26.12.2013 Views

Nominal Compounding as a Productive Word-formation Process in ...

Nominal Compounding as a Productive Word-formation Process in ...

Nominal Compounding as a Productive Word-formation Process in ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> <strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong><br />

<strong>as</strong> a <strong>Productive</strong> <strong>Word</strong>-<strong>formation</strong><br />

<strong>Process</strong> <strong>in</strong> Chuxnabán Mixe<br />

Carmen Jany<br />

California State University, San Bernard<strong>in</strong>o<br />

cjany@csusb.edu<br />

Fieldwork Forum, UC Berkeley March 13, 2013


Introduction: What is a compound?<br />

• Proposed criteria for def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g compound words<br />

•Complex<br />

•Spelled together<br />

•Right-headed<br />

•Inflected <strong>as</strong> a whole<br />

•Conceptual units<br />

•Formed w/o word-<strong>formation</strong> affixes<br />

•Specific stress pattern<br />

•Include l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g elements<br />

•Syntactically <strong>in</strong>separable<br />

•Syntactico-semantic islands<br />

• Bauer (2006), Lieber & Štekauer (2009), Scalise &<br />

Vogel (2010)


Introduction: What is a compound?<br />

• Many not universally accepted criteria<br />

• Lieber & Štekauer (2009) emph<strong>as</strong>ize importance<br />

of at le<strong>as</strong>t three criteria for compoundhood<br />

• Stress and phonological patterns<br />

• Behavior of compound with respect to <strong>in</strong>flection<br />

• Syntactic impenetrability<br />

• This work<br />

• Ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s that criteria are best viewed languagespecifically


Introduction: <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compounds<br />

• Aikhenvald (2007:24)<br />

• <strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong> refers to the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of at le<strong>as</strong>t<br />

two potentially free forms<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compound<strong>in</strong>g results <strong>in</strong> the creation of<br />

new nouns<br />

• Some def<strong>in</strong>ition problems<br />

• What is a lexeme/word (Bauer 2006)<br />

• Roots vs. derivational affixes (Lieber & Štekauer 2009)


Introduction: <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compounds<br />

• Some def<strong>in</strong>ition problems (con’t)<br />

• Postulated criteria: Gradient rather than<br />

categorial dist<strong>in</strong>ction (Lieber & Štekauer 2009)<br />

• Construction vs. lexcial unit (Bauer 2006)<br />

• Deictic compounds (Down<strong>in</strong>g 1977)<br />

• Common word-<strong>formation</strong> process <strong>in</strong><br />

Mixean and other Mesoamerican languages<br />

(Campbell et al. 1986; Romero 2010; Ruiz De Bravo Ahuja<br />

1980; Schoenhals 1982; Van Haitsma 1967)


Introduction: This presentation<br />

• Common and productive word-<strong>formation</strong><br />

process <strong>in</strong> Chuxnabán Mixe<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compounds may share properties<br />

with NPs and possessive constructions => not<br />

e<strong>as</strong>ily identified <strong>in</strong> all <strong>in</strong>stances<br />

• Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g formal criteria that dist<strong>in</strong>guish<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>al compounds from NPs and from<br />

possessive constructions <strong>in</strong> Chuxnabán Mixe


Introduction: This presentation<br />

• Chuxnabán Mixe language and data<br />

• Noun phr<strong>as</strong>es & Possessive constructions<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compound<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• Semantics of compounds and types<br />

• Phonological criteria<br />

• Morphosyntactic criteria<br />

• Summary and Conclusions


Language and Data<br />

• Chuxnabán Mixe<br />

• Mixe-Zoquean language<br />

• Spoken by 900 people <strong>in</strong> one village <strong>in</strong> Oaxaca<br />

• Typologically<br />

• Polysynthetic and head-mark<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• Inverse alignment system and noun <strong>in</strong>corporation<br />

• Data<br />

• Personal field work<br />

• Elicitation (words, phr<strong>as</strong>es) & oral narratives


Language and Data<br />

• Mixean-Zoquean language family<br />

• From Wichmann 1995


Language and Data<br />

San Juan Bosco Chuxnabán


Language and Data


Language and Data


Noun Phr<strong>as</strong>es & Possession<br />

• Noun phr<strong>as</strong>es <strong>in</strong> Chuxnabán Mixe<br />

• Lack any b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g morphology<br />

• Consist of personal or demonstrative pronoun,<br />

noun, or noun + modifiers ( ADJ, NUM, DEM )<br />

• Adjectives precede or follow noun with no<br />

change <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(1) tu’uk tsaajptëëjk mutsk / tu’uk mutsk tsaajptëëjk<br />

one church small / one small church<br />

‘one small church’


Noun Phr<strong>as</strong>es & Possession<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> Morphology and Possession<br />

• Optional plural marker for humans -ëch, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

suffix –u’unk, possessive prefixes, locative c<strong>as</strong>e suffix<br />

• Possession marked <strong>as</strong> prefix on the possessed<br />

• 3rd person possessive prefix y- triggers<br />

palatalization of follow<strong>in</strong>g segment<br />

(2) tëëjk ‘house’<br />

y-tëëjk -> chëëjk ‘his, her house’<br />

u’k y-tëëjk -> u’k chëëjk ‘the dog’s house’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Semantics<br />

• Endocentric & often predictable mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• nëëj ‘water’ + yuujk ‘animal’ -> nëëjyuujk ‘fish’<br />

• naax ‘earth’ + yuujk ‘animal’ -> naaxyuujk ‘worm’<br />

• uujts ‘plant’ + atsëm ‘pig’<br />

• tsuxk ‘green’ + kat ‘toucan’<br />

->uujtsatsëm ‘ wild boar’<br />

-> tsuxkat ‘type of<br />

• wi<strong>in</strong> ‘eye’ + waay ‘hair’ -> wi<strong>in</strong>waay ‘eyel<strong>as</strong>h’<br />

• yo’k ‘neck’ + paajk ‘bone’<br />

• kaan ‘salt’+ nëëj ‘water’<br />

• këk ‘deep’ + nëëj ‘water’<br />

• They are always right-headed<br />

-> yo’kpaajk ‘Adam’s apple’<br />

-> kaanëëj ‘salt water’<br />

-> këknëëj ‘lagoon’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Semantics<br />

• Exocentric & sometimes predictable mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• ’ipx ’20’ + teky ‘foot’ -> ’ipxteky ‘centipede’<br />

• teky ‘foot’ + waj ‘horn’ -> tekywaj ‘ankle’<br />

• kë’ë ‘hand’ + kaa ‘lion’ -> kë’ëkaa ‘thumb’<br />

• të’ëny ‘excrement’ + tsik ‘coati’ -> të’ënychik ‘lizard’<br />

• kay ‘food’ + tëëjk ‘house’ -> kaychëëjk ‘stomach’<br />

• naax ‘earth’ + waay ‘hair’ -> naaxwaay ‘dust’<br />

• Coord<strong>in</strong>ate & often predictable mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• tun ‘mounta<strong>in</strong>’+ kopk ‘mounta<strong>in</strong>’ -> tunkopk ‘mounta<strong>in</strong>’<br />

• tsuxk ‘green’ + yëk ‘black’ -> tsuxkyëk ‘blue’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Semantics<br />

• New concepts <strong>as</strong> compounds<br />

• iix ‘visible’ + jok ‘smoke’ -> iixjok ‘airplane’<br />

• u’k ‘dog’ + tëëjk ‘house’ -> u’ktëëjk ‘doghouse’<br />

• Compounds with loan words from Spanish<br />

• cerë from Spanish cera ‘wax’<br />

• + pa’ak ‘sweet’ -> cerëpa’ak ‘honey’<br />

• + yuujk ‘animal -> cerëyuujk ‘bee’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Phonology<br />

Consontant <strong>in</strong>ventory<br />

Vowel <strong>in</strong>ventory


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Phonology<br />

• Compounds differ from phr<strong>as</strong>es b<strong>as</strong>ed on<br />

• Obstruent voic<strong>in</strong>g only with<strong>in</strong> words (Jany 2010)<br />

• nëëkopk ‘founta<strong>in</strong>’ (nëëj ‘water’ + kopk ‘mounta<strong>in</strong>’)<br />

• kë’ëxooky ‘f<strong>in</strong>gernail’ (kë’ë ‘hand’ + xooky ‘nail’)<br />

But: mëk uky ‘strong purple’ (mëk ‘strong’ + uky ‘purple’)<br />

• Sonorant devoic<strong>in</strong>g only at word boundary (Jany 2010)<br />

• kaajpn ‘village’, But: kaajpnkopk ‘capital’<br />

• N<strong>as</strong>al place <strong>as</strong>similation only with<strong>in</strong> words (Jany 2010)<br />

• wiimpo’k ‘forehead’ (wi<strong>in</strong> ‘eye’ + po’k ‘b<strong>as</strong>ket’)<br />

But: wi<strong>in</strong> poop ‘sclera’ (wi<strong>in</strong> ‘eye’ + poop ‘white’)


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Phonology<br />

• Compounds differ from phr<strong>as</strong>es b<strong>as</strong>ed on<br />

• Supr<strong>as</strong>egmental palatalization only with<strong>in</strong> words<br />

• kachypaajk ‘rib’ (kachy ‘b<strong>as</strong>ket’ + paajk ‘bone’)<br />

• kaychaajkn ‘kitchen’ (kay ‘food’ + taajkn ‘place’)<br />

• tsooychëëjk ‘hospital’ (tsooy ‘medic<strong>in</strong>e’ + tëëjk ‘house’)<br />

But: mony joypyp ‘early morn<strong>in</strong>g’<br />

(mony ‘early’ + joypyp ‘morn<strong>in</strong>g’)<br />

• There is only one primary stress (on the right-most root)<br />

• mëjnëëteech ‘sea’ (mëj ‘big’ + nëëj ‘water’ + teech ‘father’)<br />

• mëja’aychëëjk ‘ancestor’ (mëj ‘big’ + ja’ay ‘person’ + tëëjk<br />

‘house’)


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Phonology<br />

• Compounds differ from phr<strong>as</strong>es b<strong>as</strong>ed on<br />

• Occ<strong>as</strong>ional consonant elision & syllable reduction<br />

when compared to free forms<br />

• jëxk ‘back’ + paajk ‘bone’ -> jëpaajk ‘sp<strong>in</strong>e’<br />

• kë’ë ‘hand’ + waj ‘horn’ -> kë’waj ‘wrist’<br />

• teky ‘foot’ + kaa ‘lion’ -> tekyaa ‘big toe’<br />

• maap ‘sleep’ + taajkn ‘place’ -> maataajkn ‘bedroom’<br />

• Occ<strong>as</strong>ional l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g vowel (also <strong>in</strong> Zoque, Herrera 1995)<br />

• teky ‘foot’ + waj ‘horn’ -> tekyüwaj ‘ankle’<br />

• teky ‘foot’ + wa’axy ‘divided’ -> tekyëwa’axy ‘toes’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Morphosyntax<br />

• Morphosyntactic clues for compound<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• Formally dist<strong>in</strong>ct from nom<strong>in</strong>al possession<br />

• <strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong><br />

tsaajp ‘heaven’ + tëëjk ‘house -> tsaajptëëjk ‘church’<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> possession<br />

taajk ‘policeman’ + tëëjk ‘house ->taajk chëëjk<br />

taajk y-tëëjk<br />

policeman POSS-house<br />

‘policeman’s house’<br />

• Compare<br />

• u’k chëëjk ‘dog’s house’ versus u’ktëëjk ‘doghouse’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Morphosyntax<br />

• Phoneme vs. morpheme-<strong>in</strong>duced palatalization<br />

• tsooy ‘medic<strong>in</strong>e’ + tëëjk ‘house’ = tsooychëëjk ‘hospital’<br />

=> if the l<strong>as</strong>t consonant of the preced<strong>in</strong>g word is<br />

palatalized, compound<strong>in</strong>g and nom<strong>in</strong>al possession<br />

show the same pattern of palatalization<br />

• Possession: compounds <strong>in</strong>flected <strong>as</strong> whole<br />

• poopniij ‘white chilli’<br />

• tsi<strong>in</strong>ka’ax ‘parrot’<br />

-> pyoopniij ‘his white chilli’<br />

-> ntsi<strong>in</strong>ka’ax ‘my parrot’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Morphosyntax<br />

• <strong>Word</strong> order<br />

• Fixed order <strong>in</strong> compounds, but variable <strong>in</strong> noun<br />

phr<strong>as</strong>es (Adj + N or N + Adj)<br />

• Compounds versus Noun phr<strong>as</strong>es<br />

• këknëëj ‘lagoon’ vs. nëëj këk ‘deep water’<br />

(këk ‘deep’; nëëj ‘water’)<br />

• tookxtsooy ‘pepper’ vs. tsooy tookx ‘food is medic<strong>in</strong>e’<br />

(tookx ‘food’; tsooy ‘medic<strong>in</strong>e’)


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Morphosyntax<br />

• Componds with different mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

• uujtsatsëm ‘wild boar’ vs.<br />

(uujts ‘plant’; atsëm ‘pig’)<br />

• nëtoky ‘iguana’<br />

(nëëj ‘water’; toky ‘rotten’)<br />

• kaatsi’ixy ‘onion’<br />

(kaa ‘lion’; tsi’ixy ‘fart’)<br />

atsëmuujts ‘type of plant’<br />

vs. tokynëëj ‘rotten water’<br />

vs. tsi’ixykaa ‘lion’s fart’<br />

• tëëjkopk ‘roof’ vs. kopktëëjk ‘house <strong>in</strong> the<br />

(tëëjk ‘house’; kopk ‘mounta<strong>in</strong>’) mounta<strong>in</strong>’


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Morphosyntax<br />

• <strong>Word</strong> order: Adjectives before or after compound<br />

• po’owëën ‘leather belt’ (po’o ‘leather’; wëën ‘belt’)<br />

yëk po’owëën or po’owëën yëk ‘black leather belt’,<br />

But not: *po’o yëk wëën<br />

• Sometimes one part can’t stand alone (“cranberry-type”)<br />

• to’oxychëëjk ‘woman’ (to’oxy = female; can’t stand alone)<br />

• ye’eychëëjk ‘man’ (ye’ey = male; can’t stand alone)<br />

• Lexical categories <strong>in</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>al compounds<br />

• N + N; ADJ + N; NUM + N; N+ V, V + N


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Productivity<br />

• Morphological productivity: gives rise to new<br />

<strong>formation</strong>s on a systematic b<strong>as</strong>is (Plag 2006)<br />

• Qualitatively: process is widely available and can be<br />

used to produce new words<br />

• Quantitatively: process is be<strong>in</strong>g extensively used<br />

• This work: Productivity exam<strong>in</strong>ed qualitatively<br />

• Aikhenvald (2007): productivity correlates with<br />

• Few (phon, morph, morpho-synt) restrictions<br />

• Semantic predictability


<strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>: Productivity<br />

• Aikhenvald (2007) (con’t)<br />

• Formal regularity<br />

• Synchronic use for new forms<br />

• Recursiveness<br />

• Chuxnaban Mixe<br />

• No restrictions <strong>as</strong> long <strong>as</strong> there is semantic need<br />

• Strong semantic predictability (endocentric)<br />

• Regularity: right-headedness<br />

• New concepts, also Spanish loans


Summary and Conclusions<br />

• Compounds dist<strong>in</strong>guished from phr<strong>as</strong>es<br />

• Ma<strong>in</strong>ly phonologically, <strong>as</strong> noun phr<strong>as</strong>es are<br />

morphologically simple<br />

• Morphosyntax: Fixed order and treated <strong>as</strong> a<br />

whole for <strong>in</strong>flection<br />

• Further studies<br />

• Mismatches between phonological and<br />

grammatical word (i.e. phonology of separate<br />

words, but fixed word order)


Summary and Conclusions<br />

• <strong>Nom<strong>in</strong>al</strong> compound<strong>in</strong>g fairly productive<br />

• Used synchronically for new forms<br />

• Some regularity and semantic predictability<br />

• Largest group: endocentric (right-headed)<br />

• No restrictions due to phonological shape<br />

• Some restrictions on modify<strong>in</strong>g compounds<br />

(mëjnëëjteech ‘sea’; mëj ‘large’, nëëj ‘water’, teech’ ‘father’;<br />

can’t say ‘large sea’)


Bibliography<br />

• Aikhenvald, Alexandra. 2007. Typological dist<strong>in</strong>ctions <strong>in</strong> word<strong>formation</strong>.<br />

In Timothy Shopen ed. Language typology and syntactic<br />

description: Volume III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon.<br />

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1-65.<br />

• Bauer, Laurie. Compound. 2006. In: Keith Brown ed. Encyclopedia of<br />

Language & L<strong>in</strong>guistics. Second Edition. Elsevier. 719-726.<br />

• Campbell, Lyle, Terrence Kaufman, Thom<strong>as</strong> C. Smith-Stark. 1986.<br />

Meso-America <strong>as</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Area. Language 62:3. 530-570.<br />

• Down<strong>in</strong>g, Pamela. 1977. On the Creation and Use of English<br />

Compound Nouns. Language 53:4. 810-842.


Bibliography<br />

• Herrera, Z. Esther. 1995. Palabr<strong>as</strong> estratos y<br />

de fonologia léxica en Zoque. El Colegio de México<br />

• Jany, Carmen. 2011. The phonetics and phonology of Chuxnabán<br />

Mixe. L<strong>in</strong>guistic Discovery 9:1.<br />

• Jany, Carmen. 2010. Assimilatory processes <strong>in</strong> Chuxnabán Mixe.<br />

Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs from the Annual Workshop on Native American Languages.<br />

University of California, Santa Barbara.<br />

• Jany, Carmen. 2006. Vowel Length and Phonation Contr<strong>as</strong>ts <strong>in</strong><br />

Chuxnabán Mixe. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs from the Annual Workshop on Native<br />

American Languages. University of California, Santa Barbara.<br />

• Lieber, Rochelle and Pavol Štekauer (eds). 2009. The Oxford<br />

Handbook of <strong>Compound<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.


Bibliography<br />

• Romero-Méndez, Rodrigo. 2008. A Reference Grammar of Ayutla Mixe<br />

(Tukyo’m Ayuujk). Dissertation. University of Buffalo: New York.<br />

• Ruiz de Bravo Ahuja, Gloria. 1980. Mixe Tlahuitoltepec, Oaxaca.<br />

Archivo de Lengu<strong>as</strong> Indígen<strong>as</strong> de México. (Data by Don D. Lyon)<br />

• Schoenhals, Alv<strong>in</strong> and Louise C. Schoenhals. 1982. Vocabulario Mixe<br />

de Totontepec. Hidalgo, Mexico: Summer Institute of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.<br />

• Sergio Scalise and Irene Vogel (eds.). 2010. Cross-discipl<strong>in</strong>ary issues<br />

<strong>in</strong> compound<strong>in</strong>g. Amsterdam: John Benjam<strong>in</strong>s.<br />

• Van Haitsma, Julia Dieterman and Willard Van Haitsma. 1976. A<br />

Hierarchical Sketch of Mixe <strong>as</strong> spoken <strong>in</strong> San José El Paraíso. No. 44.<br />

Mexico: Summer Institute of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.


Dios kujuuyëp!<br />

Thank you!

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!