13.07.2015 Views

Hovland et al. 2011.pdf - Atrium - University of Guelph

Hovland et al. 2011.pdf - Atrium - University of Guelph

Hovland et al. 2011.pdf - Atrium - University of Guelph

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

118 A.L. <strong>Hovland</strong> <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>. / Applied Anim<strong>al</strong> Behaviour Science 135 (2011) 110–1204.2. The strength <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> motivationsThe test vixens were willing to continue their visits tothe shared compartment with increasing costs, both forsoci<strong>al</strong> contact and food, which showed that both theseresources were rewarding to the anim<strong>al</strong>s. Adult vixen v<strong>al</strong>uedfull soci<strong>al</strong> contact on average about 24% <strong>of</strong> the worththey put on food: a relatively low v<strong>al</strong>ue, and <strong>al</strong>so, on average,lower than the v<strong>al</strong>ue these fem<strong>al</strong>es placed on soci<strong>al</strong>contact when sub-adults, at the age <strong>of</strong> 7–8 months, whichaveraged 38% <strong>of</strong> the MPP for food (<strong>Hovland</strong> <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008). Thesoci<strong>al</strong> behaviours <strong>of</strong> adult vixens <strong>al</strong>so differed from juvenilevixens in sever<strong>al</strong> ways. As juveniles, the vixens hadspent <strong>al</strong>most h<strong>al</strong>f <strong>of</strong> the day tog<strong>et</strong>her when access werefree, but as adults these vixens spent just 1/6 <strong>of</strong> disposabl<strong>et</strong>ime in contact, again further indicative <strong>of</strong> a lowered motivationto soci<strong>al</strong>ize in adults. Potenti<strong>al</strong> explanations includechanges in the relative v<strong>al</strong>ue <strong>of</strong> food; effects <strong>of</strong> season; thefact that adult vixens were <strong>al</strong>lowed only 12 h access comparedto 24 h access in the previous study <strong>of</strong> sub-adults;differences in the nature (e.g. soci<strong>al</strong>ity) <strong>of</strong> the stimulus anim<strong>al</strong>s,as well as changes in the soci<strong>al</strong> motivation <strong>of</strong> th<strong>et</strong>est foxes. Also, the lack <strong>of</strong> physic<strong>al</strong> contact for more than2 years could possibly affect adult vixens’ soci<strong>al</strong> motivationand behaviour (in, e.g. m<strong>al</strong>e rats, previous studies haveshown that soci<strong>al</strong> isolation during adulthood reduces soci<strong>al</strong>exploration and induces high emotion<strong>al</strong> response to a novelsoci<strong>al</strong> environment (Shoji and Mizoguchi, 2011)); <strong>al</strong>thoughsince the vixens could see, smell and communicate throughthe wire mesh the potenti<strong>al</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> isolation was possiblyreduced compared to that <strong>of</strong> tot<strong>al</strong> isolation. Over<strong>al</strong>lthe evidence suggests genuine reductions in soci<strong>al</strong>ity withage. First, the nature <strong>of</strong> adult vixens’ interactions seemedmore comp<strong>et</strong>itive with higher, relative levels <strong>of</strong> aggressivedisplays compared to when juvenile (<strong>Hovland</strong> <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008;Fig. 5). Second, one rewarding aspect <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> contact thatmay decline with age and thus reduce anim<strong>al</strong>s’ motivationfor contact is soci<strong>al</strong> play (Spinka <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2001; Trezza <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>.,2009). Pair housed adult silver fox vixens tend to engage insoci<strong>al</strong> play less frequently than do pair housed juvenile vixenshoused under similar conditions (Hansen, 2007; Akre<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010b). Intriguingly, the number <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> play boutswas highest in the pair where the test subject paid the lowestrelative price for contact, suggesting that reinforcingproperties <strong>of</strong> play may indeed be reduced for adults, perhapsbecause play sequences include comp<strong>et</strong>itive elementsthat may esc<strong>al</strong>ate into aggression (e.g. Bek<strong>of</strong>f <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 1981).Over<strong>al</strong>l the difference b<strong>et</strong>ween adult and sub-adults suggeststhat housing sub-adults may be more benefici<strong>al</strong> thansoci<strong>al</strong>ly housing adults.4.3. The nature <strong>of</strong> adult vixens’ relationshipsThe nature <strong>of</strong> vixens’ relationship examined in connectionwith Food Test 1 (October) and Food Test 2 (November)reflected a lack <strong>of</strong> strong comp<strong>et</strong>itive motivations and wasmainly characterized by low-intensive agonistic displays.Apart from a few short attacks in two pairs in the food comp<strong>et</strong>itiontests, there was no serious aggression throughoutthe whole experiment. Also, the frequency <strong>of</strong> agonisticinteractions seemed to decrease from Food Test 1 to FoodTest 2. These data resemble findings <strong>of</strong> previous studiesin soci<strong>al</strong>ly housed adult silver fox vixens showing that thelevel <strong>of</strong> aggression was at its lowest in November comparedto October and December (Brøste, 2010; <strong>Hovland</strong> andBakken, 2010). The level <strong>of</strong> agonistic behaviour during thesecond test in November was predictive <strong>of</strong> later aggressiveinteractions, making it potenti<strong>al</strong>ly useful as a screeningm<strong>et</strong>hod for identifying compatible cage mates.On average, the test subject’s tot<strong>al</strong> time spent with thestimulus fox did not change significantly from the start tothe end <strong>of</strong> the experiment, further indicating that the soci<strong>al</strong>motivations <strong>of</strong> each pair were stable over the course <strong>of</strong> testing.However, pairs did seem to differ from each other in th<strong>et</strong>ype and degree <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> interaction, and in the degree towhich contact was rewarding. In this type <strong>of</strong> s<strong>et</strong>-up whereanim<strong>al</strong>s are compl<strong>et</strong>ely free to leave (and reunite with) thesoci<strong>al</strong> partner, interaction time may possibly act as a v<strong>al</strong>idindicator <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> motivation within pair because the timein shared contact is based on active choices made by bothanim<strong>al</strong>s. To illustrate, pair no. 2, in which foxes spent theleast tot<strong>al</strong> time tog<strong>et</strong>her, <strong>al</strong>so had the highest level <strong>of</strong> gapingsign<strong>al</strong>s during the operant testing and stopped interactingat DHR64 (despite the test subject’s continued visits to theshared compartment). As suggested by the high number<strong>of</strong> gaping sign<strong>al</strong>s, the motives for seeking contact in testsubject no. 2 may thus have been predominantly aggressive.Motivations for intraspecific contact are not <strong>al</strong>waysfor positive interactions, but may be for comp<strong>et</strong>ition andaggression, as previously demonstrated in, e.g. mice (Mayand Kennedy, 2009) and silver fox m<strong>al</strong>es (<strong>Hovland</strong> <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>.,2006). Consequently, such aggressive motives may thushave been one <strong>of</strong> the reasons why the stimulus anim<strong>al</strong>avoided further contact.Physic<strong>al</strong> aggression during the operant tri<strong>al</strong> constitutedmostly <strong>of</strong> upright displays which som<strong>et</strong>imes esc<strong>al</strong>ated int<strong>of</strong>ights. However, these fights did not appear to be severe,especi<strong>al</strong>ly since no injuries were observed. Moreover, it wasat times difficult to categorize these behaviours as purelyaggressive since they som<strong>et</strong>imes <strong>al</strong>so included elements<strong>of</strong> play. Nevertheless, these interactions were quite likelyrelated to mutu<strong>al</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> strength (and status) andtherefore had clear comp<strong>et</strong>itive aspects. The time spentperforming physic<strong>al</strong> aggression seemed substanti<strong>al</strong> in two<strong>of</strong> the pairs (3 and 5), however, compared to the tot<strong>al</strong> interactiontime this constituted 2.9% and 22.7% <strong>of</strong> the time,respectively. In gener<strong>al</strong>, there was no significant negativecorrelation b<strong>et</strong>ween time spent in physic<strong>al</strong> aggression andlevel <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> motivation; however, the pair (no. 5) withthe lowest RPP <strong>al</strong>so had the highest aggression level. Theexistence <strong>of</strong> such potenti<strong>al</strong> relationships should thereforebe re-examined with larger sample sizes. High levels <strong>of</strong>aggression may dev<strong>al</strong>ue potenti<strong>al</strong> benefits <strong>of</strong> soci<strong>al</strong> contact,maybe especi<strong>al</strong>ly in carnivores that are capable <strong>of</strong> inflictingserious bite injuries, making withdraw<strong>al</strong> opportunities particularlyimportant. As the test subjects’ spent three timesas long in the control cage than the shared compartmentwhen access to both was free, this strongly suggests thatthe opportunity to withdraw from soci<strong>al</strong> interaction was<strong>al</strong>so important.Nevertheless, the bulk <strong>of</strong> the time the foxes spenttog<strong>et</strong>her was peaceful, involving resting tog<strong>et</strong>her (about

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!