20.07.2013 Views

Patterned Exceptions in Phonology - UCLA Department of Linguistics

Patterned Exceptions in Phonology - UCLA Department of Linguistics

Patterned Exceptions in Phonology - UCLA Department of Linguistics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

proposal that “overapplication” <strong>of</strong> nasal substitution <strong>in</strong> F=-E-ECD=JE( results from<br />

reduplicative correspondence. Note that the overapplication shows that a nasal result<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from substitution belongs to the stem (although it may also belong to the prefix <strong>in</strong> some<br />

sense; see the discussion <strong>of</strong> coalescence <strong>in</strong> §2.4), whereas a prefix nasal that merely<br />

assimilates is not part <strong>of</strong> the stem.<br />

Second, it is not clear whether nasal substitution is possible on nasal-<strong>in</strong>itial stems:<br />

nasal-<strong>in</strong>itial stems are rare to beg<strong>in</strong> with, and among those that do exist, it is not always<br />

possible to tell what the prefix is. For example, <strong>in</strong> ma-=DE(@ ‘to become numb’, from<br />

=DE(@ ‘numb’, it is not clear whether the prefix is simply ma- (which can also form<br />

verbs, with similar semantics), or =- with nasal substitution. 17 There do exist<br />

unambiguous constructions (such as =+REDUPLICATION—there is no potentially<br />

confusable ma+REDUP), but no cases <strong>of</strong> nasal-<strong>in</strong>itial stems <strong>in</strong> these constructions.<br />

Third, glottal stop is problematic. Many researchers have assumed that <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

glottal stop <strong>in</strong> Tagalog is simply predictably <strong>in</strong>serted <strong>in</strong> vowel-<strong>in</strong>itial words (s<strong>in</strong>ce there<br />

are no strictly vowel-<strong>in</strong>itial words); the preservation <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial glottal stop <strong>in</strong> prefixed<br />

words like mag-=(M= ‘to fight’ (or =K() would then be regarded as the effect <strong>of</strong> a<br />

tendency to align morpheme boundaries with syllable boundaries (for a formal theory <strong>of</strong><br />

alignment, see McCarthy & Pr<strong>in</strong>ce 1993, Cohn & McCarthy 1998). And a word like<br />

17 Schachter and Otanes (1972) argue that these verbs are =-prefixed, because their gerunds are formed<br />

by chang<strong>in</strong>g m to p and reduplicat<strong>in</strong>g, as are the gerunds <strong>of</strong> uncontroversially =-prefixed verbs (J=J<br />

‘fear’, ma-=(J ‘to <strong>in</strong>timidate’, pa-na-=(J ‘<strong>in</strong>timidat<strong>in</strong>g’). In contrast, ma- verbs’ gerunds are formed<br />

by replac<strong>in</strong>g ma- with pagka- (=->K( ‘to get <strong>in</strong>volved’, F=C=->K( ‘gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volved’). But Carrier<br />

(1979) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that some m → p & RCV gerunds do come from ma- verbs (pa-li-E(C ‘bath<strong>in</strong>g’ from ma-<br />

E(C ‘to take a bath’).<br />

Carrier (1979) argues aga<strong>in</strong>st the =-with-substitution analysis for nasal-<strong>in</strong>itial stems, because<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the nasal-<strong>in</strong>itial stems that take ma-/=- do not substitute when comb<strong>in</strong>ed with F=-, and so<br />

should not substitute with =- (F=-(@ ‘for watch<strong>in</strong>g’). But, I have found many stems that substitute<br />

with =- but not with F=- (>KJ(J ‘tail end’, ma-KJ(J ‘to f<strong>in</strong>ish last’, pam->KJ(J ‘tailpiece’).<br />

21

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!