedvalues, and speak the same language or at least a dialect variant of it. Technically, this can be calledinhabiting a culturally homogeneous space.Although living in accus<strong>to</strong>med circumstances is the rule for most people, there have always been1 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmexceptions <strong>to</strong> this pattern. The his<strong>to</strong>ry of humankind is full of examples of persons and groups whotravelled <strong>to</strong> foreign lands for a variety of purposes, the main ones being <strong>to</strong> work, study, teach, conquer,assist, have fun in, or settle in the country. The journals of Captain Cook, Marco Polo and Chris<strong>to</strong>pherColumbus provide very good descriptions of what we have referred <strong>to</strong> elsewhere (e.g. Ward, Bochner& Furnham, 2001) as between-society culture <strong>contact</strong>. Modern day examples include employees ofinternational organisations, guest workers, overseas students, <strong>to</strong>urists, immigrants, refugees,missionaries, and peacekeepers.Between-Society ContactThe term between society culture-<strong>contact</strong> refers <strong>to</strong> individuals who travel beyond their countries oforigin for a particular purpose and for a specified period of time, and the relationships they establish<strong>with</strong> members of the host society. The term sojourner has been used <strong>to</strong> describe such culture travellers,indicating that they are temporary visi<strong>to</strong>rs intending <strong>to</strong> return home after achieving their aims. And theterm host-society member is often employed <strong>to</strong> distinguish the visi<strong>to</strong>rs from the visited. Travellingbetween societies inevitably involves some personal <strong>contact</strong> between culturally dissimilar individuals,and in the case of the sojourner, exposure <strong>to</strong> unaccus<strong>to</strong>med physical and social manifestations. This canbe unsettling, particularly if the transition is abrupt, and is the origin of the concept of "culture <strong>shock</strong>",the subject of this chapter.Within-Society ContactThe term <strong>with</strong>in-society <strong>contact</strong> describes inter-ethnic relationships in multi-cultural societies. Successfulmulti-cultural countries may contain many diverse ethnic groups integrated by institutional arrangementsthat support shared values and produce a common sense of nationhood. The United States from itsearliest days was characterised by a great deal of internal diversity. In such social systems people willinevitably meet others who are dissimilar <strong>to</strong> themselves in appearance, ancestry, values and cus<strong>to</strong>ms. Incountries that favour ethnic diversity, such cross-cultural <strong>contact</strong> enriches the lives of its citizens. Theopposite is the case in countries where inter-group relations have an ethnocentric bias.Between-Society <strong>Culture</strong> ShockDuring the last 40 years, the incidence of humans shifting across national boundaries has greatlyincreased. Reasons include the invention of the jumbo jet that made international travel quicker, easierand cheaper. Changes in the world economy have also played their part. The term "globalisation" ismuch in the news nowadays, and relates not only <strong>to</strong> industry and commerce, but also <strong>to</strong> education andleisure. For example, in 1999 overseas assignments by United States companies exceeded 350,000business persons, a figure that does not take in<strong>to</strong> account all the other nations sending their executivesabroad. It has been estimated that at any given time there are about a million and a half students andscholars attending educational institutions abroad. The figures for <strong>to</strong>urists are even greater: The WorldTourism Organisation has projected that by the year 2010 the number of international <strong>to</strong>urist-relatedjourneys will rise <strong>to</strong> a <strong>to</strong>tal of 940 million trips per year.Natural and human-made disasters such as floods, famine and regional conflicts also play a major role instimulating cross-cultural travel, and include growing numbers of refugees, immigrants and guest2 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmworkers. However, people in these categories do not fit our definition of a sojourner as most of them donot intend <strong>to</strong> or are unable <strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> their countries of origin. Consequently, their reactions are moreappropriately considered from the perspective of <strong>with</strong>in-society culture <strong>contact</strong>.The phrase culture <strong>shock</strong> has been attributed <strong>to</strong> the anthropologist Kalervo Oberg, who in an article in1960 used it <strong>to</strong> illustrate how people react <strong>to</strong> strange or <strong>unfamiliar</strong> places. In our book (Ward, Bochnerand Furnham, 2001) we have suggested that readers should be cautious about taking the expression <strong>to</strong>oliterally. There is no doubt that it reflects some of the feelings and experiences of travellers whosuddenly find themselves in new, strange, or <strong>unfamiliar</strong> places. The unknown can be an uncomfortableand at times terrifying experience. However, the use of the word "<strong>shock</strong>" places <strong>to</strong>o much emphasis onthe threatening circumstances of <strong>contact</strong> <strong>with</strong> novel situations, <strong>with</strong>out acknowledging that suchexperiences may also have beneficial consequences for the participants. This led us <strong>to</strong> contend that overthe years "culture <strong>shock</strong>" has become a widely misused term, both in popular language as well as incross-cultural psychology. This article will identify some of the empirical conditions under which travelacross <strong>cultures</strong> can be stressful, offer a theoretical explanation for such an outcome, and provide a briefaccount of the strategies that can be used <strong>to</strong> reduce <strong>contact</strong>-induced stress. As such, we will restric<strong>to</strong>urselves <strong>to</strong> the "culture <strong>shock</strong>" of between-society culture <strong>contact</strong>, that is, the psychology of thetraveller or sojourner who ventures across <strong>cultures</strong>. Other articles in this series deal <strong>with</strong> immigrant andrefugee experiences, or the psychology of acculturation that characterises <strong>with</strong>in-society culture <strong>contact</strong>.Readers may questions why we have included <strong>to</strong>urists in the discussion of culture <strong>shock</strong>. That isbecause although <strong>to</strong>urism is promoted as a tranquil and relaxing holiday experience, many studies haveshown that <strong>to</strong>urists are prone <strong>to</strong> exactly the same psychological stress as other between-culture travelers.The Determinants of <strong>Culture</strong> ShockThe Similarity-Attraction HypothesisA robust finding in social psychological research is that individuals have a preference for people whoare similar <strong>to</strong> themselves; and are less favourably disposed <strong>to</strong> others regarded as being different.Similarity is a complex matter, because individuals and groups can be alike in a variety of ways. Evenso, studies have found that most non-trivial aspects of similarity have an effect on how people willrespond <strong>to</strong> and perceive each other. In general, individuals are more likely <strong>to</strong> seek out, enjoy,understand, want <strong>to</strong> work and play <strong>with</strong>, trust, vote for, and marry others <strong>with</strong> whom they sharecharacteristics they regard as important. These include values, religion, group affiliation, skills, physicalattributes, age, language, occupation, social class, nationality, ethnicity, residential location, and mos<strong>to</strong>ther aspects on which human beings differ.It should be emphasised that we are addressing perceived rather than actual similarity, which is inferred,sometimes quite erroneously, from characteristics such as skin colour, accent, clothing, and other visiblecues. To cite a personal example, some years ago we were visiting a stately home in Britain, andencountered a middle aged man attired in a scruffy outfit engaged in pruning a rose bush. We assumedthat this person was one of the many gardeners employed by the estate, until we observed asimilarly-attired workman approach the man, deferentially doff his cap, and address the person as "MyLord". Many intercultural encounters are marred by such fallacious inferences.3 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmThe technical term for preferring like-minded people is in-group bias. The theory underlying it is basedon the principle that the similarity of another person is reassuring. The world is a complex place <strong>with</strong>many choices and alternatives <strong>to</strong> offer. It is frequently unclear as <strong>to</strong> how people should behave in socialsituations that are often ambiguous. Such uncertainty can be unpleasant, and ac<strong>to</strong>rs will seek guidanceabout how <strong>to</strong> "correctly" conduct themselves. Books and magazines on etiquette provide one source ofadvice, as does religious doctrine for the devout. However, a more common solution is <strong>to</strong> ascertain howother people deal <strong>with</strong> the problem, the technical term for this process being consensual validation.Individuals <strong>with</strong> similar values and practices provide confirmation that our opinions, actions anddecisions are righteous and correct. And conversely, a dissimilar person may undermine such security.Although the term "culture" has been defined in a variety of ways, common <strong>to</strong> all of them is theprinciple of culture as shared meanings. It follows from this definition that <strong>contact</strong> between culturallydiverse people will take place among individuals who are dissimilar, quite possibly <strong>with</strong> respect <strong>to</strong>important, deeply felt issues. A further implication is that such interactions may be aversive and cause"culture <strong>shock</strong>", that is, create anxiety, and in extreme cases fear and loathing in the participants.The <strong>Culture</strong>-Distance HypothesisResearch, much of it reviewed in our book, has shown that the greater the cultural distance separatinginteracting participants, the more difficulty they will have in establishing and maintaining harmoniousrelations. This effect has been found for most sojourners including <strong>to</strong>urists, overseas students andexpatriate business persons, all of whom perform less effectively in their personal and professional livesin <strong>cultures</strong> that are significantly different from their own. Importantly, the distance between the <strong>cultures</strong>of the participants will have an effect on the smoothness of the interaction. For instance, Australiansojourners in Britain should have an easier time of it than is the case for Australian sojourners inMainland China. "<strong>Culture</strong> <strong>shock</strong>" defined in this way is a function of the degree of separation betweenthe <strong>cultures</strong> of the sojourners and their host societies.Differences in Core ValuesA special case of culture distance derives from differences in values, and can be another major source ofculture <strong>shock</strong>. Interactions between members of societies diametrically opposed on core issues canquickly descend in<strong>to</strong> rancour and hostility. For instance, the lower standing of women in some societiesattracts condemnation in <strong>cultures</strong> that value non-discrimina<strong>to</strong>ry gender relations. Conversely, membersof male-dominated societies regard the occupational and sexual independence of Western women asrepugnant and offensive. Probably the single most potent source of friction stems from differences inreligious beliefs and practices, as many his<strong>to</strong>rical as well as contemporary examples illustrate.Outcomes of <strong>Culture</strong> ShockIn this section we review some of the psychological effects of exposure <strong>to</strong> culture <strong>contact</strong>. We will besuggesting that <strong>contact</strong> does not necessarily lead <strong>to</strong> negative reactions. However, there is no doubt thatcross-cultural interactions are inherently stressful, and an analysis of any potential adverse reactionsmust be included in the discussion.4 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htm<strong>Culture</strong>-Shock Research in His<strong>to</strong>rical PerspectiveThe bulk of past research was conducted from the perspective of culture <strong>contact</strong> as a one-way flow ofinfluence. That is, most studies set out <strong>to</strong> describe the impact of the new culture on the sojourners. Theterm typically used in this context was "adjustment", implying that sojourners had <strong>to</strong> accommodate <strong>to</strong>the host culture or suffer the consequences. Until quite recently any reciprocal effects of the visi<strong>to</strong>r onthe host country tended <strong>to</strong> be ignored. This was because it was assumed that a host society is <strong>to</strong>omonolithic <strong>to</strong> be significantly <strong>to</strong>uched by such temporary residents. The exception <strong>to</strong> this trend werestudies of the impact of <strong>to</strong>urists on the sites and <strong>cultures</strong> they visit, but even here this was mainly <strong>with</strong>respect <strong>to</strong> their economic consequences. We will not be able <strong>to</strong> discuss this issue further as it would takeus beyond our immediate focus, but readers should be reminded that the sojourner-host memberrelationship is very much a reciprocal transaction, and that both parties can experience "culture <strong>shock</strong>".The early sojourn literature was characterised by four features: First, it was assumed that culture <strong>contact</strong>is a noxious, or at least a painful experience for the sojourner. Second, flowing from the belief thatcross-cultural <strong>contact</strong> is inherently unpleasant, most of the researchers conceptualised the outcome of<strong>contact</strong> predominantly in affective terms, concentrating on the negative emotions, fears and anxieties thatsojourners supposedly experienced.Third, this in turn gave the field a distinctly "clinical", intra-individual flavour, particularly when it came<strong>to</strong> accounting for individual differences in adjustment and coping. Personality traits such as "<strong>to</strong>lerancefor ambiguity", "authoritarianism" and "neuroticism" were used <strong>to</strong> explain why some culture travellersfailed and others succeeded. One consequence of this approach was <strong>to</strong> stigmatise those who "brokedown". The many Peace Corps Volunteers in the early days who had <strong>to</strong> be repatriated because theycould not cope are a good example.Fourth, most of the research was basically a-theoretical, consisting of shotgun surveys of varioussamples of convenience, which meant that the results were difficult <strong>to</strong> interpret.Contemporary studies of "culture <strong>shock</strong>" tend <strong>to</strong> be much more theoretically driven, look at social aswell as internal determinants, and allow for the measurement of both positive as well as negativeoutcomes.The ABC of <strong>Culture</strong> ShockIn our book, we have developed what we called the ABC theory of culture <strong>contact</strong>. Unlike earlierformulations, the ABC model does not regard the response <strong>to</strong> <strong>unfamiliar</strong> cultural settings as a passive,largely negative reaction, but rather as an active process of dealing <strong>with</strong> change. The term copingbehaviour is sometimes used in the literature <strong>to</strong> emphasize this active aspect. Additionally, the modelmakes an explicit distinction between three components of this process: Affect, Behaviour, andCognitions; that is, how people feel, behave, think and perceive when exposed <strong>to</strong> second-cultureinfluences. The ABC model also links each of these elements <strong>to</strong> particular theoretical frameworks.Finally, the model has implications for interventions aimed at decreasing "culture <strong>shock</strong>" and increasingthe likelihood of achieving positive culture-<strong>contact</strong> outcomes.5 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmThe affective approach <strong>to</strong> culture <strong>contact</strong> is captured by Oberg's depiction of 'culture <strong>shock</strong>' as a buzzingconfusion. To be fair, he had in mind people who were suddenly exposed <strong>to</strong> a completely <strong>unfamiliar</strong>setting and overwhelmed by it, a phenomenon particular <strong>to</strong> the jet age. Nevertheless, it becamefashionable <strong>to</strong> characterise all culture <strong>contact</strong> in terms of negative affect, such as confusion, anxiety,disorientation, suspicion, even grief and bereavement <strong>due</strong> <strong>to</strong> a sense of loss of familiar physical objectsand social relationships.More recent formulations of the affective component in inter-cultural <strong>contact</strong> draw on the stress andcoping literature (reviewed in our book), which treats socio-cultural adjustment as an active, adaptiveresponse. Self-efficacy as described by Bandura (1986) has also been a prominent feature of thisapproach.The intervention techniques implied by the affective model tend <strong>to</strong> be variants of traditional counsellingprocedures, their aim being <strong>to</strong> reduce anxiety, increase self-efficacy and emotional resilience, and <strong>to</strong>develop effective coping strategies.During the 70' and 80' it became obvious, at least <strong>to</strong> some of us, that culture <strong>contact</strong> did not always lead<strong>to</strong> culture <strong>shock</strong>. Some sojourners seemed <strong>to</strong> thrive in what were for them exotic locations. And it wasalso apparent, at least <strong>to</strong> those of us trained as social psychologists, that culture <strong>contact</strong> was manifestlyan interpersonal, interactive event. Intra-personal characteristics and traits certainly played a role, but thatwas really only one part of the s<strong>to</strong>ry, and a minor one at that. The present writer spent some time atOxford <strong>with</strong> Michael Argyle, and was greatly impressed by his (Argyle, 1994) model of interpersonalbehaviour as a mutually skilled performance. Specifically, Argyle said that social interaction is a highlyrule-bound activity, even though the participants are mostly unaware of this underlying framework. Theguidelines that control social behaviour are largely taken for granted, rather like the presence of oxygenin the air. Only when the oxygen is reduced or missing, as in smog or carbon dioxide emissions, do wetake notice of it. And the same is true in our social world, we really only become aware of the presenceof a behavioural imperative when it is infringed or disregarded.Although at that time Argyle was constructing his social skills model <strong>with</strong>out explicit reference <strong>to</strong> anycross-cultural implications, its utility in explaining culture-<strong>contact</strong> phenomena was obvious <strong>to</strong> the writer,leading him <strong>to</strong> develop what he called a culture-learning model of <strong>contact</strong>. In this he was assisted by twobooks by E. T. Hall (1959; 1966) that did take a transcultural view - The silent language and The hiddendimension. The culture learning model extends Argyle's social skills account <strong>to</strong> propose that the rules,conventions and assumptions that regulate interpersonal interaction, particularly verbal and non-verbalcommunication, vary across <strong>cultures</strong>. One implication is that sojourners who lack culturally relevantsocial skills and knowledge will have difficulty in initiating and sustaining harmonious relations <strong>with</strong>their hosts, or in the case of immigrants, <strong>with</strong> mainstream members. Their culturally inappropriatebehaviour will lead <strong>to</strong> misunderstandings and may cause offense. Indeed, research has shown thatculturally unskilled persons are less likely <strong>to</strong> achieve their professional and personal goals. Expatriateexecutives may alienate their local counterparts and lose market share, overseas students may fail theircourses, hospitality industry workers may offend <strong>to</strong>urists, and the job prospects of migrants may beadversely affected.All of the above effects have been confirmed empirically. The present writer has conducted numerous6 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmstudies of the social networks of various groups of sojourners <strong>to</strong> confirm that poor social skills haveadverse effects, exacerbated by the extent of the distance separating host from visi<strong>to</strong>r (or majority fromminority) <strong>cultures</strong>. A critical fac<strong>to</strong>r in sojourner adjustment was the extent <strong>to</strong> which they hadhost-culture friends, the reason being that these persons acted as informal culture-skills men<strong>to</strong>rs. Thosevisi<strong>to</strong>rs who socialised exclusively <strong>with</strong> members of their own <strong>cultures</strong> did less well on a variety ofmeasures than sojourners who had established non-trivial links <strong>with</strong> their hosts. The relevant empiricalliterature has been comprehensively reviewed in our recent book.The culture learning approach emphasises the behavioural elements of culture <strong>contact</strong>. It is equallyrelevant in explaining encounters <strong>with</strong>in as well as between <strong>cultures</strong>, and it is interactive rather than justconcentrating on the visi<strong>to</strong>r or newcomer. It has clear implications for remedial action: For individuals <strong>to</strong>function effectively in a second-culture setting, they have <strong>to</strong> acquire relevant skills and knowledgespecific <strong>to</strong> the new culture; that is, they have <strong>to</strong> learn about the his<strong>to</strong>rical, philosophical and sociopoliticalfoundations of the target society, and acquire and rehearse some of the associated behaviours. Thisapproach does not stigmatise those who stumble in their second-culture interactions, because the reasonfor their failure lies not in their personalities, but in their competencies. And it is a lot easier <strong>to</strong> learn newskills than it is <strong>to</strong> change personalities.The third element of the model is the cognitive component, and owes its place in the theory <strong>to</strong> what hasbeen called the cognitive revolution in psychology, that is, <strong>to</strong> the greater emphasis during the last ten orso years on cognitive processes. Again, we have taken developments <strong>with</strong>in "mainstream" monoculturalpsychology and extended them <strong>to</strong> culture <strong>contact</strong> issues. As mentioned earlier, the broadest definition ofculture is as a system of shared meanings, very much a cognitive proposition.People interpret physical, interpersonal, institutional, existential and spiritual events as culturalmanifestations, and these vary across <strong>cultures</strong>. When different <strong>cultures</strong> come in<strong>to</strong> <strong>contact</strong>, particularly'distant' ones, such established truths lose their apparent certainty. For instance, when persons from asociety that values individualism, as is the case in most Western countries, sojourn in a collectivistculture such as Japan, the conflict between these two orientations will drift in<strong>to</strong> the cognitions of bothvisi<strong>to</strong>rs and hosts. It will affect how the participants see each other and themselves, and whether eitherparty will change their views as a consequence of the <strong>contact</strong>. Relevant theoretical models and research<strong>to</strong>pics include attribution theory, prejudice, ethnocentrism and stereotypes, and social identity theory.Interventions based on the cognitive component of culture <strong>contact</strong> involve some form of culturalsensitivity and awareness training. These techniques emphasise the cultural relativity of most values, thevalidity of the <strong>unfamiliar</strong> culture, and more generally the advantages of cultural diversity, including thecommercial, aesthetic, and adaptive advantages of a culturally heterogeneous global system.Measuring <strong>Culture</strong> ShockIn the final analysis the As, Bs, and Cs of culture <strong>contact</strong> are defined operationally in terms of theconstructs and procedures used <strong>to</strong> measure them. Some typical measures of B and C (behaviour andcognition) include Colleen Ward's Sociocultural Adjustment Scale, Kelley & Meyers Cross-CulturalAdaptability Inven<strong>to</strong>ry, and the Social Situations Questionnaire that Adrian Furnham and StephenBochner developed <strong>to</strong> measure the extent <strong>to</strong> which sojourners experience difficulties in their new7 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmsettings (Furnham & Bochner, 1986).The behavioural dimension is sometimes further divided in<strong>to</strong> three sub-categories: InstrumentalAdjustment, defined as the ability <strong>to</strong> navigate through the new environment. Examples of items include"I know where <strong>to</strong> shop for what I need." Interaction Adjustment, defined as casual interactions <strong>with</strong>host members "When I experienced unsatisfac<strong>to</strong>ry service, I did something about it; I have no difficultyin asking strangers for directions". And Relational Adjustment, defined as maintaining non-trivialfriendships and social networks <strong>with</strong> host members "I made friends <strong>with</strong> people of different ethnicbackgrounds." We have also developed a scale that measures Host-Language Proficiency, found <strong>to</strong> be agood index of behavioural adjustment.The cognitive dimension can also be regarded as having three distinct components: Interest in Other<strong>Culture</strong>s "When I meet people different from me, I want <strong>to</strong> learn more about them"; Tolerance forCultural Differences "When I meet people from other <strong>cultures</strong> I tend <strong>to</strong> feel judgmental about theirdifferences"; and Positive Attitudes Toward New or Unusual Cultural Environments "I believe I canlive a fulfilling life in another culture".Over the years, the author has been measuring the cognitive dimension of ethnic or cultural identity <strong>with</strong>the I Am Test, which consists of a sheet of paper listing ten incomplete statements beginning <strong>with</strong> "Iam.....". This measure is sensitive <strong>to</strong> whether participants are mono-cultural <strong>with</strong> respect <strong>to</strong> their new ororiginal culture, marginal <strong>with</strong> respect <strong>to</strong> both, or bi-cultural or culturally hyphenated individuals (e.g.Chinese-Australians). The procedure has its theoretical basis in Stephen Bochner's (1981)Mediating-Person Model. We also sometimes use a one-item test in which we simply ask people <strong>to</strong>write down what ethnic group they belong <strong>to</strong>. Colleen Ward and her colleagues have explored identityby conventional questionnaire items that measure how individuals perceive and relate <strong>to</strong> their owncultural groups and <strong>to</strong> members of other groups.Affective Adjustment is usually measured by means of scales adapted from the clinical literature <strong>to</strong> gaugethe amount of stress and physical and mental ill health the sojourners experience, or in positive terms,the extent of emotional well-being and satisfaction. Sub-scales include measures of anxiety, confusion,low self-esteem, homesickness, and a sense of helplessness. Subjective estimates of health are also oftenincluded, using questionnaires such as the General Health Questionnaire.ConclusionLet me conclude by commenting about the relationship between the A, B, and C elements of the model.In our book we regard each of the components as influencing and being influenced by the other twoelements. Technically, this can be described as a continuous feedback cycle reverberating among thethree components. In other words, what we feel will affect what we do and think, and vice versa cubed.And because there is no direct research regarding the relative importance of these three processes, the A,B, and C components have been accorded equal status by default. However I tend <strong>to</strong> the view, which isnot necessarily shared by my co-authors, that the primary determinants of culture <strong>shock</strong> as a negativereaction, and cross-cultural adaptation as a positive response, are in the behavioural domain. I believethat affective (emotional) responses are essentially retrospective evaluations of inter-cultural experiences,that is, behavioural episodes, which can range from the aversive <strong>to</strong> the highly satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry. And I assume8 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htmthat the main function of cognitive responses is <strong>to</strong> rationalise these emotive reactions. If thesespeculations can be supported by empirical evidence, this has implications for intervening in andmanaging inter-cultural <strong>contact</strong>s, in particular that more weight should be given <strong>to</strong> facilitating the culturelearning of the participants.REFERENCESArgyle, M. (1994). The psychology of interpersonal behaviour. 5th Edition. London: Penguin.Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive view. EnglewoodCliff, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Bochner, S. (Ed.) (1981). The mediating person: Bridges between <strong>cultures</strong>. Cambridge, Mass.:Schenkman.Furnham, A., and Bochner, S. (1986). (Reprinted 1989, 1990, 1994). <strong>Culture</strong> <strong>shock</strong>: Psychologicalreactions <strong>to</strong> <strong>unfamiliar</strong> environments. London: Methuen.Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent language. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. Garden City, NY: DoubledayWard, C., Bochner, S., & Furnham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture <strong>shock</strong>. Second edition.Hove, UK: Routledge.About the AuthorStephen Bochner is a Visiting Professor in the School of Psychology at the University of New SouthWales in Sydney, Australia. He is a cross-cultural applied psychologist, <strong>with</strong> a special interest in thepsychology of culture <strong>contact</strong> and acculturation. He is the author of several books, and numerouschapters in books and journal articles. Topics on which he has published extensively include theadaptation of overseas students and expatriates, and the social psychology of culturally diverse workplaces. His most recent publications are as co-author <strong>with</strong> Colleen Ward and Adrian Furnham of thebook "The psychology of culture <strong>shock</strong>: Second edition" published in 2001 by Routledge; and"Organisational culture and climate", a chapter in the forthcoming book "Organisational psychology inAustralia and New Zealand", edited by M. O'Driscoll, P Taylor & T. Kalliath <strong>to</strong> be published by OxfordUniversity Press in 2003. E-mail address: s.bochner@unsw.edu.auQuestions for Discussion1. Why is it easier <strong>to</strong> communicate <strong>with</strong> individuals who are similar <strong>to</strong> you than <strong>with</strong> people who aredifferent?2. Canadian students enrolled in a university in the United States are probably going <strong>to</strong> make thetransition more easily than if they were attending one of the universities in Hong Kong that use Englishas the medium of instruction. What are some of the reasons for these differences in coping?9 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM


Online Readings in Psychology and <strong>Culture</strong>, Unit 15, Chapter 8http://www.ac.wwu.edu/%7Eculture/Bochner.htm3. What would be a useful working definition of culture?4. Adapting <strong>to</strong> new cultural settings involves changes in the way in which sojourners feel, behave, andthink. If you have had personal exposure <strong>to</strong> <strong>unfamiliar</strong> <strong>cultures</strong>, reflect on your experiences and sharethese <strong>with</strong> your colleagues, using the A, B, and C categories as a framework for your account.5. What do you believe is an effective way <strong>to</strong> prepare people for living and working in <strong>unfamiliar</strong>cultural settings? Again, use the A, B, C framework <strong>to</strong> organise your suggestions.Go <strong>to</strong> Top of Page | Home | Brief Table of Contents | Complete Table of Contents10 of 10 3/19/06 5:38 PM

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!