10.07.2015 Views

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir speech striking listeners as slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>ous(Felsenfeld et al. 1992).Findings in behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> molecular genetics alsosupport links between language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech, whilehinting at previously unexamined c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s ofspeech to o<strong>the</strong>r linguistic domains. In studies of m<strong>on</strong>ozygotic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dizygotic twins, for example, <strong>the</strong> highest ratesof c<strong>on</strong>cordance have come from twins in whom <strong>the</strong> disorderwas primarily expressive; some had disorders that werepurely of <strong>the</strong> articulatory type (Bishop et al. 1995; Lewis1990; Lewis & Thomps<strong>on</strong> 1992). In typically developing7- to 13-year-old children, Bishop (2001) also found thatMZ twins were more c<strong>on</strong>cordant than DZ twins <strong>on</strong> rateof speeded articulati<strong>on</strong> of polysyllabic words. These findingsseem to fit with <strong>the</strong> possibility that selecti<strong>on</strong> acted<strong>on</strong> performance, thus <strong>on</strong> factors relating to precisi<strong>on</strong>,speed, complexity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluency of articulati<strong>on</strong>. In doingso, it may have reinforced those behaviors, indirectlyenhancing related acquisiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>s thathad occurred earlier in <strong>development</strong>.The role played by producti<strong>on</strong> factors has also beenhighlighted by studies of a particular family in Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>.In <strong>the</strong> early 1990s, researchers discovered <strong>the</strong> KE family,which had serious communicati<strong>on</strong> disorders at each of itsthree living generati<strong>on</strong>s. Although <strong>the</strong>se problemsincluded a severe oral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal dyspraxia, accompaniedby deficits at o<strong>the</strong>r levels of language (Fletcher 1990;Hurst et al. 1990; Vargha-Khadem 1990), a grammaticallyfocused investigati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> family found precisely what itwas looking for: grammatical problems (Gopnik 1990;Gopnik & Crago 1991). Nearly a decade later, geneticistsfound a defective gene in <strong>the</strong> family, FOXP2, which is situated<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g arm of chromosome 7 (Lai et al. 2000;2001). But, in parallel with <strong>the</strong> genetics work, o<strong>the</strong>rteams of clinical investigators c<strong>on</strong>firmed <strong>the</strong> dyspraxicdisorder, which involved both speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>speechmovements of <strong>the</strong> articulators (Vargha-Khadem et al.1995; 1998). In functi<strong>on</strong>al imaging studies, affectedfamily members revealed significant underactivati<strong>on</strong> ofBroca’s area in both hemispheres, c<strong>on</strong>sistent with adeficit of speech producti<strong>on</strong> (Liégeous et al. 2003).What is relevant here is that <strong>the</strong> primary problem of <strong>the</strong>KE family was rec<strong>on</strong>ceptualized as an oral-motor difficultythat was accompanied by grammatical difficulties. Several<strong>the</strong>orists toyed with <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong> KE family’s oralmotordifficulties caused <strong>the</strong>ir grammatical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>difficulties. “One possibility,” wrote Watkinset al. (2002), “is that <strong>the</strong> deviant articulati<strong>on</strong> results inpoor ph<strong>on</strong>ology, rendering morphological producti<strong>on</strong>difficult” (p. 461). “It might be <strong>the</strong> case,” Marcus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Fisher (2003) speculated, “that a deficit restricted to <strong>the</strong>motor system is fully resp<strong>on</strong>sible for <strong>the</strong> wide-rangingprofile of impairment” (p. 261). There could have been a“flow-<strong>on</strong> effect from articulati<strong>on</strong> to syntax to comprehensi<strong>on</strong>,”wrote Corballis (2004, p. 548).How did our ancestors get from articulated ph<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> tosyntax? According to Bickert<strong>on</strong> (2000):The ability to make <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> distinguish a greater range of speechsounds would make possible a wider variety of sound combinati<strong>on</strong>s,which, given a larger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more efficient memory forwords, would give rise to a steadily increasing vocabulary.Undoubtedly, <strong>the</strong>se factors <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes would have combinedto yield a much richer means of communicati<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g hominins. (p. 157)This would not automatically produce syntax, asBickert<strong>on</strong> noted, but it may have led to o<strong>the</strong>r changesthat favored ph<strong>on</strong>ology, beginning with <strong>the</strong> ritualizati<strong>on</strong>of vocal patterns (Locke 2004a; in press b; Oller 2004;Richman 2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ultimately <strong>the</strong>ir segmentati<strong>on</strong> intodiscrete linguistic units (Studdert-Kennedy 1998; 2005;Studdert-Kennedy & Goldstein 2003).12. What bel<strong>on</strong>gs in <strong>the</strong> language faculty?Evidence examined earlier revealed c<strong>on</strong>tinuity between<strong>the</strong> lexical delays of infancy – even where <strong>the</strong>y appearedto resolve – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> pragmatic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative problemsof adolescence. If we attempt to account for this c<strong>on</strong>tinuity,two possibilities emerge immediately. Accordingto <strong>the</strong> first, pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance are social applicati<strong>on</strong>sof linguistic knowledge, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus depend <strong>on</strong> thatknowledge. This could explain <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>al associati<strong>on</strong>between scores <strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard tests of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>experimental measures of pragmatics.A more satisfying possibility is that delayed lexical <strong>development</strong>points to a weakened faculty of language, sinceeven <strong>the</strong> earliest use of words requires <strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of mechanismsthat bel<strong>on</strong>g to this faculty. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with aparadox about language <strong>development</strong>. Infants rarely vocalizeat normal levels of frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complexity during<strong>the</strong> babbling stage; <strong>the</strong>y imitate aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’sspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> produce isolated words – behaviors thatostensibly require no grammatical ability at all – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>n stumble as <strong>the</strong>y enter <strong>the</strong> domains of morphology<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax (Locke 1998a). Whatever problems arise at<strong>the</strong> grammatical level of language are typically forecastby deficiencies in early lexical <strong>development</strong> (Bates &Goodman 1997), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly even <strong>the</strong> precursors toword learning (Oller et al. 1999). These facts suggestthat <strong>the</strong> earlier <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> later behaviors, as different as <strong>the</strong>yare, bel<strong>on</strong>g to a system of linked neural resources, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>that weakness in <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> of an early comp<strong>on</strong>entindexes weakness in o<strong>the</strong>rs, including <strong>the</strong> performativeareas that happen to develop late. According to this explanati<strong>on</strong>,early lexical delay predicts problems of usage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>interpretati<strong>on</strong> in adolescence even if <strong>the</strong> delay, by testsof structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent, seems to resolve l<strong>on</strong>g beforethat stage. The sec<strong>on</strong>d account is <strong>the</strong>refore able toh<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>le problems of interpretati<strong>on</strong> as well as usage.At first glance, it may seem adventurous to assign lexical<strong>development</strong> to <strong>the</strong> same faculty as o<strong>the</strong>r comp<strong>on</strong>ents oflanguage. For lexical <strong>development</strong> rests <strong>on</strong> a number ofperceptual, attenti<strong>on</strong>al, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r cognitive systems thatare not, in <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>the</strong>mselves, linguistic. But a wider <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> faculty of language has recently becomeavailable. In an attempted rec<strong>on</strong>ciliati<strong>on</strong> of evoluti<strong>on</strong>arybiology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistics, Hauser, Chomsky, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Fitchhave proposed a broad linguistic faculty that extendsbey<strong>on</strong>d a narrow computati<strong>on</strong>al core to include <strong>the</strong> “biologicalcapacity of humans that allows us (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not, forexample, chimpanzees) to readily master any humanlanguage without explicit instructi<strong>on</strong>” (Hauser et al.2002, p. 1571; also see Fitch et al. 2005).The broad model offered by Hauser <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> colleaguesoffers hope to those who might like to see linguistic knowledge<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure accommodated under <strong>the</strong> same<strong>the</strong>oretical roof as pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal performance.276 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!