11.02.2015 Views

good-practice-in-science-teaching-what-research-has-to-say

good-practice-in-science-teaching-what-research-has-to-say

good-practice-in-science-teaching-what-research-has-to-say

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

THINKING ABOUT LEARNING 73<br />

encounter <strong>in</strong> many sources, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g teachers’ lessons, books, television and<br />

the Internet.<br />

This view is sometimes <strong>in</strong>terpreted as mean<strong>in</strong>g that children need <strong>to</strong><br />

‘discover’ concepts for themselves <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> construct their own understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(see Mayer, 2004). However, besides be<strong>in</strong>g a practical impossibility<br />

(if we did not rely on the work of others who came before us, we would not<br />

have enough time <strong>to</strong> ‘discover’ the world’s aspects from scratch), <strong>research</strong><br />

<strong>has</strong> demonstrated that discovery is not as effective as guided <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong><br />

produc<strong>in</strong>g new learn<strong>in</strong>g (Fay and Mayer, 1994; Inhelder et al., 1974; Klahr<br />

and Nigam, 2004; Lee and Thompson, 1997). In fact, Pea and Kurland (1984)<br />

found that students <strong>in</strong> a pure discovery learn<strong>in</strong>g situation, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g hands-on<br />

experience, were no better at plann<strong>in</strong>g a program <strong>in</strong> LOGO, a computer environment,<br />

than were students receiv<strong>in</strong>g no experience at all with the computer<br />

environment. Such <strong>research</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicates that perhaps a better <strong>in</strong>terpretation of<br />

constructivism <strong>in</strong>volves acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g the learner’s agency <strong>in</strong> any learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

context, rather than suggest<strong>in</strong>g all responsibility lies with the learner. Instead,<br />

it may be useful for teachers <strong>to</strong> consider how learners conceive of scientific<br />

phenomena prior <strong>to</strong> engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a lesson on a given <strong>to</strong>pic.<br />

Children often develop conceptions about scientific phenomena, and require<br />

guided <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>to</strong> hone these ideas. These conceptions have been variously<br />

called by different names: misconceptions, alternative conceptions, folk<br />

<strong>science</strong>, <strong>in</strong>tuitive ideas, alternative frameworks and everyday <strong>science</strong>. Driver<br />

et al. (1994) present a wide range of studies that explore children’s ideas <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>science</strong>. For example, the view that we are able <strong>to</strong> see objects because light<br />

travels from our eyes <strong>to</strong> the object or that plants’ mass comes from nutrients<br />

<strong>in</strong> the soil are two <strong>in</strong>stances of children’s naïve scientific ideas that can be<br />

reorganized through <strong>in</strong>struction. On the other hand, learners’ alternative or<br />

everyday conceptions can be powerful and difficult <strong>to</strong> override (Novak, 2002).<br />

As teachers, you must work hard <strong>to</strong> help learners <strong>to</strong> overcome the predisposition<br />

<strong>to</strong> rely on previous ideas, at least when th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ‘scientifically’.<br />

Leach and Scott (2000) proposed the idea of a ‘learn<strong>in</strong>g demand’ <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong><br />

help teachers f<strong>in</strong>d ways <strong>to</strong> help learners make sense of scientific material. These<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g demands offer ‘a description of the differences between everyday and<br />

scientific ways of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about the world, and the resultant challenges that<br />

learners will face <strong>in</strong> com<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalize and understand scientific accounts<br />

of phenomena’ (Leach and Scott, 2000, p. 45). As they describe it, learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

demands can help teachers <strong>to</strong> identify where learners are likely <strong>to</strong> experience<br />

difficulties <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g the scientific as opposed <strong>to</strong> the everyday way of<br />

th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Sometimes constructivism can be taken <strong>to</strong> mean that because <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />

construct their own understand<strong>in</strong>gs about the world, there can be no such<br />

th<strong>in</strong>g as a right answer, or absolute fact – because each of us creates our<br />

own explanations. This <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>has</strong> been criticized on the grounds that<br />

knowledge, particularly scientific knowledge, is based upon repeated empirical<br />

observations of phenomena lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> objective facts (Osborne, 1996). Of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!