13.07.2015 Views

Investigating the Determinants of Perceived Procedural Fairness in ...

Investigating the Determinants of Perceived Procedural Fairness in ...

Investigating the Determinants of Perceived Procedural Fairness in ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013<strong>Investigat<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>Determ<strong>in</strong>ants</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Perceived</strong><strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong> <strong>in</strong>Performance EvaluationMahfud Sholih<strong>in</strong>*Richard Pike**AbstractPrevious management account<strong>in</strong>g andcontrol system studies have exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong>impact <strong>of</strong> perceived procedural fairness onmanagers’ attitudes and behaviour.However, relatively little research hasbeen conducted on <strong>the</strong> factors whichaffect perceived procedural fairness; and<strong>the</strong>ir results are <strong>in</strong>conclusive. Theobjective <strong>of</strong> this paper is to <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>the</strong>ma<strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> perceivedprocedural fairness.Us<strong>in</strong>g a sample <strong>of</strong> managers from threemajor organisations with headquarters <strong>in</strong>Europe, this study f<strong>in</strong>ds that perceivedprocedural fairness is affected by goalrelatedvariables <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong>participation <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g performancetargets, <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k,and <strong>the</strong> specificity <strong>of</strong> goals to be achievedby managers; but is not affected bywhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> performance measures usedto evaluate performance are primarilyf<strong>in</strong>ancial or nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial.KeywordsGoal-Atta<strong>in</strong>ment-Reward L<strong>in</strong>kGoal SpecificityPerformance Measurement and Evaluation<strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong>Target-Sett<strong>in</strong>g Participation* Gadjah Mada University, Indonesia**Bradford University, UKIntroductionThe performance measurement, evaluation,and reward system (PMERS) is an importantcomponent <strong>of</strong> Management Control Systems(MCS). The importance <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>design <strong>of</strong> PMERS has been recognised <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>normative literature on MCS. Anthony andGov<strong>in</strong>darajan (1998, p. 556) argue that topmanagement should be aware that “objectives,goals and standards are likely to providestrong <strong>in</strong>centives only if managers perceive<strong>the</strong>m as fair” (emphasis added). Similarly,Kaplan and Atk<strong>in</strong>son (1998, p. 682) highlight<strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>in</strong> PMERS asfollows:“There are important behaviouralconsiderations that <strong>the</strong> performancemeasurement system must reflect. First andabove all, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual must believe that<strong>the</strong> system is fair … Absent this belief, <strong>the</strong>motivational potential <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>centivecompensation will be lost.” (emphasisadded).Previous empirical studies <strong>in</strong> managementaccount<strong>in</strong>g and control systems (e.g.L<strong>in</strong>dquist, 1995; Libby, 1999; Libby, 2001;Wentzel, 2002; Lau and Tan, 2006; Sholih<strong>in</strong>and Pike, 2009) f<strong>in</strong>d that fairness is negativelyassociated with dysfunctional attitudes andbehaviour, such as job-related tension andbudgetary slack, and enhances functionalbehaviour, such as trust and organisationalcommitment, and positively associated withoutcomes such as task satisfaction, jobsatisfaction and performance.Consider<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> perceivedfairness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> design <strong>of</strong> PMERS, it isimportant to identify and understand <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>factors affect<strong>in</strong>g such perceptions. However,<strong>the</strong> literature reveals that prior studies (e.g.Hopwood, 1972; Otley, 1978; Lau andSholih<strong>in</strong>, 2005; Lau and Moser, 2008;Hartmann, Naranjo-Gil, and Perego, 2009)have yielded <strong>in</strong>conclusive, even contradictory,evidence on <strong>the</strong> particular PMERS elementsaffect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> fairness.Hopwood (1972) exam<strong>in</strong>ed whe<strong>the</strong>rsupervisory evaluative style (how superiorsevaluate <strong>the</strong> performance <strong>of</strong> subord<strong>in</strong>ates)affects <strong>the</strong> subord<strong>in</strong>ates’ attitudes, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g29


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013subord<strong>in</strong>ates’ perception <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>of</strong>performance evaluation processes. In do<strong>in</strong>g so,he constructed three categories <strong>of</strong> performanceevaluation style: Budget Constra<strong>in</strong>ed (BC)style (budgetary <strong>in</strong>formation is used <strong>in</strong> a rigidmanner), Pr<strong>of</strong>it Conscious (PC) style(budgetary <strong>in</strong>formation is used <strong>in</strong> a moreflexible manner, and Non-account<strong>in</strong>g (NA)style (budgetary <strong>in</strong>formation is seen as be<strong>in</strong>g<strong>of</strong> secondary importance). His results<strong>in</strong>dicated that BC style is perceived as less fairthan <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r styles. Otley (1978) replicatedHopwood’s (1972) study and found, contraryto Hopwood’s study, that supervisoryevaluative style is not significantly associatedwith perception <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>of</strong> performanceevaluation processes.Lau and Moser (2008) exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong>nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial performance measures onsubord<strong>in</strong>ates’ perception <strong>of</strong> proceduralfairness, argu<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong>ir use may lead tohigher perceptions <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>in</strong> performanceevaluation processes (procedural fairness).They also argue that nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures aremore consistent with <strong>the</strong> procedural fairnessrules/criteria proposed by Leventhal (1980).Their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs show a positive associationbetween nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measure usage andprocedural fairness.Lau and Sholih<strong>in</strong> (2005) contended and foundthat both nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial and f<strong>in</strong>ancial measuresare positively associated with proceduralfairness, although <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ancialmeasures on fairness was higher compared tothat <strong>of</strong> nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures. This is <strong>in</strong>contrast with Hopwood’s (1972) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that<strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> Non-Account<strong>in</strong>g style on fairnessis significantly higher than that <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Budget-Constra<strong>in</strong>ed style.A more recent study by Hartman et al. (2009)argues that <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> subjective performancemeasures is positively associated withprocedural fairness. However, <strong>the</strong>ir f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsdo not support this argument. Indeed,procedural fairness is associated with <strong>the</strong> use<strong>of</strong> objective performance measures.Hartmann and Slapnicar (2009) argue thatprocedural fairness is not affected byperformance measures or metrics used toevaluate subord<strong>in</strong>ates’ performance but isaffected by <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> formality <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> use<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> performance evaluation system. Theirempirical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, however, show nosignificant association between formality andprocedural fairness.The forego<strong>in</strong>g summary <strong>of</strong> relevant priorstudies present a confus<strong>in</strong>g, evencontradictory, picture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> antecedents <strong>of</strong>procedural fairness <strong>in</strong> PMERS. Given <strong>the</strong>importance <strong>of</strong> this variable, as identifiedearlier by Anthony and Gov<strong>in</strong>darajan (1998)and Kaplan and Atk<strong>in</strong>son (1998), <strong>the</strong> desire toclarify and better understand <strong>the</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ants<strong>of</strong> perceived procedural fairness providesstrong motivation for this study. In this paper,we <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>the</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ants, orantecedents, <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness perception<strong>in</strong> performance measurement, evaluation andreward systems.This study is different from previous studies,particularly Sholih<strong>in</strong> and Pike (2009), <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>follow<strong>in</strong>g ways. First, whilst Sholih<strong>in</strong> and Pike(2009) <strong>in</strong>vestigates <strong>the</strong> consequences <strong>of</strong>procedural fairness, this study <strong>in</strong>vestigates <strong>the</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness. Second,<strong>the</strong> variables exam<strong>in</strong>ed by Sholih<strong>in</strong> and Pike(2009) are procedural fairness, distributivefairness, trust, organisational commitment, jobsatisfaction, and managerial performance. Thevariables exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> this study areprocedural fairness, participation <strong>in</strong> targetsett<strong>in</strong>g, transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k, goal specificity, f<strong>in</strong>ancialmeasures, and nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures. Third,whilst Sholih<strong>in</strong> and Pike (2009) employsorganisational justice <strong>the</strong>ory, <strong>in</strong> this study weuse organisational justice and goal-sett<strong>in</strong>g<strong>the</strong>ories.Draw<strong>in</strong>g on organisational justice literature(e.g. L<strong>in</strong>d and Tyler, 1988; Leventhal, 1980)and goal-sett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ory (Locke and Latham,1990), this study argues that a manager’sperception <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness is <strong>in</strong>fluencedby goal-related variables <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong>participation <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g performance targets,<strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k, and <strong>the</strong>specificity <strong>of</strong> goals to be achieved bymanagers; but is not affected by <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong>performance measures used to evaluateperformance, whe<strong>the</strong>r f<strong>in</strong>ancial (objective) ornonf<strong>in</strong>ancial (subjective) measures, as arguedby Lau and Sholih<strong>in</strong> (2005), Lau and Moser(2008), and Hartmann et al., (2009).The proposition that procedural fairness isdeterm<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> participation <strong>in</strong>target sett<strong>in</strong>g draws on <strong>the</strong> literature on30


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013procedural justice, which has consistentlyfound that disputants perceive <strong>the</strong> procedure asfair if <strong>the</strong>y have process control (i.e. sufficientopportunity to present <strong>the</strong>ir case), <strong>of</strong>tenreferred to as ‘voice’ (Thibaut and Walker,1975; Folger, 1977; L<strong>in</strong>d and Tyler, 1988). Inaddition, participation is consistent withLeventhal’s (1980) procedural fairness rules.The goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k is proposedas an antecedent <strong>of</strong> procedural fairnessbecause clarity and transparency are outl<strong>in</strong>edby Leventhal (1980) as important components<strong>of</strong> procedural fairness rules. In relation to goalspecificity, Lau and Sholih<strong>in</strong> (2005) argue that<strong>the</strong> adoption <strong>of</strong> specific performancemeasures, are more likely to give rise tospecific goals and targets for employees topursue, than where <strong>the</strong>re are no specificperformance measures for performanceevaluation. The existence <strong>of</strong> specific goals andtargets, <strong>in</strong> turn, is likely to affect employeebehaviour because <strong>the</strong> specificity <strong>of</strong> goalsenhances <strong>the</strong> direction and clarity <strong>of</strong> tasks tobe performed and <strong>the</strong> roles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subord<strong>in</strong>ates.The rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper is organised as follows.Section two will discuss literature review andhypo<strong>the</strong>ses development. Section three,describes <strong>the</strong> research method employed <strong>in</strong>this study. Section four will present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsand discussions. This paper will be closed byconclusions, limitation and suggestion forfuture research <strong>in</strong> section five.Literature Review and Hypo<strong>the</strong>sesDevelopmentParticipation and <strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong>In this study, participation is conceptualised asparticipation <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g performance goals ortargets, which <strong>in</strong>clude f<strong>in</strong>ancial andnonf<strong>in</strong>ancial targets. This is a departure frommost previous account<strong>in</strong>g studies whichconceptualise participation more narrowly asbudgetary participation.<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness can be conceptualised as<strong>the</strong> judgments on <strong>the</strong> fairness <strong>of</strong> social normswhich deal with how decisions are made andhow <strong>in</strong>dividuals are treated by authorities ando<strong>the</strong>r parties (L<strong>in</strong>d and Tyler, 1988). However,s<strong>in</strong>ce this study specifically deals with <strong>the</strong>performance measurement, evaluation andreward system, Folger and Konovsky’s (1989)def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness is adopted,namely, <strong>the</strong> perceived fairness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> meansand procedures used to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> amount<strong>of</strong> reward or compensation employees receive,i.e. <strong>the</strong> fairness <strong>of</strong> all aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>organisation’s procedures that are used by <strong>the</strong>superior to evaluate a subord<strong>in</strong>ate’sperformance, to communicate performancefeedback and to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> subord<strong>in</strong>ate’srewards such as promotion and pay <strong>in</strong>creases.Leventhal (1980) contended that <strong>the</strong>perception <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness will beaffected by six rules: consistency, biassuppression, accuracy, correctability,representativeness, and ethicality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>procedures. The consistency rule states that tobe fair a procedure must be appliedconsistently across persons and across time.This implies that all parties have <strong>the</strong> samerights under <strong>the</strong> procedures and are treatedsimilarly, and that <strong>the</strong> procedure is enacted <strong>the</strong>same way each time it is used. The biassuppressionrule stipulates that procedures arefair if <strong>the</strong> decision maker does not have avested <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> any specific decision, and if<strong>the</strong> decision maker is not <strong>in</strong>fluenced by priorbeliefs. The accuracy rule states that to be fairprocedures should be based on as much goodquality<strong>in</strong>formation and <strong>in</strong>formed op<strong>in</strong>ion aspossible, while <strong>the</strong> correctability rule statesthat opportunities must exist to modify andreverse decisions made. Therepresentativeness rule stipulates that “allphases <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> allocative process must reflect<strong>the</strong> basic concerns, values, and outlooks <strong>of</strong>important subgroups <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> population <strong>of</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals affected by <strong>the</strong> allocative process”(Leventhal, 1980, pp.44-45). This rule isclosely related to power shar<strong>in</strong>g andparticipatory decision mak<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>ethicality rule states that to be fair, proceduresmust be compatible with fundamental moraland ethical values.Most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> above requirements for proceduralfairness can be promoted through participation<strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g. For example, participation isconsistent with <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong>representativeness, correctability, accuracy,bias-suppression and ethicality <strong>in</strong> that itpermits subord<strong>in</strong>ates to reflect <strong>the</strong>ir concernsand values, gives opportunity to modifydecisions, can be used as a way <strong>of</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation, provides opportunity forsubord<strong>in</strong>ate managers to correct any<strong>in</strong>appropriate prior beliefs held by <strong>the</strong>irsuperiors, and is consistent with <strong>the</strong> moralvalue that <strong>in</strong>dividuals should have <strong>the</strong>31


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013opportunity to engage <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g goals. It is<strong>the</strong>refore reasonable to propose thatparticipation is positively associated withprocedural fairness. Additionally, L<strong>in</strong>d andTyler (1988, p. 236) argue that “<strong>the</strong>opportunity to exercise voice (participation)constitutes a visible marker <strong>of</strong> groupmembership … mute procedures are seen as… unjust because <strong>the</strong>y appear to deny fullmembership rights to those denied voice”(paren<strong>the</strong>ses added) and “…one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mostpotent determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> procedural fairness<strong>of</strong> a social decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g procedure is <strong>the</strong>extent to which those affected by <strong>the</strong> decisionare allowed to participate <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> decisionmak<strong>in</strong>gprocess through <strong>the</strong> exercise <strong>of</strong>process control or voice” (p. 176). Similarly,Early and Kanfer (1985) argue that s<strong>in</strong>ceparticipation gives opportunity for <strong>in</strong>put, itprovides <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual with perceived masteryor control over a situation; thus participationmay enhance perceived fairness.In a budget<strong>in</strong>g context, Wentzel (2002) studied88 cost centre managers <strong>of</strong> a downsizedhospital <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> US and found that budgetparticipation is positively associated withprocedural fairness. Similar results were foundby Lau and Tan (2006) among managers <strong>of</strong>manufactur<strong>in</strong>g companies <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gapore. Thisstudy <strong>the</strong>refore argues that a positiveassociation holds between participation <strong>in</strong>sett<strong>in</strong>g targets and procedural fairness.Ha1: Participation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g ispositively associated with procedural fairness.Goal-Atta<strong>in</strong>ment-Reward L<strong>in</strong>k and<strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong>The management control systems literaturesuggests that <strong>the</strong> performance evaluation andreward system is an important component <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> management control system designed tomotivate organisational members to performbetter and <strong>in</strong> accordance with <strong>the</strong>organisation’s objectives (Otley, 1999;Merchant and Van der Stede, 2003).Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Emmanuel et al. (1990) andMerchant and Van der Stede (2003), one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>cipal means <strong>of</strong> motivat<strong>in</strong>g subord<strong>in</strong>atestowards effective performance is to l<strong>in</strong>korganisational rewards to <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong>performance achieved. This implies that <strong>the</strong>l<strong>in</strong>k between <strong>the</strong> achievement <strong>of</strong> goals orperformance targets set and rewards should betransparent. A recent empirical study byKom<strong>in</strong>is and Emmanuel (2007), us<strong>in</strong>g asample <strong>of</strong> middle managers <strong>in</strong> a large UKbasedf<strong>in</strong>ancial <strong>in</strong>stitution, found that <strong>the</strong>effect <strong>of</strong> transparency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> performancerewardl<strong>in</strong>k on motivation is <strong>in</strong>direct via <strong>the</strong>value <strong>of</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic rewards. In o<strong>the</strong>r words,Kom<strong>in</strong>is and Emmanuel’s (2007) studysuggests that <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>performance-reward l<strong>in</strong>k on motivation maybe mediated by o<strong>the</strong>r variables, and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>irstudy this was <strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic rewards.Draw<strong>in</strong>g on Emmanuel et al.’s (1990) andMerchant and Van der Stede’s (2003)contentions and extend<strong>in</strong>g Kom<strong>in</strong>is andEmmanuel’s (2007) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, this study arguesthat <strong>the</strong> transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k is likely to enhance motivation viaperceived procedural fairness. This is becausea transparent rewards system should beconsistent, unbiased, and accurate, all <strong>of</strong> whichare characteristics <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness(Leventhal, 1980). Therefore, this studyhypo<strong>the</strong>sises that transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goalatta<strong>in</strong>ment-rewardl<strong>in</strong>k will be positivelyassociated with procedural fairness.Ha2: Transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k will be positively associated withprocedural fairness.Goal Specificity and <strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong>Goal specificity refers to “<strong>the</strong> extent to which<strong>the</strong> goals are clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed by a supervisor”(Fang et al., 2005). In <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong>performance measurement, evaluation andreward systems <strong>the</strong> goals can be f<strong>in</strong>ancial ornonf<strong>in</strong>ancial. Organisations which develop andadopt specific performance measures, whe<strong>the</strong>rf<strong>in</strong>ancial or nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial, for performanceevaluation are more likely to develop specificgoals and targets for employees to pursue,than are organisations which have no specificperformance measures for performanceevaluation. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> prespecifiedgoals is likely to provide clearerunderstand<strong>in</strong>g (goal clarity) for organisationalmembers and <strong>in</strong>dicate how <strong>the</strong>y will beevaluated. In addition, goal specificity andclarity <strong>in</strong>forms employees <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irresponsibilities and performance targets. Ino<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> specific goalswill guide employees <strong>in</strong> decid<strong>in</strong>g where <strong>the</strong>yshould direct <strong>the</strong>ir attention and effort.32


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013In relation to procedural justice, <strong>the</strong> existence<strong>of</strong> goal specificity/clarity is <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with <strong>the</strong>consistency and bias-suppression rules, as aspecific goal is likely to facilitate <strong>the</strong>application <strong>of</strong> procedures consistently acrosstime and persons and be less biased and lesssubjective. In addition, <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong>specific goals <strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>the</strong>re is a standardto follow which provides opportunity formanagers to challenge/rebut evaluation. Anempirical study by Greenberg (1986) withsamples <strong>of</strong> middle managers <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> US foundthat <strong>the</strong> ability to rebut/challenge evaluationand consistent application <strong>of</strong> standards areimportant determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> perceivedprocedural fairness. Hence this studyhypo<strong>the</strong>sises that goal specificity is positivelyassociated with procedural fairness.Ha3: Goal specificity is positively associatedwith procedural fairness.Research MethodData and SampleData used <strong>in</strong> this study were ga<strong>the</strong>red bymeans <strong>of</strong> questionnaire survey <strong>in</strong> three majororganisations, followed by <strong>in</strong>terviews withselected respondents. The organisations wereapproached to participate <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> study because<strong>the</strong>y were lead<strong>in</strong>g companies <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir sectorthat had been operat<strong>in</strong>g PMERS for some timewith vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees <strong>of</strong> satisfaction.CO1 is part <strong>of</strong> a highly centralised globalorganisation operat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a specialist, researchdrivensector whose headquarter is <strong>in</strong>Switzerland. Much <strong>of</strong> its recent performancegrowth is through reduc<strong>in</strong>g complexity, us<strong>in</strong>gfewer suppliers and clos<strong>in</strong>g smaller plants.CO2 and CO3 are f<strong>in</strong>ancial servicesorganisations, provid<strong>in</strong>g specialist mortgageand sav<strong>in</strong>gs products through a large number<strong>of</strong> branches. Their corporate <strong>of</strong>fices are both <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> UK.For <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> fill<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> questionnaire,purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g was employed. Thepr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>in</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g is to get allpossible cases that fit particular criteria(Neuman, 2003). The criteria to be <strong>in</strong>cluded as<strong>the</strong> samples are: 1) managers have beenwork<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> organisations for more thanone year; 2) <strong>the</strong>y have been evaluated by <strong>the</strong>irsuperiors; and 3) <strong>the</strong>y have received <strong>the</strong>performance evaluation feedback. Thesecriteria are to ensure that <strong>the</strong> samples are valid.The survey was adm<strong>in</strong>istered as follows.Work<strong>in</strong>g closely with managers from <strong>the</strong> threeorganisations, a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary notification wascirculated encourag<strong>in</strong>g managers to participate<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> survey. After obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g seniormanagement permission to conduct <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>dependent research study <strong>the</strong> survey<strong>in</strong>strument was distributed to potentialrespondents, toge<strong>the</strong>r with assurance <strong>of</strong>confidentiality. Rem<strong>in</strong>ders were sent one,three and seven weeks after <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>almail<strong>in</strong>g. The survey package and rem<strong>in</strong>derletters were sent via e-mail. Respondents couldreturn <strong>the</strong> completed questionnaireselectronically by e-mail or send a hard copyversion by post.The distribution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> survey <strong>in</strong>struments is asfollows: 102 to CO1, 99 to <strong>the</strong> CO2, and 95 toCO3. Of 296 questionnaires distributed, 174were returned (55 from CO1, 52 from CO2,and 67 from CO3) yield<strong>in</strong>g a total responserate <strong>of</strong> 59 per cent. Careful exam<strong>in</strong>ationrevealed that 9 responses (1 from CO1, 2 fromCO2, and 6 from CO3) were not usable,yield<strong>in</strong>g a total <strong>of</strong> 165 usable responses (56%).Respondents were <strong>in</strong>vited to take part <strong>in</strong>follow-up <strong>in</strong>terviews or group discussions.These <strong>in</strong>terviews sought to better understand<strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> quantitative results and ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> context <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> PMERSoperated.Variables and Their MeasurementThe dependent variable <strong>in</strong> this study isperceived procedural fairness and <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>dependent variables are participation <strong>in</strong>target sett<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goalatta<strong>in</strong>ment-rewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and goal specificity. Inaddition to <strong>the</strong>se variables, <strong>the</strong> study <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>the</strong> type <strong>of</strong> performance measures <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> modelas <strong>the</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g variable. To <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>the</strong>validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> measures and to be morecomparable with previous studies, allmeasures were taken from previous literature. 11 All <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>struments were pilot-tested prior to<strong>the</strong> distribution to <strong>the</strong> respondents, on executiveMBA students <strong>of</strong> an UK university.33


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013<strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong>This variable is measured us<strong>in</strong>g a four-item<strong>in</strong>strument developed by McFarl<strong>in</strong> andSweeney (1992). In management account<strong>in</strong>gstudies it has been used by, for example, Lauand Sholih<strong>in</strong> (2005) and Lau and Tan (2006).Respondents were requested to rate <strong>the</strong>fairness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> procedures used to evaluate<strong>the</strong>ir performance, to communicateperformance feedback, and to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong>irpay <strong>in</strong>creases and promotion, rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1(very unfair) to 7 (very fair).Participation <strong>in</strong> Target Sett<strong>in</strong>gTo measure this variable, respondents wereasked to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong>ir level <strong>of</strong> agreementus<strong>in</strong>g a 7-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-type scale, rang<strong>in</strong>g fromstrongly disagree to strongly agree, on a s<strong>in</strong>gleitem <strong>of</strong> “my superior allows me to participate<strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g my performance goals/targets”. Thismeasure is adapted from <strong>the</strong> goal-sett<strong>in</strong>gquestionnaire developed by Locke and Latham(1984).Goal-Atta<strong>in</strong>ment-Reward L<strong>in</strong>kTo measure this variable, respondents wereasked to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong>ir level <strong>of</strong> agreementus<strong>in</strong>g a 7-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-type scale, rang<strong>in</strong>g fromstrongly disagree to strongly agree, on a s<strong>in</strong>gleitem <strong>of</strong> “my rewards are tied to <strong>the</strong>achievement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> performance goals”. This<strong>in</strong>strument is adapted from Kom<strong>in</strong>is andEmmanuel (2007).Goal SpecificityTo measure goal specificity, three items weretaken from Fang et al. (2005): (1) my superiorspecifically expla<strong>in</strong>ed what my performancegoals are; (2) I have very specific performancegoals <strong>in</strong> my job; and (3) I understand <strong>the</strong> exactlevel <strong>of</strong> my assigned performance goals.Respondents were requested to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong>irlevel <strong>of</strong> agreement to <strong>the</strong> above items, us<strong>in</strong>g aseven-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-type scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).Performance MeasureThe performance measure is employed as <strong>the</strong>controll<strong>in</strong>g variable. To measure this variable,respondents were requested to <strong>in</strong>dicate howmuch importance <strong>the</strong>y thought <strong>the</strong>irsupervisors attach to certa<strong>in</strong> performancemeasures when evaluat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir performance,us<strong>in</strong>g a seven-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scale, anchored 1(no importance) and 7 (always important) with0 if not applicable. 2 The performancecategories used are based on those <strong>of</strong> Ittner etal. (2003), which <strong>the</strong>y consider to be importantdrivers <strong>of</strong> long-term organisational success.The categories are: relations with customers,relations with employees, operationalperformance, product and service quality,alliances with o<strong>the</strong>r organisations, relationswith suppliers, environmental performance,product and service <strong>in</strong>novations, communityperformance, and f<strong>in</strong>ancial performance.F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and DiscussionsAs mentioned <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> previous section, a total<strong>of</strong> 296 questionnaires were distributed to threedifferent organisations and 174 were returned.Of <strong>the</strong>se responses, 9 were unusable due to asubstantive part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> questionnaire be<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>complete, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> 165 f<strong>in</strong>alresponses used <strong>in</strong> this study. The demographic<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> respondents, presented <strong>in</strong>table 1, consists <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> average number <strong>of</strong>employees that respondents directly managed,<strong>the</strong>ir length <strong>of</strong> service (tenure) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>organisation, tenure <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir current position,and <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> time <strong>the</strong>y have beensupervised by <strong>the</strong>ir current supervisor.The table <strong>in</strong>dicates that respondents from CO1rated all demographic variables more highlythan <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r two organisations, with CO2hav<strong>in</strong>g far lower rat<strong>in</strong>gs for length <strong>of</strong> serviceand supervision under current supervisor.However, <strong>of</strong> particular importance to thisstudy is that all respondents had experienced<strong>the</strong> performance review process on at least oneoccasion.To exam<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r response bias exists, nonresponsebias test was performed. We splitresponses <strong>in</strong>to “early” and “late” responses.Follow<strong>in</strong>g Hall (2008), “Early” is def<strong>in</strong>ed as<strong>the</strong> first 20 percent <strong>of</strong> responses and “late” isdef<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>the</strong> last 20 percent <strong>of</strong> responses.The responses from those two groups werecompared by runn<strong>in</strong>g t-tests. Table 2 presents<strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> non-response bias tests for eachvariable. The results <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong> majority<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scores <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> variables do not differbetween <strong>the</strong> early and late responses.2 In <strong>the</strong> analysis, follow<strong>in</strong>g Ittner at al. (2003), 0was converted <strong>in</strong>to 1.34


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013However, <strong>the</strong>re are some differences,particularly for CO2. For example, earlyrespondents <strong>in</strong>dicated that <strong>the</strong>y are evaluatedmore through nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures, andperceived that <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>kis more transparent and <strong>the</strong> reward distributionis fairer. It is difficult to <strong>of</strong>fer a good reasonfor <strong>the</strong> particular differences but it is sensibleto <strong>in</strong>terpret results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se variables withcaution. However, <strong>the</strong> table shows <strong>the</strong>re is nosignificant difference for <strong>the</strong> dependentvariable studied, i.e. procedural fairness, <strong>in</strong>any organisation.Table 1: Demographic Information <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Respondents Means (with Range Shown <strong>in</strong> Brackets)CO1 CO2 CO3Employees report<strong>in</strong>g to respondent 8(1-60)7(1-60)5(1-21)Tenure <strong>in</strong> organisation 3 16.5(1-36)5.6(1-19)15.4(2-37)Tenure <strong>in</strong> current position 7.8(1-36)2.7(1-8)5.2(1-25)Supervision under current superior 3.7(1-18)2.6(1-8)2.5(1-6)Table 2: Results <strong>of</strong> Non-Response Bias TestsCO1 CO2 CO3VariableMean p- value Mean p- value Mean p- valuedifferencedifferencedifferenceNonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 0.574 0.361 0.827 0.004 0.094 0.784F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 0.200 0.869 1.015 0.156 0.628 0.443Participation 0.500 0.459 0.492 0.161 0.263 0.562Goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k -0.100 0.906 1.121 0.002 0.462 0.317Goal specificity -0.367 0.597 0.005 0.986 0.336 0.297<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness 0.525 0.355 0.466 0.211 0.444 0.340Table 3: Descriptive Statistics <strong>of</strong> Variables StudiedOrganisation/Variable M<strong>in</strong>imum Maximum Mean StandardDeviationCO1Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.78 7.00 4.01 1.33F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.00 7.00 4.88 2.30Participation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g 1.00 7.00 4.60 1.64Transparency <strong>of</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k1.00 7.00 3.82 1.75Goal specificity 1.33 7.00 4.51 1.57<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness 1.75 6.00 3.89 1.23CO2Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.67 6.56 3.83 1.03F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.00 7.00 4.74 1.99Participation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g 3.00 7.00 5.43 1.09Transparency <strong>of</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k3.00 7.00 5.87 0.93Goal specificity 2.00 7.00 5.51 1.01<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness 3.00 6.50 5.16 0.96CO33 Tenure (both <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> organisation and job) and <strong>the</strong> period with <strong>the</strong>ir current superior are given <strong>in</strong> years.35


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.00 7.00 4.95 1.90F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 1.00 7.00 4.97 1.47Participation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g 1.00 7.00 5.02 1.31Transparency <strong>of</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k1.00 7.00 4.95 1.90Goal specificity 3.33 7.00 5.57 0.83<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness 1.75 6.25 4.52 1.07The descriptive statistics <strong>of</strong> variables studiedare presented <strong>in</strong> table 3. This <strong>in</strong>dicates that for<strong>the</strong> performance measures variable, <strong>the</strong> highestscores both for nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial and f<strong>in</strong>ancialmeasures are found <strong>in</strong> CO3 (for nonf<strong>in</strong>ancialmeasures mean=4.95; SD=1.90 and forf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures mean=4.97; SD=1.47). Theresults suggest that <strong>in</strong> this organisationrespondents perceived that <strong>the</strong>ir superiors relyon both f<strong>in</strong>ancial and nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures.The lowest score for nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measuresand f<strong>in</strong>ancial measures are found <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> CO2(for nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures mean=3.83,SD=1.03 and for f<strong>in</strong>ancial measuresmean=4.74, SD=1.99). For <strong>the</strong> dependentvariable, i.e. procedural fairness, <strong>the</strong> lowestscore is found <strong>in</strong> CO1 (mean= 3.89; SD= 1.23)and <strong>the</strong> highest score is found <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> CO2.Interviews with selected respondents fromCO1 established that <strong>the</strong>y viewed <strong>the</strong>procedure as somewhat unfair because <strong>the</strong>performance evaluation system was vague,<strong>in</strong>adequate and <strong>of</strong>fer<strong>in</strong>g little reward foremployees atta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g performance measures. Inaddition <strong>the</strong>re was little opportunity forpromotion, low or nonexistent pay rises, andlittle <strong>in</strong>centive through f<strong>in</strong>ancial ornonf<strong>in</strong>ancial rewards. This is also evidencedby <strong>the</strong> lower scores for <strong>the</strong> transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k. The follow<strong>in</strong>gcomments ga<strong>the</strong>red from CO1 managersre<strong>in</strong>force this:“… Yearly appraisal is merely formality”(market<strong>in</strong>g managers).“Performance evaluation is <strong>in</strong>adequateand vague...at present <strong>the</strong> appraisal systemis <strong>in</strong>formal and not enforced…<strong>in</strong>novationand creativity are not be<strong>in</strong>g rewarded andencouraged…no rewards for meet<strong>in</strong>gperformance target …noth<strong>in</strong>g happenwhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y (targets) are achieved or not”(R & D manager).“I’m asked to give more effort than o<strong>the</strong>rswho appear to get more reward f<strong>in</strong>anciallyand credit for what <strong>the</strong>y do… (but I’m) notbe<strong>in</strong>g given <strong>the</strong> credit for (my)achievement” (production manager).The highest score <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness isfound <strong>in</strong> CO2. The follow<strong>in</strong>g quote ga<strong>in</strong>edfrom <strong>the</strong> group discussion re<strong>in</strong>forced <strong>the</strong>survey f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs:“I thought <strong>the</strong> process was fair though I amnot happy with my pay rise but expect thatthis has been affected by <strong>the</strong> credit crunchwhich has impacted on everyone’s pay andbonus”.To test <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses, data were analysedus<strong>in</strong>g both multiple regression and PLS. 4 Priorto <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> analysis, ANOVA tests betweengroups were performed to see whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>reare any differences among sub-samples. It wasfound that variability <strong>of</strong> variance between <strong>the</strong>groups (i.e. organisations) for proceduralfairness, participation, transparency <strong>of</strong> goalatta<strong>in</strong>ment-rewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and goal specificity issignificant (see table 4). Therefore <strong>in</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>gregression we controlled <strong>the</strong> organisationeffect.In addition to <strong>the</strong> ANOVA test, <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>herentassumptions relat<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> adequacy<strong>of</strong> regression models - normality,homoscedasticity, multicol<strong>in</strong>earity andl<strong>in</strong>earity - were tested. Normality refers to <strong>the</strong>shape <strong>of</strong> data distribution for a metric variableand its correspondence to <strong>the</strong> normaldistribution. Hair et al. (1998) argue that thisassumption is <strong>the</strong> most fundamentalassumption <strong>in</strong> regression analysis. To assess<strong>the</strong> normality, Cohen and Cohen (1983)recommend an exam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> residual scatter4The regression was executed us<strong>in</strong>g an OLSapproach with SPSS s<strong>of</strong>tware and <strong>the</strong>n verifiedus<strong>in</strong>g PLS with PLS Graph 3 s<strong>of</strong>tware.36


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013plots <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> dependent and <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables. The results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> test <strong>in</strong>dicate that<strong>the</strong> normal probability plots <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> residuals <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> regression models used <strong>in</strong> this study arescattered along a relatively straight l<strong>in</strong>e. Thismeans that <strong>the</strong> residuals are normallydistributed and thus, <strong>the</strong> normality assumptionis met.With homoscedasticity, it is assumed thatdependent variables exhibit constant varianceacross <strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent variables(Hair et al., 1998). This assumption can betested by plott<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> residuals aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>the</strong>predicted values (Cohen and Cohen, 1983;Hair et al., 1998). The homoscedasticityassumption is not violated when <strong>the</strong> plots <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> residuals aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g fitted(predicted) values for <strong>the</strong> models show that allplots are scattered randomly betweenapproximately equal horizontal bands with nodiscernible patterns or systematic variations.The results <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong> homoscedasticityassumption is not violated.Ano<strong>the</strong>r assumption tested is multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity.Multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity occurs when <strong>the</strong> correlationsamong variables are so high that certa<strong>in</strong>statistical analysis cannot be performed.Multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity signifies that at least twoseparate variables are measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sameth<strong>in</strong>g. Some <strong>in</strong>dications <strong>of</strong> multicoll<strong>in</strong>earityare: <strong>the</strong> correlation between <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables be<strong>in</strong>g greater than 0.85 (Kl<strong>in</strong>e,1998); <strong>the</strong> tolerance levels for <strong>the</strong> variablesand any value be<strong>in</strong>g very small, less than 0.1;and <strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> variance <strong>in</strong>flation factors(VIF) be<strong>in</strong>g greater than 10 (Kl<strong>in</strong>e, 1998;Pallant, 2005). The regression model showsthat <strong>the</strong> tolerance levels for <strong>the</strong> variables areall above 0.1, and that <strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> variance<strong>in</strong>flation factors (VIF) is less than 10. Hence,<strong>the</strong>re is no multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity problem. Inaddition, none <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> correlations between<strong>in</strong>dependent variables is greater than 0.85.L<strong>in</strong>earity refers to <strong>the</strong> extent to which anychanges <strong>in</strong> dependent variables are associatedwith <strong>the</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent variables.Follow<strong>in</strong>g Cohen and Cohen (1983), scatterplots <strong>of</strong> residuals were exam<strong>in</strong>ed. The result<strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>the</strong> appropriateness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>earmodels is not violated.Table 5 presents <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> bivariatecorrelation analysis and table 6 presents <strong>the</strong>results <strong>of</strong> regression analysis. Table 5 showsthat participation, reward l<strong>in</strong>k, and goalspecificity are positively associated withprocedural fairness. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, nei<strong>the</strong>rf<strong>in</strong>ancial nor nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures areassociated with procedural fairness. Hence, <strong>the</strong>results provide <strong>in</strong>itial support for <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>ses that procedural fairness is affectedby participation, reward l<strong>in</strong>k, and goalspecificity, but do not provide <strong>in</strong>itial supportfor <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses that procedural fairness isaffected by <strong>the</strong> type <strong>of</strong> performance measureused to evaluate <strong>the</strong> respondents’ performance.In addition, <strong>the</strong> table also <strong>in</strong>dicates that allgoal-related attributes (goal sett<strong>in</strong>gparticipation, goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k, andgoal specificity) are correlated significantlywith each o<strong>the</strong>r. The table also reveals thatf<strong>in</strong>ancial performance measures are perceivedto be more specific compared to that <strong>of</strong>nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures.Table 6 presents <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> regressionanalysis with procedural fairness as <strong>the</strong>dependent variable and goal-related attributes(goal-sett<strong>in</strong>g participation, goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and goal specificity) as <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>dependent variables, controll<strong>in</strong>g forperformance measures (f<strong>in</strong>ancial andnonf<strong>in</strong>ancial) and <strong>the</strong> company. The table<strong>in</strong>dicates that participation, reward l<strong>in</strong>k andgoal specificity significantly affect proceduralfairness, although <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> goal specificityis marg<strong>in</strong>ally significant (p


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013Table 4: The Results <strong>of</strong> ANOVA TestsVariable F Sig<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness 17.580 0.000Participation 4.448 0.013Transparency 29.802 0.000Goal specificity 14.042 0.000F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 0.152 0.859Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 0.579 0.449Table 5: The Results Of Correlation AnalysisPF Part L<strong>in</strong>k GS FM NM<strong>Procedural</strong> <strong>Fairness</strong> 1(PF)0.447** 0.570** 0.467** 0.059 0.005Participation1(Part)0.433** 0.484** 0.070 0.148Reward l<strong>in</strong>k1(L<strong>in</strong>k)0.515** 0.225** 0.128Goal specificity1(GS)0.191* 0.083F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures1(FM)0.223*Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial1measures (NM)** Correlation is significant at <strong>the</strong> 0.01 level (2-tailed).* Correlation is significant at <strong>the</strong> 0.05 level (2-tailed).Table 6: The Results Of Regression Analysis Us<strong>in</strong>g OLS Approach.Variables Coefficient Coefficient value p-valueConstant b 0 1.759 .001Participation b 1 .199 .036Transparency l<strong>in</strong>k b 2 .415 .000Goal specificity b 3 .169 .097F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures b 4 -.064 .444Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures b 5 -.077 .349Company b 6 .015 .862Adjusted R 2 = 36.8%F = 10.993; p= 0.000Table 7: The Results <strong>of</strong> Regression Us<strong>in</strong>g PLS Approach (T-Statistics <strong>in</strong> Brackets)VariablePath to<strong>Procedural</strong> fairnessParticipation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g 0.191 (2.218)*Goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k 0.374 (4.771)**Goal specificity 0.175 (2.077)*F<strong>in</strong>ancial measures -0.071 (1.214)Nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures 0.112 (0.821)Company 0.026 (0.314)R 2 41.4%** Significant at <strong>the</strong> 0.01 level (2-tailed).* Significant at <strong>the</strong> 0.05 level (2-tailed).38


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013Overall, <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> statistical analysissupport <strong>the</strong> arguments that procedural fairnessis affected by participation <strong>in</strong> target sett<strong>in</strong>g,transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-rewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and goal specificity. As argued earlier,<strong>the</strong>se three variables are consistent with <strong>the</strong>justice rules proposed by Leventhal (1980) ascontended <strong>in</strong> this study. However, <strong>the</strong> resultsdo not support <strong>the</strong> argument that <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ancial and nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial measures isassociated with procedural fairness (c.f. Lauand Sholih<strong>in</strong>, 2005; Lau and Moser, 2008).The <strong>in</strong>terview results support <strong>the</strong> abovef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. For example, a production managerfrom CO1 emphasised <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> goalspecificity as follows:“… well def<strong>in</strong>ed targets help me to getorganised which improves myperformance…if targets are not welldef<strong>in</strong>ed (I am) not able to achieve <strong>the</strong>m”.Ano<strong>the</strong>r manager, a market<strong>in</strong>g manager, from<strong>the</strong> same company said:“…(specific goals) allow me (to be) morefocused on achiev<strong>in</strong>g targets, (to make)more focused effort”.The above two managers argued that specificgoals/targets are important because <strong>the</strong>y help<strong>the</strong>m to achieve better performance. Hence, itis likely that goal specificity is positivelyassociated with procedural fairness because itis <strong>in</strong>strumental <strong>in</strong> achiev<strong>in</strong>g betterperformance (c.f. Locke and Latham, 1990;2002). To some extent, it supports <strong>the</strong><strong>in</strong>strumental approach to procedural fairness,i.e. an <strong>in</strong>dividual prefers fair proceduresbecause it will give <strong>the</strong>m better outcomes(L<strong>in</strong>d and Tyler, 1998). A similar reason israised by a market<strong>in</strong>g manager from CO2:“Specific goals are important to mebecause specific goals motivate me tohigher performance and provide me withrequired focus”.With regard to participation, <strong>the</strong> abovemarket<strong>in</strong>g manager <strong>of</strong>fers reasons why it isimportant:“Participation allows me to give <strong>in</strong>put and…I feel <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> bus<strong>in</strong>ess and …I canexpress my op<strong>in</strong>ions and I can <strong>in</strong>fluenceoutcomes”.Likewise, <strong>the</strong> production managers argued:“Participation gives me a betterunderstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> how to achieve targetsand understand why I have to do it … itimproves my motivation …”.However, a health and safety manager saidthat participation was “…just for personalsatisfaction”. Presumably, part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>satisfaction is an assurance that <strong>the</strong> evaluationprocedures are conducted <strong>in</strong> a fair manner.The above quotes suggest various reasons whymanagers value participation. This isconsistent with previous studies <strong>in</strong>organisational justice on <strong>the</strong> positive effects <strong>of</strong>participative decision mak<strong>in</strong>g on proceduralfairness (see Greenberg and Folger 1983; Biesand Shapiro 1988; Magner et al. 1992). Ehlenand Welker (1996) identified whyparticipation affects <strong>the</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> fairness.They are: (1) participation serves to enhance aperception <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> s<strong>in</strong>cerity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> positiontaken by a decision maker; (2) participationmay generate a perception <strong>of</strong> enhanceddecision quality; (3) participation sends amessage to participants about how <strong>the</strong>y areperceived by o<strong>the</strong>rs; and (4) people desirecontrol over decision outcomes which affect<strong>the</strong>m, and participation is seen as a way toachieve this control.Conclusion, Limitations andSuggestion for Future ResearchThe objective <strong>of</strong> this study is to <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>the</strong>antecedents <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>context <strong>of</strong> PMERS. This study is important asprevious studies <strong>of</strong> fairness with<strong>in</strong> PMERScontext produce <strong>in</strong>conclusive f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs onfactors affect<strong>in</strong>g fairness perception. Draw<strong>in</strong>gon organisational justice literature and goal<strong>the</strong>ory <strong>the</strong> study argues that perceivedprocedural fairness is affected by goal-relatedvariables, i.e. participation <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>gperformance targets, <strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>mentrewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and <strong>the</strong> specificity <strong>of</strong> goals to beachieved by managers. Us<strong>in</strong>g a questionnairesurvey with samples derived from three pr<strong>of</strong>itorientedorganisations, one manufactur<strong>in</strong>gcompany and two f<strong>in</strong>ancial servicesorganisations, this study f<strong>in</strong>ds that perceivedprocedural fairness is affected by participation<strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g performance targets, <strong>the</strong> goalatta<strong>in</strong>ment-rewardl<strong>in</strong>k, and <strong>the</strong> specificity <strong>of</strong>39


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013goals to be achieved by managers.Additionally, <strong>the</strong> study f<strong>in</strong>ds that <strong>the</strong> type <strong>of</strong>performance measures (whe<strong>the</strong>r f<strong>in</strong>ancial ornonf<strong>in</strong>ancial) used to evaluate managers is notassociated with perceived procedural fairness.It would seem that much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prioraccount<strong>in</strong>g literature has overemphasised <strong>the</strong>importance that performance measure typesmay have on procedural fairness, whe<strong>the</strong>rbudget constra<strong>in</strong>ed, pr<strong>of</strong>it conscious or nonaccount<strong>in</strong>g,to <strong>the</strong> neglect <strong>of</strong> more criticalvariables.This study contributes to <strong>the</strong> literature <strong>of</strong>procedural fairness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> PMERSby provid<strong>in</strong>g empirical evidence on factorswhich <strong>in</strong>fluenced perceived proceduralfairness. Look<strong>in</strong>g at <strong>the</strong> practical implications<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> study, <strong>the</strong> results suggest that superiorsshould focus on goal-related variables, i.e.participation <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g performance targets,<strong>the</strong> goal-atta<strong>in</strong>ment-reward l<strong>in</strong>k, and <strong>the</strong>specificity <strong>of</strong> goals to be achieved bysubord<strong>in</strong>ates <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g PMERS ra<strong>the</strong>r thanon <strong>the</strong> types <strong>of</strong> performance measures.The study, however, should be <strong>in</strong>terpretedcautiously for <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g reasons. The firstone is <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>herent limitations associated withsurvey method. Future study should exam<strong>in</strong>e<strong>the</strong> issue us<strong>in</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>r approaches, such asexperimental design. Secondly, this studydraws on managers from just threeorganisations. Future study could exam<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong>model us<strong>in</strong>g samples derived from moreorganisations. Thirdly, some variables weremeasured us<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle item. However, tocompensate this we have conducted<strong>in</strong>terviews. Future study could use <strong>the</strong> bettermeasure to study <strong>the</strong> same topic..Notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se limitations, we believethat this study provides clarity on factorsaffect<strong>in</strong>g perceived procedural fairness.ReferencesAnthony, R. and Gov<strong>in</strong>darajan, V. (1998),Management Control Systems, Irw<strong>in</strong>, Chicago.Bies, R. J. and Shapiro, D. L. (1998), “Voiceand justification: Their <strong>in</strong>fluence onprocedural fairness judgments”, Academy <strong>of</strong>Management Journal, September, 3: pp. 676-685.Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. (1983), AppliedMultiple Regression: Correlation Analysis for<strong>the</strong> Behavioral Sciences, L. ErlbaumAssociates, New Jersey, USA.Earley, P. C. and Kanfer, R. (1985), “The<strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> component participation and rolemodels on goal acceptance, goal satisfaction,and performance”, Organizational Behaviorand Human Decision Processes, 36 (3): pp.378-390.Ehlen, C. R. and Welker, R. B. (1996),“<strong>Procedural</strong> fairness <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> peer and qualityreview programs”, Audit<strong>in</strong>g: A Journal <strong>of</strong>Practice & Theory, 15 (1): pp. 38-52.Emmanuel, C. R., Otley, D. T., Merchant, K.A. (1990), Account<strong>in</strong>g for ManagementControl, Chapman and Hall, London, UK.Fang, E., Evans, K. R., Zou, S. (2005), “Themoderat<strong>in</strong>g effect <strong>of</strong> goal-sett<strong>in</strong>gcharacteristics on <strong>the</strong> sales control systems-jobperformance relationship”, Journal <strong>of</strong> Bus<strong>in</strong>essResearch, 58 (9): pp. 1214-1222.Folger, R. (1977), “Distributive andprocedural justice: comb<strong>in</strong>ed impact <strong>of</strong>"voice" and improvement on experienced<strong>in</strong>equity”, Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and SocialPsychology, 35 (2): pp. 108 - 119.Folger, R. and Konovsky, M. A. (1989),“Effects <strong>of</strong> procedural and distributive justiceon reactions to pay raise decisions”, Academy<strong>of</strong> Management Journal, 32 (1): pp. 115-130.Greenberg, J. (1986), “<strong>Determ<strong>in</strong>ants</strong> <strong>of</strong>perceived fairness <strong>of</strong> performanceevaluations”, Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Psychology,71 (2): pp. 340-342.Greenberg, J. and Folger, R. (1983),“<strong>Procedural</strong> justice, participation and <strong>the</strong> fairprocess effect <strong>in</strong> groups and organizations”,In: PAULUS, P. (Ed.) Basic Group Process,Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag, New York, USA.Hair, J. R., Anderson, R. L., Tatham, R.E.W.C. B. (1998), Multivariate Data Analysis:With Read<strong>in</strong>gs, Pearson, New Jersey, USA.Hall, M. (2008), “The effect <strong>of</strong> comprehensiveperformance measurement systems on roleclarity, psychological empowerment andmanagerial performance”, Account<strong>in</strong>g,40


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013Organizations and Society, 33 (2-3): pp. 141-163.Hartmann, F. And Slapnicar, S. (2009), “Howformal performance evaluation affects trustbetween superior and subord<strong>in</strong>ate managers”,Account<strong>in</strong>g, Organizations and Society, 34 (6-7): pp. 722-737.Hartmann, F., Naranjo-Gil, D., and Perego, P.(2009), “The effects <strong>of</strong> leadership styles anduse <strong>of</strong> performance measures on managerialwork-related attitudes”, European Account<strong>in</strong>gReview, 19 (2): pp. 275-310.Hopwood, A. G. (1972), “An empirical study<strong>of</strong> role <strong>of</strong> account<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> performanceevaluation”, Journal <strong>of</strong> Account<strong>in</strong>g Research,Supplement (10): pp. 156-182.Ittner, C. D., Larcker, D. F., Randall, T.(2003), “Performance implications <strong>of</strong> strategicperformance measurement <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ancialservices firms”, Account<strong>in</strong>g, Organizationsand Society, 28 (7-8): pp. 715-741.Kaplan, R. S. and Atk<strong>in</strong>son, A. A. (1998),Advanced Management Account<strong>in</strong>g, PrenticeHall, New York, USA.Kl<strong>in</strong>e, R. (1998), Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and Practice <strong>of</strong>Structural Equation Modell<strong>in</strong>g, The GuilfordPress, New York.Kom<strong>in</strong>is, G. and Emmanuel, C. R. (2007),“The expectancy-valence <strong>the</strong>ory revisited:Develop<strong>in</strong>g an extended model <strong>of</strong> managerialmotivation”, Management Account<strong>in</strong>gResearch, 18 (1): pp. 49-75.Lau, C. M. and Moser, A. (2008),“Behavioural effects <strong>of</strong> nonf<strong>in</strong>ancialperformance measures: The role <strong>of</strong> proceduralfairness”, Behavioral Research <strong>in</strong> Account<strong>in</strong>g,20 (2): pp. 55 - 71.Lau, C. M. and Sholih<strong>in</strong>, M. (2005),“F<strong>in</strong>ancial and nonf<strong>in</strong>ancial performancemeasures: How do <strong>the</strong>y affect jobsatisfaction?”, The British Account<strong>in</strong>g Review,37 (4): pp. 389-413.Lau, C. M. and Tan, S. L. C. (2006), “Theeffects <strong>of</strong> procedural fairness and <strong>in</strong>terpersonaltrust on job tension <strong>in</strong> budget<strong>in</strong>g”,Management Account<strong>in</strong>g Research, 17 (2): pp.171-186.Leventhal, G. S. (1980), “What should be donewith equity <strong>the</strong>ory? New approaches to <strong>the</strong>study <strong>of</strong> fairness <strong>in</strong> social relationships”, In:Gergen, K., et al. (Eds.) Social Exchange:Advances <strong>in</strong> Theory and Research, PlenumPress, New York.Libby, T. (1999), “The <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> voice andexplanation on performance <strong>in</strong> a participativebudget<strong>in</strong>g sett<strong>in</strong>g”, Account<strong>in</strong>g, Organizationsand Society, 24 (2): pp. 125-137.Libby, T. (2001), “Referent cognitions andbudgetary fairness: A research note”, Journal<strong>of</strong> Management Account<strong>in</strong>g Research, 13 (1):pp. 91-105.L<strong>in</strong>dquist, T. M. (1995), “<strong>Fairness</strong> asantecedent to participative budget<strong>in</strong>g:Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> distributive justice,procedural justice and referent cognitions onsatisfaction and performance”, Journal <strong>of</strong>Management Account<strong>in</strong>g Research, 7 (1): pp.122-147.L<strong>in</strong>d, E. A. and Tyler, T. R. (1988), The SocialPsychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>Procedural</strong> Justice, PlenumPress, New York, USA.Locke, E. A. and Latham, G. P. (1984), GoalSett<strong>in</strong>g: A Motivational Techniques that WorksPrentice-Hall, New Jersey, USA.Locke, E. A. and Latham, G. P. (1990), ATheory <strong>of</strong> Goal Sett<strong>in</strong>g and Task Performance,Prentice-Hall, New Jersey, USA.Locke, E. A. and Latham, G. P. (2002),“Build<strong>in</strong>g a practically useful <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> goalsett<strong>in</strong>g and task motivation: A 35-yearodyssey”, American Psychologist, 57 (9): pp.705-717.Magner, N. R., Welker, R. B., Johnson, G. G.(1992), “Test<strong>in</strong>g a model <strong>of</strong> voice, control,procedural justice, and organizationalcommitment with latent variable structuralequation analysis”, The International Joumal<strong>of</strong> Conflict Management, 3 (3): pp. 223-236.McFarl<strong>in</strong>, D. B. and Sweeney, P. D. (1992),“Distributive and procedural justice aspredictors <strong>of</strong> satisfaction with personal andorganizational outcomes”, Academy <strong>of</strong>Management Journal, 35 (3): pp. 626-637.41


JAMAR Vol. 11 · No. 1 2013Merchant, K. A. and Van Der Stede, W. A.(2003), Management Control Systems:Performance Measurement, Evaluation andIncentives, Pearson Education, Essex, UK.Neuman, W. L. (2003), Social ResearchMethods: Qualitative and QuantitativeApproaches, Allyn and Bacon, Boston, USA.Otley, D. T. (1978), “Budget use andmanagerial performance”, Journal <strong>of</strong>Account<strong>in</strong>g Research, 16 (1): pp. 122-149.Otley, D. (1999), “Performance management:a framework for management control systemsresearch”, Management Account<strong>in</strong>g Research,10 (4): pp. 363-382.Pallant, J. (2005), SPSS Survival Manual,Open University Press, Beckshire, UK.Sholih<strong>in</strong>, M and Pike, R. (2009), “<strong>Fairness</strong> <strong>in</strong>performance evaluation and its behaviouralconsequences”, Account<strong>in</strong>g and Bus<strong>in</strong>essResearch, 39 (4): pp. 397-413.Thibaut, J. and Walker, J. (1975), <strong>Procedural</strong>Justice: A Psychological Analysis, John Wiley,New York, USA.Wentzel, K. (2002), “The <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> fairnessperceptions and goal commitment onmanagers’ performance <strong>in</strong> a budget sett<strong>in</strong>g”,Behavioral Research <strong>in</strong> Account<strong>in</strong>g, 14 (1):pp. 247-271.42

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!