29.12.2014 Views

Narrative perspective and the emotion regulation of a narrating person

Narrative perspective and the emotion regulation of a narrating person

Narrative perspective and the emotion regulation of a narrating person

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ETC – Empirical Text <strong>and</strong> Culture Research 3, 2007, 50-61<br />

<strong>Narrative</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> a <strong>narrating</strong> <strong>person</strong> 1<br />

Tibor Pólya*, Balázs Kis**, Mátyás Naszódi**, János László*<br />

*Institute for Psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Hungarian Academy <strong>of</strong> Sciences<br />

P. O. Box 398, Budapest, Hungary, H-1394<br />

**Morphologic Ltd., 5. Orbánhegyi út. Budapest, Hungary, H-1126<br />

Abstract<br />

The article defines <strong>the</strong> structural feature <strong>of</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> in self-narratives.<br />

Concentrating on <strong>the</strong> spatio-temporal component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong>, it describes<br />

three formal variations <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong>, termed as retrospective, experiencing, <strong>and</strong><br />

metanarrative forms. The article also provides <strong>the</strong> linguistic operationalisation <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>se <strong>perspective</strong> forms. Following, <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm developed<br />

for <strong>the</strong> automatic coding <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms is presented. This algorithm reliably<br />

codes retrospective <strong>and</strong> experiencing forms <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong>. Finally, an empirical<br />

study regarding <strong>the</strong> relationships between <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> forms <strong>and</strong> trait<br />

features <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> is presented. In this study 83 participants recounted<br />

self-narratives about five significant life story events. The results reveal relationships<br />

between <strong>the</strong> using <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coherence <strong>and</strong> stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong><br />

<strong>regulation</strong>.<br />

Perspective in a self-narrative<br />

Perspective is a ubiquitous structural feature <strong>of</strong> all kinds <strong>of</strong> a narrative text. The term refers to<br />

<strong>the</strong> phenomenon that <strong>the</strong> content <strong>of</strong> a narrative, <strong>the</strong> so called narrative elements such as<br />

events, characters <strong>and</strong> circumstances, are presented from some position or point <strong>of</strong> view<br />

(Peer, <strong>and</strong> Chatman, 2001; Prince, 1987). In this sense, <strong>perspective</strong> is a relational concept<br />

(e.g. Bal, 1998). Two components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> can be differentiated (e.g. Wiebe, 1991).<br />

The first component consists <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spatial <strong>and</strong> temporal dimensions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> position <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

narrative elements. The second component consists <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> possibility that <strong>the</strong> narrator may<br />

express <strong>the</strong> character‟s beliefs, <strong>emotion</strong>s or evaluations. Since <strong>the</strong>se are unperceivable for a<br />

narrator, in <strong>the</strong>se expressions <strong>the</strong> character‟s <strong>perspective</strong> is adopted. This latter component <strong>of</strong><br />

a <strong>perspective</strong> is sometimes called a psychological <strong>perspective</strong> (e.g. Uspensky, 1974). Although<br />

<strong>the</strong> two components <strong>of</strong> a <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten coincide, <strong>the</strong>y do not need to.<br />

Most analysts <strong>of</strong> narrative structure agree that <strong>the</strong>re is no essential difference in a<br />

<strong>perspective</strong> <strong>of</strong> a first-<strong>person</strong> (or a self-) narrative <strong>and</strong> a third-<strong>person</strong> narrative (e.g. Cohn,<br />

1978). However, taking into consideration <strong>the</strong> aforementioned two components <strong>of</strong> a <strong>perspective</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>re is at least one significant difference. In that case when a third-<strong>person</strong> narrator<br />

takes <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>of</strong> one <strong>of</strong> his or her characters, it can be certainly said that ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

1 This research was performed in cooperation between <strong>the</strong> Morphologic Ltd., <strong>the</strong> Institute <strong>of</strong> Psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Hungarian Academy <strong>of</strong> Sciences, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Institute <strong>of</strong> Psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> University <strong>of</strong> Pécs. It was supported by<br />

<strong>the</strong> grant NKFP 2001/5/26, NKFP 2005/6/074; OTKA T-1386 <strong>and</strong> OTKA T-049413. The authors are grateful to<br />

T,M. Lillis for reviewing <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> manuskript. Correspondence should be addressed to <strong>the</strong> first<br />

author: polya@mtapi.hu


NARRATING PERSPECTIVE AND EMOTION REGULATION 51<br />

<strong>person</strong>‟s <strong>perspective</strong> is adopted. However, in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a self-narrative, this idea may easily<br />

prove wrong. It is because <strong>the</strong> narrator <strong>of</strong> a self-narrative is identical with one <strong>of</strong> his or her<br />

characters. Based on this difference, it can be concluded that <strong>the</strong> spatial <strong>and</strong> temporal<br />

component <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> is more useful in an investigation <strong>of</strong> self-narratives.<br />

In self-narratives two kinds <strong>of</strong> events can be differentiated: <strong>the</strong> narrated events <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

narration itself which can be seen as a sequence <strong>of</strong> verbal acts. Since <strong>the</strong>re are two kinds <strong>of</strong><br />

events <strong>the</strong>y provide two options for a spatio-temporal location <strong>of</strong> both <strong>the</strong> narrative content<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position. Considering this, at least <strong>the</strong> following three forms <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> in a selfnarrative<br />

can be described (see Table 1). In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a retrospective form, <strong>the</strong> narrative content<br />

is located in <strong>the</strong> narrated events while <strong>the</strong> position is located in <strong>the</strong> narration. In <strong>the</strong> case<br />

<strong>of</strong> an experiencing form, both <strong>the</strong> narrative content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position are located in <strong>the</strong> narrated<br />

events. Finally, in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a metanarrative form, both <strong>the</strong> narrative content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

position are located in <strong>the</strong> narration.<br />

Table 1. Three <strong>perspective</strong> forms <strong>of</strong> a self-narrative<br />

Perspective forms<br />

Spatio-temporal location<br />

<strong>Narrative</strong> content<br />

Position<br />

Retrospective form Narrated events Narration<br />

Experiencing form Narrated events Narrated events<br />

Metanarrative form Narration Narration<br />

Linguistic operationalisation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms<br />

This concept <strong>of</strong> a <strong>perspective</strong> in a self-narrative can be defined linguistically by taking into<br />

consideration those linguistic markers which have a role in spatio-temporal location. There<br />

are two ways <strong>of</strong> locating spatially <strong>and</strong> temporally both <strong>the</strong> narrative content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position.<br />

The narrator may provide <strong>the</strong> location independently from his or her actual location. In this<br />

case <strong>the</strong> narrator uses specific terms referring to places (e.g. name <strong>of</strong> a town or a street) or<br />

dates (name <strong>of</strong> a month or day). Alternatively, <strong>the</strong> narrator may consider his or her actual<br />

location. In this dependent case, <strong>the</strong> narrator uses spatial (e.g. <strong>the</strong>re, here) or temporal (e.g.<br />

<strong>the</strong>n, now) deictic terms. Both <strong>the</strong> independent <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> dependent ways appear in a selfnarrative,<br />

<strong>and</strong> both <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m are able to define a particular spatial or temporal location. However,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re are important differences between <strong>the</strong>ir uses. The terms used in an independent<br />

location are based on a well elaborated spatial <strong>and</strong> temporal system, while <strong>the</strong> deictic terms<br />

are related to a more simpler spatial <strong>and</strong> temporal system, since <strong>the</strong>y locate ei<strong>the</strong>r distally or<br />

proximally <strong>the</strong> content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position to each o<strong>the</strong>r. Ano<strong>the</strong>r difference reflects <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

frequency in a self-narrative. Deictic terms appear much more frequently than terms referring<br />

to places <strong>and</strong> dates.<br />

The linguistic operationalisation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three <strong>perspective</strong> forms consists <strong>of</strong> four groups<br />

<strong>of</strong> linguistic markers (see Table 2). The most frequently occurring group <strong>of</strong> linguistic markers<br />

are <strong>the</strong> temporal deictic markers, since this group consists <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> markers <strong>of</strong> verb tense. The<br />

use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> present verb tense locates narrative content <strong>and</strong> position proximally, while <strong>the</strong> use<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> past verb tense locates <strong>the</strong>m distally. In addition, <strong>the</strong>re are several temporal adverbs<br />

which are able to locate ei<strong>the</strong>r proximally or distally <strong>the</strong> narrative content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position to<br />

each o<strong>the</strong>r (e.g. most [now], akkor [<strong>the</strong>n]). The second group <strong>of</strong> linguistic markers consists <strong>of</strong><br />

those spatial adverbs <strong>and</strong> demonstrative pronouns which also can be used in a proximal or a<br />

distal location (e.g. itt [here], ez [this], ott [<strong>the</strong>re], az [that]). The third group <strong>of</strong> linguistic


52<br />

PÓLYA, KIS, NASZÓDI, LÁSZLÓ<br />

markers consists <strong>of</strong> specific terms which are related to one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three <strong>perspective</strong> forms.<br />

Those terms which are used in a narrator independent way <strong>of</strong> location are related to <strong>the</strong><br />

retrospective form since <strong>the</strong>ir meaning refers to a distal spatial or temporal location (e.g.<br />

Budapest [Budapest], Január [January]). Interjections are related to <strong>the</strong> experiencing form<br />

since in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se terms <strong>the</strong> content <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> position are located proximally to each<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r (Wilkins, 1995) (e.g. hupsz [ops]). Finally, <strong>the</strong>re are some verbs <strong>and</strong> several modifiers<br />

which are related to <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form (Kiefer, 1990) since <strong>the</strong>se terms ei<strong>the</strong>r refer to a<br />

present mental act <strong>of</strong> a narrator (e.g. emlékszem [I remember]) or express <strong>the</strong> present stance <strong>of</strong><br />

a narrator toward <strong>the</strong> narrative content (e.g. valószínűleg [probably]) respectively. The last<br />

group <strong>of</strong> linguistic markers consists <strong>of</strong> sentence types. Clauses with <strong>the</strong> retrospective <strong>perspective</strong><br />

form are always statements. However, clauses with <strong>the</strong> experiencing or metanarrative<br />

forms can be a statement, a question, an exclamation or an optative.<br />

Linguistic markers<br />

Time deixis<br />

Tense<br />

Temporal adverbs<br />

Space deixis<br />

Spatial adverbs<br />

Demonstrative pronouns<br />

Specific terms<br />

Table 2. Linguistic markers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms<br />

Retrospective<br />

form<br />

Past<br />

e.g. Akkor (Then)<br />

Experiencing<br />

form<br />

Present<br />

e.g.Most (Now)<br />

Metanarrative form<br />

Present<br />

e.g. Most (Now)<br />

e.g. Ott (There)<br />

e.g. Az (That)<br />

e.g Itt (Here)<br />

e.g. Ez (This)<br />

e.g Itt (Here)<br />

e.g. Ez (This)<br />

Date <strong>and</strong> space terms Interjections Mental verbs <strong>and</strong><br />

e.g. Január (January), e.g. Hupsz (Ops) modifiers<br />

Budapest (Budapest)<br />

e.g. Emlékszem<br />

(I remember),<br />

Valószínűleg<br />

(Probably)<br />

Sentence type Only statement No constrain No constrain<br />

<strong>Narrative</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm<br />

The first algorithm for <strong>the</strong> automatic coding <strong>of</strong> a <strong>perspective</strong> in a narrative text was developed<br />

by Wiebe (1991). This algorithm analyses <strong>the</strong> psychological component <strong>of</strong> a <strong>perspective</strong> in<br />

third-<strong>person</strong> narratives. The algorithm has two main components. The first component makes<br />

a differentiation between sentences that objectively narrate events (objective sentence) <strong>and</strong><br />

sentences which present character‟s thoughts, perceptions, <strong>and</strong> inner states (subjective sentence,<br />

see Banfield, 1982). The second component identifies whose psychological point <strong>of</strong><br />

view is expressed in a subjective sentence. Based on <strong>the</strong>ir function, linguistic markers can be<br />

grouped into three groups since <strong>the</strong>y ei<strong>the</strong>r initiate, continue or resume a character‟s point <strong>of</strong><br />

view.<br />

Our narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm differs in three respects from that <strong>of</strong> Wiebe. First,<br />

this algorithm reflects <strong>the</strong> spatio-temporal component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> instead <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

psychological component. Second, <strong>the</strong> unit <strong>of</strong> analysis is <strong>the</strong> narrative clause. Finally, <strong>the</strong><br />

algorithm makes a decision about every narrative clause independently from <strong>the</strong> preceding or<br />

subsequent units. Recently, this line <strong>of</strong> research continued in a development <strong>of</strong> sentiment<br />

analyzers which aim at identifying automatically positive <strong>and</strong> negative opinions, <strong>emotion</strong>s


NARRATING PERSPECTIVE AND EMOTION REGULATION 53<br />

<strong>and</strong> evaluations in a text (e.g. Wilson, Wiebe, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>of</strong>fmann, 2005; Yi, Nasukawa, Bunescu,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Niblack, 2003).<br />

The narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm is based on morphological analysis which is<br />

carried out by <strong>the</strong> HUMOR s<strong>of</strong>tware (Prószéky, <strong>and</strong> Tihanyi, 1993). It lists all linguistic<br />

markers which constitute <strong>the</strong> linguistic operationalisation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> concept. The<br />

linguistic markers are grouped into 12 codes (see Table 3). There are three codes for reflecting<br />

verb tense: present tense (VERB_PRESENT), past tense (VERB_PAST), <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

tense (VERB_OTHER). These codes are assigned by considering <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> a morphological<br />

analysis. While <strong>the</strong> tokens <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> following codes are listed in dictionary files. Temporal<br />

adverbs <strong>and</strong> spatial adverbs with demonstrative pronouns are grouped into two groups<br />

<strong>of</strong> proximal or distal codes (PROXTIME, DISTTIME, <strong>and</strong> PROXLOC, DISTLOC). Among<br />

<strong>the</strong> specific terms interjections (SPEC_INT), relevant mental verbs (SPEC_VERB), <strong>and</strong><br />

relevant modifiers (SPEC_MOD) are also listed in dictionary files. Finally, punctuation marks<br />

are grouped into two groups: sentences closed by a dot (PUNCMARK_.) <strong>and</strong> sentences<br />

closed by an exclamation or a question mark (PUNCMARK_!).<br />

The adopted <strong>perspective</strong> form in a narrative clause is identified by <strong>the</strong> co-presence <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>se linguistic codes. As an example, consider <strong>the</strong> next short excerpt from a hypo<strong>the</strong>tical<br />

narrator who recounts a traffic accident.<br />

(1) I took <strong>the</strong> road to <strong>the</strong> grocery store.<br />

(2) I see a car running to me!<br />

(3) And I do not remember more, even now.”<br />

The narrator adopts retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> form in <strong>the</strong> first sentence, because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> past<br />

verb tense (VERB_PAST). In <strong>the</strong> second sentence, <strong>the</strong> narrator adopts experiencing <strong>perspective</strong><br />

form, indicated by three linguistic markers: present verb tense (VERB_PRESENT),<br />

proximal spatial adverb (PROXLOC) <strong>and</strong> exclamation mark (PUNCMARK_!). Finally, <strong>the</strong><br />

narrator adopts metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong> form in <strong>the</strong> third sentence. It is also indicated by<br />

three markers: <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> verb <strong>of</strong> remember (SPEC_VERB), present verb tense<br />

(VERB_PRESENT), <strong>and</strong> proximal temporal adverb (PROXTIME).<br />

Linguistic markers<br />

Time deixis<br />

Tense<br />

Table 3. Linguistic codes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm<br />

Retrospective<br />

form<br />

VERB_PAST<br />

Experiencing<br />

form<br />

VERB_PRESENT<br />

VERB_OTHER<br />

PROXTIME<br />

Metanarrative<br />

form<br />

VERB_PRESENT<br />

Temporal adverbs DISTTIME<br />

PROXTIME<br />

Space deixis<br />

Spatial adverbs DISTLOC PROXLOC PROXLOC<br />

Demonstrative pronouns DISTLOC PROXLOC PROXLOC<br />

Specific terms -- SPEC_INT SPEC_VERB<br />

SPEC_MOD<br />

Sentence PUNCMARK_. PUNCMARK_! PUNCMARK_!


54<br />

PÓLYA, KIS, NASZÓDI, LÁSZLÓ<br />

Reliability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm<br />

Two studies have investigated <strong>the</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm. The first<br />

study analysed 3 self-narratives which consisted <strong>of</strong> 220 narrative clauses. The performance <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> algorithm was measured by two variables <strong>of</strong> recall <strong>and</strong> accuracy. As <strong>the</strong>se two variables<br />

show, <strong>the</strong> algorithm is most successful in identifying <strong>the</strong> retrospective form, followed by <strong>the</strong><br />

experiencing <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form (see Table 4).<br />

Table 4. Performance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm<br />

Perspective forms<br />

Recall<br />

(%)<br />

Accuracy<br />

(%)<br />

Retrospective form 88.1 96.7<br />

Experiencing form 84.5 83.3<br />

Metanarrative form 62.5 71.4<br />

The second study analysed 130 self-narratives recounted by homosexual men <strong>and</strong> women<br />

participating in an In Vitro Fertilisation (henceforth IVF) treatment (Pólya, 2007). The length<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se self-narratives were 15 696 narrative clauses which were coded manually <strong>and</strong> by <strong>the</strong><br />

narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm as well. The correlation coefficients between two ways <strong>of</strong><br />

coding reflect <strong>the</strong> algorithm‟s performance. The correlation coefficient was r = 0.89, p < 0.01<br />

in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a retrospective form, r = 0.85, p < 0.01 in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> an experiencing form, <strong>and</strong><br />

finally it was r = 0.63, p < 0.01 in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a metanarrative form.<br />

In sum, <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm codes reliably <strong>the</strong> retrospective, <strong>and</strong><br />

experiencing forms, however <strong>the</strong> identification <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form does not reach <strong>the</strong><br />

80% level which was set as a performance criterion. Consequently, <strong>the</strong> results regarding <strong>the</strong><br />

metanarrative form should be treated with some caution.<br />

Perspective in a self-narrative <strong>and</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong><br />

Two studies have investigated <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms in conveying hidden information<br />

about those subjective experiences <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>narrating</strong> <strong>person</strong> which relate to social identity.<br />

Three components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se subjective experiences can be differentiated: meaning, <strong>emotion</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> self-esteem. In <strong>the</strong> first study (Pólya, László, <strong>and</strong> Forgas, 2005), participants were read<br />

short self-narratives. These stories were about significant life events strongly related to some<br />

social identity category: a homosexual man coming out to his parents, a woman learning<br />

about a failure <strong>of</strong> her In Vitro Fertilisation (henceforth IVF) treatment, <strong>and</strong> a Jewish man<br />

recalling how he was informed about his secret ethnic origin. The first two life events are<br />

related to <strong>the</strong> gender identity, while <strong>the</strong> third one is related to <strong>the</strong> ethnic identity <strong>of</strong> a <strong>narrating</strong><br />

<strong>person</strong>. The <strong>perspective</strong> forms taken by <strong>the</strong> narrators were systematically manipulated.<br />

Results revealed that <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> form taken in <strong>the</strong>se self-narratives reflects all three<br />

components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subjective experiences. Considering <strong>the</strong> meaning component, narrators<br />

taking <strong>the</strong> retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> form were perceived as mentally more coherent than<br />

narrators taking <strong>the</strong> experiencing form. Considering <strong>the</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> component, narrators taking<br />

<strong>the</strong> experiencing <strong>perspective</strong> form were perceived as experiencing higher affective intensity<br />

compared to narrators taking <strong>the</strong> retrospective form. Finally, in terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> self-esteem<br />

component, narrators taking a retrospective form were also perceived as more positively


NARRATING PERSPECTIVE AND EMOTION REGULATION 55<br />

valued socially, <strong>and</strong> social value can be seen as a trace <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> self-esteem <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>tical<br />

narrator.<br />

The second study (Pólya, 2007) aimed at exploring <strong>the</strong> same relationships however<br />

taking into consideration self-narratives recounted by real <strong>person</strong>s. Self-narratives were also<br />

about significant life events which were strongly related to a social identity category.<br />

Homosexual men <strong>and</strong> women enrolled in an IVF treatment narrated life events threatening<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir gender identity (e.g. expressing attraction towards a man, or visiting a friend who has<br />

two children, respectively). The quality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> social identity related subjective experience<br />

was explored by several questionnaires. The analysis revealed a similar pattern <strong>of</strong> relationships<br />

as in <strong>the</strong> first study between <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>and</strong> social identity related subjective<br />

experiences. However, <strong>the</strong> most significant relationship emerged between <strong>the</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>al<br />

component <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong>. Frequent use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> retrospective <strong>and</strong> metanarrative<br />

<strong>perspective</strong> forms were related to more coherent <strong>emotion</strong>al experiences, while frequent use <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> experiencing form was related to higher affective intensity. One possible explanation for<br />

this latter result is that <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> evaluation devices is responsible for <strong>the</strong> emergence <strong>of</strong> this<br />

relationship. Among o<strong>the</strong>rs, direct quotation <strong>of</strong> past utterances, thoughts <strong>and</strong> feelings have<br />

been considered as an evaluation device (Labov, 1972, 1997). As Sakita (2002) argues, one<br />

function <strong>of</strong> direct quotation is to show an <strong>emotion</strong> ra<strong>the</strong>r than to simply state it. This argument<br />

can explain <strong>the</strong> explored relationship between <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> adopting <strong>the</strong> experiencing<br />

form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> felt <strong>emotion</strong>al intensity, since according to our definition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> narrative<br />

<strong>perspective</strong>, direct quotations are coded as an experiencing <strong>perspective</strong> form.<br />

This study continues this research line, but it is an extension <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> earlier studies in<br />

several respects. First, this study concentrates on trait features <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>, instead<br />

<strong>of</strong> state features <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>al experience. It can be assumed that <strong>the</strong>se features are related to<br />

each o<strong>the</strong>r to some extent. For example, <strong>person</strong>s who are experiencing more coherent <strong>and</strong> less<br />

intensive <strong>emotion</strong>s during verbal recounting <strong>of</strong> negative life events presumably are more<br />

efficient at regulating <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>emotion</strong>al experiences. Based on this consideration, a positive<br />

correlation is hypo<strong>the</strong>sised between <strong>the</strong> coherent <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> forms, <strong>and</strong> negative correlations are hypo<strong>the</strong>sised between <strong>the</strong> stable<br />

<strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> experiencing <strong>perspective</strong> form. Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

relatively low reliability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong> codes, we did not set a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

for this <strong>perspective</strong> form. Second, an important difference from earlier studies reflects <strong>the</strong> fact<br />

that <strong>emotion</strong>al experiences investigated here are not related to a social identity category.<br />

Finally, whereas earlier studies have focused solely on life stories regarding negative life<br />

events, here positively valued life events also will be considered.<br />

Study<br />

Subjects<br />

83 <strong>person</strong>s participated in this study. However, participants who did not recount all <strong>the</strong> five<br />

stories investigated in this study were not considered, so data from 75 <strong>person</strong>s (28 males <strong>and</strong><br />

47 females) have been analysed. Participants were recruited from two age cohorts: young<br />

adults (18-35 years old: 46 <strong>person</strong>s, M = 23.04, SD = 3.94) <strong>and</strong> adults (45-60 years old: 29<br />

<strong>person</strong>s, M = 52.17, SD = 3.92). Persons were paid 1000 HUF.<br />

Procedure<br />

Participants recounted eleven stories in this study. The first story was about an earliest<br />

memory. Then <strong>the</strong>y constructed five stories reflecting pictures selected from <strong>the</strong> Thematic


56<br />

PÓLYA, KIS, NASZÓDI, LÁSZLÓ<br />

Apperception Test. Finally, participants told five stories about <strong>the</strong> following life story events:<br />

an event during which a <strong>person</strong> experienced achievement, loss, fear, <strong>and</strong> two events with<br />

„significant o<strong>the</strong>rs‟, one is valenced as negative, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r as positive. In <strong>the</strong> second part<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> session, <strong>person</strong>ality measures were administered. Recounted narratives were taperecorded<br />

<strong>and</strong> later transcribed. Only <strong>the</strong> latter five stories were analysed. The s<strong>of</strong>tware was<br />

run in a way that a sequence <strong>of</strong> an identical <strong>perspective</strong> form was counted as one occurrence<br />

irrespective <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> a sequence. That is to say in a sequence <strong>of</strong> narrative clauses<br />

„retrospective – retrospective – experiencing – experiencing – metanarrative – metanarrative‟<br />

each form is counted once, while in <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>of</strong> „retrospective – experiencing –<br />

metanarrative – retrospective – experiencing – metanarrative‟ each form is counted twice.<br />

Personality measures<br />

Trait features <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> were measured by questionnaires. The coherence <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> was measured by administering <strong>the</strong> Clarity factor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Trait Meta-Mood<br />

Scale (Salovey, et al., 1995) which reflects on how clearly a <strong>person</strong> sees his or her feelings.<br />

The Purpose to Life Scale (Antonovsky, 1987) was also used to measure <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong><br />

coherence construct. This construct “is a global orientation that expresses <strong>the</strong> extent to which<br />

one ha s a pervasive, enduring though dynamic feeling <strong>of</strong> confidence that (1) <strong>the</strong> stimuli<br />

deriving from one‟s internal <strong>and</strong> external environments in <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> living are structured,<br />

predictable, <strong>and</strong> explicable; (2) <strong>the</strong> resources are available to one to meet <strong>the</strong> dem<strong>and</strong>s posed<br />

by <strong>the</strong>se stimuli; <strong>and</strong> (3) <strong>the</strong>se dem<strong>and</strong>s are challenges, worthy <strong>of</strong> investment <strong>and</strong> engagement<br />

(Antonovsky, 1987: 19).” Accordingly, sense <strong>of</strong> coherence construct has three factors: (1)<br />

comprehensibility, (2) manageability <strong>and</strong> (3) meaningfulness.<br />

Stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> was reflected by administering <strong>the</strong> Emotion stability<br />

factor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Big Five Questionnaire (Caprara, Barbaranelli, <strong>and</strong> Borgogni, 1993). This factor<br />

consists <strong>of</strong> two subfactors. The <strong>emotion</strong>al control subfactor measures <strong>the</strong> capacity for coping<br />

with anxiety <strong>and</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>s. The impulse control subfactor measures <strong>the</strong> capacity for managing<br />

irritability, discontent, <strong>and</strong> anger.<br />

Results<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations for <strong>the</strong> relative frequencies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three <strong>perspective</strong> forms are<br />

presented in Table 5. Consistent with <strong>the</strong> earlier study, participants used <strong>the</strong> retrospective<br />

form (M = 0.51, SD = 0.08) significantly more frequently than <strong>the</strong> experiencing form (M =<br />

0.38, SD = 0.07) (t = 8.21, df = 74, p < 0.001) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y also adopted <strong>the</strong> experiencing form<br />

more frequently than <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form (M = 0.11, SD = 0.05) (t = 25.72, df = 74, p <<br />

0.001). Potential differences in using <strong>perspective</strong> forms between groups <strong>of</strong> males <strong>and</strong> females,<br />

<strong>and</strong> young adults <strong>and</strong> adults were examined by using t-tests. The results did not reveal any<br />

gender difference. Whit regard to age differences, young adults used more frequently <strong>the</strong><br />

metanarrative form (M = 0.12, SD = 0.05) than adult participants (M = 0.09, SD = 0.05) (t =<br />

2.31, df = 73, p < 0.05). However, using <strong>the</strong> retrospective <strong>and</strong> experiencing forms <strong>the</strong>re were<br />

no age differences. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, considering <strong>the</strong> content <strong>of</strong> stories two patterns <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong><br />

forms emerged (see Table 5). In <strong>the</strong> stories regarding relation episodes <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

frequencies <strong>of</strong> retrospective <strong>and</strong> experiencing forms were almost equal, varying between 0.41<br />

<strong>and</strong> 0.44, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> a metanarrative form was considerable, 0.15. In contrast, <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r three stories were dominated by <strong>the</strong> retrospective form, varying between 0.55 <strong>and</strong> 0.59,<br />

<strong>the</strong> experiencing form was used less frequently, in a range between 0.34 <strong>and</strong> 0.37, while <strong>the</strong><br />

metanarrative form was adopted very rarely, 0.07 or 0.08.


NARRATING PERSPECTIVE AND EMOTION REGULATION 57<br />

Table 5. Descriptive data for relative frequencies <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms<br />

Content <strong>of</strong> narratives<br />

Perspective forms<br />

Retrospective Experiencing Metanarrative<br />

M SD M SD M SD<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> a bad relation 0,42 0,12 0,43 0,11 0,15 0,09<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> loss 0,55 0,15 0,37 0,15 0,08 0,06<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> fear 0,56 0,16 0,36 0,15 0,08 0,07<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> a good relation 0,44 0,14 0,41 0,13 0,15 0,11<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> achievement 0,59 0,12 0,34 0,11 0,07 0,05<br />

Total 0,51 0,08 0,38 0,07 0,11 0,05<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations for variables measuring <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> are presented in<br />

Table 6 <strong>and</strong> 7. There is only one marginal difference between male <strong>and</strong> female participants (see<br />

Table 6). Male participants have higher Emotional control subfactor (M = 38.07, SD = 12.31)<br />

than female participants (M = 33.34, SD = 9.79) (t = 1.84, df = 73, p < 0.10).<br />

Table 6. Descriptive data for measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong><br />

Measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong><br />

All<br />

Males Females t-tests<br />

<strong>regulation</strong><br />

N = 75 N = 28 N = 47<br />

M SD M SD M SD<br />

TMMS: Clarity factor 43,82 7,80 44,79 9,55 43,24 6,57 n. s.<br />

PLS: Sense <strong>of</strong> coherence 46,19 9,67 47,57 12,08 45,36 7,93 n. s.<br />

Comprehensibility factor 16,63 4,46 17,57 5,02 16,06 4,03 n. s.<br />

Manageability factor 13,69 3,19 14,07 3,89 13,47 2,72 n. s.<br />

Meaningfulness factor 15,87 3,09 15,93 3,91 15,83 2,53 n. s.<br />

BFQ: Emotional stability 68,97 18,47 73,43 20,67 66,32 16,6 n. s.<br />

factor<br />

Emotional control<br />

subfactor<br />

9<br />

35,11 10,96 38,07 12,31 33,34 9,79 T(73) =<br />

1,84<br />

p < 0,10<br />

Impulse control subfactor 34,19 8,14 35,36 9,53 33,49 7,20 n. s.<br />

TMMS: Trait Meta-Mood Scale; PLS: Purpose to Life Scale; BFQ: Big Five Questionnaire<br />

There is also one marginal difference between age groups (see Table 7). Adult participants<br />

have higher (M = 17.72, SD = 4.74) Comprehensibility factor than young adults (M = 15.93,<br />

SD = 4.18) (t = 1.72, df = 73, p < 0.10).<br />

Since gender <strong>and</strong> age differences were moderate, in fur<strong>the</strong>r analysis participants were<br />

pooled.<br />

To explore <strong>the</strong> relationships between <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>emotion</strong><br />

<strong>regulation</strong> measures several correlation analyses were run (see Table 8).


58<br />

PÓLYA, KIS, NASZÓDI, LÁSZLÓ<br />

Table 7<br />

Descriptive data for measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong><br />

Measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong> Young adults Adults t-tests<br />

N = 46<br />

N = 29<br />

M SD M SD<br />

TMMS: Clarity factor 43,04 6,43 45,03 9,55 n. s.<br />

PLS: Sense <strong>of</strong> coherence 45,36 8,18 47,55 11,68 n. s.<br />

Comprehensibility factor 15,93 4,18 17,72 4,74 T(73) =<br />

1,72<br />

p < 0,10<br />

Manageability factor 13,63 2,84 13,79 3,74 n. s.<br />

Meaningfulness factor 15,76 2,43 16,03 3,96 n. s.<br />

BFQ: Emotional stability factor 70,48 14,47 66,59 23,56 n. s.<br />

Emotional control subfactor 36,07 9,02 33,59 13,52 n. s.<br />

Impulse control subfactor 34,41 7,01 33,83 9,78 n. s.<br />

TMMS: Trait Meta-Mood Scale; PLS: Purpose to Life Scale; BFQ: Big Five Questionnaire<br />

With regard to retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> form, <strong>the</strong>re were several positive correlations with<br />

<strong>the</strong> coherence <strong>of</strong> an <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>. In <strong>the</strong> story <strong>of</strong> a bad relation relative frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

retrospective form correlated positively with <strong>the</strong> Clarity factor (r = 0.28, p < 0.01), <strong>the</strong> Sense<br />

<strong>of</strong> coherence construct (r = 0.27, p < 0.05), <strong>and</strong> its three factors, such as Comprehensibility (r<br />

= 0.17, p < 0.10), Manageability (r = 0.26, p < 0.05) <strong>and</strong> Meaningfulness (r = 0.32, p < 0.01).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> story <strong>of</strong> loss, relative frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> retrospective form also correlated positively with<br />

<strong>the</strong> Clarity factor (r = 0.22, p < 0.05). However, regarding <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> coherence construct,<br />

only with <strong>the</strong> Meaningfulness factor was <strong>the</strong>re a positive correlation (r = 0.15, p < 0.10).<br />

These results confirm <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sised positive correlation between retrospective <strong>perspective</strong><br />

form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coherent <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>. However, in a positively valued achievement story<br />

a negative correlation appeared between <strong>the</strong> retrospective form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Meaningfulness factor<br />

(r = -0.16, p < 0.10).<br />

Considering <strong>the</strong> experiencing form, negative correlations were found between <strong>the</strong><br />

occurrence <strong>of</strong> an experiencing <strong>perspective</strong> form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>al experience in<br />

two negative stories. In <strong>the</strong> story <strong>of</strong> a bad relation <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> using <strong>the</strong> experiencing<br />

form correlated negatively with <strong>the</strong> Impulse control subfactor (r = -0.17, p < 0.10). While in<br />

<strong>the</strong> story <strong>of</strong> loss, <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> taking <strong>the</strong> experiencing form correlated negatively with <strong>the</strong><br />

Emotional stability factor (r = -0.15, p < 0.10) <strong>and</strong> its Emotional control subfactor (r = -0.16,<br />

p < 0.10).<br />

Finally, regarding <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form, negative correlations appeared in <strong>the</strong> story<br />

<strong>of</strong> a bad relation between using <strong>the</strong> metanarrative form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coherence <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong><br />

<strong>regulation</strong>. The relative frequency correlates negatively with <strong>the</strong> Clarity factor (r = -0.25, p <<br />

0.05), <strong>the</strong> Sense <strong>of</strong> coherence construct (r = -0.26, p < 0.05), <strong>and</strong> its three factors, such as<br />

Comprehensibility (r = -0.24, p < 0.05), Manageability (r = -0.25, p < 0.05) <strong>and</strong><br />

Meaningfulness (r = -0.21, p < 0.01). Although negative correlations appeared solely in <strong>the</strong><br />

story <strong>of</strong> a bad relation, <strong>the</strong>se results are consistent with <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sised negative correlation<br />

between <strong>the</strong> metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong> form <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coherent <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>.


Content <strong>of</strong><br />

narratives<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> a<br />

bad relation<br />

Table 8<br />

Correlations between <strong>the</strong> relative frequencies <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> forms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong><br />

Perspective<br />

form<br />

TMMS:<br />

Clarity<br />

factor<br />

PLS: Sense<br />

<strong>of</strong><br />

coherence<br />

Comprehens<br />

ibility factor<br />

Manageability<br />

factor<br />

Meaningfulness<br />

factor<br />

BFQ: Emotional<br />

stability factor<br />

Emotional<br />

control<br />

subfactor<br />

Impulse<br />

control<br />

subfactor<br />

Retrospective .28** .27* .17+ .26* .32** .27* .30** .15<br />

Experiencing -.09 -.08 .01 -.07 -.18+ .06 -.01 -.17+<br />

Metanarrative -.25* -.26* -.24* -.25* -.21* -.42** -.38** -.40**<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> loss Retrospective .22* .14 .10 .12 .15+ .15 .16+ .15<br />

Experiencing -.20* -.13 -.10 -.11 -.15 -.15+ -.16+ -.15<br />

Metanarrative -.09 -.03 -.02 -.05 -.03 -.01 -.02 -.02<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> fear Retrospective .07 .04 .05 .04 .02 .00 .03 -.09<br />

Experiencing -.13 -.08 -.07 -.06 -.09 -.01 -.07 .09<br />

Metanarrative .10 .07 .02 .03 .15 .03 .06 .01<br />

Story <strong>of</strong> a<br />

good relation<br />

Story <strong>of</strong><br />

achievement<br />

Retrospective -.02 -.07 -.07 -.07 -.03 .09 .09 .05<br />

Experiencing -.04 .06 .06 .05 .06 .02 .01 .01<br />

Metanarrative .07 .01 .02 .02 -.03 -.14 -.13 -.08<br />

Retrospective -.12 -.13 -.08 -.11 -.16+ -.07 .01 -.08<br />

Experiencing -.10 .11 -.08 .10 .12 -.01 -.04 .04<br />

Metanarrative .08 .07 .01 .06 .12 .09 .08 .11<br />

** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05, + p < 0.10


Discussion<br />

This article described a algorithm developed for <strong>the</strong> automatic coding <strong>of</strong> spatio-temporal<br />

component <strong>of</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> in self-narratives. Subsequently, a study was presented which<br />

explored <strong>the</strong> relationship between <strong>the</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>of</strong> recounted significant life events<br />

<strong>and</strong> trait features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>narrating</strong> <strong>person</strong>‟s <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>.<br />

The results have confirmed our hypo<strong>the</strong>ses. The use <strong>of</strong> a retrospective <strong>perspective</strong><br />

form indicates coherent <strong>regulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>s, while <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> an experiencing form reflects<br />

<strong>the</strong> instability <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>. These results are consistent with <strong>the</strong> relationships identified<br />

in two earlier studies (Pólya, 2007; Pólya, László, <strong>and</strong> Forgas, 2005), <strong>and</strong> also with<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r studies (Pennebaker, Mayne, <strong>and</strong> Francis, 1997). Pennebaker <strong>and</strong> his co-workers have<br />

found that <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> distress is predicted by <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> using past tense verbs: <strong>person</strong>s<br />

who used frequently past tense verbs experienced less distress. Since past verb tense is<br />

indicative <strong>of</strong> a retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> form, this result is in line with <strong>the</strong> explored relationship<br />

between using retrospective <strong>perspective</strong> form <strong>and</strong> coherent <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>.<br />

Surprisingly, as opposed to earlier stories, in this study <strong>the</strong> metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong> behaved<br />

similarly to <strong>the</strong> experiencing <strong>perspective</strong>: Strong negative correlations were found between<br />

<strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coherence <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>. This result<br />

may be due to <strong>the</strong> relatively low reliability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> automatic coding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> linguistic patterns <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> metanarrative <strong>perspective</strong>. Never<strong>the</strong>less, it can be concluded from this study that <strong>the</strong><br />

narrative <strong>perspective</strong> <strong>of</strong> a self-narrative is informative about <strong>the</strong> coherence <strong>and</strong> stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>narrating</strong> <strong>person</strong>‟s <strong>emotion</strong> <strong>regulation</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> algorithm validly measures<br />

<strong>emotion</strong>al coherence <strong>and</strong> stability through <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> retrospective <strong>and</strong> experiencing <strong>perspective</strong><br />

forms.<br />

However, <strong>the</strong>re are at least two important constraints which should be taken into consideration.<br />

First, <strong>the</strong> explored relationships seem to be dependent on <strong>the</strong> valence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> stories,<br />

since <strong>the</strong> confirming results have emerged only in <strong>the</strong> self-narratives recounting negatively<br />

valenced life events, <strong>and</strong> were absent in <strong>the</strong> self-narratives about positively valenced life<br />

events. This valence dependence can also be observed in relation to o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>al<br />

processes. The organization <strong>and</strong> dynamics <strong>of</strong> social sharing are markedly different for positively<br />

versus negatively valued <strong>emotion</strong>al events (Rimé, <strong>and</strong> Zech, 2001; Zech <strong>and</strong> Rimé,<br />

2005). Second, <strong>the</strong> explored relationships also seem to be content dependent, since <strong>the</strong><br />

strength <strong>of</strong> relationships were different in <strong>the</strong> three negatively valenced stories (bad relation,<br />

loss <strong>and</strong> fear). Although <strong>the</strong>se constraints raise questions for fur<strong>the</strong>r studies <strong>the</strong> new method<br />

<strong>of</strong> analysis presented here, <strong>the</strong> automatic coding <strong>of</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong> in self-narratives<br />

may contribute effectively to this work.<br />

References<br />

Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unravelling <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong> health. How people manage stress <strong>and</strong><br />

stay well. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Bal, M. (1998). Narratology. Introduction to <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> narrative. 2nd edition, Toronto:<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto Press.<br />

Banfield, A. (1982). Unspeakable sentences: Narration <strong>and</strong> representation in <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong><br />

fiction. Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul.<br />

Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C., & Borgogni (1993). The Big 5 questionnaire. A new<br />

questionnaire to assess <strong>the</strong> 5 factor model. Personality <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences,<br />

15(3), 281-288.<br />

Cohn, D. (1978). Transparent minds. Princeton: Princeton University Press.


NARRATING PERSPECTIVE AND EMOTION REGULATION 61<br />

Kiefer, F. (1990). Modalitás. [Modality.] Budapest: MTA Nyelvtudományi Intézete.<br />

Labov, W. (1972). The transformation <strong>of</strong> experience in narrative syntax. In Language in <strong>the</strong><br />

inner city. (pp. 354-396). Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

Labov, W. (1997). Some fur<strong>the</strong>r steps in narrative analysis, The Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Narrative</strong> <strong>and</strong> Life<br />

History, 7(1-4), 395-415.<br />

Peer, W. v., & Chatman, S. (Eds.), (2001). New <strong>perspective</strong>s on narrative <strong>perspective</strong>.<br />

Albany: State University <strong>of</strong> New York Press.<br />

Pennebaker, J.W., Mayne, T.J., & Francis, M.E. (1997). Linguistic predictors <strong>of</strong> adaptive<br />

bereavement, Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 72(4), 863-871.<br />

Pólya, T. (2007). Identitás az elbeszélésben. Szociális identitás és narratív perspektíva<br />

[Identity in a self-narrative. Social identity <strong>and</strong> narrative <strong>perspective</strong>.] Budapest: Új<br />

M<strong>and</strong>átum Kiadó.<br />

Pólya, T., László, J., & Forgas, J.P. (2005). Making sense <strong>of</strong> life stories: The role <strong>of</strong> narrative<br />

<strong>perspective</strong> in communicating hidden information about social identity, European<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Social Psychology, 35(6), 785-796.<br />

Prince, G. (1987). Dictionary <strong>of</strong> narratology. Lincoln: University <strong>of</strong> Nebraska Press.<br />

Prószéky, G., & Tihanyi, L. (1993). Humor: High-speed unification morphology <strong>and</strong> its<br />

applications for agglutinative languages. La tribune des industries de la langue. No.<br />

10. 28-29, OFIL, Paris.<br />

Rimé, B., & Zech, E. (2001). The social sharing <strong>of</strong> <strong>emotion</strong>: Inter<strong>person</strong>al <strong>and</strong> collective<br />

dimensions. Boletin di Psicologia, 70, 97-108.<br />

Sakita, T.I. (2002). Reporting discourse, tense, <strong>and</strong> cognition. Amsterdam: Elsevier.<br />

Salovey, P., Mayer, J.D., Goldman, S.L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T.P. (1995). Emotional<br />

attention, clarity, <strong>and</strong> repair: Exploring <strong>emotion</strong>al intelligence using <strong>the</strong> Trait Meta-<br />

Mood Scale. In J.W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, <strong>and</strong> health. (pp. 125-<br />

154). Washington: American Psychological Association.<br />

Wiebe, J. (1991). Tracking point <strong>of</strong> view in narrative. Computational Linguistics, 20(2), 233-<br />

287.<br />

Wilkins, D.P. (1995). Exp<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>the</strong> traditional category <strong>of</strong> deictic elements: Interjections as<br />

deictics. In J.F. Duchan, G.A. Bruder, & L.E. Hewitt (Eds.), Deixis in narrative. A<br />

cognitive science <strong>perspective</strong>. (pp. 359-386). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.<br />

Uspensky, B.A. (1974). The poetics <strong>of</strong> composition: Structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artistic text <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

typology <strong>of</strong> compositional forms. Berkeley: University <strong>of</strong> California Press.<br />

Wilson, T., Wiebe,J., & H<strong>of</strong>fmann, P. (2005). Recognizing contextual polarity in phrase level<br />

sentiment analysis. In Proceedings <strong>of</strong> HLT/EMNLP-05, pages 347-354.<br />

Yi, J., Nasukawa, T., Bunescu, R., & Niblack, W. (2003). Sentiment analyzer: Extracting<br />

sentiments about a given topic using natural language processing techniques. In<br />

Proceedings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Third IEEE International Conference on Data Mining, pages 427-<br />

434.<br />

Zech, E. & Rimé, B. (2005). Is it talking about an <strong>emotion</strong>al experience helpful Effects on<br />

<strong>emotion</strong>al recovery <strong>and</strong> perceived benefits. Clinical Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psycho<strong>the</strong>rapy,<br />

12, 270-287.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!