01.08.2013 Views

A Large-Scale Study of the Characteristics of Asperger Syndrome

A Large-Scale Study of the Characteristics of Asperger Syndrome

A Large-Scale Study of the Characteristics of Asperger Syndrome

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2007, 42(4), 448–459<br />

© Division on Developmental Disabilities<br />

A <strong>Large</strong>-<strong>Scale</strong> <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong><br />

Brenda Smith Myles, Hyo Jung Lee,<br />

Sheila M. Smith, Kai-Chien Tien,<br />

Yu-Chi Chou,<br />

and Terri Cooper Swanson<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Kansas<br />

Jill Hudson<br />

Ohio Center for Autism and Low Incidence<br />

Abstract: This article presents <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> a large-scale study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> 156 individuals with<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> (AS) ages 12 to 18. Specifically, cognitive (intellectual, empathizing, systemizing), adaptive<br />

behavior, behavior, temperament, and sensory pr<strong>of</strong>iles <strong>of</strong> study participants are overviewed. These characteristics<br />

are discussed as <strong>the</strong>y relate to diagnostic criteria and instructional planning for adolescents with AS.<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> (AS) is a developmental<br />

disability that is defined by severe and sustained<br />

impairment in social interaction and<br />

restrictive, repetitive patterns <strong>of</strong> behavior, interest,<br />

and activity. As first described in 1944<br />

by an Austrian pediatrician, Hans <strong>Asperger</strong><br />

(translated in Frith, 1991), <strong>the</strong> syndrome involved<br />

a qualitative impairment in reciprocal<br />

social interaction and behavior oddities, such<br />

as repetitive and stereotyped play, without delays<br />

in speech acquisition. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>Asperger</strong><br />

noted that children with this disability<br />

demonstrated poor motor coordination and<br />

extreme interest in memorizing information.<br />

Although <strong>Asperger</strong> described this syndrome<br />

in <strong>the</strong> 1940s, it was not recognized by <strong>the</strong><br />

English-speaking world until Wing introduced<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong>’s work in 1981 and suggested that AS<br />

was a type <strong>of</strong> autism spectrum disorder (ASD).<br />

In 1994, <strong>the</strong> American Psychiatric Association<br />

(APA) recognized AS as a subtype <strong>of</strong><br />

pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) with<br />

specific diagnostic criteria that differ from autism.<br />

Never<strong>the</strong>less, differential diagnosis be-<br />

The work reported here was supported by a research<br />

grant from <strong>the</strong> Organization for Autism Research.<br />

Correspondence concerning this article<br />

should be addressed to Brenda Smith Myles, Chief<br />

<strong>of</strong> Programs and Development, Ohio Center for<br />

Autism and Low Incidence, 5220 N. High Street,<br />

Building C1, Columbus, OH 43221. Email:<br />

brenda_myles@ocali.org<br />

448 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007<br />

tween AS and autism is <strong>of</strong>ten problematic, and<br />

disagreements exist on <strong>the</strong> specific characteristics<br />

<strong>of</strong> AS (Barnhill, 2001; Kasari & Ro<strong>the</strong>ram-Fuller,<br />

2005; Khouzam, El-Gabalawi,<br />

Pirwani, & Priest, 2004; Myles, Barnhill, Hagiwara,<br />

Griswold, & Simpson, 2001; Woodbury-<br />

Smith, Klin, & Volkmar, 2005). For instance,<br />

according to both <strong>the</strong> International Classification<br />

<strong>of</strong> Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Organization<br />

[WHO], 1992) and <strong>the</strong> Diagnostic and<br />

Statistical Manual <strong>of</strong> Mental Disorders—4 th Edition,<br />

Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000)<br />

criteria, <strong>the</strong>re is no clinically significant delay<br />

in language or cognitive development. On <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r hand, Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) included<br />

possible delayed language development<br />

in <strong>the</strong>ir criteria, whereas Szatmari,<br />

Bremner, and Nagy (1989) mentioned communication<br />

and speech impairments but without<br />

directly mentioning delays in ei<strong>the</strong>r area.<br />

As an additional example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> continuing<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> agreement, <strong>the</strong> DSM-IV-TR listed no<br />

specific motor deficit, whereas <strong>the</strong> ICD-10<br />

stated, “This disorder is <strong>of</strong>ten associated with<br />

marked clumsiness” (WHO, 1992, p. 377).<br />

Woodbury-Smith et al. acknowledged that<br />

“<strong>the</strong> onset criteria, as currently defined, are<br />

unreliable differentiators <strong>of</strong> autism and AS,<br />

and do not reflect a differentiation between<br />

autism and AS on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> research on<br />

developmental pathways, but, ra<strong>the</strong>r, have<br />

been set arbitrarily” (p. 239).<br />

In light <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se inconsistencies in <strong>the</strong> diag-


nostic criteria for a disorder that is becoming<br />

more and more widespread, researchers are<br />

still attempting to understand and define diagnostic<br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> AS. As a result, several<br />

additional characteristics have been suggested,<br />

including motor clumsiness (Ehlers &<br />

Gillberg, 1993; Ghaziuddin & Butler, 1998;<br />

Gillberg, 1985; Williams, 2001), academic difficulties<br />

(Myles & Simpson, 2001; Myles et al.,<br />

2001; Williams, 2001), unique sensory responses<br />

(Church, Alisanski, & Amanullah,<br />

2000; Myles, Cook, Miller, Rinner, & Robbins,<br />

2000; Myles & Simpson, 2001; Myles et al.,<br />

2001), emotional vulnerability and difficulties<br />

(Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-Mikhail, & Ghaziuddin,<br />

1998; Gillberg, 1985; Ryan, 1992; Wing,<br />

1981; Williams, 2001), and poor adaptive behavior<br />

(Myles et al., 2001).<br />

Despite a growing body <strong>of</strong> research and<br />

descriptions <strong>of</strong> symptoms from a variety <strong>of</strong><br />

sources, an empirically valid pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se individuals is urgently<br />

needed. The purpose <strong>of</strong> this study was to fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

describe <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> AS exhibited<br />

in 156 adolescents identified with this<br />

syndrome. The investigation was a part <strong>of</strong> a<br />

larger national study conducted at a university<br />

in <strong>the</strong> midwestern United States.<br />

Method<br />

Sample and Setting<br />

Thirty-three females (21%) and 123 (79%)<br />

male youths with AS ages 12 through 18 years<br />

(mean age: 14.97 years) participated in <strong>the</strong><br />

study. Participants had received <strong>the</strong>ir diagnoses<br />

independently from a licensed pr<strong>of</strong>essional<br />

(e.g., psychologist, psychiatrist) using<br />

<strong>the</strong> DSM-IV or <strong>the</strong> DSM-IV-TR criteria for AS.<br />

The diagnoses were verified using <strong>the</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong><br />

<strong>Syndrome</strong> Diagnostic <strong>Scale</strong> (ASDS;<br />

Myles, Bock, & Simpson, 2000). Mean scores<br />

on this instrument <strong>of</strong> 107.29 (SD 16.70)<br />

indicated that <strong>the</strong>y were very likely to have AS.<br />

Their mean full scale intelligence quotient<br />

(FSIQ) as measured by <strong>the</strong> Wechsler Intelligence<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s (Wechsler, 1991) was 102.63<br />

(SD 22.25). Ten percent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sample had<br />

IQs greater than 130. Three had an FSIQ in<br />

<strong>the</strong> 60s; two had a verbal IQ (VIQ) in <strong>the</strong><br />

average range, while one had a similar performance<br />

IQ (PIQ).<br />

Eighty-six (55%) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> participants had comorbid<br />

diagnoses, which included attention<br />

deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD; 40%),<br />

depression (12%), anxiety disorder (7%),<br />

Tourette <strong>Syndrome</strong> (3%), oppositional defiant<br />

disorder (5%), obsessive compulsive disorder<br />

(5%), and bipolar disorder (2%). Fur<strong>the</strong>r,<br />

<strong>the</strong>se adolescents were prescribed a<br />

mean <strong>of</strong> 2.89 (range 1–9) medications classified<br />

as central nervous system stimulant<br />

(49%), antidepressant (41%), antipsychotic<br />

(24%), or antihypertensive (9%).<br />

Procedure<br />

This study was part <strong>of</strong> an extensive investigation<br />

conducted through <strong>the</strong> Special Education<br />

Department in <strong>the</strong> College <strong>of</strong> Education<br />

at a large midwestern university. After receiving<br />

approval for <strong>the</strong> study from a university<br />

institutional review board, calls for participants<br />

were posted on <strong>the</strong> websites <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three<br />

major parent organizations focusing on <strong>Asperger</strong><br />

<strong>Syndrome</strong>. Interested parties were<br />

asked to contact <strong>the</strong> authors and subsequently<br />

completed <strong>the</strong> instruments via mail and personal<br />

interview.<br />

Instruments included <strong>the</strong> following: (a) <strong>the</strong><br />

Family Demographic Pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Myles, Hagiwara,<br />

Carlson, & Simpson, 1999); (b) <strong>the</strong> Vineland<br />

Adaptive Behaviors <strong>Scale</strong>s (VABS; Sparrow,<br />

Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984); (c) <strong>the</strong> Behavior Assessment<br />

System for Children—Parent Rating System<br />

(BASC PRS; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992);<br />

and (d) The Early Adolescent Temperament (Ellis<br />

& Rothbart, 2001). Fur<strong>the</strong>r, parents asked<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir child’s teacher to complete <strong>the</strong> BASC—<br />

Teacher Rating <strong>Scale</strong> (TRS; Reynolds & Kamphaus).<br />

Students were given <strong>the</strong> option to<br />

complete <strong>the</strong> (a) BASC—Student Self-Report <strong>of</strong><br />

Personality (SRP; Reynolds & Kamphuas), (b)<br />

<strong>the</strong> Empathy Quotient (EQ) Questionnaire (Baron-Cohen,<br />

2003), (c) <strong>the</strong> Systemizing Quotient<br />

(SQ) Questionnaire (Baron-Cohen, 2003), and<br />

(d) <strong>the</strong> Adolescent/Adult Sensory Pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Brown<br />

& Dunn, 2002) independently or through an<br />

interview with <strong>the</strong> authors. Finally, parents<br />

were asked to report <strong>the</strong>ir child’s intellectual<br />

quotient as ga<strong>the</strong>red from school or clinical<br />

records.<br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 449


Instruments<br />

A brief overview <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instruments reported<br />

in this article follows:<br />

The Family Demographic Pr<strong>of</strong>ile. This instrument<br />

(Myles et al., 1999) was developed to<br />

identify children with AS and family characteristics,<br />

including <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> immediate<br />

and extended family characteristics.<br />

Empathy Quotient Questionnaire. The EQ<br />

(Baron-Cohen & 2003) is a self-report questionnaire<br />

consisting <strong>of</strong> an affective component,<br />

a cognitive component, and a mixed<br />

component. It was originally developed for<br />

use with adults <strong>of</strong> normal intelligence. This<br />

60-item scale measures <strong>the</strong> ability to empathize.<br />

Specifically, <strong>the</strong> EQ measures an individual’s<br />

drive to identify ano<strong>the</strong>r person’s<br />

emotions and thoughts and to provide an appropriate<br />

emotional response to his or her<br />

emotional state. EQ ranges from low to maximum.<br />

Systemizing Quotient Questionnaire. The SQ<br />

(Baron-Cohen, 2003) is a 60-item scale that<br />

measures an individual’s drive to analyze, explore,<br />

and construct a system. Participants can<br />

receive a score in one <strong>of</strong> five categories that<br />

range from low to maximum.<br />

Vineland Adaptive Behavior <strong>Scale</strong>. The VABS<br />

(Sparrow et al., 1984) is a semi-structured interview<br />

designed to assess personal and social<br />

skills. Specifically, <strong>the</strong> VABS assesses adaptive<br />

behavior by measuring a person’s performance<br />

in <strong>the</strong> following four domains: Communication<br />

Skills, Daily Living Skills, Social<br />

Skills, and Motor Skills. Standard scores are<br />

used.<br />

Behavior Assessment System for Children. The<br />

BASC (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) is a multidimensional<br />

assessment used to evaluate a<br />

child’s behavior, emotions, self-perceptions,<br />

and personal history. It consists <strong>of</strong> three types<br />

<strong>of</strong> tools, <strong>the</strong> Parent Rating <strong>Scale</strong>s (PRS), <strong>the</strong><br />

Teacher Rating <strong>Scale</strong>s (TRS), and <strong>the</strong> Self-<br />

Report <strong>of</strong> Personality (SRP).<br />

The PRS measures a child’s adaptive and<br />

problem behaviors in community and home<br />

settings whereas <strong>the</strong> TRS is a comprehensive<br />

measure <strong>of</strong> both adaptive and problem behaviors<br />

in <strong>the</strong> school setting, Both <strong>the</strong> PRS and<br />

<strong>the</strong> TRS adaptive skills composite scores consist<br />

<strong>of</strong> Leadership and Social Skills. The TRS<br />

also includes study skills in this domain. Sim-<br />

ilar to <strong>the</strong> VABS, social skills include social<br />

adaptation in communication and socialization.<br />

The SRP contains <strong>the</strong> following three<br />

subdomains, which comprise <strong>the</strong> composite<br />

score: School Maladjustment, Clinical Maladjustment,<br />

and Personal Adjustment. Clinical<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s are scored on a 5-point scale using tscores<br />

wherein a score <strong>of</strong> 70 and above is<br />

Clinical Significant and a score <strong>of</strong> 30 and below<br />

is Very Low. The Adaptive <strong>Scale</strong>s also use<br />

a 5-point scale; however, here a score <strong>of</strong> 70<br />

and above is considered Very High and a score<br />

<strong>of</strong> 30 and below is considered Clinically Significant.<br />

Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire—<br />

Revised. The EATQ-R (Ellis & Rothbart,<br />

2001) assesses temperament and mood in adolescents.<br />

The Temperament <strong>Scale</strong>s, which focus<br />

primarily on self-regulation, include Activation<br />

Control, Affiliation, Attention, Fear,<br />

Frustration, High-Intensity Pleasure, Inhibitory<br />

Control, and Shyness. The Behavioral<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s, which focus on temperament traits related<br />

to socialization, include Aggression and<br />

Depressive Mood. The instrument is scored<br />

using a 5-point scale wherein 1 almost always<br />

untrue and 5 almost always true. This instrument<br />

is completed by parents.<br />

Results<br />

Results <strong>of</strong> this study as <strong>the</strong>y pertain to adolescents<br />

with AS will be presented below under<br />

<strong>the</strong> following major headlines: family history,<br />

cognitive pr<strong>of</strong>iles, adaptive behavior, behavior,<br />

temperament, and sensory issues.<br />

Family History<br />

450 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007<br />

Data on family history were collected on participants<br />

and <strong>the</strong>ir family members using <strong>the</strong><br />

Family Demographic Pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Myles et al.,<br />

1999). In total, 86 parents completed <strong>the</strong> measure.<br />

As shown in Table 1, fa<strong>the</strong>rs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> participants<br />

with AS, followed by mo<strong>the</strong>rs and paternal<br />

grandfa<strong>the</strong>rs were reported to have <strong>the</strong><br />

majority <strong>of</strong> disabilities or mental health conditions<br />

presented. The most frequently occurring<br />

disabilities or mental health issues included<br />

ADHD, general behavior issues,<br />

depression, and AS.<br />

Participants’ siblings were primarily diag-


TABLE 1<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> Family History <strong>of</strong> Related Diagnoses or Challenges<br />

Descriptors <strong>of</strong> Diagnoses or Challenges<br />

AS ADHD BD Behavior Bipolar LD Sch Speech TS Dep Anxiety OCD PD Total<br />

Family Member<br />

Fa<strong>the</strong>r 9 (10) 13 (15) 3 (4) 8 (9) 4 (5) 4 (5) 0 (0) 1 (1) 1 (1) 3 (3) 2 (2) 1 (1) 1 (1) 50<br />

Mo<strong>the</strong>r 3 (3) 17 (20) 1 (1) 3 (3) 4 (5) 3 (5) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 5 (6) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 37<br />

Paternal<br />

Grandfa<strong>the</strong>r 3 (3) 4 (5) 1 (1) 6 (7) 3 (3) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 19<br />

Paternal<br />

Grandmo<strong>the</strong>r 1 (1) 1 (1) 0 (0) 4 (5) 1 (1) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 4 (5) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 11<br />

Maternal<br />

Grandfa<strong>the</strong>r 1 (1) 3 (3.49) 0 (0) 4 (5) 2 (2) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (1) 1 (1) 1 (1) 1 (1) 15<br />

Maternal<br />

Grandmo<strong>the</strong>r 0 (0) 3 (3.49) 1 (1) 4 (5) 0 (0) 4 (5) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 5 (6) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 18<br />

Total 17 (3) 41 (8) 6 (1) 29 (6) 14 (3) 11 (2) 2 (1) 2 (1) 1 (1) 18 (3) 3 (1) 4 (1) 2 (1) 150<br />

Note. Based on a sample size <strong>of</strong> 86. Raw data are presented. Percentages appear in paren<strong>the</strong>ses.<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome (AS), Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD), Behavior Disorder (BD), General Issues with Behavior (B), Depression (Dep), Learning<br />

Disability (LD), Schizophrenia (Sch), Tourette <strong>Syndrome</strong> (TS), Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Personality Disorder (PD).<br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 451


TABLE 2<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> Siblings History <strong>of</strong> Related Diagnoses<br />

nosed with academic challenges (25%), followed<br />

by ADHD (13%) and AS (4%). Data on<br />

sibling order revealed that 27% <strong>of</strong> first-born<br />

and 26% <strong>of</strong> second-born siblings had exceptionalities.<br />

By comparison, 13% <strong>of</strong> third-born<br />

siblings had disabilities whereas no reported<br />

special needs were reported among fourthborn<br />

siblings (see Table 2).<br />

Cognitive Pr<strong>of</strong>iles<br />

Gender Descriptors <strong>of</strong> Diagnoses or Challenges<br />

Birth<br />

Order N M (%) F (%) AcD AS ADHD AU Bipolar LD MR Speech TS Total<br />

1 st<br />

2 nd<br />

3 rd<br />

4 th<br />

73 1 31 (42) 41 (56) 20 (27) 4 (5) 13 (18) 0 (0) 1 (1) 1 (1) 0 (0) 1 (1) 0 (0) 40 (55)<br />

38 26 (68) 12 (32) 10 (26) 1 (3) 4 (11) 1 (3) 1 (3) 1 (3) 1 (3) 0 (0) 1 (3) 20 (53)<br />

16 6 (38) 10 (62) 3 (19) 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (6) 1 (6) 1 (6) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 6 (38)<br />

7 4 (57) 3 (43) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)<br />

Total 134 67 66 33 5 17 2 3 3 1 1 1 66<br />

Note. Based on a sample size <strong>of</strong> 86. 1 Missing data on gender. Raw data are presented. Percentages are in<br />

paren<strong>the</strong>ses.<br />

Academic Difficulties (AcD), <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> (AS), Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD),<br />

Autism (AU), Behavior Disorder (BD), Learning Disability (LD), Mental Retardation (MR), Non-verbal<br />

Learning Disability (NLD), Pervasive Development Disorder-Not O<strong>the</strong>rwise Specified (PDD-NOS), Schizophrenia<br />

(Sch), Tourette <strong>Syndrome</strong> (TS).<br />

Intellectual skills. According to one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

primary diagnostic standards for AS, <strong>the</strong> DSM-<br />

IV-TR (APA, 2000), <strong>the</strong>re is no clinically significant<br />

delay in cognitive development<br />

among individuals with AS.<br />

Participants with AS in <strong>the</strong> current study<br />

had a significantly higher Verbal Intellectual<br />

Quotient (VIQ) (M 109.28, SD 19.62)<br />

than Performance IQ (PIQ) (M 99.42, SD <br />

19.78), t(59) 4.054, p .001 (.000). Both <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>se scores, as well as participants’ FSIQ<br />

(M 102.63, SD 22.25) fell in <strong>the</strong> Average<br />

range.<br />

Social cognitive skills. According to Baron-<br />

Cohen (2003), <strong>the</strong> primary difference between<br />

male and female brains is related to<br />

empathizing and systemizing. Empathizing, as<br />

a prominent feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> female brain, is <strong>the</strong><br />

drive to identify o<strong>the</strong>rs’ emotions and<br />

thoughts, and to respond to <strong>the</strong>se in socially<br />

appropriate ways. Thus, empathizing allows<br />

people to understand o<strong>the</strong>rs and to predict<br />

<strong>the</strong> social world around <strong>the</strong>m. Unlike empathizing,<br />

systemizing is <strong>the</strong> drive to analyze,<br />

explore, and construct a system in order to<br />

understand <strong>the</strong> basic principles and rules <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> behavior system and control it (Baron-<br />

Cohen, Richler, Bisarya, Gurunathan, &<br />

Wheelwright, 2003). As reported by <strong>the</strong> adolescents<br />

in this study, no significant difference<br />

was found between <strong>the</strong> male and female<br />

groups, t(85) 1.149, p .05 (.151). Both<br />

groups’ scores (females: M 30.71, SD <br />

13.82; males: M 25.55, SD 11.35) were<br />

categorized as “low” on <strong>the</strong> EQ scale. Similar<br />

to findings related to <strong>the</strong> EQ scale, no significant<br />

differences were noted between <strong>the</strong> systemizing<br />

skills <strong>of</strong> males (M 28.77, SD <br />

11.84) and females (M 24.52, SD 11.25),<br />

both groups scoring in <strong>the</strong> Average range on<br />

<strong>the</strong> SQ scale. Finally, <strong>the</strong> relationship between<br />

scores on <strong>the</strong> EQ and SQ was also investigated.<br />

The EQ and <strong>the</strong> SQ <strong>of</strong> participants with AS<br />

were significantly positively correlated with<br />

each o<strong>the</strong>r (r .340, p .01).<br />

Adaptive Behavior<br />

452 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007<br />

According to DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), individuals<br />

with AS demonstrate no clinically significant<br />

delay in adaptive behavior o<strong>the</strong>r than<br />

in social interaction. In order to assess <strong>the</strong><br />

adaptive behaviors <strong>of</strong> participants with AS, <strong>the</strong>


current study utilized two assessment tools:<br />

<strong>the</strong> VABS (Sparrow et al., 1984) and <strong>the</strong> BASC<br />

(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992).<br />

Vineland Adaptive Behavior <strong>Scale</strong>s. Results<br />

on <strong>the</strong> VABS revealed that <strong>the</strong> participants<br />

with AS had low or moderately low levels <strong>of</strong><br />

adaptive skills across all domains, including<br />

communication (moderately low; M 76.10,<br />

SD 21.33), daily living skills (low; M 67.75,<br />

SD 18.26), socialization (low; M 62.27,<br />

SD 20.25), adaptive composite score (low;<br />

M 64.28, SD 19.42), and maladaptive<br />

behavior (low; M 21.36, SD 8.43).<br />

Behavior Assessment System for Children. The<br />

BASC’s Adaptive Skills subdomain, which is<br />

composed <strong>of</strong> Leadership, Social Skills, and<br />

<strong>Study</strong> Skills (<strong>the</strong> latter on <strong>the</strong> TRS version for<br />

educators only), was also used to assess adaptive<br />

behaviors. Parents reported that <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

children’s adaptive behavior skills were in <strong>the</strong><br />

At-Risk range while <strong>the</strong> teachers indicated that<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir students’ skills were in <strong>the</strong> Average<br />

range. A significant difference was found between<br />

<strong>the</strong> two groups’ perceptions on Leadership,<br />

t(67) 6.718, p .001 (.000), Social<br />

Skills, t(67) 8.122, p .001 (.000), and<br />

Composite Score, t(67) 9.700, p .001<br />

(.000).<br />

Behavior<br />

The BASC was also used to assess behavior<br />

issues in <strong>the</strong> adolescents with AS as perceived<br />

by parents, teachers, and <strong>the</strong> students <strong>the</strong>mselves.<br />

Externalizing Problems Composite. A significant<br />

difference existed between parents’ and<br />

teachers’ perceptions <strong>of</strong> Hyperactivity, Aggression,<br />

and Conduct Problems, t(67) 6.725,<br />

p .001 (.000). Specifically, parents reported<br />

that <strong>the</strong>ir adolescent obtained an At-Risk Externalizing<br />

Problems Composite (M 60.96,<br />

SD 12.94). In contrast, teachers indicated<br />

that <strong>the</strong>ir student performed in <strong>the</strong> Average<br />

range (M 51.53, SD 9.04).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> three subscales comprising <strong>the</strong> Externalizing<br />

Problems Composite, parents reported<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir adolescent’s functioning as being<br />

in <strong>the</strong> At-Risk range in Hyperactivity (M <br />

68.35, SD 16.91), and in <strong>the</strong> Average range<br />

for Aggression (M 56.96, SD 11.71) and<br />

Conduct Problems (M 52.49, SD 10.61).<br />

In contrast, teachers’ data indicated that At-<br />

tention (M 56.40, SD 10.38), Hyperactivity<br />

(M 54.88, SD 12.15), and Conduct<br />

Problems (M 46.50, SD 5.31) were all<br />

perceived to be in <strong>the</strong> Average range.<br />

Internalizing Problems Composite. Parents’<br />

and teachers’ data differed significantly with<br />

regard to <strong>the</strong> Internalizing Problems Composite,<br />

t(67) 3.062, p .005 (.003). Specifically,<br />

parents reported that <strong>the</strong>ir adolescent<br />

children were At-Risk in Anxiety (M 65.38,<br />

SD 12.94) and Depression (M 65.04, SD <br />

16.25), but Average in Somatization (M <br />

55.09, SD 13.89). Teachers, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

hand, considered <strong>the</strong>ir students to be Average<br />

in all <strong>the</strong> subscales, with ratings similar to<br />

parents in Somatization (see Table 3).<br />

School Problems Composite. The BASC<br />

teacher rating scale contains three domains<br />

that assess school problems; <strong>the</strong> parent rating<br />

scale contains only one subscale (Attention<br />

Problems). On <strong>the</strong> School Problems Composite<br />

and its subscales, Learning Problems and<br />

Attention Problems, students with AS were<br />

rated by <strong>the</strong>ir teachers as Average. In contrast,<br />

parents indicated <strong>the</strong>ir adolescent were At-<br />

Risk for Attention Problems (M 65.01, SD <br />

10.67).<br />

Behavioral Symptoms Index. A significant difference<br />

was found in parents’ and teachers’<br />

perceptions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> overall behavior <strong>of</strong> adolescents<br />

with AS, t(66) 5.96, p .000 (.000).<br />

Parents and teachers concurred in <strong>the</strong>ir ratings<br />

for Atypicality, both indicating that <strong>the</strong><br />

adolescent was At Risk. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, parents reported<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir children were in <strong>the</strong> Clinically<br />

Significant range for Withdrawal (M 72.40,<br />

SD 15.44), whereas teacher ratings fell<br />

within <strong>the</strong> At-Risk range (M 60.54, SD <br />

14.46) (see Table 3).<br />

Item analysis. Both parents and pr<strong>of</strong>essionals<br />

have voiced concern regarding specific areas<br />

<strong>of</strong> behavior among individuals with AS. To<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r investigate <strong>the</strong>se areas, items related<br />

to Aggression, Anxiety, Atypicality, Depression,<br />

and Withdrawal were analyzed (see Table<br />

4). Items that received a mean score <strong>of</strong> at<br />

least 2.5 on a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 to 4 (1 never, 2 <br />

sometimes, 3 <strong>of</strong>ten, 4 always) are shown,<br />

as <strong>the</strong>se are considered to be areas <strong>of</strong> concern<br />

Reynolds and Kamphaus (1992).<br />

BASC Self-Report Pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Unlike <strong>the</strong> Parent<br />

Rating <strong>Scale</strong>s and <strong>the</strong> Teacher Rating <strong>Scale</strong>s,<br />

which allow for direct comparison across <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 453


TABLE 3<br />

Descriptive Data on BASC PRS and TRS<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s and Composites<br />

two types <strong>of</strong> raters, <strong>the</strong> Student Self-Report Pr<strong>of</strong>ile<br />

(SRP) does not permit such analysis. That is,<br />

students’ self-evaluations do not allow for comparisons<br />

with adults’ perceptions. As shown in<br />

Table 5, students with AS perceived <strong>the</strong>mselves<br />

to be similar to neurotypical peers in all areas.<br />

Parent Rating <strong>Scale</strong> Teacher Rating <strong>Scale</strong><br />

M SD M SD<br />

Aggression 56.96 11.71 52.63 9.91<br />

Hyperactivity 68.35 16.91 54.88 12.15<br />

Conduct Problems 52.49 10.61 46.50 5.31<br />

Externalizing Problems Composite 60.96 12.94 51.53 9.04<br />

Anxiety 65.38 12.94 57.50 12.76<br />

Depression 65.04 16.25 57.12 16.32<br />

Somatization 55.09 13.89 55.20 14.55<br />

Internalizing Problems Composite 63.94 14.48 57.85 14.68<br />

Attention Problems 65.01 10.67 56.40 10.38<br />

Learning Problems 50.19 6.79<br />

School Problems Composite 53.45 8.24<br />

Atypicality 68.49 18.47 62.79 19.05<br />

Withdrawal 72.40 15.44 60.54 14.46<br />

Behavioral Symptoms Index 70.48 15.31 58.69 14.24<br />

Leadership 38.25 7.01 45.06 6.93<br />

Social Skills 36.49 7.43 44.93 7.66<br />

<strong>Study</strong> Skills 47.23 7.32<br />

Adaptive Skills Composite 35.74 6.74 45.43 6.79<br />

Note. The Levene’s Test was significant at <strong>the</strong> .05 level for <strong>the</strong> Externalizing Problems Composite and <strong>the</strong><br />

subscales <strong>of</strong> Hyperactivity and Conduct Problems; <strong>the</strong>refore scores for “equal variance not assumed” were used.<br />

TABLE 4<br />

Temperament<br />

Parents completed <strong>the</strong> EATQ-R on <strong>the</strong>ir adolescent<br />

with AS to assess his or her temperament<br />

using eight subscales and behaviors divided<br />

into two domains.<br />

Item Analysis <strong>of</strong> BASC—Aggression, Anxiety, Atypicality, Depression, and Withdrawal<br />

Items PRS TRS<br />

Aggression<br />

Argues when denied own way 2.78<br />

Anxiety<br />

Is nervous 2.65<br />

Says, “I’m not very good at this.” 2.53<br />

Worries 2.81<br />

Depression<br />

Is easily upset 2.74<br />

Withdrawal<br />

Avoids competing with o<strong>the</strong>r adolescents 2.56<br />

Has trouble making new friends 3.13 2.62<br />

454 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007


TABLE 5<br />

Descriptive Data on BASC-SRP<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s and Composites M SD<br />

Attitude to School 55.26 12.06<br />

Attitude to Teachers 50.69 11.41<br />

Sensation Seeking 48.63 10.36<br />

School Maladjustment Composite 51.83 11.20<br />

Anxiety 51.37 10.10<br />

Atypicality 56.00 11.30<br />

Locus <strong>of</strong> Control 55.51 11.45<br />

Social Stress 56.04 10.63<br />

Somatization 52.84 12.05<br />

Clinical Maladjustment Composite 55.30 10.86<br />

Depression 53.33 10.69<br />

Sense <strong>of</strong> Inadequacy 50.53 9.63<br />

Interpersonal Relations 40.13 13.59<br />

Relations with Parents 49.46 9.32<br />

Self-Esteem 47.66 10.87<br />

Self-Reliance 45.22 12.25<br />

Personal Adjustment Composite 44.40 11.98<br />

Emotional Symptoms Index 54.82 10.75<br />

Temperament <strong>Scale</strong>s. Parents reported<br />

higher scores on <strong>the</strong> temperament scales <strong>of</strong><br />

Affiliation (M 2.09, SD .75), Fear (M <br />

2.95, SD .79), Frustration (M 3.94, SD <br />

.66), and Shyness (M 3.00, SD 1.05). The<br />

lowest scores were assigned to <strong>the</strong> subscale <strong>of</strong><br />

Activation Control (M 2.09, SD .75). All<br />

scale scores appear in Table 6.<br />

Behavioral <strong>Scale</strong>s. The EATQ-R Behavioral<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s include Aggression and Depressive<br />

TABLE 6<br />

Descriptive Data on EATQ-R<br />

Mood (see Table 6). Parents’ mean scores<br />

were above those <strong>of</strong> peers in <strong>the</strong> norming<br />

sample in <strong>the</strong> area <strong>of</strong> Aggression (M 3.09,<br />

SD .84) and Depressive Mood (M 2.99,<br />

SD .62).<br />

Item analysis. Just as with <strong>the</strong> BASC, an<br />

analysis on items related to Aggression, Depressive<br />

Mood, Frustration, Inhibitory Control,<br />

and Shyness was conducted (see Table 7).<br />

Items that received a mean score <strong>of</strong> at least 3.0<br />

<strong>Scale</strong>s M SD<br />

Temperament <strong>Scale</strong>s<br />

Activation Control 2.09 .75<br />

Affiliation 2.95 .77<br />

Attention 2.30 .70<br />

Fear 2.95 .79<br />

Frustration 3.94 .66<br />

Surgency/High-Intensity Pleasure 2.52 .87<br />

Inhibitory Control 2.78 .59<br />

Shyness 3.00 1.05<br />

Behavioral <strong>Scale</strong>s<br />

Aggression 3.09 .84<br />

Depressive Mood 2.99 .62<br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 455


TABLE 7<br />

Item Analysis <strong>of</strong> EATQ-R<br />

on a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 to 5 (1 almost always untrue,<br />

2 usually untrue, 3 sometimes true, sometimes<br />

untrue, 4 usually true, 5 almost<br />

always true) are shown.<br />

Sensory Issues<br />

Although not mentioned as diagnostic criteria<br />

in <strong>the</strong> DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), sensory issues<br />

have been reported as inherent in AS (cf.<br />

Shore, 2003; Willey, 1999). In <strong>the</strong> current<br />

study, 94 adolescents with AS completed <strong>the</strong><br />

Adolescent and Adult Sensory Pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Brown<br />

& Dunn, 2002) as a measure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir sensory<br />

challenges. Results showed that in Sensory<br />

Sensitivity (M 41.48, SD 9.31) and Sensation<br />

Avoiding (M 45.23, SD 12.36), this<br />

population scored “More Than Most People.”<br />

However, <strong>the</strong>ir Low Registration (M 40.30,<br />

Items M SD<br />

Aggression<br />

When angry at someone, says thing s/he knows will hurt that<br />

person’s feelings. 3.12 1.20<br />

If very angry, might hit someone. 2.99 1.37<br />

Tends to be rude to people s/he doesn’t like. 3.14 1.36<br />

Tends to try to blame mistakes on someone else. 3.53 1.31<br />

Slams doors when angry. 3.60 1.28<br />

Depressive Mood<br />

Often does not seem to enjoy things as much as his/her<br />

friends. 3.47 1.04<br />

Is sad more <strong>of</strong>ten than o<strong>the</strong>r people realize. 3.68 1.19<br />

Sometimes seems sad even when s/he should be enjoying<br />

her/himself like at Christmas or on a trip. 3.07 1.20<br />

Frustration<br />

Is annoyed by little things o<strong>the</strong>r kids do. 3.95 1.06<br />

Gets very irritated when someone criticizes him/her. 4.05 1.00<br />

Gets irritated when I will not take him/her someplace s/he<br />

wants to go. 3.84 1.09<br />

Gets irritated when s/he has to stop doing something s/he is<br />

enjoying. 4.40 .85<br />

Hates it when people don’t agree with him/her. 3.79 .84<br />

Gets very frustrated when s/he makes a mistake in his/her<br />

schoolwork. 3.58 1.16<br />

Inhibitory Control<br />

Has a hard time waiting his/her turn to speak when excited. 4.26 .95<br />

Shyness<br />

Can generally think <strong>of</strong> something to say, even to strangers. 3.22 1.34<br />

Is not shy. 3.10 1.39<br />

SD 8.35) and Sensation Seeking (M <br />

42.39, SD 9.58) scores were “Similar to Most<br />

People.”<br />

Discussion<br />

456 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007<br />

Based on <strong>the</strong> findings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> current study, a<br />

picture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> adolescent with AS emerges.<br />

Overall, <strong>the</strong> adolescents with AS who participated<br />

in this study had an average FSIQ consistent<br />

with that found in o<strong>the</strong>r research (cf.,<br />

Barnhill, Hagiwara, Myles, & Simpson, 2000;<br />

Ghaziuddin & Mountain-Kimchi, 2004;<br />

Koyama, Tachimori, Osada, Takeda, & Kurita,<br />

2007) and <strong>the</strong> DSM-IV-TR criteria (APA,<br />

2000). However, three individuals identified<br />

as having AS by medical pr<strong>of</strong>essionals did not<br />

have an FSIQ in <strong>the</strong> Average range; ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir PIQ or VIQ was in <strong>the</strong> Average range.


Giftedness appears to be a characteristic <strong>of</strong><br />

many individuals with AS although this study<br />

revealed that a smaller number <strong>of</strong> adolescents<br />

possessed this cognitive characteristic than<br />

found in a previous a study (Barnhill et al.,<br />

2000).<br />

Despite having average to above-average IQ,<br />

individuals with AS tend to have difficulties<br />

empathizing with o<strong>the</strong>rs because <strong>of</strong> a reported<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> mind or social cognition<br />

(Caro<strong>the</strong>rs & Taylor, 2004; Davies, Bishop,<br />

Manstead, & Tantum, 1994), consistent with<br />

<strong>the</strong> research <strong>of</strong> Baron-Cohen et al. (2003).<br />

While it has been found that individuals with<br />

AS have a strength in analyzing, exploring,<br />

categorizing, or constructing systems and that<br />

this skill is <strong>of</strong>ten found to be negatively correlated<br />

with empathizing (Baron-Cohen et al.),<br />

<strong>the</strong> adolescents in <strong>the</strong> current study demonstrated<br />

only average skills in this area. Previous<br />

research by Baron-Cohen and colleagues using<br />

<strong>the</strong> EQ and SQ with adults with AS has led<br />

to <strong>the</strong> conjecture that while empathizing is a<br />

challenge from individuals with AS from adolescence<br />

to adulthood, systemizing is a skill<br />

that develops over time. Understanding <strong>the</strong><br />

role <strong>of</strong> empathizing and systemizing in individuals<br />

with AS may allow educators to use <strong>the</strong><br />

average systemizing skills <strong>of</strong> individuals with<br />

AS to teach concepts, including nonverbal<br />

language and <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> mind.<br />

As demonstrated in this study, <strong>the</strong> gap between<br />

IQ and <strong>the</strong> adaptive behavior skills <strong>of</strong><br />

individuals with AS can be marked. This finding<br />

is supported by o<strong>the</strong>r researchers (see Lee<br />

& Park, in press, for a review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> literature<br />

on this topic). In short, studies on <strong>the</strong> adaptive<br />

behaviors <strong>of</strong> individuals with AS suggest<br />

that challenges in this area are part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> AS despite <strong>the</strong>ir exclusion<br />

from current diagnostic criteria.<br />

Again, this finding has tremendous ramifications<br />

for instruction. That is, while daily<br />

living skills are <strong>of</strong>ten taught to individuals with<br />

cognitive disabilities, instruction in this area<br />

needs to be expanded to include individuals<br />

with AS, despite <strong>the</strong>ir average to above-average<br />

IQ, to ensure that <strong>the</strong>y can live and work<br />

independently with success.<br />

In terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> behavioral issues <strong>of</strong> individuals<br />

with AS, <strong>the</strong> research is equivocal in that<br />

parents and educators disagree on <strong>the</strong> presence<br />

<strong>of</strong> internalizing or externalizing behav-<br />

iors (Barnhill et al., 2000). In brief, parents<br />

tend to identify problem behaviors whereas<br />

teachers do not. The reasons for <strong>the</strong>se perceived<br />

differences between <strong>the</strong> two groups are<br />

open to speculation. Do data reflect actual<br />

behavioral differences at home and school or<br />

differences <strong>of</strong> perception? Do <strong>the</strong> demands <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> home and school differ, such that behavioral<br />

issues exist in one environment and not<br />

<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r? Or do adolescents with AS “hold it<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r” in a stressful school environment<br />

only to exhibit behavior issues in <strong>the</strong> confines<br />

<strong>of</strong> a safe environment—<strong>the</strong> home?<br />

Additional research is needed to answer<br />

<strong>the</strong>se questions. Of concern is <strong>the</strong> finding that<br />

adolescents did not report experiencing behavior<br />

challenges. Consistent with previous research<br />

(Barnhill, Hagiwara, Myles, Simpson,<br />

Brick, et al., 2000), <strong>the</strong>se data suggest a lack <strong>of</strong><br />

awareness or denial by participants. As with<br />

<strong>the</strong> interpretations <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r data in this study,<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir meaning awaits fur<strong>the</strong>r research.<br />

Finally, adolescents’ sensory sensitivity suggests<br />

that <strong>the</strong>y are relatively more distractible<br />

and hyperactive in response to sensory stimuli<br />

than neurotypical peers, a finding that appears<br />

consistent with parents’ and teachers’<br />

reports <strong>of</strong> hyperactivity. This may result in a<br />

failure to complete tasks due to switched attention<br />

to non-task related stimuli during<br />

work time. In addition, adolescents in this<br />

study were sensitive to environmental input,<br />

and reported attempting to reduce incoming<br />

stimuli, <strong>of</strong>ten through withdrawal or resistance.<br />

Behavioral manifestations in this regard<br />

include having frequent meltdowns or refusing<br />

to participate in transitions, or novel or<br />

unpredictable activities. Such reactions may<br />

be directly related to parents’ reports <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

children’s behavior. That is, behavioral issues<br />

experienced by individuals with AS may have a<br />

sensory base and, thus, may require sensory<br />

interventions (Dunn, Myles, & Orr, 2002;<br />

Myles et al., 2004).<br />

Overall, this research yields a pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

complexity <strong>of</strong> AS as expressed by adolescents:<br />

an average to above-average IQ with adaptive<br />

behavior skills that are not commensurate<br />

with each o<strong>the</strong>r; a lack <strong>of</strong> ability to empathize<br />

with o<strong>the</strong>rs as well as a deficit in recognizing<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir own needs; behavioral issues, both overt<br />

and covert, that may be related to sensory<br />

concerns; and average skills in providing<br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 457


<strong>the</strong>mselves with much-needed order and predictability.<br />

Research in <strong>the</strong> field <strong>of</strong> AS is still in its early<br />

stages, and this is particularly true in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

our understanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

individuals with AS. Clearly, more research on<br />

this topic is needed to develop an empirical<br />

foundation for understanding students with<br />

AS as an essential component <strong>of</strong> improving<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir educational experiences. While <strong>the</strong> sample<br />

size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> present study as well as its<br />

nature as a volunteer sample limits generalization,<br />

it is hoped that <strong>the</strong>se findings will serve<br />

as one facet leading to more positive outcomes<br />

for individuals with AS.<br />

References<br />

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic<br />

and statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (4 th ed.).<br />

Washington, DC: Author.<br />

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic<br />

and statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (4 th ed.,<br />

text revision). Washington, DC: Author.<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong>, H. (1991). Autistic psychopathy in childhood<br />

(U. Frith, Trans.). In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism<br />

and <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome (pp. 37–92). New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press. (Original work published<br />

1944).<br />

Barnhill, G. P. (2001). Social attributions and depression<br />

in adolescents with <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>.<br />

Focus on Autism and O<strong>the</strong>r Developmental Disabilities,<br />

16, 46–53.<br />

Barnhill, G., Hagiwara, T., Myles, B. S., & Simpson,<br />

R. L. (2000). <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>: A study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

cognitive pr<strong>of</strong>iles <strong>of</strong> 37 children and adolescents.<br />

Focus on Autism and O<strong>the</strong>r Developmental Disabilities,<br />

15, 146–153.<br />

Barnhill, G. P., Hagiwara, T., Myles, B. S., Simpson,<br />

R. L., Brick, M. L., & Griswold, D. E. (2000).<br />

Parent, teacher, and self-report <strong>of</strong> problem and<br />

adaptive behaviors in children and adolescents<br />

with <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>. Diagnostique, 25, 147–<br />

167.<br />

Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Male<br />

and female brains and <strong>the</strong> truth about autism. Cambridge,<br />

MA: Basic Books. Baron-Cohen, S., &<br />

Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient:<br />

An investigation <strong>of</strong> adults with <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome<br />

or high-functioning autism, and normal<br />

sex differences. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism and Developmental<br />

Disorders, 34, 163–175.<br />

Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan,<br />

N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing<br />

quotient: An investigation <strong>of</strong> adults with<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome or high-functioning autism,<br />

458 / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities-December 2007<br />

and normal sex differences. In U. Frith & E. Hill<br />

(Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 161–186).<br />

Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.<br />

Brown, C. E., & Dunn, W. (2002). Adolescent/Adult<br />

Sensory Pr<strong>of</strong>ile: User’s manual. San Antonio, TX:<br />

The Psychological Corporation.<br />

Caro<strong>the</strong>rs, D., & Taylor, R. (2004). Social cognitive<br />

processing in elementary school children with<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome. Education and Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 39, 177–187.<br />

Church, C., Alisanski, S., & Amanullah, S. (2000).<br />

The social, behavioral, and academic experiences<br />

<strong>of</strong> children with <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome. Focus on Autism<br />

and O<strong>the</strong>r Developmental Disabilities, 15, 57–65.<br />

Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A.S.R., & Tantam,<br />

D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism<br />

and <strong>Asperger</strong>’s syndrome. Journal <strong>of</strong> Child<br />

Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1033–1057.<br />

Dunn, W., Myles, B. S., & Orr, S. (2002). Sensory<br />

processing issues associated with <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome:<br />

A preliminary investigation. American Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Occupational Therapy, 56, 97–102.<br />

Ehlers, S., & Gilberg, C. (1993). The epidemiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome: A total population study.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1327–<br />

1350.<br />

Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Revision <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

early adolescent temperament questionnaire. Poster<br />

presented at <strong>the</strong> 2001 Biennial Meeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis,<br />

MN.<br />

Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in<br />

autism and <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome: A fur<strong>the</strong>r report.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Intellectual Ability Research, 42, 43–48.<br />

Ghaziuddin M., & Mountain-Kimchi K. (2004). Defining<br />

<strong>the</strong> intellectual pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome:<br />

Comparison with high-functioning autism.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism and Developmental Disorders,<br />

34, 279–284.<br />

Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmer-Mikhail, F., & Ghazziuddin,<br />

N. (1998). Comorbidity <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome:<br />

A preliminary report. Journal <strong>of</strong> Intellectual<br />

Disability Research, 42, 279–283.<br />

Gillberg, C. (1985). <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome in 23 Swedish<br />

children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,<br />

31, 520–531.<br />

Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (1989). <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome—Some<br />

epidemiological considerations: A<br />

research note. Journal <strong>of</strong> Child Psychology and Psychiatry,<br />

30, 631–638.<br />

Kasari, C., & Ro<strong>the</strong>ram-Fuller, E. (2005). Current<br />

trends in psychological research on children with<br />

high-functioning autism and <strong>Asperger</strong> disorder.<br />

Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 18, 497–501.<br />

Khouzam, H. R., El-Gabalawi, F., Pirwani, N., &<br />

Priest, F. (2004). <strong>Asperger</strong>’s disorder: A review <strong>of</strong><br />

its diagnosis and treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry,<br />

45, 184–191.


Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., Takeda, T.,<br />

& Kurita, H. (2007). Cognitive and symptom pr<strong>of</strong>iles<br />

in <strong>Asperger</strong>’s syndrome and high-functioning<br />

autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61,<br />

99–104.<br />

Lee, H. J., & Park, H. R. (2007). Integrate review <strong>of</strong><br />

literature on <strong>the</strong> adaptive behavior <strong>of</strong> individuals<br />

with <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>. Remedial and Special Education,<br />

28, 132–139.<br />

Myles, B. S., Barnhill, G. P., Hagiwara, T., Griswold,<br />

D. E., & Simpson, R. L. (2001). A syn<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>of</strong><br />

studies on <strong>the</strong> intellectual, academic, social/emotional<br />

and sensory characteristics <strong>of</strong> children and<br />

youth with <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>. Education and<br />

Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 36, 304–311.<br />

Myles, B. S., Bock, S. J., & Simpson, R. L. (2000).<br />

<strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> Diagnostic <strong>Scale</strong>. Austin, TX: Pro-<br />

Ed.<br />

Myles, B. S., Cook, K. T., Miller, N. F., Rinner, L., &<br />

Robbins, L. A. (2000). <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> and sensory<br />

issue: Practical solutions for making sense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

world. Shawnee Mission, KS: Autism <strong>Asperger</strong><br />

Publishing Company.<br />

Myles, B. S., Hagiwara, T., Carlson, J. K., & Simpson,<br />

R. L. (1999). Family Demographic Pr<strong>of</strong>ile. Unpublished<br />

manuscript.<br />

Myles, B. S., Hagiwara, T., Dunn, W., Rinner, L.,<br />

Reese, M., Huggins, A., Becker, S. (2004). Sensory<br />

issues in children with <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome and<br />

autism. Education and Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 39, 283–290.<br />

Myles, B. S., & Simpson, R. L. (2001). <strong>Asperger</strong> syndrome:<br />

A guide for educators and practitioners (2 nd<br />

ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behavior<br />

Assessment System for Children. Circle Pines, MN:<br />

American Guidance Services.<br />

Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Early Adolescent Temperament<br />

Questionnaire (EATQ-R). Retrieved November 12,<br />

2006, from http://www.bowdoin.edu/sputnam/<br />

rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/instrumentdescriptions/early-adolescent-temperament.html<br />

Ryan, R. M. (1992). Treatment resistant chronic<br />

mental illness: Is it <strong>Asperger</strong>’s syndrome: Hospital<br />

and Community Psychiatry, 43, 807–811.<br />

Shore, S. (2003). Beyond <strong>the</strong> wall: Personal experiences<br />

with autism and <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> (2 nd ed.). Shawnee<br />

Mission, KS: Autism <strong>Asperger</strong> Publishing<br />

Company.<br />

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).<br />

Vineland Adaptive Behavior <strong>Scale</strong>s: Interview edition<br />

survey form manual. Circle Pines, MN: American<br />

Guidance Service.<br />

Szatmari, P., Bremner, R., & Nagy, J. (1989). <strong>Asperger</strong>’s<br />

syndrome and autism: Comparison <strong>of</strong> early<br />

history and outcome. Developmental Medicine and<br />

Child Neurology, 31, 709–720.<br />

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence <strong>Scale</strong> for<br />

Children—Third Edition. New York: Psychological<br />

Corp. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.<br />

Willey, L. H. (1999). Pretending to be normal. London,<br />

England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, Ltd.<br />

Williams, K. (2001). Understanding <strong>the</strong> student<br />

with <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong>: Guideline for teachers.<br />

Intervention in School and Clinic, 36, 287–292.<br />

Wing, L. (1981). <strong>Asperger</strong>’s syndrome: A clinical<br />

account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.<br />

Woodbury-Smith, M., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F.<br />

(2005). <strong>Asperger</strong>’s syndrome: A comparison <strong>of</strong><br />

clinical diagnoses and those made according to<br />

<strong>the</strong> ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Journal <strong>of</strong> Autism and<br />

Developmental Disorders, 35, 235–240.<br />

World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification<br />

<strong>of</strong> mental and behavioral disorders: Clinical<br />

descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland:<br />

Author.<br />

<strong>Characteristics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Asperger</strong> <strong>Syndrome</strong> / 459

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!