17.09.2020 Views

To make or not to make? Effect of implementation on idea selection

Pre-implementation activities like idea selection play a crucial role in the innovation process. We found that children inhibit themselves in selecting original ideas once expecting idea implementation. Interested for more? See this video presentation at MIC Conference 2020.

Pre-implementation activities like idea selection play a crucial role in the innovation process. We found that children inhibit themselves in selecting original ideas once expecting idea implementation. Interested for more? See this video presentation at MIC Conference 2020.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

To make or not to

make?

The effect of expected

implementation on

idea selection

Kim van Broekhoven 1 , Barbara Belfi 1 & Lex Borghans 2

1

Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market (ROA),

Maastricht University, the Netherlands

2

Department of Economics, Maastricht University, The

Netherlands

16 September 2020

MIC Conference 2020

1/14


Motivation

• Primary schools spend more attention to the development of creative and innovative

thinking skills.

• Routine tasks may be automated by robots and AI-supported agents, while nonroutine

tasks require higher-order skills, such as creativity and innovation.

(E.g. OECD, 2019; WEF, 2016; 2018)

• Constructivist pedagogies and strategies have spread across primary schools (e.g.

Montessori education, design thinking, project-problem or research-based learning).

(E.g. McLoughlin & Lee, 2008; Piagèt, 1964; Vygotsky, 1980)

• Children are asked to work on transforming their ideas into a tangible and physical

product (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2010; Cardarello, 2014; Davies et al., 2013).

• Little is known how the (expected) transformation of ideas into tangible products

affects idea selection among primary school children.

Aim

• To investigate the impact of expected implementation of ideas on the selection of

novel and feasible ideas.

• To investigate whether children’s personality moderates this relationship.

1/14


Effect of expected implementation on idea selection

• Children may experiment with different types of ideas and materials, and take risks.

VS

• Children may be afraid “it will be too difficult” or “other children may laugh at me”.

• Yuan and Zhou (2008) investigated expected evaluation on idea selection.

- Students who expected evaluation modified their ideas more to make them appropriate to implement.

• Extrinsic constraints have a facilitating effect on the performance of algorithmic tasks

(Amabile & Pratt, 2016; McGraw, 1978).

• Novelty and usefulness trade-off (E.g. Manske & Davis, 1968; Mueller et al., 2012; Rietzschel et al., 2010).

Hypotheses

1) Children who expect implementation, compared to children who do not, are more

likely to select feasible ideas, but less likely to select original ideas.

2) Children who expect implementation, compared to children who do not, feasibility

will increase more than originality will decrease.

2/14


The moderating role of personality

• Personality traits have been strongly linked to creativity (E.g. Batey & Furnham, 2006; Feist, 1998).

• Openness to experience, conscientiousness and agreeabless have been linked to idea

selection (Toh & Miller, 2016).

Hypotheses

• Children open to experience are more flexible in embracing novel ideas, even though

these novel ideas may be untested or fanciful (Goldberg, 1990; Choi, 2004; George & Zhou, 2001).

- The effect of expected implementation on idea selection (i.e. higher feasibility, lower originality) will

become smaller among children who are more open to experience (H3a).

• Children high on conscientiousness tend to focus on “doing things right” (Shiner & Caspi, 2003).

- The effect of expected implementation on idea selection (i.e. higher feasibility, lower originality) will

become smaller among children who are more conscientiousness (H3b).

• Children high on agreeableness tend to care about others’ feelings and avoid being in

conflict with other children. Novel ideas challenge status quo and can cause conflict

with other children (Choi, 2007; Lim & Choi, 2009).

- The effect of expected implementation on idea selection (i.e. higher feasibility, lower originality) will

become bigger among children who are more agreeable (H3c).

3/14


Method: sample

• 403 grade-8 children from thirteen primary schools in the South of the

Netherlands between February and June 2019.

- 49.9% girls, 50.1% boys

- 11.6 years of age (SD = 0.48)

• From the 403 children, of 353 children personality traits were measured.

• The Ethics Review Committee from Maastricht University approved the

study.

4/14


Method: experimental design

5/14


Method: manipulation

Expected implementation (N=202)

“A toy factory needs your help! The toy

factory makes toy animals, such as

elephants, dogs, rabbits and so on. They

would like to receive innovative ideas to

change a toy elephant. They will first test

these ideas on a toy elephant made of paper.

You will craft these ideas.”

Non-expected implementation (N=201)

“A toy factory needs your help! The toy

factory makes toy animals, such as

elephants, dogs, rabbits and so on. They

would like to receive innovative ideas to

change a toy elephant. They will first test

these ideas on a toy elephant made of paper.

You will NOT craft these ideas, because you

will be crafting ideas for monkey toys.”

6/14


Method: experimental design

7/14


Method: measures

Idea selection task

Prior to study, we conducted a pilot study:

36 grade-8 pupils generated ideas to improve a stuffed toy elephant

Product improvement of TTCT, Torrance, 1974

‣ 438 ideas → 63 ideas (excluding non-play uses, similar ideas)

• These 63 ideas were rated by seven experts

(i.e., 4 primary school teachers and 3 creativity researchers):

- Feasibility

- Originality

• To reduce the list of 63 ideas, a set of twenty ideas was selected:

- The interrater reliability was high (ICC, two-way random, consistency analysis):

- Overall intraclass correlation coefficient was .90

‣ Feasibility ICC = .94

‣ Originality ICC = .86

• By averaging the scores of the seven experts, each idea received a feasibility and

originality score.

8/14


Method: measures

Idea evaluation: Feasibility rating (Charles & Runco, 2001)

• Children were asked to select one of five faces that best

showed what they thought of the idea

1. Frown = very difficult to craft this idea;

2. Slight frown = difficult to craft this idea;

3. No expression = neither difficult nor easy to craft this idea;

4. Slight smile = easy to craft this idea;

5. Smile = very easy to craft this idea.

Idea evaluation: Originality rating (Charles & Runco, 2001)

• Popularity rating (1-10):

- Children were asked to guess how many children, out of 10, they thought would give a particular response

• Originality rating (1-5):

- Reversed values of (popularity rating / 2)

Personality traits: International Personality Item Pool (Goldberg et al., 2006)

• Openness to experience α = .75

• Conscientiousness α = .80

• Extraversion α = .71

• Agreeableness α = .74

• Neuroticism α = .82

9/14


Results: children’ own-rating

t(401) = -10.31, p = .000,

partial η2 = .210

t(401) = 3.78, p = .000,

partial η2 = .034

10/14


Results: experts’ rating

t(401) = -7.71, p = .000,

partial η2 = .129

t(401) = 2.90, p = .004,

partial η2 = .021

11/14


Results: the moderating role of conscientiousness

11/12


Discussion

Conclusion

• Children who expected idea implementation select more feasible ideas, at the cost of

originality

• Children become better able to juggle between the novelty and feasibility aspects of

ideas when they expect idea implementation.

• Children with high levels of conscientiousness were less affected by the expectation

of implementation than children demonstrating low levels of conscientiousness.

Implications for practice

1) Instructions towards children should not mention the implementation of the ideas

(at first instance).

2) Teachers should support children when they face highly novel, but seemingly

unrealistic ideas.

3) Tailor the instruction in assignments to the individual needs of children

14/14


Questions?

Kim.vanbroekhoven@docentenacademie.ru.nl

@KimvanBroekhov

14/14


References

Amabile, & Pratt, M. G. (2016). The dynamic componential model of creativity and innovation in organizations: Making progress, making

meaning. Research in Organizational Behavior, 36, 157-183.

Baer, M., Oldham, G. R., Jacobsohn, G. C., & Hollingshead, A. B. (2008). The personality composition of teams and creativity: The moderating

role of team creative confidence. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 42(4), 255-282.

Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and

General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355-429.

Beghetto, R. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Nurturing creativity in the classroom: Cambridge University Press.

Cardarello, R. (2014). Enhancing scientific thinking in children: Suggestions based on studies about creativity. Paper presented at the Conference

proceedings. New perspectives in science education.

Charles, R. E., & Runco, M. A. (2001). Developmental trends in the evaluative and divergent thinking of children. Creativity Research Journal,

13(3-4), 417-437.

Choi, J. N. (2004). Individual and contextual predictors of creative performance: The mediating role of psychological processes. Creativity

Research Journal, 16(2-3), 187-199.

Davies, D., Jindal-Snape, D., Collier, C., Digby, R., Hay, P., & Howe, A. (2013). Creative learning environments in education—A systematic

literature review. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 80-91.

Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290-309.

George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: an interactional

approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 513.

Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative" description of personality": the big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

59(6), 1216.

Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. G. (2006). The international personality item

pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(1), 84-96.

Lim, H. S., & Choi, J. N. (2009). Testing an alternative relationship between individual and contextual predictors of creative performance. Social

Behavior and Personality: an international journal, 37(1), 117-135.


References

Manske, M. E., & Davis, G. A. (1968). Effects of simple instructional biases upon performance in the unusual uses test. The Journal of general

psychology, 79(1), 25-33.

McGraw, K. O. (1978). The detrimental effects of reward on performance: A literature review and a prediction model. The hidden costs of

reward: New perspectives on the psychology of human motivation, 33-60.

McLoughlin, C., & Lee, M. J. (2008). The three p's of pedagogy for the networked society: Personalization, participation, and productivity.

International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 20(1), 10-27.

Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas. Psychological

Science, 23(1), 13-17. doi:10.1177/0956797611421018.

Piagèt, J. (1964). Development and learning. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 40(2), 176-186.

Rietzschel, Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2010). The selection of creative ideas after individual idea generation: Choosing between creativity and

impact. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 47-68. doi:10.1348/000712609X414204.

Shiner, & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 2-32.

Toh, C. A., & Miller, S. R. (2016). Creativity in design teams: the influence of personality traits and risk attitudes on creative concept selection.

Research in Engineering Design, 27(1), 73-89.

Torrance, E. (1974). The Torrance tests of creative thinking-TTCT Manual and Scoring Guide: Verbal test A, figural test. Lexington, KY: Ginn.

Vincent-Lancrin, S., González-Sancho, C., Bouckaert, M., Fernández-Barrerra, M., Jacotin, G., Urgel, J., & Vidal, Q. (Eds.). (2019). Fostering

Students' Creativity and Critical Thinking: What it Means in School. Educational Research and Innovation. OECD Publishing: Paris.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1980). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes: Harvard university press.

World Economic Forum (2016). The future of jobs: Employment, skills and workforce strategy for the fourth industrial revolution. Paper

presented at the Global Challenge Insight Report, World Economic Forum, Geneva.

World Economic Forum (2018). The future of jobs report 2018. Paper presented at the World Economic Forum, Geneva.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!