29.01.2015 Views

Towards a psychological basis for a theory of anaphora - Centre for ...

Towards a psychological basis for a theory of anaphora - Centre for ...

Towards a psychological basis for a theory of anaphora - Centre for ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

suggests that five year-olds also have problems with<br />

pronoun anaphors in the comprehension <strong>of</strong> utterances.<br />

The experiment used stimuli <strong>of</strong> the following sort:<br />

1) Jan was trying to catch some butterflies<br />

2al The butterflies had beautiful wints (wings)<br />

and...<br />

2b) They had beautiful wints (wings) and.. .<br />

The children heard short texts, containing these<br />

two-sentence pairs - i.e. (1) followtd by either (2a)<br />

or (2b) - and their task was to press a response key<br />

whenever they heard a "mispronounced" word. In the<br />

example here, the word "wings" is mispronounced as<br />

"wints" (the actual experiment was run in Dutch, and the<br />

above example is a loose translation <strong>of</strong> one <strong>of</strong> the<br />

stimulus sets). Earlier experiments by Cole and Jakimik<br />

(1980) had used this task successfully with first-grade<br />

children.<br />

The reason <strong>for</strong> using the mispronunciation detection<br />

task here was that reaction-times in this task are<br />

facilitated when the original word - the source <strong>for</strong> the<br />

mispronounced word - is more contextually predictable.<br />

To determine that a word has been mispronounced, one<br />

needs to establish what the source word was, and this<br />

process is apparently facilitated when the contextual<br />

constraints on the source word are stronger. In the<br />

present example, the word "wings" (the source <strong>for</strong><br />

"wints") is only predictable if one knows that it is<br />

"butterflies" that are the subject <strong>of</strong> the sentence. But<br />

in sentences like (2b) one can only know this if one has<br />

established that the antecedent <strong>of</strong> "They" is indeed<br />

"butterflies". Thus, if there are any developmental<br />

differences in childrens' ability to resolve pronoun<br />

anaphors on-line, then this should produce a slower<br />

response in (2b) relative to (2a), where "butterflies"<br />

are explicitly mentioned in the sentence.<br />

Table 2<br />

Mean Mispronunciation Detection Latencies (msec)<br />

Continuation Type<br />

Age-qroup Nounphrase Pronoun Diff.<br />

5 Years 709 769 60'<br />

7 Years 560 561 1<br />

10 Years 495 500<br />

........................................................<br />

5<br />

The results are given in Table 2, <strong>for</strong> the three<br />

age-groups we tested. The seven and the ten year-old<br />

groups show no differences between the pronoun and the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!