11.12.2015 Views

League

LinkClick

LinkClick

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

how to see<br />

what to see<br />

Movement and Mindfulness<br />

Figure 1<br />

20 LINES from the <strong>League</strong><br />

Nicki Orbach<br />

Viewing art and creating<br />

art involves you as a whole<br />

person, not just a disembodied<br />

eye. Both involve a<br />

type of self-awareness that<br />

encompasses conscious<br />

and preconscious attention,<br />

intuition, a kinesthetic sensibility,<br />

and an awareness of<br />

the experience in the present<br />

moment. How we think,<br />

feel, and experience the<br />

world influences the way we<br />

see and paint.<br />

This world is filled with objects to which<br />

we apply names. The ability to categorize<br />

helps us to make instantaneous decisions,<br />

since we don’t<br />

have to figure<br />

out each individual<br />

visual<br />

experience<br />

every time we<br />

encounter it.<br />

We know what<br />

to expect.<br />

However, when<br />

we view art,<br />

seeing within<br />

categories can<br />

be limiting and<br />

actually obscure<br />

what is seen. As Paul Valery says, “to<br />

see is to forget the name of the thing one<br />

sees.” We are looking at things through a<br />

bias of names and the preconceptions that<br />

attend to those names. We actually don’t<br />

see things as they really are. One<br />

skill that is helpful when looking<br />

at a painting is to first just look,<br />

without using words. Then add<br />

meaning or a narrative later. Art<br />

critic Roberta Smith says, “I learn<br />

from everything I look at, good,<br />

bad, or indifferent. I follow my eye<br />

reflexively; if it is drawn toward<br />

something, I pay attention and<br />

try to find out why. You train your<br />

eye, build up a mental image bank,<br />

and constantly try to pinpoint why<br />

some things are convincing and<br />

others aren’t.”<br />

Figure 2<br />

Viewing a scene involves sensory,<br />

cognitive, and motor processes. Seeing is<br />

action. There is only a small portion of the<br />

retina, the fovea, where we see things in<br />

sharp focus. Figure 1 illustrates what we<br />

can make out in the visual scene if we fix<br />

our eyes on the woman’s face. Her face<br />

would be in sharp focus but as things get<br />

farther away, they get harder to make out<br />

and the colors are grayer. This part of the<br />

visual scene is in our peripheral vision.<br />

We fix on something in the environment<br />

to see it clearly, and then our eyes move<br />

to the next point of fixation in order to<br />

see that part in sharp focus. This allows<br />

different parts of the visual field to appear<br />

on the fovea over time. Alfred L. Yarbus<br />

performed a number of experiments in<br />

1965 pertaining to eye movements using<br />

Ilya Repin’s painting The Unexpected<br />

Visitor (Figure 2). He asked his subjects to<br />

just look at the painting, and he recorded<br />

Figure 3

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!