14.01.2014 Views

Roger, Me, and My Attitude: Film Propaganda and Cynicism Toward ...

Roger, Me, and My Attitude: Film Propaganda and Cynicism Toward ...

Roger, Me, and My Attitude: Film Propaganda and Cynicism Toward ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Journal of Applied Psychology<br />

1992, Vol. 77, No. 5, 768-771<br />

Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.<br />

0021-9010/92/$3.00<br />

<strong>Roger</strong>, <strong>Me</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>My</strong> <strong>Attitude</strong>: <strong>Film</strong> Propag<strong>and</strong>a<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Cynicism</strong> <strong>Toward</strong> Corporate Leadership<br />

Thomas S. Bateman<br />

Kenan-Flagler Business School<br />

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill<br />

Tomoaki Sakano <strong>and</strong> Makoto Fujita<br />

School of Commerce<br />

Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan<br />

Two studies of U.S. <strong>and</strong> Japanese respondents assessed attitudes resulting from viewing the film<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> (Moore, 1989). In Experiment 1, the responses of 162 adults who had seen the film in<br />

the U.S. were contrasted with 106 people about to view the film. Those who saw the film exhibited<br />

more cynical (negative) attitudes toward General Motors in particular <strong>and</strong> toward U.S. business in<br />

general. Experiment 2 employed a Solomon four-group design in Tokyo, Japan, to assess the<br />

generalizability of the U.S. results <strong>and</strong> also to assess attitude change from pretest to posttest.<br />

Consistent with the U.S. results, viewing the film had negative effects on Japanese attitudes toward<br />

General Motors <strong>and</strong> toward U.S. business in general. Furthermore, attitudes toward Japanese<br />

business became slightly more positive as a function of viewing the film. Implications <strong>and</strong> future<br />

research needs are discussed.<br />

As U.S. corporations restructure, American workers are being<br />

laid off in record numbers. For this <strong>and</strong> other reasons, cynicism—or<br />

negative, distrustful attitudes toward authority <strong>and</strong><br />

institutions—was described in the late 1980s as a "consensus<br />

worldview" with implications for not only the workplace but<br />

also society as a whole (Kanter & Mirvis, 1989). Public opinion<br />

research data showed a crisis of confidence in big business,<br />

government, <strong>and</strong> labor unions (Upset & Schneider, 1987).<br />

These attitudes have a variety of causes. Even feature movies<br />

help shape (<strong>and</strong> reflect) public opinion (Doob, 1948). <strong>Film</strong>s are<br />

said to represent the status of the American dream, whether<br />

characterized by belief or doubt, optimism or pessimism<br />

(Nimmo & Combs, 1983). This portrayal of film describes at<br />

least one film distributed recently in movie theaters across the<br />

United States (<strong>and</strong> in Japan). The film, <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> (Moore,<br />

1989), was highly critical of General Motors' plant closings <strong>and</strong><br />

layoffs of tens of thous<strong>and</strong>s of workers in Flint, Michigan. The<br />

film's potential importance was captured by the remarks of<br />

Newsweek's media critic:<br />

General Motors wrote the book on stupid public relations. The<br />

thrashing of the company <strong>and</strong> its CEO in the smash-hit movie<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> is one of the worst examples of corporate PR since<br />

GM hired a detective to tail an unknown auto-safety advocate<br />

named Ralph Nader in 1966. That fiasco . . . helped kick off a<br />

public backlash against American business that lasted for IS<br />

years. [<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>] could do the same. (Alter, 1990, p. 52)<br />

!<br />

The purpose of the present study was to assess empirically<br />

the potential impact of the popular media on people's attitudes<br />

We wish to thank the Center for Management Studies of the Kenan-<br />

Flagler Business School, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill,<br />

for financial support of this project. Thanks also to Bruce Barry, Edie<br />

Busija, <strong>and</strong> Mike Grant for their assistance with data collection <strong>and</strong><br />

analysis, <strong>and</strong> to two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.<br />

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to<br />

Thomas S. Bateman, Kenan-Flagler Business School, University of<br />

North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27599-3490.<br />

768<br />

toward U.S. corporations <strong>and</strong> their leadership. Specifically, we<br />

found that <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> had significant <strong>and</strong> predictable effects<br />

on viewers' attitudes toward these issues.<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>, though based in fact, can be described as propag<strong>and</strong>a—a<br />

deliberate, directive communication that attempts to<br />

change the target's perceptions, cognitions, <strong>and</strong> behavior in<br />

ways that further the objective of the propag<strong>and</strong>ist (Jowett &<br />

O'Donnell, 1986). The film was the medium used by the producer,<br />

Michael Moore, to shape public opinion toward General<br />

Motors, its leadership, corporate America, <strong>and</strong> the plight of<br />

U.S. workers. The messages intended by Michael Moore were<br />

that the American dream is dead, that corporations are<br />

disloyal, <strong>and</strong> that "we are living in a modern-day 'Grapes of<br />

Wrath' where millions of Americans are struggling to survive"<br />

(Donahue, 1990). The story described the effects on Moore's<br />

hometown, Flint, Michigan, of General Motors plant closings<br />

that put some 30,000 employees out of work. Moore was the<br />

champion of the unemployed, <strong>and</strong> General Motor's chief executive<br />

officer, <strong>Roger</strong> Smith, was depicted as the perpetrator of<br />

widespread harm.<br />

The film's impact can be inferred from anecdotal <strong>and</strong> empirical<br />

evidence. Anecdotally, reactions included such statements<br />

as "The big shots are not concerned about the little people"<br />

(Donahue, 1990) <strong>and</strong> "I'm sure [<strong>Roger</strong> Smith] would cut your<br />

throat in a second. Like he did the people in Flint" (Duffy,<br />

1990, p. 44). A movie reviewer declared, "I swear you'll<br />

never watch a cheery GM ad in quite the same way" (Ringel,<br />

1990, p. 17).<br />

Empirically, some research (Adams et al., 1985; Elliott &<br />

Schenck-Hamlin, 1979; Roberts & Maccoby, 1985) has shown<br />

that a single exposure to a film can affect viewers' attitudes. No<br />

study known to us has tested the impact of film on work- or<br />

business-related attitudes. On the basis of the anecdotal evidence<br />

noted in the previous paragraph, the premise <strong>and</strong> findings<br />

of empirical studies of the attitudinal impact of film, <strong>and</strong><br />

the generalized conclusion from propag<strong>and</strong>a research that peo-


ROGER & ME 769<br />

pie respond favorably to a message even when it is obvious that<br />

it is biased (Jowett & O'Donnell, 1986), we predicted that <strong>Roger</strong><br />

& <strong>Me</strong> would have a direct effect on audience attitudes toward<br />

the targets criticized in the film, in a direction consistent with<br />

that intended by the producer. That is, people who had seen the<br />

film were expected to express more cynical (negative) attitudes<br />

toward <strong>Roger</strong> Smith <strong>and</strong> General Motors than were people who<br />

had not viewed the film.<br />

Although the explicit targets of Michael Moore's attacks were<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> Smith <strong>and</strong> his corporation, the ultimate "villain" was big<br />

business (Duffy, 1990). Thus, we anticipated that audience reactions<br />

would generalize beyond General Motors. This prediction<br />

follows not only from Michael Moore's objectives but also<br />

from prominent theories of attitude structure <strong>and</strong> cognitive inference.<br />

<strong>Attitude</strong>s have a certain degree of consistency with one<br />

another, as known from decades of theorizing <strong>and</strong> research in<br />

cognitive consistency (Festinger, 1957; Heider, 1958; cf. Judd &<br />

Krosnick, 1988). Moreover, people are readily influenced in<br />

their attitudes toward populations by information from a single<br />

case (Hamill, Wilson, & Nisbett, 1980; Nisbett & Ross, 1980).<br />

Given that General Motors is a single case in the population of<br />

major U.S. corporations, <strong>and</strong> that it is vividly <strong>and</strong> negatively<br />

portrayed in <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>, cognitive consistency theory <strong>and</strong> sample-to-population<br />

inferential biases led to the prediction that<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> would affect audience attitudes toward U.S. business<br />

in general. People who viewed the film, compared with<br />

those who had not yet viewed the film, were predicted to express<br />

more cynical (negative) attitudes toward U.S. corporations<br />

<strong>and</strong> their leadership.<br />

<strong>Me</strong>thod<br />

Experiment 1<br />

Sample <strong>and</strong> procedure. The sample consisted of 268 adults who<br />

attended a movie theater in Durham, North Carolina, for the purpose<br />

of viewing <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>. The average age of the study sample was 36, <strong>and</strong><br />

49% of the respondents were male.<br />

A graduate assistant distributed <strong>and</strong> collected questionnaires in the<br />

theater lobby, with the permission of the theater manager. Over a 2-<br />

week time period, beginning immediately after the film's release,<br />

theater patrons were solicited both as they entered <strong>and</strong> exited the<br />

theater in which the film appeared. This resulted in two subsamples of<br />

volunteer respondents: one that had not yet viewed the film (« = 106)<br />

<strong>and</strong> one that had (« = 162). Thus, the study design contrasted a pretest<br />

group with a posttest group. The two groups did not differ significantly<br />

with respect to age, gender, tendency to sympathize with labor<br />

versus management (as measured by a single item in the survey), or<br />

interest in the film <strong>and</strong> the subject matter (as inferred from the fact that<br />

all respondents had purchased tickets for <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>). This comparability<br />

established the viability of using pretest scores as control group<br />

data against which to compare posttest attitudes of the experimental<br />

group.<br />

<strong>Me</strong>asures. Respondents completed a written survey. The instructions<br />

introduced the nature of the study <strong>and</strong> conveyed the anonymity<br />

<strong>and</strong> confidentiality of the results. Care was taken to encourage complete<br />

honesty in responding, to indicate that there were no right or<br />

wrong answers, <strong>and</strong> to imply a lack of bias, preconception, or social or<br />

political agenda on the part of the researchers.<br />

The survey was constructed to assess two dependent measures: attitude<br />

toward General Motors, <strong>and</strong> general cynicism toward U.S. business<br />

<strong>and</strong> its leadership. The General Motors measure consisted of two<br />

7-point Likert items asking respondents to indicate their general opinions<br />

of General Motors <strong>and</strong> of <strong>Roger</strong> Smith. The cynicism measure<br />

consisted of nine 7-f>oint items, to which respondents indicated the<br />

extent of their agreement or disagreement. To reduce the negative connotation<br />

<strong>and</strong> inflammatory potential of cynical phrases in the survey,<br />

most of the items were phrased in a positive rather than negative direction.<br />

Examples are "Most of our top business leaders are devoted to the<br />

service <strong>and</strong> good of their employees" "I have great respect for the<br />

major corporations of our country," <strong>and</strong> "The American dream, in<br />

which anyone can work hard <strong>and</strong> get ahead, is alive <strong>and</strong> well." Lower<br />

scores on this scale indicate more negative attitudes <strong>and</strong> higher levels<br />

of cynicism.<br />

Results <strong>and</strong> Discussion<br />

The two-item General Motors variable had internal reliabilities<br />

of .83 (r = .70) in the pretest group <strong>and</strong> .80 (r = .67) in the<br />

posttest group. The cynicism measure had internal reliabilities<br />

of .75 <strong>and</strong> .82 in the two groups.<br />

The two dependent variables were significantly correlated<br />

(r = .48, p < .001). We conducted two separate univariate analyses<br />

of variance, which was appropriate given our independent<br />

interest in the two variables <strong>and</strong> our cognizance of the implications<br />

of multiple tests (Huberty & Morris, 1989). People who<br />

had seen the film exhibited a more negative attitude toward<br />

General Motors (M = 3.8, SD = 2.3) than people who had not<br />

seen the film (M = 6.15, SD = 2.8), F(l, 246) = 51.40, p < .001.<br />

Those who had seen the film also reported greater cynicism or<br />

negative attitudes toward U.S. business in general (M = 23A,<br />

SD = 8.8) than did those who had not seen the film (M= 26.9,<br />

SD = 8.3), F(l, 260) = 9.95, p < .05. Thus, the results support<br />

the predictions that viewing <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> would affect audience<br />

attitudes in directions intended by the film's producer.<br />

Because of practical constraints on the study design, limitations<br />

temper the strength of the conclusions regarding the impact<br />

of the film. Demonstrable attitude change—coupled with<br />

no change in a pre-post control group—would be the most<br />

compelling evidence of the film's impact. Therefore, a second<br />

study was conducted. This study differed from the first in that<br />

we added measures of attitude change <strong>and</strong> an appropriate control<br />

group. In addition, the study measured the film's impact on<br />

a sample of moviegoers in a different country.<br />

Experiment 2<br />

In the summer of 1990, Warner Brothers released a subtitled<br />

version of <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> (Moore, 1989) in Japan. Experiment 2<br />

offered methodological advances over the first experiment <strong>and</strong><br />

served to test the generalizability of the U.S. results as well as to<br />

assess attitudes toward Japanese business. We predicted a replication<br />

(with extension to pretest-posttest attitude change) of<br />

the results of Experiment 1: People were expected to express<br />

more negative attitudes toward General Motors <strong>and</strong> U.S. business<br />

after viewing the film, compared with their own pre-viewing<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> with the attitudes of respondents who had not<br />

viewed the film.<br />

Moreover, we predicted that the film would lead the audience<br />

to perceive Japanese business more positively. Given the<br />

Japanese philosophy that workers <strong>and</strong> their firms share the<br />

same fate, viewing the negative consequences of the layoffs im-


770 T. BATEMAN, T. SAKANO, AND M. FUJITA<br />

posed by American executives on their employees was expected<br />

to provide a st<strong>and</strong>ard of comparison by which Japanese company<br />

policies would look all the more fair <strong>and</strong> honorable.<br />

Through processes of social comparison (Kelley & Thibaut,<br />

1978) <strong>and</strong> pressures toward cognitive consistency (Judd &<br />

Krosnick, 1988), we predicted that people would express more<br />

positive attitudes toward Japanese business after viewing the<br />

film, when compared with their own pre-viewing attitudes <strong>and</strong><br />

with the attitudes of people who had not viewed the film.<br />

<strong>Me</strong>thod<br />

Sample <strong>and</strong> procedure. The sample consisted of 542 university<br />

students in Tokyo, Japan. The average age of the sample was 20.8 years,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 87.6% of the sample was male. Twelve sections of a management<br />

course required of all commerce students were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned to<br />

four experimental conditions in a Solomon four-group design. Two of<br />

the groups completed a questionnaire in class before <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> was<br />

released in Japan. Approximately half of the students who completed<br />

the survey, <strong>and</strong> half who did not, then viewed <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> in a local<br />

theater. Subsequently, all students completed the same questionnaire.<br />

Thus, two groups replicated Study 1: Group 1 (« = 132) saw the film <strong>and</strong><br />

completed a posttest (without a pretest), <strong>and</strong> Group 2 (n = 144) was a<br />

control group that completed a posttest concomitantly with Group 1<br />

but without having viewed the film. Two other groups were added to<br />

the Study 1 design: Group 3 (n = 137) was an experimental group that<br />

completed a pretest, saw the film, <strong>and</strong> then completed a posttest, <strong>and</strong><br />

Group 4 (n = 129) was a control group that completed two surveys<br />

concomitantly with Group 3 but did not view the film.<br />

<strong>Me</strong>asures. Survey items from Experiment 1 were translated into<br />

Japanese for use in Experiment 2. The Experiment 2 questionnaire<br />

also measured a third dependent variable not used in Experiment 1.<br />

This scale adapted Experiment 1 items to assess attitudes toward Japanese<br />

business <strong>and</strong> its leaders. Six of the nine items of the cynicism<br />

scale—excluding items that were presumed to be too difficult for the<br />

average Japanese to validly express an attitude toward (prior to seeing<br />

the film)—were reworded with a Japanese rather than U.S. business<br />

referent.<br />

Results <strong>and</strong> Discussion<br />

The General Motors measure had a coefficient alpha of .64<br />

(r = .48) in the pretest-only groups <strong>and</strong> .82 (r = .69) in the<br />

posttest-only groups. The scale measuring cynicism toward<br />

U.S. business showed reliabilities of .64 (pretest-only groups)<br />

<strong>and</strong> .73 (posttest-only groups). The measure of attitudes toward<br />

Japanese business had a coefficient alpha of .78 in both the<br />

pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest samples.<br />

Consistent with the recommendations of Solomon (1954),<br />

we conducted a two-way analysis of variance, using Time 2<br />

measures as dependent variables. The independent variables<br />

were whether or not subjects had (a) seen the film <strong>and</strong> (b) taken<br />

a pretest. Results revealed significant effects of film viewing on<br />

all dependent variables. As in Study 1, the group that had seen<br />

the film had more negative attitudes toward General Motors<br />

(M= 3.75, SD = 1.9) than did the group that had not (M = 5.5,<br />

SD = 2.75), F(\, 538) = 130.39, p < .001. Those who saw the<br />

film also held more cynical (negative) attitudes toward U.S. business<br />

(M = 28.8, SD = 7.2) than those who had not (M = 33.8,<br />

SD = 5.9), F(\, 533) = 76.42, p < .001. In addition, the group<br />

that saw the film reported slightly more positive attitudes toward<br />

Japanese business (M = 22.3, SD = 5.2) than the group<br />

that did not (M= 20.7, SD = 5.6), F(\, 535) = 12.32, p < .05.<br />

Analysis of the posttest scores further revealed that the pretest,<br />

either as a main effect or in interaction with film viewing,<br />

had no significant effect on general attitudes toward either U.S.<br />

or Japanese business. There was a main effect of the pretest on<br />

attitudes toward General Motors, F(\, 538) = 3.94, p < .05, <strong>and</strong><br />

an interaction effect as well, F(l, 538) = 6.29, p < .05. The<br />

significant main effect stemmed from the interaction, in which<br />

posttest scores for subjects who did not see the film were comparable<br />

to the scores of those who did (M= 7.6, SD = 1.8) <strong>and</strong> did<br />

not (M= 7.5, SD = 2.0) complete the pretest, whereas filmgoers<br />

who completed the pretest had more negative attitudes (M =<br />

5.1, SD = 2.2) than filmgoers who did not (M = 5.9, SD = 2.2).<br />

Separate analyses were performed on the treatment <strong>and</strong> control<br />

groups that provided measures of attitude change (Groups<br />

3 <strong>and</strong> 4). <strong>Me</strong>ans for pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest scores for the three<br />

dependent variables, in the treatment <strong>and</strong> control groups, are<br />

shown in Table 1. Paired (correlated) t tests showed that the<br />

changes in the group that viewed the film were highly significant<br />

for all variables. In contrast, the control group did not<br />

change attitudes toward General Motors or U.S. business; the<br />

change in attitude toward Japanese business was significant but<br />

quite small.<br />

General Discussion<br />

<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> was described by a leading critic (Gene Siskel) as<br />

one of the two films that most epitomize the Reagan era<br />

(Moore, 1990). As such, it was an important film release, one<br />

intended to shape public opinion in the United States. <strong>Roger</strong> &<br />

<strong>Me</strong> had an impact on Japanese audiences as well. Taken to-<br />

Table 1<br />

<strong>Me</strong>ans, St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations, <strong>and</strong> Paired t Tests for Changes in Dependent Variables<br />

Treatment group<br />

Control group<br />

Dependent<br />

variable<br />

M<br />

Pretest<br />

SD<br />

M<br />

Posttest<br />

SD<br />

1(136)<br />

M<br />

Pretest<br />

SD<br />

M<br />

Postest<br />

SD<br />

t(128)<br />

<strong>Attitude</strong> toward General Motors<br />

<strong>Attitude</strong> toward U.S. business<br />

<strong>Attitude</strong> toward Japanese business<br />

3.92<br />

3.87<br />

3.38<br />

0.83<br />

0.76<br />

0.86<br />

2.56<br />

3.16<br />

3.77<br />

1.11<br />

0.85<br />

0.86<br />

12.39**<br />

8.64**<br />

-5.73**<br />

3.71<br />

3.80<br />

3.33<br />

0.91<br />

0.63<br />

0.92<br />

3.80<br />

3.77<br />

3.44<br />

0.92<br />

0.60<br />

0.93<br />

-1.55<br />

0.42<br />

-2.52*<br />

*/>


ROGER & ME 771<br />

gether, the two studies revealed (a) significant negative effects of<br />

the film on U.S. audiences' attitudes toward General Motors<br />

<strong>and</strong> US. business in general; (b) similar effects on Japanese<br />

audiences' attitudes toward General Motors <strong>and</strong> U.S. business;<br />

(c) a significant positive impact on Japanese attitudes toward<br />

Japanese business; <strong>and</strong> (d) attitude change, in directions consistent<br />

with the other findings, in the Japanese audience. Thus,<br />

the presumed potential of the popular media to influence audience<br />

attitudes toward business has been empirically demonstrated.<br />

Longer term effects, both attitudinal <strong>and</strong> behavioral, should<br />

be the focus of future research. Theoretically, the essential issue<br />

with regard to both long-term attitude <strong>and</strong> behavioral change<br />

may be how the message is processed. Enduring attitude<br />

change, <strong>and</strong> attitudes that are most strongly predictive of corresponding<br />

behaviors, are more likely to occur through central<br />

rather than peripheral information processing (Petty & Cacioppo,<br />

1986). A central route implies active processing in<br />

which the person consciously thinks about the issue; the central<br />

route is activated when the audience is motivated <strong>and</strong> able to<br />

process the information. Specific pertinent conditions include<br />

repetition <strong>and</strong> comprehensibility of the message, lack of distraction,<br />

<strong>and</strong> audience involvement (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). It<br />

is likely that most if not all of these conditions were met in the<br />

present study by the self-selection of participants <strong>and</strong> the viewing<br />

of a vivid, 2-hr film in a closed environment. For the Japanese<br />

sample, completion of the pretest may have enhanced this<br />

effect.<br />

How corporations <strong>and</strong> their leaders respond to media criticism,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the effectiveness of different responses in influencing<br />

the perceptions <strong>and</strong> behaviors of various stakeholder<br />

groups, are related areas ripe for future study. Ultimately, a key<br />

question is whether public opinion toward corporations, as<br />

shaped by the media, carries any practical import. In Japan, a<br />

film reviewer used <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> as a warning to his country: "If<br />

we cannot make the cars which fit the customers' needs, Japan<br />

will follow the same destiny [as General Motors]" (Hirai, 1990,<br />

p. 44). In the United States, General Motors suffered some bad<br />

publicity at the least. Moreover, the success of <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

the kudos afforded its producer may pave the way for more<br />

feature films with an antibusiness agenda. To the extent that<br />

the media are successful at sustaining or increasing cynicism<br />

among workers <strong>and</strong> other stakeholders, the cumulative impact<br />

could be substantial (Kanter & Mirvis, 1989). Therefore, future<br />

research should continue to assess the impact of the media on<br />

cynicism, other attitudes, <strong>and</strong> behaviors toward U.S. business<br />

<strong>and</strong> its leadership.<br />

References<br />

Adams, W, Salzman, A., Vantine, W, Suelter, L., Baker, A., Bonvouloir,<br />

L., Brenner, B., Ely, M, Feldman, J, & Ziegel, R. (1985). The power<br />

of The Right Stuff: A quasi-experimental field test of the docudrama<br />

hypothesis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49, 330-339.<br />

Alter, J. (1990, April). Who framed <strong>Roger</strong> Smith? Blame it on General<br />

Motors. Business Month, p. 52.<br />

Donahue (1990, January 29). On location in Flint, Michigan: <strong>Film</strong>maker<br />

Michael Moore, <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>. Part 1. (Patricia McMillen, Producer;<br />

Transcript 2870, Show 0192-90.) (Journal Graphics, New<br />

York).<br />

Doob, L. W (1948). Public opinion <strong>and</strong> propag<strong>and</strong>a. New York: Holt.<br />

Duffy, S. (1990, January 8). The real villain in <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>l Big business.<br />

Newsweek, pp. 42, 44.<br />

Elliott, W, & Schenck-Hamlin, W (1979). <strong>Film</strong>, politics, <strong>and</strong> the press:<br />

The influence of All the President's <strong>Me</strong>n. Journalism Quarterly, 56,<br />

546-553.<br />

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA:<br />

Stanford University Press.<br />

Hamill, R., Wilson, T, & Nisbett, R. (1980). Insensitivity to sample<br />

bias: Generalizing from atypical cases. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Psychology, 39, 578-589.<br />

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New \brk:<br />

Wiley.<br />

Hirai, T. (1990, June 26). "<strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong>": The correct way of viewing the<br />

movie using GM chairman as a model. Asahi Shimbun Weekly AR-<br />

ERA, p. 70.<br />

Huberty, C, & Morris, J. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus multiple<br />

univariate analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 302-308.<br />

Jowett, G, & O'Donnell, D. (1986). Propag<strong>and</strong>a <strong>and</strong> persuasion. Newbury<br />

Park, CA: Sage.<br />

Judd, C, & Krosnick, J. (1988). The structural basis of consistency<br />

among political attitudes. In A. Pratkanis, S. Breckler, & A. Greenwald<br />

(Eds.), <strong>Attitude</strong> structure <strong>and</strong> function (pp. 99-128). Hillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Kanter, D., & Mirvis, P. (1989). The cynical Americans. San Francisco:<br />

Jossey-Bass.<br />

Kelley, H., & Thibaut, J. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of<br />

interdependence. New York: Wiley.<br />

Lipset, S., & Schneider, W (1987). The confidence gap. Baltimore, MD:<br />

Johns Hopkins University Press.<br />

Moore, M. (Producer & Director). (1989). <strong>Roger</strong> & <strong>Me</strong> [<strong>Film</strong>]. Burbank,<br />

CA: Warner Brothers.<br />

Moore, M. (1990, July 15). "<strong>Roger</strong>" <strong>and</strong> I, off to Hollywood <strong>and</strong> home<br />

to Flint. New York Times, pp. HI, H12.<br />

Nimmo, D., & Combs, J. (1983). <strong>Me</strong>diated political realities. New York:<br />

Longman.<br />

Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies <strong>and</strong> shortcomings<br />

of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.<br />

Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of<br />

persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social<br />

psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123-205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.<br />

Ringel, E. (1990, March 2). Nightmare version of It's a Wonderful Life.<br />

Durham Morning Herald, Preview, pp. 17-18.<br />

Roberts, D, & Maccoby, N. (1985). Effects of mass communication. In<br />

G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The h<strong>and</strong>book of social psychology<br />

(3rd ed., Vol. 2, 539-598). New York: R<strong>and</strong>om House.<br />

Solomon, R. (1954). An extension of control group design. American<br />

Journal of Psychology, 67, 573-589.<br />

Received May 13,1991<br />

Revision received April 17,1992<br />

Accepted April 17,1992 •

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!