07.11.2014 Views

Personality and culture - Wiley Online Library

Personality and culture - Wiley Online Library

Personality and culture - Wiley Online Library

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

European Journal of <strong>Personality</strong><br />

Eur. J. Pers. 23: 149–152 (2009)<br />

Published online in <strong>Wiley</strong> InterScience<br />

(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.718<br />

Editorial<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Culture<br />

In the end of 1980s only about 10 papers were published each year in journals indexed by<br />

the Web of Science where the both terms ‘personality’ <strong>and</strong> ‘<strong>culture</strong>’ appeared conjointly in<br />

the title, abstract or keywords. In the last couple of years, however, about 120 such papers<br />

were published annually. Perhaps more importantly the relationship between personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>culture</strong> is less frequently studied from the point of view of mere ethnographic curiosity<br />

or as another example of idiosyncrasy. It is more common now that cross-cultural studies of<br />

personality are inspired by a desire to solve more fundamental problems: How do different<br />

personality types adapt to various <strong>culture</strong>s? How do specific personality traits support<br />

cultural practices in the societies in which people live? In what way the mean personality<br />

can shape <strong>culture</strong> (Allik & McCrae, 2002).<br />

The development of wide-b<strong>and</strong>width personality questionnaires such as 16PF, EPQ <strong>and</strong><br />

NEO-PI in English gave an incentive to psychologists of other countries to translate these<br />

instruments into their own languages. The first attempts were encouraging (e.g. Church,<br />

1987) <strong>and</strong> soon it was possible to hypothesize that the patterns of covariation among<br />

personality traits in English-speaking populations can be generalized to diverse <strong>culture</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> distinct language families suggesting that personality trait structure is universal to the<br />

whole human species (McCrae & Costa, 1997). In a little while it became also clear that the<br />

generalizability of the trait structure is neither depending on the observer’s perspective<br />

(McCrae, Terracciano, & 78 Members of the <strong>Personality</strong> Profiles of Cultures Project, 2005)<br />

nor on the measuring instrument (Schmitt, Allik, McCrae, & Benet-Martinez, 2007).<br />

Nevertheless, the st<strong>and</strong>ard doctrine, borrowed from the anthropology, that <strong>culture</strong> shapes<br />

personality, largely through child-reading practices, was still the dominating one.<br />

Summarizing research that was done during the previous decade on the intersection of<br />

<strong>culture</strong> <strong>and</strong> personality it was still necessary to warn readers that a caution is required in<br />

arguing for cross-cultural universality of the personality trait structure (Tri<strong>and</strong>is & Suh,<br />

2002). The following years have proved that the structure of personality traits, particularly<br />

the five-factor model of personality, emerges quite consistently across <strong>culture</strong>s leaving<br />

space for relatively small but still meaningful variations (Heine & Buchtel, 2009).<br />

These small cross-cultural variations in the mean levels of personality traits turned to<br />

have a regular geographic pattern (Allik & McCrae, 2004) which were replicable in a large<br />

number of <strong>culture</strong>s (McCrae, Terracciano, & 79 Members of the <strong>Personality</strong> Profiles of<br />

Cultures Project, 2005; Schmitt et al., 2007). These large cross-cultural studies of<br />

personality lead to a question which appeared meaningless only a couple of years before:<br />

how aggregate personality trait determine the type of <strong>culture</strong> in particular countries<br />

(Hofstede & McCrae, 2004). However, the observed geographical pattern of personality<br />

Copyright # 2009 John <strong>Wiley</strong> & Sons, Ltd.


150 Editorial<br />

traits was not always in accordance with the common sense. It is hard to convince both<br />

experts <strong>and</strong> lay persons that Japanese respondents, for example, are low in order,<br />

dutifulness, <strong>and</strong> self-discipline because they regularly score very low on the<br />

conscientiousness scale. Or what sense it does make that in those US states where<br />

people score high on conscientiousness the rate of murder <strong>and</strong> robbery per capita is higher?<br />

(Rentfrow, Gosling, & Potter, 2008). The picture became even more complex when it<br />

turned out that those beliefs people hold about personality characteristics typical of<br />

members of their own <strong>culture</strong>s do not converge with assessed traits (Terracciano et al.,<br />

2005). These discrepancies pose some methodological challenges for comparison of<br />

personality traits across <strong>culture</strong>s (Ashton, 2007; Heine, Buchtel, & Norenzayan, 2008;<br />

Perugini & Richetin, 2007).<br />

Although tendencies to experience either positive or negative emotions are one of the<br />

most reliable indicators of personality dispositions (Watson, 2000), personality psychology<br />

<strong>and</strong> psychology of emotions have developed according to their own separate paths. While<br />

early personality psychologists were primarily inspired by Margaret Mead, researchers of<br />

human emotions were simply unable to escape from the powerful ideas of Charles Darwin.<br />

This is one of the reasons why the big six basic emotions (Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen,<br />

1969) were accepted earlier than the idea of the big five personality traits. Further research<br />

has convincingly demonstrated that universality emotions spreads also to antecedents of<br />

emotions (Scherer, 1997) <strong>and</strong> their display rules (Matsumoto, 1990). While links between<br />

studies of emotion <strong>and</strong> personality are growing stronger, Matsumoto proposed that many<br />

cross-cultural differences in the perception, expression <strong>and</strong> interpretation of emotions can<br />

be explained by personality traits (Matsumoto, 2006). A parallel development in these two<br />

areas, personality <strong>and</strong> emotion research, is by itself a strong argument that they could<br />

benefit from emerging integration between these areas.<br />

This special issue contains six papers from the leading research groups which have been<br />

instrumental in the shaping of the personality-<strong>culture</strong> <strong>and</strong> personality-emotions research<br />

areas during the last two decades. As the editors of the special issue we are hoping that<br />

these six papers contain several new ideas <strong>and</strong> findings that could stimulate progress in<br />

these two areas.<br />

Timothy Church (2009) discusses a possible integration of trait <strong>and</strong> cultural psychology<br />

perspectives, two dominant theoretical approaches in the study of <strong>culture</strong> <strong>and</strong> personality.<br />

This paper summarize the results of cross-cultural studies Church <strong>and</strong> his colleagues have<br />

conducted to test elements of this integrated perspective <strong>and</strong> draw a roadmap in which<br />

direction future studies could be planned.<br />

Steven Heine (Falk, Heine, Yuki, & Takemura, 2009) <strong>and</strong> his colleagues return to the<br />

problem why Westerners self-enhance more than East Asians, at least when it concerns<br />

traditional self-report questionnaires. One way to answer this question is to use some<br />

alternative assessment techniques, particularly the implicit association tests. Their study<br />

provides an answer to the question how much the implicit association test are suited for the<br />

study of cultural differences <strong>and</strong> which method gives the best approximation to ‘true’ selffeelings.<br />

Robert McCrae (2009) are introducing a new concept—ethos—in addition to more<br />

studied assessed personality traits <strong>and</strong> national character stereotypes. Ethos is social<br />

institutions that reflect the operation of personality traits. Although experts can reliably<br />

estimate ethos in Japan <strong>and</strong> US these measures of ethos are not reducible to aggregate<br />

personality traits or national character stereotypes suggesting that these three layers related<br />

to personality are operating relatively independently in the society.<br />

Copyright # 2009 John <strong>Wiley</strong> & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Pers. 23: 149–152 (2009)<br />

DOI: 10.1002/per


Editorial 151<br />

Realo et al. (2009) try to solve a puzzle why national character stereotypes do not agree<br />

with assessed personality traits. One possible answer is that national character stereotypes<br />

are not describing personality stereotypes of one’s own nation but they are formed in<br />

contrast to the perception of a dominant neighbouring nation or to people’s self-rated<br />

personality traits: the typical in-group or out-group member is portrayed in less socially<br />

desirable terms than people’s ratings of their own personality.<br />

Matsumoto, Hee Yoo <strong>and</strong> Fontaine, 2009 <strong>and</strong> his colleagues introduce a concept called<br />

context differentiation (CD) demonstrating how cultural display rules for emotional<br />

expressions are associated with context. Findings indicate that <strong>culture</strong>s were reliably<br />

associated with measures of CD. The framework <strong>and</strong> findings provide a platform for future<br />

research examining how individuals differentiate their behaviours across contexts, <strong>and</strong> how<br />

<strong>culture</strong>s facilitate that differentiation.<br />

Klaus Scherer <strong>and</strong> his team (Scherer & Brosch, 2009) try to elucidate how cultural<br />

factors may encourage the development of affective personality traits or emotional<br />

dispositions by producing or rewarding specific appraisal biases. They propose that cultural<br />

beliefs, values or practices may encourage certain types of appraisal bias <strong>and</strong> may thus<br />

provide an explanation for widely held stereotypes about cultural affect patterns.<br />

We believe that all these six studies exploring the relationship between personality <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>culture</strong> in their own way advance our current knowledge in the result of which we will be<br />

more prepared to underst<strong>and</strong> both human personality <strong>and</strong> human <strong>culture</strong>.<br />

JÜRI ALLIK <strong>and</strong> ANU REALO<br />

Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Allik, J., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A five-factor theory perspective. In McCrae, R. R., & Allik J.<br />

(Eds.), The five factor model of personality across <strong>culture</strong>s (pp. 303–322). New York: Kluwer<br />

Academic/Plenum Publishers.<br />

Allik, J., & McCrae, R. R. (2004). Toward a geography of personality traits—Patterns of profiles<br />

across 36 <strong>culture</strong>s. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 13–28.<br />

Ashton, M. C. (2007). Self-reports <strong>and</strong> stereotypes: A comment on McCrae et al. European Journal of<br />

<strong>Personality</strong>, 21, 983–986.<br />

Church, A. T. (1987). <strong>Personality</strong>-research in a nonwestern <strong>culture</strong>—the Philippines. Psychological<br />

Bulletin, 102, 272–292.<br />

Church, A. T. (2009). Prospects for an integrated trait <strong>and</strong> cultural psychology. European Journal of<br />

<strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 153–182. DOI: 10.1002/per.700<br />

Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of<br />

emotions. Science, 164, 86–88.<br />

Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Yuki, M., & Takemura, K. (2009). Why do Westerners self-enhance more<br />

than East Asians? European Journal of <strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 183–203. DOI: 10.1002/per.715<br />

Heine, S. J., & Buchtel, E. E. (2009). <strong>Personality</strong>: The universal <strong>and</strong> the culturally specific. Annual<br />

Review of Psychology, 60, 369–394.<br />

Heine, S. J., Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). What do cross-national comparisons of<br />

personality traits tell us? The case of conscientiousness. Psychological Science, 19, 309–313.<br />

Hofstede, G., & McCrae, R. R. (2004). <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>culture</strong> revisited: Linking traits <strong>and</strong><br />

dimensions of <strong>culture</strong>. Cross-Cultural Research, 38, 52–88.<br />

Copyright # 2009 John <strong>Wiley</strong> & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Pers. 23: 149–152 (2009)<br />

DOI: 10.1002/per


152 Editorial<br />

Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities <strong>and</strong> differences in display rules. Motivation <strong>and</strong> Emotion,<br />

14, 195–214.<br />

Matsumoto, D. (2006). Are cultural differences in emotion regulation mediated by personality traits?<br />

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 421–437.<br />

Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Fontaine, J., & 56 Members of the Multinational Study of Cultural<br />

Display Rules. (2009). Hypocrisy or maturity? Culture <strong>and</strong> context differentiation. European<br />

Journal of <strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 251–264. DOI: 10.1002/per.716<br />

McCrae, R. R. (2009). <strong>Personality</strong> profiles of <strong>culture</strong>s: Patterns of ethos. European Journal of<br />

<strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 205–227. DOI: 10.1002/per.712<br />

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr., (1997). <strong>Personality</strong> trait structure as a human universal. American<br />

Psychologist, 52, 509–516.<br />

McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the <strong>Personality</strong> Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005).<br />

Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 <strong>culture</strong>s.<br />

Journal of <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 88, 547–561.<br />

McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 Members of the <strong>Personality</strong> Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005).<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> profiles of <strong>culture</strong>s: Aggregate personality traits. Journal of <strong>Personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology, 89, 407–425.<br />

Perugini, M., & Richetin, J. (2007). In the l<strong>and</strong> of the blind, the one-eyed man is king. European<br />

Journal of <strong>Personality</strong>, 21, 977–981.<br />

Realo, A., Allik, J., Lönnqvist, J.-E., Verkasalo, M., Kwiatkowska, A., Kööts, L., et al. (2009).<br />

Mechanisms of the national character stereotype: How people in six neighboring countries of<br />

Russia describe themselves <strong>and</strong> the typical Russian. European Journal of <strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 229–<br />

249. DOI: 10.1002/per.719<br />

Rentfrow, P. J., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). A theory of the emergence, persistence, <strong>and</strong><br />

expression of geographic variation in psychological characteristics. Perspectives on Psychological<br />

Science, 3, 339–369.<br />

Scherer, K. R. (1997). Profiles of emotion-antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical predictions across<br />

<strong>culture</strong>s. Cognition & Emotion, 11, 113–150.<br />

Scherer, K. R., & Brosch, T. (2009). Culture-specific appraisal biases contribute to emotion<br />

dispositions. European Journal of <strong>Personality</strong>, 23, 265–288. DOI: 10.1002/per.714<br />

Schmitt, D. P., Allik, J., McCrae, R. R., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2007). The geographic distribution of<br />

big five personality traits—Patterns <strong>and</strong> profiles of human self-description across 56 nations.<br />

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 173–212.<br />

Terracciano, A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Adam, N., Adamovova, L., Ahn, C., Ahn, H. N., et al. (2005).<br />

National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 <strong>culture</strong>s. Science, 310, 96–<br />

100.<br />

Tri<strong>and</strong>is, H. C., & Suh, E. M. (2002). Cultural influences on personality. Annual Review of<br />

Psychology, 53, 133–160.<br />

Watson, D. (2000). Mood <strong>and</strong> temperament. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Copyright # 2009 John <strong>Wiley</strong> & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Pers. 23: 149–152 (2009)<br />

DOI: 10.1002/per

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!