30.07.2015 Views

Procrastination, participation, and performance in ... - Anitacrawley.net

Procrastination, participation, and performance in ... - Anitacrawley.net

Procrastination, participation, and performance in ... - Anitacrawley.net

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

250N. Mich<strong>in</strong>ov et al. / Computers & Education 56 (2011) 243–252more systematically the impact of tutors/<strong>in</strong>structors on learner behavior <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environments <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>teraction betweenlearner characteristics <strong>and</strong> the style of tutor<strong>in</strong>g.In addition to the ma<strong>in</strong> aim of the present study, the dynamics of the onl<strong>in</strong>e behavior of high <strong>and</strong> low procrast<strong>in</strong>ators over time werecompared us<strong>in</strong>g a web questionnaire adm<strong>in</strong>istered at the end of the course. More specifically, three self-reported behaviors were measured:the times (i.e. weeks) when learners wanted to start (or restart) work<strong>in</strong>g at a distance, felt motivated to do their course work <strong>and</strong> felt likedropp<strong>in</strong>g out. Although exploratory, these measures are important to identify (even <strong>in</strong>directly) crucial behaviors <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environmentsamong high <strong>and</strong> low procrast<strong>in</strong>ators.The study showed that low procrast<strong>in</strong>ators wanted to (re)start work<strong>in</strong>g remotely <strong>in</strong> the first weeks of the onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g course, whereashigh procrast<strong>in</strong>ators put off start<strong>in</strong>g their work until the middle or end of the module, exhibit<strong>in</strong>g a cubic trend over time. This result isconsistent with the literature, demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g that when high procrast<strong>in</strong>ators are not given any <strong>in</strong>formation or feedback about their<strong>performance</strong>, they start work later than low procrast<strong>in</strong>ators (e.g., Fritzsche et al., 2003). Similarly, it was found that, <strong>in</strong> contrast to lowprocrast<strong>in</strong>ators, high procrast<strong>in</strong>ators’ desire to drop out showed an <strong>in</strong>verted-U curve, <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g at the midpo<strong>in</strong>t of the module. This appearsreasonable given the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that lower self-regulation predicts lower task persistence (e.g., Valler<strong>and</strong> & Bisson<strong>net</strong>te, 1992; Valler<strong>and</strong> &Sénécal, 1992). In contrast, a U-curve was observed for motivation to work remotely among high procrast<strong>in</strong>ators. While motivationrema<strong>in</strong>ed high <strong>and</strong> stable over time among low procrast<strong>in</strong>ators, it decreased dramatically at the midpo<strong>in</strong>t of the module among highprocrast<strong>in</strong>ators (week 5) <strong>and</strong> then <strong>in</strong>creased steadily up to the deadl<strong>in</strong>e (week 10). This “midpo<strong>in</strong>t effect” is consistent with the pattern ofbehaviors observed <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e collaborative learn<strong>in</strong>g (Mich<strong>in</strong>ov & Mich<strong>in</strong>ov, 2007, 2008), although <strong>in</strong> the present case the pattern onlyconcerns learners who tend to procrast<strong>in</strong>ate. Thus, <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environments, it appears that high procrast<strong>in</strong>ators experience greaternegative effect at the midpo<strong>in</strong>t of an onl<strong>in</strong>e course when faced with a deadl<strong>in</strong>e. Studies on group development suggest that patterns ofbehaviors differ over time when there is a deadl<strong>in</strong>e (e.g., Gersick, 1988, 1989), so it is important to identify differences <strong>in</strong> the way learnersallocate time <strong>in</strong> relation to a deadl<strong>in</strong>e accord<strong>in</strong>g to their tendency to procrast<strong>in</strong>ate (e.g., Gevers, Rutte, & van Eerde, 2006). For example, some<strong>in</strong>dividuals use a last m<strong>in</strong>ute cramm<strong>in</strong>g strategy (i.e., procrast<strong>in</strong>ators), whereas others work regularly over time or show a non-l<strong>in</strong>ear patternof behavior.Other <strong>in</strong>dividual differences likely to be related to procrast<strong>in</strong>ation should be exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> order to underst<strong>and</strong> better the social regulationprocesses at work <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environments <strong>and</strong> their impact on <strong>performance</strong>. For example, it may be particularly fruitful to exam<strong>in</strong>e<strong>in</strong>dividual pac<strong>in</strong>g styles, i.e. allocation of time relative to a deadl<strong>in</strong>e (e.g., Gevers et al., 2006), temporal focus (e.g., Shipp, Edward, & Lambert,2009), <strong>and</strong> achievement goal orientations reflect<strong>in</strong>g the representation of competence-based outcomes that <strong>in</strong>dividuals strive to atta<strong>in</strong> oravoid (e.g., Howell & Buro, 2009; Howell & Watson, 2007).6. Conclusion, limitations <strong>and</strong> perspectivesTo our knowledge, this is the first study to provide empirical evidence for the relationship between procrast<strong>in</strong>ation, <strong>participation</strong> <strong>in</strong>asynchronous discussion forums <strong>and</strong> <strong>performance</strong> <strong>in</strong> an onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environment.It appears that onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g success may be related to <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> time management <strong>and</strong> can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed, at least <strong>in</strong> part,by the level of learner <strong>participation</strong> <strong>in</strong> discussion forums. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that researchers <strong>and</strong> practitioners should take <strong>in</strong>to accountthe <strong>in</strong>dividual characteristics of learners as well as the communication strategies <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> optimiz<strong>in</strong>g onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>genvironments. Researchers thus need to <strong>in</strong>vestigate more thoroughly the relationship between procrast<strong>in</strong>ation, <strong>participation</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>performance</strong>,while these research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs will enable tutors to give greater assistance to onl<strong>in</strong>e students. We hope that the results of the presentstudy will provide fruitful material for future theoretical <strong>and</strong> applied research.Like many studies, the present research has some theoretical <strong>and</strong> methodological limitations which may reduce its scope. At least threelimitations may be touched on at a methodological <strong>and</strong> theoretical level. First, the study <strong>in</strong>volved only 40 adult learners which may limitgeneralizations to similar populations. Nevertheless, although the number of onl<strong>in</strong>e learners was small, an <strong>in</strong>itial test of our theoreticalhypotheses was made us<strong>in</strong>g a bootstrapp<strong>in</strong>g method particularly suitable for small samples. Second, the choice of Tuckman’s (1991)procrast<strong>in</strong>ation scale could be criticized <strong>in</strong> that it essentially measures task avoidance rather than postponement. Consequently, futurestudies should extend <strong>and</strong> support the present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs us<strong>in</strong>g alternative scales, such as Lay’s (1986) scale, or behavioral measures ofprocrast<strong>in</strong>ation which uses log analysis to measure postponement.F<strong>in</strong>ally, at a theoretical level, it is questionable whether active <strong>participation</strong> is necessary to <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>performance</strong> <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>gsett<strong>in</strong>gs. A high level of <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>and</strong> <strong>participation</strong> is clearly desirable <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g courses as highly participatory students havebeen shown to achieve better results, although it has also been found that m<strong>in</strong>imal onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>participation</strong> does not necessarily compromiseresults (e.g., Beaudo<strong>in</strong>, 2003). It appears that non-participative variables are also good predictors of <strong>performance</strong>, for example the number ofdiscussion posts <strong>and</strong> content pages viewed, <strong>and</strong> time spent view<strong>in</strong>g discussions (e.g., Morris, F<strong>in</strong>negan, & Sz-Shyan, 2005). As po<strong>in</strong>ted out byVonderwell <strong>and</strong> Zachariah (2005), <strong>participation</strong> is more than the total number of post<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a discussion forum (see also Hrast<strong>in</strong>ski, 2008).It is reasonable to assume that the learners <strong>in</strong> the present study who participated most were also those who looked at the discussion posts<strong>and</strong> were therefore the most active <strong>in</strong> the onl<strong>in</strong>e course.Overall, these results suggest that research should not rely solely on the number of messages posted to measure learner <strong>participation</strong>,but should also consider the possibility that students also learn through passive <strong>participation</strong> <strong>in</strong> forums by read<strong>in</strong>g the contributions ofother learners. It is possible that <strong>in</strong>dividuals with high procrast<strong>in</strong>ation tendencies may learn through observation, whereas those withlow procrast<strong>in</strong>ation tendencies prefer to learn by participat<strong>in</strong>g with others on discussion forums. However, this does not expla<strong>in</strong> ourf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that high procrast<strong>in</strong>ation tendencies are related to low <strong>performance</strong>. Future studies should exam<strong>in</strong>e more thoroughly thedist<strong>in</strong>ction between “active” <strong>and</strong> “passive” <strong>participation</strong> <strong>in</strong> discussion forums <strong>in</strong> order to identify the preferred strategies of high <strong>and</strong> lowprocrast<strong>in</strong>ators <strong>and</strong> their impact on <strong>performance</strong>. Similarly, future research should dist<strong>in</strong>guish between “passive” procrast<strong>in</strong>ators whoare paralyzed by their <strong>in</strong>decision to act <strong>and</strong> fail to complete tasks on time, <strong>and</strong> “active” procrast<strong>in</strong>ators who prefer to work underpressure <strong>and</strong> take deliberate decisions to procrast<strong>in</strong>ate (Chu & Choi, 2005). It is possible that the present study only considered “passive”procrast<strong>in</strong>ators, which could expla<strong>in</strong> the deleterious effects on <strong>performance</strong> mediated by a low level of <strong>participation</strong> <strong>in</strong> discussionforums.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!