13.07.2015 Views

Three Faces of Identity - Majorsmatter.net

Three Faces of Identity - Majorsmatter.net

Three Faces of Identity - Majorsmatter.net

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.majority <strong>of</strong> work in sociology is on the “me” aspect<strong>of</strong> the self, which includes the self-conceptand the identities that are incorporated into it.Once the self emerges in the human organism,a nascent self-concept soon follows.Rosenberg (1979) defined self-concept as thetotality <strong>of</strong> a specific person’s thoughts andfeelings toward him- or herself as an object <strong>of</strong>reflection. The self-concept (or the “me”) canbe thought to consist <strong>of</strong> three broad classes<strong>of</strong> individual attributes (Rosenberg 1979,pp. 15–17): self-referring dispositions, physicalcharacteristics, and identities. Self-referringdispositions denote the abstract categoriespeople develop over their life courses andthen use to shape their response tendencies,including attitudes such as liberalism, traitssuch as altruism, values such as patriotism,and abilities such as athletic skill. Thoits &Virshup (1997) refer to these elements as one’sindividual identity because they represent waysto differentiate oneself from others (as opposedto representing communalities with othersthrough roles or group membership).Physical characteristics include one’s externalattributes such as being obese, deaf, or tall.These physical characteristics become sociologicallyinteresting when they are also incorporatedinto a person’s self-image and thus havethe potential to shape one’s behavior or one’ssocial and psychological well-being. They also,<strong>of</strong> course, have an external character that influenceshow others respond to us, shaping theirinternalization into the self-concept.The third component <strong>of</strong> Rosenberg’sself-concept is the focus <strong>of</strong> our review here:identity. There are four key sources <strong>of</strong> identitycharacterizations: personal or individualidentity, role-based identity, category-basedidentity, and group membership–based identity.Personal identity is the most elementarytype <strong>of</strong> identity, defined here as the socialclassification <strong>of</strong> an individual into a category<strong>of</strong> one (Rosenberg 1979). It denotes a uniqueindividual with self-descriptions drawn fromone’s own biography and singular constellation<strong>of</strong> experiences. Examples include: I am RoySmith, Pat Smith’s spouse; I served in Patton’sThird Army during World War II; I was bornin Robeson County Hospital at 3:14 PM; mymother had a cousin named Edna; I drovethe school bus during my junior year in highschool. Although personal identity consists <strong>of</strong>unique identifiers and an individual narrative,it is social and institutional in origin. As such,soldiers are identified and differentiated fromother soldiers by their names, ranks, and serialnumbers; academics by their names, ranks,departmental affiliations, and the institutionsfrom which their highest degree was awarded.These distinctions are created and organizedby the institutions within which they occur.As individualized as it is, one’s personal identityinformation is the basis for one’s otheridentities. “If an individual could not be recognizedfrom one occasion to another as the sameperson, no stable social relationships could beconstructed” and no other identities could beformed (McCall & Simmons 1966, p. 65). Wedistinguish between these individuated narrativesin personal identities and what Thoits& Virshup (1997) termed “individual identity.”They defined individual identity as selfideasabstracted from one’s biographical detailsand framed in terms <strong>of</strong> broader social categoriessuch as working class, Midwesterner, diabetic,snowbird, or progressive. When suchabstracted self-categorizations are internalized,they <strong>of</strong>ten correspond to group or categoricalidentities as described below.Identities based on role relationships arethe most central in the theories that stressinternalization <strong>of</strong> identity meaning into theself-structure. We define role-identity as a socialposition a person holds in a larger socialstructure, 1 considers self-descriptive, and enactsin a role relationship with at least oneother person (Thoits 1995). Because it is selfdescriptiveand internalized, it becomes part<strong>of</strong> one’s self-concept. Role-identities are predicatedon recurrent interactions between role1 Here we adopt Stryker’s (2008) definition <strong>of</strong> social structureas socially patterned interactions and relationships noted fortheir regularity, resistance to change, and capacity to reproducethemselves.www.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 479


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.before discussing the specific theories, webriefly address motivation as it generallyapplies to these internalization theories.Nearly six decades ago, Foote (1951) attemptedto clarify role theory by using a socialpsychology <strong>of</strong> motivation that both rejectedbiological determinism (“the person impelledfrom within”) and cultural determinism (“theperson driven from without”) (p. 21). Foote’stheory <strong>of</strong> situated motivation is based uponsymbolic interactionist notions <strong>of</strong> language andidentification. Specifically, language is centralto motivation because it helps shape behaviorby enabling individuals to meaningfully understandand label their past actions in order to formulatepresent and future outcomes. Throughidentification, people appropriate and committo particular identities. People have multipleidentities, and their identities give their behaviormeaning and purpose. However, becausepeople also perform roles that are attached toparticular identities, the role/identity nexus isalso an important ingredient in motivation andchoice. Indeed, the very notion <strong>of</strong> identity insymbolic interactionism connotes an intimatelinkage between self and role (Burke & Tully1977) such that human beings are confrontedregularly with choices between alternative commitmentsand actions.Two influential mid-range identity theorieswere developed in the 1960s to represent theways in which structural role positions and theirinternalizations guided choices that actors makein social interaction. Both McCall & Simmons(1966) and Stryker (1968) attempted to link thestructural-functional insights about role relationshipsand their functions in larger sociologythat dominated the mainstream sociology <strong>of</strong> theday with the dynamic, processual insights aboutself that dominated microsociological thinking.The result was a remarkably fruitful theoreticaltradition that grew out <strong>of</strong> these two similar perspectiveson identity.character that individuals devise for themselveswhen occupying specific social positions. Andas discussed earlier, they saw role-identities asstemming from the preferred perceptions thatone has <strong>of</strong> oneself as one occupies various socialpositions. In this case, role-identities influencepeople’s everyday lives by serving astheir primary source <strong>of</strong> personal action plans.The theory has a view <strong>of</strong> people capable <strong>of</strong> creativityand improvisation in the performance <strong>of</strong>their roles, yet within the overall requirementsand restrictions <strong>of</strong> their social position(s). Thiscommingling <strong>of</strong> individuality, idiosyncrasy, andimpulsiveness with behavior constrained by socialconvention occurs through a dialog betweenthe “I” and the “me” bounded by thebroad dictates <strong>of</strong> one’s role-identity.Because people have multiple and <strong>of</strong>tencompeting role-identities, which also come andgo during one’s life course, an important theoreticalproblem in McCall & Simmons’s theoryis to explain which role-identities people valuemost and will thus attempt to perform. Theyargue that a person’s various role-identitiesget organized into a hierarchy <strong>of</strong> prominence,where a role-identity’s prominence reflects therelative value it has for his or her overall conception<strong>of</strong> one’s ideal self. 2 In this way, one’s prominencehierarchy is equivalent to one’s ideal self(McCall & Simmons 1966, pp. 76–80, 264).Prominence itself is predicated on many factors.First, people must assess the degree <strong>of</strong> commitmentthey have to a particular role-identity and,by extension, how much their self-esteem isbound to its successful activation. That is, commitmentsignifies how deeply a person stakeswho he or she is by virtue <strong>of</strong> his or her roleidentityand its performance. This conceptualizationis directly relevant to James’s (1890)definition <strong>of</strong> self-esteem as the ratio <strong>of</strong> one’sperceived success in a particular role-identityto one’s desired level <strong>of</strong> success.Role-<strong>Identity</strong> TheoryMcCall & Simmons (1966) defined roleidentityin dramaturgical language as the2 Their concept <strong>of</strong> ideal self, which traces back to Horney(1945), is part wish and part obligation with regard to anindividual’s most personal aspirations and wants for him- orherself (e.g., to be a distinguished pr<strong>of</strong>essor, a loving mother,a successful businessperson).www.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 481


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.Second, when considering their ideal selvesvis-à-vis particular role-identities, people evaluatehow their prior actions generally comportwith their role-identity performances and howmuch they are living up to their ideal selves.Third, as a sign <strong>of</strong> the importance <strong>of</strong> reflectedappraisals, people try to determine how theirsignificant others will evaluate and appraise therole when and if it is activated. Fourth, individualsassess the rewards they may or may nothave received from the prior activation <strong>of</strong> a roleidentity,a notion consistent with social learningtheory (e.g., Bandura 1977).<strong>Identity</strong> TheoryStryker’s <strong>Identity</strong> Theory (1968, 1980, 2008)has been the dominant perspective on self andidentity within structural symbolic interactionismfor the past four decades. Since Stryker(2008) recently <strong>of</strong>fered a focused treatment <strong>of</strong>his theory in the Annual Review <strong>of</strong> Sociology, weonly review its basic form here (see also Owens2003). <strong>Identity</strong> Theory sees a multifaceted selfcomposed <strong>of</strong> multiple identities arranged hierarchicallyin an identity salience structure. Themore salient an identity, the higher is the probability<strong>of</strong> its being invoked in an interactionalsituation that allows some agency or choice.The salience itself is based on two dimensions <strong>of</strong>one’s commitment to the identity: interactionaland affective. Interactional commitment is theextensiveness <strong>of</strong> the interactions a person hasin a social <strong>net</strong>work through a particular identity(e.g., the number <strong>of</strong> persons one interactswith based on the identity). Affective commitmentis a person’s emotional investment in relationshipspremised on the identity (e.g., howemotionally close others in the role relationshipare to the individual). Note that Stryker’suse <strong>of</strong> the term “commitment” is more multidimensionaland less psychological than the use <strong>of</strong>the same concept label in McCall & Simmons’sRole-<strong>Identity</strong> Theory above.In an early empirical test <strong>of</strong> the theory,Serpe (1987) showed that college students’commitments to their student-related identitiesat Time 1 impacted those identities’ saliencesat Time 2, whereas Time 2 student-identitysalience impacted contemporaneous commitmentto that identity. This pattern suggestedthat commitment precedes salience. However,the degree <strong>of</strong> structural freedom students hadin choosing one role-identity over another(i.e., coursework versus dating) was key. Onlysituations involving choice showed the pattern.More recently, Owens & Serpe (2003) foundthat behavioral and affective commitment wereboth significantly related to family identitysalience for Hispanics, but only behavioralcommitment was predictive <strong>of</strong> family-identitysalience for Anglos and blacks. They arguedthat the significance <strong>of</strong> affective commitmentfor Hispanics was heightened by the family’sparticular importance for this group.Finally, Stryker et al. (2005) recentlydiscussed an ongoing problem in <strong>Identity</strong>Theory—how multilevel social structures facilitateor constrain one’s opportunity for,and the strength <strong>of</strong>, commitment to particularrole-identities (i.e., family, work, and volunteerism).They show that intermediate-levelsocial structures (e.g., neighborhood or school)influenced commitment most by fostering ingroupidentity-based relationships. Proximallevelsocial structures, though important, hadless impact than intermediate structures. Theproximal-level represents social embeddednessin multiple <strong>net</strong>works <strong>of</strong> social relations (e.g.,number <strong>of</strong> workmates who are also one’s relatives).Finally, large-scale stratification systems(e.g., gender, race, socioeconomic status) hadthe least impact on commitment to a particularidentity.<strong>Identity</strong> Accumulation TheoryThoits (1983, 1986, 2003) drew heavily onRole-<strong>Identity</strong> Theory (McCall & Simmons1966) and <strong>Identity</strong> Theory (Stryker 1968, 1980,2008) in her formulation <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> AccumulationTheory, her description <strong>of</strong> the importancethat multiple role-identities can have for a person’spsychological and emotional well-being.Her theory asserts that multiple role-identitiescan be psychological resources that help reduce482 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.emotional distress (depression) and fosterglobal self-esteem in complex selves. Underpinning<strong>Identity</strong> Accumulation Theory are twokey assumptions. The first, an essential aspect<strong>of</strong> symbolic interactionism, is that identitiesprovide individuals with meaning and purposeby answering the question: Who am I? Second,roles give individuals structure and organizationby answering the question: What shouldI do? By extension, multiple role-identitiesprovide the person with an orientation towardlife situations that help foster well-being. AsAhrens & Ryff (2006) point out, more rolesalso are associated with beneficial individualresources, such as social connections, power,and prestige. The more options one has toobtain these resources, the better the outcome.Numerous recent empirical studies supportthe hypothesis linking multiple role-identitiesto lower levels <strong>of</strong> psychological distress(Kikuzawa 2006, Sachs-Ericsson & Ciarlo2000, Wethington et al. 2000), fewer physicalhealth problems ( Janzen & Muhajarine 2003),or both (Bar<strong>net</strong>t & Hyde 2001). Conversely,Brook et al. (2008) cited other studies indicatingthat multiple role-identities canactually increase or prolong depression whenrole-identity demands are incompatible withother behavioral expectations or when the roleidentityclaims too much <strong>of</strong> a person’s time anddrains her energy. Brook et al. (2008) claimedthat the key to understanding the differencebetween positive and negative benefits <strong>of</strong> multipleroles is the mediating effect <strong>of</strong> positive ornegative emotions on the interactions betweenthe number <strong>of</strong> identities a person holds, theirsubjective importance to the person, and howharmoniously they interact with each other.Jackson (1997) added an important statusdimension involving race and ethnicity to<strong>Identity</strong> Accumulation Theory and showedthat the multiple role-identities were associatedwith lower depression and greater happinessamong non-Hispanic white and MexicanAmerican men and women but not AfricanAmericans. Kikuzawa (2006) added a person’slocation in the life course to race/ethnicity ina cross-cultural study <strong>of</strong> depression levels forAmericans and Japanese age 60 and over. Shefound that role accumulation (spouse, parent,and community volunteer) benefited olderAmericans’ mental health, but any role beyondbeing either a spouse or a parent had no impacton well-being for older Japanese. Theoreticalunderstanding <strong>of</strong> these varying patterns indifferent racial, ethnic, and age groups is stillneeded.A common thread running through manystudies <strong>of</strong> multiple identities, including thework above on <strong>Identity</strong> Accumulation Theory,is the importance <strong>of</strong> Stryker’s <strong>Identity</strong> Theoryin the framing <strong>of</strong> the empirical research. Few,however, actually employ Stryker’s conceptions<strong>of</strong> role interactional and affective commitmentin their empirical analyses. Even among thosewho try to hew closely to Stryker’s theoreticalconceptualizations, differences in the waysalience and commitment are operationalizedhelp explain empirical inconsistencies in the relationshipbetween multiple role-identities andwell-being.Serpe (1987) and Thoits (2003) may <strong>of</strong>fer animportant piece missing in <strong>Identity</strong> AccumulationTheory: assessing the degree <strong>of</strong> choice peoplehave in enacting their many role-identities.If multiple role-identities are a resource, thenthe most consequential for well-being shouldbe the identities a person has some freedomto choose. For example, the mother role andworker role are more constraining because they<strong>of</strong>fer less choice in enacting than the freedomto choose the PTA role and church choir role.<strong>Identity</strong> Control TheoryWhereas Stryker’s <strong>Identity</strong> Theory focusedon identity choices, Burke developed anelaboration <strong>of</strong> the theory to specify howinternalized meanings guided action after anidentity was adopted within an institutionalcontext (Stryker & Burke 2000). Burke’s (1991,Burke & Reitzes 1991) theoretical developmentsare <strong>of</strong>ten known as <strong>Identity</strong> ControlTheory to distinguish them from Stryker’smore structural focus on commitment andidentity enactment. Burke built on the workwww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 483


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.<strong>of</strong> Powers (1973) to represent the relationshipbetween internalized identity meanings andperceptions <strong>of</strong> an interactional situation as acontrol system or cyber<strong>net</strong>ic feedback loop[Robinson (2007) provides a recent review <strong>of</strong>control theorizing in sociology]. Meaningsare typically measured using bipolar semanticdifferential scales (Osgood et al. 1957, 1975).The dimensions on which meanings vary areassessed in each institutional context and thenmeasured at the level <strong>of</strong> the individual actor.Consistent with the other internalizationidentity theories summarized above, <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory emphasizes the importance <strong>of</strong>understanding identity not just as a state ortrait characteristic <strong>of</strong> the individual, but as acontinuous process <strong>of</strong> affirmation and reaffirmationin social situations. “An identity process,”according to Burke & Reitzes (1991), “isa continuously operating, self-adjusting, feedbackloop: individuals continually adjust behavior tokeep their reflected appraisals congruent withtheir identity standards or references” (p. 840,emphasis in the original). Furthermore, Burkeand associates follow mainstream symbolic interactionismby viewing an identity as a set <strong>of</strong>meanings applied to the self in a social role[although later presentations <strong>of</strong> the theory explicitlyexpand its scope to consider identitiesthat come from group or category membership(e.g., Burke 2004, Burke & Stets 2009)]. Thesemeaning-sets act as a standard or reference forunderstanding who one is in a given situationand what are the expectations in order to maintainthat identity in the eyes <strong>of</strong> self and others.When an identity is brought into play in aninteraction situation, a feedback process ensuesvia reflected appraisals. That is, a cyber<strong>net</strong>iccontrol process balances any identity discrepanciesthat may arise when a particular identitystandard (the internalized meanings <strong>of</strong> self inrole, group, or category) is compared to theidentity’s situated meaning via a comparator.It is hypothesized that people behave so as tocounter and reduce any discrepancy that arisesfrom interaction. Since actions are generatedto reduce discrepancies, they can vary inquality, depending on the disturbing events.This feature <strong>of</strong> the theory allows it to describehow actors respond creatively to circumstancesother than normal role performances.<strong>Identity</strong> Control Theory is thoroughlyreviewed in Burke & Stets (2009). Here wedescribe only a few studies as exemplars <strong>of</strong>this research tradition. In one <strong>of</strong> the initialstatements <strong>of</strong> the theory, Burke (1991) poseda challenge to prevailing views <strong>of</strong> social stressby positing that disruption <strong>of</strong> the otherwisecontinuous identity process and the inability toclose the gap between disapproving reflectedappraisals and an identity standard will resultin distress. Burke & Stets (1999) show howself-processes can influence social structure,while Stets & Tsushima (2001) have extendedthe theory to an examination <strong>of</strong> the moderatinginfluence <strong>of</strong> group-based identities and rolebasedidentities on how people experience andcope with the potent emotion <strong>of</strong> anger. Morerecently, Burke et al. (2007) combined <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory, Status Characteristics Theory(discussed briefly at the end <strong>of</strong> the next section),and Legitimacy Theory in an experiment onhow gender factors into leader and subordinateidentity verification. They studied taskorientedgroups <strong>of</strong> four people (two womenand two men) when leadership is conferred by ahigher, legitimate authority (the experimenter).Among their findings are that female leaders(who were legitimated by the experimenterwhen the group was formed) and males (whowere not legitimated) both had higher levels<strong>of</strong> identity verification than others. This studyillustrates an interesting new theme in <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory—the relevance <strong>of</strong> resources tothe ability <strong>of</strong> an actor to verify his or her identity(see Burke & Stets 2009, pp. 79–82, 229–33).This emphasis on a larger structure brings thetheory closer to the situational/cultural emphasis<strong>of</strong> the theories reviewed in the next section.THEORIES EMPHASIZINGCULTURE AND SITUATIONALCONTEXTPresumably, a “definition <strong>of</strong> the situation” is almostalways to be found, but those who are in484 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.the situation ordinarily do not create this definition,even though their society can be said to do so;ordinarily, all they do is to assess correctly whatthe situation ought to be for them and then actaccordingly.—Erving G<strong>of</strong>fman (1974, pp. 1–2)The theories <strong>of</strong> internalized identity meaningsreviewed in the previous section build on thefoundational insight <strong>of</strong> Mead and Cooley thatwe incorporate the social positions that we occupyinto our cognitive image <strong>of</strong> ourselves aspeople. Here, we shift attention to theories thatgive priority to situational, social structural, andcultural elements that lie outside the individual.We caution that our categorization heredoes not imply competition or contradiction.Instead, theories (and the conceptualizationsembedded within them) necessarily simplify realityby focusing on some elements to the exclusion<strong>of</strong> others. So the theories described here donot reject the idea that social roles, group memberships,and category memberships are incorporatedinto the self-image. Rather, they emphasizehow the elements <strong>of</strong> situations in whichactors are involved shape their behavioral, cognitive,and emotional reactions, rather than focuson the intraindividual features <strong>of</strong> identity inthe self-structure that are carried from situationto situation.This tradition <strong>of</strong> emphasizing the importance<strong>of</strong> situational context for social actionbegan to develop in its modern form duringthe 1950s and early 1960s. It was partially aresponse to the sense that symbolic interactionism,and sociological social psychologymore generally, had lost its connection to thestructural focus <strong>of</strong> the larger discipline (Stryker2008). Foote’s (1951) influential theory <strong>of</strong> situatedmotivation, discussed above, was publishedin this period, using the concept <strong>of</strong> identity t<strong>of</strong>ocus attention on the actor-situation nexus.G<strong>of</strong>fman (1959, 1963) emphasized that actorspresented themselves to others in a mannerthat served to maintain certain images. HisPresentation <strong>of</strong> Self in Everyday Life was concernedmore with the interactional presentation<strong>of</strong> self and how situational context supported orundermined it than with the underlying character<strong>of</strong> the “self” that was being presented [seeHochschild (1983, appendix A) for a relateddiscussion]. Similar intellectual movementswere afoot in psychology, as Heider’s (1946,1958) Balance Theory drew attention to configurations<strong>of</strong> interaction and their implications.Situated <strong>Identity</strong> TheoryAn early attempt to develop a formal, empiricallytestable theory based on this situationalfocus was Alexander’s Situated <strong>Identity</strong> Theory(Alexander & Knight 1971; summarized inAlexander & Wiley 1981). Building on Heiderand G<strong>of</strong>fman, Alexander conceptualized situatedidentities not as properties possessed bypersons or as features located in some externalenvironmental structure. Instead, the situatedidentity defined the relationship betweenan individual and the environment (especiallythe other actors within it) at a given time.Like many situational theories that followed,Alexander focused initially on relatively simplesituations in which actors were assumed tohave similar perspectives; he restricted his experimentalwork to situated activities that metthat consensus criterion. In his most cited study(Alexander & Knight 1971), he replicated a classiccognitive dissonance experiment, showingthat the desirability <strong>of</strong> the situated identity associatedwith an outcome predicted whetherpeople chose that outcome, <strong>of</strong>ten in violation<strong>of</strong> cognitive dissonance predictions. Althoughthis research tradition is not currently very active,it was a groundbreaking effort to considersystematically the impact <strong>of</strong> situational cues onidentity occupancy and social conduct flowingfrom that identity. It also pioneered the use<strong>of</strong> experimental methods in this domain withinsociology.We now move to a review <strong>of</strong> currentlyactive theories that emphasize the impact <strong>of</strong> situationaland cultural features on social interaction.We discuss two traditions in sociology andpsychology—Affect Control Theory and Social<strong>Identity</strong> Theory—that conceptualize the socioculturalenvironment in very different ways.www.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 485


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.We then briefly discuss Status CharacteristicsTheory. Although this theoretical researchprogram is not based in the identity tradition,its findings have some important connectionsto the phenomena that we explore here.Affect Control TheoryTo make the connection between the internalizedand situational/cultural conceptions <strong>of</strong>identity clear, we begin our discussion <strong>of</strong> themore situational/cultural conception <strong>of</strong> identitywith Affect Control Theory (Heise 1979, 2007),which shares many elements with Burke’s <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory. Both theories drew directlyon Powers’s (1973) insight that much <strong>of</strong>human processing <strong>of</strong> stimuli from the environmentwas described by a control system ratherthan a direct translation <strong>of</strong> inputs into outputs.But even given this shared control imagery, thesubtle differences between the theories are anexcellent demonstration <strong>of</strong> the distinction thatwe make here between personal and social theories<strong>of</strong> identity. They also illustrate the danger<strong>of</strong> making this categorization seem too rigid.Affect Control Theory was developed byHeise (1979) by combining two psychologicalresearch programs—Osgood’s work on themeasurement <strong>of</strong> affective meaning (Osgoodet al. 1957, 1975) and Gollob’s (1968) workon impression formation—with Powers’s newideas about control systems. The resultingtheory had three elements. First, it usedOsgood’s work to create a measurementsystem in which meanings were conceptualizedon three dimensions <strong>of</strong> affectivemeaning—evaluation (good-bad), potency(powerful-powerless), and activity (livelyquiet).Measuring meaning on just these threedimensions missed some nuances but allowedall the elements <strong>of</strong> a social event (the actor, thesocial action, the object-person at whom theaction is directed, and later emotions, setting,nonverbal behaviors, status characteristics, etc.)to be mapped into the same three-dimensionalspace. The second element <strong>of</strong> the theory was anempirical framework for describing how eventschanged the meanings within a situation. Usinga paradigm developed by Gollob, Heise combinedidentity labels and social behaviors intosimple event sentences (e.g., the mother slapsthe child) to study how the initial meanings <strong>of</strong>the event elements (a mother, slapping someone,a child) would be transformed by theircombination in the event (what do you think—on the three dimensions <strong>of</strong> affective meaning—<strong>of</strong> a mother who has slapped a child?).The third element <strong>of</strong> the theory is thecontrol system, adapted from Powers (1973),which it shares with <strong>Identity</strong> Control Theory.When social interaction deflects meanings awayfrom their stable, culturally determined values,Affect Control Theory proposes that actors tryto create new events to bring the transitory,situated meanings back into line with the stable,cultural meanings. Deflection <strong>of</strong> meaningsis defined mathematically, by the summed absolutedifferences between transient, situatedmeanings for event elements (actor, behavior,and object-person) and the culturally given referencevalues for each <strong>of</strong> those elements. Thesearch for a new event that, when enacted, willminimize that deflection then becomes a searchfor a three-number pr<strong>of</strong>ile: the evaluation, potency,and activity <strong>of</strong> a behavior (what could Xdo next to remedy the situation?), a new labelfor the actor (what kind <strong>of</strong> a person would dosuch a thing?), or a new label for the objectperson(what kind <strong>of</strong> a person would deserve tohave that happen to them?). Once the pr<strong>of</strong>ile isfound, one can search a cultural “dictionary” <strong>of</strong>affective meanings to find qualitative labels thatfit the affectively appropriate response.The contrast between Affect Control Theoryand the more recently developed <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory is described extensivelyin Smith-Lovin & Robinson (2006). Thetheories differ in their approaches to measurement,formalization, and emotional response.Here we emphasize only the differences in personalversus situational focus that are key to ourdiscussion.The first contrast between the theories is thereference level that is being maintained by thecontrol system in each theory. <strong>Identity</strong> ControlTheory’s reference level is the internalized486 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.meanings that an actor incorporates into theself-structure. In Affect Control Theory, peopletry to maintain meanings associated withthe entire situation—their own identities, theidentities <strong>of</strong> others, actions, and behavior settings(Smith-Lovin 1979). A second differenceflows directly from the first. In Affect ControlTheory, the actions <strong>of</strong> others are explicitlyconsidered as possible remedies for meaningdeflectingevents. In personal identity theories,the assumption is that people operate to maintaintheir own identities. Affect Control Theoryexplicitly considers how a situation in whichmeanings have been disrupted can be repairedby any <strong>of</strong> the actors that are copresent in a situation,even those who were not involved in thedeflecting event.Finally, the theories differ somewhat in theirassumptions about the stability <strong>of</strong> meaning.Since <strong>Identity</strong> Control Theory focuses on personal,internalized identity meanings, much <strong>of</strong>this research program looks at how these meaningsshift as a result <strong>of</strong> interactions over aperiod <strong>of</strong> sustained interactions (e.g., Stets &Burke 2005). In Affect Control Theory, meaningsare assumed to be quite stable and acquiredthrough long-term socialization processes fromthe culture at large. Most extreme dislocations<strong>of</strong> meaning that cannot be resolved behaviorallyare resolved through relabeling a person or action.Since both theories share the core controlimagery <strong>of</strong> Powers (1973), much research thatsupports Affect Control Theory could supporteither theory (e.g., Robinson & Smith-Lovin1992, 1999), as well as self-consistency theoriesfrom psychology. Work that is more distinctiveto this theoretical tradition looks at featuresother than the central actor. For example,one series <strong>of</strong> studies examines how emotion displays(which signal transient meanings and deflectionsfrom identity) lead to labeling after adeviant act (e.g., Robinson et al. 1994). Anotherlooks at how people work to manage the identities<strong>of</strong> others, even at some cost to their ownself-image (C.L. Rogalin, D.T. Robinson, L.Smith-Lovin, submitted manuscript).Since Affect Control Theory focuses on thesituation, it <strong>of</strong>ten deals with identities that arenot core to an actor’s self-structure. Smith-Lovin (2007) reported an experiential samplingstudy that found we actually spend most <strong>of</strong> ourtime in such noncore identities. In fact, AffectControl Theory had no real conceptualization<strong>of</strong> a stable, organized self-structure during thefirst three decades <strong>of</strong> its development (Heise1979, Smith-Lovin & Heise 1988, MacKinnon1994); it was a theory about roles, identities, andsituated action.Recent developments, however, make clearthat the boundary between personal and socialidentity theories is indistinct and everchanging.MacKinnon & Heise (2010) developa control theory <strong>of</strong> the self that is based on AffectControl Theory. The theory has two elements.First, actors use a “cultural theory <strong>of</strong> people”—the collection <strong>of</strong> categories and logical implicationsamong them—as a menu <strong>of</strong> possibilitiesfor self-identification and understanding others.Second, when self-identity meanings aredeflected in one situation (and if those deflectionscannot easily be behaviorally resolved),then actors seek out other possible identitiesin situations that, when maintained, will resolvethe deflection at the level <strong>of</strong> the self. Therefore,although it uses the mathematical control systemfrom Affect Control Theory, this new identitytheory clearly moves back into the realm <strong>of</strong>internalized, personal identity theories by concentratingon how people maintain a stable selfimageover a series <strong>of</strong> situational encounters. Inthis case, they do so by agentically entering situationsthat place them in different identitiesand lead to the experience <strong>of</strong> self-relevant situatedmeanings. Notice the similarity to the basicquestion posed (and answered) by Stryker’s<strong>Identity</strong> Theory: How do people choose whichidentities to enact, when they have a choice?MacKinnon and Heise give a more formal answerto this question than Stryker, but they aredefinitely building in his domain.Having discussed how two very similar theoriesdiffer in their emphasis on personal versussocial/cultural focus, we now turn to a perspectivethat focuses on how contextual elementsaffect self-meanings that are derived from categorymemberships. Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theorywww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 487


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.deals with cognitions about categories <strong>of</strong> ingroupsand out-groups (Tajfel & Turner 1979).Therefore, it would seem to be the prototype <strong>of</strong>an intrapersonal identity theory. However, themechanisms described by the theory actuallycenter on the impact <strong>of</strong> the social environmenton those cognitions, placing this theory in thesituational/cultural category for our treatmenthere. We emphasize those contextual elements<strong>of</strong> the theory in our description <strong>of</strong> it. 3Social <strong>Identity</strong> TheorySocial <strong>Identity</strong> Theory has its roots in work byTajfel during the late 1950s and 1960s on thesocial factors that influence perception. It wasfurther developed with Turner in the 1970s and1980s (Tajfel & Turner 1979, Turner & Tajfel1982; see also Hogg 1992). Like all the theorieswe review here, Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theorysees social actors as having multiple identitiesthat get activated by different social contexts.Here, the emphasis is on category memberships(e.g., being a Muslim or an Australian). Whena category membership becomes salient (relevantto the social context), self-perception andconduct become in-group normative. Perceptions<strong>of</strong> other groups become out-group stereotypical.Depending on the nature <strong>of</strong> the relationshipbetween the groups (e.g., whether it iscompetitive or status-ranked), self- and otherperceptionscan shift toward different types <strong>of</strong>perceptual discrimination.Unlike the control theories <strong>of</strong> identityreviewed above, Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theoryassumes a self-enhancement motive. Afterself-categorization occurs in the context <strong>of</strong> asituation, actors are motivated to make comparisonsthat favor the in-group (and sometimesdisparage the out-group). The strength <strong>of</strong> thistendency varies, depending on the potential formobility—it is stronger for immutable ascribedcategorizations (e.g., race, gender, nationality)3 Other treatments (e.g., Stets & Burke 2000) have emphasizedpoints <strong>of</strong> overlap between Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theory inpsychology and <strong>Identity</strong> Theory in sociology.than it is for achieved categorizations (e.g., educationaldegree status, membership on sportsteams), especially if the higher status is withinreach. Therefore, it is the social environmentand, in particular, the relevant group contrastswithin that environment that determinewhat dimensions <strong>of</strong> self-perception, otherstereotyping,and intergroup competition/discrimination occur. If I think <strong>of</strong> myself as anAmerican and I am in a social context wherethe British are my out-group, I might think<strong>of</strong> my American self as egalitarian (as opposedto the class-oriented British). But if I am (still)an American and I am comparing myself toan Italian, I might think <strong>of</strong> myself as orderlyand institution-upholding (as opposed to theunsettled, fractious Italians). The hypotheses<strong>of</strong> Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theory typically focus onhow variations in the social situation (therelevant out-groups, the beliefs about thedegree <strong>of</strong> mobility and social change possible,etc.) influence the content <strong>of</strong> the operativemeanings for the in-group (i.e., situated selfmeanings) and the out-group (stereotypes anddiscrimination).Notice that in Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theory, theconcept <strong>of</strong> salience has a very different meaningfrom that in <strong>Identity</strong> Theory. In Stryker’s <strong>Identity</strong>Theory, identity salience is a stable part <strong>of</strong>the self—a result <strong>of</strong> commitment (frequent andaffectively valued relationships to others). It issomething that actors carry from situation tosituation. In Social <strong>Identity</strong> Theory, salience isconceptualized as the impact <strong>of</strong> the situation onself-categorizations. I might be an American inParis, but at a Civil War reenactment, I am aSoutherner, and in the American SociologicalAssociation, I am a pr<strong>of</strong>essor and a socialpsychologist. And in each <strong>of</strong> these venues,I might have different meanings for thosecategorical identities, depending on the salientout-group at the time. In comparing Tajfel’sSocial <strong>Identity</strong> Theory and Stryker’s <strong>Identity</strong>Theory, Hogg et al. (1995, p. 263) notedthat Tajfel’s theory put more emphasis onsocial context for identity and their meanings,whereas the sociological <strong>Identity</strong> Theoryfocused on self-structure for motivating the488 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.enactment <strong>of</strong> identities and long-term socialexperience for the internalized meanings.To end our section on situational/culturaltheories <strong>of</strong> identity, we turn to a theory thatis not considered an identity theory at all—theStatus Characteristics Theory branch <strong>of</strong> the expectationstates theoretical research program.We include the theory in this review not tocover all <strong>of</strong> its rich contributions to microsociology,but to highlight the elements <strong>of</strong> thetheory that deal with the relationships amongidentity and interaction. The increasing use <strong>of</strong>the theory outside <strong>of</strong> its scope conditions byidentity researchers justifies its placement here.Status Characteristics TheoryThe intellectual roots <strong>of</strong> Status CharacteristicsTheory are not in symbolic interaction, but inthe exchange tradition (Correll & Ridgeway2003). When people work together on a taskand will be rewarded jointly for its successfulcompletion (the scope conditions <strong>of</strong> the theory),actors are motivated to assess who willcontribute most effectively to the task completion.The actors for whom task expectationsare the highest are given action opportunities,receive positive evaluations, and have other interactionaladvantages. Actors’ characteristics(e.g., gender, race) become important withina situation when those characteristics areevaluated by self and others, and are perceived(either implicitly or explicitly) to be relevantto expectations about task performance. Thetheory’s core mechanism is that group memberswill exchange deference within the groupinteraction for the high-expectation actors’contributions to the task (resulting in higherbenefits for all group members, including thelower-expectation ones).Note, however, the similarities betweenStatus Characteristics Theory and Affect ControlTheory (Ridgeway & Smith-Lovin 1994).Both theories posit that largely consensualcultural meanings are imported into a situationto organize action within that setting. Boththeories suggest that the identities (and, therefore,cultural meanings) that are relevant aredetermined by the situation, not by an internalself-structure. And both provide a generativeaccount <strong>of</strong> how action unfolds in the situation,given these meanings. The fact that StatusCharacteristics Theory is frequently nowapplied outside <strong>of</strong> its scope conditions <strong>of</strong> collaborative,jointly rewarded task orientation—which nullifies the exchange mechanism—makes it closer to a situationally based identitytheory than the status-exchange theory <strong>of</strong> itsorigins. Researchers in both the Affect ControlTheory and the <strong>Identity</strong> Control Theory traditionshave used the theory to discuss how statusmeanings organize action within social situations(Ridgeway & Smith-Lovin 1994, Rashotte& Smith-Lovin 1997, Stets 1997, Stets &Harrod 2004, Cast et al. 1999). For example,Stets & Harrod (2004) find that actors inhigher-status positions have interactionalresources that allow them to sustain theiridentity meanings within situations, avoidingnegative emotions that come with lack <strong>of</strong>identity maintenance.Having reviewed the systematically developedtheories <strong>of</strong> identity and action thatemphasize internalized meanings and situational/culturalelements, respectively, we nowturn to a body <strong>of</strong> literature on collective identitythat has a very different emphasis. Indeed,a review <strong>of</strong> this literature a decade ago(Cerulo 1997) barely mentions social psychologicaltheories. Here, we try to summarizethe differing emphasis <strong>of</strong> this substantial literatureon collective identity and to draw outconnections between its treatment <strong>of</strong> identityand the social psychological literature reviewedabove.COLLECTIVE IDENTITYThe solidarity that derives from similarities is at itsmaximum when the collective consciousness completelyenvelopes our total consciousness, coincidingwith it at every point.—Emile Durkheim (1893)The sociological literature on collective identityis less organized around distinct midrangewww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 489


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.theories than are the literatures on personalidentity and situated identity. Rather, it is awide-ranging literature coming out <strong>of</strong> a number<strong>of</strong> traditions, including cultural sociology,social movements theory, feminist sociology,and cognitive sociology. Consequently,this section <strong>of</strong> our review is organized aroundconcepts and process rather than around specifictheoretical traditions. We attempt to drawconnections between this vibrant literatureand the midrange microsociological theoriesreviewed above.The internal and situational identity processesreviewed in the section above can leadidentities to serve as an organizational force,binding us to those with commonalities <strong>of</strong> interestand providing a social glue that can serveas a foundation for mobilizing joint action. Collectiveidentities should not be seen as a thirdtype <strong>of</strong> identity as much as an attempt by thisliterature to highlight another set <strong>of</strong> identityrelateddynamics at the group level. Both internalizedidentities and situational identities canunderpin the sense <strong>of</strong> connection and shareddestiny requisite for collective identification.Indeed, Nagel (1995, p. 21) refers to the collectivevariant <strong>of</strong> ethnic identity as “a dialecticbetween internal identification and externalascription.”Most definitions <strong>of</strong> collective identityinclude a notion <strong>of</strong> identification with sharedfeatures along with a recognition <strong>of</strong> sharedopportunities and constraints afforded by thosefeatures (Melucci 1989). Taylor & Whittier(1999, p. 170) define collective identity as the“shared definition <strong>of</strong> a group that derives frommembers’ common interests, experiences, andsolidarity.” A sense <strong>of</strong> we-ness, or connectionto other members <strong>of</strong> the group/category, isan essential component <strong>of</strong> collective identity,but the concept goes far beyond that. Prenticeet al. (1994) specifically distinguish betweengroup identities based on common bonds(attachments to individual group members)and those based on common identities (attachmentsdirectly to the group or category).The latter form <strong>of</strong> attachment is necessary toproduce a collective identity.Psychologists working in the social identityand self-categorization theoretical traditionsrefer to the “collective self” as nearly interchangeablewith “social identity” as used inthose theories (Brewer & Gardner 1996) andthus focus largely on its consequences forself-definition and interpersonal judgment.Sociological use <strong>of</strong> the term collective identityfocuses more on its consequences for mobilizingjoint action. This is closer to what Heise(1998) referred to as empathic solidarity. Heise(1998, p. 197) defines empathic solidarity as“a reciprocated sense <strong>of</strong> merged consciousnessand alliance, with faith in others’ commitmentsto shared purposes.” Heise argues that whenpeople take on the same identity, experiencethe same reality, and observe one another’sparallel emotions and collateral behaviors,a sense <strong>of</strong> common destiny and empathicconnection arises. It is this phenomenon,operating at the group level, that makes thesociological literature on collective identitydistinctive from the microsociological identityliterature that we review above.Collective <strong>Identity</strong> and theNew Social MovementsA concern with understanding what mobilizesjoint action motivates much <strong>of</strong> sociology’s attentionto collective identity. Collective identityis a central organizing concept in the literatureon new social movements. New socialmovement theory (Laraña et al. 1994; Melucci1989, 1994; Offe 1985; Turner 1969) juxtaposesmodern (post-1960s) movements with earliersocial movements, arguing that contemporarymovements are less about Marxist-style conflictover material interests and more concernedwith identity meanings and other symbolic resources.For example, Turner (1969) relatedprotest participation to identity dynamics andreferred to the emerging number <strong>of</strong> “identityseeking” movements. The identity dynamicsdescribed by Turner focused on personal identitiesand the identification/disidentificationwith various groups. Later researchers refinedthe notion <strong>of</strong> collective identity as a group-level490 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.phenomenon (Melucci 1989, 1994) central togiving a group or category the coherence andenergy necessary to mobilize its constituentsinto collective action. Nearly a decade ago,Polletta & Jasper (2001) reviewed the literatureon collective identity and social movements, revealinghow identity processes are deeply relatedto all aspects <strong>of</strong> social movements, including(a) movement emergence, (b) recruitmentand participation, (c) movement strategy, and(d) interpretation <strong>of</strong> outcomes (see Stryker et al.2000).Boundary WorkTaylor & Whittier (1999) describe three factorsthat contribute to the development <strong>of</strong> a collectiveidentity capable <strong>of</strong> motivating group-levelaction: (a) the creation <strong>of</strong> social boundaries,(b) the development and recognition <strong>of</strong> socialcriteria that account for a group’s structural position,and (c) negotiation <strong>of</strong> intergroup and intragroupmeanings. Boundaries identify who isand is not a member <strong>of</strong> a collective, but it isgroup consciousness that gives significance to acollectivity (Taylor & Whittier 1999, p. 179).In the case <strong>of</strong> mobilizing collective consciousnessin response to a dominant group or oppressiveunderstanding, this significance takes theform <strong>of</strong> what Morris (1999) calls oppositionalconsciousness. Strategies and practices used bycollectives to create, maintain, and transformcultural categories are collectively referred toas boundary work (Nippert-Eng 2002).Gamson (1995) points out that fixed categoriesare the basis for both oppression andpolitical power. Consequently, boundary workregarding collective identities such as race,gender, and sexuality can sometimes vacillatebetween taking on the goal <strong>of</strong> deconstructingboundaries (e.g., Lorber 2006) and takingon the goal <strong>of</strong> fixing the boundaries tomobilize on the basis <strong>of</strong> them (e.g., Jenson1995).Within this theoretical tradition, Lamont(1992) <strong>of</strong>fered an in-depth analysis <strong>of</strong> the symbolicboundaries people draw when categorizingself and others in her comparative study <strong>of</strong>upper-middle-class culture in France and theUnited States. Her work suggests that boundarywork around collective identities operatesmultidimensionally and that the relative prominence<strong>of</strong> those dimensions can vary betweencultural settings (echoing some themes in Social<strong>Identity</strong> Theory). Her analysis distinguishedbetween three types <strong>of</strong> symbolic boundaries:moral, socioeconomic, and cultural. Accordingto Lamont’s research, French social and culturalspecialists draw stronger cultural boundaries,while social and cultural specialists inthe United States attend more to economicboundaries. More specifically, Lamont foundthat boundary-drawing activities in the UnitedStates vary with the degree to which they areembedded in occupational structures that aredependent on pr<strong>of</strong>it-making. U.S. social andcultural specialists in the nonpr<strong>of</strong>it sector feltmore similar to, and connected with, other nonpr<strong>of</strong>itworkers than to other social and culturalspecialists who were dependent on pr<strong>of</strong>itmaking.In contrast, French social and culturalspecialists identified more with the intellectualand cultural boundaries around their vocationsand less with the economic aspects.The boundary work literature focusesprimarily on the strategies and practices thatgroups use to construct collective identitiesand to manage the symbolic boundaries aroundthose identities. However, theorists have alsoattended to how external structures createand constrain the emergence and salience <strong>of</strong>collective identities. Again, this work picksup some themes emphasized in the sociocultural/situationaltheories <strong>of</strong> identity inmicrosociology.Competition and ContactTwo opposing logics guide the theoretical literatureregarding the impact <strong>of</strong> external forceson the salience <strong>of</strong> collective identity boundariesand the strength <strong>of</strong> collective identity bonds.One is that direct competition strengthensgroup boundaries and mobilizes collectiveidentity-based conflict. Competition theory(Olzak 1994) argues that when economic anddemographic changes lead to a breakdown inlabor market segmentation, the increase inwww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 491


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.intergroup interaction and the intensification<strong>of</strong> competition for scarce resources strain intergrouprelations and, consequently, increasecollective identification—particularly ethnicidentification. Olzak and colleagues haveamassed considerable support for the idea thatincreased economic competition leads to surgesin ethnic protests and collective action (Olzaket al. 1994, 1996; Soule 1992). Nagel (1995),on the other hand, found that cultural renewalamong Native Americans has taken place underexactly the kinds <strong>of</strong> conditions that are thoughtto produce cultural decline. One argument isthat positive economic conditions set the stagefor this renewal. According to Nagel, structuralexternalities such as successful land claims,increases in federal spending, and minorityset aside programs have increased the symbolicand material value <strong>of</strong> Native Americanidentity.In contrast to the idea that increasedinteraction and competition foster collectiveidentification, the cultural division <strong>of</strong> labortheory proposes that group solidarity andcollective identity arise when groups aredistinctively positioned in an occupationalstructure on the basis <strong>of</strong> cultural markers(Hechter 1978, 2000). Occupational segregationincreases intragroup interaction relativeto intergroup interaction and consequentlyincreases commonality <strong>of</strong> interests and futures.Thus, when cultural and economic boundariesoverlap, collective identity will arise on the basis<strong>of</strong> cultural, rather than economic, similarity.Okamoto (2003) extended and synthesizedthese contrasting arguments into a theory aboutthe shifting, layered nature <strong>of</strong> ethnic identities.In a study <strong>of</strong> the relationship betweeneconomic competition, cultural division <strong>of</strong> labor,and pan-Asian identity, Okamoto foundthat the collective identity boundaries trackedthe patterns <strong>of</strong> occupational segregation: WhenAsian ethnic groups occupy a shared place inthe occupational market, pan-Asian identitiesare more likely to emerge and mobilize pa<strong>net</strong>hniccollective action. When separate Asia<strong>net</strong>hnic groups are segregated into different occupations,the separate ethnic identities remainsalient and pan-ethnic collective action is suppressed.Nested IdentitiesAs Okamoto’s work highlights, boundary workby collectives is not simply a matter <strong>of</strong> sharpeningcontrasts between competing groups or emphasizingdistinctiveness from a larger oppressiveculture. At times collective identificationis a process <strong>of</strong> crystallizing subgroup boundariesthat fractionalize a larger whole, or meldingsubgroups into larger, cohesive collectives.Green (1999) describes four groups <strong>of</strong> sectarianevangelical Protestants whose efforts animatedthe first wave <strong>of</strong> a Christian Right movement,but whose intergroup conflicts ultimately contributedto the decline <strong>of</strong> the movement. Thesuccess <strong>of</strong> a second wave <strong>of</strong> the Christian Rightmovement, according to Green, was facilitatedby the development <strong>of</strong> a new collective identityfor these groups.Similarly, in her study <strong>of</strong> the emergence <strong>of</strong>a pan-tribal Native American identity, Nagel(1995) described the process by which subgroupscan join to form a larger common identity.She referred to ethnic renewal as theprocess by which new ethnic identities arebuilt/rebuilt out <strong>of</strong> historical social and symbolicsystems. Among Native Americans, ashared history <strong>of</strong> discrimination and oppression,combined with some <strong>of</strong> the positive economicexternalities described above, created acommon fate among ethnic groups with otherwisedistinctive cultural histories. These developmentsfacilitated the collective processes(e.g., institution building via the establishment<strong>of</strong> new organizations and religions) and culturalpractices (development <strong>of</strong> new shared ritualsand symbols) involved in ethnic renewal.<strong>Identity</strong>, Emotions, and MobilizationClose on the heels <strong>of</strong> renewed attention tothe role <strong>of</strong> identity in social movements hasbeen a revival <strong>of</strong> interest in how emotionsmobilize joint action. Scholars have recentlybecome more focused on how emotions create492 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.solidarity and energize collective bonds (Britt& Heise 2000, Collins 1990, Gould 2004,Heise 1998, Jasper 1998). Britt & Heise (2000)point out that shared emotion is not enoughto generate collective identification and action.Using arguments from Affect Control Theory,Britt & Heise (2000) argued that negative emotionsthat are low energy and low potency (e.g.,shame and depression) are not useful for activatingcollective bonds or motivating joint action.In contrast, negative emotions that are higherin energy and potency (e.g., anger) can motivateindividual participation in collective actions—from mild protests to large-scale conflict. Totruly energize a mobilized collective identity,however, requires both energy and a positivesense <strong>of</strong> connection within the collective. Suchmobilization is facilitated, according to Britt& Heise, by positive, powerful, and energeticemotions like pride. Britt & Heise argued thatcertain emotion transformations (e.g., shame topride) are difficult to make without transitioningbetween more affectively similar emotionsalong the way. They illustrated how aspects<strong>of</strong> participation in the gay rights movementcould be seen as efforts to transform shame t<strong>of</strong>ear to anger to pride in an effort to constructa solidary and energized collective identity.In a participant observation study <strong>of</strong> politicalavoidance in several volunteer organizations,Eliasoph (2002) examined the complement tothis process. Eliasoph studied how political disengagementis socially produced in interaction.Her analysis focused on how individuals balancedideas <strong>of</strong> citizenship with feelings <strong>of</strong> powerlessness.She described the process <strong>of</strong> “culturework” done by volunteers working to addresspolitical avoidance by transforming feelings <strong>of</strong>powerlessness into expansion <strong>of</strong> self-interest. Inthis way, Eliasoph showed how emotion transformationin an effort to protect personal identitycan also inhibit activation and mobilization<strong>of</strong> a collective identity.Collective <strong>Identity</strong> and ModernityMuch has been written about the relationshipbetween modernity and contemporary loci <strong>of</strong>collective identity. The conventional argumentis that social and institutional complexity <strong>of</strong>late modernity (Giddens 1991) or postmodernity(Gergen 1991) has fractionalized the contemporaryself. Wimmer (2002) speaks to thisidea in his study <strong>of</strong> nationalism and ethnicity,but argues for the opposite relationship. Accordingto Wimmer, contemporary notions <strong>of</strong>identity are responsible for the emergence <strong>of</strong>modernity. Wimmer distinguishes three positionson the issue <strong>of</strong> nationalism and ethnicity.The first is that nations and ethnic groups aretruly modern phenomena. Second, nations andethnic groups as we currently understand themare transitory—what he refers to as “birth pains<strong>of</strong> modernity.” Third, national and ethnic identitiesare perennial and basic to human socialorganization. Wimmer then presents what hedescribes as a radical modern argument: thatethnicity nationalism produced modernity aswe understand it. According to Wimmer, it wasthe fusion <strong>of</strong> three notions <strong>of</strong> peoplehood thatresulted in the modern politicization <strong>of</strong> ethnicity:(a) the notion <strong>of</strong> the people as a sovereignentity—with power by means <strong>of</strong> political (suchas democratic) procedures; (b) the notion <strong>of</strong>people as citizens—with rights and responsibilities;and (c) the notion <strong>of</strong> people as an ethniccommunity bound by common political destinyand shared cultural features. The commingling<strong>of</strong> these notions translated into three modernpolitical principles: democracy, citizenship,and national self-determination. Wimmer furtherargues that nations differ in the relativesalience <strong>of</strong> these principles. According toWimmer, the French and Swiss emphasizedemocracy and rely on the concept <strong>of</strong> democracyto derive the principles <strong>of</strong> citizenship andnationalism. In contrast, Wimmer argues thatGermany, Greece, and Israel stress nationalityand from it derive the principles <strong>of</strong> democracyand citizenship.While social movement theorists focuson both aspects <strong>of</strong> collective identity—common interests/fate and common symbolicmeanings—there is a strong thread in themodernity/postmodernity literature thatdocuments a shift over time in the relativewww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 493


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.importance <strong>of</strong> these two aspects in shapingcategory-based action. Pre-1960s movementsare presumed to be motivated more by morestructural, ascriptive category membershipsand jointness <strong>of</strong> interest, whereas post-1960smovements are thought to be driven moreby meaning-based bonds (Laraña et al. 1994,Melucci 1994, Offe 1985). This argument aboutthe trend away from the importance <strong>of</strong> structurallydefined group identities toward morepersonally and culturally defined group identitiesparallels the findings in social psychologyabout the transition (over a similar historicalperiod) from more structural/institutional definitions<strong>of</strong> self to more personal/dispositionaldefinitions <strong>of</strong> self (Turner 1976).Fractal Organization<strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> DynamicsCollective identities are to groups what socialidentities are to individuals (Owens 2003,p. 227). Therefore, collective identity processescan sometimes mirror the processes describedby the theories about internal and situationalidentity dynamics reviewed above. The process<strong>of</strong> boundary work, <strong>of</strong>ten fueled by competitionor contact, can lead to the development <strong>of</strong> anidentity, making it available for members <strong>of</strong> acategory to see themselves as having a commonset <strong>of</strong> interests or fate. Sometimes new labelsare developed and added to a culture’s “culturaltheory <strong>of</strong> people” (MacKinnon & Heise 2010),making them available for individuals to adoptand incorporate into a self-structure. Further,intergroup dynamics can lead to situations thatallow groups and/or individuals to adopt differentidentity orientations at different momentsin a political/social process.While identity accumulation theory (Thoits1983, 2003) and self-complexity theory(Linville 1987) argue that having access tomultiple role-identities can provide the selfwith resources that help provide a bufferagainst stress, the social movement literaturereveals that collective identities with layers<strong>of</strong> nested identities can provide a movementwith a richer set <strong>of</strong> resources for mobilizingeffective collective action (Heckathorn 1993,Lichterman 1999, Oliver & Marwell 1988,Richards 2004). Moreover, just as multipleidentities within self-structures can sometimeslead to identity conflict, competing or layeredidentities within collectives can createopportunities for conflict and fractionalization.Richards (2004) illustrates this point withher examination <strong>of</strong> the delicate balancing actrequired to mobilize and maintain collectiveidentity in the ethnically and economically diversewomen’s movements in Chile. She studiedthe relationships between the poor, workingclasspobladoras, the indigenous Mapuche, andthe National Women’s Service (SERNAM).Nested class and ethnic identities set the stagefor potential fractures among these variousgroups involved in the women’s movement.Both the pobladoras and the Mapuche describebeing left out <strong>of</strong> the programs implemented bySERNAM. Pobladoras identified gender as importantin explaining their activism—yet initiallyorganized around roles as wives andmothers. Richards describes how the pobladoras’self-views became transformed through participationin the movement. Meanwhile, theMapuche claims <strong>of</strong> difference and disadvantageconflicted with SERNAM’s goal <strong>of</strong> equality/sameness,causing schisms in both discourseand agendas. Richards’s work showed howmembers <strong>of</strong> the women’s movement navigatethe danger <strong>of</strong> fragmentation by suppressing theseparate identities in the superordinate collective.This careful negotiation <strong>of</strong> a coherent,integrated collective identity echoes the findings<strong>of</strong> Lichterman’s (1999) study <strong>of</strong> a sexualminority activist group as it attempted to createand maintain a solidary collective identityas a coalition-building <strong>net</strong>work. Lichtermanfound that compared to some <strong>of</strong> its constituentgroups, the LGBT coalition-buildinggroup actively avoided reflective discourse andengaged in more unified talk carefully designednot to raise the specter <strong>of</strong> differenceor to activate potentially schismatic organizingprocesses.494 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.CONCLUSIONS ANDOPPORTUNITIESThis review, with its unusual structure and itscoverage <strong>of</strong> the relatively disconnected literatureson identity within the microsociology andsocial movements areas, is designed to allowreaders to see opportunities for enriching bothtraditions. We deal first with the opportunityfor connections within the microlevel traditionsand then with potential for enrichment acrossthe two distinct literatures.The relatively subtle distinction that wehave drawn between theories that emphasizehow self-structures and internalized meaningsorganize social life, on the one hand, and howwe import cultural meanings and shape themwithin the context <strong>of</strong> situated interaction, onthe other, points to a need for two types <strong>of</strong> newresearch. While much <strong>of</strong> the microsociologicalliterature has used either surveys (to studyself-structure and internalized meanings) orexperiments (to study situated meanings andhow they shift in context), we may need newstudy <strong>of</strong> naturally occurring situations to fullyexploit the linkages between internalized structureand situated action. Some insights fromStryker’s <strong>Identity</strong> Theory could be linked to thefast-growing literature on <strong>net</strong>works to developthe linkage between commitment and saliencemore fully and to explore how it impacts situatedaction. The new work by MacKinnon &Heise (2010; see also Moore & Robinson 2006)about how people use movement from onesituation to another to maintain fundamentalsentiments toward the self could be linkedto Thoit’s work on identity accumulation toshow how situated action accomplishes mentalhealth benefits (or, among some categories <strong>of</strong>people, fails to do so). Exploring the few competingtheoretical predictions from <strong>Identity</strong>Control Theory and Affect Control Theory(Smith-Lovin & Robinson 2006) might allowus to assess the relative value <strong>of</strong> emic, individualizedmeaning measurement (as used inBurke’s work) and mathematical formalization(as used by Heise). It might also reveal somescope conditions for when people privilegeinternalized, idiosyncratic meanings over adefinition <strong>of</strong> situation that is institutionallyimposed.Connections between the microsociologicalliterature and the group-level treatment <strong>of</strong> collectiveidentity are more challenging. The obviouspoint <strong>of</strong> departure is that the people who aremotivated by collective identities toward politicalor social action are undergoing the processesalready well described by the microsociologicaltheories. This fact alone might provide someleverage for determining when recruitment, reframing<strong>of</strong> a social situation, and mobilizationmight succeed or fail. More interesting are theways in which the collective identity literaturecan suggest new topics for microsociological research.Much <strong>of</strong> this literature focuses on thestructural conditions under which new optionsare created for a definition <strong>of</strong> the situation. Thedefinition <strong>of</strong> the situation is a central processin microsociological identity theories, but it israrely studied explicitly. The collective identityliterature’s use <strong>of</strong> G<strong>of</strong>fman’s framing concept,and its attention to the inter- and intragroupprocesses that successfully motivate actors toview a contested situation in a new way, couldpoint to a useful exploration <strong>of</strong> these key processesat the microlevel. A second area <strong>of</strong> potentialenrichment comes from the collectiveidentity focus on the group as a unit that generatessocial action. As we noted above, thereis a parallel between processes at the individualand group levels, both in defining situations andin generating action. Although theorists shouldavoid loose analogies, the processes by whichgroups manage multiple, nested, layered identitiesand use particular identities (and the emotionalresponses associated with them) to motivateorganized collective action may mirror theprocess <strong>of</strong> managing nested identity structureswithin individual selves. An exploration <strong>of</strong> thisprocess might require a refocusing <strong>of</strong> attentionon group-level studies in microsociology. Wehave excellent midlevel theories <strong>of</strong> group processin sociological social psychology, and theyare becoming increasingly connected intellectuallyto the issue <strong>of</strong> identity [see, for an excellentexample, Lawler et al.’s (2009) new bookwww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 495


on affective commitments to groups]. We needto push these developments to see how groupsorganize actions vis-à-vis other outside grouplevelinterests.DISCLOSURE STATEMENTThe authors are not aware <strong>of</strong> any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings thatmight be perceived as affecting the objectivity <strong>of</strong> this review.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSWe thank Tiffani Everett, Steven Foy, Robert Freeland, Elizabeth Hordge-Freeman, ChristopherD. Moore, Victor Ray, Kim Rogers, Daniel B. Shank, and Allison Wisecup for helpful commentson an earlier draft <strong>of</strong> this review.Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.LITERATURE CITEDAhrens C, Ryff C. 2006. Multiple roles and well-being: sociodemographic and psychological moderators. SexRoles 55:801–15Alexander CN Jr, Knight GW. 1971. Situated identities and social psychological experimentation. Sociometry34:65–82Alexander CN Jr, Wiley MG. 1981. Situated activity and identity formation. In Social Psychology: SociologicalPerspectives, ed. M Rosenburg, RH Turner, pp. 269–82. New York: Basic BooksBandura A. 1977. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice HallBar<strong>net</strong>t RC, Hyde JS. 2001. Women, men, work, and family. Am. Psychol. 56:781–96Brewer MB, Gardner WL. 1996. Who is this “we”? Levels <strong>of</strong> collective identity and self representations.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 71:83–93Britt L, Heise DR. 2000. From shame to pride in identity politics. See Stryker et al. 2000, pp. 252–71Brook AT, Garcia J, Fleming M. 2008. The effects <strong>of</strong> multiple identities on psychological well-being. Personal.Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34:1588–600Burke PJ. 1991. <strong>Identity</strong> processes and social stress. Am. Sociol. Rev. 56:836–49Burke PJ. 2004. Identities and social structure: the 2003 Cooley-Mead Award address. Soc. Psychol. Q. 67:5–15Burke PJ, Reitzes DC. 1991. An identity theory approach to commitment. Soc. Psychol. Q. 54:239–51Burke PJ, Stets JE. 1999. Trust and commitment through self-verification. Soc. Psychol. Q. 62:347–66Burke PJ, Stets JE. 2009. <strong>Identity</strong> Theory. New York: Oxford Univ. PressBurke PJ, Stets JE, Cerven C. 2007. Gender, legitimation, and identity verification in groups. Soc. Psychol. Q.70:27–42Burke PJ, Tully JT. 1977. The measurement <strong>of</strong> role/identity. Soc. Forces 55:880–97Cast AD, Stets JE, Burke PJ. 1999. Does the self conform to the views <strong>of</strong> others? Soc. Psychol. Q. 62:68–82Cerulo KA. 1997. <strong>Identity</strong> construction: new issues, new directions. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 23:385–409Collins R. 1990. Stratification, emotional energy, and the transient emotions. In Research Agendas in the Sociology<strong>of</strong> Emotions, ed. TD Kemper, pp. 27–57. Albany: SUNY PressCooley CH. 1902. Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: ScribnerCorrell SJ, Ridgeway CL. 2003. Expectation states theory. In Handbook <strong>of</strong> Social Psychology, ed. J Delamater,pp. 29–51. New York: KluwerDurkheim E. 1893 (1984). The Division <strong>of</strong> Labor in Society. Transl. WD Halls. New York: Free PressEliasoph N. 2002. “Close to Home”: the work <strong>of</strong> avoiding politics. In Cultural Sociology, ed. L Spillman,pp. 130–40. Oxford: Oxford Univ. PressFoote NN. 1951. Identification as the basis for a theory <strong>of</strong> motivation. Am. Sociol. Rev. 16:14–21Gamson WA. 1995. Constructing social protest. In Social Movements and Culture, ed. H Johnston,B Klandermans, pp. 85–106. New York: RoutledgeGergen KJ. 1991. The Saturated Self: Dilemmas <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> in Contemporary Life. NewYork:BasicBooks496 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.Giddens A. 1991. Modernity and Self-<strong>Identity</strong>: Self and Society in the Late Modern Age. Stanford, CA: StanfordUniv. PressG<strong>of</strong>fman E. 1959. The Presentation <strong>of</strong> Self in Everyday Life. Garden City, NJ: DoubledayG<strong>of</strong>fman E. 1963. Stigma: Notes on the Management <strong>of</strong> Spoiled <strong>Identity</strong>. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-HallG<strong>of</strong>fman E. 1974. Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization <strong>of</strong> Experience. NewYork:Harper&RowGollob HF. 1968. Impression formation and word combination in sentences. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 10:341–53Gould DB. 2004. Passionate political processes: bringing emotions back into the study <strong>of</strong> social movements.In Rethinking Social Movements: Structure, Meaning and Emotion, ed. J Goodwin, JM Jasper, pp. 155–75.Lanham, MD: Roman & LittlefieldGreen JC. 1999. The spirit willing: collective identity and the development <strong>of</strong> the Christian right. In Waves<strong>of</strong> Protest: Social Movements Since the Sixties, ed. J Freeman, V Johnson, pp. 153–68. Oxford: Oxford Univ.PressHechter M. 1978. Group formation and the cultural division <strong>of</strong> labor. Am. J. Sociol. 84:293–318Hechter M. 2000. Containing Nationalism. Oxford: Oxford Univ. PressHeckathorn DD. 1993. Collective action and group heterogeneity: voluntary provision versus selective incentives.Am. Sociol. Rev. 58:329–50Heider F. 1946. Attitudes and cognitive organization. J. Psychol. 21:107–12Heider F. 1958. The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Interpersonal Relations. New York: WileyHeise DR. 1979. Understanding Events: Affect and the Construction <strong>of</strong> Social Action. Cambridge, UK/New York:Cambridge Univ. Press. 197 pp.Heise DR. 1998. Conditions for empathic solidarity. In The Problem <strong>of</strong> Solidarity: Theories and Models, ed.P Doreian, T Fararo, pp. 197–212. Amsterdam: Gordon & BreachHeise DR. 2007. Expressive Order: Confirming Sentiments in Social Actions. New York: Springer. 163 pp.Hochschild AR. 1983. The Managed Heart: Commercialization <strong>of</strong> Human Feeling. Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press.307 pp.Hogg MA. 1992. The Social Psychology <strong>of</strong> Group Cohesiveness: From Attraction to Social <strong>Identity</strong>. Hemel Hempstead,UK: Harvester WheatsheafHogg MA, Terry DJ, White KM. 1995. A tale <strong>of</strong> two theories: a critical comparison <strong>of</strong> identity theory withsocial identity theory. Soc. Psychol. Q. 58:255–69Horney K. 1945. Our Inner Conflicts: A Constructive Theory <strong>of</strong> Neurosis. New York: NortonJackson PB. 1997. Role occupancy and minority mental health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 38:237–55James W. 1890. The Principles <strong>of</strong> Psychology. New York: Henry HoltJanzen BL, Muhajarine N. 2003. Social role occupancy, gender, income adequacy, life stage and health: alongitudinal study <strong>of</strong> employed Canadian men and women. Soc. Sci. Med. 57:1491–503Jasper JM. 1998. The emotions <strong>of</strong> protest: affective and reactive emotions in and around social movements.Sociol. Forum 13:397–424Jenson J. 1995. What’s in a name? Nationalist movements and public discourse. In Social Movements andCulture, ed. HJB Klandermans, pp. 107–26. New York: RoutledgeKikuzawa S. 2006. Multiple roles and mental health in cross-cultural perspective: the elderly in the UnitedStates and Japan. J. Health Soc. Behav. 47:62–76Lamont M. 1992. Money, Morals, and Manners. Univ. Chicago PressLawler EJ, Thye SR, Yoon J. 2009. Social Commitments in a Depersonalized World. New York: Russell SageFound.Laraña E, Johnston H, Gusfield JR. 1994. New Social Movements: From Ideology to <strong>Identity</strong>. Philadelphia: TempleUniv. PressLichterman P. 1999. Talking identity in the public sphere: broad visions and small spaces in sexual identitypolitics. Theory Soc. 28:101–41Linville PW. 1987. Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and depression. J. Personal.Soc. Psychol. 52:663–76Lorber J. 2006. Shifting paradigms and challenging categories. Soc. Probl. 53:448–53MacKinnon NJ. 1994. Symbolic Interactionism as Affect Control. Albany: SUNY PressMacKinnon NJ, Heise DR. 2010. Self, <strong>Identity</strong>, and Social Institutions. New York: Palgrave MacMillanwww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 497


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.McCall GJ, Simmons JL. 1966. Identities and Interactions. New York: Free PressMcPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook J. 2001. Birds <strong>of</strong> a feather: homophily in social <strong>net</strong>works. Annu. Rev.Sociol. 27:415–44McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Brashears ME. 2006. Social isolation in America: changes in core discussion<strong>net</strong>works over two decades. Am. Sociol. Rev. 71:353–75Mead GH. 1934. Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint <strong>of</strong> a Social Behaviorist. Chicago: Univ. ChicagoPressMelucci A. 1989. Nomads <strong>of</strong> the Present. Philadelphia: Temple Univ. PressMelucci A. 1994. A strange kind <strong>of</strong> newness: What’s new in new social movements? See Laraña et al. 1994,pp. 101–32Moore CD, Robinson DT. 2006. Selective identity preferences: choosing from among alternative occupationalidentities. Adv. Group Process. 23:253–81Morris AD. 1999. A retrospective on the civil rights movement: political and intellectual landmarks. Annu.Rev. Sociol. 25:517–39Nagel J. 1995. American Indian Renewal: Red Power and the Resurgence <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> and Culture. Oxford: OxfordUniv. PressNippert-Eng CE. 2002. Boundary work: sculpting work and home. In Cultural Sociology, ed. L Spillman,pp. 79–86. Oxford: Oxford Univ. PressOffe C. 1985. New social movements: challenging the boundaries <strong>of</strong> institutional politics. Soc. Res. 52:817–68Okamoto DG. 2003. Toward a theory <strong>of</strong> pa<strong>net</strong>hnicity: explaining Asian American collective action. Am. Sociol.Rev. 68:811–42Oliver PE, Marwell G. 1988. The paradox <strong>of</strong> group size in collective action: a theory <strong>of</strong> the critical mass. II.Am. Sociol. Rev. 53:1–8Olzak S. 1994. The Dynamics <strong>of</strong> Ethnic Competition and Conflict. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. PressOlzak S, Shanahan S, McEneaney EH. 1996. Poverty, segregation, and race riots: 1960 to 1993. Am. Sociol.Rev. 61:590–613Olzak S, Shanahan S, West E. 1994. School desegregation, interracial exposure, and antibusing activity incontemporary urban America. Am. J. Sociol. 100:196–241Osgood CE, May WH, Miron MS. 1975. Cross-Cultural Universals <strong>of</strong> Affective Meaning. Urbana: Univ. Ill.PressOsgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum PH. 1957. The Measurement <strong>of</strong> Meaning. Urbana: Univ. Ill. PressOwens TJ. 2003. Self and identity. In Handbook <strong>of</strong> Social Psychology, ed. JD Delamater, pp. 205–32. New York:KluwerOwens TJ, Aronson PJ. 2000. Self-concept as a force in social movement involvement. See Stryker et al. 2000,pp. 132–51Owens TJ, Serpe R. 2003. The role <strong>of</strong> self-esteem in family identity salience and commitment among African-Americans, Latinos, and whites. In Advances in <strong>Identity</strong> Theory and Research, ed. PJ Burke, TJ Owens,R Serpe, PA Thoits, pp. 85–104. New York: KluwerPolletta F, Jasper JM. 2001. Collective identity and social movements. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27:283–305Powers WT. 1973. Behavior: The Control <strong>of</strong> Perception. Chicago: AldinePrentice DA, Miller DT, Lightdale JR. 1994. Asymmetries in attachments to groups and to their members:distinguishing between common-identity and common-bond groups. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20:484–93Rashotte LS, Smith-Lovin L. 1997. Who benefits from being bold: the interactive effects <strong>of</strong> task cues andstatus characteristics. Adv. Group Process. 14:235–55Richards P. 2004. Pobladoras, Indigenas, and the State: Conflicts over Women’s Rights in Chile. New Brunswick,NJ: Rutgers Univ. PressRidgeway C, Smith-Lovin L. 1994. Structure, culture and interaction: comparing two generative theories.Adv. Group Process. 11:213–39Robinson DT. 2007. Control theories in sociology. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 33:157–74Robinson DT, Smith-Lovin L. 1992. Selective interaction as a strategy for identity maintenance: an affectcontrol model. Soc. Psychol. Q. 55:12–28498 Owens·Robinson·Smith-Lovin


Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.Robinson DT, Smith-Lovin L. 1999. Emotion display as a strategy for identity negotiation. Motiv. Emot.23:73–104Robinson DT, Smith-Lovin L, Tsoudis O. 1994. Heinous crime or unfortunate accident? The effects <strong>of</strong>remorse on responses to mock criminal confessions. Soc. Forces 73:175–90Rosenberg M. 1979. Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic BooksSachs-Ericsson N, Ciarlo JA. 2000. Gender, social roles, and mental health: an epidemiological perspective.Sex Roles 43:605–28Serpe RT. 1987. Stability and change in self: a structural symbolic interactionist explanation. Soc. Psychol. Q.50:44–55Smith-Lovin L. 1979. Behavior settings and impressions formed from social scenarios. Soc. Psychol. Q. 42:31–43Smith-Lovin L. 2007. The strength <strong>of</strong> weak identities: social structural sources <strong>of</strong> self, situation and emotionalexperience. Soc. Psychol. Q. 70:106–24Smith-Lovin L, Heise DR, eds. 1988. Analyzing Social Events: Advances in Affect Control Theory. New York:Gordon & BreachSmith-Lovin L, Robinson DT. 2006. Control theories <strong>of</strong> identity, action and emotion: in search <strong>of</strong> testabledifferences between affect control theory and identity control theory. In Purpose, Meaning and Action:Control Systems Theories in Sociology, ed. K McClelland, T Fararo, pp. 163–88. New York: MacMillanSoule SA. 1992. Populism and black lynching in Georgia, 1890–1900. Soc. Forces 71:431–49Stets JE. 1997. Status and identity in marital interaction. Soc. Psychol. Q. 60:185–217Stets JE, Burke PJ. 2000. <strong>Identity</strong> theory and social identity theory. Soc. Psychol. Q. 63:224–37Stets JE, Burke PJ. 2005. <strong>Identity</strong> verification, control, and aggression in marriage. Soc. Psychol. Q. 68:160–78Stets JE, Harrod MM. 2004. Verification across multiple identities: the role <strong>of</strong> status. Soc. Psychol. Q. 67:155–71Stets JE, Tsushima TM. 2001. Negative emotion and coping responses within identity control theory.Soc. Psychol. Q. 64:283–95Stryker S. 1968. <strong>Identity</strong> theory and role performance. J. Marriage Fam. 30:558–64Stryker S. 1980. Symbolic Interactionism: A Social Structural Version. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin CummingsStryker S. 2008. From Mead to a structural symbolic interactionism and beyond. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 34:15–31Stryker S, Burke PJ. 2000. The past, present, and future <strong>of</strong> an identity theory. Soc. Psychol. Q. 63:284–97Stryker S, Owens TJ, White RW, eds. 2000. Self, <strong>Identity</strong>, and Social Movements. Minneapolis: Univ. Minn.PressStryker S, Serpe RT, Hunt MO. 2005. Making good on a promise: the impact <strong>of</strong> larger social structures oncommitments. Adv. Group Process. 22:93–124Tajfel H, Turner JC. 1979. The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/ColeTaylor V, Whittier NE. 1999. Collective identity in social movement communities: lesbian feminist mobilization.In Waves <strong>of</strong> Protest: Social Movements Since the Sixties, ed. J Freeman, V Johnson, pp. 169–94.Oxford: Oxford Univ. PressThoits PA. 1983. Multiple identities and psychological well-being: a reformulation and test <strong>of</strong> the socialisolation hypothesis. Am. Sociol. Rev. 48:174–87Thoits PA. 1986. Social support as coping assistance. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 54:416–23Thoits PA. 1995. <strong>Identity</strong>-relevant events and psychological symptoms: a cautionary tale. J. Health Soc. Behav.36:72–82Thoits PA. 2003. Personal agency in the accumulation <strong>of</strong> multiple role-identities. In Advances in <strong>Identity</strong> Theoryand Research, ed. PJ Burke, TJ Owens, RT Serpe, PA Thoits, pp. 179–94. New York: KluwerThoits PA, Virshup LK. 1997. Me’s and we’s: forms and functions <strong>of</strong> social identities. In Self and <strong>Identity</strong>:Fundamental Issues, ed. RD Ashmore, L Jussim, pp. 106–33. New York: Oxford Univ. PressTurner JC, Tajfel H. 1982. Social <strong>Identity</strong> and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. PressTurner RH. 1969. The public perception <strong>of</strong> protest. Am. Sociol. Rev. 34:815–31Turner RH. 1976. The real self: from institution to impulse. Am. J. Sociol. 81:989–1016Weinstein EA, Wiley MG, DeVaughn W. 1966. Role and interpersonal style as components <strong>of</strong> social interaction.Soc. Forces 45:210–16Wethington E, Moen P, Glasgo N, Pillemer K. 2000. Multiple roles, social integration, and health. In SocialIntegration in the Second Half <strong>of</strong> Life, ed. K Pillemer, P Moen, E Wethington, N Glasgo, pp. 48–71.Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Univ. PressWimmer A. 2002. Nationalist Exclusion and Ethnic Conflict. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Presswww.annualreviews.org • <strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong> 499


ContentsAnnual Review<strong>of</strong> SociologyVolume 36, 2010Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.FrontispieceJohn W. Meyer ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣xivPrefatory ChapterWorld Society, Institutional Theories, and the ActorJohn W. Meyer ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣1Theory and MethodsCausal Inference in Sociological ResearchMarkus Gangl ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣21Causal Mechanisms in the Social SciencesPeter Hedström and Petri Ylikoski ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣49Social ProcessesA World <strong>of</strong> Standards but not a Standard World: Toward a Sociology<strong>of</strong> Standards and StandardizationStefan Timmermans and Steven Epstein ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣69Dynamics <strong>of</strong> Dyads in Social Networks: Assortative, Relational,and Proximity MechanismsMark T. Rivera, Sara B. Soderstrom, and Brian Uzzi ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣91From the Sociology <strong>of</strong> Intellectuals to the Sociology <strong>of</strong> InterventionsGil Eyal and Larissa Buchholz ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣117Social Relationships and Health Behavior Across the Life CourseDebra Umberson, Robert Crosnoe, and Corinne Reczek ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣139Partiality <strong>of</strong> Memberships in Categories and AudiencesMichael T. Hannan ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣159v


Institutions and CultureWhat Is Sociological about Music?William G. Roy and Timothy J. Dowd ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣183Cultural Holes: Beyond Relationality in Social Networks and CultureMark A. Pachucki and Ronald L. Breiger ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣205Formal OrganizationsAnnu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.Organizational Approaches to Inequality: Inertia, Relative Power,and EnvironmentsKevin Stainback, Donald Tomaskovic-Devey, and Sheryl Skaggs ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣225Political and Economic SociologyThe Contentiousness <strong>of</strong> Markets: Politics, Social Movements,and Institutional Change in MarketsBrayden G King and Nicholas A. Pearce ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣249Conservative and Right-Wing MovementsKathleen M. Blee and Kimberly A. Creasap ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣269The Political Consequences <strong>of</strong> Social MovementsEdwin Amenta, Neal Caren, Elizabeth Chiarello, and Yang Su ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣287Comparative Analyses <strong>of</strong> Public Attitudes Toward Immigrantsand Immigration Using Multinational Survey Data: A Review<strong>of</strong> Theories and ResearchAlin M. Ceobanu and Xavier Escandell ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣309Differentiation and StratificationIncome Inequality: New Trends and Research DirectionsLeslie McCall and Christine Percheski ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣329Socioeconomic Disparities in Health BehaviorsFred C. Pampel, Patrick M. Krueger, and Justin T. Denney ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣349Gender and Health InequalityJen’nan Ghazal Read and Bridget K. Gorman♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣371Incarceration and StratificationSara Wakefield and Christopher Uggen ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣387Achievement Inequality and the Institutional Structure <strong>of</strong> EducationalSystems: A Comparative PerspectiveHerman G. Van de Werfhorst and Jonathan J.B. Mijs ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣407viContents


Historical Studies <strong>of</strong> Social Mobility and StratificationMarco H.D. van Leeuwen and Ineke Maas ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣429Individual and SocietyRace and TrustSandra Susan Smith ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣453<strong>Three</strong> <strong>Faces</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Identity</strong>Timothy J. Owens, Dawn T. Robinson, and Lynn Smith-Lovin ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣477Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010.36:477-499. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.orgby Vanderbilt University on 01/03/12. For personal use only.PolicyThe New Homelessness RevisitedBarrett A. Lee, Kimberly A. Tyler, and James D. Wright ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣501The Decline <strong>of</strong> Cash Welfare and Implications for Social Policyand PovertySandra K. Danziger ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣523IndexesCumulative Index <strong>of</strong> Contributing Authors, Volumes 27–36 ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣547Cumulative Index <strong>of</strong> Chapter Titles, Volumes 27–36 ♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣♣551ErrataAn online log <strong>of</strong> corrections to Annual Review <strong>of</strong> Sociology articles may be found athttp://soc.annualreviews.org/errata.shtmlContentsvii

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!