16.12.2022 Views

vdoc.pub_an-introduction-to-cognitive-linguistics

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

PROTOTYPES AND CATEGORIES 31

lines in Figure 1.8); category membership is also supported by the fact that

the ostrich has a long neck like a flamingo, and decorative feathers like a

peacock (see thin lines in Figure 1.8). In defining the position of a category

member in its category, we are thus justified in considering any sensible

attribute proposed for this item. This is the theoretical background of the

attribute-listing experiments carried out by Rosch and her associates and

the typicality ratings that were based on them.

Attribute listing and attribute-based typicality ratings

Given the ease with which we seem to be able to call up the attributes for

familiar objects and organisms, attributes can be collected in a fairly simple

test procedure that can be easily administered to a large number of subjects.

In the attribute-listing experiments conducted by Rosch and Mervis

(1975), each of the subjects (400 American psychology students) was given

six sheets of paper with the test item written on the top of the page. The

subjects had a minute and a half to write down all the attributes that they

could think of. To eliminate answers that were obviously false or wrongly

attributed to an item or too general in meaning, the attribute lists were

checked by two judges.

The test items used were selected from the lists obtained in the goodnessof-example

ratings described in Figure 1.3 and consisted of sets of 20

graded category members, one set for each of the categories FURNITURE, VEHI-

CLE, FRUIT, WEAPON, VEGETABLE and CLOTHING. Altogether, 120 items from >CHAIR<

to >TELEPHONE< (for FURNITURE), >CAR< to >ELEVATOR< (for VEHICLE) and from

>ORANGE< to >OLIVE< (for FRUIT) were tested.

The experiment had two aims: to demonstrate the notion of family resemblance

(see above) and, more important for Rosch and Mervis, to supply

attribute-based typicality ratings (this neutral term seems preferable to Rosch

and Mervis’s own term ‘measure of family resemblance’). These typicality ratings

could then be used to verify the earlier goodness-of-example ratings.

How were the attribute-based ratings calculated? Leaving aside mathematical

details, two stages can be distinguished. First, the attributes were

‘weighted’, that is, it was established for how many of the 20 tested category

members each attribute had been listed. The top score of 20 was given if

an attribute was shared by all category members (‘means of transport’ in

the case of vehicles). An attribute listed for only one category member (think

of ‘installed in buildings’, which would only fit >LIFT< or >ELEVATOR<) received

the score 1; attributes applying to several but not all category members were

assigned intermediate scores. The result was a list of weighted attributes.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!