12.07.2015 Views

A comparison of methods for ERP assessment in a P300-based GKT

A comparison of methods for ERP assessment in a P300-based GKT

A comparison of methods for ERP assessment in a P300-based GKT

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsychoA <strong>comparison</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>methods</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>ERP</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>in</strong> a <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong>Vahid Abootalebi a , Mohammad Hassan Moradi a, ⁎ , Mohammad Ali Khalilzadehb,ca Biomedical Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g Faculty, Amirkabir University <strong>of</strong> Technology, Tehran, Iranb Islamic Azad University, Mashhad Branch, Iranc Research Center <strong>of</strong> Intelligent Signal Process<strong>in</strong>g, Tehran, IranReceived 16 January 2006; received <strong>in</strong> revised <strong>for</strong>m 25 May 2006; accepted 26 May 2006Available onl<strong>in</strong>e 24 July 2006Abstract<strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> (guilty knowledge test) has been suggested as an alternative approach <strong>for</strong> conventional polygraphy. The purpose <strong>of</strong> this studyis to evaluate three classify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>methods</strong> <strong>for</strong> this approach and compare their per<strong>for</strong>mances <strong>in</strong> a lab analogue. Several subjects went through thedesigned <strong>GKT</strong> paradigm and their respective bra<strong>in</strong> signals were recorded. For the analysis <strong>of</strong> signals, BAD (bootstrapped amplitude difference)and BCD (bootstrapped correlation difference) <strong>methods</strong> as two predef<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>methods</strong> alongside a new approach consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> wavelet features and astatistical classifier were implemented. The rates <strong>of</strong> correct detection <strong>in</strong> guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent subjects were 74–80%. The results <strong>in</strong>dicate thepotential <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> <strong>for</strong> detect<strong>in</strong>g concealed <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation, although further research is required to <strong>in</strong>crease its accuracy and precision andevaluat<strong>in</strong>g its vulnerability to countermeasures.© 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.Keywords: Psychophysiological detection <strong>of</strong> deception; Event-related potential (<strong>ERP</strong>); <strong>P300</strong>; Guilty knowledge test (<strong>GKT</strong>); Lie detection; Wavelet1. IntroductionIn recent years, various approaches to psychophysiologicaldetection <strong>of</strong> deception have been developed (National ResearchCouncil [NRC], 2002). The polygraphic tests are the mosttraditional <strong>methods</strong> <strong>for</strong> this approach. Most commonly usedphysiological measures <strong>in</strong> polygraph systems are respiration,cardiovascular measures and electrodermal response (EDR).These measures are derived pr<strong>in</strong>cipally from structures <strong>in</strong>nervatedby the autonomic nervous system. Emotional effects <strong>of</strong>deception are presumed to be reflected <strong>in</strong> such autonomic measures.A polygraph system records these measures dur<strong>in</strong>g a specifiedtest <strong>in</strong> which the exam<strong>in</strong>er asks some questions <strong>of</strong> thesubject. By analyz<strong>in</strong>g these physiological signs dur<strong>in</strong>g relevantand irrelevant questions, the polygraph exam<strong>in</strong>er can detect guiltor <strong>in</strong>nocence. Hence the polygraph and other measure <strong>of</strong> autonomicactivity reflect the peripheral manifestations <strong>of</strong> very complexcognitive and affective operations that occur when peoplegive deceptive or nondeceptive answers to questions.⁎ Correspond<strong>in</strong>g author. Fax: +98 21 66495655.E-mail address: mhmoradi@aut.ac.ir (M.H. Moradi).By their very nature, polygraph measurements provide anextremely limited and <strong>in</strong>direct view <strong>of</strong> complex underly<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong>processes. A researchable hypothesis is that by look<strong>in</strong>g at bra<strong>in</strong>function more directly, it might be possible to understand andultimately detect deception. There are ma<strong>in</strong>ly two techniques <strong>for</strong>study<strong>in</strong>g on bra<strong>in</strong> functions: (1) functional bra<strong>in</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>g and (2)record<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> potentials. In the <strong>for</strong>mer, imag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>methods</strong> suchas positron emission tomography (PET) or functional magneticresonance imag<strong>in</strong>g (fMRI) are used to measure local bra<strong>in</strong> activity.The latter technique measures event-related changes <strong>in</strong> theEEG that are known as event-related potentials (<strong>ERP</strong>). The ma<strong>in</strong>advantage <strong>of</strong> the imag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>methods</strong> is their good spatial resolutionand their ma<strong>in</strong> disadvantage is their low time resolution. On theother hand, <strong>ERP</strong>s have better time resolution and lower spatialresolution. Besides that, functional imag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>methods</strong> are moreexpensive and time consum<strong>in</strong>g than <strong>ERP</strong> record<strong>in</strong>g.Both <strong>of</strong> the techniques described above have been used asmeans <strong>for</strong> lie detection. There are few recent studies that use fMRIto identify association between deception and specific bra<strong>in</strong>activity (Ganis et al., 2003; Kozel et al., 2004a,b; Langleben et al.,2005; Lee et al., 2002; Nunez et al., 2005; Phan et al., 2005).These <strong>in</strong>vestigations seek to identify signatures <strong>of</strong> deception0167-8760/$ - see front matter © 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.05.009


310 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320<strong>in</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> processes. In spite <strong>of</strong> some good results, which werereported from these studies, the number <strong>of</strong> subjects studied is notsufficient enough and they also have some limitation as a soundmethod <strong>for</strong> detection <strong>of</strong> deception (NRC, 2002). In contrast t<strong>of</strong>MRI approaches to lie detection, <strong>ERP</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>methods</strong> havebeen more widely studied and have achieved more satisfy<strong>in</strong>gresults (Rosenfeld, 2002; Farwell and Donch<strong>in</strong>, 1991; Allen etal., 1992).<strong>ERP</strong>s are recorded from the central nervous system and areconsidered to be affected by the recognition <strong>of</strong> important events,which is more cognitively determ<strong>in</strong>ed activity than autonomicresponses. An endogenous <strong>ERP</strong>, which has been extensivelystudied, is the <strong>P300</strong> (P3) wave. It is seen <strong>in</strong> response to rare,mean<strong>in</strong>gful stimuli <strong>of</strong>ten called “oddball” stimuli (Polich, 2000).For example, if a subject is view<strong>in</strong>g a Bernoulli (random) series <strong>of</strong>names, one every three seconds, and occasionally, one <strong>of</strong> these isthe subject's name, a P3 wave is evoked <strong>in</strong> response to this rarelypresented, recognized, mean<strong>in</strong>gful (autobiographical) stimulus.P3 is a positive-go<strong>in</strong>g wave with a scalp amplitude distribution <strong>in</strong>which it is largest parietally (at Pz) and smallest frontally (Fz),tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>termediate values centrally (Cz). (Fz, Cz, and Pz are scalpsites along the midl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>of</strong> the head.) Its peak has a typical latency<strong>of</strong> 300–1000 ms from stimulus onset. The size or amplitude <strong>of</strong> P3at a given record<strong>in</strong>g site is <strong>in</strong>versely proportional to the rareness <strong>of</strong>presentation; <strong>in</strong> practice, probabilities I while <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>nocents Pis another I and so no P−I difference is expected. Based on thistheory, the BAD method (bootstrapped amplitude difference)was <strong>in</strong>troduced which will be described <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g section.The second approach, <strong>in</strong>troduced by Farwell and Donch<strong>in</strong>(1991), is <strong>based</strong> on the expectation that <strong>in</strong> a guilty person, the Pand T stimuli should evoke similar <strong>P300</strong> responses, whereas <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>nocent subject, P responses will look more like I responses.Thus, <strong>in</strong> this method we called here bootstrapped correlationdifference (BCD), the cross correlation <strong>of</strong> P and T wave<strong>for</strong>ms iscompared with that <strong>of</strong> P and I. In guilty subjects, the P–Tcorrelation is expected to exceed the P–I correlation and theopposite is expected <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>nocents.Despite <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> lie detectors <strong>in</strong> 1985–1990 by some research groups, its efficacy is yet controversial.There are different results reported from <strong>in</strong>dependent laboratorieson the accuracy <strong>of</strong> this method. Farwell and Donch<strong>in</strong>reported a detection rate <strong>of</strong> 87.5% us<strong>in</strong>g BCD method (Farwelland Donch<strong>in</strong>, 1991). But <strong>in</strong> recent years, Farwell claims that hisupdated method – named Bra<strong>in</strong>F<strong>in</strong>gerPr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g – has reached toa detection rate <strong>of</strong> 100% (see www.bra<strong>in</strong>wavescience.com),although these results have not been published <strong>in</strong> any peerreviewedpaper and are highly controversial (Rosenfeld, 2005).Rosenfeld lab has reported a typically 80–95% detection rateus<strong>in</strong>g BAD and BCD <strong>methods</strong> and found the <strong>for</strong>mer to be moreaccurate (Rosenfeld et al., 2004). Allen and his coworkers hadreported 90% and above hit rate <strong>in</strong> detection concealed thoughtover-learned <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation (Allen et al., 1992). But <strong>in</strong> their recentpaper, they report poor detection (27–47%) <strong>of</strong> mock crimedetails (Mertens et al., 2003). Apart from these lab analogues,there has been only one <strong>in</strong>dependent field study <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong>lie detection, which has reported only 48% detection <strong>of</strong> guiltysubjects (Miyake et al., 1993).The purpose <strong>of</strong> the present study was to exam<strong>in</strong>e the <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> <strong>in</strong> a lab analogue and also to compare three differentclassifiers <strong>for</strong> this approach. Similar to all lie detection systems,this method <strong>in</strong>cluded a paradigm <strong>for</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g the test andrecord<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> signals and also a method <strong>for</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g therecords to detect the guilty/<strong>in</strong>nocent subjects. Our scenario <strong>in</strong>


V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320311the test was a mock crime (steal<strong>in</strong>g a jewel), which was similarto paradigms used <strong>in</strong> other studies on <strong>GKT</strong>. In the analysis <strong>of</strong> thesignal, BAD and BCD <strong>methods</strong> – with some modifications –were implemented. In addition to these <strong>methods</strong>, a new approach<strong>based</strong> on some wavelet features and a statistic classifierwas designed and applied on the data. The results <strong>of</strong> these<strong>methods</strong> will be reported and compared.2. Methods <strong>of</strong> <strong>ERP</strong> <strong>assessment</strong>2.1. Bootstrapped amplitude difference (BAD)In the BAD method, we want to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether theamplitude <strong>of</strong> the <strong>P300</strong> evoked by P stimuli, are significantlygreater than that evoked by irrelevants with<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dividual ornot? Answer<strong>in</strong>g this statistical question requires know<strong>in</strong>g thedistributions <strong>of</strong> average <strong>P300</strong> waves <strong>for</strong> each set or at leastenough samples <strong>of</strong> the averages. However <strong>for</strong> each subject, onehas available only one average <strong>P300</strong> from each set <strong>of</strong> stimuli(Ts, Ps, and Is). The statistical technique <strong>of</strong> bootstrapp<strong>in</strong>g(Wasserman and Bockenholt, 1989) provides a solution to thisproblem. The bootstrap variation used here is as follows: acomputer program goes through the probe set (all s<strong>in</strong>glesweeps) and selects at random, with replacement, a set <strong>of</strong> N 1s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps. It averages these wave<strong>for</strong>ms and calculates <strong>P300</strong>amplitude from this average as described later. Similarly a set <strong>of</strong>N 2 s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps is selected from the irrelevant set and theaverage <strong>P300</strong> amplitude is calculated from them. The numbersN 1 and N 2 are the actual number <strong>of</strong> accepted probe sweeps andirrelevant sweeps <strong>for</strong> that subject, respectively. The calculatedirrelevant mean <strong>P300</strong> is subtracted from the comparable probevalue, and one thus obta<strong>in</strong>s a difference value between P andI (D 1 =P−I). Then a set <strong>of</strong> N 3 s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps is selected similarlyfrom the target set and an average <strong>P300</strong> amplitude iscalculated <strong>for</strong> this >set. Next a difference value between P andT (D 2 =T−P) is calculated. In the present study, the ma<strong>in</strong> BADparameter used <strong>for</strong> decision about guilt/<strong>in</strong>nocence <strong>of</strong> subject isD=D 1 −D 2 . 100 iterations <strong>of</strong> this algorithm will yield 100values <strong>for</strong> the D parameter. The number <strong>of</strong> D-values <strong>in</strong> whichD>0 is then counted (N D_0 ). The larger this number (N D_0 )is, the more different P is from I and the more similar is to T andthus the subject is more likely to be guilty. One can use athreshold (N th )onN D_0 and make a decision on the guilt <strong>of</strong> thesubject if N D_0 >N th . It must be noted that, the greater N th is, themore certa<strong>in</strong> the guilty decisions will be; however, some reallyguilty subjects – with poor <strong>P300</strong> <strong>in</strong> their P stimuli – may be undetectedand appear to be <strong>in</strong>nocent.2.2. Bootstrapped correlation difference (BCD)BCD answers this question: “Are the cross correlation coefficientsbetween <strong>ERP</strong> responses to probe and target stimulisignificantly greater than the correspond<strong>in</strong>g cross correlation <strong>of</strong>responses to probe and irrelevant stimuli?” if so, the subject isfound to be guilty. To answer this question, <strong>for</strong> each subject, theprogram first averages all s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps <strong>of</strong> P, T and I stimuli, anddeterm<strong>in</strong>es the with<strong>in</strong>-subject <strong>ERP</strong> average over all stimuli. Thisoverall average is saved <strong>in</strong> a vector called M. Thenbythebootstrapp<strong>in</strong>g method, one value <strong>for</strong> the average <strong>of</strong> each stimulustype is obta<strong>in</strong>ed. M is subtracted from each P, T and I averagevalues. This enlarges the difference between the wave<strong>for</strong>ms. Thecross correlation coefficients <strong>of</strong> the P and T (r(P,T)) and <strong>of</strong> the Pand I (r(P,I)) are now computed. The difference between thesetwo r-values (D=r(P,T)−r(P,I)) is computed next. The D is ascale <strong>for</strong> the existence or absence <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong> <strong>in</strong> the probe stimuli. Theabove process is iterated 100 times yield<strong>in</strong>g 100 values <strong>for</strong> Dparameter. Us<strong>in</strong>g the method <strong>of</strong> Farwell and Donch<strong>in</strong> (1991),thenumber <strong>of</strong> D-values <strong>in</strong> which D>0 is counted (N D_0 ). If thisnumber is greater than a predef<strong>in</strong>ed threshold (N th ), a guiltydecision will be made. Similar to the BAD method, the greater N this, the more accurate the guilty decision will be, but the efficiency<strong>of</strong> the method <strong>in</strong> detection <strong>of</strong> all guilties is lost.2.3. Wavelet classifier methodThe new approach considered <strong>in</strong> this study, is a patternrecognition method <strong>based</strong> on wavelet features and a statisticalclassifier. In such <strong>methods</strong>, first some suitable features areextracted from the raw data. These selected features will bethose, which conta<strong>in</strong> suitable <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation about the <strong>in</strong>spectedphenomena.Typically, <strong>ERP</strong> analysis is per<strong>for</strong>med <strong>in</strong> the time doma<strong>in</strong>,whereby the amplitudes and latencies <strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ent peaks <strong>in</strong> theaveraged potentials are measured and correlated to <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mationprocess<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms. This Conventional approach has twomajor drawbacks: (i) <strong>ERP</strong>s are time-vary<strong>in</strong>g signals reflect<strong>in</strong>gthe sum <strong>of</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g neural events dur<strong>in</strong>g stimulus process<strong>in</strong>goperat<strong>in</strong>g on different time scales rang<strong>in</strong>g from milliseconds toseconds. Various procedures have been employed to separatefunctionally mean<strong>in</strong>gful events that partly or completely overlap<strong>in</strong> time, however, the reliable identification <strong>of</strong> these components<strong>in</strong> <strong>ERP</strong> wave<strong>for</strong>m still rema<strong>in</strong>s as a problem. (ii) Analysis<strong>in</strong> the frequency doma<strong>in</strong> has revealed that EEG/<strong>ERP</strong> frequencycomponents <strong>in</strong> different frequency ranges (delta, theta, alpha,beta, gamma) are functionally related to <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation process<strong>in</strong>gand behavior (Basar et al., 2001a,b; Herrmann and Knight,2001; Basar and Schurmann, 2001). However, the Fouriertrans<strong>for</strong>m (FT) <strong>of</strong> <strong>ERP</strong> lacks the <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation about the timelocalization <strong>of</strong> transient neural events. There<strong>for</strong>e, efficient algorithms<strong>for</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g a signal <strong>in</strong> a time–frequency plane areextremely important <strong>in</strong> extraction and analysis <strong>of</strong> these functionalcomponents.The wavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m is one such algorithm, which can beused to represent the EEG/<strong>ERP</strong> signal <strong>in</strong> both time and frequency(Akay, 1997; Mallat, 1999; Unser and Aldroubi, 1996;Yordanova et al., 2002). The wavelet representations provideprecise measurements <strong>of</strong> when and to what degree transientevents occur <strong>in</strong> a neuroelectric wave<strong>for</strong>m and <strong>of</strong> when and howthe frequency content <strong>of</strong> a neuroelectric wave<strong>for</strong>m changesover time (Samar et al., 1999). This is achieved by us<strong>in</strong>gfamilies <strong>of</strong> functions (wavelets) that are generated from a s<strong>in</strong>glefunction (basic wavelet, which can be a smooth and quicklyvanish<strong>in</strong>g oscillation) by operations <strong>of</strong> scal<strong>in</strong>g (stretch<strong>in</strong>g orshr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g the basic wavelet) and translation (mov<strong>in</strong>g the basic


312 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320wavelet to different time positions at any scale withoutchang<strong>in</strong>g its shape).In this way, as shown <strong>in</strong> Fig. 1 and described <strong>in</strong> Samar et al.(1999), the WT per<strong>for</strong>ms a time-frequency decomposition <strong>of</strong> thesignal, i.e. at each resolution level (correspond<strong>in</strong>g roughly to agiven frequency band) and each time <strong>in</strong>terval, the waveletfunction is correlated with the shape <strong>of</strong> the neuroelectric wave<strong>for</strong>mat that position. This correlation, known as a waveletcoefficient, measures how much <strong>of</strong> the wavelet at that resolutionlevel and position is <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the neuroelectric wave<strong>for</strong>m. Thisprocess produces a sequence <strong>of</strong> wavelet coefficients at each level.The sequences from different levels <strong>of</strong> decomposition can bearranged <strong>in</strong> a hierarchical scheme called multi-resolution decomposition(Mallat, 1999). Signals correspond<strong>in</strong>g to different levelscan be reconstructed by apply<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>verse trans<strong>for</strong>m. The ma<strong>in</strong>algorithms and equations <strong>for</strong> the calculation <strong>of</strong> wavelet coefficientsare expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> Appendix A.In summary, the wavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m gives a time-frequencyrepresentation <strong>of</strong> a signal that has three ma<strong>in</strong> advantages overprevious <strong>methods</strong>: (a) reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g both time and frequency <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation,(b) optimal resolution even <strong>in</strong> the time and frequencydoma<strong>in</strong>s, (c) does not require the signal to be stationarity.In the present study, a multi-resolution decomposition(Mallat, 1999) was per<strong>for</strong>med by apply<strong>in</strong>g a decimated discreteWT. In the discrete WT, the parameters <strong>of</strong> scal<strong>in</strong>g and translationhave discrete values, which can also be taken at logarithmic(dyadic) scales. Quadratic B-Spl<strong>in</strong>e functions wereused here as mother wavelets due to their similarity with theevoked response. This function had been used <strong>in</strong> similar studies,and its efficiency <strong>for</strong> <strong>ERP</strong> decomposition and detection hasbeen reported (Quiroga, 2000; Ademoglu et al., 1997; Quirogaet al., 2001; Abootalebi et al., 2004). The filter coefficientsassociated with quadratic B-Spl<strong>in</strong>es are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 1. Thefirst column corresponds to the high-pass filter G used to obta<strong>in</strong>the details and second column the ones <strong>of</strong> low-pass filter H usedto obta<strong>in</strong> the successive approximations. The third and fourthcolumns are the <strong>in</strong>verse filter coefficients used <strong>for</strong> reconstruct<strong>in</strong>gthe signal.Extracted wavelet coefficients can be used as features <strong>of</strong>s<strong>in</strong>gle trial <strong>ERP</strong>s. These features has more <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation thansimple features such as amplitude, latency and correlationwhich are used <strong>in</strong> BAD and BCD analysis and also more<strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation than frequency features which are derived fromspectral analysis.After the feature extraction step, a statistical classifier can beused <strong>for</strong> the separation <strong>of</strong> <strong>ERP</strong>s from Guilty subjects and thosefrom <strong>in</strong>nocents. In the present study, l<strong>in</strong>ear discrim<strong>in</strong>ant analysiswas used <strong>for</strong> classification (Fukunaga, 1990). This proceduregenerates a discrim<strong>in</strong>ant function <strong>based</strong> on l<strong>in</strong>earcomb<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>of</strong> the features that provide the best discrim<strong>in</strong>ationbetween two different groups (guilties and <strong>in</strong>nocents). Thefunctions are generated <strong>based</strong> on some cases <strong>for</strong> which groupmembership is known (the tra<strong>in</strong> set); the functions can then beapplied to new cases with known features but unknown groupmembership (the test set). In this method a normal densitydistribution is fitted to each group, with group means andpooled covariance estimated from the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g set. Hence, anyother subject who does not perta<strong>in</strong> to the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g set can beFig. 1. Wavelet decomposition <strong>of</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle trial <strong>ERP</strong>: (a) the wavelet coefficients <strong>in</strong> the A (0.3–4 Hz), B (4–8 Hz), C (8–16 Hz) and D (16–30 Hz) bands; (b) thereconstructed signal <strong>in</strong> these bands.


V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320313Table 1Filter coefficients correspond<strong>in</strong>g to quadratic B-Spl<strong>in</strong>e waveletsk G(k) H (k) G′ (k) H′ (k)−10 0.0016 −0.0039−9 0.0191 −0.0342−8 −0.0050 0.0342−7 −0.0444 0.0793−6 0.0117 −0.0210−5 0.1033 −0.1840−4 −0.0259 0.0498 −0.0021−3 −0.2437 0.4239 0.0604−2 0.0340 −0.1403 −0.3063 0.25−1 0.6552 −0.9004 0.6312 0.750 0.6552 0.9004 −0.6312 0.751 0.0340 0.1403 0.3063 0.252 −0.2437 −0.4239 0.06043 −0.0259 −0.0498 0.00214 0.1033 0.18415 0.0117 0.02106 −0.0444 −0.07937 −0.0050 −0.00908 0.0191 0.03429 0.0016 0.0039G and H are the high-pass and low-pass filters, respectively. H′ and G′ are the<strong>in</strong>verse filters used <strong>for</strong> the reconstruction [23].evaluated <strong>based</strong> on the distribution with higher density at thecorrespond<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> feature space.3. Methods3.1. SubjectsSixty-two subjects (59 male, 3 female) participated <strong>in</strong> thestudy. They were generally undergraduate or postgraduate studentsand all had normal or corrected vision.3.2. Data acquisitionThe electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded us<strong>in</strong>g Ag/AgClelectrodes placed at the Fz (Frontal), Cz (Central) and Pz (Parietal)sites (10–20 <strong>in</strong>ternational system). All sites were referenced tol<strong>in</strong>ked mastoids. Only the results from Pz will be reported here.Electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from sub- and supraorbitalelectrodes (above and below the right eye). The subjects weregrounded at the <strong>for</strong>ehead. Bra<strong>in</strong> electrical activities were amplifiedand digitized at a rate <strong>of</strong> 256 samples per second. Digitized datawere subsequently analyzed <strong>of</strong>fl<strong>in</strong>e us<strong>in</strong>g MATLAB s<strong>of</strong>tware.Prior to data analysis, all data were digitally filtered <strong>in</strong> the 0.3–30 Hz range. This is the frequency range which is used typically <strong>in</strong><strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> studies (Rosenfeld et al., 2004).3.3. ProcedureAll subjects were tra<strong>in</strong>ed and then per<strong>for</strong>med a mock crimescenario. After a tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g phase about the protocol, a box –conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a jewel – was given to the subject. The exam<strong>in</strong>er leftthe room and permitted the subject to per<strong>for</strong>m his role <strong>in</strong> thescenario. In this step the subject could choose and implement one<strong>of</strong> two possible roles (Guilty/Innocent). The guilty subject openedthe box, saw the jewel precisely and imag<strong>in</strong>ed that he/she hasstolen the jewel. The subject was asked to memorize the jeweldetails, so he/she can act as the actual robber. For reassurance, thesubject was asked to write the detail <strong>of</strong> the object on a piece <strong>of</strong>paper and deliver it after the test. The <strong>in</strong>nocent subject has neveropened the box and thus has no <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation about the object. Thisprotocol is a s<strong>in</strong>gle-probe protocol similar to the protocols whichwere usually used by Rosenfeld et al. (2004).Then the exam<strong>in</strong>er returned to the room and executed the<strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> <strong>for</strong> the knowledge <strong>of</strong> the scenario. After theattachment <strong>of</strong> electrodes and start<strong>in</strong>g the record<strong>in</strong>g, stimuliconsist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle pictures <strong>of</strong> five pieces <strong>of</strong> jewelry, werepresented randomly and successively on a monitor to thesubject. Each item rema<strong>in</strong>ed 1.1 s on the screen, <strong>of</strong> which 1 swere used <strong>for</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g. The <strong>in</strong>ter-stimulus <strong>in</strong>terval was 2 s.Dur<strong>in</strong>g the test, pictures <strong>of</strong> five items (<strong>for</strong> example: r<strong>in</strong>g,wristwatch, necklace, earr<strong>in</strong>g and hairp<strong>in</strong>) were randomlypresented one at a time (each with 30 iterations). These objectsconta<strong>in</strong>ed one target, one probe and three irrelevants. The probewas the object <strong>in</strong> the box. The target was another object, whichhad been previously presented to the subject <strong>in</strong> the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gphase at the start <strong>of</strong> the protocol.One push button was given to each hand <strong>of</strong> the subject, rightclick as “YES” and left click as “NO”. The subjects were askedto reply to the items by press<strong>in</strong>g one <strong>of</strong> the two buttons; “YES”<strong>for</strong> familiar items and “NO” <strong>for</strong> unknowns. This task hadalready been practiced dur<strong>in</strong>g tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g phase. The subjects alsowere <strong>in</strong>structed to try to hide their <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation about the probeitem <strong>in</strong> the guilty condition and to behave mentally as <strong>in</strong>nocent.Any covert action was permitted, provid<strong>in</strong>g that it did notdisarrange the totality <strong>of</strong> the test and was not observable to theexam<strong>in</strong>er either by visual <strong>in</strong>spection <strong>of</strong> the subject or from therecorded signal. Hence, actions such as voluntary bl<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g orbody movement detected by the exam<strong>in</strong>er were not permitted.Indeed, all subjects were supposed to press “YES” <strong>for</strong> targetsand “NO” <strong>for</strong> probes and irrelevants. Thus, guilties and<strong>in</strong>nocents, both replied honestly to targets and irrelevants; but<strong>for</strong> probes, the guilty subjects replied falsely while the <strong>in</strong>nocentsanswered honestly. Subjects with more than 10% error <strong>in</strong>correct click<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong> all stimuli were removed from analysis.Each subject participated <strong>in</strong> two experiments <strong>in</strong> which theboxes and all displayed jewels were different. The jewel <strong>in</strong> the boxwas a gold co<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> the first experiment and a hairp<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> the secondexperiment. Subjects were <strong>in</strong>structed to select a guilty role <strong>in</strong> oneexperiment and an <strong>in</strong>nocent role <strong>in</strong> the other, by their choice.Signals from these two experiments were analyzed <strong>in</strong>dependently.There<strong>for</strong>e with 62 subjects, a total number <strong>of</strong> 124 tests wereper<strong>for</strong>med. 32 subjects chose the <strong>in</strong>nocent role and 30 subjectschose the guilty role <strong>in</strong> their first experiment. A few test resultswere removed due to misdo<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> protocol or <strong>in</strong>appropriatelyrecorded signals. F<strong>in</strong>ally, 59 guilty cases and 51 <strong>in</strong>nocent caseswere chosen to be used <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigations.3.4. Data analysisAfter the filter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> signals, each cont<strong>in</strong>uous record wasseparated to s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps accord<strong>in</strong>g to the known times <strong>of</strong>


314 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320stimulus presentation. EOG data were checked <strong>for</strong> bl<strong>in</strong>k artifactby visual <strong>in</strong>spection and sweeps with bl<strong>in</strong>k artifacts wereremoved. Then BAD, BCD and wavelet analyses were appliedon the signals and the detection rates <strong>of</strong> them were assessed.3.4.1. BAD analysisIn BAD, <strong>P300</strong> amplitude was measured by the Peak–Peak(P–P) method. The algorithm searches with<strong>in</strong> a w<strong>in</strong>dow from400 to 900 ms after-stimulus <strong>for</strong> the maximally positivesegment average <strong>of</strong> 100 ms. The midpo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>of</strong> the maximumpositivity def<strong>in</strong>es <strong>P300</strong> latency. Then the algorithm searchesfrom <strong>P300</strong> latency to the end <strong>of</strong> sweep <strong>for</strong> the maximum 100 msnegativity. The difference between the maximum positivity andnegativity def<strong>in</strong>es the P–P measure. In some researches theBasel<strong>in</strong>e–Peak (B–P) amplitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong> has been used, but it isrepeatedly shown that us<strong>in</strong>g the BAD method, P–P is better<strong>in</strong>dex than B–P <strong>for</strong> diagnosis <strong>of</strong> guilt vs. <strong>in</strong>nocence <strong>in</strong> detection<strong>of</strong> deception (Sosk<strong>in</strong>s et al., 2001).3.4.2. BCD analysisIn BCD, be<strong>for</strong>e comput<strong>in</strong>g the cross correlation coefficients,a time w<strong>in</strong>dow was applied on s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps between 300 and900 ms after-stimulus, and the correlation <strong>of</strong> sweeps were onlynoticed <strong>in</strong> this time limits, because we expected that the <strong>P300</strong>appeared only <strong>in</strong> this region. In this study, only the correlationcoefficient at lag=0 was considered.3.4.3. Wavelet analysis3.4.3.1. Signal decomposition. The data were decomposed<strong>in</strong>to five-octaves us<strong>in</strong>g the wavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m. Six sets <strong>of</strong>coefficients (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g residual scale) with<strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gfrequency bands were obta<strong>in</strong>ed; 0–4 Hz,4–8 Hz,8–16 Hz,16–32 Hz, 32–64 Hz and 64–128 Hz. The coefficients <strong>in</strong> each setare concerned with sequential time bands between 0 and 1000 ms.Because <strong>of</strong> previous filter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the signal, coefficients with<strong>in</strong> 30–128 Hz and 0–0.3 Hz ranges had not any useful <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation. Butother coefficients represent the signal <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation <strong>in</strong> fourfrequency bands: A, 0.3–4 Hz;B,4–8 Hz; C, 8–16 Hz; and D,16–30 Hz. Fig. 1 shows the decomposition and reconstruction <strong>of</strong>a s<strong>in</strong>gle trial <strong>ERP</strong> <strong>in</strong>to five octaves, us<strong>in</strong>g the quadratic B-Spl<strong>in</strong>ewavelet. As shown <strong>in</strong> Fig. 1(a), each octave conta<strong>in</strong>ed the number<strong>of</strong> coefficients necessary to provide the relevant time resolution<strong>for</strong> that frequency range. The <strong>in</strong>terpolation <strong>of</strong> coefficients byquadratic spl<strong>in</strong>e functions was used to reconstruct the analyzedsignal with<strong>in</strong> the particular band <strong>in</strong> Fig 1(b). In the present study,8 coefficients <strong>of</strong> band A, 8 coefficients <strong>of</strong> band B and 16coefficients <strong>of</strong> band C were obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>for</strong> the post-stimulus epoch.The wavelet coefficients are designated with a lettercorrespond<strong>in</strong>g to each frequency band (A <strong>for</strong> 0.3–4 Hz,B<strong>for</strong>4–8Hz,andC<strong>for</strong>8–16 Hz) together with the coefficient numberand two other numbers <strong>in</strong> brackets represent<strong>in</strong>g the approximatetime w<strong>in</strong>dow <strong>in</strong> millisecond (<strong>for</strong> example, A5 (450–580)).3.4.3.2. Statistical analysis. For statistical analysis, theselected wavelet coefficients (8 A-band, 8 B-band and 16 C-band coefficients) <strong>for</strong> all sweeps <strong>of</strong> all subjects were saved <strong>in</strong> afile and fed <strong>in</strong>to the SPSS program. There were totally 2508Target, 7651 Irrelevant, and 2552 Probe (1371 guilty+1181<strong>in</strong>nocent) s<strong>in</strong>gle sweeps. To determ<strong>in</strong>e the most relevant timefrequency components differ<strong>in</strong>g between probes from guiltysubjects (G-probes) and probes from <strong>in</strong>nocents (I-probes), thewavelet coefficients <strong>of</strong> probe sweeps were subjected to <strong>in</strong>dependent-samplet-test, with the subject type (Guilty/Innocent) asgroup<strong>in</strong>g variable.3.4.3.3. Classification. Coefficients <strong>of</strong> probe stimuli weresubjected to discrim<strong>in</strong>ant analysis. This method was used t<strong>of</strong><strong>in</strong>d a l<strong>in</strong>ear comb<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> coefficients provides the bestdiscrim<strong>in</strong>ation between G-probes and I-probes. Per<strong>for</strong>mance <strong>of</strong>this algorithm was estimated us<strong>in</strong>g a leave-one-out jackknif<strong>in</strong>gmethod. For each subject, the probe sweeps <strong>of</strong> other subjectswith their real labels (G-probe/I-probe) were used as tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdata, and then the tra<strong>in</strong>ed classifier applied on the probe sweeps<strong>of</strong> the given subject. Then the number <strong>of</strong> sweeps labeled as G-probe were counted and saved as N G . N G is an <strong>in</strong>dex <strong>of</strong> guilt<strong>for</strong> each subject. Similar to BAD and BCD, if the N G wasgreater than a predef<strong>in</strong>ed threshold (N th ), a guilty decision wasmade.4. Results4.1. Time doma<strong>in</strong> analysisIn all descriptions <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong> to follow, only the results at sitePz was noted, s<strong>in</strong>ce Pz is the site where <strong>P300</strong> is usually reportedto be maximal and s<strong>in</strong>ce the analytic procedures (below) wereper<strong>for</strong>med on Pz data only.Fig. 2 below shows grand averages <strong>in</strong> guilty group <strong>for</strong> superimposedprobe, target and irrelevant responses. Also, Fig. 3shows superimposed <strong>ERP</strong>s to P, T, and I items <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>nocentgroup. The responses were as predicted. As can be seen <strong>in</strong> thefigures, a large <strong>P300</strong> was elicited by the target stimuli but not bythe irrelevant stimuli. The probes elicited a <strong>P300</strong> when theywere relevant to subject's “crime”, i.e. <strong>in</strong> guilty condition. Avery small <strong>P300</strong>, if any, was elicited by probes when the subjectwas “<strong>in</strong>nocent”.Fig. 2. Superimposed grand averaged P, T and I responses <strong>in</strong> guilty group.


V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320315Fig. 3. Superimposed grand averaged P, T and I responses <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>nocent group.4.2. BAD methodFig. 4(a) displays the N D_0 parameter as the output <strong>of</strong> BADmethod <strong>for</strong> all observed subjects. Solid circles belong to guiltysubjects and empty circles to <strong>in</strong>nocents. It was expected thatsolid circles would have large positive measures and <strong>in</strong> contrastempty circles would have small measures. If this happened, asuitable threshold could easily discrim<strong>in</strong>ate the groups. But <strong>in</strong>real state, the groups have overlaps and each selected thresholdresults <strong>in</strong> some misclassification. For better <strong>in</strong>spection <strong>of</strong> theoverlaps, histograms <strong>of</strong> these measures <strong>in</strong> two groups are drawn<strong>in</strong> Fig. 4(b). Statistical analysis reveals that N D_0 is significantlygreater <strong>in</strong> the guilty group (t=7.3, p


316 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320Fig. 5. BCD method: (a) N D_0 parameter <strong>for</strong> all subjects; (b) superimposed histograms <strong>of</strong> N D_0 measures <strong>in</strong> guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent groups; (c) superimposed cumulativehistograms <strong>of</strong> N D_0 measures <strong>in</strong> guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent groups.threshold that can give the best detection <strong>in</strong> all subjects is near46%, which results <strong>in</strong> 80% correct detection <strong>of</strong> two groups.4.4. Wavelet analysisAverages <strong>of</strong> 32 def<strong>in</strong>ed features correspond<strong>in</strong>g to probe stimuli<strong>in</strong> all guilty and all <strong>in</strong>nocent subjects are drawn <strong>in</strong> Fig. 6. Thisdiagram reveals the difference <strong>of</strong> each features' average <strong>in</strong> twogroups, But to determ<strong>in</strong>e the significance level <strong>of</strong> this difference,the t-test was applied on data. Table 2 displays the t-values andsignificance levels (p-values) measured <strong>for</strong> difference betweenguilties' and <strong>in</strong>nocents' correspond<strong>in</strong>g coefficients. Accord<strong>in</strong>g tothese parameters, 13 <strong>of</strong> 32 def<strong>in</strong>ed features have significant differencebetween two groups (p-value


V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320317Table 2Results <strong>of</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g probe response between guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent group us<strong>in</strong>gstatistical t-test on wavelet featuresFeature no. Feature name t-value p-value1 A1 (0–125)⁎ 4.21 0.0002 A2 (125–250)⁎ 5.72 0.0003 A3 (250–375) −0.01 0.9934 A4 (375–500)⁎ −11.63 0.0005 A5 (500–625)⁎ −15.21 0.0006 A6 (625–750)⁎ −5.16 0.0007 A7 (750–875)⁎ 2.08 0.0378 A8 (875–1000)⁎ 2.56 0.0109 B1 (0–125) 0.59 0.55510 B2 (125–250) 1.02 0.31011 B3 (250–375) −1.42 0.15512 B4 (375–500)⁎ 3.32 0.00113 B5 (500–625) −0.12 0.90514 B6 (625–750) 0.27 0.78515 B7 (750–875) 1.07 0.28716 B8 (875–1000)⁎ 2.08 0.03817 C1 (0–62)⁎ 2.11 0.03418 C2 (63–125)⁎ −3.43 0.00119 C3 (125–187) 0.59 0.55520 C4 (188–250) 0.75 0.45621 C5 (250–312) 1.47 0.14122 C6 (313–375)⁎ −2.12 0.03423 C7 (375–437) 0.72 0.47224 C8 (438–500) 0.93 0.35025 C9 (500–562) −1.10 0.27326 C10 (563–625) −1.12 0.26127 C11 (625–687) 1.63 0.10228 C12 (688–750) −1.91 0.05629 C13 (750–812) −1.32 0.18630 C14 (813–875) 1.26 0.20831 C15 (875–937) 0.47 0.63932 C16 (938–1000)⁎ 3.80 0.000* p-value


318 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320Fig. 7. Wavelet classifier: (a) N G parameter <strong>for</strong> all subjects; (b) superimposed histograms <strong>of</strong> N G measures <strong>in</strong> guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent groups; (c) superimposed cumulativehistograms <strong>of</strong> N G measures <strong>in</strong> guilty and <strong>in</strong>nocent groups.obta<strong>in</strong> better per<strong>for</strong>mances us<strong>in</strong>g maximum correlation <strong>in</strong> all lags,<strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> a correlation only at lag=0. This modification wasapplied to the BCD method and the results were computed aga<strong>in</strong>.The total accuracy <strong>of</strong> detection over all subjects was 80% and nosignificant change could be seen <strong>in</strong> the per<strong>for</strong>mance after thismodification. It is possible that this modification, <strong>in</strong> addition toour expected improvement, has improved probe–irrelevantcorrelation more than probe–target correlation and thus lead<strong>in</strong>gto some false negatives.The detection accuracies <strong>of</strong> applied <strong>methods</strong> are lower thanwhat some other groups have reported. However, it must be notedthat the implemented <strong>methods</strong> were applied on different datasets.The obta<strong>in</strong>ed f<strong>in</strong>al accuracy depends on the subject type, usedprotocol, and the process<strong>in</strong>g algorithm. So, the process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>methods</strong>can be compared fairly only when the subjects and protocols aresimilar. The ma<strong>in</strong> difference between this study and some previousstudies is the number <strong>of</strong> irrelevant questions used. There are threeirrelevant questions <strong>in</strong> this study, whereas previous studies usedtypically four irrelevant questions (Rosenfeld et al., 2004; Farwelland Donch<strong>in</strong>, 1991). Smaller number <strong>of</strong> irrelevant questions<strong>in</strong>creases the probe probability and thus decreases the amplitude <strong>of</strong><strong>P300</strong> peak <strong>in</strong> probe question and thus the detection rate.It should also be noted that like conventional polygraphy,<strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> <strong>GKT</strong> is potentially vulnerable to countermeasures.(Countermeasures are covert actions taken by the subjects so asFig. 8. Error-plots <strong>of</strong> the implemented <strong>methods</strong>.Table 3Results <strong>of</strong> two statistical <strong>comparison</strong>s between the implemented <strong>methods</strong>Compared <strong>methods</strong>Paired t-test (on thearea under error-plotcurve)McNemar's test (onthe detection rate)t-value p-value Z p-valueBAD – BCD 2.896 0.009 −1.04 0.15BAD – Wavelet analysis −0.134 0.448 −0.83 0.203BCD – Wavelet analysis \2.646 0.013 0.20 0.427


V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320319Fig. 9. Recursive implementation <strong>of</strong> the multiresolution decomposition and reconstruction. H and G are the low and high-pass filters, respectively, and H′ and G′ the<strong>in</strong>verse filters used <strong>for</strong> the reconstruction. Downward arrows mean decimation by 2 and upward arrows upsampl<strong>in</strong>g.prevent detection by the <strong>GKT</strong> (Honts and Amato, 2002).) Thisstudy did not focus on any specific countermeasure and thesubjects were permitted to use any covert action, provid<strong>in</strong>g that itdid not disarrange the totality <strong>of</strong> the test and was not observable tothe exam<strong>in</strong>er. This matter is extensively <strong>in</strong>vestigated by someresearchers (Rosenfeld et al., 2004; Sasaki et al., 2001). The result<strong>of</strong> such researches can help <strong>in</strong> the design <strong>of</strong> more reliableparadigms <strong>for</strong> <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> lie detection.AcknowledgementThis research was supported by the research center <strong>of</strong><strong>in</strong>telligent signal process<strong>in</strong>g (RCISP). The authors would like tothank them and also all those who took part <strong>in</strong> this study.Appendix A. Wavelet trans<strong>for</strong>mWavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m is def<strong>in</strong>ed as the convolution between thesignal x(t) and the wavelet function ψ a,b (t) W w X ða;bÞ ¼ xðtÞjw a;b ðtÞð1Þwhere ψ a,b (t) are dilated (contracted) and shifted versions <strong>of</strong> aunique wavelet function ψ(t)w a;b ðÞ¼jaj t−1=2 w t−b ð2Þa(a, b are the scale and translation parameters, respectively). Thewavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m gives a decomposition <strong>of</strong> x(t) <strong>in</strong> differentscales tend<strong>in</strong>g to be maximum at those scales and time locationswhere the wavelet best resembles x(t). Moreover, Eq. (1) can be<strong>in</strong>verted, thus giv<strong>in</strong>g the reconstruction <strong>of</strong> x(t).The wavelet trans<strong>for</strong>m maps a signal <strong>of</strong> one <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable t onto a function <strong>of</strong> two <strong>in</strong>dependent variables a, b. Thisprocedure is redundant and not efficient <strong>for</strong> algorithm implementations.In consequence, it is more practical to def<strong>in</strong>e the wavelettrans<strong>for</strong>m only at discrete scales a and discrete times b by choos<strong>in</strong>gthe set <strong>of</strong> parameters {a j =2 −j ; b jk =2 −j k}, with <strong>in</strong>tegers j, k.Contracted versions <strong>of</strong> the wavelet function will match thehigh frequency components <strong>of</strong> the orig<strong>in</strong>al signal and on the otherhand, the dilated versions will match low frequency oscillations.Then, by correlat<strong>in</strong>g the orig<strong>in</strong>al signal with wavelet functions <strong>of</strong>different sizes we can obta<strong>in</strong> the details <strong>of</strong> the signal at differentscales. These correlations with the different wavelet functions canbe arranged <strong>in</strong> a hierarchical scheme called multiresolutiondecomposition (Mallat, 1999). The multiresolution decompositionseparates the signal <strong>in</strong>to ‘details’ at different scales, therema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g part be<strong>in</strong>g a coarser representation <strong>of</strong> the signal called‘approximation’. Moreover, it was shown (Mallat, 1999)thateachdetail (D j ) and approximation signal (A j ) could be obta<strong>in</strong>ed fromthe previous approximation A j−1 via a convolution with high-passand low-pass filters, respectively (see Fig. 9).ReferencesAbootalebi, V., Moradi, M.H., Khalilzadeh, M.A., 2004. Detection <strong>of</strong> the cognitivecomponents <strong>of</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> potentials us<strong>in</strong>g wavelet coefficients. Iranian Journal <strong>of</strong>Biomedical Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g 1 (1), 25–46 (Article <strong>in</strong> Persian).Ademoglu, A., Micheli-Tzanakou, E., Istefanopulos, Y., 1997. Analysis <strong>of</strong> patternreversal visual evoked potentials by spl<strong>in</strong>e wavelets. IEEE Transactions onBiomedical Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g 44, 881–890.Akay, M. (Ed.), 1997. Time Frequency and Wavelets <strong>in</strong> Biomedical SignalProcess<strong>in</strong>g. Wiley-IEEE Press. ISBN: 0-7803-1147-7.Allen, J., Iacono, W.G., Danielson, K.D., 1992. The identification <strong>of</strong> concealedmemories us<strong>in</strong>g the event-related potential and implicit behavioral measures:a methodology <strong>for</strong> prediction <strong>in</strong> the face <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences.Psychophysiology 29, 504–522.Basar, E., Schurmann, M., 2001. Toward new theories <strong>of</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> function and bra<strong>in</strong>dynamics. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 39, 87–89.Basar, E., Basar-Eroglu, C., Karakas, S., Schurmann, M., 2001a. Gamma, alpha,delta, and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes. International Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 39, 241–248.Basar, E., Schurmann, M., Demiralp, T., Basar-Eroglu, C., Ademoglue, A.,2001b. Event-related oscillations are ‘real bra<strong>in</strong> responses’: wavelet analysisand new strategies. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 39, 91–127.Ben-Shakhar, G., Elaad, E., 2002. The guilty knowledge test (<strong>GKT</strong>) as anapplication <strong>of</strong> psychophysiology: future prospects and obstacles. In: Kle<strong>in</strong>er,M. (Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> Polygraph Test<strong>in</strong>g. Academic Press, N.Y., pp. 87–102.Farwell, L.A., Donch<strong>in</strong>, E., 1991. The truth will out: <strong>in</strong>terrogative polygraphy(lie detection) with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 28, 531–547.Fukunaga, J., 1990. Statistical Pattern Recognition, 2nd ed. Academic Press,New York.Ganis, G., Kosslyn, S.M., Stose, S., Thompson, W.L., Yurgelun-Todd, D.A., 2003.Neural correlates <strong>of</strong> different types <strong>of</strong> deception: an fMRI <strong>in</strong>vestigation.Cerebral Cortex 13, 830–836.


320 V. Abootalebi et al. / International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 62 (2006) 309–320Herrmann, C.S., Knight, R.T., 2001. Mechanisms <strong>of</strong> human attention: eventrelatedpotentials and oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews25 (6), 465–476.Honts, C.R., Amato, S.L., 2002. Countermeasures. In: Kle<strong>in</strong>er, M. (Ed.),Handbook <strong>of</strong> Polygraph Test<strong>in</strong>g. Academic Press, N.Y., pp. 251–264.Kozel, F.A., Revell, L.J., Lorberbaum, J.P., Shastri, A., Elhai, J.D., Horner, M.D.,Smith, A., Nahas, Z., Bohn<strong>in</strong>g, D.E., George, M.S., 2004a. A pilot study <strong>of</strong>functional magnetic resonance imag<strong>in</strong>g bra<strong>in</strong> correlates <strong>of</strong> deception <strong>in</strong> healthyyoung men. The Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuropsychiatry and Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Neurosciences 16,295–305.Kozel, F.A., Padgett, T.M., George, M.S., 2004b. A replication study <strong>of</strong> theneural correlates <strong>of</strong> deception. Behavioral Neuroscience 118, 852–856.Langleben, D.D., Loughead, J.W., Bilker, W.B., Ruparel, K., Childress, A.R.,Busch, S.I., Gur, R.C., 2005. Tell<strong>in</strong>g truth from lie <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual subjectswith fast event-related fMRI. Human Bra<strong>in</strong> Mapp<strong>in</strong>g 26, 262–272.Lee, T.M.C., Liu, H.L., Tan, L.H., Chan, C.C.H., Mahankali, S., Feng, C.M.,Hou, J., Fox, P.T., Gao, J.H., 2002. Lie detection by functional magneticresonance imag<strong>in</strong>g. Human Bra<strong>in</strong> Mapp<strong>in</strong>g 15 (3), 157–164.Mallat, S., 1999. A Wavelet Tour <strong>of</strong> Signal Process<strong>in</strong>g, 2nd ed. Academic Press,San Diego.Matte, J.A., 1996. Forensic Psychophysiology Us<strong>in</strong>g the Polygraph: ScientificTruth Verification – Lie Detection. J.A.M Publications, Williamsville, NewYork.Mertens, R., Allen, J., Culp, N., Craw<strong>for</strong>d, L., 2003. The detection <strong>of</strong> deceptionus<strong>in</strong>g event-related potentials <strong>in</strong> a highly realistic mock crime scenario.Psychophysiology 40, S60 (abstract).Miyake, Y., Mizutanti, M., Yamahura, T., 1993. Event related potentials as an<strong>in</strong>dicator <strong>of</strong> detect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation <strong>in</strong> field polygraph exam<strong>in</strong>ations. Polygraph22, 131–149.National Research Council, 2002. The Polygraph and Lie Detection. NationalAcademies Press, Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.Nunez, J.M., Casey, B.J., Egner, T., Hare, T., Hirsch, J., 2005. Intentional falserespond<strong>in</strong>g shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitivecontrol. Neuroimage 25, 267–277.Phan, K.L., Magalhaes, A., Ziemlewicz, T.J., Fitzgerald, D.A., Green, C., Smith,W., 2005. Neural correlates <strong>of</strong> tell<strong>in</strong>g lies: a functional magnetic resonanceimag<strong>in</strong>g study at 4 Tesla. Academic Radiology 12, 164–172.Polich, J., 2000. <strong>P300</strong> <strong>in</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical applications, In: NiederMeyer, E., Lopes DaSilva, F. (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, Cl<strong>in</strong>icalApplications, and Related Fields, 4th ed. Lipp<strong>in</strong>cott Williams and Wilk<strong>in</strong>s,Baltimore, Maryland, USA.Quiroga, R.Q., 2000. Obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle stimulus evoked potentials with waveletdenois<strong>in</strong>g. Physica D 145, 278–292.Quiroga, R.Q., Sakowitz, O.W., Basar, E., Schurmann, M., 2001. Wavelettrans<strong>for</strong>m <strong>in</strong> the analysis <strong>of</strong> the frequency composition <strong>of</strong> evoked potentials.Bra<strong>in</strong> Research Protocols 8, 16–24.Rosenfeld, J.P., 2002. Event-related potentials <strong>in</strong> the detection <strong>of</strong> deception,mal<strong>in</strong>ger<strong>in</strong>g, and false memories. In: Kle<strong>in</strong>er, M. (Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong>Polygraph Test<strong>in</strong>g. Academic Press, N.Y., pp. 265–286.Rosenfeld, J.P., 2005. ‘Bra<strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>gerpr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g’: a critical analysis. ScientificReview <strong>of</strong> Mental Health Practice 4, 20–37.Rosenfeld, J.P., Sosk<strong>in</strong>s, M., Bosh, G., Ryan, A., 2004. Simple, effective countermeasuresto <strong>P300</strong>-<strong>based</strong> tests <strong>of</strong> detection <strong>of</strong> concealed <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation. Psychophysiology41 (2), 205–219.Samar, V.J., Bopardikar, A., Rao, R., Swartz, K., 1999. Wavelet analysis <strong>of</strong>neuroelectric wave<strong>for</strong>ms: a conceptual tutorial. Bra<strong>in</strong> Language 66, 7–60.Sasaki, M., Hira, S., Matsuda, T., 2001. Effects <strong>of</strong> a mental countermeasure onthe physiological detection <strong>of</strong> deception us<strong>in</strong>g event related bra<strong>in</strong> potentials.Sh<strong>in</strong>rigaku Kenkyu 72 (4), 322–328 (Article <strong>in</strong> Japanese).Sosk<strong>in</strong>s, M., Rosenfeld, J.P., Niendam, T., 2001. The case <strong>for</strong> peak-to-peakmeasurement <strong>of</strong> <strong>P300</strong> recorded at .3 Hz high pass filter sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> detection<strong>of</strong> deception. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychophysiology 40, 173–180.Unser, M., Aldroubi, A., 1996. A review <strong>of</strong> wavelets <strong>in</strong> biomedical applications.Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> the IEEE 84 (4), 626–638.Wasserman, S., Bockenholt, U., 1989. Bootstrapp<strong>in</strong>g: applications to psychophysiology.Psychophysiology 26, 208–221.Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Rosso, O.A., Schurmann, M., Sakowitz, O.W., Ozgoren,M., Basar, E., 2002. Wavelet entropy analysis <strong>of</strong> event-related potentials<strong>in</strong>dicates modality-<strong>in</strong>dependent theta dom<strong>in</strong>ance. Journal <strong>of</strong> NeuroscienceMethods 117, 99–109.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!