17.08.2013 Views

Opioids, Reward and Addiction: An Encounter of Biology ...

Opioids, Reward and Addiction: An Encounter of Biology ...

Opioids, Reward and Addiction: An Encounter of Biology ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ables <strong>and</strong>/or individual characteristics contribute to<br />

whether or not an individual becomes dependent on the<br />

drug. At this point a basic emotional feature may have<br />

been altered by repeated drug use, which in turn is<br />

responsible for the need to experience the effect <strong>of</strong> the<br />

drug again <strong>and</strong> again. This need is basically <strong>of</strong> a psychic<br />

nature, but it can contain physical elements such as<br />

physical dependence. Once a person has become dependent<br />

on a drug, discontinuation <strong>of</strong> drug use is difficult.<br />

Even after a prolonged period <strong>of</strong> abstinence, addicts can<br />

relapse into their former habit <strong>of</strong> drug dependence. A<br />

factor that may be important for relapse is craving, a<br />

(intense) desire to re-experience the effects <strong>of</strong> the drug<br />

(Rankin et al., 1979; Markou et al., 1993). Drug craving<br />

can be conceptualized as the incentive motivation to<br />

self-administer a previously consumed drug. This craving<br />

may be present during continuous use <strong>of</strong> the drug<br />

<strong>and</strong> long after abstinence, <strong>and</strong> may develop on basis <strong>of</strong><br />

incentive sensitization mechanisms in which associative<br />

learning plays a role (Bolles, 1975; Stewart et al., 1984;<br />

Robinson <strong>and</strong> Berridge, 1993). Besides craving, other<br />

factors may contribute to relapse, which is the major<br />

target for treatment programs <strong>of</strong> drug addiction.<br />

II. Reinforcement <strong>and</strong> Motivation<br />

Alterations in the organism’s environment trigger<br />

sensory mechanisms <strong>and</strong> thus generate information that<br />

is conveyed to the CNS. This information <strong>and</strong> other<br />

inputs into the brain are integrated at several levels <strong>and</strong><br />

can activate or inhibit the brain output systems, including<br />

motor systems, thus eliciting behavioral changes.<br />

The purpose <strong>of</strong> these behavioral changes is the adaptation<br />

<strong>of</strong> an organism to changes in environmental conditions,<br />

with the ultimate result that survival <strong>of</strong> the organism<br />

or its species is ensured. The extreme <strong>of</strong> an<br />

environmental continuum is that the organism approaches<br />

a desirable (pleasant) <strong>and</strong> avoids a noxious<br />

(aversive) environment.<br />

The setpoint <strong>of</strong> behavioral reactions is determined by<br />

genetic factors but its value is being modulated continuously<br />

by new experiences <strong>and</strong>, as a consequence, by<br />

acquired behavioral patterns. Behavioral reactions can<br />

be acquired through the association <strong>of</strong> stimuli that are<br />

originally neutral to innate reactions. The processes involved<br />

are types <strong>of</strong> associative learning. Forms <strong>of</strong> nonassociative<br />

learning include habituation <strong>and</strong> sensitization.<br />

During habituation, the reflex reaction elicited by a<br />

nonnoxious stimulus decreases when the stimulus is<br />

presented repeatedly. Sensitization involves an increased<br />

reflex reaction to a wide range <strong>of</strong> stimuli given<br />

shortly after the presentation <strong>of</strong> an intense or noxious<br />

stimulus. Through nonassociative learning the organism<br />

learns about the properties <strong>of</strong> one particular stimulus.<br />

Two major classes <strong>of</strong> associative learning are distinguished:<br />

classical <strong>and</strong> instrumental conditioning. During<br />

classical conditioning, a concept which was intro-<br />

OPIOIDS, REWARD AND ADDICTION 345<br />

duced by Pavlov (1927), the organism learns about the<br />

relationship between one stimulus in its environment<br />

<strong>and</strong> another stimulus (the unconditioned <strong>and</strong> the “neutral”<br />

conditioned stimulus). The unconditioned stimulus<br />

activates an established reflex <strong>and</strong> thus elicits an unconditioned<br />

reaction (e.g., the presence <strong>of</strong> food in the<br />

mouth results in salivation). Before conditioning the<br />

conditioned stimulus does not elicit the unconditioned<br />

reaction. After association <strong>of</strong> the conditioned stimulus<br />

<strong>and</strong> the unconditioned stimulus, the conditioned stimulus<br />

evokes a conditioned reaction that resembles the<br />

unconditioned one (e.g., when a sound is presented repeatedly,<br />

either immediately before or while food is in<br />

the mouth, salivation will ultimately follow after the<br />

presentation <strong>of</strong> the sound). Classical conditioning allows<br />

the organism to predict the coherence between events in<br />

its environment. The conditioned stimulus has become<br />

an anticipating signal for the occurrence <strong>of</strong> the unconditioned<br />

stimulus. The conditioned response can prepare<br />

the organism to deal with the result <strong>of</strong> the unconditioned<br />

stimulus more efficiently.<br />

Instrumental conditioning, introduced by Thorndike<br />

(1913), refers to the process <strong>of</strong> learning about the relationship<br />

between a stimulus <strong>and</strong> the behavior <strong>of</strong> the<br />

organism. When a certain behavioral act is followed by a<br />

favorable change in its environment, the organism tends<br />

to repeat this behavior (law <strong>of</strong> effect). This change in<br />

environment can be the occurrence <strong>of</strong> a pleasant stimulus<br />

or the removal <strong>of</strong> an aversion or noxious stimulus. In<br />

instrumental conditioning, in contrast to classical conditioning,<br />

the (behavioral) response changes the probability<br />

that the unconditioned stimulus will appear, allowing<br />

the organism to have more or less control over its<br />

environment. Four types <strong>of</strong> instrumental conditioning<br />

can be distinguished: positive reinforcement (presentation<br />

<strong>of</strong> a pleasant stimulus), punishment (presentation<br />

<strong>of</strong> an aversive stimulus), negative punishment (removal<br />

<strong>of</strong> a pleasant stimulus), <strong>and</strong> negative reinforcement (removal<br />

<strong>of</strong> an aversive stimulus). The frequency <strong>of</strong> behavioral<br />

responses usually increases when positive or negative<br />

reinforcement is operative <strong>and</strong> decreases in the<br />

case <strong>of</strong> punishment, including negative punishment.<br />

Many studies on positive reinforcement in experimental<br />

animals use lever manipulation as the behavioral<br />

response, <strong>and</strong> the conditioning in such experiments is<br />

also termed operant conditioning. This type <strong>of</strong> conditioning<br />

is <strong>of</strong>ten investigated in the so-called “Skinner box”<br />

(Skinner, 1938). A typical experiment involves placement<br />

<strong>of</strong> a hungry animal in a box in which a horizontal<br />

lever protrudes from a wall. Pressing the lever is followed<br />

by presentation <strong>of</strong> food. The animal learns that<br />

this behavioral act is reinforced by food. Thus, when the<br />

animal is hungry <strong>and</strong> is placed in the same box it is<br />

likely to press the lever to obtain food. The behavioral<br />

act in operant conditioning is termed “operant”, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

pleasant stimulus that tends to increase the frequency <strong>of</strong><br />

the operant is called “positive reinforcer”.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!