03.06.2014 Views

Attributional Models of Depression, Loneliness, and Shyness

Attributional Models of Depression, Loneliness, and Shyness

Attributional Models of Depression, Loneliness, and Shyness

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

250 CRAIG A. ANDERSON AND LYNN H. ARNOULT<br />

negative outcomes in many situations, as suggested earlier in Figure 9.1.<br />

If such a maladaptive style is acquired in the absence <strong>of</strong> depression, it<br />

may also serve as a predisposing cause. Data relevant to these two questions<br />

will be considered later.<br />

CONTROLLABILITY AS AN ATTRIBUTIONAL DIMENSION<br />

Weiner's model <strong>and</strong> the reformulated learned helplessness model are<br />

extremely similar in form <strong>and</strong> content. Weiner is not sure that a third<br />

attributional dimension (in addition to locus <strong>and</strong> stability) is needed, but<br />

postulates that controllability is important. The learned helplessness<br />

model postulates a third dimension <strong>of</strong> globality <strong>and</strong> excludes controllability<br />

as an attributional dimension. It appears that the learned helplessness<br />

theorists prefer to think <strong>of</strong> controllability (<strong>and</strong> uncontrollability)<br />

as a characteristic <strong>of</strong> events, rather than <strong>of</strong> attributions. The notion is<br />

that the dimensions <strong>of</strong> locus, stability, <strong>and</strong> globality capture any controllability<br />

differences in attributions, making it superfluous as an attributional<br />

dimension.<br />

Other researchers have questioned this dismissal <strong>of</strong> controllability as<br />

an attributional dimension. Indeed, several have suggested that the perceived<br />

controllability <strong>of</strong> a cause may be the most important factor in<br />

determining future motivational, cognitive, <strong>and</strong> performance effects <strong>of</strong><br />

negative outcomes (e.g., Anderson & Arnoult, in press; Anderson et aL,<br />

1983; Wortman & Dintzer, 1978). We suggest that changes in success<br />

expectancies as a result <strong>of</strong> an outcome depend primarily on the extent<br />

to which the perceived cause <strong>of</strong> the outcome is seen as modifiable,<br />

changeable, or controllable by the person.2 If a negative outcome is seen<br />

as being caused by a controllable factor, subsequent success expectancies<br />

(hence, motivation <strong>and</strong> performance) should not be adversely affected.<br />

If controllability explains the various attributional style-problems<br />

in living data as well as the locus <strong>and</strong> stability <strong>and</strong> globality dimensions,<br />

then there is no reason to adopt the more complex models. That is, the<br />

controllability <strong>of</strong> attributions may determine success expectancy<br />

changes, motivation, performance, <strong>and</strong> affect (including self-esteem).<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the controllability dimension may not be sufficient.<br />

Locus may be particularly important in determining affective reactions.<br />

2We prefer the term changeability (Anderson et a1., 1983) because it implies some control<br />

in the future, whereas controllability may be either future or past directed. Because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

popularity <strong>of</strong> the control concept, <strong>and</strong> because data shows that subjects see the two as<br />

having the same meaning (when the control questions are future oriented, Anderson,<br />

1983c), we have decided to use controllability with this future-oriented focus.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!