13.07.2015 Views

View - ResearchGate

View - ResearchGate

View - ResearchGate

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

FIG. 3. Amount of closure voicing, in milliseconds top, and as a percentageof the closure duration bottom. Closed bars represent /b/; open represent/p/.III. RESULTSSections III A and III B address group i.e., age effectsin voicing and pressure measures. Section III C assesses therelationship between voicing and pressure data. Section III Dcompares the current data with those of Müller and Brown1980, incorporating the post-hoc analysis of two men.FIG. 4. top and normalized n bottom values for each speaker group.Closed bars represent /b/; open represent /p/.percentages for /p/ voicing, in ascending age order, were12%, 16%, and 12%; for /b/, they were 36%, 59%, and 64%.The proportions of devoiced stops for all speakers aregiven in Table II. Although devoicing was most common onaverage in the 5 year olds, the incidence of devoiced /b/varied considerably within groups, and the group effect didnot reach significance F2,21=3.968, p=.035.A. Stop closure and voicing durationsClosure voicing durations and percentages of closurevoicing are shown for each speaker group in Fig. 3. ANOVAresults for the two measures are given in Table I. As expected,voicing was more extensive for /b/ than /p/, measuredboth as a duration and as a percentage of the closure. Theconsonant effect was highly significant in both ANOVAs. Amain effect of age was not significant for either analysis. Thegroup-by-consonant interactions did not reach significanceeither, although the one for voicing percentage approached itp=0.018. Qualitatively, the percentage of closure voicingfor /p/ was similar across ages, whereas for /b/ it increasedsomewhat over age see bottom panel of Fig. 3. The actualTABLE I. ANOVA results for closure voicing measured as a duration and asa percentage of the closure duration.Voicing durationEffect dfs F pConsonant 1, 21 115.432 0.0001Group 2, 21 0.678 0.518Consonantgroup 2, 21 2.955 0.074Voicing percentageEffect dfs F pConsonant 1, 21 102.595 0.0001Group 2, 21 2.314 0.124Consonantgroup 2, 21 4.927 0.018B. Pressure contoursThe analysis was intended to quantify pulse shape,with values of 0 indicating convexity. The n values arethe normalized, unitless counterparts designed to correct forspeech rate differences, all else being equal. Group resultsfor and n are shown in Fig. 4. The ANOVA results aresummarized in Table III.For , the consonant effect was highly significant, butthe group effect failed to reach significance p=0.024, asdid the group-by-consonant interaction. The results for nwere rather different: Neither of the main effects nor thegroup-by-consonant interaction approached significance. Asshown in Fig. 4 lower panel, the 10 year olds and womenstill had the expected pattern of higher values in /p/ comparedto /b/; the 5 year olds did not, and had a markedlyhigher standard deviation s.d. for /b/.To determine why n means and s.d.s differed so muchfrom those of in the 5 year olds, the two parameters used toobtain n from , namely closure duration To and the pressuredifference between release and closure Pd, were inspected.The results for pressure at closure Pc and releasePr were also reviewed to clarify the Pd data. The groupmeans and s.d.s for these four measures are given in TableIV.Table IV shows that the 5 year old’s high n s.d.s for /b/reflect variability in both Pd and To, but compared to womenthe s.d.s for Pd were proportionally higher than those for To1082 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 123, No. 2, February 2008 L. L. Koenig and J. C. Lucero: Voicing and intraoral pressure

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!