22.03.2013 Views

Effect of Sound Intensity Level on Judgement of - Audio Engineering ...

Effect of Sound Intensity Level on Judgement of - Audio Engineering ...

Effect of Sound Intensity Level on Judgement of - Audio Engineering ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

frequency resp<strong>on</strong>se remains relativily<br />

unchanged over a range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intensities<br />

limited at the upper extreme by the <strong>on</strong>-<br />

set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> overloading. In this c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>, it<br />

is noteworthy that the 75-db intensity<br />

level is far below the ear's overload<br />

limit. Therefore, we cannot say that the<br />

apparent broadening <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the flat top <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the curve is necessarily related to over-<br />

excitati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the auditory mechanism.<br />

Another point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> importance is that<br />

when we raise signal intensity from 50<br />

db to 75 db, we effectively add to the<br />

listener's range at 1 s<strong>on</strong>e, or 1000 milli-<br />

s<strong>on</strong>es, loudness, something over three<br />

octaves downward frpm 400 cps. Put<br />

another way,'by raising the intensity-<br />

level from 50 db to 75 db, we render a<br />

125-cps t<strong>on</strong>e over 90 times louder for<br />

the average listener. By either stater<br />

ment, it is evident that a tremendous im-<br />

provement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ear's effective bass<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se follows a rise in over-all signal<br />

intensity. To a somewhat lesser extent,<br />

the same is true at the treble end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

spectrum, as these curves suggest. It is<br />

expected that the 50-db c<strong>on</strong>tour, if ex-<br />

tended, would intersect the 100 millis<strong>on</strong>e<br />

(0.1 s<strong>on</strong>e) loudness ordinate just below<br />

20,000 cps, or near the upper frequency<br />

limit <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> normal hearing.<br />

Listener Preferences<br />

Recently a number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> experimenters<br />

have investigated the preference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lis-<br />

teners for various c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> fre-<br />

quency range and intensity-level in the<br />

reproducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> music and speech. Of<br />

the several studies reported, the most ex-<br />

tensively documented appear to be<br />

Eisenberg and Chinn's4 and Ols<strong>on</strong>>.s<br />

In both <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these experiments, it will be<br />

recalled, listeners were required to com-<br />

pare c<strong>on</strong>secutive passages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> musical se-<br />

lecti<strong>on</strong>s played through adjustable fre-<br />

quency filters. In Eisenberg and Chinn's<br />

study, electrical filters were employed<br />

restricting the ,range at both ends simul-<br />

taneously. Figzcre 3 shows the three<br />

band-pass characteristics up<strong>on</strong> which<br />

their listeners based judgments <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> "t<strong>on</strong>al<br />

range" preference. The authors note<br />

that these curves apply <strong>on</strong>ly to the elec-<br />

trical porti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their apparatus; in dis-<br />

cussing their results, they imply ,that<br />

these curves also represent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

the acoustical field surrounding their<br />

listeners. It will also be recalled that<br />

Eisenberg and Chinn studied preference<br />

for program intensity-level, choosing 50,<br />

60, and 70 db as the three values for<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong>. In <strong>on</strong>e series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subexperi-<br />

'P. Eisenberg and H. A. Chinn.' T<strong>on</strong>al<br />

range and volume level preferences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> broad-<br />

cast listeners. J. Ex@. Psychol. 35, 5, 374,<br />

October 1945.<br />

6H. F. Ols<strong>on</strong>. Frequency range pref-<br />

erences for speech and music. J. Acow. Soc.<br />

AM. 19,4, 549, July 1947.<br />

ments, these three intensi6-levels were<br />

combined with the three frequency passbands<br />

already described, and the listeners<br />

asked to state preference for the resultant<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both "t<strong>on</strong>al range"<br />

and ''volu~e level."' As we have already<br />

seen, when a sufficient change in signal<br />

intensity occurs listeners will report an<br />

apparent change in frequency range<br />

even though no physical c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> passband<br />

has 'been imposed. Now in the<br />

measurement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> preferences, 'it is important<br />

that the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s serving as<br />

bases for judgment be clearly discrim-<br />

-<br />

100 1000<br />

FREQUENCY-'CYCLES PER SECOND<br />

inable to the average observer. If the Fig. 4. Subiective effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Eisenberg and<br />

differences between them be discernible Ckinn's "medium t<strong>on</strong>al range" filters at "high"<br />

(70-db), "moderate" (60-db), and "low" (50<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

by chance, then the observer is db) volume levels. Broken lines indicate cut<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f<br />

plainly handicapped in stating his pref- effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> filters. Note the apparent ineffectiveness<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high-pass filters at 50 and<br />

erence with certainty. ~~~~i~~ this in<br />

60 db.<br />

mind, then, let us look at the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Eisenberg and Chinn's filters up<strong>on</strong> the<br />

auditory spectrum as perceived by an<br />

average observer under the three in-<br />

tensity-levels used. Returning to Fig. 2,<br />

we can estimate from the 50, 60, and 70<br />

db curves the perceptual results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sound<br />

transmissi<strong>on</strong> through a system nominally<br />

flat from 40 to 10,000 cps, corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

totthe "wide t<strong>on</strong>al range" <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these ex-<br />

periments. Figure 4.shows the subjective<br />

effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the "medium-t<strong>on</strong>al-range," that<br />

in<br />

W<br />

z<br />

in 0<br />

10 -<br />

I<br />

% 1-<br />

W z<br />

0 3<br />

3<br />

70 db<br />

100 XMO moo0<br />

FREQUENCY - CYCLES PER SECOND<br />

is, 60 to 6,000 cps, filtering: it will be<br />

noted that a the 'igh Fig. 5. f ubjectivc effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Iisenberg and<br />

level" do the restricti<strong>on</strong>s at both high Chinn's "narrow t<strong>on</strong>al range" filters at ''hirrh"<br />

and low ends become noticeable. At (70-db), "moderate" (60-2b) and "low" (50db)<br />

volume levels. Broken lines indicate cut<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f<br />

lower levels, the be effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> filters. Note relative ineffectiveness<br />

expected to report,mainly - a change in <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high-pass filter at 50 db, and sliht effect<br />

/<br />

hi&s as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> filteri&'but a iaria- <strong>on</strong> subjective bandwidth at' high eid caused<br />

by change in intensity-levels.<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both lows and highs would be<br />

reported as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the-changes in<br />

level. Figz~re.5, showing the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

"narrow-range," that is, 150 to 4,900<br />

cps, filtering, -suggests an opposite c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

: here the greatest perceived differences<br />

occur at the low-frequency end<br />

as level is varied. Restricti<strong>on</strong> at the high<br />

end is so severe that raising level from<br />

50 to 70 db expands subjective frequency<br />

range hardly more than half an<br />

100<br />

octave upward at 1 s<strong>on</strong>e loudness. Fig-<br />

FREQUENCY -CYCLES PER SECOND<br />

ures 2,4 and 5 indicate that a growth in<br />

the perceived range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequencies prob- Fig. 6. Subjective effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Eisenberg and<br />

Chinn's "wide," "medium" and "narrow t<strong>on</strong>al<br />

ably occurred independently <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the band- range" filters at 704, intensity-level ("high<br />

pass imposed by means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> fil- volume level"). Broken lines indicate cut<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f<br />

ters.<br />

effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> filters.<br />

In Figs. 6, 7 a%d 8 we can see the "medium", that is, "A" with "B", diseffects<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> filtering at any <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the criminativn is likely to be uncertain,<br />

three intensity-levels. Figure 6 suggests yieldin2 statements <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> preference not acthat<br />

at "high-volume level" the average ceptably reliable. Figzcre 7 shows an aglistener<br />

would experience little difficulty gravati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this difficulty, brought about<br />

discerning the shift from "wide" to "nar- by reducing intensity-level to 60 db.<br />

row" ranges, that is, from band A to Here the three batids become more easily<br />

band C. Likewise, the shift from "me- c<strong>on</strong>fused, although discriminati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

dium" to "narrow", band B to band C; :wide" (A) from "narrow" (C) might<br />

probably would be easily detected. HOW- still be accomplished with better than<br />

ever, in comparis<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> "wide" with chance success. Figure 8 presents a pic-<br />

AUDIO ENGINEERING * JUNE, 1951

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!