11.03.2015 Views

How the Mind Works - michaeljgoodnight.com

How the Mind Works - michaeljgoodnight.com

How the Mind Works - michaeljgoodnight.com

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Revenge of <strong>the</strong> Nerds 191<br />

WHY US?<br />

Why did some miocene ape first enter <strong>the</strong> cognitive niche? Why not a<br />

groundhog, or a catfish, or a tapeworm? It only happened once, so no<br />

one knows. But I would guess that our ancestors had four traits that<br />

made it especially easy and worth <strong>the</strong>ir while to evolve better powers of<br />

causal reasoning.<br />

First, primates are visual animals. In monkeys such as <strong>the</strong> rhesus<br />

macaque, half <strong>the</strong> brain is dedicated to sight. Stereoscopic vision, <strong>the</strong><br />

use of differences in <strong>the</strong> vantage points of <strong>the</strong> two eyes to give a sense of<br />

depth, developed early in <strong>the</strong> primate lineage, allowing early nocturnal<br />

primates to move among treacherous fine branches and to grab insects<br />

with <strong>the</strong>ir hands. Color vision ac<strong>com</strong>panied <strong>the</strong> switch of <strong>the</strong> ancestors<br />

of monkeys and apes to <strong>the</strong> day shift and <strong>the</strong>ir new taste for fruits, which<br />

advertise <strong>the</strong>ir ripeness with gaudy hues.<br />

Why would <strong>the</strong> vision thing make such a difference? Depth perception<br />

defines a three-dimensional space filled with movable solid objects.<br />

Color makes objects pop out from <strong>the</strong>ir backgrounds, and gives us a sensation<br />

that corresponds to <strong>the</strong> stuff an object is made of, distinct from<br />

our perception of <strong>the</strong> shape of <strong>the</strong> stuff. Toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y have pushed <strong>the</strong><br />

primate brain into splitting <strong>the</strong> flow of visual information into two<br />

streams: a "what" system, for objects and <strong>the</strong>ir shapes and <strong>com</strong>positions,<br />

and a "where" system, for <strong>the</strong>ir locations and motions. It can't be a coincidence<br />

that <strong>the</strong> human mind grasps <strong>the</strong> world—even <strong>the</strong> most abstract,<br />

e<strong>the</strong>real concepts—as a space filled with movable things and stuff (see<br />

Chapters 4 and 5). We say that John went from being sick to being well,<br />

even if he didn't move an inch; he could have been in bed <strong>the</strong> whole<br />

time. Mary can give him many pieces of advice, even if <strong>the</strong>y merely talked<br />

on <strong>the</strong> phone and nothing changed hands. Even scientists, when <strong>the</strong>y try<br />

to grasp abstract ma<strong>the</strong>matical relationships, plot <strong>the</strong>m in graphs that<br />

show <strong>the</strong>m as two- and three-dimensional shapes. Our capacity for<br />

abstract thought has co-opted <strong>the</strong> coordinate system and inventory of<br />

objects made available by a well-developed visual system.<br />

It is harder to see how a standard mammal could have moved in that<br />

direction. Most mammals hug <strong>the</strong> ground sniffing <strong>the</strong> rich chemical<br />

tracks and trails left behind by o<strong>the</strong>r living things. Anyone who has

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!