12.09.2013 Views

Presuppositional Effects of Scrambling Reconsidered*

Presuppositional Effects of Scrambling Reconsidered*

Presuppositional Effects of Scrambling Reconsidered*

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Mithun 1992 examines the ordering <strong>of</strong> constituents in three languages: Cayuga,<br />

Ngandi and Coos. She points out that these languages pose a problem for the common<br />

assumption <strong>of</strong> theme-rheme (or topic-comment) ordering in that they exhibit the<br />

opposite rheme-theme (or comment-topic) order. 23 As a subcase <strong>of</strong> this tendency,<br />

Mithun discusses cases <strong>of</strong> what she calls “topic shift,” where an NP representing a new<br />

topic or a new point <strong>of</strong> view is placed at the beginning <strong>of</strong> the sentence when the topic <strong>of</strong><br />

the discourse shifts from one to another. While Japanese does not exhibit the<br />

rheme-theme order as a basic pattern, it is likely that what is happening in (56) is “topic<br />

shift” in the sense that the sentence-initial NP introduces a new topic (or a new point <strong>of</strong><br />

view). Thus, this NP should not be defocalized but receives another kind <strong>of</strong> focus,<br />

whose exact nature will be left for future research. Notice that this “topic” is different<br />

from those marked with the topic marker wa as in (57). While the wa-marked NP in (57)<br />

has a contrastive reading, the preposed object NP in (56) does not. Still, the preposed<br />

object NP in (56) plays a role similar to that <strong>of</strong> wa-marked NPs in that it serves as a<br />

topic. This suggests that not only the wa-marked topic but also this new topic (“topic<br />

shift” topic) is not included in TP. It follows then that it need not be contiguous with<br />

the presupposed verb. 24<br />

(57) Sono okane-wa dare-ga nusunda no?<br />

that money-TOP who-NOM stole Q<br />

‘As for that money, who stole it?’<br />

(58) and (59) further illustrate the “topic shift” in M-scrambling.<br />

(58) Taroo-wa sakana-o tutta. Sono sakana-o Hanako-ga nite tabeta.<br />

Taro-TOP fish-ACC caught that fish-ACC Hanako-NOM cook ate<br />

‘Taro caught a fish. Hanako cooked and ate that fish.’<br />

(59) Senba-yama ni-wa tanuki-ga otte sa. Sore-o ryoosi-ga<br />

Senba mountain in-TOP raccoon-NOM was that-ACC hunter-NOM<br />

teppoo-de utte sa.<br />

gun-with shot<br />

‘In Mt. Senba lived a raccoon. A hunter shot it with a gun.’ (Children’s play<br />

song)<br />

In (58), the first sentence introduces a fish into the discourse. The second sentence<br />

then turns to this fish and says what happened to it. The same applies to the raccoon in<br />

(59).<br />

A similar pragmatic role is played by NPs moved to the sentence-initial position by<br />

L-scrambling.<br />

23 Ojibwa, discussed in Tomlin and Rhodes 1992 also belongs to this category.<br />

24 The “topic shift” topic can appear in embedded clauses from which wa-marked topics are excluded.<br />

Possibly, the “topic shift” topic is adjoined to TP and the contiguity condition applies to only those<br />

elements properly included by TP.<br />

19

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!