11.05.2013 Views

Predictors of Resilience in Maltreated and Nonmaltreated Latino ...

Predictors of Resilience in Maltreated and Nonmaltreated Latino ...

Predictors of Resilience in Maltreated and Nonmaltreated Latino ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association<br />

2005, Vol. 41, No. 2, 338–351 0012-1649/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.338<br />

<strong>Predictors</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o Children<br />

Elisa Flores, Dante Cicchetti, <strong>and</strong> Fred A. Rogosch<br />

Mt. Hope Family Center, University <strong>of</strong> Rochester<br />

To date, few studies have sought to <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effects <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment <strong>and</strong> processes<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g maladaptation <strong>and</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation, multiple aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g, personal resources, <strong>and</strong> relationship features were exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> school-age maltreated <strong>and</strong><br />

nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. <strong>Maltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were found to have fewer areas <strong>of</strong> resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g. Ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control, as personal resources, <strong>and</strong> the ability to form a positive<br />

relationship with an adult figure outside <strong>of</strong> the immediate family predicted resilience. However, certa<strong>in</strong><br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal function<strong>in</strong>g were differentially related to resilience for maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have implications for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g how resilience can be<br />

promoted <strong>in</strong> maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children are among the largest grow<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>ority group <strong>in</strong><br />

the United States, compris<strong>in</strong>g 16% <strong>of</strong> children younger than 18<br />

years <strong>of</strong> age (Flores et al., 2002). Historically, there has been a<br />

paucity <strong>of</strong> research <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the developmental processes <strong>and</strong><br />

outcomes <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (Dihn, Roosa, Te<strong>in</strong>, & Lopez, 2002;<br />

Fisher, Jackson, & Villarruel, 1998; Flores et al., 2002). Ow<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

issues such as immigration, language barriers, discrim<strong>in</strong>ation, acculturative<br />

stress, <strong>and</strong> parental factors such as teenage pregnancy,<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children may be at high risk for maladaptive outcomes,<br />

even more so than children from other m<strong>in</strong>ority groups <strong>of</strong> similar<br />

socioeconomic status (SES; Arcia, Keyes, & Gallagher, 1994;<br />

Can<strong>in</strong>o, Gould, Prupris, & Shaffer, 1986; Centers for Disease<br />

Control <strong>and</strong> Prevention, 1999; Fisher et al., 1998; Flores et al.,<br />

2002). Consequently, there is now a great urgency to conduct<br />

research to elucidate the processes contribut<strong>in</strong>g to adaptive <strong>and</strong><br />

maladaptive function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this population <strong>of</strong> children.<br />

In addition, many environmental risk factors have been identified<br />

that predict maladaptive function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> children <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

<strong>of</strong> race/ethnicity, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g limited maternal education, unskilled<br />

occupational status <strong>of</strong> the head <strong>of</strong> the household, low SES, dependency<br />

on the state for f<strong>in</strong>ancial support, parental unemployment,<br />

low number <strong>of</strong> parents liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the home, large family size,<br />

parental mental illness, <strong>and</strong> parental relationship <strong>in</strong>stability<br />

(Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, & Aber, 1997; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998;<br />

Elisa Flores, Dante Cicchetti, <strong>and</strong> Fred A. Rogosch, Mt. Hope Family<br />

Center, University <strong>of</strong> Rochester.<br />

This work was supported <strong>in</strong> part by grants from the U.S. Adm<strong>in</strong>istration<br />

for Children <strong>and</strong> Families, Office <strong>of</strong> Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Spunk Fund, Inc. We thank Michael Lynch, Jennifer Larter, Rob<strong>in</strong> Sturm,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Enid De Jesus for their outst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g leadership <strong>in</strong> direct<strong>in</strong>g the research<br />

camp throughout the years that this work was conducted. We also appreciate<br />

the cont<strong>in</strong>uous cooperation <strong>of</strong> the Monroe County <strong>and</strong> the New York<br />

State Departments <strong>of</strong> Social Services. We are especially grateful to all <strong>of</strong><br />

the children <strong>and</strong> families who participated <strong>in</strong> the camp <strong>and</strong> made this work<br />

possible. F<strong>in</strong>ally, we thank Sheree L. Toth <strong>and</strong> Jungmeen Kim for their<br />

comments <strong>and</strong> feedback on prior versions <strong>of</strong> this article.<br />

Correspondence concern<strong>in</strong>g this article should be addressed to Elisa<br />

Flores or Dante Cicchetti, Mt. Hope Family Center, University <strong>of</strong> Rochester,<br />

187 Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh Street, Rochester, NY 14608. E-mail: eaflores@<br />

psych.rochester.edu or mhfc@netacc.net<br />

338<br />

Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Jessor, 1993; Luthar, 1999;<br />

McLoyd, 1998b; Rutter, 1979; Samer<strong>of</strong>f, Seifer, Barocas, Zax, &<br />

Greenspan, 1987; Seifer, Samer<strong>of</strong>f, Baldw<strong>in</strong>, & Baldw<strong>in</strong>, 1992).<br />

Furthermore, many children liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come families tend to<br />

experience negative outcomes such as higher rates <strong>of</strong> academic<br />

failure, emotional distress, <strong>and</strong> mental health problems (Duncan &<br />

Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994;<br />

Huston, McLoyd, & García Coll, 1997; Luthar, 1999; McLoyd,<br />

1998a, 1998b).<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the National Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect Data System<br />

(U. S. Department <strong>of</strong> Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services, National<br />

Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse on Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect Information, 2002),<br />

approximately 879,000 children <strong>in</strong> the United States are victims <strong>of</strong><br />

child maltreatment, <strong>and</strong> about 15% <strong>of</strong> those victims are Lat<strong>in</strong>o/<br />

Hispanic. Prior research has shown that maltreated children are at<br />

high risk for maladaptive function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> psychopathology across<br />

the life span because <strong>of</strong> the many detrimental effects on biological,<br />

cognitive, social, emotional, representational, <strong>and</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic development<br />

associated with child maltreatment (Cicchetti, 2002; Cicchetti<br />

& Lynch, 1995; Cicchetti & Rogosch; 2001a, 2001b; De-<br />

Bellis, Baum, et al., 1999; DeBellis, Keshaven, et al., 1999;<br />

Mal<strong>in</strong>owsky-Rummel & Hansen, 1993; Trickett & McBride-<br />

Chang, 1995). Maltreatment also is associated with disruptions <strong>in</strong><br />

several stage-salient areas <strong>of</strong> development, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g emotion regulation,<br />

the formation <strong>of</strong> secure attachment relationships, the development<br />

<strong>of</strong> an autonomous <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrated self-system, effective<br />

peer relations, <strong>and</strong> successful adaptation to school (Cicchetti &<br />

Lynch, 1993; Cicchetti & Toth, 1995; Eckenrode, Laird, & Doris,<br />

1993; Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Shonk & Cicchetti, 2001; Toth,<br />

Cicchetti, Macfie, & Emde, 1997; Trickett & McBride-Chang,<br />

1995). Because <strong>of</strong> the extensive risk factors for maladaptive development<br />

faced by many Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, the deleterious effects<br />

<strong>of</strong> child maltreatment may place some Lat<strong>in</strong>o children at even<br />

greater risk for negative outcomes.<br />

A content analysis <strong>of</strong> three major journals specializ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> child<br />

maltreatment research revealed that very few studies <strong>of</strong> child<br />

maltreatment exam<strong>in</strong>e the ethnicity <strong>of</strong> their participants (Behl,<br />

Crouch, May, Valente, & Conyngham, 2001). Of the 1,133 articles<br />

that were published <strong>in</strong> these journals from 1977 to 1998, only 6.7%<br />

(N 76) focused on ethnicity <strong>in</strong> their participant sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

study designs. Fewer than half <strong>of</strong> all studies reviewed <strong>in</strong>cluded


<strong>in</strong>formation regard<strong>in</strong>g the ethnic representation <strong>of</strong> their participants,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> those studies, the mean percentage <strong>of</strong> study participants<br />

reported to be Lat<strong>in</strong>o was 7% (Behl et al., 2001).<br />

As suggested by Cohen, Debl<strong>in</strong>ger, Mannar<strong>in</strong>o, <strong>and</strong> de Arellano<br />

(2001), m<strong>in</strong>ority children who have been abused may be at greater<br />

risk for develop<strong>in</strong>g more detrimental sequelae <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment<br />

than are maltreated children from more ma<strong>in</strong>stream cultures.<br />

Although few studies <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children have <strong>in</strong>vestigated the<br />

deleterious effects <strong>of</strong> different types <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment, several<br />

studies have demonstrated greater rates <strong>of</strong> behavioral problems,<br />

depressive symptoms, self-esteem issues, <strong>and</strong> emotional trauma<br />

among sexually abused African American <strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children than<br />

among European American sexually abused children (Morrow &<br />

Sorrell, 1989; Russell, 1986; S<strong>and</strong>ers-Phillips, Moisan, Wadl<strong>in</strong>gton,<br />

Morgan, & English, 1995; Ste<strong>in</strong>, Gold<strong>in</strong>g, Siegel, Burnam, &<br />

Sorenson, 1988). For <strong>in</strong>stance, a study <strong>of</strong> African American <strong>and</strong><br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>a sexually abused girls by S<strong>and</strong>ers-Phillips <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(1995) found higher rates <strong>of</strong> depression among sexually abused<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>a girls than among sexually abused African American girls <strong>of</strong><br />

the same socioeconomic background. Compared with African<br />

American girls, Lat<strong>in</strong>a girls were more likely to have been sexually<br />

abused by a family member, experienced lower self-esteem <strong>and</strong><br />

feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>adequacy, <strong>and</strong> received less maternal support after<br />

disclosure <strong>of</strong> the abuse.<br />

Thus, prior <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>of</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (primarily<br />

sexually abused children) have demonstrated that these children<br />

are at high risk for maladaptive outcomes (e.g., depression, behavior<br />

problems; Morrow & Sorrell, 1989; Russell, 1986; S<strong>and</strong>ers-<br />

Phillips et al., 1995; Ste<strong>in</strong> et al., 1988). Because very little is<br />

known about the predictors <strong>of</strong> developmental outcomes <strong>in</strong> general<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> resilient outcomes <strong>in</strong> particular <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, it is<br />

imperative that we beg<strong>in</strong> to evaluate what processes may contribute<br />

to resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> both maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong> is conceptualized as a dynamic process that <strong>in</strong>fluences<br />

an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s capacity to adapt <strong>and</strong> function successfully<br />

despite experienc<strong>in</strong>g chronic stress <strong>and</strong> adversity; it does not<br />

represent a personality trait or other static condition (Egel<strong>and</strong>,<br />

Carlson, & Sroufe, 1993; Luthar, 2003; Luthar, Cicchetti, &<br />

Becker, 2000). Although previous studies <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong> maltreated<br />

children have not focused solely on Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, a prior<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>of</strong> predictors <strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation evaluated multiple<br />

areas <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a heterogeneous sample <strong>of</strong> economically<br />

disadvantaged maltreated children <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated comparison<br />

children (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Lynch, & Holt, 1993).<br />

<strong>Maltreated</strong> children were rated as more disruptive–aggressive <strong>and</strong><br />

more withdrawn, had significantly greater levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behavior problems, <strong>and</strong> ev<strong>in</strong>ced lower overall competence on the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dices <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g that were assessed than nonmaltreated children.<br />

However, among the maltreated children, ego-resiliency,<br />

ego-overcontrol, <strong>and</strong> positive self-esteem were found to be predictors<br />

<strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation. In contrast, among the nonmaltreated<br />

children, only ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> positive self-esteem were<br />

predictive <strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation. Likewise, <strong>in</strong> a 3-year longitud<strong>in</strong>al<br />

study <strong>of</strong> another heterogeneous sample, Cicchetti <strong>and</strong> Rogosch<br />

(1997) demonstrated that <strong>in</strong> maltreated, socioeconomically disadvantaged<br />

children, factors such as positive self-esteem, egoresiliency,<br />

<strong>and</strong> ego-overcontrol predicted resilient function<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

whereas <strong>in</strong> nonmaltreated children, relationship features, as well as<br />

ego-resiliency, proved to be more <strong>in</strong>fluential.<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

339<br />

Intellectual function<strong>in</strong>g is another factor that may predict resilience<br />

among high-risk children. Prior <strong>in</strong>vestigations have shown<br />

that <strong>in</strong>tellectual function<strong>in</strong>g, as typically measured by <strong>in</strong>telligence<br />

tests, not only predicts academic achievement but also may serve<br />

as a protective factor aga<strong>in</strong>st the development <strong>of</strong> antisocial behavior<br />

problems (Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen, 1984; Luthar, 1991;<br />

Masten et al., 1999; White, M<strong>of</strong>fitt, & Silva, 1989). High <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

abilities may be associated with normal bra<strong>in</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> cognitive development despite the experience <strong>of</strong> adversity<br />

(Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003). Furthermore, high <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability<br />

may <strong>in</strong>dicate effective <strong>in</strong>formation-process<strong>in</strong>g skills that a highrisk<br />

child may use when cop<strong>in</strong>g with environmental hardships,<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g better problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>and</strong> the ability to attract the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>of</strong> teachers <strong>and</strong> improve school function<strong>in</strong>g (Masten &<br />

Coatsworth, 1998). In a study conducted by Herrenkohl, Herrenkohl,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Egolf (1994), average or above average <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

performance was found to be an <strong>in</strong>dividual factor that contributed<br />

to ongo<strong>in</strong>g competent function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a longitud<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated children. Because previous studies<br />

have shown that there is a l<strong>in</strong>k between <strong>in</strong>tellectual function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> positive developmental outcomes, <strong>in</strong> the present <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

we exam<strong>in</strong>ed the role <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tellectual function<strong>in</strong>g as a predictor <strong>of</strong><br />

resilience <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Despite the fact that past research has sought to identify predictors<br />

<strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation <strong>in</strong> maltreated children, current studies<br />

have not conducted with<strong>in</strong>-group analyses <strong>of</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children, <strong>and</strong> it is not known if the same predictive factors <strong>of</strong><br />

resilience would be applicable to this group. For <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>in</strong> many<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultures, relationships are extremely valued <strong>and</strong> considered<br />

<strong>of</strong> central importance (Harrison, Wilson, P<strong>in</strong>e, Chan, & Buriel,<br />

1990). Yet it is not known whether relationship features would<br />

serve as predictors <strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation for both maltreated <strong>and</strong><br />

nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. In other diverse populations, the<br />

ability to form relationships with others has been shown to promote<br />

resilience (Luthar, 2003; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). In<br />

addition, <strong>in</strong> a sample <strong>of</strong> high-risk children, Werner (1993) identified<br />

protective factors associated with the development <strong>of</strong> resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> Hawaiian children followed over the course <strong>of</strong> a 32-year<br />

longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. One <strong>of</strong> the important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> that <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

was that the ability to establish <strong>and</strong> seek out emotional<br />

support systems that extended beyond the immediate family network<br />

promoted resilience <strong>in</strong> adulthood. That is, external support<br />

systems served as a protective factor for high-risk Hawaiian children<br />

<strong>and</strong> made a significant impact on the development <strong>of</strong> resilience<br />

over time. To date, it is unknown whether this pattern would<br />

emerge <strong>in</strong> a sample <strong>of</strong> high-risk, socioeconomically disadvantaged,<br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Likewise, there is very little known about the role <strong>of</strong> gender <strong>in</strong><br />

developmental outcomes among maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. Previous<br />

literature has suggested that gender may <strong>in</strong>fluence responses<br />

to adversity; specifically, it has been noted that girls may be less<br />

reactive to family stress than boys, <strong>and</strong> they may be less at risk for<br />

develop<strong>in</strong>g externaliz<strong>in</strong>g responses (Emery & O’Leary, 1982; Fergusson<br />

& Horwood, 2001; Hether<strong>in</strong>gton, 1989; Porter & O’Leary,<br />

1980). However, to our knowledge, no <strong>in</strong>vestigations have exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

whether gender <strong>in</strong>fluences resilient function<strong>in</strong>g among Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children or the impact <strong>of</strong> cultural sex roles <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g resilience.<br />

Because traditional sex roles such as machismo (a man who carries<br />

himself with respect, responsibility, <strong>and</strong> honor) <strong>and</strong> marianismo (a<br />

woman who is self-sacrific<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> virtuous) are prom<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>in</strong>


340<br />

many Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultures, they may <strong>in</strong>fluence early socialization <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioral development <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o boys <strong>and</strong> girls (Bem, 1981; de<br />

Rios, 2001; Vasquez, 1994).<br />

Hypotheses<br />

On the basis <strong>of</strong> extant theory <strong>and</strong> empirical research, we hypothesized<br />

(a) that maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children would manifest less<br />

adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g than their equally disadvantaged nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o counterparts; (b) that for nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children,<br />

ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence would predict resilient function<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

whereas <strong>in</strong> the maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o group, ego-overcontrol,<br />

ego-resiliency, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence would predict resilient function<strong>in</strong>g;<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) that for maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children,<br />

relationship features would prove to be significant <strong>in</strong> contribut<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to resilience. In addition, we conducted exploratory analyses <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigated the potential role <strong>of</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> the function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong>cluded 133 Lat<strong>in</strong>o children from an<br />

upstate New York urban sett<strong>in</strong>g who attended a summer day camp research<br />

program designed for maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated low-<strong>in</strong>come, disadvantaged<br />

children. In this <strong>in</strong>vestigation, the term Lat<strong>in</strong>o refers to children<br />

“whose orig<strong>in</strong>s can be traced back to Spanish speak<strong>in</strong>g regions <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong><br />

America, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>of</strong> the Caribbean, Mexico, Central America, <strong>and</strong><br />

South America” (Flores et al., 2002, p. 83). The children were identified by<br />

their primary caregivers as Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>and</strong> were, on average, 8.68 years <strong>of</strong> age<br />

(SD 1.78; see Table 1); 30.8% <strong>of</strong> the children were girls <strong>and</strong> 69.2% were<br />

boys. All Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who attended the camp were fluent <strong>in</strong> English<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to parent report <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions with the child. The Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

sample was drawn from the larger group <strong>of</strong> camp attendees <strong>and</strong> was<br />

composed <strong>of</strong> all the Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who had attended the day camp dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the years 1986–2000. This selection criterion is a strength <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

because it provides the unique opportunity to evaluate a large sample <strong>of</strong><br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children with<strong>in</strong> the context <strong>of</strong> a diverse pool <strong>of</strong> children <strong>of</strong> varied<br />

racial <strong>and</strong> ethnic backgrounds. By select<strong>in</strong>g a sample <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

from among a larger representation <strong>of</strong> ethnicities, counselors were not<br />

exclusively rat<strong>in</strong>g Lat<strong>in</strong>o children but were simultaneously evaluat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

children from many different ethnicities <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g as they would <strong>in</strong> an<br />

everyday context. Moreover, the counselors were unaware <strong>of</strong> the experimental<br />

hypotheses <strong>and</strong> the children’s maltreatment status.<br />

This sample comprised both maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (N 76; 56<br />

boys <strong>and</strong> 20 girls) <strong>and</strong> demographically comparable nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children (N 57; 36 boys <strong>and</strong> 21 girls). Consistent with the larger<br />

percentage <strong>of</strong> boys <strong>in</strong> the Monroe County Department <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>and</strong><br />

Health Services (MCDHHS) population <strong>and</strong> consequently <strong>in</strong> the overall<br />

camp sample, the Lat<strong>in</strong>o sample had a higher percentage <strong>of</strong> boys (69.2%)<br />

than girls (30.8%). However, the gender distribution did not differ significantly<br />

for the maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated groups.<br />

Prior to enroll<strong>in</strong>g their children <strong>in</strong> the camp, parents <strong>of</strong> all children<br />

provided <strong>in</strong>formed consent for their children’s participation as well as for<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> any MCDHHS records. <strong>Maltreated</strong> children were identified<br />

by MCDHHS as hav<strong>in</strong>g experienced child abuse <strong>and</strong>/or neglect. All exist<strong>in</strong>g<br />

MCDHHS records were screened <strong>and</strong> coded by raters us<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

Barnett, Manly, <strong>and</strong> Cicchetti (1993) classification system for child maltreatment.<br />

In order to obta<strong>in</strong> a demographically comparable comparison<br />

group, nonmaltreated children were recruited from families receiv<strong>in</strong>g Aid<br />

to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC) <strong>and</strong> through notices placed<br />

<strong>in</strong> neighborhoods where the maltreated children resided. MCDHHS record<br />

searches were conducted on the comparison group families, <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

research assistants also <strong>in</strong>terviewed them, <strong>in</strong> order to further verify that<br />

there was no history <strong>of</strong> prior MCDHHS <strong>in</strong>volvement. Families were<br />

excluded from the nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o comparison group if any family<br />

history <strong>of</strong> child abuse <strong>and</strong>/or neglect existed or if the family had received<br />

preventive services through MCDHHS because <strong>of</strong> concerns over risk for<br />

maltreatment.<br />

Demographic Characteristics<br />

The sample as a whole can be characterized as disadvantaged <strong>and</strong> at risk<br />

for maladaptive outcomes because <strong>of</strong> environmental risk factors such as<br />

liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent homes with multiple children, low SES, dependency<br />

on the state for f<strong>in</strong>ancial support, <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority status (see Table 1).<br />

Procedure<br />

The week-long summer day camp is designed to provide children with<br />

a variety <strong>of</strong> recreational activities <strong>and</strong> opportunities for peer <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

while <strong>in</strong> a supportive environment. At the day camp, the children were<br />

Table 1<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> Demographic Characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong> Children<br />

Variable<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

<strong>Maltreated</strong><br />

(n 76)<br />

<strong>Nonmaltreated</strong><br />

(n 57)<br />

M SD % M SD %<br />

Gender<br />

Male 73.7 63.2<br />

Female 26.3 36.8<br />

Age 8.53 1.47 8.90 2.12<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> adults <strong>in</strong> home 1.66 0.69 1.50 0.56<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> children <strong>in</strong> home 3.27 2.41 3.06 1.18<br />

Marital status<br />

S<strong>in</strong>gle, not liv<strong>in</strong>g with a partner 38.0 42.9<br />

Married or liv<strong>in</strong>g with a partner 40.8 26.5<br />

Divorced, separated, widowed 21.1 30.6<br />

Family Holl<strong>in</strong>gshead (Levels 1 <strong>and</strong> 2) 90.0 81.6<br />

Receiv<strong>in</strong>g public assistance 89.9 76.6<br />

Note. There were no significant differences between maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated children on any <strong>of</strong> the<br />

demographic variables. Holl<strong>in</strong>gshead Holl<strong>in</strong>gshead Index <strong>of</strong> Social Position.


placed <strong>in</strong> groups consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> three counselors (one head counselor <strong>and</strong><br />

two co-counselors) <strong>and</strong> 6–8 children who were matched accord<strong>in</strong>g to age<br />

<strong>and</strong> gender. The percentages <strong>of</strong> maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated children <strong>in</strong><br />

the groups were approximately equal (50% maltreated <strong>and</strong> 50% nonmaltreated).<br />

With<strong>in</strong> the groups, many different ethnicities (e.g., African American,<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o, European American) were represented among the children.<br />

New groups <strong>of</strong> children attended the camp every week.<br />

A typical camp day lasted 7 hr, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> 35 hr <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction between<br />

the children <strong>and</strong> the counselors dur<strong>in</strong>g the week. In addition to recreational<br />

activities, the children also were asked to participate <strong>in</strong> a variety <strong>of</strong> research<br />

assessments. Child assent was obta<strong>in</strong>ed prior to any adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>of</strong><br />

research assessments. Research assistants were tra<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g<br />

assessments such as the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised<br />

(PPVT–R) to <strong>in</strong>dividual children (see the follow<strong>in</strong>g section). Camp counselors<br />

completed a variety <strong>of</strong> assessments that measured their <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

with, <strong>and</strong> observations <strong>of</strong>, the children assigned to their respective groups.<br />

Measures<br />

Maltreatment Classification System<br />

In order to determ<strong>in</strong>e children’s maltreatment experiences, cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

MCDHHS records was conducted by tra<strong>in</strong>ed research assistants, doctoral<br />

students, <strong>and</strong> PhD-level psychologists us<strong>in</strong>g the Maltreatment Classification<br />

System (MCS) developed at the Mt. Hope Family Center (Barnett et<br />

al., 1993). The MCS uses MCDHHS records detail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>and</strong><br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs regard<strong>in</strong>g maltreatment occurrences <strong>in</strong> identified families. Rather<br />

than rely<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>of</strong>ficial designations <strong>and</strong> case dispositions, the MCS codes<br />

all <strong>in</strong>formation available on a designated family from MCDHHS records,<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependent determ<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>of</strong> maltreatment experiences. (See<br />

Manly, Kim, Rogosch, & Cicchetti, 2001, for a more detailed explanation<br />

<strong>of</strong> this cod<strong>in</strong>g system.)<br />

Among the maltreated sample <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong> this study, 74.6%<br />

experienced physical neglect, 83.1% experienced emotional maltreatment,<br />

66.2% experienced physical abuse, <strong>and</strong> 15.5% experienced sexual abuse.<br />

Consistent with previous maltreatment literature, there was a high comorbidity<br />

<strong>of</strong> multiple subtypes among the maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o sample. Multiple<br />

subtypes were experienced by 77.6% <strong>of</strong> the Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. Because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

high comorbidity among the maltreatment subtypes, discrete subgroups for<br />

use <strong>in</strong> subtype comparisons were not feasible.<br />

Counselor Measures<br />

Camp counselors each evaluated the children <strong>in</strong> their respective groups<br />

by provid<strong>in</strong>g assessments <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual child function<strong>in</strong>g. The counselors<br />

were each tra<strong>in</strong>ed on how to complete the assessments described below,<br />

<strong>and</strong> counselors conducted their rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> the children <strong>in</strong>dependently at the<br />

end <strong>of</strong> each week, after 35 hr <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with the children. Interrater<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ations were made by comput<strong>in</strong>g Cronbach’s alphas among the<br />

raters <strong>in</strong> each camp group. Individual counselor measures were averaged<br />

for each <strong>in</strong>dividual child, thus comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>dependent perspectives <strong>of</strong><br />

two to three different adult raters.<br />

Behavior rat<strong>in</strong>gs (Wright, 1983). Counselors rated the behavior <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual children on n<strong>in</strong>e items that assessed three different aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>terpersonal function<strong>in</strong>g: prosocial behavior, aggression, <strong>and</strong> withdrawal.<br />

Seven-po<strong>in</strong>t rat<strong>in</strong>gs were completed each day <strong>and</strong> based on 45-m<strong>in</strong> observations<br />

<strong>of</strong> children <strong>in</strong> unstructured <strong>and</strong> structured camp sett<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g.,<br />

sports, art, free play). Interrater reliabilities across the years <strong>of</strong> assessment<br />

(1986–2000) ranged from .67 to .85 for prosocial behavior, from .70 to .88<br />

for aggression, <strong>and</strong> from .65 to .80 for withdrawal. Individual counselor<br />

assessments were averaged together to generate <strong>in</strong>dividual child scores for<br />

each <strong>of</strong> the three scales.<br />

Pupil Evaluation Inventory (PEI; Pekarik, Pr<strong>in</strong>z, Liebert, We<strong>in</strong>traub, &<br />

Neale, 1976). Counselors completed the PEI at the end <strong>of</strong> each week. The<br />

PEI makes use <strong>of</strong> a nom<strong>in</strong>ation technique <strong>and</strong> consists <strong>of</strong> 35 items that<br />

assess social behavior. It yields three homogeneous <strong>and</strong> stable factors:<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

341<br />

Aggression, Withdrawal, <strong>and</strong> Likability. Counselors nom<strong>in</strong>ated up to 2<br />

children <strong>in</strong> their groups who were characterized best by each <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

item. Interrater reliabilities across camp years ranged from .88 to .90.<br />

Aggregate scores for each <strong>of</strong> the three scales were generated on the basis<br />

<strong>of</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>ations each child received on the respective scale<br />

items. Scores were then averaged across counselors to obta<strong>in</strong> three subscale<br />

scores per child.<br />

Teacher Report Form <strong>of</strong> the Child Behavior Checklist (TRF; Achenbach,<br />

1991b). At the end <strong>of</strong> each camp week, counselors rated each child’s<br />

behavioral symptomatology by complet<strong>in</strong>g the TRF (a teacher-completed<br />

version <strong>of</strong> the Child Behavior Checklist; Achenbach, 1991a). The TRF is<br />

a widely used <strong>and</strong> validated <strong>in</strong>strument for assess<strong>in</strong>g behavioral disturbance<br />

from the perspective <strong>of</strong> teachers. In the present study, because camp<br />

counselors were able to observe behaviors similar to those observed by<br />

teachers, camp counselors completed the measure to provide an analogous<br />

assessment <strong>of</strong> child function<strong>in</strong>g by an adult external to the family. The TRF<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>s 118 items rated for frequency that assess two broadb<strong>and</strong> dimensions<br />

<strong>of</strong> child symptomatology: externaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavioral problems (e.g.,<br />

del<strong>in</strong>quent <strong>and</strong> aggressive behavior) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavioral problems<br />

(e.g., withdrawal, anxiety–depression, <strong>and</strong> somatic compla<strong>in</strong>ts). The scores<br />

for each child, <strong>in</strong> each group, were averaged among counselors to obta<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual child scores for both <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> externaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavior<br />

problems. Interrater reliabilities ranged from .83 to .91 for externaliz<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behavior problems <strong>and</strong> from .70 to .88 for <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavior problems.<br />

California Child Q-Set (CCQ; J. H. Block & Block, 1969). At the end<br />

<strong>of</strong> each week, the three counselors <strong>in</strong> each group <strong>in</strong>dependently completed<br />

the CCQ for each child <strong>in</strong> their respective group on the basis <strong>of</strong> their<br />

week-long <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>in</strong>teractions with <strong>and</strong> observations <strong>of</strong> the children. The<br />

CCQ consists <strong>of</strong> 100 diverse items about the personality, cognitive, <strong>and</strong><br />

social characteristics <strong>of</strong> children. Raters sort the <strong>in</strong>dividual items pr<strong>in</strong>ted on<br />

cards <strong>in</strong>to a fixed distribution <strong>of</strong> piles depict<strong>in</strong>g n<strong>in</strong>e categories rang<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from most to least characteristic <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual child. Thus, <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>iles consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the 100 items are generated for each child. Interrater<br />

agreement ranged from .68 to .86. Two-dimensional scores are derived<br />

from the Q-set data: ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control. On the basis <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iles<br />

for prototypical children display<strong>in</strong>g high ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> high egoundercontrol,<br />

each item was assigned a criterion score for each respective<br />

dimension (J. H. Block & Block, 1980). Each <strong>in</strong>dividual child’s pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong><br />

Q-sorted items was correlated with the criterion sorts. The result<strong>in</strong>g correlations<br />

<strong>of</strong> each child’s pr<strong>of</strong>ile with the prototypical ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong><br />

ego-control criterion sorts represented how similar to or different from the<br />

prototype pr<strong>of</strong>iles the <strong>in</strong>dividual child was. For <strong>in</strong>stance, a high positive<br />

correlation with the ego-resiliency sort would <strong>in</strong>dicate that a child had a<br />

high level <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency, <strong>and</strong> a negative correlation would be <strong>in</strong>dicative<br />

<strong>of</strong> low ego-resiliency, or ego-brittleness (J. H. Block & Block, 1980).<br />

Similarly, the ego-control dimension is organized such that high scores on<br />

the ego-control dimension would <strong>in</strong>dicate ego-undercontrol, <strong>and</strong> low scores<br />

would <strong>in</strong>dicate ego-overcontrol. The correlations <strong>of</strong> each counselor’s<br />

Q-sort pr<strong>of</strong>iles for each child with the criterion sorts were averaged to<br />

obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual scores <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control for each child.<br />

Student–Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS; Pianta & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 1992).<br />

The STRS is a 30-item measure assess<strong>in</strong>g the quality <strong>of</strong> the relationship<br />

between a child <strong>and</strong> a teacher, as reported from the teacher’s perspective.<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> the similarity between the roles <strong>of</strong> a teacher <strong>and</strong> a camp<br />

counselor <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> these <strong>in</strong>dividuals represent<strong>in</strong>g nonfamilial adults, the<br />

STRS was deemed to be appropriate for the counselors to use to rate their<br />

relationships with the <strong>in</strong>dividual children <strong>in</strong> their respective groups. Items<br />

are rated on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scale represent<strong>in</strong>g how applicable each item is for a<br />

particular child. Subscales for this <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong>clude Warm, Open Communication,<br />

Conflicted, Dependent, <strong>and</strong> Closed/Troubled. A total relationship<br />

score can also be obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> was used <strong>in</strong> these analyses. Interrater<br />

reliability was not obta<strong>in</strong>ed because relationship quality with each child<br />

could vary from counselor to counselor. Because the head counselors <strong>of</strong><br />

each group were considered the group leaders <strong>and</strong> typically had the most<br />

experience work<strong>in</strong>g with high-risk children, the rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> the head coun-


342<br />

selors for each respective group were used to characterize the relationship<br />

quality for each child.<br />

Peer Measure<br />

Peer nom<strong>in</strong>ations (Coie & Dodge, 1983) were the peer measure used. On<br />

the last day <strong>of</strong> each week, after children had <strong>in</strong>teracted with each other<br />

across the week <strong>of</strong> summer camp, children evaluated the characteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

their peers, with<strong>in</strong> their respective camp groups, us<strong>in</strong>g a peer nom<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

method (Coie & Dodge, 1983). Each child was asked to select one peer<br />

from his or her group who best fit a series <strong>of</strong> statements that described the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g behavioral categories: cooperative, leader, shy, disruptive,<br />

fighter, liked most, <strong>and</strong> liked least. These descriptions tapped <strong>in</strong>to the<br />

aggressive, withdrawn, <strong>and</strong> prosocial dimensions assessed <strong>in</strong> the study. A<br />

photograph <strong>of</strong> the group was used to facilitate adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>of</strong> the<br />

measure, <strong>and</strong> the children were assured that their responses would be kept<br />

confidential. The total number <strong>of</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>ations each <strong>in</strong>dividual child received<br />

from his or her peers <strong>in</strong> each category was determ<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>and</strong> these<br />

totals were converted to proportions <strong>of</strong> the possible nom<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>in</strong> each<br />

category. The proportions were subsequently st<strong>and</strong>ardized with<strong>in</strong> each<br />

camp year.<br />

Child Self-Report Measure<br />

The PPVT–R (Dunn & Dunn, 1981) measures an important aspect <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>telligence by assess<strong>in</strong>g receptive vocabulary 1 (Aber & Allen, 1987). The<br />

PPVT–R has been demonstrated to have adequate <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency (a<br />

split-half reliability <strong>of</strong> .80) <strong>and</strong> has been shown to correlate .64 with the<br />

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) Full Scale IQ (Dunn &<br />

Dunn, 1981). The PPVT–R was adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> English by tra<strong>in</strong>ed research<br />

assistants <strong>in</strong> a one-on-one <strong>in</strong>terview sett<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Composite Scores <strong>of</strong> Resilient Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

An overall resilient function<strong>in</strong>g score was created to assess multiple<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g social competence <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

symptomatology). Indicators <strong>of</strong> social competence were evaluated<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g measures obta<strong>in</strong>ed from both peers <strong>and</strong> counselors; <strong>in</strong>dicators <strong>of</strong><br />

behavioral symptomatology were obta<strong>in</strong>ed only from counselors. A total <strong>of</strong><br />

n<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicators were assessed <strong>and</strong> used <strong>in</strong> the creation <strong>of</strong> an overall resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g score.<br />

First, three composite variables assess<strong>in</strong>g prosocial behavior, aggressiveness,<br />

<strong>and</strong> withdrawal were created from the perspective <strong>of</strong> counselors.<br />

These aspects <strong>of</strong> a child’s <strong>in</strong>terpersonal behavior are important because<br />

they portend long-term adjustment (Deater-Deckard, 2001; Masten et al.,<br />

1999; Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993; Parker, Rub<strong>in</strong>, Price, &<br />

DeRosier, 1995). Scores from the behavior rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> the PEI were used.<br />

A prosocial score was created by averag<strong>in</strong>g z scores for the behavior<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs score <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior <strong>and</strong> the PEI score for Likability ( <br />

.66). Likewise, z scores for the behavior rat<strong>in</strong>gs’ <strong>and</strong> PEI’s aggression<br />

scales were averaged ( .69), <strong>and</strong> z scores for the behavior rat<strong>in</strong>gs’ <strong>and</strong><br />

PEI’s withdrawal scales were averaged ( .68).<br />

From the perspective <strong>of</strong> peers, four <strong>in</strong>dicators <strong>of</strong> social function<strong>in</strong>g based<br />

on peer nom<strong>in</strong>ations for “cooperative,” “disruptive,” “shy,” <strong>and</strong> “fighter”<br />

were used. In addition to these measures <strong>of</strong> social function<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>dicators <strong>of</strong><br />

behavioral symptomatology also were used <strong>in</strong> the creation <strong>of</strong> the composite<br />

<strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. These <strong>in</strong>dicators <strong>in</strong>cluded the counselor rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> externaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavior problems from the TRF.<br />

Because resilient function<strong>in</strong>g may be manifested <strong>in</strong> different doma<strong>in</strong>s by<br />

different <strong>in</strong>dividuals (Luthar, 1991, 1993; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998;<br />

Masten et al., 1999), we used a person-centered strategy to evaluate the<br />

patterns <strong>of</strong> strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual children across multiple<br />

areas. Thus, <strong>in</strong> order to ga<strong>in</strong> a clearer depiction <strong>of</strong> variation across the<br />

multiple doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, criteria<br />

for high function<strong>in</strong>g were established for each <strong>of</strong> the n<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicators.<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

Specifically, whether children scored <strong>in</strong> the most adaptive third <strong>of</strong> the<br />

distribution was determ<strong>in</strong>ed for each <strong>in</strong>dicator (i.e., <strong>in</strong> the highest third for<br />

counselor-rated prosocial behavior <strong>and</strong> peer-rated “cooperative”; <strong>in</strong> the<br />

lowest third for counselor-rated aggression <strong>and</strong> withdrawal; <strong>in</strong> the lowest<br />

third for peer-rated “shy,” “fighter,” <strong>and</strong> “disruptive”; <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the lowest<br />

third for <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> externaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavior problems).<br />

For each <strong>of</strong> the n<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicators, scores were dichotomized to reflect high<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g (a score <strong>of</strong> 1) or no high function<strong>in</strong>g (a score <strong>of</strong> 0). Summ<strong>in</strong>g<br />

across the n<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicators resulted <strong>in</strong> a total score <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

with a possible range <strong>of</strong> scores from 0 to 9 (M 3.04; SD 2.12). No<br />

child scored highly on all <strong>in</strong>dicators <strong>of</strong> adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., received<br />

a score <strong>of</strong> 9). However, 4 Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong> the nonmaltreated group<br />

achieved a score <strong>of</strong> 8.<br />

The composite <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g was also used to categorize<br />

children <strong>in</strong>to three levels <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g. The range <strong>of</strong> scores from 0 to 8<br />

was divided <strong>in</strong>to three levels. Resilient children were def<strong>in</strong>ed as those<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g high function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> 6–8 areas. These children were one st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

deviation above the mean on the resilience composite. Children with high<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> 2–5 areas were considered <strong>in</strong> the middle range <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

<strong>and</strong> children with scores <strong>of</strong> 0–1 were considered <strong>in</strong> the low range, as<br />

they evidenced either no high function<strong>in</strong>g or high function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> only one<br />

area <strong>and</strong> were one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation below the mean.<br />

Results<br />

Contrasts <strong>of</strong> <strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>and</strong> Comparison Children<br />

In order to exam<strong>in</strong>e the differences between the maltreated <strong>and</strong><br />

nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, an omnibus multivariate analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

variance was first conducted to determ<strong>in</strong>e if there was a significant<br />

multivariate ma<strong>in</strong> effect for maltreatment. The ma<strong>in</strong> effect <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreatment was significant, F(9, 121) 2.24, p .05. Individual<br />

t tests were then performed for each <strong>of</strong> the n<strong>in</strong>e components <strong>of</strong><br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g (see Table 2). <strong>Maltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

exhibited significantly greater aggressive behavior, exhibited significantly<br />

less prosocial behavior, were rated more frequently by<br />

their peers as fighters, had more <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g symptoms, <strong>and</strong> had<br />

more externaliz<strong>in</strong>g symptoms than did nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o comparison<br />

children.<br />

The resilient function<strong>in</strong>g composite, which summarized the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> which children exhibited levels <strong>of</strong> high<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g, also was evaluated. <strong>Maltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children had<br />

significantly fewer areas <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g (2.61 vs. 3.61),<br />

t(131) 2.79, p .01, than did nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Because the children were categorized <strong>in</strong>to three levels <strong>of</strong> adaptive<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g (low, medium, <strong>and</strong> high), the distribution <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children at each <strong>of</strong> these<br />

levels also was evaluated <strong>in</strong> greater detail. Table 3 summarizes the<br />

percentages <strong>of</strong> maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong><br />

each <strong>of</strong> the three levels <strong>of</strong> competent function<strong>in</strong>g. There was a<br />

significant difference between maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, 2 (2, N 133) 6.88, p .05. At the high end<br />

<strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g, there were fewer maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (9.2%)<br />

represented than nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (17.5%). In addition,<br />

9.2% (N 7) <strong>of</strong> the maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children versus 8.8%<br />

(N 5) <strong>of</strong> the nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children achieved a score <strong>of</strong><br />

0, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g no areas <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. When the 30.8% <strong>of</strong><br />

1 In the summer day camp, the PPVT–R was used <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> the<br />

PPVT–III <strong>in</strong> order to rema<strong>in</strong> consistent with data that had been collected<br />

prior to the <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>of</strong> the PPVT–III.


children <strong>in</strong> the low adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g group were compared<br />

with all others, significantly more maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, 2 (1,<br />

N 133) 6.22, p .02, than nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

were <strong>in</strong> the low group.<br />

Predict<strong>in</strong>g Resilient Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

In order to identify contributors to differences <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

among the Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, several process variables were<br />

evaluated as potential predictors <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. Personal<br />

resource variables were assessed <strong>in</strong> these analyses, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g egoresiliency,<br />

ego-control, <strong>and</strong> receptive vocabulary. In addition to the<br />

personal resource variables, aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relations that<br />

could serve to promote resilience among high-risk Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

were evaluated. In particular, the ability <strong>of</strong> a child to form a<br />

positive relationship with the head camp counselor was assessed<br />

<strong>and</strong> considered an <strong>in</strong>dication <strong>of</strong> a child’s capacity to relate successfully<br />

to a new adult figure. Table 4 provides a summary <strong>of</strong><br />

each <strong>of</strong> these variables contrast<strong>in</strong>g maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. 2 A number <strong>of</strong> significant differences were found.<br />

Specifically, maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children ev<strong>in</strong>ced lower levels <strong>of</strong><br />

ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> receptive vocabulary than did nonmaltreated<br />

children. <strong>Maltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children also ev<strong>in</strong>ced marg<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

higher levels <strong>of</strong> ego-undercontrol than did nonmaltreated children.<br />

In terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships, maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

were rated by their counselors as more conflicted than were<br />

nonmaltreated children. Similarly, nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children,<br />

compared with maltreated children, were rated as be<strong>in</strong>g warmer,<br />

were rated as hav<strong>in</strong>g more open communication, <strong>and</strong> received<br />

higher total relationship scores, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that the nonmaltreated<br />

children were better able to achieve positive relationships with<br />

their head counselors.<br />

In order to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the process variables predicted<br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> whether they related differentially to<br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g for maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children,<br />

we conducted a series <strong>of</strong> hierarchical regressions. Gender<br />

was <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the regression analyses to explore whether it<br />

contributed significantly to resilient function<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Ego-Resiliency <strong>and</strong> Ego-Control<br />

In the first hierarchical regression analysis, resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

total scores were regressed onto the predictor variables <strong>of</strong> ego-<br />

Table 2<br />

Comparisons <strong>of</strong> <strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o Children<br />

on N<strong>in</strong>e Indicators <strong>of</strong> Resilient Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Indicator<br />

<strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong><br />

M SD M SD<br />

t (131) p<br />

Prosocial 0.27 0.82 0.26 0.95 3.48 .001<br />

Aggressive 0.16 0.93 0.20 0.80 2.33 .02<br />

Withdrawn 0.07 0.91 0.07 0.90 0.89 ns<br />

Cooperative 0.05 0.94 0.32 0.93 1.64 ns<br />

Disruptive 0.12 0.78 0.36 0.66 1.85 .06<br />

Shy 0.001 0.91 0.19 0.97 1.15 ns<br />

Fighter 0.05 0.88 0.38 0.56 2.61 .01<br />

Internaliz<strong>in</strong>g 53.12 8.08 49.71 8.20 2.39 .02<br />

Externaliz<strong>in</strong>g 57.11 8.79 52.49 8.24 3.08 .003<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong><br />

composite 2.61 1.93 3.61 2.23 2.79 .006<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

Table 3<br />

Levels <strong>of</strong> Resilient Function<strong>in</strong>g by Maltreatment Status<br />

Resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong> Total<br />

n % n % N %<br />

Low (0–1 <strong>in</strong>dicators) 30 39.5 11 19.3 41 30.8<br />

Middle (2–5 <strong>in</strong>dicators) 39 51.3 36 63.2 75 56.4<br />

High ( 6 <strong>in</strong>dicators) 7 9.2 10 17.5 17 12.8<br />

Total 76 57 133 100<br />

343<br />

resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control. In Steps 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 <strong>of</strong> the regression,<br />

maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> gender were entered followed by the ma<strong>in</strong><br />

effects <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control entered simultaneously at<br />

Step 3. Last, the l<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teractions between ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong><br />

maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> between ego-control <strong>and</strong> maltreatment<br />

status were entered simultaneously at Step 4. The unique contributions<br />

to the variance expla<strong>in</strong>ed (reported as sr 2 ) for each predictor<br />

variable are summarized <strong>in</strong> Table 5. The ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreatment status, gender, ego-resiliency, <strong>and</strong> ego-control each<br />

made significant contributions to the prediction <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

with ego-resiliency serv<strong>in</strong>g as the most powerful predictor.<br />

High ego-resiliency scores were associated with high resilience<br />

( .60), <strong>and</strong> high ego-undercontrol scores were associated<br />

with low resilience ( .17). Together these variables resulted<br />

<strong>in</strong> a highly significant regression equation, F(6, 126) 21.17, p <br />

.01, account<strong>in</strong>g for 50.2% <strong>of</strong> the variance <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(adjusted R 2 .48).<br />

The l<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>of</strong> both ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control<br />

with maltreatment status were found to be not significant. Because<br />

the personal resource variable <strong>of</strong> ego-control theoretically has a<br />

curvil<strong>in</strong>ear relation with adaptation <strong>and</strong> maladaptation (i.e., extreme<br />

ego-overcontrol <strong>and</strong> ego-undercontrol are associated with<br />

maladaptation; Kremen & Block, 1998), a separate analysis <strong>of</strong> a<br />

possible quadratic effect was evaluated for the <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> ego-control on resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. In<br />

Steps 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 <strong>of</strong> this analysis, the ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> maltreatment<br />

status <strong>and</strong> ego-control were entered <strong>in</strong>to the regression equation,<br />

respectively, followed by the quadratic term <strong>of</strong> ego-control at Step<br />

3. Next, the product <strong>of</strong> ego-control <strong>and</strong> maltreatment was entered<br />

at Step 4 to evaluate the l<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teraction, followed by the product<br />

<strong>of</strong> maltreatment <strong>and</strong> the quadratic term for ego-control at the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

step, to evaluate the quadratic <strong>in</strong>teraction. The ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong><br />

maltreatment status, ego-control, <strong>and</strong> the quadratic effect <strong>of</strong> egocontrol<br />

were significant, R 2 .21, F(3, 129) 11.46, p .01,<br />

together account<strong>in</strong>g for 21% <strong>of</strong> the variance <strong>in</strong> the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

composite. The l<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>and</strong> curvil<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>of</strong> mal-<br />

2 Twenty-eight children were miss<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship scores<br />

because <strong>in</strong> the early years <strong>of</strong> camp, the STRS measure was not adm<strong>in</strong>istered.<br />

In addition, 27 children were miss<strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>of</strong> receptive vocabulary<br />

because <strong>of</strong> issues such as absence from camp or concerns <strong>of</strong> validity. Thus,<br />

ow<strong>in</strong>g to differences <strong>in</strong> sample size result<strong>in</strong>g from miss<strong>in</strong>g data, <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

relationship variables <strong>and</strong> personal resource variables were evaluated<br />

<strong>in</strong> separate hierarchical regression analyses. Receptive vocabulary, as<br />

measured by the PPVT–R, was not significantly correlated with scores <strong>of</strong><br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g (r .05); thus it was not <strong>in</strong>cluded as a variable <strong>in</strong> the<br />

regression analyses.


344<br />

Table 4<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> <strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o Children on Personal Resource<br />

Characteristics, Receptive Vocabulary, Interpersonal Relationship Factors, <strong>and</strong> Overall<br />

Relationship With Head Camp Counselor<br />

Variable<br />

treatment status <strong>and</strong> ego-control were not significant, thus show<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that regardless <strong>of</strong> maltreatment status, Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who demonstrated<br />

either high ego-overcontrol or high ego-undercontrol<br />

demonstrated less resilient function<strong>in</strong>g than children who had<br />

ego-control scores that were more moderate to slightly overcontrolled.<br />

In other words, for Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, mid-range ego-control<br />

promotes resilient function<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Although there was a significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> gender <strong>in</strong> the<br />

overall regression evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the predictors <strong>of</strong> ego-resilience <strong>and</strong><br />

ego-control, the <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>of</strong> gender with each <strong>of</strong> the personal<br />

resource variables were not significant, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the predictor<br />

variables <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-control did not relate differently<br />

to resilient function<strong>in</strong>g for Lat<strong>in</strong>o girls <strong>and</strong> boys.<br />

Relationship Quality With an Extrafamilial Adult<br />

Two subsequent hierarchical regression analyses were conducted<br />

to evaluate the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables that represent aspects<br />

<strong>Maltreated</strong> <strong>Nonmaltreated</strong><br />

M SD M SD<br />

t df p<br />

Ego-resiliency 0.15 0.30 0.34 0.34 3.40 131 .001<br />

Ego-undercontrol 0.06 0.23 0.01 0.22 1.94 131 .06<br />

Receptive vocabulary (PPVT–R) 81.74 14.53 88.02 16.32 2.10 104 .04<br />

Open communication 2.93 0.85 3.49 0.80 3.47 103 .001<br />

Dependent 1.96 0.64 1.93 0.70 0.18 103 ns<br />

Closed/troubled 2.75 0.66 2.69 0.50 0.52 103 ns<br />

Warm 3.33 0.66 3.67 0.61 2.74 103 .007<br />

Conflicted 2.38 0.82 1.94 0.81 2.78 103 .006<br />

Overall relationship 3.42 0.51 3.77 0.53 3.46 103 .001<br />

Note. PPVT–R Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised.<br />

<strong>of</strong> relationship features (i.e., the STRS overall relationship score<br />

<strong>and</strong> the “conflicted,” “warm,” “open communication,” “dependent,”<br />

<strong>and</strong> “closed/troubled” variables), <strong>in</strong> order to determ<strong>in</strong>e if<br />

they related to the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g composite. The overall<br />

relationship quality was exam<strong>in</strong>ed first; <strong>in</strong>dividual components <strong>of</strong><br />

this relationship were subsequently evaluated.<br />

In the first regression analysis, maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> gender<br />

were entered first, followed by the overall quality <strong>of</strong> the relationship<br />

with the head counselor. Significant ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> maltreatment<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> the overall relationship with the head counselor were<br />

found (see Table 6). There was no significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect <strong>of</strong><br />

gender. The <strong>in</strong>teraction between maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> the overall<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> the relationship with the head counselor was also<br />

nonsignificant. Thus, for Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, regardless <strong>of</strong> maltreatment<br />

status, the overall quality <strong>of</strong> the relationship with the head<br />

counselor significantly predicted higher resilient function<strong>in</strong>g, contribut<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to 50.6% <strong>of</strong> the variance <strong>in</strong> the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g score,<br />

R 2 .51, F(1, 101) 124.20, p .01, .76.<br />

Table 5<br />

Influences <strong>of</strong> Maltreatment, Gender, <strong>and</strong> Personal Resource Variables on Level <strong>of</strong> Resilient<br />

Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Step F df R 2<br />

Adjusted R 2<br />

Step 1 7.79** 1, 131 .06 .05<br />

Maltreatment status .24 .06**<br />

Step 2 6.07** 2, 130 .03 .07<br />

Maltreatment status .22 .05*<br />

Gender .17 .03*<br />

Step 3 31.56** 4, 128 .41 .48<br />

Maltreatment status .02 .00<br />

Gender .15 .02*<br />

Ego-resiliency .60 .31**<br />

Ego-control .17 .02*<br />

Step 4 21.17** 6, 126 .01 .48<br />

Maltreatment status .01 .00<br />

Gender .14 .02*<br />

Ego-resiliency (ER) .61 .31**<br />

Ego-control (EC) .16 .02*<br />

Maltreatment Status ER .06 .00<br />

Maltreatment Status EC .07 .00<br />

* p .05. ** p .01.<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

sr 2


Table 6<br />

Influences <strong>of</strong> Maltreatment, Gender, the Overall Counselor Relationship Variable, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Interpersonal Relationship Variables on Level <strong>of</strong> Resilient Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Step F df R 2<br />

In the second hierarchical regression analysis, the <strong>in</strong>fluences <strong>of</strong><br />

the <strong>in</strong>dividual features <strong>of</strong> the relationship with the head counselor<br />

were exam<strong>in</strong>ed. Maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> gender were entered at<br />

Steps 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 <strong>of</strong> the regression equation, followed by the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

variables, which were simultaneously entered <strong>in</strong> the third<br />

step <strong>of</strong> the analysis (see Table 6). Maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables <strong>of</strong> “conflicted,” “warm,” <strong>and</strong> “closed/troubled”<br />

uniquely contributed to the variance <strong>in</strong> the total resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g score. Together, maltreatment status, gender, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables resulted <strong>in</strong> a highly significant regression<br />

equation, F(7, 97) 22.5, p .01, account<strong>in</strong>g for 61.9% <strong>of</strong> the<br />

variance <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g (adjusted R 2 .59). The <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

variables uniquely accounted for 53.5% <strong>of</strong> the variance <strong>in</strong><br />

the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g score, R 2 .54, F(5, 97) 27.25, p <br />

.01, which is similar to the amount <strong>of</strong> variance accounted for by<br />

the overall quality <strong>of</strong> the relationship with the head counselor.<br />

Next, separate regression analyses were conducted to explore<br />

whether any <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship variables related to<br />

the composite <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g differentially for maltreated<br />

<strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. To do so, we evaluated possible<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction effects between maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

variables. In each <strong>of</strong> these analyses, maltreatment status<br />

was entered first, followed by one <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables,<br />

<strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ally by the <strong>in</strong>teraction between maltreatment <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

variable. No significant <strong>in</strong>teractions were found for the<br />

“warm,” “dependent,” <strong>and</strong> “conflicted” <strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables. In<br />

contrast, the <strong>in</strong>teraction between maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> the<br />

“closed/troubled” variable was found to be significant, account<strong>in</strong>g<br />

for an additional 2.6% <strong>of</strong> the variance <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

scores, R 2 .03, F(1, 101) 3.76, p .05. As shown <strong>in</strong> Figure<br />

1, the negative relation between closed/troubled relationship features<br />

<strong>and</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g was stronger for nonmaltreated<br />

children ( .54) than maltreated children ( .40).<br />

The <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong> maltreatment status <strong>and</strong> open communication<br />

was also found to be significant, account<strong>in</strong>g for 3.1% <strong>of</strong> the<br />

variance <strong>in</strong> the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g scores, R 2 .03, F(1,<br />

101) 4.37, p .05. As shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 2, the positive relation<br />

Adjusted R 2<br />

Step 1 6.32* 1, 103 .06 .05<br />

Maltreatment status .24 .06*<br />

Step 2 4.64* 2, 102 .03 .07<br />

Maltreatment status .21 .04*<br />

Gender .16 .03<br />

Step 3a 48.22** 3, 101 .51 .58<br />

Maltreatment status .01 .00<br />

Gender .04 .00<br />

Overall counselor relationship .76 .51**<br />

Step 3b 22.49** 7, 97 .54 .59<br />

Maltreatment status .01 .00<br />

Gender .01 .00<br />

Conflicted .43 .07**<br />

Warm .26 .02*<br />

Open communication .12 .01<br />

Dependent .17 .01<br />

Closed/troubled .18 .02*<br />

* p .05. ** p .01.<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

between open communication <strong>and</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g was stronger<br />

for nonmaltreated ( .58) than maltreated ( .30) Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children.<br />

Discussion<br />

This <strong>in</strong>vestigation was designed to ga<strong>in</strong> an underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the<br />

impact <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment on Lat<strong>in</strong>o children as well as to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e the processes contribut<strong>in</strong>g to resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children at high risk for adverse outcomes. Consistent with<br />

our first hypothesis, maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children ev<strong>in</strong>ced more<br />

difficulty across multiple aspects <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g than their equally<br />

socioeconomically disadvantaged nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o peers.<br />

Even though both maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

experienced multiple adversities that placed them at high risk for<br />

maladaptive function<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent homes with<br />

multiple children, low SES, dependency on the state for f<strong>in</strong>ancial<br />

support, <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority status), maltreated children ev<strong>in</strong>ced more<br />

difficulties <strong>in</strong> several important areas <strong>of</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g. In accord<br />

with prior <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong> samples <strong>of</strong> maltreated<br />

children that have been composed predom<strong>in</strong>antly <strong>of</strong> African<br />

American <strong>and</strong> European American children (Cicchetti & Rogosch,<br />

1997; Cicchetti et al., 1993), maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were rated<br />

by both peers <strong>and</strong> counselors as hav<strong>in</strong>g more difficulties <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

relations (e.g., exhibit<strong>in</strong>g greater aggressive behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

less prosocial behavior <strong>and</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g more likely to be rated as<br />

fighters) than nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, <strong>and</strong> they also displayed<br />

more symptoms <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> externaliz<strong>in</strong>g behavior<br />

problems than nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Furthermore, consistent with other studies <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>and</strong><br />

maltreatment (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Cicchetti et al., 1993;<br />

McGlo<strong>in</strong> & Widom, 2001; Moran & Eckenrode, 1992), maltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children demonstrated a lower level <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

than did equally disadvantaged nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Moreover, several Lat<strong>in</strong>o children ev<strong>in</strong>ced a paucity <strong>of</strong> resilient<br />

striv<strong>in</strong>gs (39.5% maltreated vs. 19.3% nonmaltreated), highlight<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the fact that <strong>in</strong> addition to ethnicity, SES, <strong>and</strong> other high-risk<br />

sr 2<br />

345


346<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

Figure 1. Interaction <strong>of</strong> maltreatment <strong>and</strong> closed/troubled relationship features.<br />

Figure 2. Interaction <strong>of</strong> maltreatment <strong>and</strong> open communication.


environmental factors, child maltreatment promotes maladaptive<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. In this sample, twice as many<br />

maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children as nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g maladaptively. Because resilience was determ<strong>in</strong>ed by<br />

scores <strong>of</strong> adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g only at one po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time, future<br />

studies should focus on evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Lat<strong>in</strong>o children’s resilience<br />

longitud<strong>in</strong>ally.<br />

In terms <strong>of</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g factors that would expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g among both maltreated <strong>and</strong><br />

nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, analyses revealed that maltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were rated as significantly more conflicted <strong>in</strong><br />

relationships with an adult counselor than were nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>and</strong> ev<strong>in</strong>ced higher levels <strong>of</strong> ego-undercontrol;<br />

conversely, nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children evidenced higher egoresiliency<br />

<strong>and</strong> higher receptive vocabulary <strong>and</strong> were rated as<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g more open communication, more warmth, <strong>and</strong> demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a more positive relationship with their head camp counselor.<br />

Thus, the nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children displayed higher levels <strong>of</strong><br />

the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship factors previously associated with<br />

higher resilient function<strong>in</strong>g than did the maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

(Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997).<br />

For both maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, analyses<br />

<strong>of</strong> personal resource variables revealed that higher ego-resiliency<br />

<strong>and</strong> moderate ego-overcontrol were associated with higher resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g. Be<strong>in</strong>g female also was associated with higher<br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. Contrary to the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> previous literature<br />

(Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997), the effects <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong><br />

ego-control did not relate differentially <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g resilience for<br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children; thus, only partial<br />

support for our second hypothesis was found. Ego-resiliency was<br />

found to be the strongest predictor <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g for this<br />

sample, <strong>and</strong> consequently, the child’s ability to modify his or her<br />

own characteristic level <strong>of</strong> ego-control, <strong>in</strong> order to accommodate<br />

<strong>and</strong> susta<strong>in</strong> possible stressors <strong>in</strong> the environment as they arise, was<br />

an adaptive resource (J. Block, 1982). For the children <strong>in</strong> this<br />

sample, high ego-resiliency may have helped them to overcome<br />

the many environmental hardships they experienced <strong>and</strong>, thus,<br />

fostered resilient function<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The significant quadratic ma<strong>in</strong> effect for ego-control found <strong>in</strong><br />

this study suggests that there is an optimal amount <strong>of</strong> ego-control<br />

that both maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children should<br />

exhibit for higher resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. For Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g was found to decrease significantly as scores <strong>of</strong><br />

ego-undercontrol <strong>in</strong>creased; that is, Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who had difficulty<br />

regulat<strong>in</strong>g behavioral, affective, <strong>and</strong> cognitive expressions<br />

<strong>of</strong> impulse had worse outcomes. Likewise, Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who<br />

were extremely overcontrolled, <strong>and</strong> excessively <strong>in</strong>hibited <strong>in</strong> emotional<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavioral expressiveness, also had fewer areas <strong>of</strong><br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g. For the Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong> this sample, a<br />

moderate amount <strong>of</strong> ego-overcontrol was found to be the most<br />

adaptive. Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are similar to those <strong>of</strong> prior <strong>in</strong>vestigations<br />

<strong>in</strong> show<strong>in</strong>g that demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g more ego-overcontrol, or the ability<br />

to adopt more reserved <strong>and</strong> controlled ways <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

others, may help maltreated children function successfully <strong>and</strong><br />

cope with their adverse home environments (Cicchetti & Rogosch,<br />

1997; Cicchetti et al., 1993). However, <strong>in</strong> this study, both maltreated<br />

<strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were found to benefit<br />

from moderate ego-overcontrol; thus, the ability to exhibit greater<br />

ego-control may also be important for nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Previous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have suggested that <strong>in</strong> other samples <strong>of</strong><br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

347<br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated children, ego-overcontrol has been<br />

an important predictor <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong> maltreated children only<br />

(Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Cicchetti et al., 1993). Because<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children may be at an even greater risk for maladaptive<br />

outcomes than children <strong>of</strong> more ma<strong>in</strong>stream cultures, there may be<br />

a greater tendency for both resilient maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children to exhibit moderate amounts <strong>of</strong> behavioral <strong>and</strong><br />

emotional expressiveness. As described by J. H. Block <strong>and</strong> Block<br />

(1980), some Q-sort items that characterize features <strong>of</strong> children<br />

with a resilient overcontroll<strong>in</strong>g type <strong>of</strong> personality organization<br />

<strong>in</strong>clude “compliant,” “calm/relaxed,” <strong>and</strong> “empathic.” Similarly,<br />

<strong>in</strong> the Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture, characteristics such as respect <strong>and</strong> control <strong>of</strong><br />

emotions (de Rios, 2001; Durrett, O’Bryant, & Pennebaker, 1975;<br />

Simoni & Perez, 1995) are <strong>of</strong>ten valued by many Lat<strong>in</strong>o parents.<br />

Thus, certa<strong>in</strong> value systems may <strong>in</strong>culcate modulation <strong>of</strong> expressiveness<br />

through expectations for respect <strong>and</strong> foster<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> restra<strong>in</strong>t,<br />

<strong>and</strong> these features appear to be valuable for promot<strong>in</strong>g resilience <strong>in</strong><br />

nonmaltreated as well as maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

In addition, <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship factors that<br />

predict resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, the ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong><br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g a positive relationship with the head camp counselor <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

be<strong>in</strong>g rated as less conflicted, warmer, <strong>and</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g less closed/<br />

troubled were also significant <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g adaptive function<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The fact that these <strong>in</strong>terpersonal variables were significantly associated<br />

with higher function<strong>in</strong>g supports the notion that <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

factors play a role <strong>in</strong> overcom<strong>in</strong>g environmental hardships.<br />

However, not all <strong>of</strong> these factors were found to relate to resilient<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g differentially for maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children. As <strong>in</strong>dicated above, the <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>of</strong> maltreatment<br />

status with the relationship variables <strong>of</strong> “closed/troubled” <strong>and</strong><br />

“open communication” were significant, lend<strong>in</strong>g partial support to<br />

our second <strong>and</strong> third hypotheses. Analyses revealed that, as <strong>in</strong><br />

previous <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>of</strong> resilience (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997),<br />

the relations between these <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship features <strong>and</strong><br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g were stronger for nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

That is, although relationship features do promote higher<br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children than <strong>in</strong> nonmaltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, the deleterious effects <strong>of</strong> maltreatment<br />

may lessen the relative effectiveness <strong>of</strong> relationship features <strong>in</strong><br />

promot<strong>in</strong>g positive outcomes. <strong>Maltreated</strong> children may not be able<br />

to make use <strong>of</strong> relationships with adults <strong>in</strong> order to succeed <strong>and</strong><br />

surmount environmental hardships as well as nonmaltreated children<br />

may. Thus, for maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, <strong>in</strong>ternal strengths<br />

such as ego-resiliency <strong>and</strong> ego-overcontrol may be more essential<br />

than relationship features <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g resilience.<br />

In terms <strong>of</strong> gender differences, be<strong>in</strong>g female was associated with<br />

higher levels <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. However,<br />

none <strong>of</strong> the l<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>of</strong> gender with any <strong>of</strong> the predictor<br />

variables was significant, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the predictors <strong>of</strong> resilience<br />

did not differ for Lat<strong>in</strong>o girls <strong>and</strong> boys. Caution should be<br />

taken when <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the role <strong>of</strong> gender <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g resilience<br />

because there were small numbers <strong>of</strong> girls <strong>in</strong> both the maltreated<br />

group <strong>and</strong> the nonmaltreated comparison group. Thus, we recommend<br />

that future <strong>in</strong>vestigators not only <strong>in</strong>crease their sample size<br />

to <strong>in</strong>clude more Lat<strong>in</strong>a girls but also that they cont<strong>in</strong>ue to explore<br />

possible gender differences that may exist <strong>in</strong> the development <strong>of</strong><br />

resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. Because strong gender<br />

roles <strong>and</strong> cultural ideals such as machismo <strong>and</strong> marianismo exist <strong>in</strong><br />

the Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture, they may promote differences <strong>in</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

between Lat<strong>in</strong>o girls <strong>and</strong> boys. For Lat<strong>in</strong>o boys grow<strong>in</strong>g up


348<br />

<strong>in</strong> an environment that adheres to ideals <strong>of</strong> machismo, maltreatment<br />

experiences may promote feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> shame <strong>and</strong> frustration<br />

around not be<strong>in</strong>g able to defend oneself or one’s family, thus<br />

lead<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>creased aggression <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased externaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g behaviors. Likewise, many Lat<strong>in</strong>a girls who are socialized<br />

to be self-sacrific<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> obedient may develop the ability<br />

to better cope with environmental adversities <strong>and</strong> avoid conflict.<br />

Because it rema<strong>in</strong>s unclear, for example, how certa<strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

traditions, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> attitudes may promote resilient function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> both maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, future studies<br />

need to <strong>in</strong>corporate models <strong>of</strong> development that <strong>in</strong>vestigate the<br />

importance <strong>of</strong> culture <strong>and</strong> its role <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g resilience. Future<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigations should use an emic (culture-specific) (García Coll,<br />

Akerman, & Cicchetti, 2000; Shweder et al., 1998) approach by<br />

obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g measures that target specific cultural factors, <strong>in</strong> order to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e patterns <strong>of</strong> adaptation <strong>and</strong> maladaptation, <strong>and</strong> predictors<br />

<strong>of</strong> resilience with<strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture. One such paradigm, proposed<br />

by García Coll et al. (1996), recognizes the importance <strong>of</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

theory that would take <strong>in</strong>to account many <strong>of</strong> the cultural<br />

characteristics unique to m<strong>in</strong>ority cultures. This perspective has<br />

been lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> most exist<strong>in</strong>g models <strong>of</strong> child development. García<br />

Coll <strong>and</strong> her colleagues proposed a social stratification model for<br />

study<strong>in</strong>g developmental pathways <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority children. Several<br />

constructs proposed <strong>in</strong> this model were <strong>of</strong> particular relevance to<br />

this study <strong>and</strong> may have contributed to how developmental processes<br />

operated to promote resilience <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. For<br />

example, social position variables (e.g., race, social class, ethnicity,<br />

gender), family (e.g., structure <strong>and</strong> roles, family values), <strong>and</strong><br />

developmental competencies (e.g., cognitive, social, emotional)<br />

may differentially <strong>in</strong>fluence what factors facilitate resilience. Future<br />

research on resilience should be guided by models sensitive to<br />

the effects <strong>of</strong> culture on developmental processes <strong>and</strong> to culture’s<br />

unique contributions to how successful adaptation is achieved <strong>in</strong><br />

the face <strong>of</strong> adversity.<br />

Despite important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> this study, several limitations must<br />

be addressed. First, contrary to past research <strong>in</strong> child maltreatment,<br />

receptive vocabulary, which is <strong>in</strong>directly related to <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g, was not found to be a predictor <strong>of</strong> resilience for either<br />

maltreated or nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>vestigation.<br />

This is not to suggest that for Lat<strong>in</strong>o children receptive vocabulary<br />

is not related to resilient outcomes. Instead, one explanation may<br />

be that the PPVT–R may not have been a valid measure <strong>of</strong><br />

receptive vocabulary for Lat<strong>in</strong>o children because <strong>of</strong> language<br />

differences <strong>and</strong> acculturation issues. In this study, scores for both<br />

maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children were generally below<br />

average (84% achieved a score <strong>of</strong> less than 100). This limited<br />

range <strong>of</strong> scores may provide an explanation as to why the PPVT–R<br />

was not related to the resilient function<strong>in</strong>g composite. However, it<br />

is important to note that the PPVT–R scores for the Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

<strong>in</strong> this sample were comparable to those <strong>of</strong> the other children<br />

attend<strong>in</strong>g the summer day camp. Therefore, the low scores may not<br />

be due to cultural factors but <strong>in</strong>stead may be more associated with<br />

the demographic characteristics <strong>of</strong> this group. Thus, future <strong>in</strong>vestigations<br />

should use measures <strong>of</strong> receptive vocabulary <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g that have proven validity for Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

populations.<br />

Second, measures <strong>of</strong> acculturation were not obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> this<br />

study, nor were specific Lat<strong>in</strong>o subgroups identified (e.g., Puerto<br />

Rican, Mexican American, Cuban, Dom<strong>in</strong>ican). Because there are<br />

many different traditions <strong>and</strong> value systems with<strong>in</strong> the Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

culture, it is important to measure acculturation <strong>and</strong> to recognize<br />

the diversity with<strong>in</strong> specific groups <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>os <strong>in</strong> future <strong>in</strong>vestigations.<br />

Third, because <strong>of</strong> the sample size <strong>and</strong> the high comorbidity<br />

<strong>of</strong> maltreatment subtypes, the potential differential effects <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

neglect, emotional maltreatment, physical abuse, <strong>and</strong> sexual<br />

abuse among Lat<strong>in</strong>o children could not be rigorously <strong>in</strong>vestigated.<br />

Future <strong>in</strong>vestigations should use a much larger sample <strong>of</strong> maltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>in</strong> order to perform further subtype analyses.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, self-system processes were not evaluated <strong>in</strong> this study.<br />

Processes such as self-esteem, self-confidence, self-reliance, <strong>and</strong><br />

social self-efficacy have been <strong>in</strong>dicated as protective factors <strong>in</strong><br />

reduc<strong>in</strong>g behavior problems among maltreated children (Cicchetti<br />

& Rogosch, 1997; Kim & Cicchetti, 2003) <strong>and</strong> may predict resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> maltreated children. In other m<strong>in</strong>ority cultures, it has been<br />

found that the presence <strong>of</strong> a positive ethnic identity is associated<br />

with the development <strong>of</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tegrated self-system <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

self-esteem (Spencer, Dupree, & Hartmann, 1997; Spencer, Fegley,<br />

& Harpalani, 2003). For Lat<strong>in</strong>o children who are able to<br />

establish a positive ethnic identity, these self-system processes<br />

may be <strong>of</strong> particular importance. Thus, future studies need to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e the <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> such processes on adaptive <strong>and</strong> maladaptive<br />

development <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Def<strong>in</strong>itive <strong>in</strong>tervention implications await additional research on<br />

pathways to adaptive <strong>and</strong> maladaptive function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Because certa<strong>in</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> relationship features differed as<br />

predictors <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> nonmaltreated <strong>and</strong> maltreated<br />

Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, one cl<strong>in</strong>ical implication that should be considered<br />

is the development <strong>and</strong> foster<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> relationships with persons<br />

outside <strong>of</strong> the nuclear family for maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. Relationship<br />

features may not have been as effective <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g<br />

resilience <strong>in</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children because, unlike many<br />

nonmaltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children, maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children may not<br />

have experienced a positive relationship with<strong>in</strong> their families <strong>and</strong><br />

may f<strong>in</strong>d it more difficult to relate to, or to rely on, others. Thus,<br />

future work with this population should focus on promot<strong>in</strong>g resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children through the development <strong>of</strong><br />

supportive relationships with adults <strong>and</strong> peers outside <strong>of</strong> the immediate<br />

family system. F<strong>in</strong>ally, because ego-resiliency was found<br />

to be the strongest predictor <strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> high-risk Lat<strong>in</strong>o<br />

children, <strong>and</strong> may be essential to the development <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong><br />

maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children (J. Block, 1993), future <strong>in</strong>terventions<br />

should focus on how to enhance the development <strong>of</strong> ego-resiliency <strong>in</strong><br />

both nonmaltreated <strong>and</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children.<br />

Although the Lat<strong>in</strong>o population is rapidly <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g, our current<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> pathways to resilience <strong>in</strong> high-risk Lat<strong>in</strong>o children<br />

has not shown commensurate progress. In fact, this <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

is among the first to advance our knowledge <strong>of</strong> the deleterious<br />

effects <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment on Lat<strong>in</strong>o children <strong>and</strong> to<br />

elucidate some <strong>of</strong> the predictors <strong>of</strong> resilient adaptation <strong>in</strong> these<br />

youngsters. The important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs emanat<strong>in</strong>g from this <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

highlight the need to conduct additional work on the predictors<br />

<strong>of</strong> resilient function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> maltreated Lat<strong>in</strong>o children. As our<br />

knowledge base exp<strong>and</strong>s, our ability to design <strong>and</strong> implement the<br />

preventive <strong>in</strong>terventions necessary to promote resilience <strong>in</strong> this<br />

high-risk population <strong>of</strong> children will be greatly enhanced.<br />

References<br />

Aber, J. L., & Allen, J. P. (1987). The effects <strong>of</strong> maltreatment on young<br />

children’s socioemotional development: An attachment theory perspective.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 23, 406–414.


Achenbach, T. (1991a). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist <strong>and</strong> 1991<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burl<strong>in</strong>gton: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.<br />

Achenbach, T. (1991b). Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form <strong>and</strong> 1991<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Burl<strong>in</strong>gton: University <strong>of</strong> Vermont, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.<br />

Arcia, E., Keyes, L., & Gallagher, J. J. (1994). Indicators <strong>of</strong> developmental<br />

<strong>and</strong> functional status <strong>of</strong> Mexican American <strong>and</strong> Puerto Rican children.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 5, 27–33.<br />

Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g child maltreatment:<br />

The <strong>in</strong>terface between policy <strong>and</strong> research. In D. Cicchetti & S. L.<br />

Toth (Eds.), Child abuse, child development, <strong>and</strong> social policy (pp.<br />

7–74). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

Behl, L. E., Crouch, J. L., May, P. F., Valente, A. L., & Conyngham, H. A.<br />

(2001). Ethnicity <strong>in</strong> child maltreatment research: A content analysis.<br />

Child Maltreatment, 6, 143–147.<br />

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account <strong>of</strong> sex<br />

typ<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.<br />

Block, J. (1982). Assimilation, accommodation, <strong>and</strong> the dynamics <strong>of</strong><br />

personality development. Child Development, 53, 281–295.<br />

Block, J. (1993). Study<strong>in</strong>g personality the long way. In D. Funder, R.<br />

Parke, C. Toml<strong>in</strong>son-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Study<strong>in</strong>g lives<br />

through time: Personality <strong>and</strong> development (pp. 3–41). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton,<br />

DC: American Psychological Association.<br />

Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1969). The California Child Q-Set. Berkeley:<br />

University <strong>of</strong> California, Institute <strong>of</strong> Human Development.<br />

Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role <strong>of</strong> ego-control <strong>and</strong> ego-resiliency<br />

<strong>in</strong> the organization <strong>of</strong> behavior. In W. A. Coll<strong>in</strong>s (Ed.), M<strong>in</strong>nesota<br />

Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 13. Development <strong>of</strong> cognition,<br />

affect, <strong>and</strong> social relations (pp. 39–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., & Aber, J. L. (Eds.). (1997). Neighborhood<br />

poverty: Policy implications <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g neighborhoods (Vol. 2).<br />

New York: Russell Sage Foundation.<br />

Can<strong>in</strong>o, I. A., Gould, M. S., Prupris, S., & Shaffer, D. (1986). A comparison<br />

<strong>of</strong> symptoms <strong>and</strong> diagnosis <strong>in</strong> Hispanic <strong>and</strong> Black children <strong>in</strong> an<br />

outpatient mental health cl<strong>in</strong>ic. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Child Psychology, 25, 254–259.<br />

Centers for Disease Control <strong>and</strong> Prevention (1999). Youth risk behavior<br />

surveillance: United States. Retrieved May 2, 2002, from http://www<br />

.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrthml/ss4905a.1htm<br />

Cicchetti, D. (2002). The impact <strong>of</strong> social experience on neurobiological<br />

systems: Illustration from a constructivist view <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment.<br />

Cognitive Development, 17, 1407–1428.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional<br />

model <strong>of</strong> community violence <strong>and</strong> child maltreatment: Consequences for<br />

children’s development. Psychiatry, 56, 96–117.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1995). Failures <strong>in</strong> the expectable environment<br />

<strong>and</strong> their impact on <strong>in</strong>dividual development: The case <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment.<br />

In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology:<br />

Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, <strong>and</strong> adaptation (pp. 32–71). New York:<br />

Wiley.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role <strong>of</strong> self-organization <strong>in</strong> the<br />

promotion <strong>of</strong> resilience <strong>in</strong> maltreated children. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

9, 797–815.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001a). Diverse patterns <strong>of</strong> neuroendocr<strong>in</strong>e<br />

activity <strong>in</strong> maltreated children. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

13, 677–693.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001b). The impact <strong>of</strong> child maltreatment<br />

<strong>and</strong> psychopathology on neuroendocr<strong>in</strong>e function<strong>in</strong>g. Development <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychopathology, 13, 783–804.<br />

Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Lynch, M., & Holt, K. D. (1993). <strong>Resilience</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong> maltreated children: Processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to an adaptive outcome. Development<br />

<strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5, 629–647.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental perspective on child<br />

abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect. Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> Child & Adolescent<br />

Psychiatry, 34, 541–565.<br />

Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). Perspectives on research <strong>and</strong> practice<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

349<br />

<strong>in</strong> developmental psychopathology. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A.<br />

Renn<strong>in</strong>ger (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology<br />

<strong>in</strong> practice (pp. 479–583). New York: Wiley.<br />

Cohen, J. A., Debl<strong>in</strong>ger, E., Mannar<strong>in</strong>o, A. P., & de Arellano, M. A.<br />

(2001). The importance <strong>of</strong> culture <strong>in</strong> treat<strong>in</strong>g abused <strong>and</strong> neglected<br />

children: An empirical review. Child Maltreatment, 6, 148–157.<br />

Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Cont<strong>in</strong>uities <strong>and</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> children’s<br />

social status: A five-year longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,<br />

29, 261–282.<br />

Curtis, J. W., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Mov<strong>in</strong>g research on resilience <strong>in</strong>to<br />

the 21st century: Theoretical <strong>and</strong> methodological considerations <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

the biological contributors to resilience. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

15, 773–810.<br />

Deater-Deckard, K. (2001). Annotation: Recent research exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the<br />

role <strong>of</strong> peer relationships <strong>in</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> psychopathology. Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> Allied Discipl<strong>in</strong>es, 42,<br />

565–579.<br />

DeBellis, M. D., Baum, A. S., Birmaher, B., Keshaven, M. S., Eccard,<br />

C. H., Bor<strong>in</strong>g, A., et al. (1999). Developmental traumatology. Part I:<br />

Biological stress systems. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1259–1270.<br />

DeBellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Casey, B. J., Clark, D. B., Giedd, J.,<br />

Bor<strong>in</strong>g, A. M., et al. (1999). Developmental traumatology: Biological<br />

stress systems <strong>and</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> development <strong>in</strong> maltreated children with PTSD.<br />

Part II: The relationship between characteristics <strong>of</strong> trauma <strong>and</strong> psychiatric<br />

symptoms <strong>and</strong> adverse bra<strong>in</strong> development <strong>in</strong> maltreated children<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents with PTSD. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1271–1284.<br />

de Rios, M. D. (2001). Brief psychotherapy with the Lat<strong>in</strong>o immigrant<br />

client. B<strong>in</strong>ghamton, NY: Haworth Press.<br />

Dihn, K. T., Roosa, M. W., Te<strong>in</strong>, J. Y., & Lopez, V. A. (2002). The<br />

relationship between acculturation <strong>and</strong> problem behavior proneness <strong>in</strong> a<br />

Hispanic youth sample: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al mediation model. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 295–309.<br />

Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (Eds.). (1997). Consequences <strong>of</strong> grow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.<br />

Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic<br />

deprivation <strong>and</strong> early childhood development. Child Development, 65,<br />

296–318.<br />

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—<br />

Revised manual. Circle P<strong>in</strong>es, MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Durrett, M. E., O’Bryant, S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1975). Child-rear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

reports <strong>of</strong> White, Black, <strong>and</strong> Mexican-American families. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 2, 871.<br />

Eckenrode, J., Laird, M., & Doris, J. (1993). School performance <strong>and</strong><br />

discipl<strong>in</strong>ary problems among abused <strong>and</strong> neglected children. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 29, 53–62.<br />

Egel<strong>and</strong>, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, A. L. (1993). <strong>Resilience</strong> as process.<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology 5, 517–528.<br />

Emery, R. E., & O’Leary, K. D. (1982). Children’s perceptions <strong>of</strong> marital<br />

discord <strong>and</strong> behavior problems <strong>of</strong> boys <strong>and</strong> girls. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<br />

Child Psychology, 10, 11–24.<br />

Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, J. L. (2001). <strong>Resilience</strong> to childhood<br />

adversity. In S. S. Luthar (Ed.), <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>and</strong> vulnerability: Adaptation<br />

<strong>in</strong> the context <strong>of</strong> childhood adversities (pp. 130–155). New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Fisher, C. B., Jackson, J. F., & Villarruel, F. A. (1998). The study <strong>of</strong><br />

African American <strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong> American children <strong>and</strong> youth. In W. Damon<br />

& R. M. Lerner (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical<br />

models <strong>of</strong> human development (pp. 1145–1207). New York: Wiley.<br />

Flores, G., Fuentes-Afflick, E., Barbot, O., Carter-Pokras, O., Claudio, L.,<br />

Lara, M., et al. (2002). The health <strong>of</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o children: Unanswered<br />

priorities, unanswered questions, <strong>and</strong> a research agenda. Journal <strong>of</strong> the<br />

American Medical Association, 288, 82–90.<br />

García Coll, C., Akerman, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences<br />

on developmental processes <strong>and</strong> outcomes: Implications for the study <strong>of</strong>


350<br />

development <strong>and</strong> psychopathology. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

12, 333–356.<br />

García Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, R., McAdoo, H. P., Crnic, K.,<br />

Wasik, B. H., & Vasquez García, H. (1996). An <strong>in</strong>tegrative model for the<br />

study <strong>of</strong> developmental competencies <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority children. Child Development,<br />

67, 1891–1914.<br />

Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study <strong>of</strong> stress <strong>and</strong><br />

competence <strong>in</strong> children: A build<strong>in</strong>g block for developmental psychopathology.<br />

Child Development, 55, 97–111.<br />

Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., P<strong>in</strong>e, C. J., Chan, E. Q., & Buriel, R.<br />

(1990). Family ecologies <strong>of</strong> ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority children. Child Development,<br />

61, 347–362.<br />

Herrenkohl, E. C., Herrenkohl, R., & Egolf, M. (1994). Resilient early<br />

school-age children from maltreat<strong>in</strong>g homes: Outcomes <strong>in</strong> late adolescence.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry, 64, 301–309.<br />

Hether<strong>in</strong>gton, E. M. (1989). Cop<strong>in</strong>g with family transitions: W<strong>in</strong>ners,<br />

losers, <strong>and</strong> survivors. Child Development, 60, 1–14.<br />

Huston, A. C., McLoyd, V. C., & García Coll, C. (1997). Poverty <strong>and</strong><br />

behavior: The case for multiple levels <strong>of</strong> analysis. Developmental Review,<br />

17, 376–393.<br />

Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth <strong>in</strong><br />

high-risk sett<strong>in</strong>gs. American Psychologist, 48, 117–126.<br />

Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Social self-efficacy <strong>and</strong> behavior problems<br />

<strong>in</strong> maltreated <strong>and</strong> nonmaltreated children. Journal <strong>of</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Child <strong>and</strong><br />

Adolescent Psychology, 32, 106–117.<br />

Kremen, A. M., & Block, J. (1998). The roots <strong>of</strong> ego-control <strong>in</strong> young<br />

adulthood: L<strong>in</strong>ks with parent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> early childhood. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 75, 1062–1075.<br />

Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability <strong>and</strong> resilience: A study <strong>of</strong> high-risk<br />

adolescents. Child Development, 62, 600–616.<br />

Luthar, S. S. (1993). Annotation: Methodological <strong>and</strong> conceptual issues <strong>in</strong><br />

the study <strong>of</strong> resilience. Journal <strong>of</strong> Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry <strong>and</strong><br />

Allied Discipl<strong>in</strong>es, 34, 441–453.<br />

Luthar, S. S. (1999). Poverty <strong>and</strong> children’s adjustment. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks,<br />

CA: Sage.<br />

Luthar, S. S. (Ed.). (2003). <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>and</strong> vulnerability. New York: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct <strong>of</strong> resilience:<br />

A critical evaluation <strong>and</strong> guidel<strong>in</strong>es for future work. Child Development,<br />

71, 543–562.<br />

Mal<strong>in</strong>owsky-Rummel, R., & Hansen, D. J. (1993). Long-term consequences<br />

<strong>of</strong> childhood physical abuse. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 114,<br />

68–79.<br />

Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions<br />

<strong>of</strong> child maltreatment <strong>and</strong> children’s adjustment: Contributions <strong>of</strong><br />

developmental tim<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> subtype. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

13, 759–782.<br />

Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development <strong>of</strong> competence<br />

<strong>in</strong> favorable <strong>and</strong> unfavorable environments: Lessons from research<br />

on successful children. American Psychologist, 53, 205–220.<br />

Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N., &<br />

Ramirez, M. (1999). Competence <strong>in</strong> the context <strong>of</strong> adversity: Pathways<br />

to resilience <strong>and</strong> maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence.<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 11, 143–169.<br />

McGlo<strong>in</strong>, J. M., & Widom, C. S. (2001). <strong>Resilience</strong> among abused <strong>and</strong><br />

neglected children grown up. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 13,<br />

1021–1038.<br />

McLoyd, V. C. (1998a). Children <strong>in</strong> poverty: Development, public policy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> practice. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A. Renn<strong>in</strong>ger (Eds.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology <strong>in</strong> practice (pp.<br />

135–208). New York: Wiley.<br />

McLoyd, V. C. (1998b). Socioeconomic disadvantage <strong>and</strong> child development.<br />

American Psychologist, 53, 185–204.<br />

Moran, P. B., & Eckenrode, J. (1992). Protective personality characteristics<br />

FLORES, CICCHETTI, AND ROGOSCH<br />

among adolescent victims <strong>of</strong> maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16,<br />

743–754.<br />

Morrow, K. B., & Sorrell, G. T. (1989). Factors affect<strong>in</strong>g self-esteem,<br />

depression, <strong>and</strong> negative behaviors <strong>in</strong> sexually abused female adolescents.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Marriage <strong>and</strong> the Family, 51, 677–686.<br />

Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer<br />

relations: A meta-analytic review <strong>of</strong> popular, rejected, neglected, controversial,<br />

<strong>and</strong> average sociometric status. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 113,<br />

99–128.<br />

Parker, J. G., Rub<strong>in</strong>, K. H., Price, J. M., & DeRosier, M. E. (1995). Peer<br />

relationships, child development, <strong>and</strong> adjustment: A developmental psychopathology<br />

perspective. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental<br />

psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, <strong>and</strong> adaptation (pp.<br />

96–161). New York: Wiley.<br />

Pekarik, E., Pr<strong>in</strong>z, R., Liebert, D., We<strong>in</strong>traub, S., & Neale, J. (1976). The<br />

Pupil Evaluation Inventory: A sociometric technique for assess<strong>in</strong>g children’s<br />

social behavior. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Child Psychology, 4,<br />

83–97.<br />

Pianta, R. C., & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, M. (1992). Teacher–child relationships <strong>and</strong> the<br />

process <strong>of</strong> adjust<strong>in</strong>g to school. New Directions for Child Development,<br />

57, 61–80.<br />

Porter, B., & O’Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord <strong>and</strong> childhood<br />

behavior problems. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 287–295.<br />

Russell, D. E. H. (1986). The secret trauma: Incest <strong>in</strong> the lives <strong>of</strong> girls <strong>and</strong><br />

women. New York: Basic Books.<br />

Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors <strong>in</strong> children’s responses to stress <strong>and</strong><br />

disadvantage. In M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf (Eds.), Primary prevention <strong>in</strong><br />

psychopathology: Social competence <strong>in</strong> children (Vol. 8, pp. 49–74).<br />

Hanover, NH: University Press <strong>of</strong> New Engl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Samer<strong>of</strong>f, A. J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987).<br />

Intelligence Quotient scores <strong>of</strong> 4-year-old children: Social–<br />

environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 79, 343–350.<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ers-Phillips, K., Moisan, P., Wadl<strong>in</strong>gton, S., Morgan, S., & English,<br />

K. (1995). Ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> psychological function<strong>in</strong>g among Black<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o sexually abused girls. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19, 691–706.<br />

Seifer, R., Samer<strong>of</strong>f, A., Baldw<strong>in</strong>, C., & Baldw<strong>in</strong>, C. (1992). Child <strong>and</strong><br />

family factors that ameliorate risk between 4 <strong>and</strong> 13 years <strong>of</strong> age.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong> Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry,<br />

31, 893–903.<br />

Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated<br />

children: The contributions <strong>of</strong> attention <strong>and</strong> emotional dysregulation.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Child Psychology, 27, 381–395.<br />

Shonk, S. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment, competency deficits,<br />

<strong>and</strong> risk for academic <strong>and</strong> behavioral maladjustment. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 37, 3–17.<br />

Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J., Hatano, G., LeV<strong>in</strong>e, R. A., Markus, H., &<br />

Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology <strong>of</strong> development: One m<strong>in</strong>d,<br />

many mentalities. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> child psychology<br />

(5th ed., Vol. 4, pp. 865–937). New York: Wiley.<br />

Simoni, J. M., & Perez, L. (1995). Lat<strong>in</strong>os <strong>and</strong> mutual support groups: A<br />

case for consider<strong>in</strong>g culture. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Orthopsychiatry, 65,<br />

440–445.<br />

Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., & Hartmann, T. (1997). A phenomenological<br />

variant <strong>of</strong> ecological systems theory (PVEST): A self-organization perspective<br />

<strong>in</strong> context. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 9, 817–833.<br />

Spencer, M. B., Fegley, S. G., & Harpalani, V. (2003). A theoretical <strong>and</strong><br />

empirical exam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> identity as cop<strong>in</strong>g: L<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g cop<strong>in</strong>g resources to<br />

the self processes <strong>of</strong> African American youth. Applied Developmental<br />

Science, 7, 181–188.<br />

Ste<strong>in</strong>, J. A., Gold<strong>in</strong>g, J. M., Siegel, J. M., Burnam, M. A., & Sorenson, S.<br />

(1988). Long-term psychological sequelae <strong>of</strong> child sexual abuse: The<br />

Los Angeles epidemiologic catchment area study. In G. Wyatt & G.<br />

Powell (Eds.), Last<strong>in</strong>g effects <strong>of</strong> child sexual abuse (pp. 135–154).<br />

Newbury Park, CA: Sage.<br />

Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., & Emde, R. N. (1997). Representa-


tions <strong>of</strong> self <strong>and</strong> other <strong>in</strong> the narratives <strong>of</strong> neglected, physically abused,<br />

<strong>and</strong> sexually abused preschoolers. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

9, 781–796.<br />

Trickett, P. K., & McBride-Chang, C. (1995). The developmental impact <strong>of</strong><br />

different forms <strong>of</strong> child abuse <strong>and</strong> neglect. Developmental Review, 15,<br />

311–337.<br />

U. S. Department <strong>of</strong> Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services, National Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse<br />

on Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect Information. (2002, April). Children’s<br />

Bureau, Adm<strong>in</strong>istration on Children, Youth, <strong>and</strong> Families, National<br />

Child Abuse <strong>and</strong> Neglect Data System (NCANDS): Summary <strong>of</strong> key<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from calendar year 2000. Retrieved May 13, 2003, from http://<br />

www.calib.com/nccanch/pubs/factsheets/canstats.cfm<br />

Vasquez, M. (1994). Lat<strong>in</strong>as. In L. Comas-Diaz & B. Greene (Eds.),<br />

Women <strong>of</strong> color (pp. 78–92). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

PREDICTORS OF RESILIENCE IN LATINO CHILDREN<br />

351<br />

Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, <strong>and</strong> recovery: Perspectives from the<br />

Kauai Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Study. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5, 503–<br />

515.<br />

White, J. L., M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1989). A prospective replication<br />

<strong>of</strong> the protective effects <strong>of</strong> IQ <strong>in</strong> subjects at high risk for juvenile<br />

del<strong>in</strong>quency. Journal <strong>of</strong> Consult<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 57, 719–<br />

724.<br />

Wright, J. (1983). The structure <strong>and</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> behavioral consistency.<br />

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.<br />

Received January 13, 2004<br />

Revision received May 15, 2004<br />

Accepted July 9, 2004

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!