03.04.2013 Views

Motor Function and Social Participation in Kindergarten Children

Motor Function and Social Participation in Kindergarten Children

Motor Function and Social Participation in Kindergarten Children

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong> K<strong>in</strong>dergarten <strong>Children</strong><br />

Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart, Tel-Aviv University<br />

Abstract<br />

This study focused on the associations between <strong>in</strong>dividual variations <strong>in</strong> children’s<br />

motor abilities <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> social participation <strong>and</strong> play behavior.<br />

Indoor <strong>and</strong> outdoor play behavior patterns of 88 k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children were observed,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a battery of st<strong>and</strong>ard assessments of basic motor functions was adm<strong>in</strong>istered. The<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate significant associations between children’s motor abilities <strong>and</strong> social<br />

<strong>and</strong> nonsocial forms of play. Results are discussed <strong>in</strong> relation to exist<strong>in</strong>g conceptual<br />

models of the underly<strong>in</strong>g causes for nonsocial behavior.<br />

Keywords: motor skills; motor ability; social play; reticence; solitary behavior<br />

<strong>Social</strong> play <strong>and</strong> peer <strong>in</strong>teraction provide a framework for children to explore their<br />

physical <strong>and</strong> social environments. Conversely, lack of social <strong>in</strong>teraction dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood<br />

has been associated with a variety of social <strong>and</strong> emotional difficulties <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behavior problems, peer rejection, depression, <strong>and</strong> low self-esteem (for reviews, see<br />

Cheah, Nelson & Rub<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Kupersmidt, Coie & Dodge, 1990; Rub<strong>in</strong>, Bukowski<br />

& Parker, 1998; Rub<strong>in</strong>, Burgess & Coplan, 2002). Not<strong>in</strong>g the detrimental effect that<br />

lack of peer <strong>in</strong>teraction may have on children’s psychological <strong>and</strong> behavioral adjustment,<br />

much research has focused on possible etiologic <strong>and</strong> mediat<strong>in</strong>g factors relat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to solitary behavior. These <strong>in</strong>clude, among others, <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships, temperamental<br />

tendencies, cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences, social-cognitive deficiencies, <strong>and</strong> biological<br />

markers (e.g., Coplan, Rub<strong>in</strong>, Fox, Calk<strong>in</strong>s & Stewart, 1994; Fox, Henderson,<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, Calk<strong>in</strong>s & Schmidt, 2001; Henderson, Marshall, Fox & Rub<strong>in</strong>, 2004; H<strong>in</strong>de,<br />

Tampl<strong>in</strong> & Barrett, 1993; Kagan, Reznick & Snidman, 1987; Kochanska & Radke-<br />

Yarrow, 1992; Rub<strong>in</strong> & Asendorpf, 1993; Sp<strong>in</strong>ard, Eisenberg, Harris, Hanish, Fabes<br />

& Kupanoff, 2004).<br />

Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, one rather obvious potential background factor <strong>in</strong> the development of<br />

social participation, that is, children’s motor abilities, has been relatively neglected <strong>in</strong><br />

the research literature. The present study explored whether <strong>in</strong>dividual variations <strong>in</strong><br />

children’s motor abilities are associated with <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> social <strong>and</strong><br />

solitary play behavior. The logic beh<strong>in</strong>d this implied association relies on the fact<br />

that the process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> organization of external <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal sensory–motor <strong>in</strong>put,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the coord<strong>in</strong>ation of motor output must be reflected <strong>in</strong> children’s social <strong>in</strong>teractive<br />

style.<br />

Correspondence should be addressed to Yair Bar-Haim, Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv<br />

University, Ramat Aviv, Tel-Aviv 69978, Israel. Email: yair1@post.tau.ac.il<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006. Published by Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g, 9600 Gars<strong>in</strong>gton Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK <strong>and</strong> 350 Ma<strong>in</strong> Street,<br />

Malden, MA 02148, USA.


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 297<br />

Individual variations <strong>in</strong> posture <strong>and</strong> movement control, motor plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> execution,<br />

<strong>and</strong> visual–motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation provide children with different types of movement<br />

challenges that they must solve <strong>in</strong> order to exercise efficient social engagement<br />

(Fogel, 1992). For example, the ability to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g postures significantly<br />

<strong>in</strong>creases the amount of social <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> 8- to 12-month-olds (Gustafson, 1984).<br />

Locomotor experience has been shown to facilitate the development of children’s<br />

social referenc<strong>in</strong>g skills (Campos, Kermoian, Wither<strong>in</strong>gton, Chen & Dong, 1997).<br />

And, upright locomotion has been shown to play a significant role <strong>in</strong> the affective<br />

organization of mother–<strong>in</strong>fant <strong>in</strong>teraction (Bir<strong>in</strong>gen, Emde, Campos & Appelbaum,<br />

1995). However, if age-related motor capabilities are not achieved, a child’s social<br />

behavior may be compromised. For <strong>in</strong>stance, Smyth <strong>and</strong> Anderson (2000) reported<br />

that 6- to 10-year-olds with Developmental Coord<strong>in</strong>ation Disorder (DCD) spent more<br />

time alone <strong>and</strong> were onlookers more often dur<strong>in</strong>g school playground activity compared<br />

with normally coord<strong>in</strong>ated children. These authors suggest that children with<br />

impaired coord<strong>in</strong>ation can become isolated or solitary on the school playground.<br />

Difficulties <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g motor actions can occur <strong>in</strong> children who<br />

have no diagnostically identifiable neuromotor impairment. Nevertheless, such difficulties<br />

may result <strong>in</strong> play dysfunction. Dur<strong>in</strong>g play, children are required to reta<strong>in</strong> static<br />

postures that dem<strong>and</strong> efficient balance control. <strong>Children</strong> also need to move freely <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

out of postural positions <strong>in</strong> order to take part <strong>in</strong> peer play. Likewise, normal muscle tone<br />

is essential for execut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g such motor actions <strong>and</strong> movements. Hypotonic<br />

children (i.e., children with low muscle tone) typically f<strong>in</strong>d it difficult to stay<br />

engaged <strong>in</strong> motor action for prolonged periods of time <strong>and</strong> therefore tend to avoid gross<br />

motor activities that <strong>in</strong>volve cont<strong>in</strong>uous muscle endurance (Ayres, 1980a; Lane, 2002).<br />

In addition, children’s play behavior is highly dependent on the ability to motor plan<br />

activities <strong>in</strong> an efficient way. The motor plann<strong>in</strong>g of a play task <strong>in</strong>volves generat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

an idea of the motor task to be performed, sequenc<strong>in</strong>g the idea, <strong>and</strong> execut<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

sequence efficiently. If these complex neurobehavioral processes are successful, the<br />

child is able to produce an adaptive behavior <strong>and</strong> a goal-directed action that meets<br />

the environmental challenges of social activity. However, poor <strong>in</strong>tegration <strong>and</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of sensory–motor <strong>in</strong>formation may adversely affect play behavior <strong>and</strong> limit a<br />

child’s choice of play activities. It has been suggested that children characterized by<br />

poor motor plann<strong>in</strong>g abilities tend to have poor play skills <strong>and</strong> often fail to participate<br />

<strong>in</strong> activities or tasks performed <strong>in</strong> a group sett<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., Ayres, 1980a; Schaaf, Merril<br />

& K<strong>in</strong>sella, 1987). Cl<strong>in</strong>ical observations suggest that such children tend to move<br />

slowly <strong>and</strong> fearfully <strong>and</strong> may avoid gross motor activity (e.g., Parham & Fazio, 1997).<br />

In conclusion, despite the <strong>in</strong>tuitively expected association between sensory–motor<br />

capabilities <strong>and</strong> social behavior, ma<strong>in</strong>stream experimental <strong>and</strong> developmental psychology<br />

have traditionally considered the question of how children plan <strong>and</strong> execute<br />

movement to be of relatively little <strong>in</strong>terest to the field of social <strong>and</strong> emotional<br />

development (cf. Campos, Anderson, Barbu-Roth, Hubbard, Hertenste<strong>in</strong> &<br />

Wither<strong>in</strong>gton, 2000; Pellegr<strong>in</strong>i & Smith, 1998; Smyth & Anderson, 2000). Therefore,<br />

the objective of the present study was to provide an analysis of the associations<br />

between <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> children’s motor abilities <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> social <strong>and</strong> solitary behavior.<br />

<strong>Children</strong> may play alone for different reasons. Researchers have identified at<br />

least three dist<strong>in</strong>ct forms of solitary behavior that appear to differ <strong>in</strong> terms of their<br />

behavioral manifestation <strong>and</strong> motivational underp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g., Coplan et al., 1994;<br />

Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil & Armer, 2004; Rub<strong>in</strong> & Asendorpf, 1993).<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


298 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

<strong>Social</strong> reticence is characterized by a cluster of solitary behaviors consist<strong>in</strong>g of prolonged<br />

onlook<strong>in</strong>g at other children’s play without mak<strong>in</strong>g attempts to jo<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> of<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g unoccupied (Coplan et al., 1994). <strong>Children</strong> who engage <strong>in</strong> reticent behavior<br />

appear socially motivated but are wary, vigilant, <strong>and</strong> socially anxious (Rub<strong>in</strong> &<br />

Asendorpf, 1993). Reticent behaviors are readily detected as problematic by parents,<br />

teachers, <strong>and</strong> peers, <strong>and</strong> have been related to overt <strong>in</strong>dicators of anxiety, poor performance<br />

on cooperative tasks, poor social skills, <strong>and</strong> deficiencies <strong>in</strong> the ability to<br />

regulate negative emotions (Coplan, Gav<strong>in</strong>ski-Mol<strong>in</strong>a, Lagace-Segu<strong>in</strong> & Wichmann,<br />

2001; Rub<strong>in</strong>, Coplan, Fox & Calk<strong>in</strong>s, 1995). We hypothesize that part of the variance<br />

<strong>in</strong> children’s reticent behavior may be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by deficiencies <strong>in</strong> their motor ability.<br />

Specifically, children with suboptimal motor performance may feel more anxious <strong>and</strong><br />

disadvantaged <strong>in</strong> sequenc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> perform<strong>in</strong>g the motor tasks associated with social<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction.<br />

A second cluster of social withdrawal behaviors detected <strong>in</strong> children has been<br />

labeled solitary–passive play (Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1982). This behavioral pattern <strong>in</strong>volves the quiet<br />

solitary exploration of objects <strong>and</strong> constructive play activities (e.g., draw<strong>in</strong>g, build<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with blocks, dress<strong>in</strong>g dolls). It has been hypothesized that these behaviors <strong>in</strong>dicate low<br />

motivation to approach or avoid others (Asendorpf, 1990, 1991), <strong>and</strong> therefore reflect<br />

social dis<strong>in</strong>terest. In general, solitary—passive play has not been associated with maladjustment<br />

(e.g., Coplan & Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1998; Coplan et al., 1994; Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1982). It is not<br />

entirely clear, however, that solitary–passive play is never associated with social or<br />

emotional difficulties. For example, Coplan et al. (2001) reported that observed solitary–passive<br />

play <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarteners was associated with temperamental shyness <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dices of maladjustment for boys but not for girls. Henderson et al. (2004) also<br />

reported an association between a high frequency of solitary–passive play <strong>and</strong> maternal<br />

reports of temperamental shyness. Such f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs may suggest that solitary–passive<br />

play is not always a matter of preference for solitude. However, because it has been<br />

suggested that solitary–passive behavior is more a function of low social <strong>in</strong>terest,<br />

rather than <strong>in</strong>ability for social <strong>in</strong>teraction; <strong>and</strong> because solitary–passive play <strong>in</strong>volves<br />

high frequencies of constructive <strong>and</strong> exploratory behavior, both requir<strong>in</strong>g good posture<br />

control, motor plann<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> coord<strong>in</strong>ation, no specific associations were expected<br />

between high frequency of solitary–passive play <strong>and</strong> motor abilities.<br />

Another form of solitary play that has been described <strong>in</strong> the literature is solitary–<br />

functional play (Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1982). 1 This form of solitary behavior <strong>in</strong>volves simple motor<br />

activities <strong>and</strong> repetitive motor movements performed with or without objects (e.g.,<br />

repeatedly bang<strong>in</strong>g two blocks together, jump<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>and</strong> off a chair, sw<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g on a<br />

sw<strong>in</strong>g). It has been suggested that solitary–functional play is the first structural category<br />

of play to appear <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy <strong>and</strong> that through functional play, children come to<br />

acquire basic motor skills <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> their daily activities (Piaget, 1962; Smilansky,<br />

1968). From a motor development perspective, solitary–functional behavior requires<br />

only low levels of motor plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> execution. In contrast to the expected high<br />

occurrence of solitary–functional behavior <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>and</strong> toddlers, high frequencies<br />

of solitary–functional play <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children may imply low levels of play<br />

development <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>creased need for simple motor practice. We hypothesized that<br />

suboptimal motor development <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children may <strong>in</strong>duce an <strong>in</strong>creased need<br />

for simple sensory–motor exploration <strong>in</strong> the form of solitary–functional play.<br />

In summary, we expected to f<strong>in</strong>d an <strong>in</strong>teraction between motor ability <strong>and</strong> type of<br />

play behavior. Specifically, we expected that children with high motor ability would<br />

display a higher frequency of social play compared with children of low motor ability,<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 299<br />

reflect<strong>in</strong>g the efficient physical support that good motor abilities may provide for social<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction. Conversely, children with low motor ability were expected to display a<br />

higher frequency of social reticence <strong>and</strong> solitary–functional play compared with children<br />

with high motor ability. Unlike the expected associations between motor ability<br />

<strong>and</strong> social play, solitary–functional play, <strong>and</strong> reticent behavior, no specific associations<br />

were expected between solitary–passive play <strong>and</strong> motor abilities, as solitary–passive<br />

play <strong>in</strong>volves constructive <strong>and</strong> exploratory behavior, both requir<strong>in</strong>g good posture<br />

control, motor plann<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> coord<strong>in</strong>ation.<br />

In addition, it is possible that there are effects of context (<strong>in</strong>door versus outdoor)<br />

<strong>and</strong> gender on children’s frequencies of social <strong>and</strong> solitary play behavior. For example,<br />

Sp<strong>in</strong>ard et al. (2004) suggested that solitary play exhibited dur<strong>in</strong>g outdoor activities<br />

may be less appropriate <strong>and</strong> less voluntary than this type of behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g quiet<br />

<strong>in</strong>door activities. With respect to gender differences, there is a grow<strong>in</strong>g literature<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that social withdrawal is a greater risk factor for boys than for girls (Caspi,<br />

Elder & Bem, 1988; Coplan et al., 2001; Dettl<strong>in</strong>g, Gunnar & Donzella, 1999; Rub<strong>in</strong>,<br />

Chen & Hymel, 1993; Stevenson-H<strong>in</strong>de & Glover, 1996). In that respect, an <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

between motor ability <strong>and</strong> gender <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g solitary behavior may be of<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest. However, given the scant research data about gender differences <strong>and</strong> context<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence on social <strong>and</strong> nonsocial behavior, <strong>and</strong> the lack of previous studies on the<br />

association between motor abilities <strong>and</strong> nonsocial behavior, hypotheses concern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

possible context <strong>and</strong> gender differences were largely exploratory <strong>in</strong> nature.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants were 88 children (53 girls, 35 boys), recruited from seven r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

selected ma<strong>in</strong>stream public k<strong>in</strong>dergarten classes situated <strong>in</strong> the greater Tel Aviv area,<br />

Israel. Their mean age was 5.83 years (SD = 0.46; Range = 5.04 to 6.36 years).<br />

Although approximately half the children <strong>in</strong> the selected k<strong>in</strong>dergarten classes were<br />

4-year-olds, age-related norms for several of the motor measures used <strong>in</strong> the study were<br />

available start<strong>in</strong>g from five years of age. Therefore, an age of five years or higher was<br />

set as an <strong>in</strong>clusion criterion for the study. Parents of all eligible children <strong>in</strong> each of<br />

the selected classes were contacted, of whom 68 percent agreed to participate <strong>in</strong> the<br />

study. Excluded from the study were four children who, based on their parent reports,<br />

had auditory or visual impairments, or received treatment for sensory–motor problems.<br />

By virtue of attend<strong>in</strong>g ma<strong>in</strong>stream public k<strong>in</strong>dergartens, none of the children<br />

<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the study had a major developmental disorder such as Autism or Cerebral<br />

Palsy. Fifty-six percent of the participants were first-borns. N<strong>in</strong>ety-five percent of participants’<br />

fathers <strong>and</strong> 98 percent of their mothers reported high school or higher levels<br />

of education. Eighty-eight percent of the parents were married <strong>and</strong> liv<strong>in</strong>g together.<br />

Procedure <strong>and</strong> Measures<br />

Assessment of <strong>Social</strong> Behavior. <strong>Children</strong>’s <strong>in</strong>door <strong>and</strong> outdoors behavior was coded<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g selected codes from Rub<strong>in</strong>’s (2001) Play Observation Scale. Data were collected<br />

through eight visits to each k<strong>in</strong>dergarten over a period of three months. Observations<br />

were scheduled for the second semester of the school year to ensure that the children<br />

had a fair opportunity to adjust to their k<strong>in</strong>dergarten sett<strong>in</strong>gs, their classmates, <strong>and</strong><br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


300 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

their teachers. This provided a familiar context aga<strong>in</strong>st which to assess children’s social<br />

behavior. Dur<strong>in</strong>g each visit, each child’s predom<strong>in</strong>ant free play behavior was recorded<br />

for a series of ten-second <strong>in</strong>tervals, summ<strong>in</strong>g up to one m<strong>in</strong>ute of unstructured play<br />

<strong>in</strong>doors <strong>and</strong> one m<strong>in</strong>ute of unstructured play outdoors. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, a total of 16<br />

m<strong>in</strong>utes of play data were collected for each child, eight m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>in</strong>doors <strong>and</strong> eight<br />

m<strong>in</strong>utes outdoors, yield<strong>in</strong>g 96 ten-second cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervals per child (48 <strong>in</strong>tervals of<br />

<strong>in</strong>door <strong>and</strong> 48 <strong>in</strong>tervals of outdoor activity). To ensure good underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

context of the observed activity, each ten-second <strong>in</strong>terval of cod<strong>in</strong>g was preceded by<br />

30 seconds of free observation. A stopwatch was used to beep-signal the <strong>in</strong>itiation <strong>and</strong><br />

term<strong>in</strong>ation of the cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terval <strong>and</strong> behaviors were coded onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>to st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

cod<strong>in</strong>g sheets.<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g each visit, children were observed <strong>in</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>om order by one of three graduate<br />

students tra<strong>in</strong>ed by the first author, who has been tra<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> use of the Play Observation<br />

Scale <strong>in</strong> K. H. Rub<strong>in</strong>’s laboratory, <strong>and</strong> achieved criterion levels of reliability.<br />

Inter-coder reliabilities were calculated on a r<strong>and</strong>omly selected set of cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervals<br />

total<strong>in</strong>g 20 percent of the sampled behavior. Percent agreement for the total variable<br />

matrix ranged between 93 <strong>and</strong> 96 percent; Cohen’s kappa ranged between .88 <strong>and</strong> .95.<br />

Of specific <strong>in</strong>terest to the present study were the sampled codes of social play (i.e.,<br />

group play <strong>and</strong> conversation with peers), reticent behavior (i.e., unoccupied <strong>and</strong><br />

onlook<strong>in</strong>g behaviors), solitary–passive play (i.e., solitary–constructive <strong>and</strong> solitary–<br />

exploratory behaviors), <strong>and</strong> solitary–functional play. These raw scores were proportionalized<br />

by divid<strong>in</strong>g by the total number of cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervals.<br />

Assessment of <strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong>. Three tra<strong>in</strong>ed occupational therapy students, who<br />

were bl<strong>in</strong>d to the study’s hypotheses <strong>and</strong> goals, adm<strong>in</strong>istered a battery of motor skills<br />

assessment at each child’s home. This home visit <strong>in</strong>cluded st<strong>and</strong>ardized assessments<br />

of balance, k<strong>in</strong>esthesia, imitation of postures, muscle tone, <strong>and</strong> visual–motor <strong>in</strong>tegration.<br />

The selected motor tests were chosen so that collectively, they provided a comprehensive<br />

representation of children’s core motor abilities. That is, the comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

of posture control (balance <strong>and</strong> muscle tone), motor plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> execution (k<strong>in</strong>esthesia<br />

<strong>and</strong> imitation of postures), <strong>and</strong> visual–motor <strong>in</strong>tegration. Each home visit lasted<br />

approximately 60 m<strong>in</strong>utes.<br />

Balance. The balance subscale of the Brun<strong>in</strong>ks–Oseretsky Test of <strong>Motor</strong> Proficiency<br />

(Brun<strong>in</strong>ks, 1978) is a widely used measure that consists of eight items: (1) st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g on<br />

one foot on the floor with eyes open; (2) st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g on a balance beam with eyes open;<br />

(3) st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g with eyes closed; (4) walk<strong>in</strong>g forward on a l<strong>in</strong>e on the floor; (5) walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

on a balance beam; (6) heel-to-toe walk<strong>in</strong>g on a l<strong>in</strong>e; (7) heel-to-toe walk<strong>in</strong>g on a<br />

balance beam; (8) stepp<strong>in</strong>g over a stick while walk<strong>in</strong>g on a balance beam. The test has<br />

established age-related norms <strong>and</strong> shows adequate <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency.<br />

K<strong>in</strong>esthesia. The k<strong>in</strong>esthetic sense arises from jo<strong>in</strong>ts, tendons, <strong>and</strong> muscle receptors,<br />

<strong>and</strong> provides awareness of body position <strong>and</strong> movement. K<strong>in</strong>esthesia was measured<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g the k<strong>in</strong>esthesia test (KIN; Ayres, 1980b). In this test, the experimenter<br />

moves the child’s h<strong>and</strong> from one predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed po<strong>in</strong>t on a paper chart to another while<br />

the child’s h<strong>and</strong>s rema<strong>in</strong> out of his or her visual field. The child is then asked to repeat<br />

the motion on the k<strong>in</strong>esthesia chart, aga<strong>in</strong> without see<strong>in</strong>g his or her h<strong>and</strong>s. The KIN<br />

test consists of ten test items, five for each h<strong>and</strong>. H<strong>and</strong>s are tested <strong>in</strong> an alternated<br />

sequence.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 301<br />

The KIN score is based on how accurately the child replicates the rehearsed motion,<br />

measured as the distance <strong>in</strong> millimeters of the child’s f<strong>in</strong>ger from the predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

target po<strong>in</strong>ts. The raw score is then converted to a st<strong>and</strong>ard age-related score.<br />

Imitation of postures. <strong>Children</strong>’s ability to perceive <strong>and</strong> replicate different body<br />

positions was determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the Imitation of Postures test (Ayres, 1980b). This test<br />

requires the child to assume a series of twelve positions or postures demonstrated by<br />

the exam<strong>in</strong>er. The completion of each task requires motor plann<strong>in</strong>g of a familiar or<br />

unfamiliar motor act. Two po<strong>in</strong>ts are given for postures that are imitated correctly<br />

with<strong>in</strong> three seconds after the exam<strong>in</strong>er has assumed the posture. One po<strong>in</strong>t is noted if<br />

the child imitates the posture correctly with<strong>in</strong> four to ten seconds. No po<strong>in</strong>ts are scored<br />

if the correct posture is assumed after ten seconds or if it does not meet the criteria for<br />

a score of one or two po<strong>in</strong>ts. The raw score of the test is the total number of po<strong>in</strong>ts noted<br />

for the 12 items. Raw scores are converted to st<strong>and</strong>ard age-related scores.<br />

Muscle tone (prone extension <strong>and</strong> sup<strong>in</strong>e flexion). To obta<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dex of the participants’<br />

muscle tone <strong>and</strong> strength, the children were asked to assume <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><br />

prone extension <strong>and</strong> sup<strong>in</strong>e flexion positions. The prone extension assessment measures<br />

the child’s ability simultaneously to lift the head, flexed arms, upper trunk, <strong>and</strong> extended<br />

legs from a prone–ly<strong>in</strong>g position. For the sup<strong>in</strong>e flexion assessment, the children were<br />

asked to assume a position of simultaneous flexion aga<strong>in</strong>st gravity of the knees, hips,<br />

trunk, <strong>and</strong> neck from a sup<strong>in</strong>e–ly<strong>in</strong>g position, where the top of the head had to<br />

approximate the knees. The number of seconds a child ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> each of these<br />

positions was noted as the raw score. <strong>Children</strong>’s raw scores of the prone extension <strong>and</strong><br />

sup<strong>in</strong>e flexion tests were highly correlated, r = .62; p < .0001. Therefore, the scores of<br />

the two measures were summed to create an aggregate <strong>in</strong>dex referred to as muscle tone.<br />

The developmental test of visual–motor <strong>in</strong>tegration (VMI). The VMI (Beery, 1989)<br />

consists of 24 geometric forms to be copied <strong>in</strong> sequence from a test booklet. The<br />

geometric forms become progressively more complex, <strong>and</strong> the score cont<strong>in</strong>ues to<br />

accumulate until all 24 forms have been successfully copied or until three consecutive<br />

forms are copied <strong>in</strong>correctly. The VMI is designed for children rang<strong>in</strong>g from 2 to 15<br />

years of age <strong>and</strong> comes with age-specific norms.<br />

The VMI conta<strong>in</strong>s two supplemental st<strong>and</strong>ardized subtests. The first is the<br />

Visual–Spatial Perception Test assess<strong>in</strong>g visual–spatial perception components<br />

without the requirement of motor action. On this test, the child is shown a target figure<br />

<strong>and</strong> is asked to select a match<strong>in</strong>g figure from a set of two to seven alternatives. The<br />

test consists of 24 figures that become progressively more complex. A child’s score<br />

on this test cont<strong>in</strong>ues to accumulate until all 24 figures have been matched or until<br />

three consecutive failures <strong>in</strong> match<strong>in</strong>g occur. The second test is the F<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Motor</strong> Accuracy<br />

Test, <strong>in</strong> which the child is required to draw a clear, dark l<strong>in</strong>e while stay<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>side<br />

a set of def<strong>in</strong>ed l<strong>in</strong>es. There are 24 configurations that require progressively ref<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

motor accuracy <strong>and</strong> control.<br />

Because the three subtests of the VMI were significantly correlated with rs rang<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from .38 to .43, ps < .001, their z-scores were aggregated to form a visual–motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

<strong>in</strong>dex.<br />

Data Reduction of <strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> Measures. Table 1 presents the <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations<br />

among the z-scores of the different motor function measures. Pr<strong>in</strong>cipal component<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


302 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

Table 1. Intercorrelations (N = 88) between the Different <strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong><br />

Measures<br />

Balance K<strong>in</strong>esthesia Imitation Muscle Tone<br />

K<strong>in</strong>esthesia .16 — — —<br />

Imitation .36*** .56*** — —<br />

Muscle Tone .31** .22* .17<br />

VMI .26** .47*** .29** .25*<br />

*p < .05, **p < .01,***p < .001.<br />

analysis <strong>in</strong>dicated that all the motor function measures loaded on a s<strong>in</strong>gle factor with<br />

Eigen value = 2.25, which expla<strong>in</strong>s 44.92 percent of the exist<strong>in</strong>g variance. Based on<br />

the results of the <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations <strong>and</strong> the factor analysis, we created a global <strong>in</strong>dex<br />

of motor ability by aggregat<strong>in</strong>g the z-scores of all tested motor skills (i.e., balance,<br />

k<strong>in</strong>esthesia, imitation of postures, muscle tone, <strong>and</strong> visual–motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation). Based<br />

on their scores on the aggregated motor function <strong>in</strong>dex, children were assigned to one<br />

of three groups. One group consisted of 22 children (11 girls) represent<strong>in</strong>g the bottom<br />

quartile of the distribution on the comb<strong>in</strong>ed motor aggregate. A second group consisted<br />

of 22 well-coord<strong>in</strong>ated children (16 girls), featur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the top quartile of the<br />

distribution. A third group consisted of 44 children (26 girls) whose scores on the<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed motor aggregate were <strong>in</strong> the middle quartiles of the distribution.<br />

Results<br />

Effects of <strong>Motor</strong> Ability, Gender, <strong>and</strong> Context on Frequencies of <strong>Social</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Solitary Play<br />

A repeated-measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was computed with <strong>Motor</strong><br />

Ability (low, average, high) <strong>and</strong> Gender (Male, Female) as ‘between-group’ factors<br />

<strong>and</strong> Play Type (Solitary–Passive, <strong>Social</strong> Play, <strong>Social</strong> Reticence) as a ‘with<strong>in</strong>-subject’<br />

factor. Context (Indoor, Outdoor) served as a repeated ‘with<strong>in</strong>-subject’ factor.<br />

Table 2 presents the means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations of social play, solitary–passive<br />

play, <strong>and</strong> social reticence proportional frequencies by motor ability group <strong>and</strong> context.<br />

Significant ma<strong>in</strong> effects of play type <strong>and</strong> motor ability were qualified by a play-typeby-motor-ability<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction, F(4,164) = 3.72, p < .01. In addition, a play-type-bycontext<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction was found, F(2,164) = 19.94, p < .0001. Ma<strong>in</strong> or <strong>in</strong>teraction effects<br />

<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g gender were nonsignificant. 2 To further assess the above <strong>in</strong>teractions,<br />

separate follow-up ANOVAs for each play type were performed with motor ability<br />

(low, average, high) as a ‘between-groups’ factor <strong>and</strong> context (<strong>in</strong>door, outdoor) as a<br />

repeated ‘with<strong>in</strong>-subject’ factor.<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Play. <strong>Children</strong> displayed higher frequency of social play outdoors (M = .62,<br />

SD = .21) than <strong>in</strong>doors (M = .51, SD = .22), F(1,85) = 28.54, p < .001. In addition,<br />

children with low motor abilities showed lower frequencies of social play (M = .47,<br />

SD = .37) than children with average <strong>and</strong> high motor abilities (Ms = .57 <strong>and</strong> .66,<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 303<br />

Table 2. Means <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations (<strong>in</strong> parentheses) of <strong>Social</strong> Play,<br />

Solitary–Passive Play, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Reticence Proportional Frequencies by <strong>Motor</strong><br />

Ability Group <strong>and</strong> Context<br />

High <strong>Motor</strong> Mid <strong>Motor</strong> Low <strong>Motor</strong><br />

Outdoor Indoor Outdoor Indoor Outdoor Indoor<br />

.74 (.17) .57 (.18) .62 (.20) .52 (.24) .50 (.21) .44 (.20) <strong>Social</strong> Play<br />

.10 (.11) .15 (.13) .12 (.10) .13 (.16) .17 (.10) .21 (.15) <strong>Social</strong> Reticence<br />

.03 (.06) .11 (.11) .08 (.10) .17 (.18) .05 (.07) .15 (.15) Solitary Passive<br />

SDs = .26 <strong>and</strong> .37, respectively), F(1,85) = 5.15, p < .01. The <strong>in</strong>teraction between<br />

motor ability <strong>and</strong> context was nonsignificant.<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Reticence. Higher frequency of social reticence was displayed <strong>in</strong>doors (M =<br />

.16, SD = .15) than outdoors (M = .13, SD = .11), F(1,85) = 4.64, p < .05. <strong>Children</strong><br />

with low motor ability showed a higher frequency of social reticence (M = .19, SD =<br />

.22) than children with average <strong>and</strong> high motor abilities (Ms = .12 <strong>and</strong> .13, SDs = .15<br />

<strong>and</strong> .22, respectively), F(1,85) = 3.11, p < .05. The <strong>in</strong>teraction between motor ability<br />

<strong>and</strong> context was aga<strong>in</strong> nonsignificant.<br />

Solitary–Passive Play. Higher frequency of solitary–passive play was displayed<br />

<strong>in</strong>doors (M = .15, SD = .16) than outdoors (M = .06, SD = .08), F(1,85) = 25.79,<br />

p < .001. <strong>Children</strong> <strong>in</strong> the three motor ability groups did not differ <strong>in</strong> their frequency<br />

of solitary–passive play, <strong>and</strong> no <strong>in</strong>teraction between motor ability <strong>and</strong> context was<br />

found.<br />

Solitary–<strong>Function</strong>al Play. Because solitary–functional play occurred <strong>in</strong> less than 3<br />

percent of cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervals, <strong>and</strong> because many children did not display this behavior<br />

at all, this variable was recoded to create two groups of children (see Coplan et al.,<br />

2001). One group consisted of children who did not demonstrate any solitary–<br />

functional behaviors (n = 40), the other group consisted of children who demonstrated<br />

any number of solitary–functional behaviors (n = 48).<br />

To assess the effect of context (<strong>in</strong>door versus outdoor) on the occurrence of<br />

solitary–functional behavior (‘none’ versus ‘any’), a Chi-square analysis was performed.<br />

The results show that when solitary–functional play occurred, it occurred<br />

more outdoors (46 percent) than <strong>in</strong>doors (31 percent), c 2 = 9.75, p < .01. More importantly,<br />

additional Chi-square analyses revealed that dur<strong>in</strong>g outdoor free play, 68 percent<br />

of the children with low motor ability displayed solitary–functional behavior, whereas<br />

only 23 percent of children with high motor ability showed this behavior, c 2 = 9.17,<br />

p < .01 (see Table 3 for cross-tabulations of <strong>in</strong>door <strong>and</strong> outdoor solitary–functional<br />

behavior by motor ability). Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>doors free play, children with low motor ability<br />

aga<strong>in</strong> displayed higher occurrence of solitary–functional behavior (46 percent) compared<br />

with children with high motor ability (18 percent). This difference however did<br />

not reach statistical significance, c 2 = 3.90, p = .14.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


304 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

Table 3. Cross-tabulations of Indoor <strong>and</strong> Outdoor Occurrence of Solitary–<br />

<strong>Function</strong>al Behavior by <strong>Motor</strong> Ability<br />

Indoors Outdoors<br />

Solitary–<strong>Function</strong>al Solitary–<strong>Function</strong>al<br />

‘none’ ‘any’ ‘none’ ‘any’<br />

Low <strong>Motor</strong> 12 10 7 15<br />

Mid <strong>Motor</strong> 31 13 24 20<br />

High <strong>Motor</strong> 18 4 17 5<br />

Consistency of Play Behavior Across Context<br />

Pearson correlations between <strong>in</strong>door <strong>and</strong> outdoor frequencies of social play, solitary–<br />

passive play, social reticence, <strong>and</strong> solitary–functional play revealed significant consistency<br />

of these play behaviors across context, rs = .63, .34, .47, <strong>and</strong> .34, respectively,<br />

ps < .001.<br />

Discussion<br />

Taken together, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from the present study <strong>in</strong>dicate significant associations<br />

between social engagement <strong>and</strong> motor adeptness. Specifically, <strong>in</strong> accord with predictions,<br />

children with low motor abilities displayed a lower frequency of social play <strong>and</strong><br />

a higher frequency of social reticence compared with children of average or high motor<br />

abilities. In addition, children with low motor abilities displayed a higher frequency<br />

of solitary–functional play compared with children with high motor abilities. F<strong>in</strong>ally,<br />

as expected, motor abilities were not significantly associated with solitary–passive<br />

play, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that motor function may not be a major factor <strong>in</strong> the development of<br />

this pattern of solitary play behavior.<br />

High frequency of solitary–functional play typically characterizes <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>and</strong> young<br />

toddlers (Piaget, 1962; Smilansky, 1968). <strong>Function</strong>al play provides young children<br />

with the opportunity to manipulate objects <strong>in</strong> a sensory–motor fashion that <strong>in</strong>forms<br />

them about the physical qualities of objects (Cheah et al., 2001; Hutt, 1970). In<br />

addition, repetitive sensory–motor behavior normally provides sensory–motor feedback<br />

that is important to the plann<strong>in</strong>g of goal-directed movement (Bundy, Lane &<br />

Murray, 2002). Developmental deficiencies <strong>in</strong> motor function may therefore result <strong>in</strong><br />

difficulties obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g good <strong>in</strong>formation about objects as well as poor feedback on body<br />

motion. From a motor function perspective, children display<strong>in</strong>g a high frequency of<br />

solitary–functional play may use repetitive movements to <strong>in</strong>crease their dim<strong>in</strong>ished<br />

sensory–motor feedback <strong>and</strong> to practice simple motor actions.<br />

Poor motor ability also makes the physical activity associated with social engagement<br />

more dem<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> may reduce a child’s capacity to deal with the social<br />

environment (Smyth & Anderson, 2000). The association between high frequency of<br />

social reticence <strong>and</strong> poor motor abilities fits well with the documented l<strong>in</strong>ks between<br />

poor balance control <strong>and</strong> anxiety on the one h<strong>and</strong> (Balaban & Thayer, 2001; Balaban,<br />

2002; Sklare, Konrad, Maser & Jacob, 2001), <strong>and</strong> social reticence <strong>and</strong> anxiety symp-<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 305<br />

toms on the other (e.g., Rub<strong>in</strong> & Asendorpf, 1993; Rub<strong>in</strong>, Chen, McDougall, Bowker,<br />

& McK<strong>in</strong>non, 1995). It may be the case that the observed anxious behavior typically<br />

tied with social reticence partly reflects low motor abilities. The child with poor motor<br />

abilities may be motivated towards social <strong>in</strong>teraction, but at the same time may feel<br />

anxious about the motor challenges posed by social engagement. Such children may<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d themselves onlook<strong>in</strong>g at other children’s play without the ability to motor plan<br />

<strong>and</strong> execute the required actions for jo<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>. Alternatively, they may avoid the motor<br />

challenges of goal-oriented social behavior <strong>and</strong> w<strong>and</strong>er about unoccupied. Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g<br />

about approach–withdrawal motivational conflicts (e.g., Asendorpf, 1990) from the<br />

viewpo<strong>in</strong>t of motor development may provide a new <strong>and</strong> viable approach to the assessment<br />

<strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the factors that drive reticent behavior.<br />

Neurophysiologic studies also suggest possible l<strong>in</strong>ks between motor abilities <strong>and</strong><br />

social behavior. It is widely recognized that there are extensive reciprocal connections<br />

between the pre-frontal cortex (PFC) <strong>and</strong> the amygdala, <strong>and</strong> that these two bra<strong>in</strong> structures<br />

form a circuitry that is highly <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> various aspects of social behavior (for<br />

reviews, see Amaral, Price, Pitkanen & Carmichael, 1992; Davidson, 2002; Fox, 1991;<br />

Kagan, 1994). The PFC/amygdala circuitry is also tightly connected to motor plann<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> execution areas <strong>in</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong>. For example, there is evidence suggest<strong>in</strong>g functional<br />

associations between the PFC, amygdala, <strong>and</strong> nucleus accumbens (NAc), the<br />

latter be<strong>in</strong>g described as the bra<strong>in</strong>’s motor-limbic <strong>in</strong>terface (e.g., Goto & O’Donnell,<br />

2002). These associations seem to play an important role <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrat<strong>in</strong>g emotional <strong>and</strong><br />

contextual cues with frontal motor plann<strong>in</strong>g, to determ<strong>in</strong>e response selection <strong>in</strong> social<br />

or otherwise challeng<strong>in</strong>g contexts (e.g., Burns, Annett, Kelley, Everitt & Robb<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

1996; Grace, 2000; Jackson & Moghaddam, 2001). Such transactions between these<br />

bra<strong>in</strong> structures may be compromised <strong>in</strong> some cl<strong>in</strong>ical conditions.<br />

One example of a cl<strong>in</strong>ical condition that <strong>in</strong>volves both motor <strong>and</strong> social deficiencies<br />

is the spectrum of autistic disorders. The cl<strong>in</strong>ical profile of these disorders conta<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

among other difficulties, disturbances <strong>in</strong> reciprocal social <strong>in</strong>teraction (APA,<br />

1994), <strong>and</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ically significant motor impairments (e.g., Ghaziudd<strong>in</strong>, Butler, Tsai &<br />

Ghaziudd<strong>in</strong>, 1994; Manjiviona & Prior, 1995; R<strong>in</strong>ehart, Bradshaw, Brereton & Tonge,<br />

2001). It should be noted however, that motor difficulties <strong>in</strong> autism are typically considered<br />

secondary to abnormal communication <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships. In contrast,<br />

<strong>in</strong> DCD, motor difficulties <strong>and</strong> clums<strong>in</strong>ess are the def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g features. Compared<br />

with normally coord<strong>in</strong>ated children, children with DCD are reported to display high<br />

frequencies of onlook<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> unoccupied behavior <strong>in</strong> the playground (e.g., Smyth &<br />

Anderson, 2000, 2001), higher levels of anxiety (Sk<strong>in</strong>ner & Piek, 2001), <strong>and</strong> poor psychosocial<br />

adjustment (Dewey, Kaplan, Crawford & Wilson, 2002). Such differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical focus may suggest that the comorbidity between social <strong>and</strong> motor<br />

deficiencies does not necessarily <strong>in</strong>volve a similar etiology. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from the<br />

present study further suggest that even <strong>in</strong> a normative population, a mild <strong>and</strong> noncl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />

deficiency <strong>in</strong> motor function may be associated with a significant reduction <strong>in</strong><br />

social engagement.<br />

The association between motor abilities <strong>and</strong> reticent behavior <strong>and</strong> solitary–<br />

functional play may also imply the possible application of motor-oriented <strong>in</strong>terventions<br />

for socially withdrawn children. Teachers, parents, <strong>and</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>icians may wish to<br />

consider a st<strong>and</strong>ard motor evaluation for children display<strong>in</strong>g excessive frequencies of<br />

these solitary behaviors. Based on the results of such assessment, sensory–motor <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

(e.g., occupational therapy, physiotherapy) may be applied along with conventional<br />

psychotherapy. For example, a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary study <strong>in</strong> our laboratory revealed<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


306 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

that twelve sessions of sensory–motor therapy can significantly reduce anxiety symptoms<br />

<strong>in</strong> highly anxious children with balance deficiencies (Karmon-Weisman, 2003).<br />

It is important to note, however, that the design of the present study does not allow<br />

the <strong>in</strong>ference of causality <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terplay between motor function <strong>and</strong> social participation.<br />

Some motor abilities may be regarded as prerequisites for the participation<br />

<strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> social activities. Alternatively, however, social play provides children with<br />

the opportunity to practice <strong>and</strong> perfect motor skills. Increased social participation may<br />

therefore account for the <strong>in</strong>creased motor proficiency <strong>in</strong> highly sociable children.<br />

In addition, social behaviors are also <strong>in</strong>fluenced by other skills such as language,<br />

self-regulation, problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g ability, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal cooperation. Normally<br />

develop<strong>in</strong>g children who are poor <strong>in</strong> motor skills may f<strong>in</strong>d alternative strategies, such<br />

as verbal communication, to achieve social engagement. A more accurate description<br />

of these elements <strong>in</strong> relation to motor ability awaits further research attention.<br />

The present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs also revealed <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g simple ma<strong>in</strong> effects of context on play<br />

behavior. Considerable consistency of play behavior was found across context,<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that children are likely to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> their relative rank<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the peer<br />

group <strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>door <strong>and</strong> outdoor frequencies of social <strong>and</strong> solitary behavior.<br />

However, children also displayed higher frequencies of social play <strong>and</strong> solitary–<br />

functional play outdoors than <strong>in</strong>doors, <strong>and</strong> higher frequencies of social reticence <strong>and</strong><br />

solitary–passive play <strong>in</strong>doors than outdoors. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that different<br />

physical sett<strong>in</strong>gs may elicit different types of play behavior (Barbour, 1999).<br />

Sp<strong>in</strong>ard et al. (2004) speculated that solitary play exhibited dur<strong>in</strong>g outdoor<br />

activities may be less appropriate than this type of behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g quiet <strong>in</strong>door activities.<br />

Our data <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>in</strong>deed solitary–passive play <strong>and</strong> reticent behavior were<br />

more frequent dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>door than outdoor activity. However, <strong>in</strong> contrast to Sp<strong>in</strong>ard,<br />

Eisenberg, Harris, Hanish, Fabes, <strong>and</strong> Kupanoff’s (2004) suggestion, solitary–<br />

functional play was significantly more frequent outdoors than <strong>in</strong>doors. It could be that<br />

solitary–functional play, when enacted outdoors, is <strong>in</strong> fact perceived as appropriate<br />

<strong>and</strong> functional. For example, we coded the rather normative actions of solitary sw<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g<br />

or bounc<strong>in</strong>g a ball outdoors as solitary–functional play.<br />

Another aspect of the associations between motor abilities <strong>and</strong> social <strong>and</strong> solitary<br />

behavior is gender differences. Similar to previous studies (e.g., Coplan & Rub<strong>in</strong>,<br />

1998; Coplan et al., 1994, 2001; Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1982) the present study found no gender differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> the occurrence of social play, solitary–passive play, reticent behavior, <strong>and</strong><br />

solitary–functional play. In addition, we found no gender differences <strong>in</strong> the tested<br />

motor abilities. Furthermore, <strong>in</strong> the present study, no significant gender differences<br />

were observed <strong>in</strong> the pattern of associations between motor ability <strong>and</strong> social or solitary<br />

behavior. We therefore conclude that, based on the present data, gender may not<br />

be a significant contributor to the association between motor ability <strong>and</strong> social behavior.<br />

However, the present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs should be <strong>in</strong>terpreted with caution, because sample<br />

size may have limited the power for detect<strong>in</strong>g gender differences. Fabes, Mart<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

Hanish (2003) showed that boys’ play is more active, forceful, <strong>and</strong> stereotyped compared<br />

to girls’ play, <strong>and</strong> that this pattern is even more exaggerated <strong>in</strong> group sett<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Therefore, boys with low motor abilities may f<strong>in</strong>d it more difficult than girls with low<br />

motor abilities do to participate <strong>in</strong> active group play. Further research is needed <strong>in</strong><br />

order to clarify this issue.<br />

In summary, the results of the present study provide prelim<strong>in</strong>ary evidence for an<br />

association between <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> children’s motor ability <strong>and</strong> variations<br />

<strong>in</strong> their social <strong>and</strong> solitary behavior. Specifically, stable <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> motor<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 307<br />

capabilities may be viewed as a temperamental factor contribut<strong>in</strong>g to the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

of <strong>in</strong>dividual variations <strong>in</strong> social behavior. An important issue for future research<br />

would be to assess the neural substrates of the associations between bra<strong>in</strong> areas l<strong>in</strong>ked<br />

with nonsocial <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>hibited temperament (e.g., Fox et al., 2001; Fox, Schmidt,<br />

Calk<strong>in</strong>s, Rub<strong>in</strong> & Coplan, 1996; Kagan et al., 1988) <strong>and</strong> activity <strong>in</strong> specific motor<br />

control areas. In addition, it would be of <strong>in</strong>terest to assess the relative <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

contributions of motor abilities <strong>and</strong> other important factors that also expla<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> children’s social <strong>and</strong> solitary behavior. Specifically, motor<br />

capabilities should be considered along with more traditionally cited factors such as<br />

temperament (e.g., Burgess, Marshall, Rub<strong>in</strong> & Fox, 2003; Fox & Calk<strong>in</strong>s, 1993; Fox<br />

& Henderson, 1999; Fox et al., 2001; Henderson et al., 2004; Kagan, 1989), parental<br />

behavior (e.g., Ladd & Golter, 1988; Ladd & Hart, 1992; Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1995), parent–child<br />

relationships (e.g., Booth, Rose-Krasnor, McK<strong>in</strong>non & Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1994; Rose-Krasnor,<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, Booth & Coplan, 1996), cultural beliefs <strong>and</strong> norms (e.g., Chen, Hast<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, Chen, Cen & Stewart, 1998; Chen, Rub<strong>in</strong> & Li, 1995; Rub<strong>in</strong>, 1998), <strong>and</strong> status<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the peer group (e.g., Harrist, Zaia, Bates, Dodge & Pettit, 1997; Rub<strong>in</strong> et al.,<br />

1993).<br />

References<br />

Amaral, D. G., Price, J. L., Pitkanen, A. & Carmichael, S. T. (1992). Anatomical organization<br />

of the primate amygdaloid complex. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The Amygdala: Neurobiological<br />

Aspects of Emotion, Memory <strong>and</strong> Mental Dysfunction (pp. 1–66). New York: Wiley.<br />

APA (1994). Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn). DSM-IV.<br />

Wash<strong>in</strong>gton DC: American Psychiatric Association.<br />

Asendorpf, J. (1990). Development of <strong>in</strong>hibition dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood: Evidence for situational<br />

specificity <strong>and</strong> a two-factor model. Developmental Psychology, 26, 721–730.<br />

Asendorpf, J. (1991). Development of <strong>in</strong>hibited children’s cop<strong>in</strong>g with unfamiliarity. Child<br />

Development, 62, 1460–1474.<br />

Ayres, A. J. (1980a). Sensory Integration <strong>and</strong> the Child. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological<br />

Services.<br />

Ayres, A. J. (1980b). Southern California Sensory Integration Tests: Manual. Los Angeles, CA:<br />

Western Psychological Services.<br />

Balaban, C. D. (2002). Neural substrates l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g balance control <strong>and</strong> anxiety. Physiology <strong>and</strong><br />

Behavior, 77, 469–475.<br />

Balaban, C. D. & Thayer, J. F. (2001). Neurological bases for balance–anxiety l<strong>in</strong>ks. Journal<br />

of Anxiety Disorders, 15, 53–79.<br />

Barbour, A. C. (1999). The impact of playground design on the play behaviors of children with<br />

different levels of physical competence. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14, 75–98.<br />

Beery, K. E. (1989). The Developmental Test of Visual–<strong>Motor</strong> Integration (3rd edn). Clevel<strong>and</strong>,<br />

OH: Modern Curriculum Press.<br />

Bir<strong>in</strong>gen, Z., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J. & Appelbaum, M. I. (1995). Affective reorganization<br />

<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>fant, the mother, <strong>and</strong> the dyad: The role of upright locomotion <strong>and</strong> its tim<strong>in</strong>g. Child<br />

Development, 66, 499–514.<br />

Booth, C. L., Rose-Krasnor, L., McK<strong>in</strong>non, J. A. & Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (1994). Predict<strong>in</strong>g<br />

social-adjustment <strong>in</strong> middle childhood—the role of preschool attachment security <strong>and</strong><br />

maternal style. <strong>Social</strong> Development, 3, 189–204.<br />

Brun<strong>in</strong>ks, R. H. (1978). The Brun<strong>in</strong>ks–Oseretsky Test of <strong>Motor</strong> Proficiency. Circle P<strong>in</strong>es, MN:<br />

American Guidance Service.<br />

Bundy, A. C., Lane, S. J. & Murray, E. A. (2002). Sensory Integration: Theory <strong>and</strong> Practice<br />

(2nd edn). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis.<br />

Burgess, K. B., Marshall, P. J., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. & Fox, N. A. (2003). Infant attachment <strong>and</strong> temperament<br />

as predictors of subsequent externaliz<strong>in</strong>g problems <strong>and</strong> cardiac physiology. Journal<br />

of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44, 819–831.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


308 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

Burns, L. H., Annett, L., Kelley, A. E., Everitt, B. J. & Robb<strong>in</strong>s, T. W. (1996). Effects of lesions<br />

to amygdala, ventral subiculum, medial prefrontal cortex, <strong>and</strong> nucleus accumbens on the<br />

reaction to novelty: Implication for limbic-striatal <strong>in</strong>teractions. Behavioral Neuroscience,<br />

110, 60–73.<br />

Campos, J. J., Kermoian, R., Wither<strong>in</strong>gton, D., Chen, H. & Dong, Q. (1997). Activity, attention,<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental transitions <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy. In P. J. Lang & R. F. Simons (Eds.).<br />

Attention <strong>and</strong> Orient<strong>in</strong>g: Sensory <strong>and</strong> Motivational Processes (pp. 393–415). Mahwah, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenste<strong>in</strong>, M. J. &<br />

Wither<strong>in</strong>gton, D. (2000). Travel broadens the m<strong>in</strong>d. Infancy, 1, 149–219.<br />

Caspi, A., Elder, G. H. & Bem, D. J. (1988). Mov<strong>in</strong>g away from the world: Life-course<br />

patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 824–831.<br />

Cheah, C. S., L., Nelson, L. J. & Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (2001). Nonsocial play as a risk factor <strong>in</strong> social<br />

development. In A. Goncu & E. L. Kle<strong>in</strong> (Eds.). <strong>Children</strong> <strong>in</strong> Play, Story, <strong>and</strong> School<br />

(pp. 39–71). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Chen, X. Y., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. & Li, B. S. (1995). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>and</strong> school adjustment of shy <strong>and</strong> aggressive-children<br />

<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 7, 337–349.<br />

Chen, X. Y., Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, P. D., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Chen, H. C., Cen, G. Z. & Stewart, S. L. (1998).<br />

Child-rear<strong>in</strong>g attitudes <strong>and</strong> behavioral <strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Canadian toddlers: A crosscultural<br />

study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677–686.<br />

Coplan, R. J. & Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (1998). Explor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g nonsocial play <strong>in</strong> the preschool:<br />

The development <strong>and</strong> validation of the preschool play behavior scale. <strong>Social</strong> Development,<br />

7, 72–91.<br />

Coplan, R. J., Gav<strong>in</strong>ski-Mol<strong>in</strong>a, M. H., Lagace-Segu<strong>in</strong>, D. G. & Wichmann, C. (2001). When<br />

girls versus boys play alone: Nonsocial play <strong>and</strong> adjustment <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 37, 464–474.<br />

Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K. & Armer, M. (2004). Do you ‘want’ to play? Dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g<br />

between conflicted shyness <strong>and</strong> social dis<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> early childhood. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 40, 244–258.<br />

Coplan, R. J., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calk<strong>in</strong>s, S. D. & Stewart, S. L. (1994). Be<strong>in</strong>g alone,<br />

play<strong>in</strong>g alone, <strong>and</strong> act<strong>in</strong>g alone—dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g among reticence <strong>and</strong> passive <strong>and</strong> active<br />

solitude <strong>in</strong> young-children. Child Development, 65, 129–137.<br />

Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety <strong>and</strong> affective style: Role of prefrontal cortex <strong>and</strong> amygdala.<br />

Biological Psychiatry, 51, 68–80.<br />

Dettl<strong>in</strong>g, A. C., Gunnar, M. R. & Donzella, B. (1999). Cortisol levels of young children <strong>in</strong> fullday<br />

childcare centers: Relations with age <strong>and</strong> temperament. Psychoneuroendocr<strong>in</strong>ology, 24,<br />

519–536.<br />

Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G. & Wilson, B. N. (2002). Developmental coord<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

disorder: Associated problems <strong>in</strong> attention, learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> psychosocial adjustment. Human<br />

Movement Science, 21, 905–918.<br />

Fabes, R. A., Mart<strong>in</strong>, C. L. & Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children’s play qualities <strong>in</strong> same- ,<br />

other- <strong>and</strong> mixed-sex peer groups. Child Development, 74, 921–932.<br />

Fogel, A. (1992). Movement <strong>and</strong> communication <strong>in</strong> human <strong>in</strong>fancy: The social dynamics of<br />

development. Human Movement Science, 11, 387–423.<br />

Fox, N. A. (1991). If its not left, its right—electroencephalograph asymmetry <strong>and</strong> the<br />

development of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 863–872.<br />

Fox, N. A. & Calk<strong>in</strong>s, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression <strong>and</strong> social withdrawal: Interactions<br />

among temperament, attachment <strong>and</strong> regulation. In K. H. Rub<strong>in</strong> & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.).<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Withdrawal, Inhibition <strong>and</strong> Shyness <strong>in</strong> Childhood (pp. 81–100). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Fox, N. A. & Henderson, H. A. (1999). Does <strong>in</strong>fancy matter? Predict<strong>in</strong>g social behavior from<br />

<strong>in</strong>fant temperament. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 445–455.<br />

Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Calk<strong>in</strong>s, S. D. & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Cont<strong>in</strong>uity<br />

<strong>and</strong> discont<strong>in</strong>uity of behavioral <strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>and</strong> exuberance: Psychophysiological <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1–21.<br />

Fox, N. A., Schmidt, L. A., Calk<strong>in</strong>s, S. D., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. & Coplan, R. J. (1996). The role of<br />

frontal activation <strong>in</strong> the regulation <strong>and</strong> dysregulation of social behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g the preschool<br />

years. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 8, 89–102.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


<strong>Motor</strong> <strong>Function</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> <strong>Participation</strong> 309<br />

Ghaziudd<strong>in</strong>, M., Butler, E., Tsai, L. & Ghaziudd<strong>in</strong>, N. (1994). Is clums<strong>in</strong>ess a marker for<br />

Asperger syndrome?. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 38, 519–527.<br />

Goto, Y. & O’Donnell, P. (2002). Tim<strong>in</strong>g-dependent limbic-motor synaptic <strong>in</strong>tegration <strong>in</strong><br />

the nucleus accambens. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99,<br />

13189–13193.<br />

Grace, A. A. (2000). Gat<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>formation flow with<strong>in</strong> the limbic system <strong>and</strong> the<br />

pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Bra<strong>in</strong> Research Bra<strong>in</strong> Research Reviews, 31, 330–341.<br />

Gustafson, G. E. (1984). Effects of the ability to locomote on <strong>in</strong>fant’s social <strong>and</strong> exploratory<br />

behavior: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 20, 397–405.<br />

Harrist, A. W., Zaia, A. F., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A. & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Subtypes of social<br />

withdrawal <strong>in</strong> early childhood: Sociometric status <strong>and</strong> social-cognitive differences across four<br />

years. Child Development, 68, 278–294.<br />

Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Fox, N. A. & Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (2004). Psychophysiological <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioral evidence for vary<strong>in</strong>g forms <strong>and</strong> functions of nonsocial behavior <strong>in</strong> preschoolers.<br />

Child Development, 75, 251–263.<br />

H<strong>in</strong>de, R. A., Tampl<strong>in</strong>, A. & Barrett, J. (1993). <strong>Social</strong> isolation <strong>in</strong> 4-year-olds. British Journal<br />

of Developmental Psychology, 11, 211–236.<br />

Hutt, C. (1970). Specific <strong>and</strong> diverse exploration. In H. Reese & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances <strong>in</strong><br />

Child Development <strong>and</strong> Behavior (pp. 119–181). New York: Academic Press.<br />

Jackson, M. E. & Moghaddam, B. (2001). Amygdala regulation of nucleus accumbens<br />

dopam<strong>in</strong>e output is governed by the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 676–681.<br />

Kagan, J. (1989). Temperamental contributions to social-behavior. American Psychologist, 44,<br />

668–674.<br />

Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s Prophecy: Temperament <strong>in</strong> Human Nature. New York: Basic Books.<br />

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S. & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology <strong>and</strong> psychology of behavioral<br />

<strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>in</strong> children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473.<br />

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S. & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science,<br />

240, 167–171.<br />

Karmon-Weisman, E. (2003). Treatment of Balance Deficiency <strong>and</strong> its Effect on <strong>Children</strong>’s<br />

Anxiety <strong>and</strong> Self-esteem. Unpublished MA thesis, Tel Aviv University.<br />

Kochanska, G. & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1992). Inhibition <strong>in</strong> toddlerhood <strong>and</strong> the dynamics of<br />

the child’s <strong>in</strong>teraction with an unfamiliar peer at age five. Child Development, 63,<br />

325–335.<br />

Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D. & Dodge, K. A. (1990). The role of poor peer relationships <strong>in</strong><br />

the development of disorder. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer Rejection <strong>in</strong> Childhood<br />

(pp. 274–305). New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Ladd, G. W. & Golter, B. S. (1988). Parents’ management of preschooler’s peer relations: Is it<br />

related to children’s social competence? Developmental Psychology, 24, 109–117.<br />

Ladd, G. W. & Hart, C. H. (1992). Creat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formal play opportunities: Are parents’ <strong>and</strong><br />

preschoolers’ <strong>in</strong>itiations related to children’s competence with peers? Developmental Psychology,<br />

28, 1179–1187.<br />

Lane, S. J. (2002). Structure <strong>and</strong> function of the sensory system. In A. C. Bundy, S. J. Lane &<br />

E. A. Murray (Eds.), Sensory Integration Theory <strong>and</strong> Practice (2nd edn, pp. 35–69). Philadelphia,<br />

PA: F. A. Davis Company.<br />

Manjiviona, J. & Prior, M. (1995). Comparison of Asperger syndrome <strong>and</strong> high-function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

autistic children on a test of motor impairment. Journal of Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disorders, 25, 23–29.<br />

Parham, L. D. & Fazio, L. S. (1997). Play <strong>in</strong> Occupational Therapy for <strong>Children</strong>. London:<br />

Mosby.<br />

Pellegr<strong>in</strong>i, A. D. & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature <strong>and</strong> function of a<br />

neglected aspect of play<strong>in</strong>g. Child Development, 69, 577–598.<br />

Piaget, J. (1962). Play, Dreams, <strong>and</strong> Imitation <strong>in</strong> Childhood. New York: Norton.<br />

R<strong>in</strong>ehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Brereton, A. V. & Tonge, B. J. (2001). Movement preparation<br />

<strong>in</strong> high-function<strong>in</strong>g autism <strong>and</strong> Asperger disorder: A serial choice reaction time task <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

motor reprogramm<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 31, 79–88.<br />

Rose-Krasnor, L., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Booth, C. L. & Coplan, R. (1996). The relation of maternal<br />

directiveness <strong>and</strong> child attachment security to social competence <strong>in</strong> preschoolers. International<br />

Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 309–325.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006


310 Yair Bar-Haim <strong>and</strong> Orit Bart<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (1982). Nonsocial play <strong>in</strong> preschoolers: Necessarily evil?. Child Development,<br />

53, 651–657.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (1995). Parents of aggressive <strong>and</strong> withdrawn children. In M. H. Bornste<strong>in</strong> (Ed.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of Parent<strong>in</strong>g, Vol. 1: <strong>Children</strong> <strong>and</strong> Parent<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 255–284). Mahwah, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (1998). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>and</strong> emotional development from a cultural perspective. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 34, 611–615.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. (2001). The Play Observation Scale (POS). Unpublished Manual, University of<br />

Maryl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H. & Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). <strong>Social</strong> withdrawal, <strong>in</strong>hibition, <strong>and</strong> shyness <strong>in</strong> childhood:<br />

Conceptual <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itional issues. In K. H. Rub<strong>in</strong> & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.). <strong>Social</strong><br />

Withdrawal, Inhibition, <strong>and</strong> Shyness <strong>in</strong> Childhood (pp. 3–17). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. (1998). Peer <strong>in</strong>teraction, relationships, <strong>and</strong> groups.<br />

In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of Child Psychology: <strong>Social</strong>, Emotional, <strong>and</strong> Personality<br />

Development (5th edn, pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Burgess, K. B. & Coplan, R. J. (2002). <strong>Social</strong> withdrawal <strong>and</strong> shyness. In P. K.<br />

Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell’s H<strong>and</strong>book of Childhood <strong>Social</strong> Development (pp.<br />

329–352). London: Blackwell.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Chen, X. Y. & Hymel, S. (1993). Socioemotional characteristics of withdrawn<br />

<strong>and</strong> aggressive children. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 39, 518–534.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A. & Calk<strong>in</strong>s, S. D. (1995). Emotionality, emotion<br />

regulation, <strong>and</strong> preschoolers social adaptation. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 7, 49–62.<br />

Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Chen, X. Y., McDongall, P., Bowker, A. & Mck<strong>in</strong>non, J. (1995). The Waterloo<br />

longitud<strong>in</strong>al propect – predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> externaliz<strong>in</strong>g problems <strong>in</strong> adolescence.<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 7, 751–764.<br />

Schaaf, R., Merril, S. & K<strong>in</strong>sella, N. (1987). Sensory <strong>in</strong>tegration <strong>and</strong> play behavior: A<br />

case study of the effectiveness of occupational therapy us<strong>in</strong>g sensory <strong>in</strong>tegrative techniques.<br />

Occupational Therapy <strong>in</strong> Health Care, 4, 61–75.<br />

Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, R. A. & Piek, J. P. (2001). Psychosocial implications of poor motor coord<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong><br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Human Movement Science, 20, 73–94.<br />

Sklare, D. A., Konrad, H. R., Maser, J. D. & Jacob, R. G. (2001). Special issue on the <strong>in</strong>terface<br />

of balance disorders <strong>and</strong> anxiety: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>and</strong> overview. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,<br />

15, 1–7.<br />

Smilansky, S. (1968). The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool <strong>Children</strong>.<br />

New York: Wiley.<br />

Smyth, M. M. & Anderson, H. I. (2000). Cop<strong>in</strong>g with clums<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> school playground: <strong>Social</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> physical play <strong>in</strong> children with coord<strong>in</strong>ation impairments. British Journal of Developmental<br />

Psychology, 18, 389–413.<br />

Smyth, M.M. & Anderson, H.I. (2001). Football participation <strong>in</strong> the primary school<br />

playground: The role of coord<strong>in</strong>ation impairments. British Journal of Developmental<br />

Psychology, 19, 369–379.<br />

Sp<strong>in</strong>ard, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Harris, E., Hanish, L., Fabes, R. A., Kupanoff, K., R<strong>in</strong>gwald, S.<br />

& Holmes, J. (2004). The relation of children’s everyday nonsocial peer play behavior to their<br />

emotionality, regulation, <strong>and</strong> social function<strong>in</strong>g. Developmental Psychology, 40, 67–80.<br />

Stevenson-H<strong>in</strong>de, J. & Glover, A. (1996). Shy girls <strong>and</strong> boys: A new look. Journal of Child<br />

Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> Allied Discipl<strong>in</strong>es, 37, 181–187.<br />

Notes<br />

1. Solitary–functional play is typically aggregated with solitary–dramatic play to form a category<br />

labeled solitary–active behavior. In the present study however, we observed practically no <strong>in</strong>cidence of<br />

dramatiz<strong>in</strong>g while play<strong>in</strong>g alone <strong>in</strong> the company of peers. Therefore, we treated solitary–functional play<br />

as a separate category.<br />

2. Although we entered Gender <strong>in</strong>to the analysis as a between-subjects factor, it is important to note<br />

that the sample size may have prevented a fully adequate assessment of the effect of this variable. We reran<br />

the above-described analysis with Gender entered as a covariate. The results were practically the same as<br />

those reported <strong>in</strong> the text.<br />

© Blackwell Publish<strong>in</strong>g Ltd. 2006 <strong>Social</strong> Development, 15, 2, 2006

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!