12.07.2015 Views

Working Memory Impairments in Children with ... - University of York

Working Memory Impairments in Children with ... - University of York

Working Memory Impairments in Children with ... - University of York

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Child Psychology 74, 240–260 (1999)Article ID jecp.1999.2516, available onl<strong>in</strong>e at http://www.idealibrary.com on<strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>Impairments</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Children</strong> <strong>with</strong>Specific Arithmetic Learn<strong>in</strong>g DifficultiesJanet F. McLean and Graham J. HitchLancaster <strong>University</strong>, Lancaster, United K<strong>in</strong>gdom<strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory impairments <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> difficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic have previouslybeen <strong>in</strong>vestigated us<strong>in</strong>g questionable selection techniques and control groups,lead<strong>in</strong>g to problems conclud<strong>in</strong>g where deficits may occur. The present study attempted toovercome these criticisms by assess<strong>in</strong>g 9-year-old children <strong>with</strong> difficulties specific toarithmetic, as <strong>in</strong>dicated by normal read<strong>in</strong>g, and compar<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>with</strong> both age-matchedand ability-matched controls. A battery <strong>of</strong> 10 tasks was used to assess different aspects <strong>of</strong>work<strong>in</strong>g memory, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g subtypes <strong>of</strong> executive function. Relative to age-matchedcontrols, children <strong>with</strong> poor arithmetic had normal phonological work<strong>in</strong>g memory butwere impaired on spatial work<strong>in</strong>g memory and some aspects <strong>of</strong> executive process<strong>in</strong>g.Compared to ability-matched controls, they were impaired only on one task designed toassess executive processes for hold<strong>in</strong>g and manipulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> long-term memory.These deficits <strong>in</strong> executive and spatial aspects <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory seem likely to beimportant factors <strong>in</strong> poor arithmetical atta<strong>in</strong>ment. © 1999 Academic PressKey Words: work<strong>in</strong>g memory; executive processes; arithmetic; children; learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties.There are many reasons children may fail to learn arithmetic. Examples<strong>in</strong>clude anxiety about mathematics, lack <strong>of</strong> experience and poor motivation(Ashcraft & Faust, 1994; Lev<strong>in</strong>e, 1987), read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties (Muth, 1984; Richman,1983), and neuropsychological damage (McCloskey, Harley, & Sokol,1991). A grow<strong>in</strong>g body <strong>of</strong> evidence suggests that arithmetical learn<strong>in</strong>g difficultiescan be associated <strong>with</strong> cognitive deficits (e.g., Bull & Johnston, 1997; Geary& Brown, 1991; Geary, Brown, & Samanayake, 1991; Hitch & McAuley, 1991;Rourke & F<strong>in</strong>dlayson, 1978; Rourke & Strang, 1978; Siegel & Ryan, 1989;Temple, 1991). The present study builds on this work by focus<strong>in</strong>g on cognitivedeficits <strong>in</strong> a subgroup <strong>of</strong> children <strong>with</strong><strong>in</strong> the general population who have specificdifficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic but normal read<strong>in</strong>g.This research was conducted <strong>in</strong> partial fulfillment <strong>of</strong> a Ph.D. by the first author. The cooperation<strong>of</strong> schools <strong>in</strong> the Lancaster area and the f<strong>in</strong>ancial support <strong>of</strong> the Economic and Social ResearchCouncil are gratefully acknowledged.Address correspondence and repr<strong>in</strong>t requests to Janet McLean, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Florent<strong>in</strong>eHouse, <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8BQ, United K<strong>in</strong>gdom.0022-0965/99 $30.00Copyright © 1999 by Academic PressAll rights <strong>of</strong> reproduction <strong>in</strong> any form reserved.240


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES241Describ<strong>in</strong>g the cognitive deficits <strong>of</strong> this subgroup can be problematic. Studiesbased on cl<strong>in</strong>ical samples have suggested three different types <strong>of</strong> cognitive deficit<strong>in</strong> arithmetic (see Badian, 1983, and Geary, 1993, for reviews). These arecharacterized as visuospatial deficits (Hartje, 1987; Rourke, 1993; Rourke &F<strong>in</strong>dlayson, 1978; Rourke & Strang, 1978), difficulties <strong>with</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and writ<strong>in</strong>g<strong>of</strong> numbers (Kosc, 1974; Temple, 1989), and problems <strong>of</strong> retriev<strong>in</strong>g arithmeticfacts from long-term memory (Badian, 1983; Benson & Weir, 1972; Jackson &Warr<strong>in</strong>gton, 1986). White, M<strong>of</strong>fitt, and Silva (1992) found that arithmetic disabledchildren performed poorly not only on the visuospatial tasks used byRourke and colleagues, but also on the Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails tasks (Reitan, 1958), theGrooved Pegboard (Klove, 1963), and the WISC-R Cod<strong>in</strong>g (Wechsler, 1974),which they <strong>in</strong>terpreted as reflect<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>with</strong> visual–motor <strong>in</strong>tegration.Taken together, cl<strong>in</strong>ical studies suggest that difficulties <strong>with</strong> arithmetic areassociated <strong>with</strong> a variety <strong>of</strong> deficits. However, it is difficult to extrapolate fromthese observations to the general population.In a comprehensive review <strong>of</strong> cognitive deficits <strong>in</strong> arithmetic disabled childrenwhich <strong>in</strong>cluded noncl<strong>in</strong>ical studies, Geary (1993) identified two basic categories<strong>with</strong> different developmental trajectories. The first is manifested by the use <strong>of</strong>developmentally immature arithmetic procedures which usually rega<strong>in</strong> normallevels after 2 or 3 years <strong>of</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g. This category has been associated <strong>with</strong>deficits <strong>in</strong> count<strong>in</strong>g, computational skill, and work<strong>in</strong>g memory (Geary, 1990;Geary, Bow-Thomas, & Yao, 1992). The second category <strong>in</strong>volves a morepersistent difficulty <strong>with</strong> the representation and retrieval <strong>of</strong> arithmetic facts fromlong-term semantic memory. Fact-retrieval difficulties have been l<strong>in</strong>ked to deficits<strong>in</strong> count<strong>in</strong>g speed and work<strong>in</strong>g memory (Garnett & Fleischner, 1983; Geary,1990; Geary et al., 1991). It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to note that both <strong>of</strong> these categories<strong>in</strong>clude work<strong>in</strong>g memory, the limited capacity system responsible for transform<strong>in</strong>gand ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g temporary <strong>in</strong>formation (see, e.g., Baddeley & Hitch, 1974).A number <strong>of</strong> previous studies have compared measures <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong>normal children, children <strong>with</strong> arithmetic difficulties, and children <strong>with</strong> enhancedarithmetical ability (e.g., Bull & Johnston, 1997; Dark & Benbow, 1990, 1991;Geary et al., 1991; Hitch & McAuley, 1991; Siegel & Ryan, 1989; Swanson,1993, 1994). Although these studies differ <strong>in</strong> their assessment and aims, theyagree <strong>in</strong> suggest<strong>in</strong>g that work<strong>in</strong>g memory is related to differences <strong>in</strong> the abilityto perform arithmetic. For example, Geary et al. (1991) <strong>in</strong>vestigated arithmeticallydisabled and normal children over 1 year. The disabled group receivedremedial education <strong>in</strong> mathematics and were below the 46th percentile onnational achievement tests. Both forward and backward digit span were significantlyreduced for the disabled group, consistent <strong>with</strong> a work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficit.Geary et al. (1991; see also Geary, 1990, 1993) proposed that although skills suchas count<strong>in</strong>g knowledge and strategy choice are the primary areas <strong>of</strong> deficit <strong>in</strong> themathematically disabled child, work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficits may lead to a failure todevelop long-term memory representations <strong>of</strong> basic facts. Geary et al. (1991)


242 MCLEAN AND HITCHsuggest that if the representation <strong>of</strong> a problem’s <strong>in</strong>tegers is lost more rapidly fromwork<strong>in</strong>g memory it is less likely to be associated <strong>with</strong> the answer. However, theirarithmetic disabled children also had poor read<strong>in</strong>g skills. Their children’s averagepercentile rank<strong>in</strong>g for read<strong>in</strong>g was actually below that for arithmetic, and wasmuch lower than the read<strong>in</strong>g rank<strong>in</strong>g for the control group. This is importantbecause a significant amount <strong>of</strong> language is used <strong>in</strong> mathematics, and problems<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and mathematics <strong>of</strong>ten co-occur (Muth, 1984; Richman, 1983; seeGeary, 1993, for a review). Therefore, children <strong>with</strong> more general academicdifficulties have been <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> Geary and colleagues’ studies, and this leads toproblems <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>with</strong> respect to arithmetic per se.Evidence that work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficits differ as a function <strong>of</strong> the specificity<strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties is presented by Siegel and Ryan (1989). They gavechildren two complex span tasks designed to assess work<strong>in</strong>g memory capacity.These were a count<strong>in</strong>g span task (Case, Kurland, & Goldberg, 1982) and asentence span task (adapted from Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). <strong>Children</strong> <strong>with</strong>specific arithmetic difficulties (i.e., WRAT Arithmetic 25th percentile andWRAT Read<strong>in</strong>g 30th percentile) were impaired on count<strong>in</strong>g span but not onsentence span. In contrast, children who were impaired on read<strong>in</strong>g as wellas on arithmetic had significantly lower count<strong>in</strong>g and sentence spans. Siegel andRyan suggested that the read<strong>in</strong>g-and-arithmetic disabled group had a generalwork<strong>in</strong>g memory impairment, whereas the arithmetic disabled group had adoma<strong>in</strong>-specific work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficit. In a related study, Hitch and McAuley(1991) confirmed that children <strong>with</strong> specific arithmetic difficulties were impairedon count<strong>in</strong>g span but not on other complex span tasks. They also showedthat these children had significantly lower digit spans than did age-matchedcontrols.A general criticism <strong>of</strong> the studies described above is that they have employeda design <strong>in</strong> which children <strong>with</strong> arithmetic difficulties are compared to normalachievers <strong>of</strong> the same chronological age. This type <strong>of</strong> design cannot determ<strong>in</strong>ethe cause <strong>of</strong> the disability, as any cognitive deficits may be a consequence ratherthan a cause <strong>of</strong> low arithmetic atta<strong>in</strong>ment. Vellut<strong>in</strong>o, Pruzek, Steger, and Meshoulam(1973) attempted to resolve this problem <strong>in</strong> the area <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesby suggest<strong>in</strong>g that a younger ability-matched control group should be<strong>in</strong>cluded. The logic is that if the poor achievers perform worse on some cognitivetask than do younger, ability-matched controls, the deficit is unlikely to be asimple consequence <strong>of</strong> their low level <strong>of</strong> academic achievement. Given poorachievers’ greater age and experience, such a deficit would suggest they arearriv<strong>in</strong>g at similar levels <strong>of</strong> ability via different routes. However, Backman,Mamen, and Ferguson (1984) noted that age-matched controls rema<strong>in</strong> useful foridentify<strong>in</strong>g differences <strong>in</strong> cognitive function at the same level <strong>of</strong> exposure to theskill <strong>in</strong> question.A second concern about previous work is the sparsity <strong>of</strong> attempts to assess thevarious facets <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> poor arithmetic. If this system


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES243is impaired, it is clearly important to know more precisely how it is affected.There are several different ways <strong>of</strong> view<strong>in</strong>g work<strong>in</strong>g memory (see, e.g., Baddeley& Hitch, 1974; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Just & Carpenter, 1992; Turner &Engle, 1989). The Baddeley and Hitch (1974) multicomponent model was usedhere, as it expla<strong>in</strong>s a wide range <strong>of</strong> experimental and neuropsychological evidence(see also Baddeley, 1986) and tasks have been developed for assess<strong>in</strong>gsome <strong>of</strong> its components. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this model, work<strong>in</strong>g memory consists <strong>of</strong>a central executive processor which <strong>in</strong>teracts <strong>with</strong> two slave subsystems, thephonological loop and the visuospatial sketch pad. The phonological loop isspecialized for the storage and rehearsal <strong>of</strong> speech-based verbal <strong>in</strong>formation(Baddeley, 1992a, 1992b), whereas the sketch pad is specialized for hold<strong>in</strong>gvisual and spatial material (Logie, 1986; Qu<strong>in</strong>n & McConnell, 1996). In a recentdevelopment, Baddeley (1996) proposed a fractionation <strong>of</strong> the central executive<strong>in</strong>to separate but overlapp<strong>in</strong>g functions <strong>of</strong> coord<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g concurrent activities,switch<strong>in</strong>g retrieval plans, attend<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>puts, and hold<strong>in</strong>g and manipulat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> long-term memory.Evidence from studies <strong>of</strong> the normal population suggests that different components<strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory may have specialized roles <strong>in</strong> arithmetic (Ashcraft,1995). For example, the phonological loop appears to be <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> count<strong>in</strong>g(Logie & Baddeley, 1987) and <strong>in</strong> hold<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> complex calculations(Logie, Gilhooly, & Wynn, 1994; see also Fürst & Hitch, <strong>in</strong> press). Thevisuospatial sketch pad appears to be <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> multidigit problems wherevisual and spatial knowledge <strong>of</strong> column position<strong>in</strong>g is required (Heathcote,1994). Other researchers have noted the use <strong>of</strong> a visual number l<strong>in</strong>e (Dehaene,1992) and spatial representations <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual numbers (Hartje, 1987), but<strong>with</strong>out l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g them to work<strong>in</strong>g memory. The role <strong>of</strong> the central executive hasbeen noted several times (see, e.g., Ashcraft, Donley, Halas, & Vakali, 1992;Lemaire, Abdi, & Fayol, 1996; Logie et al., 1994). Ashcraft suggested that theexecutive is responsible for <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g and direct<strong>in</strong>g process<strong>in</strong>g, comprehension,and retrieval from long-term memory. However, remarkably little is known.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, the follow<strong>in</strong>g discussion generates tentative hypotheses about therole <strong>of</strong> different executive functions <strong>in</strong> arithmetic.The first executive function identified by Baddeley (1996) is the capacity tocoord<strong>in</strong>ate performance on two or more separate tasks. Arithmetic can beconsidered a multiple task when it <strong>in</strong>volves subtasks such as calculat<strong>in</strong>g partialtotals and keep<strong>in</strong>g track <strong>of</strong> other <strong>in</strong>formation. However, given that the subtasksare parts <strong>of</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tegrated skill, the requirement for coord<strong>in</strong>ation is presumablylow relative to perform<strong>in</strong>g multiple <strong>in</strong>dependent tasks. The second executivefunction is switch<strong>in</strong>g retrieval strategies. This is clearly necessary for problemssuch as multidigit multiplication, which typically <strong>in</strong>volves both multiply<strong>in</strong>g andadd<strong>in</strong>g. Experimental evidence suggests that switch<strong>in</strong>g is also required forcarry<strong>in</strong>g operations (Fürst & Hitch, <strong>in</strong> press). The third executive function isattend<strong>in</strong>g selectively to different <strong>in</strong>puts. This is clearly a feature <strong>of</strong> multidigit


244 MCLEAN AND HITCHproblems carried out as a series <strong>of</strong> subtasks, where attention is paid to selectedparts <strong>of</strong> a problem at different times. The fourth executive function is activat<strong>in</strong>gand manipulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> long-term memory. This seems likely to be<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> operations such as us<strong>in</strong>g the equivalence <strong>of</strong> two relationships (e.g.,5 2 4 3) <strong>in</strong> order to simplify a calculation. In summary, therefore, nearlyall the components <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory seem likely to be <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> arithmeticalcalculation, each play<strong>in</strong>g a somewhat different role.In light <strong>of</strong> the forego<strong>in</strong>g considerations, the present study had two aims. Thefirst was to ga<strong>in</strong> a more complete picture <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficits <strong>in</strong> children<strong>with</strong> specific arithmetic difficulties. This was achieved by us<strong>in</strong>g a battery <strong>of</strong> tasksassess<strong>in</strong>g different components <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory. The second aim was toconsider whether work<strong>in</strong>g memory deficits are responsible for children’s difficulties<strong>in</strong> arithmetic. This was <strong>in</strong>vestigated by apply<strong>in</strong>g the age-matched andability-matched design described above. To ensure the selectivity <strong>of</strong> arithmeticaldeficits, all three groups were matched on read<strong>in</strong>g. That is, <strong>in</strong> all three groups,children’s read<strong>in</strong>g was at normal, age-appropriate levels. An <strong>in</strong>cidental advantage<strong>of</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this way is that it avoids controversial issues associated<strong>with</strong> match<strong>in</strong>g groups on general <strong>in</strong>telligence (Siegel, 1988).The battery for assess<strong>in</strong>g work<strong>in</strong>g memory comprised two phonological looptasks, Digit Span and Nonword Repetition (Gathercole, Willis, Baddeley, &Emslie, 1994); two visuospatial sketch pad tasks, Visual Matrix Span (Wilson,Scott, & Power, 1987) and Corsi Block Span (Milner, 1971); and five centralexecutive tasks. The central executive tasks consisted <strong>of</strong> three Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trailstasks (Reitan, 1958), to assess switch<strong>in</strong>g between retrieval plans; a Cross<strong>in</strong>g Outtask (Moran & Mefford, 1959), to measure selective attention; and a novel“Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task,” designed as a measure <strong>of</strong> the ability to hold and manipulate<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> long-term memory. All the tasks used numerical materials whereverpossible, <strong>in</strong> order to maximize chances <strong>of</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g deficits <strong>in</strong> the poorarithmetic group. A general assessment <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> the arithmeticdoma<strong>in</strong> was also made. This was provided by Addition Span (Adams & Hitch,1997), a measure <strong>of</strong> how complex a mental addition children could performaccurately. More detailed rationales for these tasks are given <strong>in</strong> the Methodsection.Expectations for comparisons between children <strong>with</strong> poor arithmetic (PoorArithmetic) and their age-matched peers (Age-Matched) were as follows. Follow<strong>in</strong>gprevious work it was expected that children <strong>with</strong> Poor Arithmetic wouldbe impaired on Digit Span (see, e.g., Geary et al., 1991; Hitch & McAuley, 1991;Siegel & L<strong>in</strong>der, 1984). Nonword Repetition should be similarly impaired if thereduction <strong>in</strong> digit span is due to a deficit associated <strong>with</strong> the phonological loop.If Rourke’s ideas generalize to the normal population, the Poor Arithmetic groupshould be impaired on the two visuospatial tasks, Corsi Blocks Span and VisualMatrix Span. Least clear <strong>of</strong> all, but perhaps most <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, were expectationsfor the central executive tasks. Previous work demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g a deficit <strong>in</strong> count<strong>in</strong>g


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES245span (Siegel & Ryan, 1989) could be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as suggest<strong>in</strong>g an executivedeficit. However, this conclusion may not be justified (Hitch & McAuley, 1991;see also Towse & Hitch, 1995). Furthermore, if the executive can be fractionated,then the possibility arises <strong>of</strong> deficits <strong>in</strong> specific executive functions. Of thoseconsidered, activat<strong>in</strong>g long-term memory seemed particularly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g givenprevious work suggest<strong>in</strong>g impaired access to long-term memory representations<strong>in</strong> arithmetic disabled children (Koontz & Berch, 1996).No predictions were made for comparisons between the Poor Arithmetic groupand the Ability-Matched controls, as this comparison has not previously beenmade. However, any deficit <strong>in</strong> the Poor Arithmetic group would be particularly<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, as it would suggest a cognitive impairment that is not simply aconsequence <strong>of</strong> low atta<strong>in</strong>ment.<strong>Children</strong> were selected from a large sample attend<strong>in</strong>g normal schools byapply<strong>in</strong>g cut<strong>of</strong>fs to arithmetic and read<strong>in</strong>g scores. This was considered moreappropriate than us<strong>in</strong>g discrepancy scores (e.g., Swanson, 1994) or regressioncriteria, given the aim <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>with</strong> poor arithmetic <strong>in</strong> comparisonto the chronological age norm. It also overcomes problems <strong>with</strong> poorapproximation to the normal distribution. (For a review <strong>of</strong> selection techniquessee Fletcher et al., 1989). The cut<strong>of</strong>f procedure selects children that fall below aset criterion for arithmetic ability and above one for read<strong>in</strong>g ability. <strong>Children</strong> <strong>in</strong>the poor arithmetic group were chosen from the 4th year <strong>of</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g, as it wasfelt that by this age differences between read<strong>in</strong>g and arithmetic would haveemerged and it would be possible to f<strong>in</strong>d younger ability-matched controls.METHODParticipantsOne hundred twenty-two children (64 females and 58 males) participated <strong>in</strong>the <strong>in</strong>itial part <strong>of</strong> the study. Twenty-two were <strong>in</strong> their 3rd year <strong>of</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g (meanage 7 years 11 months), and 100 were <strong>in</strong> their 4th year <strong>of</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g (mean age9 years 0 months). They attended five local primary schools <strong>in</strong> the Lancaster area.All children were adm<strong>in</strong>istered the Graded Arithmetic–Mathematics Test (GAM;Vernon & Miller, 1976), the Primary Read<strong>in</strong>g Test (France, 1979), and six 1-m<strong>in</strong>written tests <strong>of</strong> speeded calculation (s<strong>in</strong>gle- plus s<strong>in</strong>gle-digit addition, s<strong>in</strong>gleplustwo-digit addition, two- plus two-digit addition, subtraction, multiplication,and division; adapted from Hitch, 1978).The Graded Arithmetic–Mathematics Test is a standardized group test forchildren aged 6–12 years. It has a time limit <strong>of</strong> 30 m<strong>in</strong> and conta<strong>in</strong>s 70 writtencomputation problems. These beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>with</strong> arithmetical items and end <strong>with</strong> mathematicalitems. In the present study only arithmetic was assessed, as childrennever got as far as the mathematical items <strong>in</strong> the time limit. The Primary Read<strong>in</strong>gTest (PRT) is another standardized group test suitable for 6- to 12-year-oldchildren. It comprises 10 picture–word match<strong>in</strong>g items and 38 sentence completionitems and has a time limit <strong>of</strong> 30 m<strong>in</strong>. Level 2 <strong>of</strong> the test was used <strong>in</strong> the


246 MCLEAN AND HITCHpresent study, as this was suitable for children aged above 7 years. Given that thestandardized scores for the two assessment tests were calculated more than 20years ago, raw scores were used to allocate children to groups.Twelve children (7 girls and 5 boys) were selected for the Poor Arithmeticgroup on the basis that they fell <strong>in</strong> the bottom 25% <strong>of</strong> raw scores on the GradedArithmetic–Mathematics Test and the middle 50% <strong>of</strong> raw scores on the PrimaryRead<strong>in</strong>g Test. This number constituted 8.5% <strong>of</strong> the 4th-year sample. TwelveAge-Matched controls (6 girls and 6 boys) were selected whose arithmetic andread<strong>in</strong>g raw scores were <strong>in</strong> the middle 50% <strong>of</strong> the sample. A further 12Ability-Matched controls (8 girls and 4 boys) were selected from the 3rd-yearsample on the basis that their arithmetic raw scores were similar to those <strong>of</strong> thePoor Arithmetic group and their read<strong>in</strong>g was normal for their age. Descriptivestatistics for each group are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 1.T tests were performed to ensure that the groups were adequately matched.These showed that the Poor Arithmetic group and Age-Matched controls did notdiffer on read<strong>in</strong>g, t(22) 1, but did on arithmetic, t(22) 10.1, p .001. ThePoor Arithmetic group and Ability-Matched controls did not differ on arithmetic,t(22) 1, but did on read<strong>in</strong>g, t(22) 2.59, p .05.<strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Memory</strong> BatteryPhonological LoopAuditory digit span. The forward auditory Digit Span task from the WISC-Rsubtest (Wechsler, 1974) was used as a standard measure <strong>of</strong> phonologicalshort-term memory. <strong>Children</strong> were given the test twice, and digit span wascalculated as the mean <strong>of</strong> the two scores. The reported reliability for 8- to16-year-old children on this task is 0.85 (Weschler, 1974).Nonword repetition. The <strong>Children</strong>’s Test <strong>of</strong> Nonword Repetition (Gathercoleet al., 1994) was used as a second <strong>in</strong>dex <strong>of</strong> phonological short-term memory. Thistest <strong>in</strong>volves the immediate repetition <strong>of</strong> auditorily presented nonwords and thusrequires temporary storage <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar phonological sequences. Its reliabilityfor 7-year-old children (the eldest reported) is 0.80 (Gathercole et al., 1994).Nonword repetition scores correlate reasonably well but not highly <strong>with</strong> digitspan (Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, & Baddeley, 1992), suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the twotasks tap a mixture <strong>of</strong> common and separate abilities.The test consists <strong>of</strong> 40 nonwords, <strong>with</strong> 10 items at each length <strong>of</strong> from two t<strong>of</strong>ive syllables. The child was asked to listen to a tape record<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> a femalespeaker read<strong>in</strong>g aloud the nonwords <strong>in</strong> a neutral English accent and to repeat eachone immediately after its presentation. Items were presented <strong>in</strong> a random orderand there was a 3-s <strong>in</strong>terval between the end <strong>of</strong> one item and the onset <strong>of</strong> the next.Credit was given for each correct repetition, <strong>with</strong> immediate self-correctionscredited as a correct response.


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES247TABLE 1Descriptive Statistics for the Selected GroupsMeasuresPoor Arithmetic(N 12)Age-Matched(N 12)Ability-Matched(N 12)Chronological ageMean 9.08 8.90 7.94SD 0.35 0.37 0.32ArithmeticRaw scoresMean 15.92 23.25 16.08SD 1.98 1.60 1.56Arithmetic ageMean 8.02 9.59 8.05SD 0.49 0.29 0.39Read<strong>in</strong>gRaw scoresMean 29.00 29.58 25.75SD 3.51 1.93 2.56Read<strong>in</strong>g ageMean 9.00 9.10 8.33SD 0.68 0.33 0.54Speeded calculationTotal scoreMean 44.42 72.67 40.10SD 3.92 22.01 11.12Visuospatial Sketch PadVisual matrix span. A matrix span task adapted from Wilson, Scott, and Power(1987) was used to measure visual work<strong>in</strong>g memory. This task assesses immediaterecognition memory for random visuospatial patterns <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g complexity.Stimuli are presented relatively briefly <strong>in</strong> order to m<strong>in</strong>imize the possibility<strong>of</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g verbal descriptions.The present version was adm<strong>in</strong>istered us<strong>in</strong>g a laptop computer. Stimuli consisted<strong>of</strong> a set <strong>of</strong> 15 matrices made up <strong>of</strong> different numbers <strong>of</strong> black boxes andwhite boxes. Each box was 2 cm square, and was drawn on a dark background.


248 MCLEAN AND HITCHThe first matrix had two boxes, one black and one white, and appeared <strong>in</strong> the topleft-hand side <strong>of</strong> the screen for 2000 ms. This was followed by a blank screen for2000 ms before a similar matrix appeared. The second matrix differed from thefirst <strong>in</strong> that the black box had changed to white. <strong>Children</strong> were asked to po<strong>in</strong>t tothe square which had changed. Once the child had given a response, the matrix<strong>in</strong>creased by two more squares either down or across the screen. The boxes werefilled black and white randomly, and the procedure was repeated as before. Trialscont<strong>in</strong>ued up to a 30 30 matrix. Span was measured as the largest matrixremembered correctly before two successive failures. Each child completed thetask twice and the two values <strong>of</strong> span were averaged.Corsi blocks span. The Corsi blocks task (Corsi, 1972), described by Milner(1971), was adm<strong>in</strong>istered to measure spatial span. The apparatus for this task isa set <strong>of</strong> identical blocks glued to random positions on a board. On each trial theparticipant observes the experimenter tap a sequence <strong>of</strong> blocks and then attemptsto reproduce the sequence. Span is measured by gradually <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g sequencelength to f<strong>in</strong>d the po<strong>in</strong>t where performance breaks down. Evidence that performanceon this spatiotemporal task dissociates from verbal serial recall is presentedby Isaacs and Vargha-Khadem (1989) and by Picker<strong>in</strong>g, Gathercole, andPeaker (1998).The apparatus consisted <strong>of</strong> n<strong>in</strong>e 4.4-cm square wooden blocks which werepa<strong>in</strong>ted black and fastened <strong>in</strong> random positions on a black board. To aid theexperimenter, the numbers 1–9 were pa<strong>in</strong>ted on the side <strong>of</strong> each block fac<strong>in</strong>gaway from the child. Blocks were tapped at a rate <strong>of</strong> one per second and no blockwas tapped more than once <strong>in</strong> a trial. The first trial was a sequence <strong>of</strong> length 2.If this was repeated correctly, then a sequence <strong>of</strong> length 3 was tapped. If an errorwas made the child was given a second chance at sequence length 2. If the childsucceeded on the second attempt, the task cont<strong>in</strong>ued at length 3. If not, the taskwas stopped. This <strong>in</strong>cremental procedure was cont<strong>in</strong>ued to determ<strong>in</strong>e span, thelongest sequence correctly repeated before two successive failures. Each childcompleted the task twice and the two values <strong>of</strong> span were averaged.Central ExecutiveMak<strong>in</strong>g Trails tasks. Follow<strong>in</strong>g Baddeley (1996), the ability to switch retrievalstrategies was assessed us<strong>in</strong>g variants <strong>of</strong> the Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails task (Reitan, 1958).This task <strong>in</strong>volves generat<strong>in</strong>g a stream <strong>of</strong> responses by alternat<strong>in</strong>g between twodifferent sequences. The dependent variable was the time taken to complete thetrail (or as much as could be done). There were three versions <strong>of</strong> the basic task,as follows.For Trails Written the material consisted <strong>of</strong> 22 circles on a page <strong>of</strong> A4 paper.Half the circles had a number <strong>in</strong> the center (1–11), and half had a letter (A–K).<strong>Children</strong> were asked to start at number 1 and make a trail <strong>with</strong> a pencil so thateach number alternated <strong>with</strong> its correspond<strong>in</strong>g letter (e.g., 1-A-2-B-. . .-11-K; seeFig. 1). They were not prompted if a mistake was made <strong>in</strong> any <strong>of</strong> the sequences.


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES249FIG 1.Example <strong>of</strong> Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails Written.Reported reliabilities for this version <strong>of</strong> the Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails task lie between 0.60and 0.90 (Lezak, 1995).Trails Verbal was similar to Trails Written except that the children were askedto do the task orally. Aga<strong>in</strong> they began at 1 and f<strong>in</strong>ished <strong>with</strong> K, or (<strong>in</strong> case <strong>of</strong>error) the 22nd response.Trails Color also was similar and was designed to m<strong>in</strong>imize any effects <strong>of</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g ability. It was adapted for use <strong>with</strong> children from an adult version (D’Elia& Satz, 1989). The materials consisted <strong>of</strong> 22 circles, half <strong>of</strong> which were p<strong>in</strong>k andhalf yellow, on a page <strong>of</strong> A4 paper. Each circle conta<strong>in</strong>ed a number from 1 to 11such that each number appeared once <strong>in</strong> a p<strong>in</strong>k circle and once <strong>in</strong> a yellow one.<strong>Children</strong> were asked to make a written trail start<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> yellow 1, then p<strong>in</strong>k 1,and so on, alternat<strong>in</strong>g colors <strong>with</strong> each number.Cross<strong>in</strong>g Out task. A Cross<strong>in</strong>g Out task adapted from Moran and Mefford(1959) was used to measure selective attention. It is similar to a standardcancellation task except that the target is repeatedly shifted. Demands on selectiveattention are considered high because ignor<strong>in</strong>g nontargets <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>in</strong>hibit<strong>in</strong>gresponses to items previously designated as targets.Each child was given a sheet <strong>of</strong> paper <strong>with</strong> 10 rows <strong>of</strong> digits. The first digit <strong>in</strong>each l<strong>in</strong>e was colored red and identified the target; the rest were black. The childwas asked to cross out all the digits <strong>in</strong> the l<strong>in</strong>e that were the same as the red one.


250 MCLEAN AND HITCHEach l<strong>in</strong>e conta<strong>in</strong>ed four randomly positioned targets. After the first l<strong>in</strong>e, whichwas used as practice, the child was <strong>in</strong>structed that the number to be crossed outwas different for each l<strong>in</strong>e. The dependent variable was the time taken tocomplete the 9 rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g rows.Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task. This was a novel task developed to measure the capacity tohold and manipulate <strong>in</strong>formation accessed <strong>in</strong> long-term memory. It was apaper-and-pencil task <strong>with</strong> items consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> one addition and a second,<strong>in</strong>complete addition (e.g., 2 3 4 ? ?). The child was asked to completethe equivalence and then the total. Three practice trials were given <strong>in</strong> which theexperimenter talked through how to do the task. After this the child completed 15more problems under <strong>in</strong>structions to be as accurate as possible. No prompt<strong>in</strong>gwas given for <strong>in</strong>correct responses. The dependent variable was the time taken tocomplete all 15 problems. The rationale for this task was that the child had toaccess long-term memory to complete the first relationship (2 3 ?) and thenma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>formation and access long-term memory aga<strong>in</strong> to complete thesecond relationship (5 4 ?).General ResourcesAddition Span task. This task was developed by Adams and Hitch (1997), and<strong>in</strong>volved mentally add<strong>in</strong>g a pair <strong>of</strong> numbers. The size <strong>of</strong> the numbers wasgradually <strong>in</strong>creased to f<strong>in</strong>d a span beyond which performance breaks down.Addition Span shares the requirement to comb<strong>in</strong>e temporary <strong>in</strong>formation storageand ongo<strong>in</strong>g process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> complex span tasks such as read<strong>in</strong>g span andlisten<strong>in</strong>g span (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). However, given evidence thatmental arithmetic loads on more than one component <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory (Fürst& Hitch, <strong>in</strong> press; Logie et al., 1994), it was considered here as a nonspecificmeasure <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory resources.The test was conducted orally and began <strong>with</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle-digit plus s<strong>in</strong>gle-digitaddition. If the answer was correct, a one-digit plus two-digit addition was givennext. If this was also answered correctly, the size <strong>of</strong> the next addition was<strong>in</strong>creased by one digit accord<strong>in</strong>g to the protocol described by Adams and Hitch(1997). Span was recorded as the largest problem answered correctly before twosuccessive failures. Each child was given the task twice and a mean span wascalculated from the two estimates. None <strong>of</strong> the additions <strong>in</strong>volved carry operations,as pairs <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegers mak<strong>in</strong>g up the additions always had totals between 5and 9.ProcedureThe task battery was adm<strong>in</strong>istered 3 months after the <strong>in</strong>itial screen<strong>in</strong>g, andtoward the end <strong>of</strong> the school year. Each child was tested <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong> a quietarea and completed the 10 tasks <strong>in</strong> two sessions about a week apart. Tasks weregiven <strong>in</strong> a different random order for each child, <strong>with</strong> the exception that the threeTrail Mak<strong>in</strong>g tasks were given together.


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES251TABLE 2Performance Characteristics <strong>of</strong> the Selected Groups on the Experimental MeasuresPoor-ArithmeticAge-MatchedAbility-MatchedANOVAMeasureM SD M SD M SD df F pPhonological loopDigit Span 4.79 0.50 5.42 a 0.79 4.75 0.92 2, 33 2.92 .07Nonword Repetition 26.17 5.98 26.17 4.93 26.92 6.39 2, 33 .07 ns(max 40)Visuo-spatial sketch padMatrix Span 7.83 2.88 7.29 1.98 6.41 1.77 2, 33 1.20 nsCorsi Span 3.92 0.56 4.92*** 0.67 4.00 0.77 2, 33 8.22 .01Central executiveTrails Written (s) 75.1 16.1 53.9* 12.5 78.5 31.2 2, 33 4.54 .05Trails Verbal (s) 66.8 26.3 38.9** 13.9 61.3 30.2 2, 33 4.38 .05Trails Color (s) 52.2 13.3 33.4** 11.5 49.2 13.6 2, 33 7.51 .01Cross<strong>in</strong>g Out (s) 88.3 23.6 77.7 13.8 100.9 30.8 2, 33 2.85 .07Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item (s) 282.1 74.5 157.1*** 38.5 215.4** 41.5 2, 33 16.10 .001General resourcesAddition Span 3.33 0.54 4.25*** 0.54 3.45 0.72 2, 33 8.06 .01Note. Asterisks apply to planned comparisons between Poor Arithmetic and Age-Matched andbetween Poor Arithmetic and Ability-Matched.a p .051.* p .05.** p .01.*** p .001.RESULTSDifferences between groups were analyzed us<strong>in</strong>g one-way analysis <strong>of</strong> variance(ANOVA) for each task supplemented by planned comparisons between the PoorArithmetic and Age-Matched groups, and between the Poor Arithmetic andAbility-Matched groups. Table 2 shows means and standard deviations together<strong>with</strong> the results <strong>of</strong> the ANOVAs. There were a number <strong>of</strong> significant ma<strong>in</strong>effects, but there were no significant effects for Nonword Repetition and VisualMatrix Span, and only marg<strong>in</strong>al effects for the Digit Span and Cross<strong>in</strong>g Outtasks.Planned comparisons revealed that the Poor Arithmetic group had significantlylower scores than did the Age-Matched controls on Corsi Span, F(1, 33) 13.35,partial ( 2 ) .29; Trails Written, F(1, 33) 5.73, partial ( 2 ) .15; TrailsVerbal, F(1, 33) 7.82, partial ( 2 ) .19; Trails Color, F(1, 33) 13.04,


252 MCLEAN AND HITCHpartial ( 2 ) .28; Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item, F(1, 33) 32.15, partial ( 2 ) .49; andAddition Span, F(1, 33) 13.70, partial ( 2 ) .29. The Poor Arithmetic groupalso had lower digit spans than did the Age-Matched controls and this differenceapproached significance F(1, 33) 4.09, p .051. Comparisons aga<strong>in</strong>st theAbility-Matched controls revealed that the Poor Arithmetic group were significantlyimpaired on the Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task, F(1, 33) 9.17, partial ( 2 ) .22, butnot on any <strong>of</strong> the other tasks.Error rates were recorded for the Cross<strong>in</strong>g Out and Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item tasks.Generally, these were low. The overall mean percentage was 0.6% for theCross<strong>in</strong>g Out task and 6.2% for the Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task. Error rates on the TrailMak<strong>in</strong>g tasks were not recorded because <strong>of</strong> difficulties scor<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>gthem.Table 3 shows partial correlations (to control for effects <strong>of</strong> age) between the10 work<strong>in</strong>g memory tasks and scores on the Graded Arithmetic–MathematicsTest, the Primary Read<strong>in</strong>g Test, and the Calculation Test. Five <strong>of</strong> the work<strong>in</strong>gmemory measures correlated significantly <strong>with</strong> arithmetic ability. These were theMiss<strong>in</strong>g Item task, Addition Span, Trails Verbal, Trails Color, and Corsi Span.The Calculation Test also showed a high correlation <strong>with</strong> arithmetic ability. It is<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to note that none <strong>of</strong> the measures correlated <strong>with</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g ability.DISCUSSIONThe results show that children <strong>with</strong> specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic areimpaired <strong>in</strong> several, but not all, aspects <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory. However, asexpla<strong>in</strong>ed above, tasks on which the Poor Arithmetic group were impairedrelative to Ability-Matched controls were considered the most likely to be<strong>in</strong>formative about the basis <strong>of</strong> their learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulty. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, this aspect<strong>of</strong> the results is discussed first.Relative to the Ability-Matched group, the Poor Arithmetic group were significantlyimpaired only <strong>in</strong> time to complete the Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task. As nodifference was noted <strong>in</strong> errors, this is unlikely to reflect a simple speed–accuracytrade-<strong>of</strong>f. The Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task was designed to assess the executive function <strong>of</strong>hold<strong>in</strong>g and manipulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> long-term memory. However, identification<strong>of</strong> the critical feature <strong>of</strong> this task must be tentative bear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d itsnovelty and the current status <strong>of</strong> the proposed fractionation <strong>of</strong> executive function.Some <strong>in</strong>formation is provided by the observation that children <strong>with</strong> Poor Arithmeticdid not differ from Ability-Matched controls on the Calculation Test andAddition Span. These tasks require access to similar arithmetical knowledge butdo not appear to <strong>in</strong>volve such demand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>with</strong> long-term memory.However, it is still open whether the children’s problem lies <strong>with</strong> resources for<strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>with</strong> long-term memory or elsewhere. An alternative explanation isthat the problem is one <strong>of</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g part–whole relations. Yet another is thatthe Poor Arithmetic group had weak conceptual and procedural knowledge <strong>of</strong>equivalence (see, e.g., Rittle-Johnson & Alibali, 1999). Thus, even though error


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES253TABLE 3Partial Correlation Coefficients between GAM Score, PRT Score, Calculation Test 1 Score,and the <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Memory</strong> Measures, Controll<strong>in</strong>g for Chronological Age .001 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 131. GAM Score 1.00 .28 .62*** .31 .06 .07 .42* .33 .52*** .47** .25 .60*** .54**2. PRT Score 1.00 .20 .16 .16 .11 .15 .12 .06 .07 .12 .12 .263. Calculation Test 1 1.00 .36* .30 .05 .30 .34 .53** .54** .22 .60*** .51**4. Digit Span 1.00 .46** .22 .31 .23 .07 .30 .06 .31 .265. Nonword Repetition 1.00 .34* .08 .02 .16 .08 .09 .11 .126. Matrix Span 1.00 .04 .24 .02 .12 .04 .18 .077. Corsi Span 1.00 .40* .42* .44** .26 .40* .34*8. Trails Written (s) 1.00 .26 .53** .40* .38* .209. Trails Verbal (s) 1.00 .37* .21 .34* .38*10. Trails Color (s) 1.00 .30 .47** .2211. Cross<strong>in</strong>g Out (s) 1.00 .42* .1312. Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item (s) 1.00 .39*13. Arithmetic Span 1.00Note. Correlation coefficients: df 33 <strong>in</strong> all cases.* p .05.** p .01.*** p .001.


254 MCLEAN AND HITCHrates were low, difficulty <strong>with</strong> the idea <strong>of</strong> equivalence could have reduced thespeed <strong>with</strong> which appropriate procedures could be carried out. However, further(as yet unpublished) data argue aga<strong>in</strong>st both <strong>of</strong> these <strong>in</strong>terpretations. A somewhatdifferent perspective is provided by Ericsson and K<strong>in</strong>tsch’s (1995) concept <strong>of</strong>long-term work<strong>in</strong>g memory as activated knowledge structures. These authorsproposed that expertise <strong>with</strong><strong>in</strong> a doma<strong>in</strong> is associated <strong>with</strong> more developedknowledge structures and an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the functional capacity <strong>of</strong> long-termwork<strong>in</strong>g memory. In this framework, therefore, poor performance on the Miss<strong>in</strong>gItem task could be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as reflect<strong>in</strong>g an impairment <strong>in</strong> long-term work<strong>in</strong>gmemory relative to children’s level <strong>of</strong> expertise.More generally, a number <strong>of</strong> researchers have suggested ways <strong>in</strong> whichlearn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic might reflect l<strong>in</strong>ks between work<strong>in</strong>g memoryand long-term memory (e.g., Geary et al., 1991; Hitch & McAuley, 1991; Siegler& Shrager, 1984). To discuss all <strong>of</strong> these would be beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> thepresent study. However, the present results do speak to one particular suggestion,namely that some children fail to develop long-term memory for basic numberfacts because <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory decays too quickly for relevantassociations to be formed (Geary et al., 1991). To the extent that simple andcomplex spans for verbal materials are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by decay (see, e.g., Baddeley,1986; Towse & Hitch, 1995), this account is supported by the lower digit spansand addition spans for the Poor Arithmetic group compared <strong>with</strong> their Age-Matched controls. However, faster decay <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation would not expla<strong>in</strong> theabsence <strong>of</strong> deficits <strong>in</strong> other retention tasks such as Nonword Repetition andMatrix Span. Furthermore, if faster decay were a causal factor, children <strong>with</strong>Poor Arithmetic might have been expected to perform significantly worse thanAbility-Matched controls on all the above tasks, and this was not the case.Overall, therefore, the present results provide little support for the hypothesis thatfaster decay <strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory is a major factor <strong>in</strong> poor arithmetical atta<strong>in</strong>ment(Geary et al., 1991).Other studies have attempted to expla<strong>in</strong> the deficits <strong>of</strong> children <strong>with</strong> arithmeticdifficulties <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> speed <strong>of</strong> calculation (Garnett & Fleischner, 1983; Geary,Widaman, Little, & Cormier, 1987; Geary et al., 1991). These studies have foundthat arithmetic disabled children are slower than their normal-achiev<strong>in</strong>g peers <strong>in</strong>solv<strong>in</strong>g arithmetic problems (Garnett & Fleischner, 1983; Geary & Brown, 1991)and that speed <strong>of</strong> addition fact retrieval is related to performance on arithmetictests (Geary & Burl<strong>in</strong>gham-Dubree, 1989). However, <strong>in</strong> the present study thereis no evidence to suggest that the Poor Arithmetic group were slower than theirAbility-Matched peers <strong>in</strong> solv<strong>in</strong>g simple arithmetic problems. For example, therewas no difference <strong>in</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> arithmetic problems they could do <strong>in</strong> 1 m<strong>in</strong>on the Calculation Test (see Table 1). Thus, <strong>in</strong>clusion <strong>of</strong> Ability-Matchedcontrols is valuable <strong>in</strong> show<strong>in</strong>g that children <strong>with</strong> poor arithmetic do not have anunusually slow calculation speed relative to their level <strong>of</strong> atta<strong>in</strong>ment.The second major feature <strong>of</strong> the results is that the Poor Arithmetic group were


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES255significantly impaired relative to the Age-Matched controls on 6 <strong>of</strong> the 10work<strong>in</strong>g memory measures: Corsi Span, Trails Written, Trails Verbal, TrailsColor, Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task, and Arithmetic Span (Digit Span was also nearsignificance). Given that the Corsi task has no obvious arithmetical or numericalcontent, the first observation raises the possibility that a spatiotemporal work<strong>in</strong>gmemory deficit contributes to poor arithmetic atta<strong>in</strong>ment. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>in</strong>cidentalobservation <strong>of</strong> children’s behavior suggested that none <strong>of</strong> the Poor Arithmeticgroup had unusually poor handwrit<strong>in</strong>g or misaligned their arithmetic problems,imply<strong>in</strong>g that they did not have problems <strong>with</strong> spatial coord<strong>in</strong>ation. Neuropsychologicalstudies <strong>of</strong> dyscalculia by Rourke and colleagues (e.g., Rourke &F<strong>in</strong>dlayson, 1978; Rourke & Strang, 1978) had found visual–spatial deficits <strong>in</strong>tasks such as Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, andPicture Assembly from the WISC-R (Wechsler, 1974). Given that the PoorArithmetic group were impaired on a test <strong>of</strong> spatial but not <strong>of</strong> visual work<strong>in</strong>gmemory <strong>in</strong> the present study, Rourke and colleagues’ conclusions appear togeneralize only partly to children <strong>with</strong> problems <strong>with</strong><strong>in</strong> the normal schoolpopulation.A second <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g set <strong>of</strong> differences between the Poor Arithmetic andAge-Matched groups are those on the Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails tasks. The visual versions,Trails Written and Trails Color, have been adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> previous studies <strong>of</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled children (e.g., McIntosh, Dunham, Dean, & Kundert, 1995;Share, M<strong>of</strong>fitt, & Silva, 1988; White et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1995). Whiteet al. (1992) concluded that arithmetic disabled children were impaired on thesetasks relative to normal and read<strong>in</strong>g disabled children <strong>of</strong> the same age and<strong>in</strong>terpreted this as reflect<strong>in</strong>g a problem <strong>of</strong> visuomotor <strong>in</strong>tegration. However,although their basic f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g was replicated, the observation that children werealso impaired on the verbal Trails task argues aga<strong>in</strong>st a visuomotor explanation.The idea <strong>of</strong> a deficit <strong>in</strong> switch<strong>in</strong>g between retrieval plans provides a moreparsimonious <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Moreover, given that the arithmetical content <strong>of</strong> theTrails tasks was m<strong>in</strong>imal, it seems unlikely that children’s poor performance wasa simple consequence their low arithmetic ability.It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g that the Poor Arithmetic group were impaired on Digit Spanrelative to the Age-Matched controls, though the difference just missed significance.Other studies where groups <strong>with</strong> poor and normal arithmetic werematched on read<strong>in</strong>g ability have shown small impairments <strong>in</strong> digit span (e.g.,Hitch & McAuley, 1991). However, the picture is not entirely clear, as Bull andJohnston (1997) and Swanson (1994) found no evidence for lowered digit span<strong>in</strong> their groups <strong>of</strong> arithmetic disabled children when read<strong>in</strong>g was controlled forstatistically. As noted above, the deficit <strong>in</strong> digit span does not appear to reflect aproblem associated <strong>with</strong> the phonological loop, as Nonword Repetition wasunaffected. An alternative possibility is that the deficit arises from a specificdifficulty <strong>in</strong> remember<strong>in</strong>g numerical stimuli.Results from the partial correlation matrix are useful <strong>in</strong> show<strong>in</strong>g that all the


256 MCLEAN AND HITCHtasks which gave significant differences between the groups were correlated <strong>with</strong>Graded Arithmetic–Mathematics Test score. None <strong>of</strong> the measures correlated<strong>with</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g ability. This was surpris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> regard to Digit Span and NonwordRepetition given evidence for an association between verbal short-term memoryand read<strong>in</strong>g (Rugel, 1974). However, it should be noted that the way the groupswere selected resulted <strong>in</strong> the sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> a restricted range <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g abilities,reduc<strong>in</strong>g sensitivity <strong>of</strong> this aspect <strong>of</strong> the correlational analyses.CONCLUSIONS<strong>Children</strong> <strong>with</strong> specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic were impaired relative toAge-Matched controls on a number <strong>of</strong> tasks tapp<strong>in</strong>g different aspects <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>gmemory. Two <strong>of</strong> these, Corsi Span and Mak<strong>in</strong>g Trails, suggest possible underly<strong>in</strong>gdeficits <strong>in</strong> spatiotemporal work<strong>in</strong>g memory and executive processes forswitch<strong>in</strong>g retrieval plans. When compared <strong>with</strong> younger Ability-Matched controls,children <strong>with</strong> specific difficulties <strong>in</strong> arithmetic were significantly impairedon only one measure, a Miss<strong>in</strong>g Item task. This deficit is tentatively <strong>in</strong>terpretedas reflect<strong>in</strong>g a deficit <strong>in</strong> executive processes controll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>with</strong> longtermmemory (Baddeley, 1996) or <strong>in</strong> long-term work<strong>in</strong>g memory (Ericsson &K<strong>in</strong>tsch, 1995).REFERENCESAdams, J. W., & Hitch, G. J. (1997). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory <strong>in</strong> children’s mental arithmetic. Journal <strong>of</strong>Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 21–38.Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development <strong>of</strong> mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach. DevelopmentalReview, 2, 213–236.Ashcraft, M. H. (1987). <strong>Children</strong>’s knowledge <strong>of</strong> simple arithmetic: A developmental model andsimulation. In J. Bisanz, C. J. Bra<strong>in</strong>erd, & R. Kail (Eds.), Formal methods <strong>in</strong> developmentalpsychology: Progress <strong>in</strong> cognitive development research (pp. 302–338). New <strong>York</strong>: Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag.Ashcraft, M. H. (1995). Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: A review and summary <strong>of</strong> newdirections. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 3–34.Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: Anexploratory <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 97–125.Ashcraft, M. H., Donley, R. D., Halas, M. A., & Vakali, M. (1992). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory, automaticity,and problem difficulty. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), The nature and orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> mathematical skills.Amsterdam: Elsevier.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, R. C., & Shiffr<strong>in</strong>, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its controlprocesses. In K. W. Spence (Ed.), The psychology <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g and motivation: Advances <strong>in</strong>research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New <strong>York</strong>: Academic Press.Backman, J. E., Mamen, M., & Ferguson, H. B. (1984). Read<strong>in</strong>g level design: Conceptual andmethodological issues <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g research. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 96, 560–568.Baddeley, A. D. (1986). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.Baddeley, A. D. (1992a). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory. Science, 255, 556–559.Baddeley, A. D. (1992b). Is work<strong>in</strong>g memory work<strong>in</strong>g? The fifteenth Bartlett lecture. QuarterlyJournal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology, 44A, 1–31.Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Explor<strong>in</strong>g the central executive. Quarterly Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology,49A, 5–28.Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology <strong>of</strong>


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES257learn<strong>in</strong>g and motivation: Advances <strong>in</strong> research and theory (Vol. VIII, pp. 47–90). New <strong>York</strong>:Academic Press.Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments <strong>in</strong> the concept <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory.Neuropsychology, 8, 485–493.Badian, N. A. (1983). Dyscalculia and nonverbal disorders <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g. In H. R. Myklebust (Ed.),Progress <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities (Vol. 5, pp. 235–264). New <strong>York</strong>: Stratton.Baroody, A. J. (1983). The development <strong>of</strong> procedural knowledge: An alternative explanation forchronometric trends <strong>of</strong> mental arithmetic. Developmental Review, 3, 225–230.Benson, D. F., & Weir, W. F. (1972). Acalculia: Acquired anarithmetria. Cortex, 8, 465–472.Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). <strong>Children</strong>’s arithmetic difficulties: Contributions from process<strong>in</strong>gspeed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Child Psychology,65, 1–24.Campbell, J. I. D. (1987). Network <strong>in</strong>terference and mental multiplication. Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalPsychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 13, 109–123.Campbell, J. I. D., & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: Structure, process andacquisition. Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 39, 338–366.Case, R., Kurland, M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth <strong>of</strong> short termmemory span. Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404.Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region <strong>of</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong>. Unpublisheddoctoral dissertation, McGill <strong>University</strong>, Montreal, Canada.Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences <strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory and read<strong>in</strong>g.Journal <strong>of</strong> Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450–466.Dark, V., & Benbow, C. P. (1990). Enhanced problem translation and short-term memory: Components<strong>of</strong> mathematical talent. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 82, 420–429.Dark, V., & Benbow, C. P. (1991). Differential enhancement <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>with</strong> mathematicalversus verbal precocity. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 83, 48–60.Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties <strong>of</strong> numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.D’Elia, L., & Satz, P. (1989). Color Trails 1 and 2. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment.Ericsson, K. A., & K<strong>in</strong>tsch, W. (1995). Long-term work<strong>in</strong>g memory. Psychological Review, 102,211–245.Fletcher, J. M., Epsy, K. A., Francis, D. J., Davidson, K. C., Rourke, B. P., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1989).Comparisons <strong>of</strong> cut<strong>of</strong>f score and regression-based def<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. Journal <strong>of</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 22, 334–338.France, N. (1979). The Primary Read<strong>in</strong>g Test. W<strong>in</strong>dsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.Fürst, A. J., & Hitch, G. J. (<strong>in</strong> press). Different roles for executive and phonological components <strong>of</strong>work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> mental arithmetic. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cognition.Garnett, K., & Fleischner, J. (1983). Automatization and basic fact performance <strong>of</strong> normal andlearn<strong>in</strong>g disabled children. Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities Quarterly, 6, 223–230.Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The <strong>Children</strong>’s Test <strong>of</strong>Nonword Repetition: A test <strong>of</strong> phonological work<strong>in</strong>g memory. <strong>Memory</strong>, 2, 103–127.Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. (1992). Phonological memory andvocabulary development <strong>in</strong> the early school years: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. Developmental Psychology,28, 887–898.Geary, D. C. (1990). A componential analysis <strong>of</strong> an early learn<strong>in</strong>g deficit <strong>in</strong> mathematics. Journal <strong>of</strong>Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 363–383.Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological and genetic components.Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 114, 345–362.Geary, D. C. (1994). <strong>Children</strong>’s mathematical development: Research and practical applications.Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: American Psychological Association.Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Yao, Y. (1992). Count<strong>in</strong>g knowledge and skill <strong>in</strong> cognitiveaddition: A comparison <strong>of</strong> normal and mathematically disabled children. Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalChild Psychology, 54, 372–391.


258 MCLEAN AND HITCHGeary, D. C., & Brown, S. C. (1991). Cognitive addition: Strategy choice and speed-<strong>of</strong>-process<strong>in</strong>gdifferences <strong>in</strong> gifted, normal, and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology,27, 398–406.Geary, D. C., Brown, S. C., & Samanayake, V. A. (1991). Cognitive addition: A short longitud<strong>in</strong>alstudy <strong>of</strong> strategy choice and speed-<strong>of</strong>-process<strong>in</strong>g differences <strong>in</strong> normal and mathematicallydisabled children. Developmental Psychology, 27, 787–797.Geary, D. C., & Burl<strong>in</strong>gham-Dubree, M. (1989). External validation <strong>of</strong> the strategy choice model foraddition. Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 175–192.Geary, D. C., Widaman, K. F., Little, T. D., & Cormier, P. (1987). Cognitive addition: Comparison<strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled and academically normal elementary school children. Cognitive Development,2, 249–269.Hartje, W. (1987). The effect <strong>of</strong> spatial disorders on arithmetic skills. In G. Deloche & X. Seron(Eds.), Mathematical disabilities: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective (pp. 121–135).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Heathcote, D. (1994). The role <strong>of</strong> visuo-spatial work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> the mental addition <strong>of</strong> multi-digitaddends. Current Psychology <strong>of</strong> Cognition, 13, 207–245.Hitch, G. J. (1978). The role <strong>of</strong> short-term memory <strong>in</strong> mental arithmetic. Cognitive Psychology, 10,302–323.Hitch, G. J., & McAuley, E. (1991). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> specific arithmetical learn<strong>in</strong>gdifficulties. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 82, 375–386.Isaacs, E. B., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (1989). Differential course <strong>of</strong> development <strong>of</strong> spatial and verbalmemory span: A normative study. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental Psychology, 7, 337–380.Jackson, M., & Warr<strong>in</strong>gton, E. K. (1986). Arithmetic skills <strong>in</strong> patients <strong>with</strong> unilateral cerebral lesions.Cortex, 22, 611–620.Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory <strong>of</strong> comprehension: Individual differences<strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.Klove, J. (1963). Cl<strong>in</strong>ical neuropsychology. In F. M. Forster (Ed.), The medical cl<strong>in</strong>ics <strong>of</strong> NorthAmerica (pp. 1647–1658). New <strong>York</strong>: Saunders.Koontz, K. L., & Berch, D. B. (1996). Identify<strong>in</strong>g simple numerical stimuli: Process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>efficienciesexhibited by arithmetic learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled children. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 1–23.Kosc, L. (1974). Developmental dyscalculia. Journal <strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 7, 46–58.Lemaire, P., Abdi, H., & Fayol, M. (1996). The role <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory resources <strong>in</strong> simplecognitive arithmetic. European Journal <strong>of</strong> Cognitive Psychology, 8, 73–103.Lev<strong>in</strong>e, M. D. (1987). Developmental variation and learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders. Cambridge, MA: EducatorsPublish<strong>in</strong>g Service.Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.Logie, R. H. (1986). Visuo-spatial process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory. Quarterly Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalPsychology, 38A, 229–247.Logie, R. H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Cognitive processes <strong>in</strong> count<strong>in</strong>g. Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalPsychology, 13, 310–326.Logie, R. H., Gilhooly, K. J., & Wynn, V. (1994). Count<strong>in</strong>g on work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> arithmeticproblem solv<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cognition, 22, 395–410.McCloskey, M., Harley, W., & Sokol, S. M. (1991). Models <strong>of</strong> arithmetic fact retrieval: Anevaluation <strong>in</strong> light <strong>of</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from normal and bra<strong>in</strong>-damaged subjects. Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalPsychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 17, 377–397.McIntosh, D. E., Dunham, M. D., Dean, R. S., & Kundert, D. K. (1995). Neuropsychologicalcharacteristics <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g-disabled gifted children. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience, 83,123–130.Milner, B. (1971). Interhemisperic differences <strong>in</strong> the localisation <strong>of</strong> psychological processes <strong>in</strong> man.British Medical Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 27, 272–277.Moran, L. J., & Mefford, R. B. (1959). Repetitive psychometric measures. Psychological Reports, 5,269–275.


WORKING MEMORY AND ARITHMETIC DIFFICULTIES259Muth, K. D. (1984). Solv<strong>in</strong>g arithmetic word problems: Role <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g and computational skills.Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 76, 205–210.Picker<strong>in</strong>g, S. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Peaker, S. M. (1998). Verbal and visuo-spatial short-termmemory <strong>in</strong> children: Evidence for common and dist<strong>in</strong>ct mechanisms. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cognition, 26,1117–1130.Qu<strong>in</strong>n, J. G., & McConnell, J. (1996). Irrelevant pictures <strong>in</strong> visual work<strong>in</strong>g memory. The QuarterlyJournal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology, 49A, 200–215.Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity <strong>of</strong> the trail mak<strong>in</strong>g test as an <strong>in</strong>dicator <strong>of</strong> organic bra<strong>in</strong> damage.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276.Richman, L. C. (1983). Language-learn<strong>in</strong>g disability: Issues, research, and future directions. In M.Wolraich & D. K. Routh (Eds.), Advances <strong>in</strong> developmental and behavioural pediatrics (Vol. 4,pp. 87–107). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Rittle-Johnson, B., & Alibali, M. W. (1999). Conceptual and procedural knowledge <strong>of</strong> mathematics:Does one lead to the other? Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Psychology, 91, 175–189.Rourke, B. P. (1993). Arithmetic disabilities, specific and otherwise: A neuropsychological perspective.Journal <strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 26, 214–226.Rourke, B. P., & F<strong>in</strong>dlayson, M. A. J. (1978). Neuropsychological significance <strong>of</strong> variations <strong>in</strong>patterns <strong>of</strong> academic skills: Verbal and visual–spatial abilities. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal ChildPsychology, 6, 121–133.Rourke, B. P., & Strang, J. D. (1978). Neuropsychological significance <strong>of</strong> variations <strong>in</strong> patterns <strong>of</strong>academic performance: Motor, psychomotor, and tactile–perceptual abilities. Journal <strong>of</strong> PediatricPsychology, 3, 62–66.Rugel, R. P. (1974). WISC subtest scores <strong>of</strong> disabled readers: A review <strong>with</strong> respect to Bannatyne’srecategorization. Journal <strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 7, 48–55.Share, D. L., M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988). Factors associated <strong>with</strong> arithmetic-and-read<strong>in</strong>gdisability and specific arithmetic disability. Journal <strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 21, 313–320.Siegel, L. S. (1988). Evidence that IQ scores are irrelevant to the def<strong>in</strong>ition and analysis <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gdisability. Canadian Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 42, 201–215.Siegel, L. S., & L<strong>in</strong>der, A. (1984). Short-term memory processes <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g andarithmetic disabilities. Developmental Psychology, 20, 200–207.Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory <strong>in</strong> normally achiev<strong>in</strong>g andsubtypes <strong>of</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g disabled children. Child Development, 60, 973–980.Siegler, R. S. (1988). Individual differences <strong>in</strong> strategy choices: Good students, not-so-good students,and perfectionists. Child Development, 59, 833–851.Siegler, R. S., & Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, E. (1984). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choice <strong>in</strong> addition and subtraction: How do childrenknow what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> cognitive skills (pp. 229–293). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Swanson, H. L. (1993). <strong>Work<strong>in</strong>g</strong> memory <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g disability subgroups. Journal <strong>of</strong> ExperimentalChild Psychology, 56, 87–114.Swanson, H. L. (1994). Short-term memory and work<strong>in</strong>g memory: Do both contribute to ourunderstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> academic achievement <strong>in</strong> children and adults <strong>with</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g disabilities. Journal<strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g Disabilities, 27, 34–50.Temple, C. M. (1989). Digit dyslexia: A category-specific disorder <strong>in</strong> developmental dyscalculia.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 93–116.Temple, C. M. (1991). Procedural dyscalculia and number fact dyscalculia: Double dissociation <strong>in</strong>developmental dyscalculia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 155–176.Towse, J. N., & Hitch, G. J. (1995). Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space<strong>in</strong> tests <strong>of</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g memory capacity? The Quarterly Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 48A, 108–124.Turner, M., & Engle R. W. (1989). Is work<strong>in</strong>g memory capacity task dependent? Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Memory</strong>and Language, 28, 127–154.


260 MCLEAN AND HITCHVellut<strong>in</strong>o, F. R., Pruzek, R., Steger, J. A., & Meshoulam, U. (1973). Immediate visual recall <strong>in</strong> poorreaders as a function <strong>of</strong> orthographic–l<strong>in</strong>guistic familiarity. Cortex, 9, 368–384.Vernon, P., & Miller, K. (1978). Graded Arithmetic–Mathematics Test. London: Hodder & Staughton.Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for <strong>Children</strong>–Revised. New <strong>York</strong>:Psychological Corporation.White, J. L., M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Neuropsychological and socio-emotional correlates<strong>of</strong> specific-arithmetic disability. Archives <strong>of</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Neuropsychology, 7, 1–16.Williams, J., Rickert, V., Hogan, J., Zolten, A. J., Satz, P., D’Elia, L. F., Asarnow, R. F., Zaucha, K.,& Light, R. (1995). <strong>Children</strong>’s Color Trails. Archives <strong>of</strong> Neuropsychology, 10, 211–223.Wilson, J. T. L., Scott, J. H., & Power, K. G. (1987). Developmental differences <strong>in</strong> the span <strong>of</strong> visualmemory for pattern. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental Psychology, 5, 249–255.Received June 22, 1998; revised July 12, 1999

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!