31.07.2015 Views

A Comparative Analysis of Student Motivation in ... - Anitacrawley.net

A Comparative Analysis of Student Motivation in ... - Anitacrawley.net

A Comparative Analysis of Student Motivation in ... - Anitacrawley.net

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 417Gibb, 1997). Motivated by the curiosity and demand for knowledge ratherthan by external re<strong>in</strong>forcements, learners are more likely to become <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> distance education more deeply and thus experience and enjoy the knowledgeacquisition processes to a greater extent (Klesius et al.; Hardy & Boaz,1997). This assertion warrants the cont<strong>in</strong>ued <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>of</strong> various motivationalconstructs and their relationship with desirable learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.METHODOLOGYParticipantsParticipants <strong>in</strong> the present study consisted <strong>of</strong> 353 volunteer students fromthree universities who were enrolled <strong>in</strong> either traditional classroom courses,172 (48.7 %), or e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses, 181 (51.3 %). All three universitieswere located <strong>in</strong> the same urban area <strong>of</strong> Virg<strong>in</strong>ia and are fully accredited bythe Southern Association <strong>of</strong> Colleges and Schools. A total <strong>of</strong> 24 courses wassampled, 12 onl<strong>in</strong>e and 12 face-to-face courses from the three universities,with each university contribut<strong>in</strong>g both e-learn<strong>in</strong>g and traditional students.Course selection was based on two criteria: (a) similarity <strong>of</strong> content betweenthe traditional classroom and e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses and (b) use <strong>of</strong> experiencedfull-time faculty who had reputations as excellent classroom or onl<strong>in</strong>e teachers.All pr<strong>of</strong>essors were personally contacted by one <strong>of</strong> the researchers andagreed to participate <strong>in</strong> the study. <strong>Student</strong>s were asked by their pr<strong>of</strong>essors tovolunteer for the study and were told that volunteer<strong>in</strong>g or not volunteer<strong>in</strong>gwould not <strong>in</strong>fluence their course grade. The overall student volunteer ratewas 84 %, with the traditional classroom volunteer rate slightly higher thanthat <strong>of</strong> the e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses. N<strong>in</strong>ety-five (26.9 %) student volunteersattended a state university, 115 (32.6 %) attended a private Christian university,and 143 (40.5 %) attended a private secular university. The sampleconsisted <strong>of</strong> 301 (85.3 %) females and 52 (14.7 %) males. The higher percentage<strong>of</strong> females is consistent with the typical enrollment <strong>in</strong> the teachereducation courses that were sampled.Sett<strong>in</strong>gThe semester-long undergraduate and graduate courses exam<strong>in</strong>ed by thepresent study were conducted on the ma<strong>in</strong> university campus <strong>in</strong> a traditionalclassroom or delivered at a distance by the Inter<strong>net</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g the Blackboard.comSM e-learn<strong>in</strong>g system. E-learn<strong>in</strong>g participants were widely dispersedthroughout the US, although most resided <strong>in</strong> the eastern part <strong>of</strong> thecountry. <strong>Student</strong>s enrolled <strong>in</strong> traditional courses either lived <strong>in</strong> campus dormitoriesor commuted to campus. There was no onl<strong>in</strong>e component for thetraditional courses and the e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses had no face-to-face sessions.All three universities <strong>of</strong>fered both traditional and e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses. Typicaltitles <strong>of</strong> undergraduate courses were teach<strong>in</strong>g methods, geometry for


418 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and Bakerteachers, and classroom management. Graduate courses <strong>in</strong>cluded school lawand middle school adm<strong>in</strong>istration.InstrumentationThe 28 item Academic <strong>Motivation</strong> Scale – College (AMS-C 28) was usedto measure <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic, extr<strong>in</strong>sic, and amotivation <strong>in</strong> college students(Vallerand et al., 1992). This <strong>in</strong>strument, along with demographic questionsregard<strong>in</strong>g gender, ethnicity, and age, was adm<strong>in</strong>istered to all study participantsdur<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>al three weeks <strong>of</strong> the semester so that students wouldhave substantial exposure to their respective courses.Each item on the AMS-C 28 consists <strong>of</strong> a statement <strong>in</strong> response to thequestion “Why do you go to college?” One item is “Because I experiencepleasure and satisfaction while learn<strong>in</strong>g new th<strong>in</strong>gs.” Item responses arebased on a 7-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (Does not correspond at all)to 7 (corresponds exactly). Twelve <strong>of</strong> the items measure <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation,twelve measure extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, and four measure amotivation. The<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic scales consist <strong>of</strong> three subscales each. The three <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation subscales are: (a) to know, (b) to accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs, and (c)to experience stimulation. Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation to know is def<strong>in</strong>ed as engag<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> an activity for the pleasure and the satisfaction that one experienceswhile learn<strong>in</strong>g, explor<strong>in</strong>g, or try<strong>in</strong>g to understand someth<strong>in</strong>g new (Vallerand& Fortier, 1998). <strong>Motivation</strong> to accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs focuses on engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> agiven activity for the pleasure and satisfaction experienced while one isattempt<strong>in</strong>g to surpass oneself or to accomplish or create someth<strong>in</strong>g(Vallerand et al., 1992); thus, the focus is on the process <strong>of</strong> accomplish<strong>in</strong>gand not on the end result. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation to experience stimulationoccurs when one engages <strong>in</strong> an activity <strong>in</strong> order to experience pleasantsensations associated ma<strong>in</strong>ly with one’s senses, for example, sensory andaesthetic pleasure (Vallerand et al., 1992).The three extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation subscales, listed <strong>in</strong> order from highest to lowestself-determ<strong>in</strong>ation, are: (a) identified regulation, (b) <strong>in</strong>trojected regulation,and (c) external regulation. Identified regulation is the most self-determ<strong>in</strong>edtype <strong>of</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation. It occurs when the student engages <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gbecause he or she has personally decided to do so and because that activity hasvalue related to his or her goals (Vallerand et al., 1992). Introjected regulationis an ego-form <strong>of</strong> motivation that is driven by a perception <strong>of</strong> what others mightth<strong>in</strong>k. It can also <strong>in</strong>volve actions that are carried out based on cont<strong>in</strong>gencies, forexample, adopt<strong>in</strong>g behavior to avoid guilt or anxiety. Consequently, <strong>in</strong>ternalizationmay not fully occur. Motives that are only partially <strong>in</strong>ternalized may beexperienced as <strong>in</strong>ternally coercive if the motive conflicts with other aspects <strong>of</strong>the self. External regulation, the most extreme form <strong>of</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, isbased on pressure or rewards that come from the social environment, such ascareer advancement or pass<strong>in</strong>g a course (Vallerand et al.).


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 419The f<strong>in</strong>al scale generated by the AMS-C 28 measures amotivation.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Vallerand et al. (1992), amotivation is the state <strong>of</strong> lack<strong>in</strong>g an<strong>in</strong>tention to act. Amotivation can result from not valu<strong>in</strong>g a behavior (Ryan,1995), not feel<strong>in</strong>g competent regard<strong>in</strong>g the behavior (Deci, 1975), or notbeliev<strong>in</strong>g it will yield a desired outcome (Seligman, 1975).Scales can range as follows: <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, from low<strong>of</strong> 12 to a high <strong>of</strong> 84; each <strong>of</strong> the six <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic subscales as wellas the amotivation scale, from a low <strong>of</strong> 4 to a high <strong>of</strong> 28. Vallerand et al.(1992) provided evidence <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument validity and identified the overallscale’s <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency reliability as .86 based on coefficient alpha. Inthe present study, overall AMS-C 28 reliability was .91. The reliability coefficientsfor the <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, and amotivationscales were .93, .89, and .91 respectively.Design and <strong>Analysis</strong>The present study employed a causal-comparative design to respond tothe follow<strong>in</strong>g research question: Are the population means for higher educationstudent scores on motivation the same or different based on typecourse (e-learn<strong>in</strong>g, traditional), student status (undergraduate, graduate), andethnicity (African American, Caucasian, other)? Dependent measures werethe seven subscales generated by the AMS-C 28: the three <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationsubscales, the three extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation subscales, and amotivation.Multivariate analysis <strong>of</strong> variance (MANOVA) was used to analyze the data.Specific procedures used are described <strong>in</strong> the results section.RESULTSPercent composition <strong>of</strong> traditional classroom and e-learn<strong>in</strong>g groups bygender, by age, by student status, and by ethnicity are displayed <strong>in</strong> Table 1.Chi-square cont<strong>in</strong>gency table analysis revealed no differences <strong>in</strong> the demographicmakeup <strong>of</strong> the traditional classroom and e-learn<strong>in</strong>g groups based ongender, Pearson χ 2 (1, N = 353) = .25, p = .62, age, Pearson χ 2 (2, N = 353) =.69, p = .71, and student status, Pearson χ 2 (1, N = 353) = 2.84, p = .09. However,there was a greater proportion <strong>of</strong> African American students <strong>in</strong> the traditionalclassroom group than <strong>in</strong> the e-learn<strong>in</strong>g group, Pearson χ 2 (2, N = 353)= 14.43, p = .001, Cramers’ V = .20. Consequently ethnicity was <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>the analysis to determ<strong>in</strong>e if this imbalance confounded study results.The pooled means (with standard deviations <strong>in</strong> parentheses) for thedependent measures are 57.89 (15.10) for <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, 62.60 (14.38)for extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, and 6.02 (4.57) for amotivation. Pooled descriptivestatistics for the <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation subscales are 21.95 (4.88) for to know,20.05 (5.86) for to accomplish, and 15.89 (6.13) for to stimulate. The extr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation subscales are 22.77 (4.89) for identified regulation, 19.55


420 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and BakerTable 1Demographic PercentagesCharacteristic Face-to-face % Onl<strong>in</strong>e % Total %GenderFemale 84.3 % 86.2 % 85.3 %Male 15.7 % 13.8 % 14.7 %AgeUnder 30 52.3 % 55.8 % 54.1 %30-40 27.3 % 27.1 % 27.2 %Over 40 20.3 % 17.1 % 18.7 %<strong>Student</strong> statusUndergraduate 65.1 % 56.4 % 60.6 %Graduate 34.9 % 43.6 % 39.4 %EthnicityAfrican American 34.9 % 23.2 % 28.9 %Caucasian 59.9 % 60.2 % 60.1 %Other 5.2 % 16.6 % 12.0 %(6.32) for <strong>in</strong>trojected regulation, and 20.28 (6.11) for external regulation.Means and standard deviations for dependent variables disaggregated by thetwo <strong>in</strong>dependent measures, type course and student status, are displayed <strong>in</strong>Table 2. Similarly, descriptive statistics disaggregated by type course andethnicity are displayed <strong>in</strong> Table 3. Intercorrelations are reported <strong>in</strong> Table 4.A three-way MANOVA was conducted to determ<strong>in</strong>e the effect <strong>of</strong> typecourse (e-learn<strong>in</strong>g, traditional), student status (undergraduate, graduate), andethnicity (African American, Caucasian, other) on the seven dependent measures(the three <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation subscales, the three extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationsubscales, and amotivation). Significant ma<strong>in</strong> effects were found betweenthe two course types, Wilks’s λ = .94, F(7, 336) = 2.94, p = .005, η 2 = .06,and the two student statuses, Wilks’s λ = .95, F(7, 336) = 2.62, p = .012, η 2= .05. The ethnicity ma<strong>in</strong> effect was not significant, Wilks’s λ = .93, F(14,672) = 1.68, p = .055, η 2 = .03. Significant first order <strong>in</strong>teraction effects werefound for type course x student status, Wilks’s λ = .96, F(7, 336) = 2.11, p= .043, η 2 = .04, and for ethnicity x student status, Wilks’s λ = .91, F(14,672) = 2.20, p = .007, η 2 = .04. The type course x ethnicity <strong>in</strong>teraction wasnot significant, Wilks’s λ = .98, F(14, 672) = 1.40, p = .15, η 2 = .03. The secondorder type course x student status x ethnicity <strong>in</strong>teraction was also notsignificant, Wilks’s λ = .98, F(7, 336) = .95, p = .47, η 2 = .02.Post hoc ANOVA was conducted on each dependent measure follow<strong>in</strong>gsignificant MANOVA effects. For the course type ma<strong>in</strong> effect, the e-learn<strong>in</strong>ggroup scored higher than the traditional group on all three <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationsubscales: to know, F(1, 342) = 9.39, p = .002, η 2 = .03, to accom-


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 421Table 2Means and Standard Deviations (<strong>in</strong> Parentheses) for Type Courseand <strong>Student</strong> Status (N = 353)Variable Undergraduate GraduateTraditional classroom students (n = 172)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationIntr<strong>in</strong>sic – To know 20.26 (5.34) 22.13 (4.51)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs 18.07 (6.53) 19.33 (5.89)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To experience stimulation 13.76 (6.19) 15.60 (6.09)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationExtr<strong>in</strong>sic – Identified regulation 22.42 (4.40) 21.90 (5.13)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – Introjected regulation 19.42 (6.31) 18.50 (6.67)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – External regulation 20.41 (5.57) 20.25 (6.45)Amotivation 6.10 (4.19) 5.32 (3.30)E-learn<strong>in</strong>g students (n = 181)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationIntr<strong>in</strong>sic – To know 22.50 (4.46) 23.51 (4.34)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs 21.26 (5.16) 21.82 (4.74)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To experience stimulation 16.85 (6.48) 17.90 (4.54)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationExtr<strong>in</strong>sic – Identified regulation 23.71 (5.15) 22.72 (4.92)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – Introjected regulation 20.71 (5.94) 19.04 (6.41)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – External regulation 21.44 (6.16) 18.61 (6.24)Amotivation 7.03 (5.78) 5.14 (3.89)Note: The total <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation scales can each range from a low <strong>of</strong> 12 to a high <strong>of</strong>84. All rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g scales can each range from a low <strong>of</strong> 4 to a high <strong>of</strong> 28.plish th<strong>in</strong>gs, F(1, 342) = 7.83, p = .005, η 2 = .02, and to experience stimulation,F(1, 342) = 13.15, p < .001, η 2 = .04. Differences <strong>in</strong> the three extr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation and amotivation measures were not significant. For the studentstatus ma<strong>in</strong> effect, the graduate group scored higher than the undergraduategroup on <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic to know, F(1, 342) = 6.53, p = .011, η 2 = .02, and <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicto experience stimulation, F(1, 342) = 5.30, p = .022, η 2 = .02. However,the undergraduate group scored higher on extr<strong>in</strong>sic external regulation,F(1, 342) = 4.05, p = .045, η 2 = .01.For the type course x student status <strong>in</strong>teraction effect, significant <strong>in</strong>teractionswere observed only for extr<strong>in</strong>sic external regulation, F(1, 342) = 4.03, p= .046, η 2 = .01, and amotivation, F(1, 342) = 5.96, p = .015, η 2 = .02.Although the e-learn<strong>in</strong>g group scored higher than the traditional group onexternal regulation <strong>in</strong> both the undergraduate and graduate subpopulations, thedifferences narrowed considerably <strong>in</strong> the graduate subpopulation. Moreover,although the e-learn<strong>in</strong>g group scored higher on amotivation than the tradition-


422 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and BakerTable 3Means and Standard Deviations (<strong>in</strong> Parentheses) for Type Courseand Ethnicity (N = 353)Variable African American Caucasian OtherTraditional classroom students (n = 172)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationIntr<strong>in</strong>sic – To know 21.73 (4.79) 20.65 (5.14) 18.44 (6.67)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs 19.17 (5.85) 18.09(6.56) 19.00 (6.91)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To experience stimulation 15.37 (6.49) 13.92 (5.94) 13.44 (7.00)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationExtr<strong>in</strong>sic – Identified regulation 22.92 (4.23) 22.01 (4.79) 20.33 (5.20)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – Introjected regulation 20.15 (6.47) 18.45 (6.42) 19.56 (5.98)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – External regulation 22.32 (4.94) 19.43 (6.08) 17.89 (6.33)Amotivation 7.02 (5.24) 4.98 (2.34) 7.56 (5.59)E-learn<strong>in</strong>g students (n = 181)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationIntr<strong>in</strong>sic – To know 22.14 (4.52) 23.53 (4.36) 21.90 (4.28)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs 20.64 (5.06) 22.17 (4.66) 20.30 (5.68)Intr<strong>in</strong>sic – To experience stimulation 17.07 (5.71) 17.32 (5.24) 17.60 (7.41)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationExtr<strong>in</strong>sic – Identified regulation 22.50 (6.12) 23.65 (4.86) 23.00 (4.07)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – Introjected regulation 19.21 (5.26) 20.13 (6.50) 20.50 (6.34)Extr<strong>in</strong>sic – External regulation 20.21 (6.78) 20.15 (5.81) 20.40 (7.63)Amotivation 7.93 (7.35) 5.65 (4.50) 5.80 (2.22)Note: African American, n = 102; Caucasian, n = 212; other, n = 39. The total <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation scales can each range from a low <strong>of</strong> 12 to a high <strong>of</strong> 84. All rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g scales can eachrange from a low <strong>of</strong> 4 to a high <strong>of</strong> 28.al group among undergraduates, the opposite was true for graduate students.F<strong>in</strong>ally, for the ethnicity x student status <strong>in</strong>teraction effect, significant<strong>in</strong>teractions were observed only for amotivation, F(2, 342) = 7.54, p = .001,η 2 = .04. That is, although African American students scored higher on amotivation<strong>in</strong> undergraduate versus graduate courses, amotivation scores weresimilar for undergraduate and graduate Caucasian students, and amotivationwas higher for graduate students who classified their ethnicity as other thanfor undergraduate students so classified.DISCUSSIONThe present study addressed the follow<strong>in</strong>g research question. Are thepopulation means for higher education student scores on motivation thesame or different based on type course (e-learn<strong>in</strong>g, traditional), student sta-


424 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and Bakerlearn<strong>in</strong>g environments. Such differences may be attributable to the types <strong>of</strong>students who would self-select e-learn<strong>in</strong>g as their educational mode <strong>of</strong>choice. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Rogers’ (1995) diffusion <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>novation theory, thecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> those quick to embrace new <strong>in</strong>novations (i.e., the <strong>in</strong>novatorsand early adopters who together make up the first 15 % <strong>of</strong> adopters) aredifferent from those who adopt the <strong>in</strong>novation later. These earlier adopterstend to have a greater ability to deal with uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty, higher <strong>in</strong>telligence,greater comfort with change, and a more favorable attitude toward scienceand technology. Individuals on the diffusion curve also differ socially withearly adopters hav<strong>in</strong>g more social participation and a more highly <strong>in</strong>terconnectedpersonal <strong>net</strong>work than later adopters. Most appropriate to this study,Rogers reported that earlier adopters “have higher aspirations (for formaleducation, occupations, and so on)” (p. 274) and that they more activelyseek <strong>in</strong>formation about the <strong>in</strong>novations themselves. Such patterns are consistentwith the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learners manifest<strong>in</strong>g higher <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationlevels, although the question rema<strong>in</strong>s whether such differences willcont<strong>in</strong>ue as e-learn<strong>in</strong>g becomes more common and the majority (and laggards)embrace the medium. Only through additional research can theweight <strong>of</strong> these differences be fully understood.Studies directed at support<strong>in</strong>g the hypothesized differences <strong>in</strong> self-efficacyshould also <strong>in</strong>clude measurements <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> the four sources <strong>of</strong>efficacy <strong>in</strong>formation: (a) performance accomplishments, (b) vicarious experiences,(c) verbal persuasion, and (d) physiological/emotive arousals (Bandura,1977). These sources <strong>of</strong> efficacy <strong>in</strong>formation may help expla<strong>in</strong> the differences<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation between e-learn<strong>in</strong>g and traditional studentsdue to different experiences, and <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>in</strong>structional design <strong>in</strong> a mannerthat strengthens self-efficacy beliefs and, thus, facilitates <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation.If future research confirms that onl<strong>in</strong>e pedagogy fosters higher levels <strong>of</strong>academic <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, faculty <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g lifelong learn<strong>in</strong>gmay use this <strong>in</strong>formation for <strong>in</strong>structional design. The cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>of</strong>structured education may transition from primarily face-to-face <strong>in</strong>structionto blended and even primarily onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>struction. Future research must beconducted to support this hypothesized relationship between academic<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation and lifelong learn<strong>in</strong>g as well as the appropriate timescales, for example, from k<strong>in</strong>dergarten to 12th grade, or from baccalaureateto doctoral studies, or content scales, for example, from a beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g course<strong>in</strong> life science to an advanced biology course, or from a sem<strong>in</strong>ar at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<strong>of</strong> the semester to <strong>in</strong>dependent work at the end <strong>of</strong> the semester for specificsubject matter, associated with this pedagogical transition. In addition,the present <strong>in</strong>vestigation can be extended to determ<strong>in</strong>e specifically ifobserved levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation are different for vary<strong>in</strong>g facultymembers. Such differences may <strong>of</strong>fer <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to specific <strong>in</strong>structionalstrategies that may be more effective <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation as


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 425well as <strong>in</strong>fluence the design <strong>of</strong> future <strong>in</strong>tervention experiments.While the present results suggest that both onl<strong>in</strong>e and face-to-face studentsare equally goal oriented (as evidenced by nonsignificant differences <strong>in</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation), the onl<strong>in</strong>e learners are more learn<strong>in</strong>g and/or activity oriented.Thus, this result is consistent with Houle’s observation <strong>of</strong> orientation overlap.Houle (1961) recognized that adult learners can exhibit vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees <strong>of</strong>three learner orientations that <strong>in</strong>clude engagement <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g activitiesbecause (a) such activities represent the path to realiz<strong>in</strong>g specific goals (i.e.,goal-orientation), (b) learn<strong>in</strong>g is personally gratify<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., learn<strong>in</strong>g orientation),and (c) such activities are socially gratify<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., activity orientation).Heckhausen and Kuhl (1985) “def<strong>in</strong>e [a] goal as the molar endstate whoseatta<strong>in</strong>ment requires actions by the <strong>in</strong>dividual pursu<strong>in</strong>g it” (pp. 137-138); thus,all three learner orientations represent goal-directedness with differentialideated goals. Heckhausen and Kuhl further noted that “goals rest on three levels<strong>of</strong> endstates with an ascend<strong>in</strong>g hierarchical order” (p. 138) as follows:On the first-order level the endstates are the activities themselves:the <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong>, or the enjoyment <strong>of</strong>, do<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g repetitively orcont<strong>in</strong>uously, because it provides excitement….On a second-orderlevel the endstate is an action outcome with characteristics that arerequired or preset and that are <strong>in</strong>herently valuable. F<strong>in</strong>ally, at thethird-order level, the endstate refers to desirable consequences thatmight arise from an achieved outcome. (p. 138)The three levels <strong>of</strong> endstates co<strong>in</strong>cide with Houle’s typology and re<strong>in</strong>forcethe important role <strong>of</strong> motivation <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g adult participation <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g or any other agentive activity. The pr<strong>in</strong>ciples from Grow’s (1991)staged self-directed learn<strong>in</strong>g model may also be used to guide <strong>in</strong>structionaldesign. The present results suggest that onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>structors may want todesign their courses more from a facilitator perspective rather than from ateacher perspective.<strong>Student</strong> StatusThe graduate student group scored significantly higher than the undergraduategroup on two <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation variables: to know and to experiencestimulation. However, the undergraduate group scored higher than thegraduate group on extr<strong>in</strong>sic external regulation. All rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g differenceswere not significant. The observed differences between undergraduate andgraduate students may be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by the large proportion <strong>of</strong> high schoolgraduates that enroll <strong>in</strong> college.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the National Center for Education Statistics (2003), 65% <strong>of</strong>16-24 year-olds who graduated from high school (or earned a GED) enrolled<strong>in</strong> college with<strong>in</strong> one year <strong>of</strong> graduation. While still not compulsory, the


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 427CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONSE-learn<strong>in</strong>g students manifested significantly stronger <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationthan traditional classroom students on all three <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation measures:(a) to know, (b) to accomplish th<strong>in</strong>gs, and (c) to experience stimulation.One possible explanation <strong>of</strong> these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs is that more <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicallymotivated students self-select onl<strong>in</strong>e versus traditional classroom courseswhere self-selection can apply to both new and cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g students. S<strong>in</strong>celess than 6 % <strong>of</strong> higher education students are enrolled <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e courses,they are more likely to be <strong>in</strong>novators and early adopters <strong>in</strong> Rogers’ (1995)diffusion <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>novations categories and thus have different characteristicsthan the ma<strong>in</strong>stream. This possibility is consistent with reports that <strong>in</strong>dividualswho choose distance over traditional education are different from theironcampus counterparts (Feasley, 1983) and may be more <strong>in</strong>ternally motivatedby factors such as <strong>in</strong>tellectual curiosity.Consistent with expectancy value theory (Atk<strong>in</strong>son, 1982; Vroom, 1964),which posits that people engage <strong>in</strong> specific activities due to the perceivedvalue <strong>of</strong> likely consequences, e-learn<strong>in</strong>g students may choose distance overtraditional education because <strong>of</strong> a stronger perceived correlation betweenanticipated educational experiences and personal and/or self-evaluative outcomes.In addition, because <strong>of</strong> the mediat<strong>in</strong>g role <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy <strong>in</strong> pathanalytic models <strong>of</strong> expectancy value theory, e-learn<strong>in</strong>g students may alsoperceive themselves to be more capable <strong>of</strong> perform<strong>in</strong>g to self-satisfy<strong>in</strong>g levels<strong>in</strong> the onl<strong>in</strong>e environment than those students <strong>in</strong> traditional classroomcourses. This latter conclusion, <strong>of</strong> course, should be supported with futureself-efficacy research <strong>in</strong> academic environments similar to the present <strong>in</strong>vestigation;however, the mediat<strong>in</strong>g role <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy has been supportedwith research <strong>in</strong> other doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g (Bandura, 1997).Another possible explanation for the stronger <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation <strong>of</strong> e-learn<strong>in</strong>gstudents is that onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>struction facilitates <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation thereby expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the differences between the two groups. Thisview is consistent with research (Zhang, 1998) that suggests the e-learn<strong>in</strong>gmedium provides a learn<strong>in</strong>g environment that “emphasizes <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationand self-sponsored curiosity and creative situated learn<strong>in</strong>g” (p. 4). This rationaleis consistent with cognitive evaluation theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), whichposits that <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation is maximized when <strong>in</strong>dividuals feel competentand self-determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>g with their environment. They po<strong>in</strong>ted out that“<strong>in</strong>terpersonal events and structures (e.g., rewards, communications, feedback)that conduce toward feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> competence dur<strong>in</strong>g action can enhance <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation for that action because they allow satisfaction <strong>of</strong> the basic psychologicalneed for competence” (Ryan & Deci, 2000, p. 58).The e-learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structor plays a crucial role <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and susta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gstudents’ motivational level by plann<strong>in</strong>g structures and facilitat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ter-


428 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and Bakerpersonal events. Additional research is needed to confirm the role <strong>of</strong> e-learn<strong>in</strong>gpedagogy, computer-mediated communication, and course design <strong>in</strong> nurtur<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation. Moreover, learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes that <strong>in</strong>cludereduced attrition, deeper <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g, and <strong>in</strong>creased levels <strong>of</strong> studentsuccess, task value, and better well-be<strong>in</strong>g tend to covary with <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation (Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay, 1997). Consequently research is alsoneeded to determ<strong>in</strong>e if better educational outcomes accompany the stronger<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation noted <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e courses. However, a considerable body <strong>of</strong>research <strong>in</strong> the form <strong>of</strong> comparison studies suggests no significant differencebetween a variety <strong>of</strong> distance education and traditional course educationaloutcomes (Russell, 1999). No<strong>net</strong>heless, several studies found differences <strong>in</strong>completion or student satisfaction, although various measures <strong>of</strong> achievementwere <strong>of</strong>ten the same, or nearly the same, between the two types <strong>of</strong> coursescompared. Perhaps the reason for the lack <strong>of</strong> research evidence regard<strong>in</strong>gsuperior educational outcomes <strong>in</strong> onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g rests with other learn<strong>in</strong>grelated variables, such as sense <strong>of</strong> community, which may be weaker <strong>in</strong> e-learn<strong>in</strong>g environments thereby <strong>of</strong>fsett<strong>in</strong>g the value <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased student<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation. Clearly additional research is required.It is possible is that the 12 e-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses sampled <strong>in</strong> the present studyare examples <strong>of</strong> high quality courses. Bernard et al. (2004) reported largevariability <strong>in</strong> the quality <strong>of</strong> distance education programs. In particular, theynoted that “a substantial number <strong>of</strong> [distance education programs] providebetter achievement results, are viewed more positively, and have higherretention rates than their classroom counterparts. On the other hand, a substantialnumber <strong>of</strong> [distance education programs] are far worse than classroom<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> regard to all three measures” (p. 406). Perhaps if othercourses were sampled the outcomes would be different. Consequentlyresearch to confirm and extend the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> the present study is needed.Graduate students reported stronger <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation than undergraduatestudents. Consequently, they were more likely to pursue educationalprograms for the pleasure and the satisfaction that one experiences whilelearn<strong>in</strong>g, explor<strong>in</strong>g, or try<strong>in</strong>g to understand someth<strong>in</strong>g new (Vallerand &Fortier, 1998). Graduate students, hav<strong>in</strong>g experienced undergraduate education,may be more likely to pursue advanced degrees for the <strong>in</strong>herent satisfactionor challenge rather than for the perceived need for an advanceddegree. Undergraduate students, on the other hand, are more likely to bemotivated by external factors, such as the perceived need for a college educationbased on pressures from family and the job market.This study suggests several implications for practice, provided futureresearch supports the present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Due to higher levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationpresent <strong>in</strong> e-learners versus traditional learners, course designers shouldvary the construction <strong>of</strong> these two types <strong>of</strong> courses to better match the motivationalneeds <strong>of</strong> the students. E-learn<strong>in</strong>g courses should <strong>in</strong>corporate methods


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 429better suited to the self-regulated learner such as allow<strong>in</strong>g the student a greaterrole <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g objectives, def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g activities and timel<strong>in</strong>es,and reflect<strong>in</strong>g on how well self-selected objectives have been met. Fortraditional learners with less <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation, course designs may also bemore traditional with the educator provid<strong>in</strong>g greater external control. Whilecerta<strong>in</strong>ly these suggestions for practice exist on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum (i.e., not all e-learners have greater <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation than traditional learners), the presentresults suggest that <strong>in</strong>structional methods more suited for self-directed learnersrepresent a better approach <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g successful e-learn<strong>in</strong>g.ReferencesAshby, C. M. (2002). Distance education: Growth <strong>in</strong> distance education programs and implicationsfor federal education policy (GAO Report GAO-02-1125T). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: United StatesGeneral Account<strong>in</strong>g Office.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, J. W. (1982). Old and new conceptions <strong>of</strong> how expected consequences <strong>in</strong>fluenceactions. In N. T. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models <strong>in</strong> psychology(pp. 17-52). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unify<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>of</strong> behavioral change. PsychologicalReview, 84(2), 191-215.Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise <strong>of</strong> control. New York: W. H. Freeman.Benware, C., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g with an active versus passive motivationalset. American Educational Research Journal, 21(4), 755–765.Bernard, R., M., Abrami, P. C., Lou, Y., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Wozney, L., et al., (2004). Howdoes distance education compare with classroom <strong>in</strong>struction? A meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> the empiricalliterature. Review <strong>of</strong> Educational Research, 74(3), 379-439.Bures, E. M., Abrami, P. C., & Amundsen, C. (2000). <strong>Student</strong> motivation to learn via computerconferenc<strong>in</strong>g. Research <strong>in</strong> Higher Education, 41(5), 593-621.Carnevale, D. (2005, June 28). Onl<strong>in</strong>e courses cont<strong>in</strong>ue to grow drastically, enroll<strong>in</strong>g nearly 1million, report says. Academe Today: The Chronicle <strong>of</strong> Higher Education’s Daily Report for Subscribers.Retrieved April 10, 2007, from http://chronicle.com/daily/2005/06/2005062802t.htmCoussement, S. (1995). Educational telecommunication: Does it work? An attitude study. (ERICDocument Reproduction Service No. ED391465)Cov<strong>in</strong>gton, M. V. (2000). Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: An <strong>in</strong>tegrative review.Annual Review <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 51(1), 171-200.Darkenwald, G. G., & Valent<strong>in</strong>e, T. (1985). Factor structure <strong>of</strong> deterrents to public participation <strong>in</strong>adult education. Adult Education Quarterly, 35(4), 177-193.Deci, E. L. (1975). Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.Deci, E. L., Nezlek, J., & She<strong>in</strong>man, L. (1981). Characteristics <strong>of</strong> the rewarder and <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicmotivation <strong>of</strong> the rewardee. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1–10.Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation and self-determ<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong> human behavior.New York: Plenum Press.Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The support <strong>of</strong> autonomy and the control <strong>of</strong> behavior. In E. T.Higg<strong>in</strong>s & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), <strong>Motivation</strong>al science: Social and personality perspectives. Keyread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> social psychology (pp. 128-145). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.


430 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and BakerEccles, J. S. (1983). Expectancies, values and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievementand achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 75-146). San Francisco:Freeman.Egan, M., & Gibb, G. (1997). <strong>Student</strong>-centered <strong>in</strong>struction for the design <strong>of</strong> telecourses. In T. Cyrs(Ed.), Teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g at a distance: What it takes to effectively design, deliver, andevaluate programs (pp. 33-39). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Feasley, C. E. (1983). Serv<strong>in</strong>g learners at a distance: A guide to program practices. ASHE-ERICHigher Education Research Report No. 5. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: Association for the Study <strong>of</strong> HigherEducation and ERIC Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse on Higher Education. (ERIC Document Report No.ED238350)Feather, N. T. (Ed.). (1982). Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models <strong>in</strong> psychology.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Fjort<strong>of</strong>t, N. F. (1996). Persistence <strong>in</strong> a distance learn<strong>in</strong>g program: A case <strong>in</strong> pharmaceuticaleducation. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Distance Education, 10(3), 49-59.Gibson, C. C. (2003). Learners and learn<strong>in</strong>g: The need for theory. In M. G. Moore & W. G. Anderson(Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> distance education (pp. 147-160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Gottfried, A. E., Flem<strong>in</strong>g, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Cont<strong>in</strong>uity <strong>of</strong> academic <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivationfrom childhood through late adolescence: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology,93, 3-13.Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy <strong>in</strong> children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g: An experimental and<strong>in</strong>dividual difference <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 890–898.Grow, G. (1991). Teach<strong>in</strong>g learners to be self-directed: A stage approach. Adult EducationQuarterly, 41(3), 125-149.Hardy, D. W., & Boaz, M. H. (1997). Learner development: Beyond the technology. In T. E. Cyrs(Eds.), Teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g at a distance: What it takes to effectively design, deliver, andevaluate programs (pp.41-48). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: The dead ends and short cuts on thelong way to action. In M. Frese & J. Sab<strong>in</strong>i (Eds.), Goal-directed behavior: The concept <strong>of</strong>action <strong>in</strong> psychology (pp. 134-157). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Houle, C. O. (1961). The <strong>in</strong>quir<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>d: A study <strong>of</strong> the adult who cont<strong>in</strong>ues to learn. Madison:University <strong>of</strong> Wiscons<strong>in</strong> Press.Klesius, J. P., Homan, S. P., & Thompson, T. (1997). Distance education compared to traditional<strong>in</strong>struction: The students’ view. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Instructional Media, 24(3), 207-227.Knowles, M. (1980). The modern practice <strong>of</strong> adult education: From pedagogy to andragogy (2nded.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall/Cambridge.Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decompos<strong>in</strong>g self-regulation and self-control: The volitionalcomponents checklist. In J. Heckhausen, & C. Dweck (Eds.), Life span perspectives on motivationand control (pp. 15-49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Merisotis, J. P., & Phipps, R. A. (1999). What’s the difference? Outcomes <strong>of</strong> distance vs.traditional classroom-based learn<strong>in</strong>g. Change, 31(3), 12-17.Miller, R., Brickman, P., & Bolen, D. (1975). Attribution versus persuasion as a means <strong>of</strong> modify<strong>in</strong>gbehavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 430-441.Miserand<strong>in</strong>o, M. (1996). Children who do well <strong>in</strong> school: Individual differences <strong>in</strong> perceivedcompetence and autonomy <strong>in</strong> above-average children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology,88(2), 203-214.


<strong>Student</strong> <strong>Motivation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Traditional Classroom and E-Learn<strong>in</strong>g Courses 431Moore, M. G., & Kearsley, G. (2005). Distance education: A systems view (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA:Wadsworth.National Center for Education Statistics. (2003). Digest <strong>of</strong> education statistics. Retrieved April 10,2007, from http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d03/tables/dt186.aspNewman, R. S. (1990). Children’s help-seek<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom: The role <strong>of</strong> motivational factorsand attitudes. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 82, 71-80.Oxford, R., Young, P., Ito, S., & Sumrall, M. (1993). Factors affect<strong>in</strong>g achievement <strong>in</strong> a satellitedeliveredJapanese language program. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Distance Education, 7(1), 11-25.Perdue, K. J. (2003). Web-based cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>of</strong>essional education: Uses, motivations, anddeterrents to participation. In M. G. Moore & W. G. Anderson (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> distanceeducation (pp. 615-630). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Ponton, M. K. (1999). The measurement <strong>of</strong> an adult’s <strong>in</strong>tention to exhibit personal <strong>in</strong>itiative <strong>in</strong>autonomous learn<strong>in</strong>g (Doctoral dissertation, George Wash<strong>in</strong>gton University, 1999). DissertationAbstracts International, 60, 3933.Ponton, M. K., Carr, P. B., & Derrick, M. G. (2004). A path analysis <strong>of</strong> the conative factorsassociated with autonomous learn<strong>in</strong>g. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Self-Directed Learn<strong>in</strong>g, 1(1), 59-69.Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>novations (4th ed). New York: The Free Press.Rosenbaum, M. (1989). Self-control under stress: The role <strong>of</strong> learned resourcefulness. AdvancedBehaviour Research Therapy, 11, 249-258Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for <strong>in</strong>ternal versus external control <strong>of</strong> re<strong>in</strong>forcement.Psychological Monographs, 33(1), 300-303.Russell, T. L. (1999). The no significant difference phenomenon. Raleigh: North Carol<strong>in</strong>a StateUniversity.Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrative processes. Journal <strong>of</strong>Personality, 63, 397–427.Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivations: Classic def<strong>in</strong>itions and newdirections. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54–67.Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Orig<strong>in</strong>s and pawns <strong>in</strong> the classroom: Self-report andprojective assessments <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> children’s perceptions. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personalityand Social Psychology, 50, 550–558.Schwittman, D. (1982). Time and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> distance study. Distance Education, 3(1), 141–156.Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Terrell, S., & Dr<strong>in</strong>gus, L. (1999). An <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>of</strong> the effect <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g style on studentsuccess <strong>in</strong> an onl<strong>in</strong>e learn<strong>in</strong>g environment. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Technology Systems,28(3), 231-238.Threlkeld, R., & Brzoska, K. (1994). Research <strong>in</strong> distance education. In B. Willis (Ed.), Distanceeducation: Strategies and tools (pp. 41-66). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational TechnologyPublications.Vallerand, R. J., & Fortier, M. S. (1998). Measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic and extr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation <strong>in</strong> sportand physical activity: A review and critique. In D. J. Duda (Ed.), Advancements <strong>in</strong> sport and exercisepsychology measurement (pp. 81-104). Morgantown: Fitness Information Technology.


432 Rovai, Ponton, Wight<strong>in</strong>g, and BakerVallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-determ<strong>in</strong>ation and persistence <strong>in</strong> areal-life sett<strong>in</strong>g: Toward a motivational model <strong>of</strong> high-school drop out. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personalityand Social Psychology, 72, 1161 – 1176.Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Briere, N .M., Senecal, C. B., & Vallieres, E. F. (1992).The academic motivation scale: A measure <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic, extr<strong>in</strong>sic, and amotivation <strong>in</strong> education.Educational & Psychological Measurement, 52(4), 1003-1017.Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.Zhang, P. (1998). A case study on technology use <strong>in</strong> distance learn<strong>in</strong>g. Journal <strong>of</strong> Research onComput<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Education, 30(4), 398-420.Ziel<strong>in</strong>ski, D. (2000). Can you keep learners onl<strong>in</strong>e? Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 37(3), 64-75.Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becom<strong>in</strong>g a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory <strong>in</strong>to Practice,41(2), 64 – 70.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!