01.07.2013 Views

Disclosure and Secrecy in Adolescent–Parent Relationships

Disclosure and Secrecy in Adolescent–Parent Relationships

Disclosure and Secrecy in Adolescent–Parent Relationships

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Child Development, January/February 2006, Volume 77, Number 1, Pages 201 – 217<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Adolescent–Parent</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong><br />

Judith G. Smetana, Aaron Metzger, Denise C. Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Nicole Campione-Barr<br />

University of Rochester<br />

Beliefs about parents’ legitimate authority <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ obligations to disclose to parents <strong>and</strong> actual disclosure<br />

<strong>and</strong> secrecy <strong>in</strong> different doma<strong>in</strong>s were exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> 276 ethnically diverse, lower middle-class 9th <strong>and</strong><br />

12th graders (Ms 5 14.62 <strong>and</strong> 17.40 years) <strong>and</strong> their parents (n 5 249). Adolescents were seen as more obligated<br />

to disclose prudential issues <strong>and</strong> less obligated to disclose personal than moral, conventional, <strong>and</strong> multifaceted<br />

issues; parents viewed adolescents as more obligated to disclose to parents than adolescents perceived themselves<br />

to be. Adolescents disclosed more to mothers than to fathers, particularly regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues, but<br />

mothers overestimated girls’ disclosure. Greater trust, perceived obligations to disclose, <strong>and</strong>, for personal issues,<br />

more parental acceptance <strong>and</strong> psychological control predicted more disclosure <strong>and</strong> less secrecy.<br />

Recent research has highlighted the importance of<br />

children’s disclosure as a source of parental knowledge<br />

about children’s activities (Crouter & Head,<br />

2002; Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000; Kerr, Statt<strong>in</strong>, & Trost, 1999;<br />

Statt<strong>in</strong> & Kerr, 2000). Although a great deal of research<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicates that parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g becomes<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly important <strong>in</strong> adolescence, as adolescents<br />

spend less time with parents <strong>and</strong> more time<br />

with peers (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984; Hether<strong>in</strong>gton,<br />

1993; Larson, Richards, Moneta, Holmbeck,<br />

& Duckett, 1996), Crouter <strong>and</strong> Head (2002)<br />

have noted that most of the research has assessed<br />

parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of parents’ knowledge<br />

of children’s activities <strong>and</strong> that parental knowledge<br />

can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> various ways. Kerr <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong><br />

(2000), Kerr et al. (1999), <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Kerr (2000)<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>guished among child disclosure, parental solicitation<br />

of <strong>in</strong>formation, <strong>and</strong> parental behavioral<br />

control <strong>and</strong> found that, controll<strong>in</strong>g for trust <strong>in</strong> the<br />

parent – child relationship, only adolescent disclosure<br />

was associated with lower levels of juvenile<br />

del<strong>in</strong>quency <strong>and</strong> adolescent conduct problems (but<br />

see Fletcher, Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, & Williams, 2004, who found<br />

that parental control contributed significantly to parental<br />

knowledge <strong>and</strong> also to reductions <strong>in</strong> juvenile<br />

del<strong>in</strong>quency). Furthermore, Kerr et al. (1999) found<br />

We thank Dr. Kenneth Hilton, the Rush-Henrietta School District,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the many families who participated <strong>in</strong> this research. We<br />

are also grateful to Aparajita Biswas, Emily Locker, Gregory<br />

Sherman, Pia Weston, <strong>and</strong> Alex<strong>and</strong>ra Rivera for their assistance<br />

with this study.<br />

Correspondence concern<strong>in</strong>g this article should be addressed to<br />

Judith Smetana, Department of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>and</strong> Social Sciences <strong>in</strong><br />

Psychology, Meliora Hall, RC 270266, University of Rochester,<br />

Rochester, NY 14627. Electronic mail may be sent to smetana<br />

@psych.rochester.edu.<br />

that adolescents who are more disclos<strong>in</strong>g view their<br />

parents as more trust<strong>in</strong>g of them.<br />

These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g, because they shift<br />

the focus of this large body of research from<br />

parent<strong>in</strong>g practices to adolescents’ active management<br />

of <strong>in</strong>formation about their lives. Yet the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

are <strong>in</strong>complete, both conceptually <strong>and</strong><br />

methodologically. As adolescents spend more time<br />

away from home, they have <strong>in</strong>creased opportunities<br />

to manage <strong>in</strong>formation, keep th<strong>in</strong>gs secret, <strong>and</strong> make<br />

choices about disclosure, but Kerr <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong> (2000)<br />

did not systematically consider what adolescents<br />

disclose or are secretive about. Adolescents may<br />

choose to reveal or conceal <strong>in</strong>formation to parents for<br />

different reasons, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g attempts to assert power<br />

or manipulate parents (Statt<strong>in</strong>, Kerr, & Ferrer-<br />

Wreder, 2000), avoid disapproval (Marshall, Tilton-<br />

Weaver, & Bosdet, 2005; Statt<strong>in</strong> et al., 2000), ga<strong>in</strong><br />

autonomy (F<strong>in</strong>kenauer, Engels, & Meeus, 2002;<br />

Marshall et al., 2005), or because they <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly<br />

view some aspects of their behavior as private matters<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>appropriate to disclose to parents. Furthermore,<br />

teenagers’ management of <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

may vary for different types of activities <strong>and</strong> the<br />

extent to which they believe those activities may<br />

elicit parental concern.<br />

Youniss <strong>and</strong> Smollar (1985) found that adolescent<br />

boys <strong>and</strong> girls talk to both mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers about<br />

schoolwork, future plans, <strong>and</strong> social issues, but they<br />

do not communicate much about issues like dat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(although they disclose more to mothers than fathers).<br />

Likewise, Noller <strong>and</strong> Callan (1990) found that,<br />

<strong>in</strong> general, adolescents reported not disclos<strong>in</strong>g much<br />

to their parents, with adolescent girls report<strong>in</strong>g more<br />

disclosure to mothers than to fathers <strong>and</strong> adolescent<br />

r 2006 by the Society for Research <strong>in</strong> Child Development, Inc.<br />

All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2006/7701-0014


202 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

boys report<strong>in</strong>g equal disclosure to mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers<br />

about relationships, sexual attitudes <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation,<br />

<strong>and</strong> plans. Although these studies are<br />

<strong>in</strong>formative, they lack a conceptual framework <strong>and</strong> a<br />

more systematic approach for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the<br />

types of issues that adolescents disclose or conceal<br />

from parents.<br />

Moreover, Kerr <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong> (2000) <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Kerr (2000) exam<strong>in</strong>ed disclosure as assessed by five<br />

questions: whether adolescents spontaneously tell,<br />

like to tell, usually tell, keep a lot of secrets, or hide<br />

their everyday activities from their parents. Previous<br />

research has shown that disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy are<br />

only moderately (<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>versely) correlated (F<strong>in</strong>kenauer<br />

et al., 2002), as are disclosure <strong>and</strong> concealment<br />

(F<strong>in</strong>kenauer, Frijns, Engels, & Kerkhof, 2005),<br />

but they were comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> Kerr <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong>’s (2000)<br />

measures. Kerr <strong>and</strong> Statt<strong>in</strong> (2000) found that greater<br />

disclosure was associated with more positive outcomes,<br />

but F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2002) found that for<br />

both early (12- to 13-year-olds) <strong>and</strong> late (16- to 18year-olds)<br />

adolescents, secrecy (but not disclosure)<br />

was associated with poorer relationships with parents,<br />

more physical compla<strong>in</strong>ts, <strong>and</strong> more depressed<br />

mood. Moreover, adolescents’ disclosure <strong>and</strong> concealment<br />

(as rated by parents) each have unique<br />

associations with parent<strong>in</strong>g (F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al., 2005).<br />

This research suggests that disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy<br />

are empirically dist<strong>in</strong>ct, perhaps because they vary<br />

conceptually <strong>in</strong> the extent to which they may entail<br />

acts of omission versus commission.<br />

Research from the perspective of social doma<strong>in</strong><br />

theory (Nucci, 1996; Smetana, 1995, 2006; Turiel,<br />

1983, 1998) may provide a useful way of conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g<br />

these issues. Previous research (Fuligni, 1998;<br />

Smetana, 1988a, 2000; Smetana & Asquith, 1994) has<br />

shown that adolescents consistently reject parents’<br />

legitimate authority to regulate personal issues,<br />

which perta<strong>in</strong> to control over one’s body, privacy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> choices regard<strong>in</strong>g issues such as clothes, hairstyles,<br />

or recreational activities. Personal issues are<br />

seen as not hav<strong>in</strong>g consequences for others <strong>and</strong> are<br />

therefore viewed as beyond the boundaries of legitimate<br />

moral <strong>and</strong> conventional concern. Adolescents’<br />

rejection of legitimate parental authority <strong>and</strong> their<br />

claims to jurisdiction over personal issues suggest<br />

that adolescents assert an ‘‘arena of privacy’’<br />

(Buhrmester & Prager, 1995) over these issues <strong>and</strong><br />

they may believe that they are not obligated to disclose<br />

personal issues to parents. With age, adolescents<br />

also <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly assert autonomy over<br />

multifaceted issues, which entail overlaps between<br />

doma<strong>in</strong>s. For <strong>in</strong>stance, keep<strong>in</strong>g the bedroom clean<br />

may be seen as a conventional issue by parents <strong>and</strong> a<br />

personal issue by adolescents; likewise, some<br />

friendship issues may entail overlapp<strong>in</strong>g conventional,<br />

prudential, psychological, <strong>and</strong> personal concerns.<br />

As parents generally believe that they have<br />

more authority over personal <strong>and</strong> multifaceted issues<br />

than adolescents desire, parents may view adolescents<br />

as be<strong>in</strong>g more obligated to disclose these<br />

issues than adolescents believe. However, previous<br />

research has not exam<strong>in</strong>ed adolescents’ or parents’<br />

perceptions of adolescents’ obligations to disclose<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> different doma<strong>in</strong>s. Furthermore,<br />

whether adolescents actually disclose these issues to<br />

parents may vary accord<strong>in</strong>g to the nature of the<br />

parent – adolescent relationship.<br />

In contrast, previous research has shown that<br />

adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents agree that parents should<br />

have legitimate authority over moral issues (which<br />

perta<strong>in</strong> to justice, welfare, or rights), conventional issues<br />

(the arbitrary norms, like etiquette <strong>and</strong> manners,<br />

that facilitate the smooth function<strong>in</strong>g of social<br />

systems), <strong>and</strong> prudential issues (which perta<strong>in</strong> to the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual’s comfort, safety, or health; Fuligni, 1998;<br />

Smetana, 1988a, 2000; Smetana & Asquith, 1994). In<br />

turn, this suggests that adolescents may believe that<br />

they are obligated to disclose their moral, conventional,<br />

<strong>and</strong> prudential behavior to parents.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2002) also found that<br />

adolescents’ self-disclosure was negatively associated<br />

<strong>and</strong> secrecy was positively associated with emotional<br />

autonomy, as assessed on Ste<strong>in</strong>berg <strong>and</strong><br />

Silverberg’s (1986) emotional autonomy measure.<br />

They <strong>in</strong>terpreted these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs as demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that secrecy has both positive <strong>and</strong> negative benefits<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence. Most researchers have viewed<br />

Ste<strong>in</strong>berg <strong>and</strong> Silverberg’s measure as assess<strong>in</strong>g detachment<br />

rather than healthy <strong>in</strong>dividuation (e.g.,<br />

Ryan & Lynch, 1989). Therefore, whether secrecy has<br />

positive benefits dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence rema<strong>in</strong>s unclear,<br />

although the available research is consistent <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that adolescent disclosure leads to better<br />

adjustment (Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000; Statt<strong>in</strong> & Kerr,<br />

2000). As Crouter <strong>and</strong> Head (2002) have suggested,<br />

more <strong>in</strong>formation on the correlates of disclosure <strong>and</strong><br />

secrecy is needed.<br />

The present study brought together these different<br />

l<strong>in</strong>es of research to exam<strong>in</strong>e disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy<br />

<strong>and</strong> their correlates <strong>in</strong> adolescent – parent relationships.<br />

The first aim of the study was to extend previous<br />

research on beliefs about the legitimacy of<br />

parental authority <strong>in</strong> different social-cognitive doma<strong>in</strong>s<br />

to exam<strong>in</strong>e adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> parents’ beliefs<br />

about adolescents’ obligations to disclose moral,<br />

conventional, prudential, multifaceted, <strong>and</strong> personal<br />

issues. On the basis of previous research (Smetana,


1988a, 2000; Smetana & Asquith, 1994), we hypothesized<br />

that judgments of legitimate parental authority<br />

would be highly associated with obligations<br />

to disclose to parents <strong>and</strong> that both adolescents <strong>and</strong><br />

parents would view adolescents as more obligated to<br />

disclose moral, conventional, <strong>and</strong> prudential issues<br />

rather than multifaceted <strong>and</strong> personal issues. Furthermore,<br />

we hypothesized that generation <strong>and</strong> adolescents’<br />

age would moderate these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong><br />

that parents would view adolescents as more obligated<br />

to disclose to parents, particularly regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

multifaceted <strong>and</strong> personal issues, than adolescents<br />

believed they were obligated to do. We also hypothesized<br />

that adolescents’ obligations to disclose<br />

issues <strong>in</strong> all doma<strong>in</strong>s, but particularly <strong>in</strong> the personal<br />

doma<strong>in</strong>, would decl<strong>in</strong>e with age. Because previous<br />

research on parental authority beliefs has revealed<br />

few systematic sex differences, sex differences <strong>in</strong><br />

beliefs about disclosure were exam<strong>in</strong>ed, but no specific<br />

hypotheses were tested.<br />

The second aim of this study was to exam<strong>in</strong>e adolescents’<br />

(<strong>and</strong> for disclosure, parents’) perceptions<br />

of actual disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy to parents. We focused<br />

here on everyday issues to ensure that the issues<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>ed afforded frequent opportunities for<br />

disclosure or concealment. Thus, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed perceptions<br />

of disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g schoolwork<br />

(such as whether homework was completed or<br />

how students are do<strong>in</strong>g on different subjects <strong>in</strong><br />

school), peer issues (like whether parents are home<br />

when teens are at other friends’ houses or whether<br />

they are dat<strong>in</strong>g), <strong>and</strong> personal issues (how adolescents<br />

spend their free time or what they talk about on<br />

the phone with friends). Although academic issues<br />

can be conceptualized as primarily prudential <strong>in</strong><br />

nature (e.g., hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with potential long-term<br />

harm or benefit to the self), this differs from the<br />

prudential issues exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> both the current assessment<br />

<strong>and</strong> previous research on the legitimacy of<br />

parental authority, which has focused on prudential<br />

issues of risk, such as drug <strong>and</strong> alcohol use or sexual<br />

activity (e.g., Smetana, 2000; Smetana & Asquith,<br />

1994). The peer issues exam<strong>in</strong>ed here were consistent<br />

with the multifaceted friendship issues exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong><br />

previous research on the legitimacy of parental authority<br />

<strong>in</strong> that they <strong>in</strong>cluded both personal <strong>and</strong> potentially<br />

conventional or prudential concerns.<br />

On the basis of results of F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2002,<br />

2005), we expected that adolescents’ disclosure to<br />

parents would be only moderately <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>versely<br />

associated with their rat<strong>in</strong>gs of secrecy or concealment<br />

from parents about the same issues. We also<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>ed age, sex, <strong>and</strong> topic differences <strong>in</strong> adolescents’<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy <strong>and</strong> generation differ-<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 203<br />

ences <strong>in</strong> disclosure. We expected that adolescents<br />

would disclose more <strong>and</strong> conceal less about academic<br />

than personal or peer issues <strong>and</strong> that they<br />

would disclose more <strong>and</strong> conceal less about peer<br />

than personal issues. Furthermore, previous research<br />

on disclosure has focused primarily on middle adolescence<br />

(Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000; Statt<strong>in</strong> & Kerr, 2000) or<br />

has not dist<strong>in</strong>guished between middle <strong>and</strong> late adolescence<br />

(Darl<strong>in</strong>g, Cumsille, Hames, & Caldwell,<br />

2004). In this study, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed middle (9th graders)<br />

<strong>and</strong> late adolescents’ (12th graders’) secrecy <strong>and</strong><br />

disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g these different topics. On the<br />

basis of the previous research (Buhrmester & Prager,<br />

1995; F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al., 2002; Youniss & Smollar,<br />

1985), we hypothesized that adolescents’ disclosure<br />

of personal issues to parents would decrease with<br />

age. Furthermore, we hypothesized that adolescents<br />

would disclose less, particularly at older ages <strong>and</strong><br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues, than parents perceived.<br />

Furthermore, Crouter <strong>and</strong> Head (2002) have criticized<br />

past research on parent<strong>in</strong>g as focus<strong>in</strong>g globally<br />

on ‘‘parents’’ rather than assess<strong>in</strong>g parent<strong>in</strong>g separately<br />

for mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. Thus, <strong>in</strong> this study,<br />

adolescents separately rated their disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy<br />

toward mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. On the basis of the<br />

previous research, which has shown that disclosure<br />

varies for mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teracts with<br />

child gender (Bumpus, Crouter, & McHale, 2001;<br />

Crouter, Helms-Erikson, Updegraff, & McHale, 1999;<br />

Noller & Callan, 1990; Waizenhofer, Buchanan, &<br />

Jackson-Newsom, 2004; Youniss & Smollar, 1985), we<br />

expected that adolescents, <strong>and</strong> particularly girls,<br />

would report greater disclosure overall to mothers<br />

than to fathers but that adolescent boys would disclose<br />

personal issues more to fathers than to mothers.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al aim of this study was to exam<strong>in</strong>e the<br />

correlates of adolescent disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

these different types of issues. We exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

the unique <strong>in</strong>fluences of adolescents’ beliefs about<br />

their obligations to disclose to parents, parent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g acceptance <strong>and</strong> psychological control),<br />

<strong>and</strong> the quality of parent – adolescent relationships<br />

on disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy. We hypothesized that<br />

adolescents who viewed themselves as more obligated<br />

to disclose to parents would disclose more <strong>and</strong><br />

conceal less. We also expected that the quality of the<br />

parent – adolescent relationship would <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

both disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy, but that different dimensions<br />

would be important for each. Based on<br />

Kerr et al.’s (1999) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, we expected that trust<br />

(both parents’ perceptions of trust <strong>in</strong> their adolescents<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents’ perceptions of trust <strong>in</strong><br />

their parents) would be associated with disclosure<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g all three types of issues. However, we


204 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

hypothesized that adolescents’ trust <strong>in</strong> parents<br />

would be more positively associated with disclosure<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues <strong>and</strong> more negatively associated<br />

with secrecy over personal than other issues.<br />

On the basis of the results of F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al.<br />

(2002), we expected that more problematic parent –<br />

adolescent relationships, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g greater conflict,<br />

would be associated with greater secrecy (but not<br />

disclosure), particularly over peer issues. F<strong>in</strong>ally, on<br />

the basis of recent research, which has l<strong>in</strong>ked parental<br />

psychological control specifically with parental<br />

overcontrol of adolescents’ personal doma<strong>in</strong>s<br />

(Hasebe, Nucci, & Nucci, 2004; Smetana & Daddis,<br />

2002), we expected that more parental psychological<br />

control would be associated with greater secrecy<br />

over personal issues, whereas greater acceptance<br />

would be associated with greater disclosure over<br />

personal issues.<br />

Methods<br />

Participants/Sample<br />

The sample for this study consisted of 276 adolescents,<br />

154 9th graders (M 5 14.62 years, SD 5 0.51,<br />

n 5 53 males <strong>and</strong> 101 females) <strong>and</strong> 122 12th graders<br />

(M 5 17.40 years, SD 5 0.47, n 5 42 males <strong>and</strong> 80 females),<br />

<strong>and</strong> their parents (n 5 249: 210 mothers <strong>and</strong><br />

39 fathers). Boys <strong>and</strong> girls <strong>in</strong> each grade did not<br />

differ significantly <strong>in</strong> age. The adolescents were 70%<br />

European American, 9% African American, 9%<br />

Asian, 7% biracial, 4% Hispanic, <strong>and</strong> 2% other. Most<br />

participat<strong>in</strong>g students lived <strong>in</strong> two-parent families<br />

with two biological parents (72%); the rema<strong>in</strong>der<br />

resided <strong>in</strong> stepparent families (7%), s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent<br />

families (19%, of which nearly all were motherheaded<br />

households), or other (2%). Family structure<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents’ race/ethnicity (exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms<br />

of European American vs. all else) did not differ<br />

significantly by adolescents’ grade or sex.<br />

Participat<strong>in</strong>g mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers were, on average,<br />

43.50 <strong>and</strong> 45.67 years old, respectively<br />

(SDs 5 5.00 <strong>and</strong> 5.82 years). Twenty-eight percent of<br />

both mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers had a high school diploma<br />

or less, 45% of mothers <strong>and</strong> 37% of fathers had<br />

completed some college, <strong>and</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g parents<br />

had obta<strong>in</strong>ed a college degree or higher (8% of<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> 15% of fathers had graduate degrees).<br />

Parents were primarily lower middle-class, as assessed<br />

on the socioeconomic <strong>in</strong>dex (SEI; Nakao &<br />

Treas, 1992). Scores on the SEI can range from 1 to<br />

100, with higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g greater occupational<br />

prestige (Ms 5 46.64 <strong>and</strong> 52.26 for mothers <strong>and</strong><br />

fathers, SDs 5 15.50 <strong>and</strong> 18.48). F<strong>in</strong>ally, adolescents<br />

with parents participat<strong>in</strong>g or not participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the<br />

study did not differ <strong>in</strong> sociodemographic background,<br />

except that adolescents with a participat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

parent were more likely to come from two biological<br />

parent families than adolescents whose parents did<br />

not respond (72% vs. 56%).<br />

Assessments of Parental Authority Legitimacy <strong>and</strong><br />

Obligations to Disclose<br />

Stimuli. The stimuli for these assessments were 20<br />

hypothetical issues grouped <strong>in</strong>to prudential, moral<br />

<strong>and</strong> conventional, multifaceted, <strong>and</strong> personal categories<br />

(see Appendix A for the items). Based on pilot<br />

test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> previous research (Smetana & Daddis,<br />

2002; Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Daddis, 2004), the<br />

moral <strong>and</strong> conventional items were comb<strong>in</strong>ed to<br />

form a s<strong>in</strong>gle socially regulated dimension. The def<strong>in</strong>itions<br />

of the doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> the selection of items<br />

were based on previous theoriz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> research<br />

(Smetana, 1988a, 2000; Smetana & Asquith, 1994), as<br />

well as pilot test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Legitimacy rat<strong>in</strong>gs. Adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents rated<br />

the extent to which it is okay for parents to make<br />

rules or set expectations about each item, assess<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the legitimacy of parental authority. Respondents rated<br />

the legitimacy of parental authority on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (absolutely not) to5(def<strong>in</strong>itely,<br />

yes). Thus, higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicated greater legitimacy<br />

of parental authority. as for adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

prudential, socially regulated (moral <strong>and</strong> conventional),<br />

multifaceted, <strong>and</strong> personal items were .89,<br />

.74, .77, <strong>and</strong> .76, respectively, <strong>and</strong> for parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

were .91, .77, .81, <strong>and</strong> .74, respectively.<br />

Obligations to disclose. Adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents<br />

also rated whether children ‘‘should tell parents what<br />

they are do<strong>in</strong>g, that is whether they have a duty or<br />

obligation to tell parents about their behavior,’’ assess<strong>in</strong>g<br />

their judgments of their obligations to disclose<br />

to parents. Obligations were rated on the same 5po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale as for legitimacy, with higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

greater obligation. as were .89, .87, .78, <strong>and</strong><br />

.77 for adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of prudential, social,<br />

multifaceted, <strong>and</strong> personal items, respectively, <strong>and</strong><br />

.92, .83, .81, <strong>and</strong> .75 for parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Assessments of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Adolescent disclosure. Adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents<br />

rated how often they ‘‘usually tell or are will<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

tell their mother (or father), without them ask<strong>in</strong>g’’<br />

about 12 personal, peer, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork issues,<br />

listed <strong>in</strong> Appendix A. As can be seen, the same<br />

personal items used <strong>in</strong> the assessment of beliefs


about legitimate parental authority <strong>and</strong> obligations<br />

to disclose were used <strong>in</strong> the assessment of disclosure.<br />

Respondents were <strong>in</strong>structed to consider how much<br />

they (or their adolescents) disclose or are will<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

disclose to each parent, rather than whether they<br />

actually engage <strong>in</strong> each behavior. Adolescents completed<br />

the rat<strong>in</strong>gs separately for disclosure to mothers<br />

<strong>and</strong> fathers. Responses were rated on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (never tell)to5(always tell). as for<br />

disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g personal, schoolwork, <strong>and</strong> peer<br />

issues were .81, .78, <strong>and</strong> .68 for adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

mothers, .77, .80, <strong>and</strong> .67 for adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

fathers, <strong>and</strong> .78, .86, <strong>and</strong> .68 for parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Adolescent secrecy. <strong>Secrecy</strong> was rated regard<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

same set of items used to assess disclosure. Adolescents<br />

rated how often they ‘‘keep secret or try to hide<br />

what you are do<strong>in</strong>g from your mother (father)’’ on a 5po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (never) to5(always). Aga<strong>in</strong>,<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs were completed separately for each parent. as<br />

for rat<strong>in</strong>gs of secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g personal, schoolwork,<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer issues were .78, .76, <strong>and</strong> .68 for rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> .80, .80, <strong>and</strong> .70 for rat<strong>in</strong>gs of fathers.<br />

Correlates of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Trust. Adolescents completed the Trust subscale<br />

(10 items) of the Parent – Peer Attachment Inventory<br />

(PPAI; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987), which assesses<br />

adolescents’ trust <strong>in</strong> parents. Responses were scored<br />

on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (almost never<br />

or never true) to5(almost always or always true). Adolescents<br />

completed the questionnaire twice, once<br />

for mothers (a 5 .74) <strong>and</strong> once for fathers (a 5 .77).<br />

The items were reworded to assess parents’ trust <strong>in</strong><br />

their adolescent (a 5 .69). Adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

trust <strong>in</strong> mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers were significantly associated,<br />

r(263) 5 .43 (except where otherwise noted,<br />

all pso.01); rat<strong>in</strong>gs of mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers were<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed for the analyses.<br />

Adolescent – parent conflict. Adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents<br />

rated 11 different areas of day-to-day decision<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g (dress or cloth<strong>in</strong>g, help<strong>in</strong>g out around the<br />

house, homework, time to be home, choice or volume<br />

of music or TV, time spent on the phone, who<br />

should be friends, bedroom, spend<strong>in</strong>g or manag<strong>in</strong>g<br />

money, dat<strong>in</strong>g), drawn from previous research on<br />

adolescent – parent conflict (Smetana, 1989). Us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

procedures derived from the Issues Checklist (Pr<strong>in</strong>z,<br />

Foster, Kent, & O’Leary, 1979; Rob<strong>in</strong> & Foster, 1989),<br />

respondents <strong>in</strong>dicated whether each issue was discussed<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the past 2 weeks (scored as 0 5 not<br />

discussed or 1 5 discussed). Then, for each issue discussed,<br />

respondents rated the frequency of discussion<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the previous 2 weeks on a scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 205<br />

1(not often) to4(very often) <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tensity of discussion<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the previous 2 weeks on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (calm) to5(angry). Adolescents<br />

completed these rat<strong>in</strong>gs separately for mothers <strong>and</strong><br />

fathers. Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers were significantly<br />

associated for both frequency, r(247) 5 .56,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity, r(245) 5 .43; thus, scores for mothers<br />

<strong>and</strong> fathers were comb<strong>in</strong>ed for both variables.<br />

Parent<strong>in</strong>g. Parents rated their parent<strong>in</strong>g on the<br />

Acceptance/Rejection <strong>and</strong> Psychological Control<br />

subscales (10 items each) of the Children’s Report of<br />

Parents’ Behavior Inventory (CRPBI; Schaefer, 1965a,<br />

1965b; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1970). Parents<br />

rated each item on a 3-po<strong>in</strong>t scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from<br />

1(not like the parent) to3(a lot like the parent). as for<br />

the two scales were .73 <strong>and</strong> .58.<br />

Self-esteem. Adolescents rated each of 10 items on<br />

the Rosenberg (1986) Self-Esteem Inventory on a 4po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4<br />

(strongly agree; a 5 .87).<br />

Procedures<br />

Adolescents <strong>and</strong> their parents were recruited from<br />

a suburban school district. Researchers visited all<br />

9th- <strong>and</strong> 12th-grade homerooms to <strong>in</strong>form students<br />

about the research. Interested students took the<br />

survey packages (for both students <strong>and</strong> parents)<br />

home. Because of the number of s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent families<br />

<strong>in</strong> the district, only one parent per family was<br />

asked to complete the surveys. Students were offered<br />

small honoraria for their participation. Completed<br />

surveys were returned <strong>in</strong> school or, <strong>in</strong> a few cases,<br />

mailed back to the research team.<br />

Participation rates, based on the number of students<br />

<strong>in</strong> each grade <strong>and</strong> the average daily absence<br />

rates for those grades, were 33% for the 9th grade <strong>and</strong><br />

27% for the 12th grade. The race/ethnicity <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic<br />

status of our sample closely matched the<br />

demographic characteristics of the district, which<br />

was primarily lower middle-class <strong>and</strong> 78% White, 9%<br />

African American, 1% American Indian, 7% Asian,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 4% Hispanic. Participat<strong>in</strong>g students were slightly<br />

above the district average <strong>in</strong> academic achievement<br />

(assessed by self-reported grade po<strong>in</strong>t average), <strong>and</strong><br />

girls were overrepresented <strong>in</strong> our sample.<br />

Results<br />

Legitimacy of Parental Authority <strong>and</strong> Adolescents’<br />

Obligations to Disclose to Parents<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations for adolescents’<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents’ judgments of the legitimacy of parental


206 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

authority <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ obligations to disclose<br />

their behavior <strong>in</strong> different doma<strong>in</strong>s are reported <strong>in</strong><br />

Table 1. Our hypothesis that rat<strong>in</strong>gs of the legitimacy<br />

of parental authority <strong>and</strong> obligations to disclose to<br />

parents would be highly associated was confirmed.<br />

Total rat<strong>in</strong>gs were strongly correlated for adolescents,<br />

r(275) 5 .59, <strong>and</strong> parents, r(249) 5 .62. However,<br />

although statistically significant, associations<br />

between parents’ <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of the legitimacy<br />

of parental authority, r(249) 5 .20, <strong>and</strong> obligations<br />

to disclose to parents, r(249) 5 .29, were<br />

only moderate.<br />

To test hypotheses about doma<strong>in</strong>, age, <strong>and</strong> generation<br />

differences, separate 2 (grade) 2 (sex) 2<br />

(generation: parent vs. child) 4 (doma<strong>in</strong>) repeated<br />

measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with<br />

generation <strong>and</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> as repeated measures were<br />

performed on rat<strong>in</strong>gs for the two judgments. As<br />

hypothesized, parents were seen as hav<strong>in</strong>g significantly<br />

less legitimate authority, F(1, 236) 5 6.17, <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents were perceived as be<strong>in</strong>g significantly<br />

less obligated to disclose to parents as they got older,<br />

F(1, 237) 5 7.94. Also consistent with expectations,<br />

parents viewed parents as hav<strong>in</strong>g significantly more<br />

legitimate authority, F(1, 237) 5 239.14, <strong>and</strong> adolescents<br />

as hav<strong>in</strong>g significantly more of an obligation to<br />

disclose to parents, F(1, 237) 5 220.30, than did adolescents.<br />

Furthermore, significant ma<strong>in</strong> effects for<br />

doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>gs of legitimacy, F(3, 708) 5 523.69,<br />

Table 1<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations for Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of Legitimacy of Parental Authority <strong>and</strong> Obligations to Disclose<br />

<strong>and</strong> obligations to disclose, F(3, 711) 5 327.23, revealed<br />

that prudential issues were seen as more legitimately<br />

subject to parental authority <strong>and</strong> more<br />

obligatory to disclose than all other issues (Bonferroni<br />

t-tests, all pso.01). In turn, socially regulated<br />

(moral <strong>and</strong> conventional) items were seen as more<br />

legitimately subject to parental authority than multifaceted<br />

<strong>and</strong> personal issues. In contrast, adolescents<br />

were seen as more obligated to tell parents<br />

about their behavior vis-à-vis multifaceted issues<br />

than moral, conventional, <strong>and</strong> personal issues. As<br />

hypothesized, personal issues were seen as less obligatory<br />

to disclose <strong>and</strong> less legitimately subject to<br />

parental authority than all other issues.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs regard<strong>in</strong>g the legitimacy of parental authority<br />

were qualified by significant Generation<br />

Grade, F(1, 236) 5 4.93, p 5 .03, Generation Doma<strong>in</strong>,<br />

F(3, 708) 5 42.76, <strong>and</strong> Doma<strong>in</strong> Grade<br />

Generation <strong>in</strong>teractions, F(3, 708) 5 4.50. For obligations<br />

to disclose to parents, there was a significant<br />

Generation Doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction, F(3, 711) 5 20.49,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the three-way <strong>in</strong>teraction approached significance,<br />

F(3, 711) 5 2.56, p 5 .054. Post hoc analyses<br />

revealed that there were no significant age differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> 9th <strong>and</strong> 12th graders’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of the legitimacy<br />

of parental authority for any of the doma<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

but parents of 9th graders viewed parental authority<br />

as more legitimate for multifaceted, socially regulated<br />

(moral <strong>and</strong> conventional), <strong>and</strong> personal issues<br />

Legitimacy of parental authority Obligation to disclose<br />

Adolescents Parents Adolescents Parents<br />

9th 12th M 9th 12th M 9th 12th M 9th 12th M<br />

Prudential<br />

M 4.17 3.96 4.05 4.73 4.52 4.64 3.63 3.33 3.51 4.74 4.55 4.66<br />

SD 1.12 0.98 1.08 0.88 0.92 0.90 1.37 1.28 1.30 0.75 0.70 0.73<br />

Social<br />

M 2.91 2.89 2.90 4.29 4.05 4.19 2.89 2.66 2.79 4.07 3.80 3.96<br />

SD 0.99 0.94 0.92 0.72 0.86 0.75 1.09 1.02 0.96 0.73 0.75 0.75<br />

Multifac<br />

M 2.76 2.74 2.75 4.24 3.75 4.05 3.25 3.18 3.22 4.29 3.90 4.12<br />

SD 0.91 0.85 0.87 0.72 0.79 0.78 1.02 0.89 0.94 0.70 0.71 0.73<br />

Personal<br />

M 2.11 2.18 2.15 3.16 2.80 3.02 2.55 2.49 2.52 3.35 3.07 3.23<br />

SD 0.88 0.79 0.83 0.80 0.79 0.81 0.88 0.86 0.87 0.78 0.68 0.75<br />

Total<br />

M 3.02 2.98 3.00 4.12 3.79 3.99 3.09 2.91 3.01 4.11 3.84 4.00<br />

SD 0.82 0.71 0.77 0.59 0.65 0.64 0.91 0.85 0.88 0.62 0.59 0.62<br />

Note. Multifac 5 Multifaceted; Social 5 Moral <strong>and</strong> Conventional. Items were rated on 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scales where higher scores 5 more legitimate<br />

parental authority <strong>and</strong> greater obligation to disclose to parents.


(but only marg<strong>in</strong>ally, for prudential issues) than did<br />

parents of 12th graders. As expected, parents viewed<br />

their adolescents as significantly less obligated to<br />

disclose their activities <strong>in</strong> each doma<strong>in</strong> as they got<br />

older. However, similar age-related decreases <strong>in</strong> adolescents’<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs of their obligation to voluntarily<br />

disclose their behavior to parents approached significance<br />

for moral <strong>and</strong> conventional (po.07) <strong>and</strong><br />

prudential issues (po.055), although adolescents<br />

consistently rated personal issues as less obligatory<br />

than all other issues.<br />

Overview <strong>and</strong> Descriptive F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations for adolescents’<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g personal,<br />

peer, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork issues for mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers<br />

<strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>terrelationships are presented <strong>in</strong> Table 2.<br />

As can be seen, mean levels of disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy<br />

were both moderate.<br />

Because we selected the peer issues from a larger<br />

set of multifaceted issues used to assess legitimate<br />

parental authority, the items, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, entailed<br />

overlaps between the doma<strong>in</strong>s. Thus, high correlations<br />

between personal <strong>and</strong> peer issues would be<br />

expected, <strong>and</strong> this is what we found. However, there<br />

were strong associations among rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g all three types of issues. In contrast, while<br />

adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g personal<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer issues were highly correlated, as would be<br />

expected, the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g associations were moderate,<br />

support<strong>in</strong>g their dist<strong>in</strong>ction. For both disclosure <strong>and</strong><br />

secrecy, adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers<br />

were very strongly associated, but when consider<strong>in</strong>g<br />

both mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers, adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy showed the expected moderate<br />

but negative associations.<br />

Adolescent <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Although adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers<br />

were strongly associated, the results <strong>in</strong> Table 2<br />

also suggest that there were mean level differences <strong>in</strong><br />

adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of their parents. Thus, <strong>in</strong> the<br />

next set of analyses, 2 (adolescents’ sex) 2 (sex of<br />

parent) 2 (grade) 3 (topic) repeated measures<br />

ANOVAs with sex of parent <strong>and</strong> topic as repeated<br />

measures were run to exam<strong>in</strong>e the hypothesized<br />

grade, sex of parent, sex of adolescent, <strong>and</strong> topic<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> secrecy <strong>and</strong> disclosure. (There was a<br />

slightly smaller n <strong>in</strong> these analyses than <strong>in</strong> the pre- 2<br />

vious analyses because not all adolescents rated a<br />

father. Parent – adolescent differences <strong>in</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>gs of Table<br />

Means, St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations, <strong>and</strong> Correlations Among Adolescents’ Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Disclose ! Mother Disclose ! Father <strong>Secrecy</strong> ! Mother <strong>Secrecy</strong> ! Father<br />

School Peer Pers School Peer Pers School Peer Pers School Peer<br />

M SD<br />

Disclose ! M Pers 2.83 0.94 0.61 0.68 0.77 0.46 0.50 0.43 0.23 0.40 0.33 0.20 0.28<br />

Disclose ! M School 3.82 0.94 1.00 0.54 0.47 0.77 0.38 0.22 0.34 0.27 0.14 0.30 0.20<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 207<br />

Disclose ! M Peer 2.98 1.18 1.00 0.52 0.42 0.79 0.39 0.23 0.51 0.28 0.15 0.38<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> ! F Pers 2.49 0.87 1.00 0.62 0.68 0.34 0.21 0.31 0.39 0.25 0.37<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> ! F School 3.59 1.08 1.00 0.53 0.10 0.25 0.14 0.16 0.30 0.20<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> ! F Peer 2.68 1.03 1.00 0.29 0.14 0.37 0.37 0.17 0.44<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! M Pers 2.60 0.93 1.00 0.38 0.65 0.82 0.34 0.54<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! M School 2.16 0.98 1.00 0.38 0.22 0.80 0.29<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! M Peer 2.74 1.15 1.00 0.51 0.33 0.81<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! F Pers 2.71 0.99 1.00 0.22 0.51<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! F School 2.25 1.05 1.00 0.33<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong> ! F Peer 2.87 1.23 1.00<br />

Note. M5Mother; F 5 Father; Pers 5 Personal; Underl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 5 ns. Items were rated on 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scales where higher scores 5 more disclosure <strong>and</strong> greater secrecy.<br />

po.05, all other ps o.01.


208 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

disclosure were exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> a separate analysis,<br />

described <strong>in</strong> more detail below.)<br />

Adolescent secrecy. Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations<br />

for adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of secrecy toward mothers<br />

<strong>and</strong> fathers are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3. Adolescents were<br />

more secretive about peer than personal issues or<br />

schoolwork <strong>and</strong> more secretive about personal issues<br />

than schoolwork, F(2, 246) 5 34.10. A significant Sex<br />

of Child Sex of Parent <strong>in</strong>teraction, F(1, 248) 5 5.64,<br />

was qualified by a significant Sex of Child Sex of<br />

Parent Topic <strong>in</strong>teraction, F(2, 496) 5 3.61. Boys<br />

concealed personal issues from mothers more than<br />

girls did, F(1, 248) 5 4.79.<br />

A significant Topic Grade <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(2, 496) 5 7.06, revealed that 9th graders were more<br />

secretive about peers than either schoolwork or<br />

personal issues <strong>and</strong> more secretive about personal<br />

issues than schoolwork, Ms 5 2.87, 2.68, 2.11,<br />

SDs 5 1.21, 0.93, 0.98, respectively. In contrast, 12th<br />

graders did not differentiate between personal <strong>and</strong><br />

peer issues, although they hid both more than<br />

schoolwork, Ms 5 2.59, 2.69, 2.29, SDs 5 0.89, 1.02,<br />

0.93, respectively. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were qualified by a<br />

significant Grade Child’s Sex Topic <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(2, 496) 5 5.40, which was due to a Grade Child’s<br />

Sex <strong>in</strong>teraction for schoolwork, F(3, 269) 5 2.84. Post<br />

Table 3<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations for Adolescents’ Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong> to Parents<br />

hoc tests <strong>in</strong>dicated that 12th-grade boys kept secrets<br />

about schoolwork more than did either 12th-grade<br />

girls or 9th-grade boys (see Table 3).<br />

Adolescent disclosure. Adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure<br />

to mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers are also presented <strong>in</strong><br />

Table 3. As hypothesized, adolescents reported voluntarily<br />

disclos<strong>in</strong>g more to mothers than to fathers,<br />

F(1, 252) 5 52.66, Ms 5 3.16, 2.92, SDs 5 0.85, 0.90,<br />

respectively. Mirror<strong>in</strong>g the results for secrecy, adolescents<br />

also disclosed more about schoolwork than<br />

either peers or personal issues <strong>and</strong> more about peers<br />

than personal issues, F(2, 504) 5 175.87.<br />

A significant Sex of Child Sex of Parent <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(1, 252) 5 10.54, was qualified by a significant<br />

Sex of Child Sex of Parent Topic <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(2, 504) 5 6.39. Post hoc analyses revealed that, as<br />

expected, girls disclosed more about personal issues,<br />

Ms 5 2.96, 2.65, SDs 5 0.94, 0.87, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork,<br />

Ms 5 3.93, 3.69, SDs 5 0.90, 0.91, respectively, to their<br />

mothers than did boys, but boys <strong>and</strong> girls did not<br />

differ <strong>in</strong> their voluntary disclosure to mothers about<br />

peers. Contrary to hypotheses, there were no differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> boys’ <strong>and</strong> girls’ disclosure to fathers.<br />

In addition, a significant Topic Grade <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(2, 504) 5 10.60 revealed that 12th graders voluntarily<br />

disclosed more about peers than did 9th graders,<br />

Regard<strong>in</strong>g mothers Regard<strong>in</strong>g fathers<br />

Boys Girls Boys Girls<br />

9th 12th M 9th 12th M 9th 12th M 9th 12th M<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong><br />

Personal<br />

M 2.59 2.73 2.65 2.94 2.98 2.96 2.40 2.63 2.51 2.52 2.45 2.49<br />

SD 0.93 0.81 0.87 0.93 0.94 0.94 0.90 0.76 0.85 0.87 0.88 0.87<br />

School<br />

M 3.84 3.52 3.69 3.97 3.87 3.93 3.65 3.29 3.49 3.70 3.57 3.64<br />

SD 0.99 0.79 0.91 0.91 0.89 0.90 1.07 0.91 1.01 1.13 1.09 1.11<br />

Peer<br />

M 2.72 3.13 2.91 2.98 3.19 3.08 2.65 3.00 2.81 2.55 2.77 2.65<br />

SD 1.08 1.04 1.08 1.26 1.78 1.22 1.14 1.06 1.11 1.21 1.21 1.21<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Personal<br />

M 2.78 2.72 2.79 2.55 2.48 2.52 2.77 2.68 2.73 2.76 2.59 2.69<br />

SD 0.95 0.92 0.95 0.93 0.92 0.93 1.02 0.93 0.98 1.02 0.99 1.00<br />

School<br />

M 2.10 2.57 2.34 2.07 2.09 2.12 2.17 2.58 2.36 2.17 2.23 2.20<br />

SD 1.03 1.00 1.04 0.99 0.90 0.95 1.00 1.06 1.05 1.09 1.02 1.06<br />

Peers<br />

M 2.97 2.53 2.85 2.71 2.70 2.68 3.07 2.50 2.81 2.90 2.87 2.90<br />

SD 1.17 0.88 1.06 1.26 1.13 1.21 1.20 1.02 1.15 1.35 1.18 1.27


Table 4<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations for Adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> Mothers’ Rat<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of <strong>Disclosure</strong><br />

Adolescents (rat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mothers) Mothers<br />

9th grade 12th grade 9th grade 12th grade<br />

Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls<br />

Personal<br />

M 2.57 2.87 2.81 3.06 2.72 3.31 2.71 3.33<br />

SD 0.95 0.98 0.84 0.99 0.84 0.86 0.85 0.90<br />

School<br />

M 3.80 3.86 3.58 4.01 3.14 3.58 2.72 3.56<br />

SD 0.90 0.96 0.90 0.81 0.90 0.84 0.84 0.97<br />

Peers<br />

M 2.73 2.93 3.07 3.35 3.28 3.62 2.89 3.51<br />

SD 1.07 1.31 1.09 1.20 0.94 1.04 0.96 0.99<br />

F(1, 173) 5 4.13, Furthermore, 12th graders disclosed<br />

more about peers than about personal issues, whereas<br />

9th graders did not differentiate between peers <strong>and</strong><br />

personal issues <strong>in</strong> their disclosure to parents.<br />

Parent – adolescent differences <strong>in</strong> perceived disclosure.<br />

Because most parent participants were mothers<br />

<strong>and</strong> because the previous analyses revealed that<br />

adolescents rated their fathers <strong>and</strong> mothers differently,<br />

the next analyses compared adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of disclosure to mothers with mothers’<br />

responses (n 5 202). Thus, a 2 (adolescents’ sex) 2<br />

(grade) 2 (adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of mothers vs.<br />

mothers) 3 (topic) repeated measures ANOVA<br />

with generation <strong>and</strong> topic as repeated measures was<br />

performed on disclosure rat<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

A significant Generation Grade <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(1, 198) 5 6.60, <strong>in</strong>dicated that mothers viewed their<br />

9th graders as disclos<strong>in</strong>g more than 9th graders reported<br />

disclos<strong>in</strong>g, Ms 5 3.36, 3.05, SDs 5 0.82, 0.79,<br />

respectively, but 12th graders <strong>and</strong> their mothers did<br />

not differ, Ms 5 3.24, 3.28, SDs 5 0.91, 0.86. A significant<br />

Generation Sex <strong>in</strong>teraction, F(1, 198) 5<br />

5.94, revealed that mothers viewed their daughters<br />

as disclos<strong>in</strong>g more than girls reported disclos<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

Ms 5 3.49, 3.29, SDs 5 0.84, 0.93, but that boys’ <strong>and</strong><br />

their mothers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs did not differ, Ms 5 3.05, 2.94,<br />

SDs 5 0.79, 0.80.<br />

A significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect for topic, F(1, 198) 5<br />

89.24, was qualified by significant Topic Generation,<br />

F(1, 198) 5 67.04, Topic Grade, F(1, 198) 5<br />

3.67, <strong>and</strong> Topic Generation Grade <strong>in</strong>teractions,<br />

F(1, 198) 5 4.76. Post hoc analyses revealed that while<br />

12th graders voluntarily told their mothers more<br />

about their peer relations than 9th graders did, as<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 209<br />

found <strong>in</strong> the previous analyses, mothers did not<br />

differ significantly <strong>in</strong> their views of adolescents’<br />

disclosure about these issues.<br />

Correlates of Adolescent <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

In the next set of analyses, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed correlations<br />

among the study variables, as shown <strong>in</strong> Table 5.<br />

For rat<strong>in</strong>gs of the legitimacy of parental authority<br />

<strong>and</strong> beliefs about disclosure, the moral, conventional,<br />

<strong>and</strong> prudential items were dropped, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

multifaceted <strong>and</strong> personal items were comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>to<br />

a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dex to more closely match the issues of<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy under <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Given<br />

their high <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations, adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers were comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> these analyses<br />

for disclosure, secrecy, trust, discussion frequency,<br />

<strong>and</strong> conflict <strong>in</strong>tensity. As expected,<br />

adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy were<br />

significantly <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>versely correlated, r(274) 5 .46.<br />

As can be seen, <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations among the <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

variables were generally low to moderate.<br />

We also exam<strong>in</strong>ed correlations between adolescentrated<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy <strong>and</strong> several sociodemographic<br />

background variables, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g parents’<br />

ages, educations, SEI scores, ethnicity (White vs. all<br />

else), <strong>and</strong> marital status (married, two-parent vs. all<br />

else), but no significant relationships were obta<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Next, we conducted regression analyses to exam<strong>in</strong>e<br />

the unique <strong>in</strong>fluence of study variables on disclosure<br />

<strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g different issues. As we had no a<br />

priori hypotheses about the order<strong>in</strong>g of variables, all<br />

variables were entered <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle step. Because adolescents’<br />

beliefs about the legitimacy of parental authority<br />

<strong>and</strong> their beliefs about their obligations to<br />

disclose to parents were strongly correlated (r 5.51),<br />

lead<strong>in</strong>g to concerns with multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity, we <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

only beliefs about adolescents’ obligations to<br />

disclose to parents <strong>in</strong> the regression equations.<br />

Therefore, adolescents’ age <strong>and</strong> sex, adolescents’<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs of trust, discussion frequency <strong>and</strong> conflict<br />

<strong>in</strong>tensity, self-esteem, <strong>and</strong> beliefs about obligations to<br />

disclose to parents, as well as parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

acceptance, psychological control, <strong>and</strong> trust <strong>in</strong> their<br />

child were entered <strong>in</strong> the analyses. Separate analyses<br />

were run for disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g personal,<br />

peer, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork issues, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> six<br />

separate regressions. Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary analyses revealed<br />

that parental trust was not significant <strong>in</strong> any of the<br />

analyses, <strong>and</strong> thus it was dropped from the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

analyses. The results of the analyses are presented <strong>in</strong><br />

Table 6.<br />

We also conducted exploratory analyses exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

adolescents’ separate rat<strong>in</strong>gs for each topic for


210 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

Table 5<br />

Means, St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations, <strong>and</strong> Correlations Among Variables<br />

M SD Age Sex TTrust Freq Intens Self-E Oblig PTrust Accep Psych<br />

A Age 15.87 1.49 1.00 0.04 0.02 0.05 0.03 0.05 0.12 0.01 0.06 0.06<br />

Sex (1 5 female) 0.66 0.48 1.00 0.13 0.17 0.18 0.10 0.13 0.04 0.10 0.01<br />

A Trust 3.81 0.72 1.00 0.01 0.38 0.43 0.38 0.39 0.29 0.11 +<br />

A Discussion Freq 2.15 0.63 1.00 0.52 0.06 0.23 0.09 0.01 0.07<br />

A Conflict Intensity 2.11 1.04 1.00 0.08 0.03 0.15 0.11 +<br />

0.18<br />

A Self-Esteem 3.09 0.56 1.00 0.19 0.10 +<br />

0.16 0.06<br />

A Obligations 3.11 0.86 1.00 0.17 0.20 0.04<br />

P Trust 4.13 0.53 1.00 0.46 0.17<br />

P Acceptance 2.56 0.32 1.00 0.14<br />

P - Psych Control 1.81 0.33 1.00<br />

Disclose-Personal 2.66 0.85 0.00 0.05 0.44 0.16 0.08 0.28 0.57 0.23 0.27 0.06<br />

Disclose-School 3.70 0.96 0.13 0.09 0.48 0.14 0.09 0.29 0.50 0.17 0.29 0.08<br />

Disclose-Peers 2.83 1.12 0.11 +<br />

0.00 0.40 0.14 0.10 0.18 0.50 0.19 0.13 0.01<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong>-Personal 2.64 0.92 0.02 0.01 0.28 0.02 0.08 0.22 0.30 0.08 0.01 0.12 +<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong>-School 2.19 0.96 0.12 0.10 0.25 0.01 0.12 +<br />

0.22 0.23 0.12 +<br />

0.10 0.02<br />

<strong>Secrecy</strong>-Peers 2.80 1.13 0.09 0.03 0.28 0.00 0.17 0.21 0.21 0.13 0.02 0.12 +<br />

Note. A5 Adolescent; P 5 Parent; Freq 5 Frequency; Intens 5 Intensity; Self-E 5 Self-Esteem; Auth 5 Legitimacy of Parental Authority;<br />

Oblig 5 Obligations to Disclose; Accep 5 Acceptance; Psych 5 Psychological Control. ns ranged from 276 (for associations between adolescent-rated<br />

measures) <strong>and</strong> 242 (for associations between parental <strong>and</strong> child reports).<br />

+ po.10, po.05, po.01.<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. In these analyses, we used adolescents’<br />

parent-specific rat<strong>in</strong>gs of trust, discussion<br />

frequency, <strong>and</strong> conflict <strong>in</strong>tensity, as well as measures<br />

of parental psychological control, parental acceptance,<br />

<strong>and</strong> beliefs about obligations to disclose used <strong>in</strong> the<br />

previous analyses. Results differ<strong>in</strong>g from the comb<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

analyses are noted <strong>in</strong> parentheses <strong>in</strong> the text.<br />

Unique correlates of adolescent disclosure. Older adolescents<br />

reported greater disclosure over peer issues.<br />

As can be seen, adolescents’ trust <strong>in</strong> parents<br />

Table 6<br />

Regression Models of Adolescents’ Perceptions of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong> (n 5 232)<br />

Predictor<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents’ beliefs about their obligation to<br />

disclose to parents were statistically significant <strong>in</strong><br />

each analysis. Adolescents who trusted their parents<br />

more <strong>and</strong> who believed they were more obligated to<br />

disclose to them reported greater disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

personal, peer, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork issues. In addition,<br />

more parent-reported psychological control<br />

predicted greater adolescent disclosure about personal<br />

issues (for both mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers) <strong>and</strong><br />

marg<strong>in</strong>ally (po.07) for peer issues (for fathers only,<br />

Disclose-Pers Disclose-School Disclose-Peer Hide-Personal Hide-School Hide-Peer<br />

SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b<br />

A Age .03 .09 +<br />

.03 .04 .04 .20 .04 .07 .04 .121 .05 .14<br />

A Sex .09 .01 .11 .08 .13 .03 .13 .03 .13 .111 .16 .05<br />

A Trust .08 .24 .09 .28 .11 .28 .11 .19 .11 .10 .13 .20<br />

Discussion Freq .08 .06 .09 .01 .11 .10 .12 .03 .12 .02 .14 .05<br />

Conflict Intensity .07 .01 .08 .07 .10 .04 .10 .02 .11 .10 .12 .17<br />

Obligate ! Disclose .05 .48 .06 .38 .08 .43 .08 .25 .08 .17 .09 .15<br />

Self-Esteem .09 .04 .10 .08 .12 .09 .12 .07 .12 .16 .14 .04<br />

Parent Acceptance .14 .11 .16 .14 .19 .02 .20 .13 .20 .01 .23 .09<br />

Parent Psych Control .13 .15 .15 .01 .18 .101 .18 .16 .19 .01 .22 .20<br />

Model Total R 2<br />

.46 .41 .38 .18 .14 .17<br />

Note. Pers 5 Personal; A 5 Adolescent; Freq 5 Frequency; Psych 5 Psychological.<br />

+ po.10, po.05, po.01, po.001.


5 .16, po.05). In contrast, greater parental acceptance<br />

was associated with greater disclosure about<br />

schoolwork (for both mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers) <strong>and</strong> personal<br />

issues (for mothers only, b 5 .12, when exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

separately for each parent). Older adolescents<br />

disclosed more about peer issues. Self-esteem, discussion<br />

frequency, <strong>and</strong> conflict <strong>in</strong>tensity were not<br />

significant <strong>in</strong> the analyses of disclosure. Overall, the<br />

variables accounted for 46%, 41%, <strong>and</strong> 38% of the<br />

variance <strong>in</strong> adolescents’ disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g personal,<br />

school, <strong>and</strong> peer issues, respectively.<br />

Unique correlates of adolescent secrecy. Younger adolescents<br />

reported greater secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g peers.<br />

Consistent with the previous analyses, adolescents<br />

who believed that they were more obligated to disclose<br />

to parents kept fewer secrets regard<strong>in</strong>g these<br />

issues. In contrast to the previous analyses, however,<br />

trust did not significantly <strong>in</strong>fluence adolescents’ secrecy<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g schoolwork (ow<strong>in</strong>g to a nonsignificant<br />

effect for trust <strong>in</strong> fathers, b 5 .10).<br />

As <strong>in</strong> the previous analyses, more parental psychological<br />

control was associated with less secrecy<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g personal <strong>and</strong> peer issues. More parental<br />

acceptance was associated with less secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

personal issues, better self-esteem was associated with<br />

less secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g schoolwork, <strong>and</strong>, as expected,<br />

adolescents who had more <strong>in</strong>tense conflicts with<br />

parents reported more secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g peer issues<br />

(for fathers only, b 5 .17). These variables accounted<br />

for 18%, 14%, <strong>and</strong> 17% of the variance <strong>in</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

personal, academic, <strong>and</strong> peer issues.<br />

Discussion<br />

This study extended our underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of disclosure<br />

<strong>and</strong> secrecy <strong>in</strong> parent – adolescent relationships <strong>in</strong><br />

several ways. First, us<strong>in</strong>g the framework of social<br />

doma<strong>in</strong> theory (Smetana, 1995, 2006; Turiel, 1983,<br />

1998), the study demonstrated that adolescents’ <strong>and</strong><br />

parents’ beliefs about adolescents’ obligations to<br />

disclose to parents were closely associated with their<br />

beliefs about parents’ legitimate authority to regulate<br />

acts <strong>in</strong> different social-cognitive doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> that<br />

beliefs about disclosure were strongly associated<br />

with adolescents’ actual disclosure. Second, the<br />

study elaborated on recent research on adolescent<br />

disclosure (Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000; Statt<strong>in</strong> & Kerr, 2000)<br />

by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between disclosure <strong>and</strong><br />

secrecy, by compar<strong>in</strong>g adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> parents’<br />

perceptions of adolescents’ disclosure, <strong>and</strong> by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

the unique <strong>in</strong>fluence of different correlates<br />

on adolescents’ disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />

different types of activities. These issues are discussed<br />

<strong>in</strong> turn below.<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 211<br />

Beliefs About Adolescents’ Obligations to Disclose to<br />

Parents<br />

This study demonstrated that adolescents <strong>and</strong><br />

parents made conceptual doma<strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ctions <strong>in</strong><br />

their judgments of adolescents’ obligations to tell<br />

parents about their activities <strong>and</strong> that, as hypothesized,<br />

parent – adolescent differences <strong>and</strong> adolescents’<br />

age (but not adolescents’ gender) moderated<br />

these perceptions. Parallel<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for judgments<br />

of the legitimacy of parental authority, both<br />

parents <strong>and</strong> adolescents viewed adolescents as more<br />

obligated to tell parents about their prudential behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> less obligated to disclose personal issues<br />

than any other type of issue. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

consistent with those of Perk<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Turiel (2005),<br />

who found that adolescents judged it to be acceptable<br />

to deceive parents about their moral <strong>and</strong> personal<br />

behavior but not their prudential behavior. Consistent<br />

with previous research (Smetana, 1988a, 2000;<br />

Smetana & Asquith, 1994), (comb<strong>in</strong>ed) moral <strong>and</strong><br />

conventional issues were judged to be more legitimately<br />

subject to parental authority than multifaceted<br />

<strong>and</strong> personal issues.<br />

This order<strong>in</strong>g was reversed, however, <strong>in</strong> adolescents’<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs of their obligations to disclose to parents;<br />

adolescents were seen as more obligated to tell<br />

parents about their behavior regard<strong>in</strong>g multifaceted<br />

than moral <strong>and</strong> conventional issues. Multifaceted<br />

issues (like watch<strong>in</strong>g an R-rated movie or see<strong>in</strong>g<br />

friends that parents don’t like) are at the boundaries<br />

between parental conventional regulation (or parental<br />

prudential concern) <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ personal<br />

jurisdiction <strong>and</strong>, as such, have been found to be<br />

major sources of conflict <strong>in</strong> adolescent – parent relationships<br />

(e.g., Smetana, 1988b, 1989; Smetana &<br />

Asquith, 1994). Therefore, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that<br />

both adolescents <strong>and</strong> parents viewed adolescents as<br />

more obligated to disclose these activities than moral<br />

or conventional behaviors, which, by adolescence,<br />

entail well-established <strong>and</strong> taken-for-granted expectations<br />

(Smetana & Daddis, 2002). Consistent with<br />

the def<strong>in</strong>ition of personal issues as private matters<br />

that are up to adolescents to decide (Nucci, 1996),<br />

adolescents did not perceive themselves as obligated<br />

to disclose personal issues to parents, whereas parents,<br />

on average, viewed their teens as at least<br />

sometimes obligated to tell.<br />

Unlike previous research (Fuligni, 1998; Smetana,<br />

1988a, 2000; Smetana & Asquith, 1994), age-related<br />

decl<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> judgments of parents’ legitimate authority<br />

to regulate acts <strong>in</strong> different doma<strong>in</strong>s were found only<br />

<strong>in</strong> parents’ but not adolescents’ judgments. This could<br />

be because this study focused on a more limited age


212 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

range than previous studies, which typically have<br />

spanned pre- or early to late adolescence. Furthermore,<br />

unlike the previous research, where beliefs<br />

about legitimate parental authority have been assessed<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g b<strong>in</strong>omial judgments, this study used a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Likert scale. Even though parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of their legitimate<br />

authority decl<strong>in</strong>ed significantly with adolescents’<br />

age, there were large (approximately 1-po<strong>in</strong>t)<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> parents’ <strong>and</strong> 12th graders’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Parallel f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>gs of obligations<br />

to disclose to parents. Parents consistently<br />

viewed adolescents as more obligated to disclose<br />

their behavior <strong>in</strong> each doma<strong>in</strong> than adolescents<br />

perceived, although discrepancies between parents’<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents’ judgments decreased with adolescents’<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g age, mostly because of age-related<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> parents’ beliefs. Unlike the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for<br />

legitimacy, however, <strong>and</strong> most likely reflect<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

taken-for-granted nature of these expectations<br />

(Smetana & Daddis, 2002), we did f<strong>in</strong>d that with age,<br />

adolescents viewed themselves as less obligated to<br />

disclose to parents about their moral <strong>and</strong> conventional<br />

behavior. As expected, rat<strong>in</strong>gs of adolescents’<br />

obligations to disclose to parents <strong>and</strong> the legitimacy<br />

of parental authority were strongly associated, but<br />

the strength of the correlations also <strong>in</strong>dicated that<br />

these constructs are not identical.<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

As Crouter <strong>and</strong> Head (2002) have recommended,<br />

we exam<strong>in</strong>ed adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure <strong>and</strong><br />

secrecy separately for mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. Although<br />

these rat<strong>in</strong>gs were very highly associated, there were<br />

significant mean level differences, which were further<br />

moderated by type of issue <strong>and</strong>, as others have<br />

found, adolescents’ sex (Bumpus et al., 2001; Crouter<br />

et al., 1999; Noller & Callan, 1990; Waizenhofer et al.,<br />

2004; Youniss & Smollar, 1985). Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

consistent with previous research <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that<br />

both boys <strong>and</strong> girls, but particularly girls, have<br />

closer, more supportive relationships with mothers<br />

than with fathers (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992) <strong>and</strong>,<br />

more specifically, that girls confide more <strong>in</strong> mothers<br />

than <strong>in</strong> fathers (Noller & Callan, 1990; Youniss &<br />

Smollar, 1985) about schoolwork <strong>and</strong> less often about<br />

personal issues. Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs did not support the<br />

hypothesis that boys would voluntarily disclose<br />

more about personal issues to fathers than mothers;<br />

boys <strong>and</strong> girls <strong>in</strong> this study did not differ <strong>in</strong> their<br />

disclosure or secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g any of the issues with<br />

fathers. However, consistent with previous research<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that mother – daughter relationships are<br />

closer <strong>and</strong> more supportive than mother – son rela-<br />

tionships (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992), we found<br />

that boys were more secretive about personal issues<br />

with their mothers than were girls, whereas girls<br />

disclosed more about personal issues (<strong>and</strong> also<br />

schoolwork) to mothers than did boys. Furthermore,<br />

mothers overestimated this disclosure <strong>and</strong> rated<br />

their daughters as more disclos<strong>in</strong>g than daughters<br />

actually were (although no differences between<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> sons emerged). It has been hypothesized<br />

that mothers identify more with their daughters<br />

than their sons (Chodorow, 1978). In fact, we<br />

found that girls did disclose personal issues more to<br />

mothers than did boys, which may lead mothers to<br />

overestimate parent – adolescent similarities. In addition,<br />

parents have a greater ‘‘generational stake’’<br />

(Bengston & Kuypers, 1971) <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>uity<br />

between generations, whereas adolescents, <strong>in</strong><br />

contrast, are attempt<strong>in</strong>g to differentiate from parents.<br />

Although parents viewed adolescents as <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly<br />

less obligated to disclose about multifaceted<br />

(<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g peer <strong>and</strong> friendship) issues with age, it<br />

was surpris<strong>in</strong>g that 12th graders actually disclosed<br />

more about these issues than did 9th graders. This<br />

may be because conformity to peers, particularly<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g antisocial issues, peaks <strong>in</strong> 9th grade<br />

(Berndt, 1979; Sim & Koh, 2003). Thus, 9th graders’<br />

secretiveness about these issues may reflect their<br />

greater susceptibility to peer pressure at this age. As<br />

the peer issues <strong>in</strong>cluded an item perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to dat<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

it is also possible that adolescents become more<br />

will<strong>in</strong>g to talk to parents about their romantic <strong>in</strong>terests<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer relationships as they get older. F<strong>in</strong>ally,<br />

it is unclear why 12th-grade boys were more secretive<br />

about schoolwork than either same-age girls or<br />

younger boys. The 12th graders <strong>in</strong> this study did<br />

report lower academic performance (as assessed by<br />

grade po<strong>in</strong>t averages) than did 9th graders, although<br />

no sex differences <strong>in</strong> academic performance emerged<br />

<strong>in</strong> our sample.<br />

Correlates of <strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

The results of this study did not provide strong<br />

support for F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al.’s (2002) claim that<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy are conceptually dist<strong>in</strong>ct. As<br />

expected, secrecy <strong>and</strong> disclosure were significantly<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>versely correlated. Although the strength of<br />

the association <strong>in</strong>dicated that these measures are not<br />

identical, they did have relatively similar correlates.<br />

Consistent with the previous research (F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et<br />

al., 2002; Kerr et al., 1999), we found that a context of<br />

mutual trust facilitates adolescents’ disclosure to<br />

parents over all of the issues exam<strong>in</strong>ed here. In contrast,<br />

secrecy over personal <strong>and</strong> peer issues was


associated with less trust <strong>in</strong> the parent – adolescent<br />

relationship, whereas secrecy over schoolwork was<br />

associated with lower self-esteem. <strong>Secrecy</strong> over peer<br />

issues was also uniquely associated with greater<br />

conflict <strong>in</strong>tensity. Thus, like F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2002),<br />

we found that disclosure has positive implications<br />

<strong>and</strong> secrecy has negative implications for adjustment.<br />

However, we did not f<strong>in</strong>d that secrecy has positive<br />

benefits dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence, although our assessment<br />

of adjustment was more limited than theirs.<br />

It should be noted that we assessed adolescents’<br />

perceptions of trust <strong>in</strong> parents, whereas Kerr <strong>and</strong><br />

colleagues (Kerr et al., 1999; Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000)<br />

assessed adolescents’ perceptions of parents’ trust <strong>in</strong><br />

them (which we assessed directly here through<br />

parents’ reports). Although there were significant<br />

bivariate associations between parents’ trust <strong>in</strong> their<br />

teens <strong>and</strong> adolescent-reported disclosure (as well as<br />

between adolescent- <strong>and</strong> parent-rated trust), adolescents’<br />

trust <strong>in</strong> parents was more strongly associated<br />

with disclosure than parents’ trust <strong>in</strong> their child.<br />

This may reflect an <strong>in</strong>formant bias, but it is also <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to note that parents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of trust <strong>in</strong> their<br />

adolescent were not significantly associated with<br />

two of the three measures of secrecy.<br />

Furthermore, consistent with other research exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

self-disclosure as an aspect of <strong>and</strong> route<br />

toward achiev<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>timacy (Reis & Shaver, 1988),<br />

F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2002, 2005) exam<strong>in</strong>ed self-disclosure<br />

<strong>in</strong> terms of reveal<strong>in</strong>g private thoughts <strong>and</strong><br />

feel<strong>in</strong>gs. Along with other researchers who have<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>ed disclosure as an aspect of parental<br />

knowledge (Crouter & Head, 2002), however, this<br />

research focused on adolescents’ voluntary disclosure<br />

of activities (although one personal item,<br />

‘‘hav<strong>in</strong>g a ‘crush’ on a girl or boy,’’ referred to feel<strong>in</strong>gs).<br />

Our research suggests that it is important to<br />

specify the issues that are kept secret or disclosed, as<br />

they have somewhat different correlates.<br />

Differences were particularly evident for parents’<br />

rat<strong>in</strong>gs of acceptance <strong>and</strong> psychological control.<br />

Greater parental acceptance was associated with<br />

more disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues <strong>and</strong><br />

schoolwork <strong>and</strong> less secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues.<br />

As the moderate association (r 5.43) between<br />

parental acceptance <strong>and</strong> parent-rated trust suggests,<br />

acceptance partially <strong>in</strong>dexes parents’ warmth <strong>and</strong><br />

support, which was shown to have a positive association<br />

with disclosure. As adolescents (<strong>and</strong> to a<br />

lesser extent, parents) viewed disclosure over personal<br />

issues as discretionary rather than obligatory,<br />

acceptance on the part of the parent may be particularly<br />

important <strong>in</strong> creat<strong>in</strong>g a climate where adolescents<br />

feel comfortable <strong>in</strong> reveal<strong>in</strong>g personal issues.<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 213<br />

Previous research has shown that overcontrol of<br />

the personal doma<strong>in</strong> leads to greater feel<strong>in</strong>gs of parental<br />

psychological control (Smetana & Daddis,<br />

2002) <strong>and</strong> more <strong>in</strong>ternaliz<strong>in</strong>g symptoms (Hasebe et<br />

al., 2004). Contrary to our hypotheses, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

from this study further suggest that greater parental<br />

psychological control was associated with more<br />

disclosure (<strong>and</strong> less secrecy) regard<strong>in</strong>g personal issues<br />

<strong>and</strong> less secrecy regard<strong>in</strong>g peer issues. We assessed<br />

disclosure <strong>in</strong> terms of how much ‘‘adolescents<br />

usually tell, without parents ask<strong>in</strong>g’’ about different<br />

issues. Thus, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for psychological control<br />

raise the <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g possibility that adolescents with<br />

parents who use more psychological control may feel<br />

subtly coerced to disclose personal issues without<br />

parents overtly ask<strong>in</strong>g; this issue bears further <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

<strong>in</strong> future research. Darl<strong>in</strong>g et al. (2004)<br />

have exam<strong>in</strong>ed different disclosure strategies, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

full disclosure, partial disclosure (e.g.,<br />

omitt<strong>in</strong>g some details), ly<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> avoidance; future<br />

research should exam<strong>in</strong>e whether parental psychological<br />

control is differentially associated with adolescents’<br />

use of these strategies. In addition, <strong>in</strong> this<br />

study, psychological control was assessed us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

parent reports; adolescents’ reports of parent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

were not obta<strong>in</strong>ed. As different <strong>in</strong>formants’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of parent<strong>in</strong>g are typically only moderately correlated<br />

(Pettit, Laird, Bates, Dodge, & Criss, 2001; Smetana &<br />

Daddis, 2002), future research should determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

whether adolescents’ perceptions of parental psychological<br />

control are also associated with greater<br />

disclosure, more secrecy, <strong>and</strong> use of different disclosure<br />

strategies.<br />

Limitations of this Study <strong>and</strong> Future Directions<br />

A strength of this study was that we compared<br />

adolescents’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of disclosure to mothers with<br />

mothers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of their adolescents, yield<strong>in</strong>g a more<br />

precise assessment of parent – child similarities <strong>and</strong><br />

differences than is typically obta<strong>in</strong>ed when students<br />

are asked to rate ‘‘parents.’’ However, because of the<br />

number of s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent families <strong>in</strong> our sample <strong>and</strong><br />

the difficulty of enroll<strong>in</strong>g fathers <strong>in</strong> research (Parke &<br />

Buriel, 1998), we only obta<strong>in</strong>ed responses from one<br />

parent per family. In future research, both mothers’<br />

<strong>and</strong> fathers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of adolescent disclosure should<br />

be obta<strong>in</strong>ed so that <strong>in</strong>teractions between parents’<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents’ gender <strong>in</strong> disclosure can be more<br />

fully exam<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Ow<strong>in</strong>g to the task dem<strong>and</strong>s (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the time<br />

needed to complete the surveys <strong>and</strong> the redundancy<br />

of ask<strong>in</strong>g so many questions repeatedly about the<br />

same set of items), we did not obta<strong>in</strong> adolescents’


214 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

parent-differentiated perceptions of beliefs about the<br />

legitimacy of parents’ authority or adolescents’ obligations<br />

to disclose. It would be worthwhile <strong>in</strong> future<br />

research to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether adolescents’<br />

beliefs about these issues differ when consider<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers. Likewise, we did not obta<strong>in</strong> an<br />

assessment of parents’ perceptions of their adolescents’<br />

secrecy, because of task dem<strong>and</strong>s as well as<br />

concerns about whether parents know what they do<br />

not know. Future research should exam<strong>in</strong>e parents’<br />

knowledge of or perceptions of adolescents’ secrecy.<br />

In this study, we restricted our assessment of<br />

disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy to everyday activities, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

personal, peer, <strong>and</strong> schoolwork issues. It<br />

would be especially important <strong>in</strong> future research to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g a broader set of issues,<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g prudential behaviors (such as drug <strong>and</strong><br />

alcohol use, cigarette smok<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> sexual activity)<br />

as well as other doma<strong>in</strong>s of misbehavior (such as<br />

cutt<strong>in</strong>g classes or bully<strong>in</strong>g). We did not obta<strong>in</strong> assessments<br />

of disclosure regard<strong>in</strong>g these behaviors<br />

because our pilot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dicated the need to differentiate<br />

between adolescents who engage <strong>in</strong> these behaviors<br />

but do not tell their parents <strong>and</strong> adolescents<br />

who are not <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the behaviors (<strong>and</strong> thus<br />

have noth<strong>in</strong>g to tell). Future research focus<strong>in</strong>g on<br />

normative samples of adolescents will need to disentangle<br />

these issues carefully. Previous research has<br />

shown that parents solicit more <strong>in</strong>formation when<br />

their adolescents are more <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> problem behavior<br />

(Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000; Tilton-Weaver & Galambos,<br />

2003), but it would also be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e the correlates of adolescents’ disclosure<br />

over these aspects of adolescents’ behavior. Tak<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

different approach, Darl<strong>in</strong>g et al. (2004) exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

how much adolescents disclose about issues of conflict.<br />

However, this may provide an <strong>in</strong>complete assessment<br />

of disclosure, as there may be little conflict<br />

about issues that are successfully concealed. For <strong>in</strong>stance,<br />

our results <strong>in</strong>dicated that conflict <strong>in</strong>tensity<br />

was related to greater secrecy about peer issues, but<br />

not personal or schoolwork issues.<br />

The correlates exam<strong>in</strong>ed here accounted for a<br />

sizable proportion of the variance <strong>in</strong> disclosure, but<br />

much less of the variance <strong>in</strong> secrecy. Thus, future<br />

research needs to better identify the correlates of<br />

secrecy, especially as F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al. (2005) has<br />

found that concealment <strong>in</strong> their sample had significant<br />

but low associations with different dimensions<br />

of parent<strong>in</strong>g. Whereas disclosure may reflect normative<br />

developmental processes of <strong>in</strong>dividuation,<br />

the association between poor self-esteem <strong>and</strong> greater<br />

secrecy (at least over schoolwork) suggests that secrecy<br />

may have its roots <strong>in</strong> (mal)adjustment. This<br />

hypothesis should be exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> future research<br />

that <strong>in</strong>cludes a broader range of adjustment measures;<br />

this might better illum<strong>in</strong>ate F<strong>in</strong>kenauer et al.’s<br />

(2002) claim that disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy are dist<strong>in</strong>ct.<br />

Longitud<strong>in</strong>al research is needed to exam<strong>in</strong>e the<br />

developmental trajectories <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terrelationships<br />

among beliefs about parental authority <strong>and</strong> adolescents’<br />

obligations to disclose to parents <strong>and</strong> actual<br />

disclosure. We hypothesize that authority beliefs <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

beliefs about adolescents’ obligation to disclose<br />

to parents, which <strong>in</strong> turn <strong>in</strong>fluence disclosure.<br />

As these constructs were assessed concurrently <strong>in</strong><br />

this study, however, the causal directions <strong>and</strong> mediat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(or potentially moderat<strong>in</strong>g) relationships<br />

among the variables cannot be determ<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>and</strong> secrecy should also be exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

<strong>in</strong> a broader age range of adolescents, <strong>and</strong> adolescents’<br />

justifications for disclosure or secrecy <strong>in</strong> different<br />

doma<strong>in</strong>s should be <strong>in</strong>vestigated. Adolescents<br />

may have different reasons for not disclos<strong>in</strong>g or<br />

conceal<strong>in</strong>g issues <strong>in</strong> the personal versus other doma<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong> these reasons may be differentially related<br />

to adjustment. Furthermore, as Darl<strong>in</strong>g et al.<br />

(2004) have found, disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy are only<br />

two of several possible strategies adolescents may<br />

have for manag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, <strong>and</strong> more research<br />

on adolescents’ strategies for manag<strong>in</strong>g different<br />

types of issues is needed.<br />

Although our participation rates were good <strong>and</strong><br />

our sample appeared to be representative of the<br />

larger school district from which it was drawn, previous<br />

research has <strong>in</strong>dicated that well-function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

families are overrepresented <strong>in</strong> family research, especially<br />

when active <strong>in</strong>formed consent procedures<br />

are used (We<strong>in</strong>berger, Tubl<strong>in</strong>, Ford, & Feldman,<br />

1990), as was done here. Thus, future research might<br />

<strong>in</strong>clude both normative <strong>and</strong> at-risk youth. Furthermore,<br />

future research should exam<strong>in</strong>e disclosure <strong>and</strong><br />

secrecy <strong>in</strong> different ethnic <strong>and</strong> racial groups, as it is<br />

likely that disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy <strong>in</strong> adolescent –<br />

parent relationships may have different mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong><br />

different cultural contexts.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, the results of this study have implications<br />

for the recent debates about parental knowledge <strong>and</strong><br />

parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g. It has been claimed (Crouter &<br />

Head, 2002; Kerr & Statt<strong>in</strong>, 2000) that most measures<br />

of parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g are actually measures of parental<br />

knowledge, which come primarily from adolescent<br />

disclosure <strong>in</strong> the context of trust<strong>in</strong>g parent –<br />

child relationships. Our results confirm the salience<br />

of trust <strong>in</strong> adolescent disclosure, but we also found<br />

that, even <strong>in</strong> good relationships, parents overestimate<br />

their adolescents’ disclosure, particularly at<br />

middle adolescence <strong>and</strong> with regard to peers. This


suggests the need for further specification <strong>and</strong> more<br />

precise measurement of ‘‘parental knowledge.’’ For<br />

<strong>in</strong>stance, as our results <strong>in</strong>dicate that parents th<strong>in</strong>k<br />

they know more than they do, parent reports of<br />

knowledge may be of limited value. On the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, much research has asked adolescents ‘‘how<br />

much your parents really know’’ about the youth’s<br />

activities. Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that it might be better<br />

to more directly assess how much adolescents disclose<br />

to parents, rather than ask<strong>in</strong>g adolescents to<br />

estimate how much their parents know. Furthermore,<br />

researchers have not paid sufficient attention<br />

to the types of items used to assess parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g<br />

or knowledge. Future research should dist<strong>in</strong>guish<br />

between issues that adolescents feel obligated<br />

to disclose (like prudential or peer issues) <strong>and</strong> personal<br />

issues, where disclosure is discretionary, as our<br />

results <strong>in</strong>dicate that there are age-related, conceptual,<br />

<strong>and</strong> generational differences <strong>in</strong> the way parents<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents th<strong>in</strong>k about these different dimensions<br />

of disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy.<br />

References<br />

Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The <strong>in</strong>ventory<br />

of parent <strong>and</strong> peer attachment: Individual differences<br />

<strong>and</strong> their relationship to psychological well be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> adolescence. Journal of Youth <strong>and</strong> Adolescence, 16,<br />

427 – 454.<br />

Bengston, V. L., & Kuypers, J. A. (1971). Generational difference<br />

<strong>and</strong> the developmental stake. Ag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Human<br />

Development, 2, 249 – 260.<br />

Berndt, T. (1979). Developmental changes <strong>in</strong> conformity<br />

to peers <strong>and</strong> parents. Developmental Psychology, 15,<br />

608 – 616.<br />

Buhrmester, D., & Prager, K. (1995). Patterns <strong>and</strong> functions<br />

of self-disclosure dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescence. In<br />

K. Rotenberg (Ed.), <strong>Disclosure</strong> processes <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents (pp. 10 – 56). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Bumpus, M. F., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (2001).<br />

Parental autonomy grant<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence: Explor<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gender differences <strong>in</strong> context. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 37, 164 – 173.<br />

Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mother<strong>in</strong>g. Berkeley:<br />

University of California Press.<br />

Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> knowledge of children. In M. Bornste<strong>in</strong> (Ed.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of parent<strong>in</strong>g, 2nd ed., Vol. 3: Becom<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

parent (pp. 461 – 483). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erikson, H., Updegraff, K., &<br />

McHale, S. M. (1999). Conditions underly<strong>in</strong>g parents’<br />

knowledge about children’s daily lives <strong>in</strong> middle<br />

childhood: Between- <strong>and</strong> with<strong>in</strong>-family comparisons.<br />

Child Development, 70, 246 – 249.<br />

Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Be<strong>in</strong>g adolescent.<br />

New York: Basic Books.<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 215<br />

Darl<strong>in</strong>g, N., Cumsille, P., Hames, K., & Caldwell, L. L.<br />

(2004). Adolescents as active agents <strong>in</strong> the monitor<strong>in</strong>g process;<br />

disclosure strategies <strong>and</strong> motivations. Unpublished<br />

paper, Pennsylvania State University, College Park.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>kenauer, C., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Meeus, W. (2002).<br />

Keep<strong>in</strong>g secrets from parents: Advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages<br />

of secrecy <strong>in</strong> adolescence. Journal of Youth <strong>and</strong><br />

Adolescence, 2, 123 – 136.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>kenauer, C., Frijns, T., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Kerkhof, P.<br />

(2005). Perceiv<strong>in</strong>g concealment <strong>in</strong> relationships between<br />

parents <strong>and</strong> adolescents: L<strong>in</strong>ks with parental behavior.<br />

Personal <strong>Relationships</strong>, 12, 387 – 406.<br />

Fletcher, A., Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L., & Williams, M. (2004). Parental<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluences on adolescent problem behavior: Revisit<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Statt<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Kerr. Child Development, 75, 781 – 796.<br />

Fuligni, A. J. (1998). Authority, autonomy, <strong>and</strong> parent –<br />

adolescent conflict <strong>and</strong> cohesion: A study of adolescents<br />

from Mexican, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Filip<strong>in</strong>o, <strong>and</strong> European backgrounds.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 34, 782 – 792.<br />

Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age <strong>and</strong> sex <strong>in</strong><br />

perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child<br />

Development, 63, 103 – 115.<br />

Hasebe, Y., Nucci, L., & Nucci, M. S. (2004). Parental control<br />

of the personal doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> adolescent symptoms of<br />

psychopathology. Child Development, 75, 815 – 828.<br />

Hether<strong>in</strong>gton, E. M. (1993). An overview of the Virg<strong>in</strong>ia<br />

longitud<strong>in</strong>al study of divorce <strong>and</strong> remarriage with a<br />

focus on early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology,<br />

7, 39 – 56.<br />

Kerr, M., & Statt<strong>in</strong>, H. (2000). What parents know, how they<br />

know it, <strong>and</strong> several forms of adolescent adjustment:<br />

Further support for a re<strong>in</strong>terpretation of monitor<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 36, 366 – 380.<br />

Kerr, M., Statt<strong>in</strong>, H., & Trost, K. (1999). To know you is to<br />

trust you: Parents’ trust is rooted <strong>in</strong> child disclosure of<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 737 – 752.<br />

Larson, R., Richards, M., Moneta, G., Holmbeck, G., &<br />

Duckett, E. (1996). Changes <strong>in</strong> adolescents’ daily <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

with their families from ages 10 – 18: Disengagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> transformation. Developmental Psychology,<br />

32, 744 – 754.<br />

Marshall, S. K., Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Bosdet, L. (2005).<br />

Information management: Consider<strong>in</strong>g adolescents’<br />

regulation of parental knowledge. Journal of Adolescence,<br />

22, 633 – 647.<br />

Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1992). The 1989 socioeconomic <strong>in</strong>dex of<br />

occupations: Construction from the 1989 occupational prestige<br />

scores (General Social Survey: Methodological Report No.<br />

74). Chicago: University of Chicago, National Op<strong>in</strong>ion<br />

Research Center.<br />

Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1990). Adolescents’ perceptions of<br />

the nature of their communication with parents. Journal<br />

of Youth <strong>and</strong> Adolescence, 19, 349 – 362.<br />

Nucci, L. P. (1996). Morality <strong>and</strong> personal freedom. In E. S.<br />

Reed, E. Turiel, & T. Brown (Eds.), Values <strong>and</strong> knowledge<br />

(pp. 41 – 60). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (1998). Socialization <strong>in</strong> the family:<br />

Ethnic <strong>and</strong> ecological perspectives. In N. Eisenberg


216 Smetana, Metzger, Gettman, <strong>and</strong> Campione-Barr<br />

(Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology, Fifth Edition, Vol. 3<br />

Social, emotional, <strong>and</strong> personality development (W. Damon,<br />

Series Editor; pp. 463 – 552). New York: Wiley.<br />

Perk<strong>in</strong>s, S. A., & Turiel, E. (2005). To lie or not to lie: To whom<br />

<strong>and</strong> under what circumstances. Unpublished manuscript,<br />

University of California, Berkeley.<br />

Pettit, G. S., Laird, R. D., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., &<br />

Criss, M. M. (2001). Antecedents <strong>and</strong> behavior-problem<br />

outcomes of parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> psychological<br />

control <strong>in</strong> early adolescence. Child Development, 72,<br />

583 – 598.<br />

Pr<strong>in</strong>z, R. J., Foster, S. L., Kent, R. N., & O’Leary, K. D.<br />

(1979). Multivariate assessment of conflict <strong>in</strong> distressed<br />

<strong>and</strong> nondistressed mother – adolescent dyads. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 691 – 700.<br />

Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

process. In S. Duck (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of research <strong>in</strong> personal<br />

relationships (pp. 367 – 289). London: Wiley.<br />

Rob<strong>in</strong>, A. L., & Foster, S. I. (1989). Negotiat<strong>in</strong>g parent – adolescent<br />

conflict: A behavioral family systems approach. New<br />

York: Guilford.<br />

Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiv<strong>in</strong>g the self. Melbourne, FL:<br />

Krieger.<br />

Ryan, R. M., & Lynch, J. H. (1989). Emotional autonomy<br />

versus detachment: Revisit<strong>in</strong>g the vicissitudes of adolescence<br />

<strong>and</strong> young adulthood. Child Development, 60,<br />

340 – 356.<br />

Schaefer, E. S. (1965a). A configurational analysis of children’s<br />

reports of parental behaviors: An <strong>in</strong>ventory. Child<br />

Development, 36, 417 – 424.<br />

Schaefer, E. S. (1965b). Children’s reports of parent behaviors.<br />

Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 29, 552 – 557.<br />

Schludermann, S., & Schludermann, E. (1970). Replicability<br />

of factors <strong>in</strong> Children’s Report of Parent Behavior<br />

(CRPBI). Journal of Psychology, 39, 39 – 52.<br />

Sim, T., & Koh, S. (2003). A doma<strong>in</strong> conceptualization of<br />

adolescent susceptibility to peer pressure. Journal of Research<br />

on Adolescence, 13, 57 – 80.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (1988a). Adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> parents’ conceptions<br />

of parental authority. Child Development, 59,<br />

321 – 335.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (1988b). Concepts of self <strong>and</strong> social convention:<br />

Adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> parents’ reason<strong>in</strong>g about hypothetical<br />

<strong>and</strong> actual family conflicts. In M. Gunnar &<br />

W. A. Coll<strong>in</strong>s (Eds.), 21st M<strong>in</strong>nesota Symposium on Child<br />

Psychology: Development dur<strong>in</strong>g the transition to adolescence<br />

(pp. 79 – 122). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (1989). Adolescents’ <strong>and</strong> parents’ reason<strong>in</strong>g<br />

about actual family conflict. Child Development, 60,<br />

1052 – 1067.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (1995). Morality <strong>in</strong> context: Abstractions,<br />

ambiguities, <strong>and</strong> applications. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of<br />

child development (Vol. 10, pp. 83 – 130). London: Jessica<br />

K<strong>in</strong>gsley.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (2000). Middle-class African American adolescents’<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents’ conceptions of parental authority<br />

<strong>and</strong> parent<strong>in</strong>g practices: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>vestigation.<br />

Child Development, 71, 1672 – 1686.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (2006). Social doma<strong>in</strong> theory: Consistencies<br />

<strong>and</strong> variations <strong>in</strong> children’s moral <strong>and</strong> social judgments.<br />

In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of moral<br />

development (pp. 119 – 153). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Smetana, J. G., & Asquith, P. (1994). Adolescents’ <strong>and</strong><br />

parents’ conceptions of parental authority <strong>and</strong> adolescent<br />

autonomy. Child Development, 65, 1147 – 1162.<br />

Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Daddis, C. (2004).<br />

Developmental <strong>and</strong> longitud<strong>in</strong>al antecedents of family<br />

decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g: Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g health behavioral autonomy<br />

for African American adolescents. Child Development, 75,<br />

1418 – 1434.<br />

Smetana, J. G., & Daddis, C. (2002). Doma<strong>in</strong>-specific antecedents<br />

of psychological control <strong>and</strong> parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

The role of parent<strong>in</strong>g beliefs <strong>and</strong> practices. Child<br />

Development, 73, 563 – 580.<br />

Statt<strong>in</strong>, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitor<strong>in</strong>g: A re<strong>in</strong>terpretation.<br />

Child Development, 71, 1072 – 1085.<br />

Statt<strong>in</strong>, H., Kerr, M., & Ferrer-Wreder, L. (2000). Adolescents’<br />

secret lives: Why do they hide what they’re do<strong>in</strong>g from their<br />

parents? Paper presented at the Biennial Meet<strong>in</strong>gs of the<br />

Society for Research <strong>in</strong> Adolescence, Chicago, IL.<br />

Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L., & Silverberg, S. (1986). The vicissitudes of<br />

autonomy <strong>in</strong> early adolescence. Child Development, 57,<br />

841 – 851.<br />

Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Galambos, N. L. (2003). Adolescents’<br />

characteristics <strong>and</strong> parents’ beliefs as predictors of<br />

parents’ peer management behaviors. Journal of Research<br />

on Adolescence, 13, 269 – 300.<br />

Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality<br />

<strong>and</strong> convention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Turiel, E. (1998). Moral development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology, 5th ed., Vol. 3: Social, emotional,<br />

<strong>and</strong> personality development (William Damon (Series<br />

Editor); pp. 863 – 932). New York: Wiley.<br />

Waizenhofer, R. N., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-Newsom,<br />

J. (2004). Mothers’ <strong>and</strong> fathers’ knowledge of adolescents’<br />

daily activities: Its sources <strong>and</strong> its l<strong>in</strong>ks with adolescent<br />

adjustment. Journal of Marriage <strong>and</strong> the Family,<br />

18, 348 – 360.<br />

We<strong>in</strong>berger, D., Tubl<strong>in</strong>, S., Ford, M., & Feldman, S. (1990).<br />

Preadolescents’ social-emotional adjustment <strong>and</strong> selective<br />

attrition <strong>in</strong> family research. Child Development, 61,<br />

1374 – 1386.<br />

Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescents’ relations with<br />

mothers, fathers, <strong>and</strong> friends. Chicago: University of Chicago<br />

Press.


Appendix A<br />

Table A1<br />

Stimulus Items for Assessments of Authority <strong>and</strong> <strong>Disclosure</strong> Beliefs, Actual <strong>Disclosure</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Secrecy</strong><br />

Beliefs about legitimate authority <strong>and</strong> disclosure Actual disclosure <strong>and</strong> secrecy<br />

Prudential (risk) Prudential (schoolwork)<br />

Dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol Gett<strong>in</strong>g a bad grade/not do<strong>in</strong>g well on work or tests<br />

Smok<strong>in</strong>g cigarettes F<strong>in</strong>ish<strong>in</strong>g homework or assignments<br />

Go<strong>in</strong>g to a party where alcohol is served How teens are do<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> different school subjects<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g marijuana or other illicit drugs Do<strong>in</strong>g particularly well on an assignment or test<br />

Socially regulated (moral <strong>and</strong> conventional)<br />

How friends are treated (teas<strong>in</strong>g, say<strong>in</strong>g mean th<strong>in</strong>gs)<br />

Spread<strong>in</strong>g rumors or say<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g cruel<br />

Keep<strong>in</strong>g promises to others (ly<strong>in</strong>g or dishonesty)<br />

Talk<strong>in</strong>g back or be<strong>in</strong>g rude to a teacher <strong>in</strong> class<br />

Type of language (curs<strong>in</strong>g or swear<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

<strong>Disclosure</strong> <strong>in</strong> Adolescence 217<br />

Multifaceted<br />

Websites visited on the Internet<br />

Multifaceted (peer)<br />

Types of movies watched or CDs listened to (watch<strong>in</strong>g or listen<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to R-rated movies or CDs)<br />

Hang<strong>in</strong>g out at a friend’s when no adult is home<br />

Hang<strong>in</strong>g out with friends parents don’t like Spend<strong>in</strong>g time with someone parents don’t like<br />

If or who teens are dat<strong>in</strong>g If or who teens are dat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Personal Personal<br />

How teens spend their free time How teens spend their free time<br />

How teens spend their own money How teens spend their own money<br />

What teens talk about on the phone with friends What teens talk about on the phone with friends<br />

What teens write <strong>in</strong> e-mails, letters, or journals What teens write <strong>in</strong> e-mails, letters, or journals<br />

Who teens like or have a crush on Who teens like or have a crush on

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!