13.07.2015 Views

Moral spillovers: The effect of moral violations on deviant behavior ...

Moral spillovers: The effect of moral violations on deviant behavior ...

Moral spillovers: The effect of moral violations on deviant behavior ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

1244 E. Mullen, J. Nadler / Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1239–1245% Reporing Favorable Result80757065605550not significantly more likely than chance to report tails,t(37) = 1.66, p = .106 and t(42) = 1.39, p = .173, respectively.Importantly, results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a <strong>on</strong>e-way ANOVA with c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> as theindependent variable and participants’ anger as the dependent variablerevealed that participants did not significantly differ as afuncti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in their self-reported anger at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>the experiment, F(2,102) < 1. Thus, participants in the anger and<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> violati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were equally angered at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>the experiment. In summary, results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Study 2 provide additi<strong>on</strong>alevidence that <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards (and not just anger perse) led to increased <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>. Participants who recalled a<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> violati<strong>on</strong> were more likely to cheat relative to participantsin the angry and neutral c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Moreover, these results extendStudy 1 by investigating a different operati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>deviant</strong><strong>behavior</strong> (i.e., cheating) and utilizing a different type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>.General discussi<strong>on</strong>C<strong>on</strong>trol Anger <str<strong>on</strong>g>Moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> Violati<strong>on</strong>Fig. 2. Percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> participants reporting a favorable coin flip as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, Study 2.<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current research extends previous work <strong>on</strong> MMs by exploringthe broader c<strong>on</strong>sequences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> threats to MMs. C<strong>on</strong>sistent withprior research, in Study 1 we found that participants judged outcomesto be less fair, were less willing to accept outcomes, andwere more angered when outcomes opposed rather than supportedtheir MMs. Moreover, the interactive <str<strong>on</strong>g>effect</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MM and verdict<strong>on</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> outcome fairness, decisi<strong>on</strong> acceptance, andanger were not qualified by whether procedures were proper orimproper. More importantly, we uncovered novel evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> spillover <str<strong>on</strong>g>effect</str<strong>on</strong>g>, by showing that people are more willingto engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> (i.e., take our pen or cheat) whenoutcomes oppose rather than support their MMs (Study 1), orwhen simply asked to think about another pers<strong>on</strong> violating a <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g>standard (Study 2). Taken together, our results suggest that outcomesor events that violate <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards can increase people’stendency to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>.Why are people more willing to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>after learning about outcomes that violate their MMs? <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re area number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes that can c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the relati<strong>on</strong>ship betweenMM <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> and <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>. First, it might be thathaving a MM about what c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a fair outcome allows perceiversto check whether authorities and instituti<strong>on</strong>s are just andlegitimate by assessing whether those instituti<strong>on</strong>s produce the‘‘right” outcome. When instituti<strong>on</strong>s and authorities fail to upholdpeople’s MMs, they perceive those instituti<strong>on</strong>s and authorities tobe less legitimate (Skitka, 2006). This decreased legitimacy mightlead people to be more willing to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> andflout the rules <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> authorities. In other words, when the systemseems unjust because <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards are not upheld, followingrules in general becomes less important.Another possible mechanism involves focusing participants’attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the fact that <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> happen. In the experimentsreported here, some participants focused their attenti<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> specific instances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MM <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g>, and some participants didnot (either because they did not have a relevant MM or becausethey were not asked to recall a <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> violati<strong>on</strong>). By focusing someparticipants’ attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the fact that <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> do occur inthe world, these participants may have inferred that <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g>happen with some frequency, and so engaging in a <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> violati<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong>eself may have been perceived as c<strong>on</strong>sistent with ageneral descriptive norm (cf. Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). Similarly,being primed with the noti<strong>on</strong> that <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurwith some frequency may have increased participants’ tendencyto engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> through a less c<strong>on</strong>scious process(e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996). Like Bargh’s participantswho walked more slowly after being primed with stereotypes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>the elderly, our participants may have behaved more unfairly afterbeing primed with <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Irrespective <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whether the increased <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> we observedfollowing exposure to a <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> violati<strong>on</strong> was the result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>a c<strong>on</strong>scious decisi<strong>on</strong> to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> or resulted froma more automatic process, it seems clear that the extent to whichauthorities, systems, and individuals are perceived as upholding<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards can have a pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ound influence <strong>on</strong> people’s tendencyto comply with rules. When <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards are not upheld,people are more likely to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>. This suggeststhat witnessing injustice (through a violati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standard)can ir<strong>on</strong>ically lead people to behave more unfairly themselves (Nadler,2005; Robins<strong>on</strong> & Darley, 1995, 2007).<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> noti<strong>on</strong> that people can feel justified in behaving in an im<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g>fashi<strong>on</strong> because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> experiencing past unfairness is c<strong>on</strong>sistentwith research <strong>on</strong> ‘‘equity with the world” (Austin & Walster, 1975).Austin and Walster (1975) theorized that if people feel unfairlytreated in <strong>on</strong>e instance, they can be motivated to maintain equitywith the world by recouping their losses in a later instance, evenwhen the two instances are unrelated and involve interacti<strong>on</strong>swith different people or instituti<strong>on</strong>s. For example, participantswho were underpaid in a first task were especially likely to try torecoup their losses in a subsequent task (i.e., by taking more thantheir ‘‘fair” share) when they felt more an<strong>on</strong>ymous—that is, whenthey would not have to meet with their task partner (Austin & Walster,1975). Thus, receiving an unfair allocati<strong>on</strong> led participants tobehave more unfairly in a subsequent task when an<strong>on</strong>ymity washigh but not when an<strong>on</strong>ymity was low. This research suggests thatthe perceived an<strong>on</strong>ymity we provided our participants may haveincreased their willingness to engage in <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>. In thefirst experiment, participants did not know that the pens wereunobtrusively marked, and in the sec<strong>on</strong>d experiment, participantsknew that the experimenters were unable to verify their coin flipresults. Thus, participants had every reas<strong>on</strong> to believe that their<strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong> would go undetected. Future research shouldtherefore examine whether an<strong>on</strong>ymity moderates the <str<strong>on</strong>g>effect</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>violati<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>deviant</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>.In additi<strong>on</strong>, future research should also investigate whetherthere might be pro-social c<strong>on</strong>sequences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> outcomes that affirmpeople’s MMs. Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Study 1 suggest that affirmati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>MMs (e.g., when those who are <strong>on</strong> trial for ‘‘<str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g>” acti<strong>on</strong>s areset free) might lead people to be more willing to adhere to authorities’rules. In Study 1, pro-choice MM participants did not take anypens when the outcome supported their MM. Future researchshould therefore c<strong>on</strong>tinue to explore how outcomes that supportor oppose <str<strong>on</strong>g>moral</str<strong>on</strong>g> standards can lead to more versus less adherencewith rules, respectively.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!