21.11.2013 Views

Solving the bracketing paradox - German Grammar Group FU Berlin

Solving the bracketing paradox - German Grammar Group FU Berlin

Solving the bracketing paradox - German Grammar Group FU Berlin

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ST. MÜLLER<br />

inflectional and derivational material. With such an approach, it is possible<br />

to combine Kathol’s representation of particle verbs (discussed in <strong>the</strong> previous<br />

section) with a morphology component that circumvents <strong>the</strong> <strong>paradox</strong>es.<br />

The participle aufgewacht ‘woken up’ can be analyzed as <strong>the</strong> result of<br />

a lexical rule application to a lexical item that contains auf and wach in a list<br />

of morphological objects. The lexical rule adds <strong>the</strong> morphological objects<br />

ge- and -t to this list. Linearization rules ensure that <strong>the</strong> ge- and -t attach to<br />

<strong>the</strong> verbal stem.<br />

It is interesting that such a solution is possible within <strong>the</strong> HPSG framework,<br />

but I have shown that <strong>the</strong> additional machinery that would be needed<br />

to guarantee <strong>the</strong> proper linearization of <strong>the</strong> inflectional and derivational<br />

material, and <strong>the</strong> extra list for <strong>the</strong> representation of morphological material,<br />

is not justified. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, this proposal has, of course, <strong>the</strong> problems discussed<br />

in section 6.3 since it is based on Kathol’s analysis.<br />

6.5 Ackerman & Webelhuth (1998)<br />

Ackerman & Webelhuth (1998) develop a <strong>the</strong>ory of complex predicates that<br />

integrates assumptions from both LFG and HPSG. The authors use a separate<br />

valence feature PART. The value of this feature is a list which contains a<br />

particle if <strong>the</strong> verb occurs in verb-initial position and which is empty when<br />

<strong>the</strong> verb occurs in final position. Their lexical entry for an+ruft is shown in<br />

(64), in a notation adapted to that used throughout <strong>the</strong> paper.<br />

(64) (an)ruft verb-initial version (cf. Ackerman & Webelhuth 1998:<br />

334–335)<br />

⎡<br />

⎤<br />

PHON 〈 ruft 〉∨〈anruft 〉<br />

[ 〈 〉]<br />

⎢<br />

SUBCAT NP[str], NP[str] ⎥<br />

⎣SYNSEM|LOC|CAT<br />

〈 〉 ⎦<br />

PART PART[an] ∨〈〉<br />

The proper distribution of <strong>the</strong> particle in both <strong>the</strong> valence feature and <strong>the</strong><br />

phonological representation is ensured by type constraints that rule out<br />

<strong>the</strong> cases with a phonological representation anruft+particle an and <strong>the</strong><br />

phonological representation ruft without a particle. With <strong>the</strong> types computed,<br />

(64) is equivalent to (65) and (66). (65) is <strong>the</strong> entry that is needed for<br />

clauses with <strong>the</strong> finite verb in <strong>the</strong> left sentence bracket.<br />

(65) (an)ruft (following Ackerman & Webelhuth 1998)<br />

⎡<br />

⎤<br />

PHON 〈 ruft 〉<br />

[ 〈 〉]<br />

SUBCAT NP[str], NP[str] SYNSEM|LOC|CAT<br />

⎢<br />

〈 〉<br />

⎣<br />

PART PART[an]<br />

⎥<br />

⎦<br />

partld-lci ∧ second-lci<br />

318

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!