20.11.2018 Views

Cultural Transformation

Cultural Transformation

Cultural Transformation

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The e-Advocate<br />

Monthly<br />

…a Compilation of Works on:<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Romans 12:1-21 | Galatians 5:22-23<br />

1 Corinthians 9:24-27 | Matthew 16:18<br />

2 Corinthians 5:17 | Psalm 51:10<br />

John 3:16<br />

“Helping Individuals, Organizations & Communities<br />

Achieve Their Full Potential”<br />

Special Edition| May 2020.


Turning the Improbable<br />

Into the Exceptional!<br />

Page 2 of 134


The Advocacy Foundation, Inc.<br />

Helping Individuals, Organizations & Communities<br />

Achieve Their Full Potential<br />

Since its founding in 2003, The Advocacy Foundation has become recognized as an effective<br />

provider of support to those who receive our services, having real impact within the communities<br />

we serve. We are currently engaged in community and faith-based collaborative initiatives,<br />

having the overall objective of eradicating all forms of youth violence and correcting injustices<br />

everywhere. In carrying-out these initiatives, we have adopted the evidence-based strategic<br />

framework developed and implemented by the Office of Juvenile Justice & Delinquency<br />

Prevention (OJJDP).<br />

The stated objectives are:<br />

1. Community Mobilization;<br />

2. Social Intervention;<br />

3. Provision of Opportunities;<br />

4. Organizational Change and Development;<br />

5. Suppression [of illegal activities].<br />

Moreover, it is our most fundamental belief that in order to be effective, prevention and<br />

intervention strategies must be Community Specific, <strong>Cultural</strong>ly Relevant, Evidence-Based, and<br />

Collaborative. The Violence Prevention and Intervention programming we employ in<br />

implementing this community-enhancing framework include the programs further described<br />

throughout our publications, programs and special projects both domestically and<br />

internationally.<br />

www.TheAdvocacy.Foundation<br />

ISBN: ......... ../2017<br />

......... Printed in the USA<br />

Advocacy Foundation Publishers<br />

Philadlephia, PA<br />

(878) 222-0450 | Voice | Data | SMS<br />

Page 3 of 134


Page 4 of 134


Dedication<br />

______<br />

Every publication in our many series’ is dedicated to everyone, absolutely everyone, who by<br />

virtue of their calling and by Divine inspiration, direction and guidance, is on the battlefield dayafter-day<br />

striving to follow God’s will and purpose for their lives. And this is with particular affinity<br />

for those Spiritual warriors who are being transformed into excellence through daily academic,<br />

professional, familial, and other challenges.<br />

We pray that you will bear in mind:<br />

Matthew 19:26 (NIV)<br />

Jesus looked at them and said, "With man this is impossible,<br />

but with God all things are possible." (Emphasis added)<br />

To all of us who daily look past our circumstances, and naysayers, to what the Lord says we will<br />

accomplish:<br />

Blessings!!<br />

- The Advocacy Foundation, Inc.<br />

Page 5 of 134


Page 6 of 134


The Transformative Justice Project<br />

Eradicating Juvenile Delinquency Requires a Multi-Disciplinary Approach<br />

The way we accomplish all this is a follows:<br />

The Juvenile Justice system is incredibly overloaded, and<br />

Solutions-Based programs are woefully underfunded. Our<br />

precious children, therefore, particularly young people of<br />

color, often get the “swift” version of justice whenever they<br />

come into contact with the law.<br />

Decisions to build prison facilities are often based on<br />

elementary school test results, and our country incarcerates<br />

more of its young than any other nation on earth. So we at<br />

The Foundation labor to pull our young people out of the<br />

“school to prison” pipeline, and we then coordinate the efforts<br />

of the legal, psychological, governmental and educational<br />

professionals needed to bring an end to delinquency.<br />

We also educate families, police, local businesses, elected<br />

officials, clergy, and schools and other stakeholders about<br />

transforming whole communities, and we labor to change<br />

their thinking about the causes of delinquency with the goal<br />

of helping them embrace the idea of restoration for the young<br />

people in our care who demonstrate repentance for their<br />

mistakes.<br />

1. We vigorously advocate for charges reductions, wherever possible, in the adjudicatory (court)<br />

process, with the ultimate goal of expungement or pardon, in order to maximize the chances for<br />

our clients to graduate high school and progress into college, military service or the workforce<br />

without the stigma of a criminal record;<br />

2. We then enroll each young person into an Evidence-Based, Data-Driven Restorative Justice<br />

program designed to facilitate their rehabilitation and subsequent reintegration back into the<br />

community;<br />

3. While those projects are operating, we conduct a wide variety of ComeUnity-ReEngineering<br />

seminars and workshops on topics ranging from Juvenile Justice to Parental Rights, to Domestic<br />

issues to Police friendly contacts, to CBO and FBO accountability and compliance;<br />

4. Throughout the process, we encourage and maintain frequent personal contact between all<br />

parties;<br />

5 Throughout the process we conduct a continuum of events and fundraisers designed to facilitate<br />

collaboration among professionals and community stakeholders; and finally<br />

Page 7 of 134


6. 1 We disseminate Quarterly publications, like our e-Advocate series Newsletter and our e-Advocate<br />

Quarterly electronic Magazine to all regular donors in order to facilitate a lifelong learning process<br />

on the ever-evolving developments in the Justice system.<br />

And in addition to the help we provide for our young clients and their families, we also facilitate<br />

Community Engagement through the Restorative Justice process, thereby balancing the interesrs<br />

of local businesses, schools, clergy, elected officials, police, and all interested stakeholders. Through<br />

these efforts, relationships are rebuilt & strengthened, local businesses and communities are enhanced &<br />

protected from victimization, young careers are developed, and our precious young people are kept out<br />

of the prison pipeline.<br />

This is a massive undertaking, and we need all the help and financial support you can give! We plan to<br />

help 75 young persons per quarter-year (aggregating to a total of 250 per year) in each jurisdiction we<br />

serve) at an average cost of under $2,500 per client, per year.*<br />

Thank you in advance for your support!<br />

* FYI:<br />

1. The national average cost to taxpayers for minimum-security youth incarceration, is around<br />

$43,000.00 per child, per year.<br />

2. The average annual cost to taxpayers for maximun-security youth incarceration is well over<br />

$148,000.00 per child, per year.<br />

- (US News and World Report, December 9, 2014);<br />

3. In every jurisdiction in the nation, the Plea Bargain rate is above 99%.<br />

The Judicial system engages in a tri-partite balancing task in every single one of these matters, seeking<br />

to balance Rehabilitative Justice with Community Protection and Judicial Economy, and, although<br />

the practitioners work very hard to achieve positive outcomes, the scales are nowhere near balanced<br />

where people of color are involved.<br />

We must reverse this trend, which is right now working very much against the best interests of our young.<br />

Our young people do not belong behind bars.<br />

- Jack Johnson<br />

1<br />

In addition to supporting our world-class programming and support services, all regular donors receive our Quarterly e-Newsletter<br />

(The e-Advocate), as well as The e-Advocate Quarterly Magazine.<br />

Page 8 of 134


The Advocacy Foundation, Inc.<br />

Helping Individuals, Organizations & Communities<br />

Achieve Their Full Potential<br />

…a collection of works on<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

“Turning the Improbable Into the Exceptional”<br />

Atlanta<br />

Philadelphia<br />

______<br />

John C Johnson III<br />

Founder & CEO<br />

(878) 222-0450<br />

Voice | Data | SMS<br />

www.TheAdvocacy.Foundation<br />

Page 9 of 134


Page 10 of 134


Biblical Authority<br />

______<br />

Romans 12 (ESV)<br />

A Living Sacrifice<br />

12 I appeal to you therefore, brothers, by the mercies of God, to present your bodies as<br />

a living sacrifice, holy and acceptable to God, which is your spiritual worship. 2 Do not<br />

be conformed to this world, but be transformed by the renewal of your mind, that by<br />

testing you may discern what is the will of God, what is good and acceptable and<br />

perfect.<br />

Gifts of Grace<br />

3<br />

For by the grace given to me I say to everyone among you not to think of himself more<br />

highly than he ought to think, but to think with sober judgment, each according to the<br />

measure of faith that God has assigned. 4 For as in one body we have many members,<br />

and the members do not all have the same function, 5 so we, though many, are one<br />

body in Christ, and individually members one of another. 6 Having gifts that differ<br />

according to the grace given to us, let us use them: if prophecy, in proportion to our<br />

faith; 7 if service, in our serving; the one who teaches, in his teaching; 8 the one who<br />

exhorts, in his exhortation; the one who contributes, in generosity; the one who leads,<br />

with zeal; the one who does acts of mercy, with cheerfulness.<br />

Marks of the True Christian<br />

9<br />

Let love be genuine. Abhor what is evil; hold fast to what is good. 10 Love one another<br />

with brotherly affection. Outdo one another in showing honor. 11 Do not be slothful in<br />

zeal, be fervent in spirit, serve the Lord. 12 Rejoice in hope, be patient in tribulation, be<br />

constant in prayer. 13 Contribute to the needs of the saints and seek to show hospitality.<br />

14<br />

Bless those who persecute you; bless and do not curse them. 15 Rejoice with those<br />

who rejoice, weep with those who weep. 16 Live in harmony with one another. Do not be<br />

haughty, but associate with the lowly. Never be wise in your own sight. 17 Repay no one<br />

evil for evil, but give thought to do what is honorable in the sight of all. 18 If possible, so<br />

far as it depends on you, live peaceably with all. 19 Beloved, never avenge yourselves,<br />

but leave it [i] to the wrath of God, for it is written, “Vengeance is mine, I will repay, says<br />

the Lord.” 20 To the contrary, “if your enemy is hungry, feed him; if he is thirsty, give him<br />

something to drink; for by so doing you will heap burning coals on his head.” 21 Do not<br />

be overcome by evil, but overcome evil with good.<br />

Page 11 of 134


Galatians 5:22-23<br />

22<br />

But the fruit of the Spirit is love, joy, peace, patience, kindness, goodness,<br />

faithfulness, 23 gentleness, self-control; against such things there is no law.<br />

1 Corinthians 9:24-27<br />

24<br />

Do you not know that in a race all the runners run, but only one receives the prize? So<br />

run that you may obtain it. 25 Every athlete exercises self-control in all things. They do it<br />

to receive a perishable wreath, but we an imperishable. 26 So I do not run aimlessly; I do<br />

not box as one beating the air. 27 But I discipline my body and keep it under control, lest<br />

after preaching to others I myself should be disqualified.<br />

Matthew 16:18<br />

18<br />

And I tell you, you are Peter, and on this rock I will build my church, and the gates of<br />

hell shall not prevail against it.<br />

2 Corinthians 5:17<br />

17<br />

Therefore, if anyone is in Christ, he is a new creation. [a] The old has passed away;<br />

behold, the new has come.<br />

Psalm 51:10<br />

10<br />

Create in me a clean heart, O God,<br />

and renew a right spirit within me.<br />

John 3:16<br />

For God So Loved the World<br />

16<br />

“For God so loved the world, that he gave his only Son, that whoever believes in him<br />

should not perish but have eternal life.<br />

Page 12 of 134


Table of Contents<br />

…a compilation of works on<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Biblical Authority<br />

I. Introduction: Culture Change…………………………………….. 15<br />

II. Behavioral Economics……………………………………………. 19<br />

III. <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital……………………………………………………. 37<br />

IV. <strong>Cultural</strong> Economics.................................................................. 45<br />

V. Paradigm Shifting…………………………………………………. 51<br />

VI. Conspiracy Theory……………………………………………….. 59<br />

VII. Zeitgeist Theory…………….……………………………………. 71<br />

VIII. The Illuminati……………………………….............................. 77<br />

IX. The Star Chamber………………………………………………... 95<br />

X. References……………………………………………………........ 101<br />

______<br />

Attachments<br />

A. Fundamentals of <strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

B. Culture <strong>Transformation</strong>: An Executive View<br />

C. Intercultural Interactions and <strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Copyright © 2018 The Advocacy Foundation, Inc. All Rights Reserved.<br />

Page 13 of 134


Page 14 of 134


I. Introduction<br />

Culture Change<br />

Culture Change is a term used in public policy making that emphasizes the<br />

influence of cultural capital on individual and community behavior. It has been<br />

sometimes called repositioning of culture, which means the reconstruction of the cultural<br />

concept of a society. It places stress on the social and cultural capital determinants of<br />

decision making and the manner in which these interact with other factors like the<br />

availability of information or the financial incentives facing individuals to drive behavior.<br />

These cultural capital influences include the role of parenting, families and close<br />

associates; organizations such as schools and workplaces; communities and<br />

neighborhoods; and wider social influences such as the media. It is argued that this<br />

cultural capital manifests into specific values, attitudes or social norms which in turn<br />

guide the behavioral intentions that individuals adopt in regard to particular decisions or<br />

courses of action. These behavioral intentions interact with other factors driving<br />

Page 15 of 134


ehavior such as financial incentives, regulation and legislation, or levels of information,<br />

to drive actual behavior and ultimately feed back into underlying cultural capital.<br />

In general, cultural stereotypes present great resistance to change and to their own<br />

redefinition. Culture, often appears fixed to the observer at any one point in time<br />

because cultural mutations occur incrementally. The cultural change is a long-lasting<br />

process. Policymakers need to make a great effort to improve some basics aspects of a<br />

society’s cultural traits.<br />

Achieving culture change<br />

The term is used by Knott et al. of the Prime Minister's Strategy Unit in the publication:<br />

Achieving Culture Change: A Policy Framework (Knott et al., 2008). The paper sets out<br />

how public policy can achieve social and cultural change through 'downstream'<br />

interventions including fiscal incentives, legislation, regulation and information provision<br />

and also 'upstream' interventions such as parenting, peer and mentoring programs, or<br />

development of social and community networks.<br />

The key concepts the paper is based on include:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> capital - such as the attitudes, values, aspirations and sense of selfefficacy<br />

which influence behavior. <strong>Cultural</strong> capital is itself influenced by behavior<br />

over time<br />

The shifting social zeitgeist - whereby social norms and values that predominate<br />

within the cultural capital in society evolve in over time<br />

The process by which political narrative and new ideas and innovations shift the<br />

social zeitgeist over time within the constraint of the 'elastic band' of public<br />

opinion<br />

The process of behavioral normalization - whereby behavior and actions pass<br />

through into social and cultural norms (for example, Knott et al. argue that the UK<br />

experience of seat belt enforcement established and reinforced this as a social<br />

norm)<br />

The use of customer insight<br />

The importance of tailoring policy programs around an ecological model of<br />

human behavior to account for how policy will interact with cultural capital and<br />

affect it over time<br />

Knott et al. use examples from a range of policy areas to demonstrate how the culture<br />

change framework can be applied to policymaking. For example:<br />

Page 16 of 134


To encourage educational aspiration they recommend more use of early years<br />

and parenting interventions, an improved childhood offer, and development of<br />

positive narratives on education as well as integrated advisory systems, financial<br />

assistance and targeted social marketing approaches.<br />

I trust my community<br />

My community trusts me<br />

<br />

<br />

To promote healthy living and personal responsibility they recommend building<br />

healthy living into community infrastructure, building partnerships with schools<br />

and employers, more one-to-one support for wellbeing alongside use of<br />

regulation and legislation on unhealthy products, provision of robust health<br />

information and health marketing to promote adaptive forms of behavior.<br />

To develop environmentally sustainable norms they recommend reinforcing<br />

sustainability throughout policy narratives, using schools and the voluntary sector<br />

to promote environmental messages, development of infrastructure that make<br />

sustainable choices easy, together with a wider package of measures on fiscal<br />

incentives, regulation, advisory services and coalition movements.<br />

Page 17 of 134


Page 18 of 134


II. Behavioral Economics<br />

Behavioral Economics studies the effects of psychological, cognitive,<br />

emotional, cultural and social factors on the economic decisions of individuals and<br />

institutions and how those decisions vary from those implied by classical theory.<br />

Behavioral economics is primarily concerned with the bounds of rationality of economic<br />

agents. Behavioral models typically integrate insights from psychology, neuroscience<br />

and microeconomic theory. The study of behavioral economics includes how market<br />

decisions are made and the mechanisms that drive public choice. The three prevalent<br />

themes in behavioral economics are:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Heuristics: Humans make 95% of their decisions using mental shortcuts or rules<br />

of thumb.<br />

Framing: The collection of anecdotes and stereotypes that make up the mental<br />

filters individuals rely on to understand and respond to events.<br />

Market inefficiencies: These include mis-pricing and non-rational decision<br />

making.<br />

In 2002, psychologist Daniel Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Memorial Prize in<br />

Economic Sciences "for having integrated insights from psychological research into<br />

economic science, especially concerning human judgment and decision-making under<br />

uncertainty". In 2012, economist Robert J. Shiller received the Nobel Memorial Prize in<br />

Page 19 of 134


Economic Sciences for "for his empirical analysis of asset prices." (within the field of<br />

behavioral finance) In 2017, economist Richard Thaler was awarded the Nobel<br />

Memorial Prize in Economic Sciences for "his contributions to behavioral economics<br />

and his pioneering work in establishing that people are predictably irrational in ways that<br />

defy economic theory."<br />

History<br />

During the classical period of economics, microeconomics was closely linked to<br />

psychology. For example, Adam Smith wrote The Theory of Moral Sentiments, which<br />

proposed psychological explanations of individual behavior, including concerns about<br />

fairness and justice. Jeremy Bentham wrote extensively on the psychological<br />

underpinnings of utility. Then, during the development of neo-classical economics,<br />

economists sought to reshape the discipline as a natural science, deducing behavior<br />

from assumptions about the nature of economic agents. They developed the concept of<br />

homo economicus, whose behavior was fundamentally rational.<br />

Neo-classical economists did incorporate psychological explanations: this was true of<br />

Francis Edgeworth, Vilfredo Pareto and Irving Fisher. Economic psychology emerged<br />

in the 20th century in the works of Gabriel Tarde, George Katona, and Laszlo Garai.<br />

Expected utility and discounted utility models began to gain acceptance, generating<br />

testable hypotheses about decision-making given uncertainty and intertemporal<br />

consumption, respectively. Observed and repeatable anomalies eventually challenged<br />

those hypotheses, and further steps were taken by Maurice Allais, for example, in<br />

setting out the Allais paradox, a decision problem he first presented in 1953 that<br />

contradicts the expected utility hypothesis.<br />

In the 1960s cognitive psychology began to shed more light on the brain as an<br />

information processing device (in contrast to behaviorist models). Psychologists in this<br />

field, such as Ward Edwards, Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman began to compare<br />

their cognitive models of decision-making under risk and uncertainty to economic<br />

models of rational behavior. Mathematical psychology reflects a longstanding interest in<br />

preference transitivity and the measurement of utility.<br />

Bounded Rationality<br />

Bounded rationality is the idea that when individuals make decisions, their rationality is<br />

limited by the tractability of the decision problem, their cognitive limitations and the time<br />

available. Decision-makers in this view act as satisficers, seeking a satisfactory solution<br />

rather than an optimal one. Herbert A. Simon proposed bounded rationality as an<br />

alternative basis for the mathematical modeling of decision-making. It complements<br />

"rationality as optimization", which views decision-making as a fully rational process of<br />

finding an optimal choice given the information available. Simon used the analogy of a<br />

pair of scissors, where one blade represents human cognitive limitations and the other<br />

the "structures of the environment", illustrating how minds compensate for limited<br />

resources by exploiting known structural regularity in the environment.<br />

Page 20 of 134


Bounded rationality implicates the idea that humans take shortcuts that may lead to<br />

suboptimal decision-making. Behavioral economists engage in mapping the decision<br />

shortcuts that agents use in order to help increase the effectiveness of human decisionmaking.<br />

One treatment of this idea comes from Cass Sunstein and Richard Thaler's<br />

Nudge. Sunstein and Thaler recommend that choice architectures are modified in light<br />

of human agents' bounded rationality. A widely cited proposal from Sunstein and Thaler<br />

urges that healthier food be placed at sight level in order to increase the likelihood that a<br />

person will opt for that choice instead of less healthy option. Some critics of Nudge have<br />

lodged attacks that modifying choice architectures will lead to people becoming worse<br />

decision-makers.<br />

Prospect Theory<br />

In 1979, Kahneman and Tversky published Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision<br />

Under Risk, that used cognitive psychology to explain various divergences of economic<br />

decision making from neo-classical theory. Prospect theory has two stages: an editing<br />

stage and an evaluation stage.<br />

In the editing stage, risky situations are simplified using various heuristics. In the<br />

evaluation phase, risky alternatives are evaluated using various psychological principles<br />

that include:<br />

<br />

Reference Dependence: When evaluating outcomes, the decision maker<br />

considers a "reference level". Outcomes are then compared to the reference<br />

point and classified as "gains" if greater than the reference point and "losses" if<br />

less than the reference point.<br />

Page 21 of 134


Loss Aversion: Losses are avoided more than equivalent gains are sought. In<br />

their 1992 paper, Kahneman and Tversky found the median coefficient of loss<br />

aversion to be about 2.25, i.e., losses hurt about 2.25 times more than equivalent<br />

gains reward.<br />

Non-Linear Probability Weighting: Decision makers overweight small<br />

probabilities and underweight large probabilities—this gives rise to the inverse-S<br />

shaped "probability weighting function".<br />

<br />

Diminishing Sensitivity to Gains and Losses: As the size of the gains and<br />

losses relative to the reference point increase in absolute value, the marginal<br />

effect on the decision maker's utility or satisfaction falls.<br />

Prospect theory is able to explain everything that the two main existing decision<br />

theories—expected utility theory and rank dependent utility theory—can explain.<br />

Further, prospect theory has been used to explain phenomena that existing decision<br />

theories have great difficulty in explaining. These include backward bending labor<br />

supply curves, asymmetric price elasticities, tax evasion and co-movement of stock<br />

prices and consumption.<br />

In 1992, in the Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, Kahneman and Tversky gave a revised<br />

account of prospect theory that they called cumulative prospect theory. The new theory<br />

eliminated the editing phase in prospect theory and focused just on the evaluation<br />

phase. Its main feature was that it allowed for non-linear probability weighting in a<br />

cumulative manner, which was originally suggested in John Quiggin's rank-dependent<br />

utility theory.<br />

Psychological traits such as overconfidence, projection bias, and the effects of limited<br />

attention are now part of the theory. Other developments include a conference at the<br />

University of Chicago, a special behavioral economics edition of the Quarterly Journal of<br />

Economics ("In Memory of Amos Tversky"), and Kahneman's 2002 Nobel Prize for<br />

having "integrated insights from psychological research into economic science,<br />

especially concerning human judgment and decision-making under uncertainty".<br />

Intertemporal Choice<br />

Behavioral economics has been applied to intertemporal choice. Intertemporal choice is<br />

defined as making a decision and having the effects of such decision happening in a<br />

different time. Intertemporal choice behavior is largely inconsistent, as exemplified by<br />

George Ainslie's hyperbolic discounting—one of the prominently studied observations—<br />

and further developed by David Laibson, Ted O'Donoghue and Matthew Rabin.<br />

Hyperbolic discounting describes the tendency to discount outcomes in the near future<br />

more than outcomes in the far future. This pattern of discounting is dynamically<br />

inconsistent (or time-inconsistent), and therefore inconsistent with basic models of<br />

rational choice, since the rate of discount between time t and t+1 will be low at time t-1<br />

Page 22 of 134


when t is the near future, but high at time t when t is the present and time t+1 is the near<br />

future.<br />

This pattern can also be explained through models of sub-additive discounting that<br />

distinguish the delay and interval of discounting: people are less patient (per-time-unit)<br />

over shorter intervals regardless of when they occur.<br />

Other Areas of Research<br />

Other branches of behavioral economics enrich the model of the utility function without<br />

implying inconsistency in preferences. Ernst Fehr, Armin Falk, and Rabin studied<br />

fairness, inequity aversion and reciprocal altruism, weakening the neoclassical<br />

assumption of perfect selfishness. This work is particularly applicable to wage setting.<br />

The work on "intrinsic motivation by Gneezy and Rustichini and "identity" by Akerlof and<br />

Kranton assumes that agents derive utility from adopting personal and social norms in<br />

addition to conditional expected utility. According to Aggarwal, in addition to behavioral<br />

deviations from rational equilibrium, markets are also likely to suffer from lagged<br />

responses, search costs, externalities of the commons, and other frictions making it<br />

difficult to disentangle behavioral effects in market behavior.<br />

Page 23 of 134


"Conditional expected utility" is a form of reasoning where the individual has an illusion<br />

of control, and calculates the probabilities of external events and hence their utility as a<br />

function of their own action, even when they have no causal ability to affect those<br />

external events.<br />

Behavioral economics caught on among the general public with the success of books<br />

such as Dan Ariely's Predictably Irrational. Practitioners of the discipline have studied<br />

quasi-public policy topics such as broadband mapping.<br />

Applications for behavioral economics include the modeling of the consumer decisionmaking<br />

process for applications in artificial intelligence and machine learning. The<br />

Silicon Valley-based start-up Singularities is using the AGM postulates proposed by<br />

Alchourrón, Gärdenfors, and Makinson—the formalization of the concepts of beliefs and<br />

change for rational entities—in a symbolic logic to create a "machine learning and<br />

deduction engine that uses the latest data science and big data algorithms in order to<br />

generate the content and conditional rules (counterfactuals) that capture customer's<br />

behaviors and beliefs".<br />

Applications of behavioral economics also exist in other disciplines, for example in the<br />

area of supply chain management.<br />

Natural Experiments<br />

From a biological point of view, human behaviors are essentially the same during crises<br />

accompanied by stock market crashes and during bubble growth when share prices<br />

exceed historic highs. During those periods, most market participants see something<br />

new for themselves, and this inevitably induces a stress response in them with<br />

accompanying changes in their endocrine profiles and motivations. The result is<br />

quantitative and qualitative changes in behavior. This is one example where behavior<br />

affecting economics and finance can be observed and variably-contrasted using<br />

behavioral economics.<br />

Behavioral economics' usefulness applies beyond environments similar to stock<br />

exchanges. Selfish-reasoning, 'adult behaviors', and similar, can be identified within<br />

criminal-concealment(s), and legal-deficiencies and neglect of different types can be<br />

observed and discovered. Awareness of indirect consequence (or lack of), at least in<br />

potential with different experimental models and methods, can be used as well—<br />

behavioral economics' potential uses are broad, but its reliability needs scrutiny.<br />

Underestimation of the role of novelty as a stressor is the primary shortcoming of<br />

current approaches for market research. It is necessary to account for the biologically<br />

determined diphasisms of human behavior in everyday low-stress conditions and in<br />

response to stressors.<br />

Criticism<br />

Page 24 of 134


Critics of behavioral economics typically stress the rationality of economic agents. They<br />

contend that experimentally observed behavior has limited application to market<br />

situations, as learning opportunities and competition ensure at least a close<br />

approximation of rational behavior.<br />

Others note that cognitive theories, such as prospect theory, are models of decision<br />

making, not generalized economic behavior, and are only applicable to the sort of onceoff<br />

decision problems presented to experiment participants or survey respondents.<br />

A notable concern is that despite a great deal of rhetoric, no consistent behavioral<br />

theory has yet been espoused. Behavioral economists have proposed no unified theory.<br />

Until that happens, behavioral economics is a collection of observations.<br />

Traditional economists are skeptical of the experimental and survey-based techniques<br />

that behavioral economics uses extensively. Economists typically stress revealed<br />

preferences over stated preferences (from surveys) in the determination of economic<br />

value. Experiments and surveys are at risk of systemic biases, strategic behavior and<br />

lack of incentive compatibility.<br />

Page 25 of 134


Responses<br />

Matthew Rabin dismisses these criticisms, countering that consistent results typically<br />

are obtained in multiple situations and geographies and can produce good theoretical<br />

insight. Behavioral economists however responded to these criticisms by focusing on<br />

field studies rather than lab experiments. Some economists see a fundamental schism<br />

between experimental economics and behavioral economics, but prominent behavioral<br />

and experimental economists tend to share techniques and approaches in answering<br />

common questions. For example, behavioral economists are investigating<br />

neuroeconomics, which is entirely experimental and has not been verified in the field.<br />

Other proponents note that neoclassical models often fail to predict real world<br />

outcomes. Behavioral insights can also influence neoclassical models. Behavioral<br />

economists note that these revised models reach the same correct predictions as the<br />

traditional models and correctly predict some outcomes where traditional models failed.<br />

The epistemological, ontological, and methodological components of behavioral<br />

economics are increasingly debated, in particular by historians of economics and<br />

economic methodologists.<br />

According to some researchers, when studying the mechanisms that form the basis of<br />

decision-making, especially financial decision-making, it is necessary to recognize that<br />

most decisions are made under stress because, "Stress is the nonspecific body<br />

response to any demands presented to it."<br />

Nudge Theory<br />

Applied Issues<br />

Nudge is a concept in behavioral science, political theory and economics which<br />

proposes positive reinforcement and indirect suggestions as ways to influence the<br />

behavior and decision making of groups or individuals. Nudging contrasts with other<br />

ways to achieve compliance, such as education, legislation or enforcement. The<br />

concept has influenced British and American politicians. Several nudge units exist<br />

around the world at the national level (UK, Germany, Japan and others) as well as at<br />

the international level (OECD, World Bank, UN).<br />

The first formulation of the term and associated principles was developed in cybernetics<br />

by James Wilk before 1995 and described by Brunel University academic D. J. Stewart<br />

as "the art of the nudge" (sometimes referred to as micronudges). It also drew on<br />

methodological influences from clinical psychotherapy tracing back to Gregory Bateson,<br />

including contributions from Milton Erickson, Watzlawick, Weakland and Fisch, and Bill<br />

O'Hanlon. In this variant, the nudge is a microtargetted design geared towards a specific<br />

group of people, irrespective of the scale of intended intervention.<br />

In 2008, Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein's book Nudge: Improving Decisions About<br />

Health, Wealth, and Happiness brought nudge theory to prominence. It also gained a<br />

Page 26 of 134


following among US and UK politicians, in the private sector and in public health. The<br />

authors refer to influencing behaviour without coercion as libertarian paternalism and<br />

the influencers as choice architects. Thaler and Sunstein defined their concept as:<br />

A nudge, as we will use the term, is any aspect of the choice architecture that alters<br />

people's behavior in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly<br />

changing their economic incentives. To count as a mere nudge, the intervention must be<br />

easy and cheap to avoid. Nudges are not mandates. Putting fruit at eye level counts as a<br />

nudge. Banning junk food does not.<br />

In this form, drawing on behavioral economics, the nudge is more generally applied to<br />

influence behavior.<br />

One of the most frequently cited examples of a nudge is the etching of the image of a<br />

housefly into the men's room urinals at Amsterdam's Schiphol Airport, which is intended<br />

to "improve the aim".<br />

Nudging techniques aim to use judgmental heuristics to our advantage. In other words,<br />

a nudge alters the environment so that when heuristic, or System 1, decision-making is<br />

used, the resulting choice will be the most positive or desired outcome. [41] An example<br />

of such a nudge is switching the placement of junk food in a store, so that fruit and other<br />

healthy options are located next to the cash register, while junk food is relocated to<br />

another part of the store.<br />

In 2008, the United States appointed Sunstein, who helped develop the theory, as<br />

administrator of the Office of Information and Regulatory Affairs.<br />

Page 27 of 134


Notable applications of nudge theory include the formation of the British Behavioural<br />

Insights Team in 2010. It is often called the "Nudge Unit", at the British Cabinet Office,<br />

headed by David Halpern.<br />

Both Prime Minister David Cameron and President Barack Obama sought to employ<br />

nudge theory to advance domestic policy goals during their terms.<br />

In Australia, the government of New South Wales established a Behavioural Insights<br />

community of practice.<br />

Nudge theory has also been applied to business management and corporate culture,<br />

such as in relation to health, safety and environment (HSE) and human resources.<br />

Regarding its application to HSE, one of the primary goals of nudge is to achieve a<br />

"zero accident culture".<br />

Leading Silicon Valley companies are forerunners in applying nudge theory in corporate<br />

setting. These companies are using nudges in various forms to increase productivity<br />

and happiness of employees. Recently, further companies are gaining interest in using<br />

what is called "nudge management" to improve the productivity of their white-collar<br />

workers.<br />

There are now more than 80 countries in which behavioral insights are used.<br />

Criticisms<br />

Nudging has also been criticised. Tammy Boyce, from public health foundation The<br />

King's Fund, has said: "We need to move away from short-term, politically motivated<br />

initiatives such as the 'nudging people' idea, which are not based on any good evidence<br />

and don't help people make long-term behaviour changes."<br />

Cass Sunstein has responded to critiques at length in his The Ethics of Influence<br />

making the case in favor of nudging against charges that nudges diminish autonomy,<br />

threaten dignity, violate liberties, or reduce welfare. Ethicists have debated this<br />

rigorously. These charges have been made by various participants in the debate from<br />

Bovens to Goodwin. Wilkinson for example charges nudges for being manipulative,<br />

while others such as Yeung question their scientific credibility.<br />

Some, such as Hausman & Welch have inquired whether nudging should be<br />

permissible on grounds of (distributive) justice; Lepenies & Malecka have questioned<br />

whether nudges are compatible with the rule of law. Similarly, legal scholars have<br />

discussed the role of nudges and the law.<br />

Behavioral economists such as Bob Sugden have pointed out that the underlying<br />

normative benchmark of nudging is still homo oeconomicus, despite the proponents'<br />

claim to the contrary.<br />

Page 28 of 134


It has been remarked that nudging is also a euphemism for psychological manipulation<br />

as practiced in social engineering.<br />

There exists an anticipation and, simultaneously, implicit criticism of the nudge theory in<br />

works of Hungarian social psychologists who emphasize the active participation in the<br />

nudge of its target (Ferenc Merei), Laszlo Garai).<br />

Behacnce<br />

The central issue in behavioral finance is explaining why market participants make<br />

irrational systematic errors contrary to assumption of rational market participants. Such<br />

errors affect prices and returns, creating market inefficiencies. The study of behavioral<br />

finance also investigates how other participants take advantage (arbitrage) of such<br />

errors and market inefficiencies.<br />

Behavioral finance highlights inefficiencies, such as under- or over-reactions to<br />

information, as causes of market trends and, in extreme cases, of bubbles and crashes.<br />

Such reactions have been attributed to limited investor attention, overconfidence, overoptimism,<br />

mimicry (herding instinct) and noise trading. Technical analysts consider<br />

behavioral finance to be behavioral economics' "academic cousin" and the theoretical<br />

basis for technical analysis.<br />

Other key observations include the asymmetry between decisions to acquire or keep<br />

resources, known as the "bird in the bush" paradox, and loss aversion, the<br />

unwillingness to let go of a valued possession. Loss aversion appears to manifest itself<br />

in investor behavior as a reluctance to sell shares or other equity if doing so would<br />

result in a nominal loss. It may also help explain why housing prices rarely/slowly<br />

decline to market clearing levels during periods of low demand.<br />

Benartzi and Thaler, applying a version of prospect theory, claim to have solved the<br />

equity premium puzzle, something conventional finance models so far have been<br />

unable to do. Experimental finance applies the experimental method, e.g., creating an<br />

artificial market through some kind of simulation software to study people's decisionmaking<br />

process and behavior in financial markets.<br />

Page 29 of 134


Quantitative Behavioral Finance<br />

Quantitative behavioral finance uses mathematical and statistical methodology to<br />

understand behavioral biases. In marketing research, a study shows little evidence that<br />

escalating biases impact marketing decisions. Leading contributors include Gunduz<br />

Caginalp (Editor of the Journal of Behavioral Finance from 2001–04), and collaborators<br />

include 2002 Nobel Laureate Vernon Smith, David Porter, Don Balenovich, Vladimira<br />

Ilieva and Ahmet Duran, and Ray Sturm.<br />

Financial Models<br />

Some financial models used in money management and asset valuation incorporate<br />

behavioral finance parameters. Examples:<br />

Thaler's model of price reactions to information, with three phases<br />

(underreaction, adjustment, and overreaction), creating a price trend.<br />

One characteristic of overreaction is that average returns following announcements of<br />

good news is lower than following bad news. In other words, overreaction occurs if the<br />

market reacts too strongly or for too long to news, thus requiring adjustment in the<br />

opposite direction. As a result, outperforming assets in one period are likely to<br />

underperform in the following period. This also applies to customers' irrational<br />

purchasing habits.<br />

<br />

The stock image coefficient.<br />

Criticisms<br />

Critics such as Eugene Fama typically support the efficient-market hypothesis. They<br />

contend that behavioral finance is more a collection of anomalies than a true branch of<br />

finance and that these anomalies are either quickly priced out of the market or<br />

explained by appealing to market microstructure arguments. However, individual<br />

cognitive biases are distinct from social biases; the former can be averaged out by the<br />

market, while the other can create positive feedback loops that drive the market further<br />

and further from a "fair price" equilibrium. Similarly, for an anomaly to violate market<br />

efficiency, an investor must be able to trade against it and earn abnormal profits; this is<br />

not the case for many anomalies.<br />

A specific example of this criticism appears in some explanations of the equity premium<br />

puzzle. It is argued that the cause is entry barriers (both practical and psychological)<br />

and that returns between stocks and bonds should equalize as electronic resources<br />

open up the stock market to more traders. In response, others contend that most<br />

personal investment funds are managed through superannuation funds, minimizing the<br />

effect of these putative entry barriers. In addition, professional investors and fund<br />

managers seem to hold more bonds than one would expect given return differentials.<br />

Page 30 of 134


Behavioral Game Theory<br />

Behavioral game theory, invented by Colin Camerer, analyzes interactive strategic<br />

decisions and behavior using the methods of game theory, experimental economics,<br />

and experimental psychology. Experiments include testing deviations from typical<br />

simplifications of economic theory such as the independence axiom and neglect of<br />

altruism, fairness, and framing effects. On the positive side, the method has been<br />

applied to interactive learning and social preferences. As a research program, the<br />

subject is a development of the last three decades.<br />

Economic Reasoning in Animals<br />

A handful of comparative psychologists have attempted to demonstrate quasi-economic<br />

reasoning in non-human animals. Early attempts along these lines focus on the<br />

behavior of rats and pigeons. These studies draw on the tenets of comparative<br />

psychology, where the main goal is to discover analogs to human behavior in<br />

experimentally-tractable non-human animals. They are also methodologically similar to<br />

Page 31 of 134


the work of Ferster and Skinner. Methodological similarities aside, early researchers in<br />

non-human economics deviate from behaviorism in their terminology. Although such<br />

studies are set up primarily in an operant conditioning chamber using food rewards for<br />

pecking/bar-pressing behavior, the researchers describe pecking and bar-pressing not<br />

in terms of reinforcement and stimulus-response relationships but instead in terms of<br />

work, demand, budget, and labor. Recent studies have adopted a slightly different<br />

approach, taking a more evolutionary perspective, comparing economic behavior of<br />

humans to a species of non-human primate, the capuchin monkey.<br />

Animal Studies<br />

Many early studies of non-human economic reasoning were performed on rats and<br />

pigeons in an operant conditioning chamber. These studies looked at things like peck<br />

rate (in the case of the pigeon) and bar-pressing rate (in the case of the rat) given<br />

certain conditions of reward. Early researchers claim, for example, that response<br />

pattern (pecking/bar-pressing rate) is an appropriate analogy to human labor supply.<br />

Researchers in this field advocate for the appropriateness of using animal economic<br />

behavior to understand the elementary components of human economic behavior. In a<br />

paper by Battalio, Green, and Kagel, they write,<br />

Space considerations do not permit a detailed discussion of the reasons why<br />

economists should take seriously the investigation of economic theories using<br />

nonhuman subjects....[Studies of economic behavior in non-human animals] provide a<br />

laboratory for identifying, testing, and better understanding general laws of economic<br />

behavior. Use of this laboratory is predicated on the fact that behavior as well as<br />

structure vary continuously across species, and that principles of economic behavior<br />

would be unique among behavioral principles if they did not apply, with some variation,<br />

of course, to the behavior of nonhumans.<br />

Labor Supply<br />

The typical laboratory environment to study labor supply in pigeons is set up as follows.<br />

Pigeons are first deprived of food. Since the animals become hungry, food becomes<br />

highly desired. The pigeons are then placed in an operant conditioning chamber and<br />

through orienting and exploring the environment of the chamber they discover that by<br />

pecking a small disk located on one side of the chamber, food is delivered to them. In<br />

effect, pecking behavior becomes reinforced, as it is associated with food. Before long,<br />

the pigeon pecks at the disk (or stimulus) regularly.<br />

In this circumstance, the pigeon is said to "work" for the food by pecking. The food,<br />

then, is thought of as the currency. The value of the currency can be adjusted in several<br />

ways, including the amount of food delivered, the rate of food delivery and the type of<br />

food delivered (some foods are more desirable than others).<br />

Economic behavior similar to that observed in humans is discovered when the hungry<br />

pigeons stop working/work less when the reward is reduced. Researchers argue that<br />

Page 32 of 134


this is similar to labor supply behavior in humans. That is, like humans (who, even in<br />

need, will only work so much for a given wage), the pigeons demonstrate decreases in<br />

pecking (work) when the reward (value) is reduced.<br />

Demand<br />

In human economics, a typical demand curve has negative slope. This means that as<br />

the price of a certain good increases, the amount that consumers are willing and able to<br />

purchase decreases. Researchers<br />

studying the demand curves of nonhuman<br />

animals, such as<br />

rats, also find downward<br />

slopes.<br />

Researchers have studied<br />

demand in rats in a<br />

manner<br />

distinct<br />

from<br />

studying<br />

labor<br />

supply<br />

in<br />

pigeons.<br />

Specifically,<br />

in an operant<br />

conditioning<br />

chamber<br />

containing rats as<br />

experimental<br />

subjects, we require<br />

them to press a bar,<br />

instead of pecking a small<br />

disk, to receive a reward.<br />

The reward can be food (reward<br />

pellets), water, or a commodity drink<br />

such as cherry cola. Unlike in previous pigeon studies, where the work analog was<br />

pecking and the monetary analog was reward, the work analog in this experiment is barpressing.<br />

Under these circumstances, the researchers claim that changing the number<br />

of bar presses required to obtain a commodity item is analogous to changing the price<br />

of a commodity item in human economics.<br />

Page 33 of 134


In effect, results of demand studies in non-human animals show that, as the barpressing<br />

requirement (cost) increase, the number of times an animal presses the bar<br />

equal to or greater than the bar-pressing requirement (payment) decreases.<br />

Evolutionary Psychology<br />

An evolutionary psychology perspective states that many of the perceived limitations in<br />

rational choice can be explained as being rational in the context of maximizing biological<br />

fitness in the ancestral environment, but not necessarily in the current one. Thus, when<br />

living at subsistence level where a reduction of resources may result in death, it may<br />

have been rational to place a greater value on preventing losses than on obtaining<br />

gains. It may also explain behavioral differences between groups, such as males being<br />

less risk-averse than females since males have more variable reproductive success<br />

than females. While unsuccessful risk-seeking may limit reproductive success for both<br />

sexes, males may potentially increase their reproductive success from successful riskseeking<br />

much more than females can.<br />

Artificial Intelligence<br />

Much of the decisions are more and more made either by human beings with the<br />

assistance of artificial intelligent machines or wholly made by these machines. Tshilidzi<br />

Marwala and Evan Hurwitz in their book, studied the utility of behavioral economics in<br />

such situations and concluded that these intelligent machines reduce the impact of<br />

bounded rational decision making. In particular, they observed that these intelligent<br />

machines reduce the degree of information asymmetry in the market, improve decision<br />

making and thus making markets more rational.<br />

The use of AI machines in the market in applications such as online trading and<br />

decision making has changed major economic theories. Other theories where AI has<br />

had impact include in rational choice, rational expectations, game theory, Lewis turning<br />

point, portfolio optimization and counterfactual thinking.<br />

Experimental Economics<br />

Related Fields<br />

Experimental economics is the application of experimental methods [93] to study<br />

economic questions. Data collected in experiments are used to estimate effect size, test<br />

the validity of economic theories, and illuminate market mechanisms. Economic<br />

experiments usually use cash to motivate subjects, in order to mimic real-world<br />

incentives. Experiments are used to help understand how and why markets and other<br />

exchange systems function as they do. Experimental economics have also expanded to<br />

understand institutions and the law (experimental law and economics). [94]<br />

A fundamental aspect of the subject is design of experiments. Experiments may be<br />

conducted in the field or in laboratory settings, whether of individual or group behavior.<br />

Page 34 of 134


Variants of the subject outside such formal confines include natural and quasi-natural<br />

experiments.<br />

Neuroeconomics<br />

Neuroeconomics is an interdisciplinary field that seeks to explain human decision<br />

making, the ability to process multiple alternatives and to follow a course of action. It<br />

studies how economic behavior can shape our understanding of the brain, and how<br />

neuroscientific discoveries can constrain and guide models of economics.<br />

It combines research methods from neuroscience, experimental and behavioral<br />

economics, and cognitive and social psychology. As research into decision-making<br />

behavior becomes increasingly computational, it has also incorporated new approaches<br />

Page 35 of 134


from theoretical biology, computer science, and mathematics. Neuroeconomics studies<br />

decision making by using a combination of tools from these fields so as to avoid the<br />

shortcomings that arise from a single-perspective approach. In mainstream economics,<br />

expected utility (EU) and the concept of rational agents are still being used. Many<br />

economic behaviors are not fully explained by these models, such as heuristics and<br />

framing.<br />

Behavioral economics emerged to account for these anomalies by integrating social,<br />

cognitive, and emotional factors in understanding economic decisions. Neuroeconomics<br />

adds another layer by using neuroscientific methods in understanding the interplay<br />

between economic behavior and neural mechanisms. By using tools from various fields,<br />

some scholars claim that neuroeconomics offers a more integrative way of<br />

understanding decision making.<br />

Page 36 of 134


III. <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital<br />

In sociology, cultural capital consists of the social assets of a person (education,<br />

intellect, style of speech and dress, etc.) that promote social mobility in a stratified<br />

society. <strong>Cultural</strong> capital functions as a social-relation within an economy of practices<br />

(system of exchange), and comprises all of the material and symbolic goods, without<br />

distinction, that society considers rare and worth seeking. As a social relation within a<br />

system of exchange, cultural capital includes the accumulated cultural knowledge that<br />

confers social status and power.<br />

In "<strong>Cultural</strong> Reproduction and Social Reproduction" (1977), Pierre Bourdieu and Jean-<br />

Claude Passeron presented cultural capital to conceptually explain the differences<br />

among the levels of performance and<br />

academic<br />

achievement of<br />

children within the educational system<br />

of France in the 1960s; and further developed the<br />

concept in the essay "The Forms of Capital" (1985) and in the book The State Nobility:<br />

Élite Schools in the Field of Power (1996).<br />

Types of Capital<br />

In the sociological essay, "The Forms of Capital" (1985), Pierre Bourdieu identifies three<br />

categories of capital:<br />

Page 37 of 134


Types<br />

1. Economic Capital: command of economic resources (money, assets, property).<br />

2. Social Capital: actual and potential resources linked to the possession of a<br />

durable network of institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance and<br />

recognition.<br />

3. <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital: A person's education (knowledge and intellectual skills) that<br />

provides advantage in achieving a higher social-status in society.<br />

There are three types of cultural capital: (i) Embodied capital; (ii) Objectified capital; and<br />

(iii) Institutionalised capital:<br />

1. Embodied <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital comprises the knowledge that is consciously<br />

acquired and the passively inherited, by socialization to culture and tradition.<br />

Unlike property, cultural capital is not transmissible, but is acquired over time, as<br />

it is impressed upon the person's habitus (character and way of thinking), which,<br />

in turn, becomes more receptive to similar cultural influences. Linguistic<br />

cultural capital is the mastery of language and its relations; the embodied<br />

cultural capital, which is a person's means of communication and selfpresentation,<br />

acquired from the national culture. [7]<br />

2. Objectified <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital comprises the person's property (e.g. a work of art,<br />

scientific instruments, etc.) that can be transmitted for economic profit (buyingand-selling)<br />

and for symbolically conveying the possession of cultural capital<br />

facilitated by owning such things. Yet, whilst possessing a work of art (objectified<br />

cultural-capital) the person can consume the art (understand its cultural meaning)<br />

only with the proper conceptual and historical foundations of prior cultural-capital.<br />

As such, cultural capital is not transmitted in the sale of the work of art, except by<br />

coincidental and independent causation, when the seller explains the artwork's<br />

significance to the buyer.<br />

3. Institutionalized <strong>Cultural</strong> Capital comprises an institution's formal recognition<br />

of a person's cultural capital, usually academic credentials or professional<br />

qualifications. The greatest social role of institutionalized cultural-capital is in the<br />

labor market (a job), wherein it allows the expression of the person's array of<br />

cultural capital as qualitative and quantitative measurements (which are<br />

compared against the measures of cultural capital of other people). The<br />

institutional recognition facilitates the conversion of cultural capital into economic<br />

capital, by serving as a heuristic (practical solution) with which the seller can<br />

describe his or her cultural capital to the buyer.<br />

Habitus and Field<br />

Page 38 of 134


The cultural capital of a person is linked to his or her habitus (embodied disposition and<br />

tendencies) and field (social positions), which are configured as a social-relation<br />

structure. The field is the place of social position that is constituted by the conflicts that<br />

occur when social groups endeavour to establish and define what is cultural capital,<br />

within a given social space; therefore, depending upon the social field, one type of<br />

cultural capital can simultaneously be legitimate and illegitimate. In that way, the<br />

legitimization (societal recognition) of a type of cultural capital can be arbitrary and<br />

derived from symbolic capital.<br />

The habitus of a person is composed of the intellectual dispositions inculcated to him or<br />

her by family and the familial environment, and are manifested according to the nature<br />

of the person. As such, the social formation of a person's habitus is influenced by family,<br />

by objective changes in social class, and by social interactions with other people in daily<br />

life; moreover, the habitus of a person also changes when he or she changes social<br />

positions within the field.<br />

Theoretical Research<br />

The concept of cultural capital has received widespread attention all around the world,<br />

from theorists and researchers alike. It is mostly employed in relation to the education<br />

system, but on the odd occasion has been used or developed in other discourses. Use<br />

of Bourdieu's cultural capital can be broken up into a number of basic categories. First,<br />

Page 39 of 134


are those who explore the theory as a possible means of explanation or employ it as the<br />

framework for their research. Second, are those who build on or expand Bourdieu's<br />

theory. Finally, there are those who attempt to disprove Bourdieu's findings or to<br />

discount them in favour of an alternative theory. The majority of these works deal with<br />

Bourdieu's theory in relation to education, only a small number apply his theory to other<br />

instances of inequality in society.<br />

Traditional Application<br />

Those researchers and theorists who explore or employ Bourdieu's theory use it in a<br />

similar way as it was articulated by Bourdieu. They usually apply it uncritically, and<br />

depending on the measurable indicators of cultural capital and the fields within which<br />

they measure it, Bourdieu's theory either works to support their argument totally, or in a<br />

qualified way. These works to help portray the usefulness of Bourdieu's concept in<br />

analysing (mainly educational) inequality but they do not add anything to the theory.<br />

One work which does employ Bourdieu's work in an enlightening way is that of<br />

Emirbayer & Williams (2005) who use Bourdieu's notion of fields and capital to examine<br />

the power relations in the field of social services, particularly homeless shelters. The<br />

authors talk of the two separate fields that operate in the same geographic location (the<br />

shelter) and the types of capital that are legitimate and valued in each. Specifically they<br />

show how homeless people can possess "staff-sanctioned capital" or "client-sanctioned<br />

capital" (2005:92) and show how in the shelter, they are both at the same time,<br />

desirable and undesirable, valued and disparaged, depending on which of the two fields<br />

they are operating in. Although the authors do not clearly define staff-sanctioned and<br />

client-sanctioned capital as cultural capital, and state that usually the resources that<br />

form these two capitals are gathered from a person's life as opposed to their family, it<br />

can be seen how Bourdieu's theory of cultural capital can be a valuable theory in<br />

analyzing inequality in any social setting.<br />

Expansion<br />

A number of works expand Bourdieu's theory of cultural capital in a beneficial manner,<br />

without deviating from Bourdieu's framework of the different forms of capital. In fact,<br />

these authors can be seen to explore unarticulated areas of Bourdieu's theory as<br />

opposed to constructing a new theory. For instance, Stanton-Salazar & Dornbusch<br />

(1995:121) examine how those people with the desired types of cultural (and linguistic)<br />

capital in a school transform this capital into "instrumental relations" or social capital<br />

with institutional agents who can transmit valuable resources to the person, furthering<br />

their success in the school. They state that this is simply an elaboration of Bourdieu's<br />

theory. Similarly, Dumais (2002) introduces the variable of gender to determine the<br />

ability of cultural capital to increase educational achievement. The author shows how<br />

gender and social class interact to produce different benefits from cultural capital. In fact<br />

in Distinction (1984:107), Bourdieu states "sexual properties are as inseparable from<br />

class properties as the yellowness of lemons is inseparable from its acidity". He simply<br />

Page 40 of 134


did not articulate the differences attributable to gender in his general theory of<br />

reproduction in the education system.<br />

On the other hand, two authors have introduced new variables into Bourdieu's concept<br />

of cultural capital. Emmison & Frow's (1998) work centers on an exploration of the<br />

ability of Information Technology to be considered a form of cultural capital. The authors<br />

state that "a familiarity with, and a positive disposition towards the use of bourgeoisie<br />

technologies of the information age can be seen as an additional form of cultural capital<br />

bestowing advantage on those families that possess them". Specifically computers are<br />

"machines" (Bourdieu, 1986:47) that form a type of objectified cultural capital, and the<br />

ability to use them is an embodied type of cultural capital. This work is useful because it<br />

shows the ways in which Bourdieu's concept of cultural capital can be expanded and<br />

updated to include cultural goods and practices which are progressively more important<br />

in determining achievement both in the school and without.<br />

Hage uses Bourdieu's theory of cultural capital to explore multiculturalism and racism in<br />

Australia. His discussion around race is distinct from Bourdieu's treatment of migrants<br />

and their amount of linguistic capital and habitus. Hage actually conceives of<br />

"whiteness" (in Dolby, 2000:49) as being a form of cultural capital. 'White' is not a<br />

stable, biologically determined trait, but a "shifting set of social practices" (Dolby,<br />

2000:49). He conceptualizes the nation as a circular field, with the hierarchy moving<br />

from the powerful center (composed of 'white' Australians) to the less powerful<br />

periphery (composed of the 'others'). The 'others' however are not simply dominated,<br />

but are forced to compete with each other for a place closer to the centre. This use of<br />

Bourdieu's notion of capital and fields is extremely illuminating to understand how<br />

people of non-Anglo ethnicities may try and exchange the cultural capital of their ethnic<br />

background with that of 'whiteness' to gain a higher position in the hierarchy. It is<br />

especially useful to see it in these terms as it exposes the arbitrary nature of what is<br />

Page 41 of 134


"Australian", and how it is determined by those in the dominant position (mainly 'white'<br />

Australians). In a path-breaking study, Bauder (2006) uses the notions of habitus and<br />

cultural capital to explain the situation of migrants in the labor market and society.<br />

In the article "Against School" (2003), the retired teacher John Taylor Gatto addresses<br />

education in modern schooling. The relation of cultural capital can be linked to<br />

Principles of Secondary Education (1918), by Alexander Inglis, which indicates how<br />

American schooling is what like Prussian schooling in the 1820s. The objective was to<br />

divide children into sections, by distributing them by subject, by age, and by test score.<br />

Inglis introduces six basic functions for modern schooling; functions three, four, and five<br />

are related to cultural capital, and describe the manner in which schooling enforces the<br />

cultural capital of each child, from a young age. Functions three, four, and five are: 3.<br />

Diagnosis and direction: School is meant to determine the proper social role of each<br />

student, by logging mathematic and anecdotal evidence into cumulative records. 4.<br />

Differentiation: Once the social role of a student is determined, the children are sorted<br />

by role and trained only as merited for his or her social destination. 5. Selection: This<br />

refers to Darwin's theory of natural selection applied to "the favored races".<br />

The idea is to help American society, by consciously attempting to improve the breeding<br />

stock. Schools are meant to tag the socially unfit with poor grades, remedial-schooling<br />

placement, and other notable social punishments that their peers will then view and<br />

accept them as intellectually inferior, and effectively bar them from the reproductive<br />

(sexual, economic, and cultural) sweepstakes of life. That was the purpose of petty<br />

humiliation in school: "It was the dirt down the drain." The three functions are directly<br />

related to cultural capital, because through schooling children are discriminated by<br />

social class and cognitively placed into the destination that will make them fit to sustain<br />

that social role. That is the path leading to their determined social class; and, during the<br />

fifth function, they will be socially undesirable to the privileged children, and so kept in a<br />

low social stratum.<br />

Paul DiMaggio expands on Bourdieu's view on cultural capital and its influence on<br />

education saying: "Following Bourdieu, I measure high school students' cultural capital<br />

using self-reports of involvement in art, music, and literature." In his journal article titled<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> Capital and School Success: The Impact of Status Culture Participation on the<br />

Grades of U.S. High School Students in the American Sociological Review.<br />

In the US, Richard A. Peterson and A Simkus (1992) extended the cultural capital<br />

theory, exclusively on (secondary) analysis of survey data on Americans, in 'How<br />

musical tastes mark occupational status groups', with the term "cultural omnivores" as a<br />

particular higher status section in the US that has broader cultural engagements and<br />

tastes spanning an eclectic range from highbrow arts to popular culture. Originally, it<br />

was Peterson (1992) who coined the term 'cultural omnivore' to address an anomaly<br />

observed in the evidence revealed by his work with Simkus (Peterson and Simkus,<br />

1992) which showed that people of higher social status, contrary to elite-mass models<br />

of cultural taste developed by French scholars with French data, were not averse to<br />

participation in activities associated with popular culture. The work rejected the<br />

Page 42 of 134


universal adaptation of the cultural capital theory, especially in the 20th century in<br />

advanced post-industrialist societies like the United States.<br />

In the UK, Louise Archer and colleagues (2015) developed the concept of science<br />

capital. The concept of science capital draws from the work of Bourdieu, in particular his<br />

studies focusing on the reproduction of social inequalities in society. Science capital is<br />

made up of science related cultural capital and social capital as well as habitus. It<br />

encapsulates the various influences that a young person's life experiences can have on<br />

their science identity and participation in science-related activities. The empirical work<br />

on science capital builds from a growing body of data into students' aspirations and<br />

attitudes to science, including ASPIRES and Enterprising Science. The concept of<br />

science capital was developed as a way to understand why these science-related<br />

resources, attitudes and aspirations led some children to pursue science, while others<br />

did not. The concept provides policy makers and practitioners with a useful framework<br />

to help understand what shapes young people's engagement with (and potential<br />

resistance to) science.<br />

Criticism<br />

Criticisms of Bourdieu's concept have been made on many grounds, including a lack of<br />

conceptual clarity. Perhaps due to this lack of clarity, researchers have operationalised<br />

the concept in diverse ways, and have varied in their conclusions. While some<br />

researchers may be criticised for using measures of cultural capital which focus only on<br />

certain aspects of 'highbrow' culture, this is a criticism which could also be leveled at<br />

Bourdieu's own work. Several studies have attempted to refine the measurement of<br />

Page 43 of 134


cultural capital, in order to examine which aspects of middle-class culture actually have<br />

value in the education system.<br />

It has been observed that Bourdieu's theory, and in particular his notion of habitus, is<br />

entirely deterministic, leaving no place for individual agency or even individual<br />

consciousness. Although Bourdieu claimed to have transcended the dichotomy of<br />

structure and agency, this is not necessarily convincing. For example, the Oxford<br />

academic John Goldthorpe has long argued that:<br />

Bourdieu's view of the transmission of cultural capital as a key process in social<br />

reproduction is simply wrong. And the more detailed findings of the research, as noted<br />

above, could then have been taken as helping to explain just why it is wrong. That is,<br />

because differing class conditions do not give rise to such distinctive and abiding forms<br />

of habitus as Bourdieu would suppose; because even within more disadvantaged<br />

classes, with little access to high culture, values favouring education may still prevail<br />

and perhaps some relevant cultural resources exist; and because, therefore, schools<br />

and other educational institutions can function as important agencies of re-socialisation<br />

– that is, can not only underwrite but also in various respects complement, compensate<br />

for or indeed counter family influences in the creation and transmission of "cultural<br />

capital", and not just in the case of Wunderkinder but in fact on a mass scale.<br />

Bourdieu has also been criticised for his lack of consideration of gender. Kanter (in<br />

Robinson & Garnier, 1986) point out the lack of interest in gender inequalities in the<br />

labour market in Bourdieu's work. However, Bourdieu addressed the topic of gender<br />

head-on in his 2001 book Masculine Domination. Bourdieu stated on the first page of<br />

the prelude in this book that he considered masculine domination to be a prime example<br />

of symbolic violence.<br />

Page 44 of 134


IV. <strong>Cultural</strong> Economics<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> Economics is the branch of economics that studies the relation of<br />

culture to economic outcomes. Here, 'culture' is defined by shared beliefs and<br />

preferences of respective groups. Programmatic issues include whether and how much<br />

culture matters as to economic outcomes and what its relation is to institutions. As a<br />

growing field in behavioral economics, the role of culture in economic behavior is<br />

increasingly being demonstrate to cause significant differentials in decision-making and<br />

the management and valuation of assets.<br />

Applications include the study of religion, social norms. social identity, fertility, beliefs in<br />

redistributive justice, ideology, hatred, terrorism, trust, and the culture of economics. A<br />

general analytical theme is how ideas and behaviors are spread among individuals<br />

through the formation of social capital, social networks and processes such as social<br />

learning, as in the theory of social evolution and information cascades. Methods include<br />

case studies and theoretical and empirical modeling of cultural transmission within and<br />

across social groups. In 2013 Said E. Dawlabani added the value systems approach to<br />

the cultural emergence aspect of macroeconomics.<br />

Development<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> economics develops from how wants and tastes are formed in society. This is<br />

partly due to nurture aspects, or what type of environment one is raised in, as it is the<br />

internalization of one’s upbringing that shapes their future wants and tastes. Acquired<br />

Page 45 of 134


tastes can be thought of as an example of this, as they demonstrate how preferences<br />

can be shaped socially.<br />

A key thought area that separates the development of cultural economics from<br />

traditional economics is a difference in how individuals arrive at their decisions. While a<br />

traditional economist will view decision making as having both implicit and explicit<br />

consequences, a cultural economist would argue that an individual will not only arrive at<br />

their decision based on these implicit and explicit decisions but based on trajectories.<br />

These trajectories consist of regularities, which have been built up throughout the years<br />

and guide individuals in their decision-making process.<br />

Combining Value Systems and Systems Thinking<br />

Economists have also started to look at cultural economics with a systems thinking<br />

approach. In this approach, the economy and culture are each viewed as a single<br />

system where "interaction and feedback effects were acknowledged, and where in<br />

particular the dynamic were made explicit". In this sense, the interdependencies of<br />

culture and the economy can be combined and better understood by following this<br />

approach.<br />

Said E. Dawlabani's book MEMEnomics: The Next-Generation Economic System<br />

combines the ideas of value systems (see value (ethics)) and systems thinking to<br />

provide one of the first frameworks that explores the effect of economic policies on<br />

culture. The book explores the intersections of multiple disciplines such as cultural<br />

development, organizational behavior, and memetics all in an attempt to explore the<br />

roots of cultural economics.<br />

Growth<br />

The advancing pace of new technology is transforming how the public consumes and<br />

shares culture. The cultural economic field has seen great growth with the advent of<br />

online social networking which has created productivity improvements in how culture is<br />

consumed. New technologies have also lead to cultural convergence where all kinds of<br />

culture can be accessed on a single device. Throughout their upbringing, younger<br />

persons of the current generation are consuming culture faster than their parents ever<br />

did, and through new mediums. The smartphone is a blossoming example of this where<br />

books, music, talk, artwork and more can all be accessed on a single device in a matter<br />

of seconds. This medium and the culture surrounding it is beginning to have an effect<br />

on the economy, whether it be increasing communication while lowering costs, lowering<br />

the barriers of entry to the technology economy, or making use of excess capacity.<br />

This field has also seen growth through the advent of new economic studies that have<br />

put on a cultural lens. For example, a recent study on Europeans living with their<br />

families into adulthood was conducted by Paola Sapienza, a professor at Northwestern<br />

University. The study found that those of Southern European descent tend to live at<br />

home with their families longer than those of Northern European descent. Sapienza<br />

Page 46 of 134


added cultural critique to her analysis of the research, revealing that it is Southern<br />

European culture to stay at home longer and then related this to how those who live at<br />

home longer have fewer children and start families later, thus contributing to Europe's<br />

falling birthrates. Sapienza's work is an example of how the growth of cultural<br />

economics is beginning to spread across the field.<br />

Sustainable Development<br />

An area that cultural economics<br />

has a strong presence in is<br />

sustainable development. Sustainable development has<br />

been defined as "...development that<br />

meets the needs of the present<br />

without<br />

compromising<br />

the ability of<br />

future<br />

generations<br />

to meet their<br />

own<br />

needs...".<br />

Culture plays an important<br />

role in this as it can<br />

determine how people view<br />

preparing for these future<br />

generations.<br />

Delayed<br />

gratification is a cultural<br />

economic issue that<br />

developed countries are<br />

currently dealing with.<br />

Economists argue that to<br />

ensure that the future<br />

is better than today,<br />

certain measures must be<br />

taken such as collecting taxes<br />

or "going green" to protect the<br />

environment. Policies such as<br />

these are hard for today's<br />

politicians to promote who<br />

want to win the vote of today's voters who are<br />

concerned with the present<br />

and not the future. People<br />

want to see the benefits<br />

now, not in the future.<br />

Economist David Throsby has<br />

proposed the idea of culturally<br />

sustainable development which compasses both the cultural<br />

industries (such as the arts) and culture (in the societal sense). He has created a set of<br />

criteria in regards to for which policy prescriptions can be compared to in order to<br />

ensure growth for future generations. The criteria are as follows:<br />

1. Advancement of material and non-material well-being: implies balance amongst<br />

economic, social, and cultural forces<br />

2. Intergenerational equity and the maintenance of cultural capital: current<br />

generation must recognize their responsibility to future generations<br />

Page 47 of 134


3. Equity within the present generation: distribution of cultural resources must be<br />

fair<br />

4. Recognition of interdependence: policy must understand the connections<br />

between economic, cultural and other variables within an overall system.<br />

With these guidelines, Throsby hopes to spur the recognition between culture and<br />

economics, which is something he believes has been lacking from popular economic<br />

discussions.<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> Finance<br />

As a growing field in behavioral economics, the role of culture in financial behavior is<br />

increasingly being demonstrate to cause highly significant differentials in the<br />

management and valuation of assets. Using the dimensions of culture identified by<br />

Shalom Schwartz, it has been proved that corporate dividend payments are determined<br />

largely by the dimensions of Mastery and Conservatism. Specifically, higher degrees of<br />

conservatism are associated with greater volumes and values of dividend payments,<br />

and higher degrees of mastery are associated with the total opposite. A different study<br />

assessed the role of culture on earnings management using Geert Hofstede’s cultural<br />

dimensions and the index of earnings management developed by Christian Leutz; which<br />

includes the use of accrual alteration to reduce volatility in reported earnings, the use of<br />

accrual alteration to reduce volatility in reported operating cash flows, use of accounting<br />

discretion to mitigate the reporting of small losses, and the use of accounting discretion<br />

when reporting operating earnings. It was found that Hofstede's dimension of<br />

Individualism was negatively correlated with earnings management, and that<br />

Uncertainty Avoidance was positively correlated. Behavioral economist Michael Taillard<br />

demonstrated that investment behaviors are caused primarily by behavioral factors,<br />

largely attributed to the influence of culture on the psychological frame of the investors<br />

in different nations, rather than rational ones by comparing the cultural dimensions used<br />

both by Geert Hofstede and Robert House, identifying strong and specific influences in<br />

risk aversion behavior resulting from the overlapping cultural dimensions between them<br />

that remained constant over a 20-year period.<br />

In regards to investing, it has been confirmed by multiple studies that greater<br />

differences between the cultures of various nations reduces the amount of investment<br />

between those countries. It was proven that both cultural differences between nations<br />

as well as the amount of unfamiliarity investors have with a culture not their own greatly<br />

reduces their willingness to invest in those nations, and that these factors have a<br />

negative impact with future returns, resulting in a cost premium on the degree of<br />

foreignness of an investment. Despite this, equity markets continue to integrate as<br />

indicated by equity price comovements, of which the two largest contributing factors are<br />

the ratio of trade between nations and the ratio of GDP resulting from foreign direct<br />

investment. Even these factors are the result of behavioral sources, however. The UN<br />

World Investment Report (2013) shows that regional integration is occurring at a more<br />

rapid rate than distant foreign relations, confirming an earlier study concluding that<br />

nations closer to each other tend to be more integrated. Since increased cultural<br />

Page 48 of 134


distance reduces the amount of foreign direct investment, this results in an accelerating<br />

curvilinear correlation between financial behavior and cultural distance.<br />

Culture also influences which factors are useful when predicting stock valuations. In<br />

Jordan, it was found that 84% of variability in stock returns were accounted for by using<br />

money supply, interest rate term structure, industry productivity growth, and risk<br />

premium; but were not influenced at all by inflation rates or dividend yield. In Nigeria,<br />

both real GDP and Consumer Price Index were both useful predictive factors, but<br />

foreign exchange rate was not. In Zimbabwe, only money supply and oil prices were<br />

found to be useful predictors of stock market valuations. India identified exchange rate,<br />

wholesale price index, gold prices, and market index as being useful factors. A<br />

comprehensive global study out of Romania attempted to identify if any factors of stock<br />

market valuation were culturally universal, identifying interest rates, inflation, and<br />

industrial production, but found that exchange rate, currency exchange volume, and<br />

trade were all unique to Romania.<br />

Page 49 of 134


Page 50 of 134


V. Paradigm Shifting<br />

A Paradigm Shift (also radical theory change), a concept identified by the<br />

American physicist and philosopher Thomas Kuhn (1922–1996), is a fundamental<br />

change in the basic concepts and experimental practices of a scientific discipline. Kuhn<br />

contrasted these shifts, which characterize a scientific revolution, to the activity<br />

of normal science, which he described as scientific work done within a prevailing<br />

framework (or paradigm). In this context, the word "paradigm" is used in its<br />

original Greek meaning, as "example".<br />

The nature of scientific revolutions has been studied by modern<br />

philosophy since Immanuel Kant used the phrase in the preface to his Critique of Pure<br />

Reason (1781). He referred to Greek mathematics and Newtonian physics. In the 20th<br />

century, new developments in the basic concepts of mathematics, physics,<br />

and biology revitalized interest in the question among scholars. It was against this active<br />

background that Kuhn published his work.<br />

Kuhn used the duck-rabbit optical<br />

illusion, made famous by Wittgenstein,<br />

to demonstrate the way in which a<br />

paradigm shift could cause one to see<br />

the same information in an entirely<br />

different way.<br />

Kuhn presented his notion of a paradigm shift in his influential book The Structure of<br />

Scientific Revolutions (1962). As one commentator summarizes:<br />

Kuhn acknowledges having used the term "paradigm" in two different meanings. In the<br />

first one, "paradigm" designates what the members of a certain scientific community<br />

have in common, that is to say, the whole of techniques, patents and values shared by<br />

the members of the community. In the second sense, the paradigm is a single element<br />

of a whole, say for instance Newton’s Principia, which, acting as a common model or an<br />

example... stands for the explicit rules and thus defines a coherent tradition of<br />

investigation. Thus the question is for Kuhn to investigate by means of the paradigm<br />

what makes possible the constitution of what he calls "normal science". That is to say,<br />

the science which can decide if a certain problem will be considered scientific or not.<br />

Normal science does not mean at all a science guided by a coherent system of rules, on<br />

the contrary, the rules can be derived from the paradigms, but the paradigms can guide<br />

the investigation also in the absence of rules. This is precisely the second meaning of<br />

the term "paradigm", which Kuhn considered the most new and profound, though it is in<br />

truth the oldest.<br />

Page 51 of 134


Since the 1960s, the concept of a paradigm shift has also been used in numerous nonscientific<br />

contexts to describe a profound change in a fundamental model or perception<br />

of events, even though Kuhn himself restricted the use of the term to the physical<br />

sciences.<br />

Kuhnian Paradigm Shifts<br />

An epistemological paradigm shift was called a "scientific revolution" by epistemologist<br />

and historian of science Thomas Kuhn in his book The Structure of Scientific<br />

Revolutions.<br />

A scientific revolution occurs, according to Kuhn, when scientists encounter anomalies<br />

that cannot be explained by the universally accepted paradigm within which scientific<br />

progress has thereto been made. The paradigm, in Kuhn's view, is not simply the<br />

current theory, but the entire worldview in which it exists, and all of the implications<br />

which come with it. This is based on features of landscape of knowledge that scientists<br />

can identify around them.<br />

There are anomalies for all paradigms, Kuhn maintained, that are brushed away as<br />

acceptable levels of error, or simply ignored and not dealt with (a principal argument<br />

Kuhn uses to reject Karl Popper's model of falsifiability as the key force involved in<br />

scientific change). Rather, according to Kuhn, anomalies have various levels of<br />

significance to the practitioners of science at the time. To put it in the context of early<br />

20th century physics, some scientists found the problems with calculating Mercury's<br />

perihelion more troubling than the Michelson-Morley experiment results, and some the<br />

other way around. Kuhn's model of scientific change differs here, and in many places,<br />

from that of the logical positivists in that it puts an enhanced emphasis on the individual<br />

humans involved as scientists, rather than abstracting science into a purely logical or<br />

philosophical venture.<br />

When enough significant anomalies have accrued against a current paradigm, the<br />

scientific discipline is thrown into a state of crisis, according to Kuhn. During this crisis,<br />

new ideas, perhaps ones previously discarded, are tried. Eventually a new paradigm is<br />

formed, which gains its own new followers, and an intellectual "battle" takes place<br />

between the followers of the new paradigm and the hold-outs of the old paradigm.<br />

Again, for early 20th century physics, the transition between<br />

the Maxwellian electromagnetic worldview and the Einsteinian relativistic worldview was<br />

neither instantaneous nor calm, and instead involved a protracted set of "attacks," both<br />

with empirical data as well as rhetorical or philosophical arguments, by both sides, with<br />

the Einsteinian theory winning out in the long run. Again, the weighing of evidence and<br />

importance of new data was fit through the human sieve: some scientists found the<br />

simplicity of Einstein's equations to be most compelling, while some found them more<br />

complicated than the notion of Maxwell's aether which they banished. Some<br />

found Arthur Eddington's photographs of light bending around the sun to be compelling,<br />

while some questioned their accuracy and meaning. Sometimes the convincing force is<br />

Page 52 of 134


just time itself and the human toll it takes, Kuhn said, using a quote from Max Planck: "a<br />

new scientific truth does not triumph by convincing its opponents and making them see<br />

the light, but rather because its opponents eventually die, and a new generation grows<br />

up that is familiar with it."<br />

After a given discipline has changed from one paradigm to another, this is called, in<br />

Kuhn's terminology, a scientific revolution or a paradigm shift. It is often this final<br />

conclusion, the result of the long process, that is meant when the term paradigm shift is<br />

used colloquially: simply the (often radical) change of worldview, without reference to<br />

the specificities of Kuhn's historical argument.<br />

In a 2015 retrospective on Kuhn, the philosopher Martin Cohen describes the notion of<br />

the "Paradigm Shift" as a kind of intellectual virus – spreading from hard science to<br />

social science and on to the arts and even everyday political rhetoric today. Cohen<br />

claims that Thomas Kuhn himself had only a very hazy idea of what it might mean and,<br />

in line with the American philosopher of science, Paul Feyerabend, accuses Kuhn of<br />

retreating from the more radical implications of his theory, which are that scientific facts<br />

are never really more than opinions, whose popularity is transitory and far from<br />

conclusive.<br />

Science and Paradigm Shift<br />

A common misinterpretation of paradigms is the belief that the discovery of paradigm<br />

shifts and the dynamic nature of science (with its many opportunities for subjective<br />

judgments by scientists) are a case for relativism: the view that all kinds of belief<br />

systems are equal. Kuhn vehemently denies this interpretation and states that when a<br />

Page 53 of 134


scientific paradigm is replaced by a new one, albeit through a complex social process,<br />

the new one is always better, not just different.<br />

These claims of relativism are, however, tied to another claim that Kuhn does at least<br />

somewhat endorse: that the language and theories of different paradigms cannot be<br />

translated into one another or rationally evaluated against one another—that they<br />

are incommensurable.<br />

This gave rise to much talk of different peoples and cultures having radically different<br />

worldviews or conceptual schemes—so different that whether or not one was better,<br />

they could not be understood by one another. However, the philosopher Donald<br />

Davidson published a highly regarded essay in 1974, "On the Very Idea of a Conceptual<br />

Scheme" (Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, Vol.<br />

47, (1973–1974), pp. 5–20) arguing that the notion that any languages or theories could<br />

be incommensurable with one another was itself incoherent. If this is correct, Kuhn's<br />

claims must be taken in a weaker sense than they often are.<br />

Furthermore, the hold of the Kuhnian analysis on social science has long been tenuous<br />

with the wide application of multi-paradigmatic approaches in order to understand<br />

complex human behavior (see for example John Hassard, Sociology and Organization<br />

Theory: Positivism, Paradigm and Postmodernity. Cambridge University Press,<br />

1993, ISBN 0521350344.)<br />

Paradigm shifts tend to be most dramatic in sciences that appear to be stable and<br />

mature, as in physics at the end of the 19th century. At that time, physics seemed to be<br />

a discipline filling in the last few details of a largely worked-out system. In 1900, Lord<br />

Kelvin famously told an assemblage of physicists at the British Association for the<br />

Advancement of Science, "There is nothing new to be discovered in physics now. All<br />

that remains is more and more precise measurement." [veracity of this quote challenged<br />

in Lord Kelvin article] Five years later, Albert Einstein published his paper on special<br />

relativity, which challenged the very simple set of rules laid down by Newtonian<br />

mechanics, which had been used to describe force and motion for over two hundred<br />

years.<br />

In The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, Kuhn wrote, "Successive transition from one<br />

paradigm to another via revolution is the usual developmental pattern of mature<br />

science." (p. 12) Kuhn's idea was itself revolutionary in its time, as it caused a major<br />

change in the way that academics talk about science. Thus, it could be argued that it<br />

caused or was itself part of a "paradigm shift" in the history and sociology of science.<br />

However, Kuhn would not recognise such a paradigm shift. In the social sciences,<br />

people can still use earlier ideas to discuss the history of science.<br />

Philosophers and historians of science, including Kuhn himself, ultimately accepted a<br />

modified version of Kuhn's model, which synthesizes his original view with the gradualist<br />

model that preceded it.<br />

Page 54 of 134


Examples of Paradigm Shifts<br />

Natural Sciences<br />

Some of the "classical cases" of Kuhnian paradigm shifts in science are:<br />

1543 – The transition in cosmology from a Ptolemaic cosmology to<br />

a Copernican one.<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

1543 – The acceptance of the work of Andreas Vesalius, whose work De humani<br />

corporis fabrica corrected the numerous errors in the previously-held system<br />

created by Galen.<br />

1687 – The transition in mechanics from Aristotelian mechanics to classical<br />

mechanics.<br />

1783 – The acceptance of Lavoisier's theory of chemical reactions and<br />

combustion in place of phlogiston theory, known as the chemical revolution.<br />

The transition in optics from geometrical optics to physical optics with Augustin-<br />

Jean Fresnel's wave theory.<br />

1826 – The discovery of hyperbolic geometry.<br />

1859 – The revolution in evolution from goal-directed change to Charles<br />

Darwin's natural selection.<br />

1880 - The germ theory of disease began overtaking Galen's miasma theory.<br />

1905 – The development of quantum mechanics, which replaced classical<br />

mechanics at microscopic scales.<br />

1887 to 1905 – The transition from the luminiferous aether present<br />

in space to electromagnetic radiation in spacetime.<br />

<br />

1919 – The transition between the worldview of Newtonian gravity and<br />

the Einsteinian General Relativity.<br />

Social Sciences<br />

In Kuhn's view, the existence of a single reigning paradigm is characteristic of the<br />

natural sciences, while philosophy and much of social science were characterized by a<br />

"tradition of claims, counterclaims, and debates over fundamentals." Others have<br />

applied Kuhn's concept of paradigm shift to the social sciences.<br />

Page 55 of 134


The movement known as the cognitive revolution moved away<br />

from behaviorist approaches to psychological study and the acceptance<br />

of cognition as central to studying human behaviour.<br />

The Keynesian revolution is typically viewed as a major shift<br />

in macroeconomics. According to John Kenneth Galbraith, Say's Law dominated<br />

economic thought prior to Keynes for over a century, and the shift to<br />

Keynesianism was difficult. Economists who contradicted the law, which implied<br />

that underemployment and underinvestment (coupled with oversaving) were<br />

virtually impossible, risked losing their careers. In his magnum opus, Keynes<br />

cited one of his predecessors, John A. Hobson, who was repeatedly denied<br />

positions at universities for his heretical theory.<br />

<br />

<br />

Later, the movement for monetarism over Keynesianism marked a second<br />

divisive shift. Monetarists held that fiscal policy was not effective for<br />

stabilizing inflation, that it was solely a monetary phenomenon, in contrast to<br />

the Keynesian view of the time was that both fiscal and monetary policy were<br />

important. Keynesians later adopted much of the monetarists' view of the quantity<br />

theory of money and shifting Phillips curve, theories they initially rejected.<br />

First proposed by Ferdinand de Saussure in 1879, the laryngeal theory in Indo-<br />

European linguistics postulated the existence of "laryngeal" consonants in<br />

the Proto-Indo-European language (PIE), a theory that was confirmed by the<br />

discovery of the Hittite language in the early 20th century. The theory has since<br />

been accepted by the vast majority of linguists, paving the way for the internal<br />

reconstruction of the syntax and grammatical rules of PIE and is considered one<br />

of the most significant developments in linguistics since the initial discovery of<br />

the Indo-European language family.<br />

Applied Sciences<br />

More recently, paradigm shifts are also recognisable in applied sciences:<br />

<br />

<br />

In medicine, the transition from "clinical judgment" to evidence-based medicine<br />

In software engineering, the transition from the Rational Paradigm to the<br />

Empirical Paradigm<br />

Marketing<br />

In the later part of the 1990s, 'paradigm shift' emerged as a buzzword, popularized<br />

as marketing speak and appearing more frequently in print and publication. In his<br />

book Mind The Gaffe, author Larry Trask advises readers to refrain from using it, and to<br />

use caution when reading anything that contains the phrase. It is referred to in several<br />

articles and books as abused and overused to the point of becoming meaningless.<br />

Other uses[edit]<br />

Page 56 of 134


The term "paradigm shift" has found uses in other contexts, representing the notion of a<br />

major change in a certain thought-pattern—a radical change in personal beliefs,<br />

complex systems or organizations, replacing the former way of thinking or organizing<br />

with a radically different way of thinking or organizing:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

M. L. Handa, a professor of sociology in education at O.I.S.E. University of<br />

Toronto, Canada, developed the concept of a paradigm within the context of<br />

social sciences. He defines what he means by "paradigm" and introduces the<br />

idea of a "social paradigm". In addition, he identifies the basic component of any<br />

social paradigm. Like Kuhn, he addresses the issue of changing paradigms, the<br />

process popularly known as "paradigm shift". In this respect, he focuses on the<br />

social circumstances which precipitate such a shift. Relatedly, he addresses how<br />

that shift affects social institutions, including the institution of education.<br />

The concept has been developed for technology and economics in the<br />

identification of new techno-economic paradigms as changes in technological<br />

systems that have a major influence on the behavior of the entire economy<br />

(Carlota Perez; earlier work only on technological paradigms by Giovanni Dosi).<br />

This concept is linked to Joseph Schumpeter's idea of creative destruction.<br />

Examples include the move to mass production and the introduction of<br />

microelectronics.<br />

Two photographs of the Earth from space, "Earthrise" (1968) and "The Blue<br />

Marble" (1972), are thought to have helped to usher in<br />

the environmentalist movement which gained great prominence in the years<br />

immediately following distribution of those images.<br />

Hans Küng applies Thomas Kuhn's theory of paradigm change to the entire<br />

history of Christian thought and theology. He identifies six historical<br />

"macromodels": 1) the apocalyptic paradigm of primitive Christianity, 2) the<br />

Hellenistic paradigm of the patristic period, 3) the medieval Roman Catholic<br />

paradigm, 4) the Protestant (Reformation) paradigm, 5) the modern<br />

Enlightenment paradigm, and 6) the emerging ecumenical paradigm. He also<br />

discusses five analogies between natural science and theology in relation to<br />

paradigm shifts. Küng addresses paradigm change in his books, Paradigm<br />

Change in Theology and Theology for the Third Millennium: An Ecumenical View.<br />

Page 57 of 134


Page 58 of 134


VI. Conspiracy Theory<br />

A Conspiracy Theory is an explanation of an event or situation that invokes an<br />

unwarranted conspiracy, generally one involving an illegal or harmful act carried out by<br />

government or other powerful actors. Conspiracy theories often produce hypotheses<br />

that contradict the prevailing understanding of history or simple facts. The term is often<br />

a derogatory one.<br />

According to the political scientist Michael Barkun, conspiracy theories rely on the view<br />

that the universe is governed by design, and embody three principles: nothing happens<br />

by accident, nothing is as it seems, and everything is connected. Another common<br />

feature is that conspiracy theories evolve to incorporate whatever evidence exists<br />

against them, so that they become, as Barkun writes, a closed system that<br />

is unfalsifiable, and therefore "a matter of faith rather than proof". Skeptics are among<br />

their outspoken critics.<br />

The Eye of Providence, or the all-seeing eye of God, seen here on the US $1 bill, has been<br />

taken by some to be evidence of a conspiracy involving the founders of the United<br />

States and the Illuminati.<br />

Etymology<br />

The Oxford English Dictionary defines conspiracy theory as "the theory that an event or<br />

phenomenon occurs as a result of a conspiracy between interested parties; spec. a<br />

belief that some covert but influential agency (typically political in motivation and<br />

oppressive in intent) is responsible for an unexplained event". It cites a 1909 article<br />

in The American Historical Review as the earliest usage example, although it also<br />

appears in journals as early as April 1870. The word "conspiracy" derives from the<br />

Latin con- ("with, together") and spirare ("to breathe").<br />

According to John Ayto, the phrase conspiracy theory was originally a neutral term and<br />

acquired a pejorative connotation only in the 1960s, with an implication that the theorist<br />

is paranoid. Lance deHaven-Smith has suggested that the term was deployed in the<br />

1960s by the Central Intelligence Agency (CIA) to discredit John F. Kennedy<br />

assassination conspiracy theories. Robert Blaskiewicz rejects such claims, asserting<br />

Page 59 of 134


instead that the term has always been derogatory and pointing to examples<br />

demonstrating that this has been so since the nineteenth century.<br />

Examples<br />

A conspiracy theory may take any matter as its subject, but certain subjects attract<br />

greater interest than others. Favored subjects include famous deaths, government<br />

activities, new technologies, terrorism and questions of alien life. Among the longeststanding<br />

and most widely recognized conspiracy theories are notions concerning the<br />

assassination of John F. Kennedy, the 1969 Apollo moon landings and the 9/11 terrorist<br />

attacks, as well as numerous theories pertaining to alleged plots for world domination by<br />

various groups both real and imaginary.<br />

Popularity<br />

Some scholars argue that conspiracy theories once limited to fringe audiences have<br />

become commonplace in mass media, contributing to conspiracism emerging as<br />

a cultural phenomenon in the United States of the late 20th and early 21st centuries.<br />

According to anthropologists Todd Sanders and Harry G. West, evidence suggests that<br />

a broad cross-section of Americans today gives credence to at least some conspiracy<br />

theories. Belief in conspiracy theories has therefore become a topic of interest for<br />

sociologists, psychologists and experts in folklore.<br />

Conspiracy theories are widely present on the Web in the form<br />

of blogs and YouTube videos, as well as on social media. Whether the Web has<br />

increased the prevalence of conspiracy theories or not is an open research<br />

question. The presence and representation of conspiracy theories in search<br />

engine results has been monitored and studied, showing significant variation across<br />

different topics, and a general absence of reputable, high-quality links in the results.<br />

Types of conspiracy theory<br />

Walker's Five Kinds<br />

Jesse Walker (2013) has identified five kinds of conspiracy theories:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

The "Enemy Outside" refers to theories based on figures alleged to be scheming<br />

against a community from without.<br />

The "Enemy Within" finds the conspirators lurking inside the nation,<br />

indistinguishable from ordinary citizens.<br />

The "Enemy Above" involves powerful people manipulating events for their own<br />

gain.<br />

The "Enemy Below" features the lower classes working to overturn the social<br />

order.<br />

Page 60 of 134


The "Benevolent Conspiracies" are angelic forces that work behind the scenes to<br />

improve the world and help people.<br />

Barkun's Three Types<br />

Barkun has identified three classifications of conspiracy theory:<br />

<br />

Event conspiracy theories. This refers to limited and well-defined events.<br />

Examples may include such conspiracies theories as those concerning<br />

the Kennedy assassination, 9/11, and the spread of AIDS.<br />

<br />

<br />

Systemic conspiracy theories. The conspiracy is believed to have broad goals,<br />

usually conceived as securing control of a country, a region, or even the entire<br />

world. The goals are sweeping, whilst the conspiratorial machinery is generally<br />

simple: a single, evil organization implements a plan to infiltrate and subvert<br />

existing institutions. This is a common scenario in conspiracy theories that focus<br />

on the alleged machinations of Jews, Freemasons, Communism, or the Catholic<br />

Church.<br />

Superconspiracy theories. For Barkun, such theories link multiple alleged<br />

conspiracies together hierarchically. At the summit is a distant but all-powerful<br />

evil force. His cited examples are the ideas of David Icke and Milton William<br />

Cooper.<br />

Page 61 of 134


Rothbard: Shallow vs. Deep<br />

Murray Rothbard argues in favor of a model that contrasts "deep" conspiracy theories to<br />

"shallow" ones. According to Rothbard, a "shallow" theorist observes an event and<br />

asks Cui bono? ("Who benefits?"), jumping to the conclusion that a posited beneficiary<br />

is responsible for covertly influencing events. On the other hand, the "deep" conspiracy<br />

theorist begins with a hunch, and then seeks out evidence. Rothbard describes this<br />

latter activity as a matter of confirming with certain facts one's initial paranoia.<br />

Evidence vs. Conspiracy Theory<br />

Theories involving multiple conspirators that are proven to be correct, such as<br />

the Watergate scandal, are usually referred to as "investigative journalism" or "historical<br />

analysis" rather than conspiracy theory. By contrast, the term "Watergate conspiracy<br />

theory" is used to refer to a variety of hypotheses in which those convicted in the<br />

conspiracy were in fact the victims of a deeper conspiracy.<br />

Noam Chomsky contrasts conspiracy theory to institutional analysis which focuses<br />

mostly on the public, long-term behavior of publicly known institutions, as recorded in,<br />

for example, scholarly documents or mainstream media reports. Conspiracy theory<br />

conversely posits the existence of secretive coalitions of individuals and speculates on<br />

their alleged activities.<br />

Clare Birchall at King's College London describes conspiracy theory as a "form of<br />

popular knowledge or interpretation". The use of the word 'knowledge' here suggests<br />

ways in which conspiracy theory may be considered in relation to legitimate modes of<br />

knowing. The relationship between legitimate and illegitimate knowledge, Birchall<br />

claims, is closer than common dismissals of conspiracy theory contend.<br />

Conspiracism As A World View<br />

The historian Richard Hofstadter addressed the role of paranoia and conspiracism<br />

throughout American history in his 1964 essay "The Paranoid Style in American<br />

Politics". Bernard Bailyn's classic The Ideological Origins of the American<br />

Revolution (1967) notes that a similar phenomenon could be found in America during<br />

the time preceding the American Revolution. Conspiracism labels people's attitudes as<br />

well as the type of conspiracy theories that are more global and historical in proportion.<br />

The term "conspiracism" was further popularized by academic Frank P. Mintz in the<br />

1980s. According to Mintz, conspiracism denotes "belief in the primacy of conspiracies<br />

in the unfolding of history":<br />

Conspiracism serves the needs of diverse political and social groups in America and<br />

elsewhere. It identifies elites, blames them for economic and social catastrophes, and<br />

assumes that things will be better once popular action can remove them from positions<br />

of power. As such, conspiracy theories do not typify a particular epoch or ideology.<br />

Page 62 of 134


Justin Fox of Time magazine has noted that Wall Street traders are among the most<br />

conspiracy-minded group of people, and ascribes this to the reality of some financial<br />

market conspiracies, and to the ability of conspiracy theories to provide necessary<br />

orientation in the market's day-to-day movements. According to Fox, most good<br />

investigative reporters are also conspiracy theorists.<br />

United States<br />

Harry G. West and others have noted that while conspiracy theorists may often be<br />

dismissed as a fringe minority, certain evidence suggests that a wide range of the<br />

American population maintains a belief in conspiracy theories. West also compares<br />

those theories to hypernationalism and religious fundamentalism.<br />

Specific events and trends within US history have been cited as causes of the popularity<br />

of conspiratorial thinking in the US.<br />

Page 63 of 134


Theologian Robert Jewett and philosopher John Shelton Lawrence attribute the<br />

enduring popularity of conspiracy theories in the US to the Cold War, McCarthyism,<br />

and counterculture rejection of authority. They state that among both the left-wing and<br />

right-wing there remains a willingness to use real events, such as Soviet plots,<br />

inconsistencies in the Warren Report, and the 9/11 attacks, to support the existence of<br />

unverified ongoing large-scale conspiracies.<br />

The Watergate scandal has also been used to bestow legitimacy to other conspiracy<br />

theories, with Nixon himself commenting that it served as a "Rorschach ink blot" which<br />

invited others to fill-in the underlying pattern.<br />

Historian Kathryn S Olmstead cites three reasons why Americans are prone to believing<br />

in government conspiracies theories:<br />

1. Genuine government overreach and secrecy during the Cold War, listing as<br />

examples Watergate, the Tuskegee syphilis experiment, Project MKUltra, and<br />

the CIA collaborating with Mobsters to attempt to assassinate Fidel Castro.<br />

2. The precedent set by official government-sanctioned conspiracy theories for<br />

propaganda, such as claims of German infiltration of the US during World War II<br />

or the debunked claim that Saddam Hussein played a role in 9/11.<br />

3. The distrust fostered by the government's spying and harassment of dissenters,<br />

such as the Sedition Act of 1918, COINTELPRO, and as part of various Red<br />

Scares.<br />

Middle East<br />

Matthew Gray has noted that conspiracy theories are a prevalent feature of Arab culture<br />

and politics. Variants include conspiracies involving colonialism, Zionism, superpowers,<br />

oil, and the war on terrorism, which may be referred to as a War against Islam. For<br />

example, The Protocols of the Elders of Zion, an infamous hoax document purporting to<br />

be a Jewish plan for world domination, is commonly read and promoted in the Muslim<br />

world. Roger Cohen has suggested that the popularity of conspiracy theories in the<br />

Arab world is "the ultimate refuge of the powerless". Al-Mumin Said has noted the<br />

danger of such theories, for they "keep us not only from the truth but also from<br />

confronting our faults and problems".<br />

Psychological Interpretations<br />

The widespread belief in conspiracy theories has become a topic of interest for<br />

sociologists, psychologists, and experts in folklore since at least the 1960s, when a<br />

number of conspiracy theories arose regarding the assassination of U.S. President John<br />

F. Kennedy. Sociologist Türkay Salim Nefes underlines the political nature of conspiracy<br />

theories. He suggests that one of the most important characteristics of these accounts<br />

is their attempt to unveil the "real but hidden" power relations in social groups.<br />

Page 64 of 134


The Attractions of Conspiracy Theory<br />

The political scientist Michael Barkun, discussing the usage of "conspiracy theory" in<br />

contemporary American culture, holds that this term is used for a belief that explains an<br />

event as the result of a secret plot by exceptionally powerful and cunning conspirators<br />

to achieve a malevolent end. According to Barkun, the appeal of conspiracism is<br />

threefold:<br />

<br />

"First, conspiracy theories claim to explain what institutional analysis cannot.<br />

They appear to make sense out of a world that is otherwise confusing.<br />

<br />

Second, they do so in an appealingly simple way, by dividing the world sharply<br />

between the forces of light, and the forces of darkness. They trace all evil back to<br />

a single source, the conspirators and their agents.<br />

Third, conspiracy theories are often presented as special, secret<br />

knowledge unknown or unappreciated by others. For conspiracy theorists, the<br />

masses are a brainwashed herd, while the conspiracy theorists in the know can<br />

congratulate themselves on penetrating the plotters' deceptions."<br />

Humanistic psychologists argue that even if a posited cabal behind an alleged<br />

conspiracy is almost always perceived as hostile, there often remains an element of<br />

Page 65 of 134


eassurance for theorists. This is because it is a consolation to imagine that difficulties<br />

in human affairs are created by humans, and remain within human control. If a cabal<br />

can be implicated, there may be a hope of breaking its power or of joining it. Belief in<br />

the power of a cabal is an implicit assertion of human dignity — an unconscious<br />

affirmation that man is responsible for his own destiny.<br />

People formulate conspiracy theories to explain, for example, power relations in social<br />

groups and the perceived existence of evil forces. Proposed psychological origins of<br />

conspiracy theorising include projection; the personal need to explain "a significant<br />

event [with] a significant cause;" and the product of various kinds and stages of thought<br />

disorder, such as paranoid disposition, ranging in severity to diagnosable mental<br />

illnesses. Some people prefer socio-political explanations over the insecurity of<br />

encountering random, unpredictable, or otherwise inexplicable events.<br />

According to Berlet and Lyons, "Conspiracism is a particular narrative form of<br />

scapegoating that frames demonized enemies as part of a vast insidious plot against<br />

the common good, while it valorizes the scapegoater as a hero for sounding the alarm".<br />

Psychological Origins<br />

Some psychologists believe that a search for meaning is common in conspiracism.<br />

Once cognized, confirmation bias and avoidance of cognitive dissonance may reinforce<br />

the belief. In a context where a conspiracy theory has become popular within a social<br />

group, communal reinforcement may equally play a part. Research carried out at<br />

the University of Kent suggested people may be influenced by conspiracy theories<br />

without being aware that their attitudes have changed. After reading popular conspiracy<br />

theories about the death of Princess Diana, participants in the study correctly estimated<br />

how much their peers' attitudes had changed, but significantly underestimated how<br />

much their own attitudes had grown to favor conspiracy theories.<br />

A study published in 2012 also found that conspiracy theorists frequently believe in<br />

multiple conspiracies, even when one conspiracy contradicts the other. For example,<br />

the study found that people who believe Osama Bin Laden was captured alive by<br />

Americans are also likely to believe that Bin Laden was actually killed prior to the 2011<br />

raid on his home in Pakistan.<br />

Projection<br />

Some historians have argued that there is an element of psychological projection in<br />

conspiracism. This projection, according to the argument, is manifested in the form of<br />

attribution of undesirable characteristics of the self to the conspirators. Historian Richard<br />

Hofstadter stated that:<br />

... it is hard to resist the conclusion that this enemy is on many counts the projection of<br />

the self; both the ideal and the unacceptable aspects of the self are attributed to him. The<br />

enemy may be the cosmopolitan intellectual, but the paranoid will outdo him in the<br />

apparatus of scholarship ... the Ku Klux Klan imitated Catholicism to the point of donning<br />

Page 66 of 134


priestly vestments, developing an elaborate ritual and an equally elaborate hierarchy.<br />

The John Birch Society emulates Communist cells and quasi-secret operation through<br />

"front" groups, and preaches a ruthless prosecution of the ideological war along lines<br />

very similar to those it finds in the Communist enemy. Spokesmen of the various<br />

fundamentalist anti-Communist "crusades" openly express their admiration for the<br />

dedication and discipline the Communist cause calls forth.<br />

Hofstadter also noted that "sexual freedom" is a vice frequently attributed to the<br />

conspiracist's target group, noting that "very often the fantasies of true believers reveal<br />

strong sadomasochistic outlets, vividly expressed, for example, in the delight of anti-<br />

Masons with the cruelty of Masonic punishments." A 2011 study found that<br />

highly Machiavellian people are more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, since they<br />

themselves would be more willing to engage in a conspiracy when placed in the same<br />

situation as the alleged conspirators.<br />

Epistemic Bias<br />

According to the British Psychological Society, it is possible that certain basic<br />

human epistemic biases are projected onto the material under scrutiny. One study cited<br />

by the group found that humans apply a rule of thumb by which we expect a significant<br />

event to have a significant cause. The study offered subjects four versions of events, in<br />

which a foreign president (a) was successfully assassinated, (b) was wounded but<br />

survived, (c) survived with wounds but died of a heart attack at a later date, and (d) was<br />

unharmed. Subjects were significantly more likely to suspect conspiracy in the case of<br />

the major events—in which the president died—than in the other cases, despite all other<br />

Page 67 of 134


evidence available to them being equal. Connected with apophenia, the genetic<br />

tendency of human beings to find patterns in coincidence, this allows the discovery of<br />

conspiracy in any significant event.<br />

Clinical Psychology<br />

For some individuals, an obsessive compulsion to believe, prove, or re-tell a conspiracy<br />

theory may indicate one or a combination of well-understood psychological conditions,<br />

and other hypothetical ones: paranoia, denial, schizophrenia, mean world syndrome.<br />

Sociological Interpretations<br />

Christopher Hitchens described conspiracy theory as the "exhaust fumes of<br />

democracy": the unavoidable result of a large amount of information circulating among a<br />

large number of people.<br />

Conspiracy theories may be emotionally satisfying, by assigning blame to a group to<br />

which the theorist does not belong and so absolving the theorist of moral or political<br />

responsibility in society. Likewise, Roger Cohen writing for The New York Times has<br />

said that, "captive minds; ... resort to conspiracy theory because it is the ultimate refuge<br />

of the powerless. If you cannot change your own life, it must be that some greater force<br />

controls the world."<br />

Sociological historian Holger Herwig found in studying German explanations for the<br />

origins of World War I, "Those events that are most important are hardest to<br />

understand, because they attract the greatest attention from myth makers and<br />

charlatans."<br />

Influence of Critical Theory<br />

French sociologist Bruno Latour suggests that the widespread popularity of conspiracy<br />

theories in mass culture may be due, in part, to the pervasive presence of Marxistinspired<br />

critical theory and similar ideas in academia since the 1970s.<br />

Latour notes that about 90% of contemporary social criticism in academia displays one<br />

of two approaches, which he terms "the fact position and the fairy position". The fairy<br />

position is anti-fetishist, arguing that "objects of belief" (e.g., religion, arts) are merely<br />

concepts onto which power is projected; Latour contends that those who use this<br />

approach show biases towards confirming their own dogmatic suspicions as most<br />

"scientifically supported". While the complete facts of the situation and correct<br />

methodology are ostensibly important to them, Latour proposes that the scientific<br />

process is instead laid on as a patina to one's pet theories to lend a sort of reputation<br />

high ground. The "fact position" argues that individuals are dominated, often covertly<br />

and without their awareness, by external forces (e.g., economics, gender). Latour<br />

concludes that each of these two approaches in Academia has led to a polarized,<br />

inefficient atmosphere highlighted (in both approaches) by its causticness. "Do you see<br />

Page 68 of 134


now why it feels so good to be a critical mind?" asks Latour: no matter which position<br />

you take, "You're always right!"<br />

Latour notes that such social criticism has been appropriated by those he describes as<br />

conspiracy theorists, including climate change denialists and the 9/11 Truth movement:<br />

"Maybe I am taking conspiracy theories too seriously, but I am worried to detect, in<br />

those mad mixtures of knee-jerk disbelief, punctilious demands for proofs, and free use<br />

of powerful explanation from the social neverland, many of the weapons of social<br />

critique."<br />

Fusion Paranoia<br />

Michael Kelly, a Washington Post journalist and critic of anti-war movements on both<br />

the left and right, coined the term "fusion paranoia" to refer to a political convergence of<br />

left-wing and right-wing activists around anti-war issues and civil liberties, which he said<br />

were motivated by a shared belief in conspiracism or shared anti-government views.<br />

Barkun has adopted this term to refer to how the synthesis of paranoid conspiracy<br />

theories, which were once limited to American fringe audiences, has given them mass<br />

appeal and enabled them to become commonplace in mass media, thereby<br />

Page 69 of 134


inaugurating an unrivaled period of people actively preparing<br />

for apocalyptic or millenarian scenarios in the United States of the late 20th and early<br />

21st centuries. Barkun notes the occurrence of lone-wolf conflicts with law enforcement<br />

acting as proxy for threatening the established political powers.<br />

Viability of conspiracies<br />

The physicist David Robert Grimes published in the PLOS ONE journal an estimation of<br />

the time it would take for a conspiracy to be exposed, based on the number of people<br />

involved. His calculations used data from verified events such as The National Security<br />

Agency (NSA) PRISM affair, Tuskegee Syphilis Experiment and the FBI forensic<br />

scandal.<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Moon landing hoax would require the involvement of 411,000 people and would<br />

be exposed within 3.68 years;<br />

Climate-change fraud would require 405,000 people and would be exposed<br />

within 3.70 years;<br />

Vaccination conspiracy would require a minimum of 22,000 people (without drug<br />

companies) and would be exposed within at least 3.15 years and at most 34.78<br />

years depending on the number involved;<br />

Suppressed cancer cure conspiracy would require 714,000 people and would be<br />

exposed within 3.17 years.<br />

Political Use<br />

In his book The Open Society and Its Enemies, the philosopher Karl Popper used the<br />

term "conspiracy theory" to criticize the ideologies driving historicism. Popper argued<br />

that totalitarianism was founded on "conspiracy theories" which drew on imaginary plots<br />

driven by paranoid scenarios predicated on tribalism, chauvinism, or racism. Popper<br />

acknowledged that genuine conspiracies do exist, but noted how infrequently<br />

conspirators have been able to achieve their goal.<br />

The historian Bruce Cumings similarly rejects the notion that history is controlled by<br />

conspiracies, stating that where real conspiracies have appeared they have usually had<br />

little effect on history and have had unforeseen consequences for the conspirators.<br />

Cumings concludes that history is instead "moved by the broad forces and large<br />

structures of human collectivities".<br />

In a 2009 article, the legal scholars Cass Sunstein and Adrian Vermeule considered a<br />

number of possible government responses to conspiracy theories, including censorship<br />

and taxation, and concluding that the authorities ought to engage in counter-speech and<br />

dialogue, which they termed "cognitive infiltration".<br />

Page 70 of 134


VII. Zeitgeist Theory<br />

The Zeitgeist (/ˈzaɪtɡaɪst/;) is a concept from 18th to 19th-century German<br />

philosophy, translated as "spirit of the age" or "spirit of the times". It refers to an invisible<br />

agent or force dominating the characteristics of a given epoch in world history.<br />

The term is now mostly associated with Hegel, contrasting with Hegel's use<br />

of Volksgeist "national spirit" and Weltgeist "world-spirit", but its coinage and<br />

popularization precedes Hegel, and is mostly due to Herder and Goethe.<br />

Other philosophers who were associated with such ideas<br />

include Spencer and Voltaire. The term as used contemporarily may more pragmatically<br />

refer to a fashion or fad which prescribes what is acceptable or tasteful, e.g. in the field<br />

of architecture.<br />

Hegel<br />

Hegel in Phenomenology of the Spirit (1807) uses both Weltgeist and Volksgeist but<br />

prefers the phrase Geist der Zeiten "spirit of the times" over the compound Zeitgeist.<br />

The Hegelian concept does not necessarily contrast with the Great Man theory as<br />

by Thomas Carlyle, which sees history as the result of the actions of heroes and<br />

geniuses, as Hegel perceived such "great men", specifically Napoleon, as the<br />

"embodiment of the world-spirit" (Die Weltseele zu Pferde "the world-soul on<br />

horseback")<br />

Hegel believed that art reflected, by its very nature, the culture of the time in which it is<br />

created. Culture and art are inextricable because an individual artist is a product of his<br />

or her time and therefore brings that culture to any given work of art. Furthermore, he<br />

believed that in the modern world it was impossible to produce classical art, which he<br />

believed represented a "free and ethical culture", which depended more on<br />

the philosophy of art and theory of art, rather than a reflection of the social construct,<br />

or Zeitgeist in which a given artist lives.<br />

Page 71 of 134


Theory of Leadership<br />

In Self-Help and Business Models<br />

A "zeitgeist theory of leadership" has been contrasted with Thomas Carlyle’s great man<br />

theory by Forsyth (2009). In his theory, Carlyle stresses that leaders do not become<br />

leaders by fate or accident. Instead, these individuals possess characteristics of great<br />

leaders and these characteristics allow them to obtain positions of power.<br />

According to Forsyth, Leo Tolstoy disagreed with Carlyle’s perspective, instead<br />

believing that leadership, like other things, was a product of the "zeitgeist", the social<br />

circumstances at the time.<br />

Great man theory and zeitgeist theory can be included in two main areas of thought<br />

in psychology. For instance, great man theory is very similar to the trait approach. Trait<br />

researchers are interested in identifying the various personality traits that underline<br />

human behaviors such as conformity, leadership or other social behaviors. Thus, they<br />

agree that leadership is primarily a quality of an individual and that some people are<br />

pre-dispositioned to be a leader whereas others are born to follow these leaders. In<br />

contrast, situationistresearchers believe that social behavior is a product of society. That<br />

is, social influence is what determines human behaviors. Therefore, situationism is of<br />

the same opinion as zeitgeist theory—leaders are created from the social environment<br />

and are molded from the situation. The concept of zeitgeist also relates to the<br />

sociological tradition that stems from Émile Durkheim and recently developed into social<br />

capital theory as exemplified by the work of Patrick Hunout.<br />

These two perspectives have been combined to create what is known as<br />

the interactional approach to leadership. This approach asserts that leadership is<br />

developed through the mixing of personality traits and the situation. Further, this<br />

approach was expressed by social psychologist, Kurt Lewin, by the equation B = f(P, E)<br />

where behavior (B) is a function (f) of the person (P) and the environment (E).<br />

Examples of Models in Business<br />

Executives, venture capitalists, journalists and authors have argued that the idea of a<br />

zeitgeist is useful in understanding the emergence of industries, simultaneous invention<br />

and evaluating the relative value of innovations. Malcolm Gladwell argued in his<br />

book Outliers that entrepreneurs who succeeded often share similar characteristics—<br />

early personal or significant exposure to knowledge and skills in the early stages of a<br />

nascent industry. He proposed that the timing of involvement in an industry and often in<br />

sports as well affected the probability of success. In Silicon Valley, a number of people<br />

(Peter Thiel, Alistair Davidson, Mac Levchin, Nicholas G. Carr, Vinod Khosla) have<br />

argued that much innovation has been shaped by easy access to the Internet, open<br />

source software, component technologies for both hardware and software (e.g.,<br />

software libraries, software as a service), and the ability to reach narrow markets across<br />

a global market. Peter Thiel has commented: "There is so much incrementalism now."<br />

Page 72 of 134


In a zeitgeist market, the number of new entrants is high, differentiation in high value<br />

products (the strongest predictor of new product success) is more difficult to achieve,<br />

and business models emphasizing service and solution over product and process will<br />

enhance success. Examples include innovation in product experience, legal rights and<br />

bundling, privacy rights, and agency (where businesses act on behalf of customers).<br />

Intellectual Fashion<br />

Zeitgeist in the sense intellectual or aesthetic fashion or fad:<br />

<br />

Research from empirical aesthetics investigated the relation between zeitgeist as<br />

temporal context and the evaluation of creative works. In a study of the musical<br />

originality of 15,618 classical music themes, the importance of objective<br />

characteristics and zeitgeist for popularity was examined. Both the musical<br />

originality of a theme relative to its contemporary works (the zeitgeist), as well as<br />

its "absolute" originality influenced in similar magnitude the popularity of a<br />

theme. Similarly, objective features and temporal context both influenced the<br />

evaluation of linguistic originality.<br />

Page 73 of 134


Errors of illusion are<br />

not readily apparent<br />

because the shared<br />

beliefs<br />

and<br />

assumptions of a<br />

particular era that<br />

support them come<br />

from the zeitgeist. An<br />

example can be seen<br />

with Henry H.<br />

Goddard and Lewis<br />

Terman. The<br />

consensus in the 20th<br />

century was that<br />

existing psychology<br />

tests adequately<br />

measured basic<br />

intelligence in diverse<br />

groups of people. The<br />

more<br />

recent<br />

consensus is that<br />

"culture-fair" tests<br />

need to be developed -<br />

which may or may not<br />

be true. But because<br />

of the zeitgeist, in those times, the cross-cultural validity of existing tests was not<br />

questioned.<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Failure to question research findings that agree with prevailing political and<br />

philosophical ideology represents one of the effects of the zeitgeist.<br />

The zeitgeist does not always have negative effects. It can stimulate new ideas<br />

and creative solutions to problems. An example is seen in the different models<br />

and metaphors chosen to describe behavior and consciousness.<br />

Charles Darwin's proposition that evolution occurs by natural selection has been<br />

cited as a case of the zeitgeist, since his contemporary, Alfred Russel Wallace,<br />

was outlining similar models to Darwin during the same period (ideas that were<br />

jointly presented to the public). This view is disputed, however, by accounts that<br />

emphasize the relative simplicity of Wallace's model, and which highlight a<br />

supposed novelty within Darwin's likewise simplistic contribution.<br />

The zeitgeist of the 1920s revolved around logical positivism. Due to this, the<br />

great men and women of that time were able to impact psychology, such as<br />

Watson, Tolman, and Guthrie. This is important because their work on behavioral<br />

psychology was able to work against eugenics. Before this time behavioral<br />

Page 74 of 134


psychology was not able to impact the field since it did not fit with the spirit of the<br />

times. For example, Twitmyer Edwin B. Twitmyer wrote a paper on the patellar<br />

reflex in 1902, but it came too early to have the impact it deserved.<br />

<br />

<br />

B F Skinner being unseated during the cognitive revolution is another example of<br />

the zeitgeist in psychology. The zeitgeist was changing during this time, people<br />

wanted to show more interest in humans, and more people were becoming<br />

interested in personality psychology. However, in the 1950s his new<br />

experimental approach to psychology using inductive reasoning and descriptive<br />

behaviorism was seen as novel and practical. Especially in contrast with<br />

psychoanalysts, whose assertions and interpretations were largely immune to<br />

rigorous, empirical inquiry, thus making validation a rather problematic task.<br />

In medical practice clinicians should be aware of the current zeitgeist when<br />

making a decision regarding a patients care or treatment.<br />

Page 75 of 134


Page 76 of 134


VIII. The Illuminati<br />

The Illuminati (plural of Latin illuminatus, "enlightened") is a name given to several<br />

groups, both real and fictitious. Historically, the name usually refers to the Bavarian<br />

Illuminati, an Enlightenment-era secret society founded on 1 May 1776. The society's<br />

goals were to oppose superstition, obscurantism, religious influence over public life, and<br />

abuses of state power. "The order of the day," they wrote in their general statutes, "is to<br />

put an end to the machinations of the purveyors of injustice, to control them without<br />

dominating them." The Illuminati—along with Freemasonry and other secret societies—<br />

were outlawed through edict by the Bavarian ruler Charles Theodore with the<br />

encouragement of the Catholic Church, in 1784, 1785, 1787, and 1790. In the several<br />

years following, the group was vilified by conservative and religious critics who claimed<br />

that they continued underground and were responsible for the French Revolution.<br />

Many influential intellectuals and progressive politicians counted themselves as<br />

members, including Ferdinand of Brunswick and the diplomat Xavier von Zwack, who<br />

was the Order's second-in-command. It attracted literary men such as Johann Wolfgang<br />

von Goethe and Johann Gottfried Herder and the reigning dukes of Gotha and Weimar.<br />

In subsequent use, "Illuminati" refers to various organisations which claim or are<br />

purported to have links to the original Bavarian Illuminati or similar secret societies,<br />

though these links are unsubstantiated. They are often alleged to conspire to control<br />

world affairs, by masterminding events and planting agents<br />

in government and corporations, in order to gain political power and influence and to<br />

Page 77 of 134


establish a New World Order. Central to some of the most widely known and<br />

elaborate conspiracy theories, the Illuminati have been depicted as lurking in the<br />

shadows and pulling the strings and levers of power in dozens of novels, films,<br />

television shows, comics, video games, and music videos.<br />

Adam Weishaupt (1748–1830), founder of the Bavarian Illuminati<br />

Origins<br />

History<br />

Adam Weishaupt (1748–1830) was a professor of Canon Law and practical<br />

philosophy at the University of Ingolstadt. He was the only non-clerical professor at an<br />

institution run by Jesuits, whose order had been dissolved in 1773. The Jesuits of<br />

Ingolstadt, however, still retained the purse strings and some power at the University,<br />

which they continued to regard as their own.<br />

Constant attempts were made to frustrate and discredit non-clerical staff, especially<br />

when course material contained anything they regarded as liberal or Protestant.<br />

Weishaupt became deeply anti-clerical, resolving to spread the ideals of<br />

the Enlightenment (Aufklärung) through some sort of secret society of like-minded<br />

individuals.<br />

Page 78 of 134


Finding Freemasonry to be expensive, and not open to his ideas, he founded his own<br />

society which was to have a gradal system based on Freemasonry, but his own<br />

agenda. His original name for the new order was Bund der Perfektibilisten, or Covenant<br />

of Perfectibility (Perfectibilists), later changing it because it sounded too strange. On 1<br />

May 1776 Weishaupt and four students formed the Perfectibilists, taking the Owl of<br />

Minerva as their symbol. The members were to use aliases within the society.<br />

Weishaupt became Spartacus. Law students Massenhausen, Bauhof, Merz and Sutor<br />

became respectively Ajax, Agathon, Tiberius and Erasmus Roterodamus. Weishaupt<br />

later expelled Sutor for indolence. It was not until April 1778 that the order became<br />

the Illuminatenorden, or Order of Illuminati, after Weishaupt had seriously contemplated<br />

the Bee order.<br />

Massenhausen was initially the most active in expanding the society. Significantly, while<br />

studying in Munich shortly after the formation of the order, he recruited Xavier von<br />

Zwack, a former pupil of Weishaupt at the beginning of a significant administrative<br />

career. (At the time, he was in charge of the Bavarian National Lottery.)<br />

Massenhausen's enthusiasm soon became a liability in the eyes of Weishaupt, often<br />

attempting to recruit unsuitable candidates. Later, his erratic love-life made him<br />

neglectful, and as Weishaupt passed control of the Munich group to Zwack, it became<br />

clear that Massenhausen had misappropriated subscriptions and intercepted<br />

correspondence between Weishaupt and Zwack. In 1778, Massenhausen graduated<br />

and took a post outside Bavaria, taking no further interest in the order. At this time, the<br />

order had a nominal membership of twelve.<br />

With the departure of Massenhausen, Zwack immediately applied himself to recruiting<br />

more mature and important recruits. Most prized by Weishaupt was Hertel, a childhood<br />

friend and a canon of the Munich Frauenkirche. By the end of summer 1778 the order<br />

had 27 members (still counting Massenhausen) in 5 commands; Munich (Athens),<br />

Page 79 of 134


Ingolstadt (Eleusis), Ravensberg (Sparta), Freysingen (Thebes), and Eichstaedt<br />

(Erzurum).<br />

During this early period, the order had three grades of Novice, Minerval, and Illuminated<br />

Minerval, of which only the Minerval grade involved a complicated ceremony. In this the<br />

candidate was given secret signs and a password. A system of mutual espionage kept<br />

Weishaupt informed of the activities and character of all his members, his favourites<br />

becoming members of the ruling council, or Areopagus. Some novices were permitted<br />

to recruit, becoming Insinuants. Christians of good character were actively sought, with<br />

Jews and pagans specifically excluded, along with women, monks, and members of<br />

other secret societies. Favoured candidates were rich, docile, willing to learn, and aged<br />

18–30.<br />

Transition<br />

Having, with difficulty, dissuaded some of his members from joining the Freemasons,<br />

Weishaupt decided to join the older order to acquire material to expand his own ritual.<br />

He was admitted to lodge "Prudence" of the Rite of Strict Observance early in February<br />

1777. His progress through the three degrees of "blue lodge" masonry taught him<br />

nothing of the higher degrees he sought to exploit, but in the following year a priest<br />

called Abbé Marotti informed Zwack that these inner secrets rested on knowledge of the<br />

older religion and the primitive church. Zwack persuaded Weishaupt that their own order<br />

should enter into friendly relations with Freemasonry, and obtain the dispensation to set<br />

up their own lodge. At this stage (December 1778), the addition of the first three<br />

degrees of Freemasonry was seen as a secondary project.<br />

With little difficulty, a warrant was obtained from the Grand Lodge of Prussia called the<br />

Royal York for Friendship, and the new lodge was called Theodore of the Good Council,<br />

with the intention of flattering Charles Theodore, Elector of Bavaria. It was founded in<br />

Munich on 21 March 1779, and quickly packed with Illuminati. The first master, a man<br />

called Radl, was persuaded to return home to Baden, and by July Weishaupt's order ran<br />

the lodge.<br />

The next step involved independence from their Grand Lodge. By establishing masonic<br />

relations with the Union lodge in Frankfurt, affiliated to the Premier Grand Lodge of<br />

England, lodge Theodore became independently recognized, and able to declare its<br />

independence. As a new mother lodge, it could now spawn lodges of its own. The<br />

recruiting drive amongst the Frankfurt masons also obtained the allegiance of Adolph<br />

Freiherr Knigge.<br />

Adolph Knigge<br />

Reform<br />

Knigge was recruited late in 1780 at a convention of the Rite of Strict Observance by<br />

Costanzo Marchese di Costanzo, an infantry captain in the Bavarian army and a fellow<br />

Page 80 of 134


Freemason. Knigge, still in his twenties, had already reached the highest initiatory<br />

grades of his order, and had arrived with his own grand plans for its reform.<br />

Disappointed that his scheme found no support, Knigge was immediately intrigued<br />

when Costanzo informed him that the order that he sought to create already existed.<br />

Knigge and three of his friends expressed a strong interest in learning more of this<br />

order, and Costanzo showed them material relating to the Minerval grade. The teaching<br />

material for the grade was "liberal" literature which was banned in Bavaria, but common<br />

knowledge in the Protestant German states. Knigge's three companions became<br />

disillusioned and had no more to do with Costanzo, but Knigge's persistence was<br />

rewarded in November 1780 by a letter from Weishaupt. Knigge's connections, both<br />

within and outside of Freemasonry, made him an ideal recruit. Knigge, for his own part,<br />

was flattered by the attention, and drawn towards the order's stated aims of education<br />

and the protection of mankind from despotism. Weishaupt managed to acknowledge,<br />

and pledge to support, Knigge's interest in alchemy and the "higher sciences". Knigge<br />

replied to Weishaupt outlining his plans for the reform of Freemasonry as the Strict<br />

Observance began to question its own origins.<br />

Weishaupt set Knigge the task of recruiting before he could be admitted to the higher<br />

grades of the order. Knigge accepted, on the condition that he be allowed to choose his<br />

own recruiting grounds. Many other masons found Knigge's description of the new<br />

masonic order attractive, and were enrolled in the Minerval grade of the Illuminati.<br />

Knigge appeared at this time to believe in the "Most Serene Superiors" which<br />

Page 81 of 134


Weishaupt claimed to serve. His inability to articulate anything about the higher degrees<br />

of the order became increasingly embarrassing, but in delaying any help, Weishaupt<br />

gave him an extra task. Provided with material by Weishaupt, Knigge now produced<br />

pamphlets outlining the activities of the outlawed Jesuits, purporting to show how they<br />

continued to thrive and recruit, especially in Bavaria. Meanwhile, Knigge's inability to<br />

give his recruits any satisfactory response to questions regarding the higher grades was<br />

making his position untenable, and he wrote to Weishaupt to this effect. In January<br />

1781, faced with the prospect of losing Knigge and his masonic recruits, Weishaupt<br />

finally confessed that his superiors and the supposed antiquity of the order were<br />

fictions, and the higher degrees had yet to be written.<br />

If Knigge had expected to learn the promised deep secrets of Freemasonry in the higher<br />

degrees of the Illuminati, he was surprisingly calm about Weishaupt's revelation.<br />

Weishaupt promised Knigge a free hand in the creation of the higher degrees, and also<br />

promised to send him his own notes. For his own part, Knigge welcomed the<br />

opportunity to use the order as a vehicle for his own ideas. His new approach would, he<br />

claimed, make the Illuminati more attractive to prospective members in the Protestant<br />

kingdoms of Germany. In November 1781 the Areopagus advanced Knigge 50 florins to<br />

travel to Bavaria, which he did via Swabia and Franconia, meeting and enjoying the<br />

hospitality of other Illuminati on his journey.<br />

Internal Problems<br />

The order had now developed profound internal divisions. The Eichstaedt command<br />

had formed an autonomous province in July 1780, and a rift was growing between<br />

Weishaupt and the Areopagus, who found him stubborn, dictatorial, and inconsistent.<br />

Knigge fitted readily into the role of peacemaker.<br />

In discussions with the Areopagus and Weishaupt, Knigge identified two areas which<br />

were problematic. Weishaupt's emphasis on the recruitment of university students<br />

meant that senior positions in the order often had to be filled by young men with little<br />

practical experience. Secondly, the anti-Jesuit ethos of the order at its inception had<br />

become a general anti-religious sentiment, which Knigge knew would be a problem in<br />

recruiting the senior Freemasons that the order now sought to attract. Knigge felt keenly<br />

the stifling grip of conservative Catholicism in Bavaria, and understood the anti-religious<br />

feelings that this produced in the liberal Illuminati, but he also saw the negative<br />

impression these same feelings would engender in Protestant states, inhibiting the<br />

spread of the order in greater Germany.<br />

Both the Areopagus and Weishaupt felt powerless to do anything less than give Knigge<br />

a free hand. He had the contacts within and outside of Freemasonry that they needed,<br />

and he had the skill as a ritualist to build their projected gradal structure, where they had<br />

ground to a halt at Illuminatus Minor, with only the Minerval grade below and the merest<br />

sketches of higher grades. The only restrictions imposed were the need to discuss the<br />

inner secrets of the highest grades, and the necessity of submitting his new grades for<br />

approval.<br />

Page 82 of 134


Meanwhile, the scheme to propagate Illuminatism as a legitimate branch of<br />

Freemasonry had stalled. While Lodge Theodore was now in their control, a chapter of<br />

"Elect Masters" attached to it only had one member from the order, and still had a<br />

constitutional superiority to the craft lodge controlled by the Illuminati. The chapter<br />

would be difficult to persuade to submit to the Areopagus, and formed a very real barrier<br />

to Lodge Theodore becoming the first mother-lodge of a new Illuminated Freemasonry.<br />

A treaty of alliance was signed between the order and the chapter, and by the end of<br />

January 1781 four daughter lodges had been created, but independence was not in the<br />

chapter's agenda.<br />

Costanza wrote to the Royal York pointing out the discrepancy between the fees<br />

dispatched to their new Grand Lodge and the service they had received in return. The<br />

Royal York, unwilling to lose the revenue, offered to confer the "higher" secrets of<br />

Freemasonry on a representative that their Munich brethren would dispatch to Berlin.<br />

Costanza accordingly set off for Prussia on 4 April 1780, with instructions to negotiate a<br />

reduction in Theodore's fees while he was there. On the way, he managed to have an<br />

argument with a Frenchman on the subject of a lady with whom they were sharing a<br />

carriage. The Frenchman sent a message ahead to the king, some time before they<br />

reached Berlin, denouncing Costanza as a spy. He was only freed from prison with the<br />

help of the Grand Master of Royal York, and was expelled from Prussia having<br />

accomplished nothing.<br />

New System<br />

Knigge's initial plan to obtain a constitution from London would, they realised, have<br />

been seen through by the chapter. Until such time as they could take over other<br />

Page 83 of 134


masonic lodges that their chapter could not control, they were for the moment content to<br />

rewrite the three degrees for the lodges which they administered.<br />

On 20 January 1782 Knigge tabulated his new system of grades for the order. These<br />

were arranged in three classes:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Class I – The nursery, consisting of the Noviciate, the Minerval, and Illuminatus<br />

minor.<br />

Class II – The Masonic grades. The three "blue lodge" grades of Apprentice,<br />

Companion, and Master were separated from the higher "Scottish" grades of<br />

Scottish Novice and Scottish Knight.<br />

Class III – The Mysteries. The lesser mysteries were the grades of Priest and<br />

Prince, followed by the greater mysteries in the grades of Mage and King. It is<br />

unlikely that the rituals for the greater mysteries were ever written.<br />

Attempts at Expansion<br />

Knigge's recruitment from German Freemasonry was far from random. He targeted the<br />

masters and wardens, the men who ran the lodges, and were often able to place the<br />

entire lodge at the disposal of the Illuminati. In Aachen, Baron de Witte, master of<br />

Constancy lodge, caused every member to join the order. In this way, the order<br />

expanded rapidly in central and southern Germany, and obtained a foothold in Austria.<br />

Moving into the Spring of 1782, the handful of students that had started the order had<br />

swelled to about 300 members, only 20 of the new recruits being students.<br />

In Munich, the first half of 1782 saw huge changes in the government of Lodge<br />

Theodore. In February, Weishaupt had offered to split the lodge, with the Illuminati<br />

going their own way and the chapter taking any remaining traditionalists into their own<br />

continuation of Theodore. At this point, the chapter unexpectedly capitulated, and the<br />

Illuminati had complete control of lodge and chapter. In June, both lodge and chapter<br />

sent letters severing relations with Royal York, citing their own faithfulness in paying for<br />

their recognition, and Royal York's failure to provide any instruction into the higher<br />

grades. Their neglect of Costanza, failure to defend him from malicious charges or<br />

prevent his expulsion from Prussia, were also cited. They had made no effort to provide<br />

Costanza with the promised secrets, and the Munich masons now suspected that their<br />

brethren in Berlin relied on the mystical French higher grades which they sought to<br />

avoid. Lodge Theodore was now independent.<br />

The Rite of Strict Observance was now in a critical state. Its nominal leader was Prince<br />

Carl of Södermanland (later Charles XIII of Sweden), openly suspected of trying to<br />

absorb the rite into the Swedish Rite, which he already controlled. The German lodges<br />

looked for leadership to Duke Ferdinand of Brunswick-Wolfenbüttel. Suspicion turned to<br />

open contempt when it transpired that Carl regarded the Stuart heir to the British throne<br />

as the true Grand Master, and the lodges of the Strict Observance all but ignored their<br />

Grand Master. This impasse led to the Convent of Wilhelmsbad.<br />

Page 84 of 134


Convent of Wilhelmsbad<br />

Delayed from 15 October 1781, the last convention of the Strict Observance finally<br />

opened on 16 July 1782 in the spa town of Wilhelmsbad on the outskirts of (now part<br />

of) Hanau. Ostensibly a discussion of the future of the order, the 35 delegates knew that<br />

the Strict Observance in its current form was doomed, and that the Convent of<br />

Wilhelmsbad would be a struggle over the pieces between the German mystics,<br />

under Duke Ferdinand of Brunswick-Wolfenbüttel and their host Prince Charles of<br />

Hesse-Kassel, and the Martinists, under Jean-Baptiste Willermoz. The only dissenting<br />

voices to mystical higher grades were Johann Joachim Christoph Bode, who was<br />

horrified by Martinism, but whose proposed alternatives were as yet unformed, and<br />

Franz Dietrich von Ditfurth, a judge from Wetzlar and master of the Joseph of the Three<br />

Helmets lodge there, who was already a member of the Illuminati. Ditfurth publicly<br />

campaigned for a return to the basic three degrees of Freemasonry, which was the least<br />

likely outcome of the convention. The mystics already had coherent plans to replace the<br />

higher degrees.<br />

The lack of a coherent alternative to the two strains of mysticism allowed the Illuminati<br />

to present themselves as a credible option. Ditfurth, prompted and assisted by Knigge,<br />

who now had full authority to act for the order, became their spokesman. Knigge's<br />

original plan to propose an alliance between the two orders was rejected by Weishaupt,<br />

who saw no point in an alliance with a dying order. His new plan was to recruit the<br />

masons opposed to the "Templar" higher degree of the Strict Observance.<br />

Page 85 of 134


At the convent, Ditfurth blocked the attempts of Willermoz and Hesse to introduce their<br />

own higher grades by insisting that full details of such degrees be revealed to the<br />

delegates. The frustration of the German mystics led to their enrolling Count Kollowrat<br />

with the Illuminati with a view to later affiliation. Ditfurth's own agenda was to replace all<br />

of the higher degrees with a single fourth degree, with no pretensions to further masonic<br />

revelations. Finding no support for his plan, he left the convent prematurely, writing to<br />

the Areopagus that he expected nothing good of the assembly.<br />

In an attempt to satisfy everybody, the Convent of Wilhelmsbad achieved little. They<br />

renounced the Templar origins of their ritual, while retaining the Templar titles, trappings<br />

and administrative structure. Charles of Hesse and Ferdinand of Brunswick remained at<br />

the head of the order, but in practice the lodges were almost independent. The<br />

Germans also adopted the name of the French order of Willermoz, les Chevaliers<br />

bienfaisants de la Cité sainte (Good Knights of the Holy City), and some Martinist<br />

mysticism was imported into the first three degrees, which were now the only essential<br />

degrees of Freemasonry. Crucially, individual lodges of the order were now allowed to<br />

fraternise with lodges of other systems. The new "Scottish Grade" introduced with the<br />

Lyon ritual of Willermoz was not compulsory, each province and prefecture was free to<br />

decide what, if anything, happened after the three craft degrees. Finally, in an effort to<br />

show that something had been achieved, the convent regulated at length on etiquette,<br />

titles, and a new numbering for the provinces.<br />

Aftermath of Wilhelmsbad<br />

What the Convent of Wilhelmsbad actually achieved was the demise of the Strict<br />

Observance. It renounced its own origin myth, along with the higher degrees which<br />

bound its highest and most influential members. It abolished the strict control which had<br />

kept the order united, and alienated many Germans who mistrusted Martinism. Bode,<br />

who was repelled by Martinism, immediately entered negotiations with Knigge, and<br />

finally joined the Illuminati in January 1783. Charles of Hesse joined the following<br />

month.<br />

Knigge's first efforts at an alliance with the intact German Grand Lodges failed, but<br />

Weishaupt persisted. He proposed a new federation where all of the German lodges<br />

would practise an agreed, unified system in the essential three degrees of<br />

Freemasonry, and be left to their own devices as to which, if any, system of higher<br />

degrees they wished to pursue. This would be a federation of Grand Lodges, and<br />

members would be free to visit any of the "blue" lodges, in any jurisdiction. All lodge<br />

masters would be elected, and no fees would be paid to any central authority<br />

whatsoever. Groups of lodges would be subject to a "Scottish Directorate", composed of<br />

members delegated by lodges, to audit finances, settle disputes, and authorise new<br />

lodges. These in turn would elect Provincial Directorates, who would elect inspectors,<br />

who would elect the national director. This system would correct the current imbalance<br />

in German Freemasonry, where masonic ideals of equality were preserved only in the<br />

lower three "symbolic" degrees. The various systems of higher degrees were dominated<br />

by the elite who could afford researches in alchemy and mysticism. To Weishaupt and<br />

Page 86 of 134


Knigge, the proposed federation was also a vehicle to propagate Illuminism throughout<br />

German Freemasonry. Their intention was to use their new federation, with its emphasis<br />

on the fundamental degrees, to remove all allegiance to Strict Observance, allowing the<br />

"eclectic" system of the Illuminati to take its place.<br />

The circular announcing the new federation outlined the faults of German freemasonry,<br />

that unsuitable men with money were often admitted on the basis of their wealth, that<br />

the corruption of civil society had infected the lodges. Having advocated the deregulation<br />

of the higher grades of the German lodges, the Illuminati now announced<br />

their own, from their "unknown Superiors". Lodge Theodore, newly independent from<br />

Royal York, set themselves up as a provincial Grand Lodge. Knigge, in a letter to all the<br />

Royal York lodges, now accused that Grand Lodge of decadence. Their Freemasonry<br />

had allegedly been corrupted by the Jesuits. Strict Observance was now attacked as a<br />

creation of the Stuarts, devoid of all moral virtue. The Zinnendorf rite of the Grand<br />

Landlodge of the Freemasons of Germany was suspect because its author was in<br />

league with the Swedes. This direct attack had the opposite effect to that intended by<br />

Weishaupt, it offended many of its readers.<br />

The Grand Lodge of the Grand Orient of Warsaw, which controlled Freemasonry in<br />

Poland and Lithuania, was happy to participate in the federation only as far as the first<br />

three degrees. Their insistence on independence had kept them from the Strict<br />

Observance, and would now keep them from the Illuminati, whose plan to annex<br />

Freemasonry rested on their own higher degrees. By the end of January 1783 the<br />

Illuminati's masonic contingent had seven lodges.<br />

Page 87 of 134


It was not only the clumsy appeal of the Illuminati that left the federation short of<br />

members. Lodge Theodore was recently formed and did not command respect like the<br />

older lodges. Most of all, the Freemasons most likely to be attracted to the federation<br />

saw the Illuminati as an ally against the mystics and Martinists, but valued their own<br />

freedom too highly to be caught in another restrictive organisation. Even Ditfurth, the<br />

supposed representative of the Illuminati at Wilhelmsbad, had pursued his own agenda<br />

at the convent.<br />

The non-mystical Frankfurt lodges created an "Eclectic Alliance", which was almost<br />

indistinguishable in constitution and aims from the Illuminati's federation. Far from<br />

seeing this as a threat, after some discussion the Illuminati lodges joined the new<br />

alliance. Three Illuminati now sat on the committee charged with writing the new<br />

masonic statutes. Aside from strengthening relations between their three lodges, the<br />

Illuminati seem to have gained no advantage from this manoeuvre. Ditfurth, having<br />

found a masonic organisation that worked towards his own ambitions for Freemasonry,<br />

took little interest in the Illuminati after his adherence to the Eclectic Alliance. In reality,<br />

the creation of the Eclectic Alliance had undermined all of the subtle plans of the<br />

Illuminati to spread their own doctrine through Freemasonry.<br />

Zenith<br />

Although their hopes of mass recruitment through Freemasonry had been frustrated, the<br />

Illuminati continued to recruit well at an individual level. In Bavaria, the succession<br />

of Charles Theodore initially led to a liberalisation of attitudes and laws, but the clergy<br />

and courtiers, guarding their own power and privilege, persuaded the weak willed<br />

monarch to reverse his reforms, and Bavaria's repression of liberal thought returned.<br />

This reversal led to a general resentment of the monarch and the church among the<br />

educated classes, which provided a perfect recruiting ground for the Illuminati. A<br />

number of Freemasons from Prudence lodge, disaffected by the Martinist rites of<br />

the Chevaliers Bienfaisants, joined lodge Theodore, who set themselves up in a<br />

gardened mansion which contained their library of liberal literature.<br />

Illuminati circles in the rest of Germany expanded. While some had only modest gains,<br />

the circle in Mainz almost doubled from 31 to 61 members. Reaction to state<br />

Catholicism led to gains in Austria, and footholds were obtained<br />

in Warsaw, Pressburg (Bratislava), Tyrol, Milan and Switzerland.<br />

The total number of verifiable members at the end of 1784 is around 650. Weishaupt<br />

and Hertel later claimed a figure of 2,500. The higher figure is largely explained by the<br />

inclusion of members of masonic lodges that the Illuminati claimed to control, but it is<br />

likely that the names of all the Illuminati are not known, and the true figure lies<br />

somewhere between 650 and 2,500. The importance of the order lay in its successful<br />

recruitment of the professional classes, churchmen, academics, doctors and lawyers,<br />

and its more recent acquisition of powerful benefactors. Karl August, Grand Duke of<br />

Saxe-Weimar-Eisenach, Ernest II, Duke of Saxe-Gotha-Altenburg with his brother and<br />

later successor August, Karl Theodor Anton Maria von Dalberg governor of Erfurt, Duke<br />

Page 88 of 134


Ferdinand of Brunswick-Wolfenbüttel(already mentioned), his chief assistant in masonic<br />

matters, Johann Friedrich von Schwarz, and Count Metternich of Koblenz were all<br />

enrolled. In Vienna, Count Brigido, governor of Galicia, Count Leopold Kolowrat,<br />

chancellor of Bohemia with his vice-chancellor Baron Kressel, Count Pálffy von Erdöd,<br />

chancellor of Hungary, Count Banffy, governor and provincial Grand Master of<br />

Transylvania, Count Stadion, ambassador to London, and Baron von Swieten, minister<br />

of public education, also joined.<br />

There were notable failures. Johann Kaspar Lavater, the Swiss poet and theologian,<br />

rebuffed Knigge. He did not believe the order's humanitarian and rationalist aims were<br />

achievable by secret means. He further believed that a society's drive for members<br />

would ultimately submerge its founding ideals. Christoph Friedrich Nicolai, the Berlin<br />

writer and bookseller, became disillusioned after joining. He found its aims chimeric,<br />

and thought that the use of Jesuit methods to achieve their aims was dangerous. He<br />

remained in the order, but took no part in recruitment.<br />

Conflict with Rosicrucians<br />

At all costs, Weishaupt wished to keep the existence of the order secret from<br />

the Rosicrucians, who already had a considerable foothold in German Freemasonry.<br />

While clearly Protestant, the Rosicrucians were anything but anticlerical, pro-monarchic,<br />

and held views clearly conflicting with the Illuminati vision of a rationalist state run by<br />

philosophers and scientists. The Rosicrucians were not above promoting their own<br />

brand of mysticism with fraudulent séances. A conflict became inevitable as the<br />

Page 89 of 134


existence of the Illuminati became more evident, and as prominent Rosicrucians, and<br />

mystics with Rosicrucian sympathies, were actively recruited by Knigge and other overenthusiastic<br />

helpers. Kolowrat was already a high ranking Rosicrucian, and the mystic<br />

Prince Charles of Hesse-Kassel had a very low opinion of the rationalist higher grades<br />

of the Illuminati.<br />

The Prussian Rosicrucians, under Johann Christoph von Wöllner, began a sustained<br />

attack on the Illuminati. Wöllner had a specially engineered room in which he convinced<br />

potential patrons of the effectiveness of Rosicrucian "magic", and his order had<br />

acquired effective control of the "Three Globes" and its attached lodges. Through this<br />

mouthpiece, the Illuminati were accused of atheism and revolutionary tendencies. In<br />

April 1783 Frederick the Great informed Charles of Hesse that the Berlin lodges had<br />

documents belonging to the Minervals or Illuminati which contained appalling material,<br />

and asked if he had heard of them. All Berlin masons were now warned against the<br />

order, which was now accused of Socinianism, and of using the liberal writings of<br />

Voltaire and others, alongside the tolerance of Freemasonry, to undermine all religion.<br />

In November 1783 the Three Globes described the Illuminati as a masonic sect which<br />

sought to undermine Christianity and turn Freemasonry into a political system. Their<br />

final anathema, in November 1784, refused to recognise any Illuminati as Freemasons.<br />

In Austria, the Illuminati were blamed for anti-religious pamphlets that had recently<br />

appeared. The Rosicrucians spied on Joseph von Sonnenfels and other suspected<br />

Illuminati, and their campaign of denunciation within Freemasonry completely shut down<br />

Illuminati recruitment in Tyrol.<br />

The Bavarian Illuminati, whose existence was already known to the Rosicrucians from<br />

an informant, were further betrayed by the reckless actions of Ferdinand Maria Baader,<br />

an Areopagite who now joined the Rosicrucians. Shortly after his admission it was made<br />

known to his superiors that he was one of the Illuminati, and he was informed that he<br />

could not be a member of both organizations. His letter of resignation stated that the<br />

Rosicrucians did not possess secret knowledge, and ignored the truly Illuminated,<br />

specifically identifying Lodge Theodore as an Illuminati Lodge.<br />

Internal Dissent<br />

As the Illuminati embraced Freemasonry and expanded outside Bavaria, the council of<br />

the Areopagites was replaced by an ineffective "Council of Provincials". The<br />

Areopagites, however, remained as powerful voices within the Order, and began again<br />

to bicker with Weishaupt as soon as Knigge left Munich. Weishaupt responded by<br />

privately slandering his perceived enemies in letters to his perceived friends.<br />

More seriously, Weishaupt succeeded in alienating Knigge. Weishaupt had ceded<br />

considerable power to Knigge in deputising him to write the ritual, power he now sought<br />

to regain. Knigge had elevated the Order from a tiny anti-clerical club to a large<br />

organization, and felt that his work was under-acknowledged. Weishaupt's continuing<br />

anti-clericalism clashed with Knigge's mysticism, and recruitment of mystically inclined<br />

Page 90 of 134


Freemasons was a cause of friction with Weishaupt and other senior Illuminati, such as<br />

Ditfurth. Matters came to a head over the grade of Priest. The consensus among many<br />

of the Illuminati was that the ritual was florid and ill-conceived, and the regalia puerile<br />

and expensive. Some refused to use it, others edited it. Weishaupt demanded that<br />

Knigge rewrite the ritual. Knigge pointed out that it was already circulated, with<br />

Weishaupt's blessing, as ancient. This fell on deaf ears. Weishaupt now claimed to<br />

other Illuminati that the Priest ritual was flawed because Knigge had invented it.<br />

Offended, Knigge now threatened to tell the world how much of the Illuminati ritual he<br />

had made up. Knigge's attempt to create a convention of the Areopagites proved<br />

fruitless, as most of them trusted him even less than they trusted Weishaupt. In July<br />

1784 Knigge left the order by agreement, under which he returned all relevant papers,<br />

and Weishaupt published a retraction of all slanders against him. In forcing Knigge out,<br />

Weishaupt deprived the order of its best theoretician, recruiter, and apologist.<br />

Decline<br />

The final decline of the Illuminati was brought about by the indiscretions of their own<br />

Minervals in Bavaria, and especially in Munich. In spite of efforts by their superiors to<br />

curb loose talk, politically dangerous boasts of power and criticism of monarchy caused<br />

the "secret" order's existence to become common knowledge, along with the names of<br />

many important members. The presence of Illuminati in positions of power now led to<br />

some public disquiet. There were Illuminati in many civic and state governing bodies. In<br />

spite of their small number, there were claims that success in a legal dispute depended<br />

on the litigant's standing with the order. The Illuminati were blamed for several antireligious<br />

publications then appearing in Bavaria. Much of this criticism sprang from<br />

vindictiveness and jealousy, but it is clear that many Illuminati court officials gave<br />

preferential treatment to their brethren. In Bavaria, the energy of their two members of<br />

the Ecclesiastical Council had one of them elected treasurer. Their opposition<br />

Page 91 of 134


to Jesuits resulted in the banned order losing key academic and church positions. In<br />

Ingolstat, the Jesuit heads of department were replaced by Illuminati.<br />

Alarmed, Karl Theodor and his government banned all secret societies including the<br />

Illuminati. A government edict dated 2 March 1785 "seems to have been deathblow to<br />

the Illuminati in Bavaria". Weishaupt had fled and documents and internal<br />

correspondence, seized in 1786 and 1787, were subsequently published by the<br />

government in 1787. Von Zwack's home was searched and much of the group's<br />

literature was disclosed.<br />

Barruel and Robison<br />

Between 1797 and 1798, Augustin Barruel's Memoirs Illustrating the History of<br />

Jacobinism and John Robison's Proofs of a Conspiracy publicised the theory that the<br />

Illuminati had survived and represented an ongoing international conspiracy. This<br />

included the claim that it was behind the French Revolution. Both books proved to be<br />

very popular, spurring reprints and paraphrases by others. A prime example of this<br />

is Proofs of the Real Existence, and Dangerous Tendency, Of Illuminism by Reverend<br />

Seth Payson, published in 1802. Some of the response to this was critical, for example<br />

Jean-Joseph Mounier's On the Influence Attributed to Philosophers, Free-Masons, and<br />

to the Illuminati on the Revolution of France.<br />

The works of Robison and Barruel made their way to the United States, and across New<br />

England, Reverend Jedidiah Morse and others gave sermons against the Illuminati.<br />

Their sermons were printed and the matter was followed in newspapers. Concern died<br />

down in the first decade of the 1800s, although it revived from time to time in the Anti-<br />

Masonic movement of the 1820s and 30s.<br />

Modern Illuminati<br />

Several recent and present-day fraternal organisations claim to be descended from the<br />

original Bavarian Illuminati and openly use the name "Illuminati". Some of these groups<br />

use a variation on the name "The Illuminati Order" in the name of their own<br />

organisations, while others, such as the Ordo Templi Orientis, have "Illuminati" as a<br />

level within their organisation's hierarchy. However, there is no evidence that these<br />

present-day groups have amassed significant political power or influence, and most,<br />

rather than trying to remain secret, promote unsubstantiated links to the Bavarian<br />

Illuminati as a means of attracting membership.<br />

In Conspiracy Theories<br />

In Popular Culture<br />

The Illuminati did not long survive their suppression in Bavaria, and their further<br />

mischief and plottings in the work of Barruel and Robison must be considered as the<br />

Page 92 of 134


invention of the writers. Conspiracy theorists and writers such as Mark Dice have<br />

argued that the Illuminati have survived to this day.<br />

Many conspiracy theories propose that world events are being controlled and<br />

manipulated by a secret society calling itself the Illuminati. Conspiracy theorists have<br />

claimed that many notable people were or are members of the Illuminati. Presidents of<br />

the United States are a common target for such claims.<br />

Other theorists contend that a variety of historical events were orchestrated by the<br />

Illuminati, from the French Revolution, the Battle of Waterloo and the assassination of<br />

U.S. President John F. Kennedy, to an alleged communist plot to hasten the "New<br />

World Order" by infiltrating the Hollywood film industry.<br />

In Fiction<br />

The Illuminati play a central role in the plots of many novels and movies such as Angels<br />

& Demons by Dan Brown, The Illuminatus! Trilogy by Robert Shea and Robert Anton<br />

Wilson and Foucault's Pendulum by Umberto Eco.<br />

References to the Illuminati are made in several video games, such as the Assassin's<br />

Creed franchise.<br />

Page 93 of 134


Page 94 of 134


IX. The Star Chamber<br />

The Star Chamber (Latin: Camera stellata) was an English court of law which sat<br />

at the royal Palace of Westminster, from the late 15th century to the mid-17th century<br />

(c.1641), and was composed of Privy Councillorsand common-law judges, to<br />

supplement the judicial activities of the common-law and equity courts in civil and<br />

criminal matters. The Star Chamber was originally established to ensure the fair<br />

enforcement of laws against socially and politically prominent people so powerful that<br />

ordinary courts would probably hesitate to convict them of their crimes. However, it<br />

became synonymous with social and political oppression through the arbitrary use and<br />

abuse of the power it wielded.<br />

In modern usage, legal or administrative bodies with strict, arbitrary rulings and<br />

secretive proceedings are sometimes called, metaphorically or poetically, "star<br />

chambers". This is a pejorative term and intended to cast doubt on the legitimacy of the<br />

proceedings. "Star Chamber" can also, rarely, be used in its original meaning, for<br />

instance when a politician uses parliamentary privilege to examine and then exculpate<br />

Page 95 of 134


or condemn a powerful organization or person. Due to the constitutional separation of<br />

powers and the ceasing of the Star Chamber, the main powers of select committees are<br />

to enhance the public debate — politicians are deemed to no longer wield powers in the<br />

criminal law, which belongs to the courts.<br />

Origin of The Name<br />

The first reference to the "star chamber" is in 1398, as the Sterred chambre; the more<br />

common form of the name appears in 1422 as le Sterne-chamere. Both forms recur<br />

throughout the fifteenth century, with Sterred Chambre last attested as appearing in<br />

the Supremacy of the Crown Act 1534. The origin of the name has usually been<br />

explained as first recorded by John Stow, writing in his Survey of London (1598), who<br />

noted "this place is called the Star Chamber, at the first all the roofe thereof was decked<br />

with images of starres gilted". Gold stars on a blue background were a common<br />

medieval decoration for ceilings in richly decorated rooms, as still to be seen<br />

at Leasowe Castle, Wirral, the Scrovegni Chapel in Padua, and elsewhere.<br />

Alternatively, William Blackstone, a notable English jurist writing in 1769, speculated<br />

that the name may have derived from the legal word "starr" meaning the contract or<br />

obligation to a Jew (from the Hebrew שטר (shetar) meaning 'document'). This term was<br />

in use until 1290, when Edward I had all Jews expelled from England. Blackstone<br />

thought the "Starr Chamber" might originally have been used for the deposition and<br />

storage of such contracts. However, the Oxford English Dictionary gives this etymology<br />

"no claim to consideration".<br />

Other etymological theories mentioned by Blackstone on the use of star include the<br />

derivation from Old English steoran (steer) meaning "to govern"; as a court used to<br />

punish (crimen stellionatus) (cozenage); or that the chamber was full of windows.<br />

History<br />

Under the Plantagenets and Tudors<br />

The Court evolved from meetings of the King's Council, with its roots going back to the<br />

medieval period. Contrary to popular belief, the so-called "Star Chamber Act" of King<br />

Henry VII's second Parliament (1487) did not actually empower the Star Chamber, but<br />

rather created a separate tribunal distinct from the King's general Council.<br />

Initially well regarded because of its speed and flexibility, Star Chamber was regarded<br />

as one of the most just and efficient courts of the Tudor era. Sir Edward Coke once<br />

described Star Chamber as "The most honorable court (Our Parliament excepted) that<br />

is in the Christian world. Both in respect of the judges in the court and its honourable<br />

proceeding."<br />

The Star Chamber was made up of Privy Counsellors, as well as common-law judges,<br />

and it supplemented the activities of the common-law and equity courts in<br />

both civil and criminal matters. In a sense, the court was a court of appeal, a<br />

Page 96 of 134


supervisory body, overseeing the operation of the lower courts, although it could hear<br />

cases by direct appeal as well. The court was set up to ensure the fair enforcement of<br />

laws against the English upper class, those so powerful that ordinary courts could never<br />

convict them of their crimes.<br />

Another function of the Court of Star Chamber was to act like a court of equity, which<br />

could impose punishment for actions which were deemed to be morally reprehensible<br />

but were not in violation of the letter of the law. This gave the Star Chamber great<br />

flexibility, as it could punish defendants for any action which the court felt should be<br />

unlawful, even when in fact it was technically lawful.<br />

However, this meant that the justice meted out by the Star Chamber could be<br />

very arbitrary and subjective, and it enabled the court to be used later on in its history as<br />

an instrument of oppression rather than for the purpose of justice for which it was<br />

intended. Many crimes which are now commonly prosecuted, such<br />

as attempt, conspiracy, criminal libel, and perjury, were originally developed by the<br />

Court of Star Chamber, along with its more common role of dealing<br />

with riots and sedition.<br />

Page 97 of 134


The cases decided in those sessions enabled both the very powerful and those without<br />

power to seek redress. Thus King Henry VII used the power of Star Chamber to break<br />

the power of the landed gentry which had been such a cause of problems in the Wars of<br />

the Roses. Yet, when local courts were often clogged or mismanaged, the Court of Star<br />

Chamber also became a site of remittance for the common people against the excesses<br />

of the nobility.<br />

In the reign of King Henry VIII, the court was under the leadership of Cardinal<br />

Wolsey (the Archbishop of York and Lord Chancellor) and Thomas<br />

Cranmer (the Archbishop of Canterbury) (1515–1529). From this time forward, the Court<br />

of Star Chamber became a political weapon for bringing actions against opponents to<br />

the policies of King Henry VIII, his Ministers and his Parliament.<br />

Although it was initially a court of appeal, King Henry, Wolsey and Cranmer<br />

encouraged plaintiffs to bring their cases directly to the Star Chamber, bypassing the<br />

lower courts entirely.<br />

The Court was used extensively to control Wales, after the Laws in Wales Acts 1535–<br />

1542 (sometimes referred to as the "Acts of Union"). The Tudor-era gentry in Wales<br />

turned to the Chamber to evict Welsh landowners and protect themselves, and in<br />

general protect the English advantages of the Laws in Wales Acts.<br />

One of the weapons of the Star Chamber was the ex officio oath where, because of<br />

their positions, individuals were forced to swear to answer truthfully all questions that<br />

might be asked. Faced by hostile questioning, this then gave them the "cruel trilemma"<br />

of having to incriminate themselves, face charges of perjury if they gave unsatisfactory<br />

answers to their accusers, or be held in contempt of court if they gave no answer.<br />

Under the Stuarts<br />

The power of the Court of Star Chamber grew considerably under the House of Stuart,<br />

and by the time of King Charles I, it had become synonymous with misuse and abuse of<br />

power by the King and his circle. King James I and his son Charles used the court to<br />

examine cases of sedition, which meant that the court could be used to suppress<br />

opposition to royal policies. It came to be used to try nobles too powerful to be brought<br />

to trial in the lower court.<br />

King Charles I used the Court of Star Chamber as Parliamentary substitute during the<br />

eleven years of Personal Rule, when he ruled without a Parliament. King Charles made<br />

extensive use of the Court of Star Chamber to prosecute dissenters, including<br />

the Puritans who fled to New England. This was also one of the causes of the English<br />

Civil War.<br />

On 17 October 1632, the Court of Star Chamber banned all "news books" because of<br />

complaints from Spanish and Austrian diplomats that coverage of the Thirty Years'<br />

War in England was unfair. As a result, newsbooks pertaining to this matter were often<br />

Page 98 of 134


printed in Amsterdam and then smuggled into the country, until control of the press<br />

collapsed with the developing ideological conflict of 1640–41.<br />

The Star Chamber became notorious for judgments favourable to the king. Archbishop<br />

Laud had William Prynne branded on both cheeks through its agency in 1637<br />

for seditious libel.<br />

In 1571 Elizabeth I had set up an equivalent Court in Ireland, the Court of Castle<br />

Chamber, to deal with cases of riot and offences against public order. Although it was<br />

initially popular with private litigants, under the Stuarts it developed the same reputation<br />

for harsh and arbitrary proceedings as its parent Court, and during the political<br />

confusion of the 1640s it simply disappeared.<br />

In the early 1900s, American<br />

poet,<br />

biographer<br />

and dramatist Edgar Lee<br />

Masters, 1868–1950,<br />

commented:<br />

In the Star Chamber the<br />

council could inflict any<br />

punishment short of death,<br />

and frequently sentenced<br />

objects of its wrath to<br />

the pillory, to whipping and to<br />

the cutting off of ears. ... With<br />

each embarrassment to<br />

arbitrary power the Star<br />

Chamber<br />

became<br />

emboldened to undertake<br />

further usurpation. ... The<br />

Star Chamber finally<br />

summoned juries before it for<br />

verdicts disagreeable to the<br />

government, and fined and<br />

imprisoned them. It spread terrorism among those who were called to do constitutional<br />

acts. It imposed ruinous fines. It became the chief defense of Charles against assaults<br />

upon those usurpations which cost him his life.<br />

Abolition and Aftermath<br />

In 1641, the Long Parliament, led by John Pym and inflamed by the severe treatment<br />

of John Lilburne, as well as that of other religious dissenters such as William<br />

Prynne, Alexander Leighton, John Bastwick and Henry Burton, abolished the Star<br />

Chamber with an Act of Parliament: the Habeas Corpus Act 1640.<br />

Page 99 of 134


The Chamber itself stood until demolished in 1806, when its materials were salvaged.<br />

The door now hangs in the nearby Westminster School and the historic Star Chamber<br />

ceiling, with its bright gold stars, was brought to Leasowe Castle on the Wirral<br />

Peninsula in Cheshire from the Court of Westminster, along with four tapestries<br />

depicting the four seasons.<br />

Recent History<br />

In the late 20th century, the expression was revived in reference to ways resolving<br />

internal high-level questions within the government, usually relating to budget<br />

appropriations. The press and some civil servants under the Premiership of Margaret<br />

Thatcher (1979–90) revived the term for private ministerial meetings at which disputes<br />

between the Treasury and high-spending departments were resolved.<br />

The term was again revived by the popular press to describe a panel set up by the<br />

Labour party's National Executive Committee to review expenses claims by Labour MPs<br />

in May 2009. In 2010, the press employed the term for a committee established by<br />

the Cameron ministry to plan spending cuts to reduce public debt.<br />

Influence on the U.S. Constitution<br />

The historical abuses of the Star Chamber are considered a primary motivating force<br />

behind the protections against compelled self-incrimination embodied in the Fifth<br />

Amendment to the United States Constitution. The meaning of "compelled testimony"<br />

under the Fifth Amendment – i.e., the conditions under which a defendant is allowed to<br />

"plead the Fifth" to avoid self-incrimination – is thus often interpreted via reference to<br />

the inquisitorial methods of the Star Chamber.<br />

As the U.S. Supreme Court described it, "the Star Chamber has, for centuries,<br />

symbolized disregard of basic individual rights. The Star Chamber not merely allowed,<br />

but required, defendants to have counsel. The defendant's answer to an indictment was<br />

not accepted unless it was signed by counsel. When counsel refused to sign the<br />

answer, for whatever reason, the defendant was considered to have confessed."<br />

Page 100 of 134


X. References<br />

1. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Culture_change<br />

2. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Behavioral_economics<br />

3. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/<strong>Cultural</strong>_capital<br />

4. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/<strong>Cultural</strong>_economics<br />

5. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Paradigm_shift<br />

6. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Conspiracy_theory<br />

7. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Zeitgeist<br />

8. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Illuminati<br />

9. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Star_Chamber<br />

10. https://www.valuescentre.com/sites/default/files/uploads/2010-07-20/Fundamentals.pdf<br />

11. http://cdn2.hubspot.net/hub/109601/file-1712448665-<br />

pdf/downloads/White_Papers/Culture_<strong>Transformation</strong>_White_Paper_0914.pdf?t=14406121390<br />

94<br />

12. file:///C:/Users/tuh41865/Downloads/Liu_et_al-2014-<br />

Asian_Journal_of_Social_Psychology.pdf<br />

Page 101 of 134


Notes<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

Page 102 of 134


Notes<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________________<br />

Page 103 of 134


Page 104 of 134


Attachment A<br />

Fundamentals of <strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Page 105 of 134


Fundamentals of <strong>Cultural</strong><br />

<strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Implementing whole system change<br />

By Richard Barrett<br />

Abstract<br />

This paper explores the concept of whole system change. It identifies the four principles<br />

necessary for whole system change, and provides a nine step process for implementing<br />

cultural transformation.<br />

Whole system change<br />

As the title of this paper suggests, for cultural transformation to occur the wholesystem<br />

has to change. I can best explain what is meant by the term “whole system” by<br />

referring to Figure 1. The four quadrants in this figure represent the four different<br />

perspectives of a human system. 1<br />

• The perspective as viewed from inside an individual—personal values and<br />

beliefs (top left quadrant)<br />

• The perspective as viewed from outside of an individual—personal actions and<br />

behaviours (top right quadrant)<br />

• The perspective as viewed from inside of a collective—cultural values and<br />

beliefs (the bottom left)<br />

• The perspective as viewed from outside of a collective—social structures,<br />

systems, processes, actions and behaviours (bottom right quadrant).<br />

1 Ken Wilbur. A Brief History of Everything. New York: Shambala, 1996; p. 71.<br />

1 | P age


Internal<br />

External<br />

Individual<br />

Values and<br />

beliefs of the<br />

individual<br />

Actions and<br />

behaviours of<br />

the individual<br />

Collective<br />

Values and<br />

beliefs of the<br />

collective<br />

Actions and<br />

behaviours of<br />

the collective<br />

The four quadrants are linked in the following way. When individuals change their<br />

values and beliefs (top left quadrant), their behaviours change (top right). When<br />

sufficient numbers of people change their values, beliefs and behaviors, then a shift in<br />

the collective values and beliefs occurs (bottom left). This results in a change in the<br />

behaviours of the whole group (bottom right).<br />

Figure 1: The four quadrants of human systems<br />

In an organisational setting, the values, beliefs (top left quadrant), and behaviours (top<br />

right quadrant) of the leader and the leadership group significantly influence the<br />

values and beliefs of the collective (bottom left quadrant) and the behaviours of the<br />

collective (bottom right quadrant). In other words, the leaders’ values, beliefs and<br />

behaviours significantly influence the culture of the organisation.<br />

To be even more precise, the culture of an organisation is a reflection of the values and<br />

beliefs of the present leaders and the instutionalised legacy of the values and beliefs of<br />

past leaders as reflected in the structures, systems, processes, policies and procedures<br />

of the organisation.<br />

Four conditions must be met for whole system change to occur. These are described<br />

below and shown in Figure 2.<br />

• Personal alignment: There must be an alignment between the values and beliefs<br />

of individuals and their words, actions, and behaviours. This is particularly<br />

important for the leadership group. It is important that leaders are authentic—<br />

that they walk their talk.<br />

2 | P age


• Structural alignment: There must be an alignment between the stated values of<br />

the organisation, and the behaviours of the organisation, as institutionalised in<br />

the structures, systems, processes, policies, incentives and procedures. It is<br />

important that the values of the organisation are fully reflected in all the<br />

structures, systems, processes, policies, incentives and procedures of the<br />

organisation to institutionalise the culture.<br />

• Values alignment: There must be an alignment between the personal values of<br />

employees and the stated values of the organisation. It is important that all<br />

employees feel at home in the organisation and can bring their whole selves to<br />

work.<br />

• Mission alignment: There must be an alignment between sense of motivation<br />

and purpose of all employees, and the mission and vision of the organisation. It<br />

is important that every employee, manager and leader has a clear line of sight<br />

between the work they do each day and the mission or vision of the<br />

organisation, so they know how they make a difference.<br />

Every culture change or transformation initiative must aim at satisfying all four of<br />

these conditions if it is to be successful: it will fail if the whole system doesn’t change.<br />

Internal<br />

External<br />

Individual<br />

Personal Alignment<br />

Values<br />

Alignment<br />

Mission<br />

Alignment<br />

Collective<br />

Structural Alignment<br />

3 | P age


Figure 2: The four conditions for whole system change<br />

The process of whole system change<br />

With this background and understanding we can now delineate the key steps in<br />

implementing a cultural transformation process.<br />

Step 1: Commitment and ownership<br />

The process of whole system change begins with the personal commitment of the<br />

leader and the leadership team to their own personal transformation. This is necessary<br />

because the culture of the organisation is a reflection of leadership consciousness. If<br />

you want transform the culture of your organisation, the leaders must transform or<br />

the leaders must change. Organisational transformation begins with the personal<br />

transformation of the leaders.<br />

If there is no commitment by the leader and his or her leadership team to personal<br />

behaviour change it will be pointless embarking on any form of cultural transformation<br />

process.<br />

A culture change initiative must be owned and personally supervised by the leader of<br />

an organisation, and fully supported by the leadership team. <strong>Cultural</strong> transformation is<br />

not something that can be delegated: nor can it be handed off to a team of outside<br />

consultants. It is something that the organisation has to do for itself, and it is always<br />

ongoing: it is not a project, it is a process!<br />

At this stage it will be important, if he or she has not already done so, for the leader to<br />

hand pick his leadership team. Getting the right people on the bus and sitting in the<br />

right seats is extremely important.<br />

It is quite usual for there to be one or two naysayers in the leadership team who are<br />

not willing to sign up for personal transformation. They are happy for others to do it,<br />

but they are not interested themselves. This is the point where they have to decide to<br />

either get on or off the bus. There is no room on the bus for anyone who is not a<br />

willing participant and committed to the process. It is usually at this point that the<br />

naysayers start looking for alternative employment. It is important that the leader be<br />

aware that this might happen, and is willing to go ahead for the good of the company.<br />

Step 2: Baseline measurement<br />

After the leader and the leadership team have committed to the process, begin by<br />

carrying out a cultural values assessment of the whole organisation, and, at the same<br />

time build a scorecard of the organisation’s current levels of performance—revenues,<br />

employee engagement, customer satisfaction, cultural entropy, values alignment, etc.<br />

The idea here is develop a set of baseline measurements from which you can measure<br />

the progress of your cultural transformation initiative.<br />

Step 3: Vision and mission<br />

4 | P age


After you have completed your baseline measurements, the next step is to define<br />

where the company is going and how it is going to get there. It is time to develop an<br />

internal and external vision and mission for the organisation using the Four Why’s<br />

process described in Liberating the Corporate Soul. 2<br />

If the organisation already has a vision and mission, it will be important to revisit it,<br />

especially if there are new people in the leadership team. Setting the vision is the job<br />

of the leadership team. This task cannot be delegated. The direct reports of the<br />

leadership team (the teams of the members of the leadership team) should be asked<br />

for their inputs and comments once the leadership team is comfortable with the vision<br />

statements they have produced.<br />

The vision statements should be:<br />

• Short and easily memorable<br />

• Inspire people in the organisation to make a difference<br />

Step 4: Values and behaviours<br />

As part of the process of the developing a vision and mission for the organisation it will<br />

be important also to define the organisation’s values and behaviours. The results of<br />

the cultural values assessment will be useful in this regard. To the extent possible, all<br />

employees should be involved in this process. The values should:<br />

• Be single words or small phrases that are easily memorable<br />

• Support the vision and mission<br />

• Be four, and not more than five<br />

• Include relationship values as well as organisational values— i.e. trust and<br />

continuous improvement<br />

Once the organisation’s espoused values have been identified, two or three behaviour<br />

statements should be developed for each value. The purpose of developing behaviour<br />

statements is twofold:<br />

• To give clarity to what each espoused value means in the context of the day‐today<br />

operations of the organisation<br />

• To provide a way of evaluating executive and employee performance.<br />

Because behaviours are always contextual, it is not unusual for different behaviours to<br />

be used for the same espoused values in different parts of the organisation. The<br />

behaviour statements should:<br />

• Be short, memorable, one‐sentence statements<br />

2 Richard Barrett. Liberating the Corporate Soul: Building a Visionary Organisation. Boston: Butterworth<br />

Heinemann, 1998; pp. 103‐123.<br />

5 | P age


• Describe the actions that support the value they represent<br />

• Be appropriate for the context of the work unit<br />

Step 5: Compelling reasons for change<br />

There must be a clear understanding among the executive population about why the<br />

organisation is embarking on a whole system change process. The change process<br />

must be grounded in reality, and driven by realistic optimism that provides the<br />

employee and executive populations with hope for success. People want to be<br />

associated with success. In companies that are suffering from low performance, the<br />

compelling reasons for change are usually obvious. The issues that underlie the poor<br />

performance can easily be identified from the results of the cultural values<br />

assessment.<br />

For high performing companies, the compelling reasons for change should focus on<br />

three factors—how the company can stay adaptable, positioning itself for the future,<br />

and building its long‐term resilience.<br />

Step 6: Personal alignment<br />

Personal alignment should begin with the leadership team. To this end, it will be<br />

important for all members of the leadership team, and the extended leadership group,<br />

to focus on their own personal mastery by seeking feedback and, if necessary, receive<br />

coaching, using a 360 0 instrument such as the Leadership Values Assessment (LVA).<br />

Once the leadership team has embarked on a process of personal mastery to enhance<br />

their emotional intelligence, the direct reports of the leadership team should follow<br />

suit. Eventually, everyone in the organisation that has a management or supervisory<br />

role should participate in some form of personal mastery process.<br />

Step 7: Structural alignment<br />

The purpose of the structural alignment programme is to reconfigure the structures,<br />

systems, processes, policies, incentives and procedures so that they fully reflect the<br />

desired vision, mission, values and behaviours of the organisation, thereby<br />

institutionalising them into the culture of the organisation. The systems and processes<br />

that may need to be reconfigured include:<br />

• New employee/executive selection<br />

• New employee/executive orientation<br />

• Employee/executive performance evaluation<br />

• Employee/executive promotion criteria<br />

• Selecting talented performers for fast track development<br />

• Leadership development programmes<br />

6 | P age


• Management training programmes<br />

• Values awareness programmes.<br />

In large organisations, structural alignment can take up to 2 to 3 years to implement.<br />

The responsibility for this usually falls to the Human Resource function. This step is the<br />

one that is most frequently forgotten in cultural transformation initiatives.<br />

Step 8: Values alignment<br />

The purpose of the values alignment programme is to inculcate the espoused values<br />

and behaviours of the organisation into the executive and employee population. Apart<br />

from the informational content, the programme should give participants the<br />

opportunity to explore their own values, and understand and practice the concept of<br />

values‐based decision‐making. We use values‐based decision‐making so that we can<br />

consciously create the future we want to experience. For example, if we value trust,<br />

then we make decisions that allow us to display trust. If we value accountability, then<br />

we make decisions that allow us to display accountability.<br />

Step 9: Mission alignment<br />

The purpose of the mission alignment programme is to inculcate the vision and mission<br />

of the organisation into the executive and employee population. Apart from the<br />

informational content, the programme should give participants the opportunity to<br />

explore their own sense of mission and vision, and see how their role supports the<br />

vision or mission of the organisation. It is very important that everyone in the<br />

organisation has a clear line of sight between the work they do each day and the vision<br />

and/or mission of the organisation. Without a clear line of sight, people are not able to<br />

value their contribution and understand how they make a difference.<br />

Frequent Mistakes<br />

The three most frequent mistakes that are made in cultural transformation initiatives<br />

are as follows:<br />

Mistake # 1: Focus on personal alignment only<br />

Many organisations focus on personal alignment without doing anything about<br />

structural alignment. This serves only to aggravate discontent and disillusionment in<br />

the executive and employee population. When employees return from personal<br />

mastery programmes, they usually come back with a higher personal awareness about<br />

how to interact with their colleagues. They quickly become disillusioned when they<br />

realise that although they have changed, the organisation has not changed. The new<br />

behaviours they have learned are not practiced by their superiors and are not<br />

rewarded.<br />

Mistake # 2: Focus on group cohesion only<br />

7 | P age


Another frequent mistake is focusing on team building without first carrying out a<br />

personal alignment programme that involves personal mastery. This limits the<br />

potential for success because people enter these programmes without the selfknowledge<br />

and interpersonal skills necessary to make the team building exercise a<br />

success. For maximum impact, team building or group cohesion programmes should be<br />

preceded by a personal alignment programme.<br />

Mistake # 3: Failure to customise the transformation programmes<br />

Change agents and consultants frequently make the mistake of using “off‐the‐shelf”<br />

personal alignment or team building programmes which have not been tailored to the<br />

specific needs of the organisation, the division, or the business unit with which they<br />

are working. When you carry out a cultural values assessment of your organisation,<br />

you immediately know what issues need to be tackled and what topics your personal<br />

alignment and team building programme should focus on.<br />

Richard Barrett<br />

February 2010<br />

www.valuescentre.com<br />

8 | P age


Page 106 of 134


Attachment B<br />

Culture <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

An Executive View<br />

Page 107 of 134


CULTURE<br />

TRANSFORMATION<br />

AN EXECUTIVE VIEW<br />

BY PHIL GELDART<br />

CEO, EAGLE’S FLIGHT


ABOUT<br />

EAGLE’S FLIGHT<br />

WHAT WE DO<br />

Eagle’s Flight is an innovative leader in the development and delivery of practical training programs<br />

for the global business community. Through the use of experiential learning, we assist organizations<br />

of all sizes in gaining a competitive edge by significantly strengthening their workforce.<br />

Our offering of training programs includes team and training experiences as well as leadership<br />

development and learning, all supported by our significant expertise in optimizing relevance and<br />

impact. Within each of these business segments, our point of difference remains the same – a focus<br />

on results, driven by our unique experiential approach and brought to life by our dynamic people.<br />

Globally, our programs are offered in more than 25 languages and represented by international<br />

licensees in over 45 countries. Our worldwide team is consistently rated as exceeding client<br />

expectations, delivering more than 200 programs per month to groups ranging in size from<br />

10 to over 2,000 participants. Our global structure also enables us to work with large multinational<br />

companies to provide them with consistent training messages and methods around the world.


Powerful, Yet Difficult<br />

Culture <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

An Executive View, by Phil Geldart, CEO, Eagle’s Flight<br />

Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> is powerful once achieved, and yet difficult to achieve.<br />

It’s powerful because, if done effectively, it results in the whole organization being<br />

aligned around a common direction, with a common language, and clearly defined<br />

standards. As a result, much of the typical waste evaporates. There is less wasted effort<br />

and energy, fewer wasted resources, and far fewer wasted opportunities.<br />

It’s difficult to achieve for two distinct reasons. Firstly, it must be leader led, and reflect<br />

organizational will to stay the course until the transformation is achieved. Secondly,<br />

once under way successfully there is a natural human tendency towards “chasing the next<br />

fat rabbit.” As a result the required effort is not sustained long enough for the Culture<br />

<strong>Transformation</strong> to take root, and bear fruit.<br />

The Difference Between Culture, Mission and Values<br />

Organizations begin with a Mission, or Vision. These terms are often used<br />

interchangeably, which is fine. Essentially the mission, or vision, states the organization's<br />

reason for being. It is a summary which clearly captures the "charter" under which the<br />

leadership of the company operates; it's a statement of the "why" the organization exists.<br />

The culture of a company is how the organization brings that mission to life.<br />

Culture is the sum total of all the behaviors of the individuals working within that<br />

organization. It reflects how they solve problems, how they interact with each other, and<br />

how they simply get things done. The operative word here is "how". A culture defines<br />

what I'd feel if I joined that organization and had to work there. The things people do (the<br />

"how's") create in me a sense of "how I feel things are done here".<br />

Culture is sometimes easy to articulate ("here everyone works long hours"), and<br />

sometimes not so easy ("here we just seem to rely a lot on one another"). Either way,<br />

those working in that environment get a "feel" for how things are done. They may like it,<br />

or not; but either way, they are clear on what it is, and can describe it in some fashion.<br />

Values, on the other hand, often provide the basis on which a culture is built. The "value"<br />

of "everyone is important" drives an empowering culture. The "value" of "trust<br />

experienced leadership above all" drives a more hierarchical culture. The "value" of<br />

"integrity" will drive an accountable culture; the value of "respect for individuals' worth"<br />

will drive a culture of greater freedom of action.<br />

Clearly an organization's culture is far more complex than just one or two principles,<br />

1


driven by one or two values. Rather it is the sum total of many factors, all in varying<br />

degrees. The key thing, though, is to distinguish between mission, culture and values.<br />

They are different, but mutually dependent, and yet all facets of an organization's<br />

identity,<br />

Mission sets the direction, culture describes how the mission is achieved, and values<br />

define what that culture will be.<br />

When the focus becomes the transformation of a culture, it's important to consider each<br />

of these aspects of the organization, and the relative weight of each; and then from that<br />

perspective determine how best, and what, to alter in order to ensure the newly<br />

transformed culture is what's wanted, sustainable, and carrying no unforeseen<br />

consequences.<br />

The Hourglass Analogy<br />

Every organization has a culture. Sometimes it’s well defined, promoted, and celebrated.<br />

Sometimes it has just evolved, unintentionally, over time; and sometimes it’s the result of<br />

an amalgamation of several different cultures.<br />

Whatever the situation, a “Culture <strong>Transformation</strong>” begins with something. It is<br />

transforming from one culture to another; or it may only be transforming a small<br />

component of a larger culture.<br />

For example, an organization may have a strong culture in every area except “customer<br />

service.” They wish to keep the current culture, but transform around customer<br />

centricity. In this case the “transformation” is not of the entire culture, but only of one<br />

aspect. Regardless, to transform is to move from one set of behaviors, to another.<br />

This is like an initially stable hourglass, with each grain of sand nestled peacefully in the<br />

bottom half of the hourglass. A Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> turns the hourglass upside down,<br />

and the sand must react. Employees affected by a Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> are like those<br />

grains of sand.<br />

Some – the early adopters – move quickly to the bottom, embrace the change, and move<br />

on; others cling to the sides, much slower to move, but ultimately get there. Lastly,<br />

some, usually those who feel the transformation affects them least – they’re further away<br />

from it – are the last to participate.<br />

A successful Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> seeks to widen the neck of the hourglass as quickly<br />

as possible and to as large a degree as possible.<br />

2


The 3 Non-Negotiables of Culture <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

1. Executive Leadership must be seen to be leading it<br />

2. A clear line of sight to the foreseen benefit must remain top of mind.<br />

3. What to stop, start, and continue doing, must be clear for everyone at the<br />

individual level.<br />

The 3 Pitfalls to avoid when Implementing a Culture <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

1. It being seen as a “program” rather than a “DNA exchange” will cause it to fail.<br />

2. Failing to include everyone, including unions if they exist, will send the message that it’s<br />

only temporary, and not here to stay.<br />

3. Lacking the will to stay the course, and keeping it a priority, until the<br />

transformation is complete, will not give it time to truly take root, and so not be<br />

truly permanent.<br />

Guaranteeing Success<br />

Consequently, for a Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> to achieve optimal success a number of<br />

components are crucial.<br />

1. Leadership by Example<br />

This leadership must occur at every level, from the front line to the corner office.<br />

The nature of this leadership must be such that all employees can confidently say<br />

their own leaders are walking the talk of the new culture, are there to coach and<br />

help, and demonstrate that the behaviors of the new culture are not optional.<br />

2. Clarity on Behavior<br />

It’s one thing to know what the new culture looks like, but it’s quite another to<br />

know, in very practical terms, how it should affect day to day priorities,<br />

decisions, and relationships. This needs to be clearly spelled out for each<br />

function, and then supported by line management.<br />

3. Capture Hearts as well as Minds and Skills<br />

People learn by doing. People engage when vision is caught, not just taught.<br />

People move best when there is a desire, or passion, to change; only then do they<br />

truly look to embrace the facts or the skills of the new way.<br />

Consequently, each Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> must include as a component an<br />

approach that engages people’s hearts, not just their will. Done well, this ensures<br />

the transformation will in fact occur, then be sustained in the near term, and<br />

ultimately retained over the long term.<br />

3


4. An Experienced Partner<br />

Given the many components affecting a successful Culture <strong>Transformation</strong>,<br />

and the need to do so within the additional priorities of day to day urgencies, an<br />

Experienced Partner can be very helpful.<br />

This Partner is typically an organization who’s successfully partnered numerous<br />

times with other organizations, and who can bring the necessary disciplines to<br />

bear. Included in this must be the methodology to capture hearts, the skill to<br />

engage the intellect, and the tools to teach the new behaviors…and clarity to do<br />

this through company leaders, already busy.<br />

20 frequently asked Questions…and the Answers<br />

1. Q: How long does it take?<br />

A: Two to four years. Two years if it’s a transformation within a culture (e.g.<br />

Innovation); four if it’s organizational (e.g. Accountability and Compliance).<br />

2. Q: What does it cost?<br />

A: Hard costs: usually an average of $2 million, depending on population size,<br />

geography, and what needs to be changed. Soft costs: an average of ½ day per<br />

month per employee.<br />

3. Q: Should we be in a hurry?<br />

A: No. It takes time to change behavior, which is what a culture transformation<br />

requires. New skills (e.g. process improvement) can be learned quickly, but<br />

behavior change is much longer because it needs to be applied effectively in<br />

many different situations before it becomes the norm.<br />

4. Q: How much of this should be led by senior people?<br />

A: Without senior people being seen front and center, clearly owning the<br />

transformation, it will not take hold. Within a division, it must be the leader<br />

of that division, within a company, the leader of the company and the<br />

executive team.<br />

5. Q: How involved should HR be?<br />

A: HR can coordinate, interface with an outside expert like Eagle’s Flight<br />

brought in to help, and take a pivotal “back room” role, but must not be seen<br />

to lead it. It must be led by line management.<br />

4


6. Q: How fast will it take effect?<br />

A: The first results are usually seen within two to four months of launch, if done<br />

properly. There are always the “early adopters". From there, the rest of the<br />

population will come on board, so at the end of one year some good<br />

momentum should be seen.<br />

7. Q: What do I do with people who don’t buy into it?<br />

A: Give them time. Not everyone learns, adapts, or applies at the same pace. If,<br />

after 18 months or so they still show clear resistance (vs. just a slow “get it”<br />

factor) they should be asked to leave. They need to find an organization with<br />

a culture they can fully support.<br />

8. Q: Do I tie it to compensation and performance appraisal?<br />

A: Some aspects of culture transformation will have either discrete measurable,<br />

or assessable, components (e.g. enhanced leadership competencies). These<br />

could, and should be linked to performance management. Similarly the<br />

recruitment and succession planning decisions should reflect the behaviors<br />

expected from the culture transformation.<br />

9. Q: Should I involve customers? Suppliers?<br />

A: Absolutely. They will be affected (presumably positively!) by it, and would<br />

probably greatly appreciate being included. By so doing, they may also be<br />

able to contribute to its success, as well as gain some valuable insights for<br />

their own organization.<br />

10. Q: How does it impact new hires and the onboarding process?<br />

A: For new hires, this is not a “culture transformation”, but the “cultural reality”.<br />

The more clearly they can understand the desired behavior, the faster they will<br />

model and demonstrate it. They can often add immediately and effectively to<br />

the group that “get it”.<br />

11. Q: Who measures the impact?<br />

A: The culture transformation should ultimately improve shareholder value, either<br />

directly (e.g. focus on safety) or indirectly (e.g. focus on customers). As such<br />

that portion is measured by the CFO. The more behavior focused aspects<br />

should be measured by HR.<br />

5


12. Q: Should I measure the impact?<br />

A: Without doubt the impact of the culture transformation efforts should be<br />

measured. Doing so provides a yardstick to track progress, creates a winning<br />

spirit as progress is made, and provides an excellent basis for a Recognition<br />

program.<br />

13. Q: What relative priority should it receive?<br />

A: Any activity or initiative must be prioritized based on the impact that will be<br />

felt once it’s completed. Culture transformation is judged the same way – the<br />

priority it receives should be directly proportional to the foreseen impact an<br />

effective transformation will have on the business.<br />

14. Q: Should everyone be involved?<br />

A: A company, or division, culture is the sum total of behaviors demonstrated by<br />

the individuals within that organization. If true culture transformation is to<br />

occur then every single individual must be included, involved, and expected to<br />

learn, and then grow into the new cultural norm.<br />

15. Q: Does the CEO have to buy in?<br />

A: The CEO as the leader of the company should be clearly seen to be a<br />

champion of this transformation, if it’s company-wide; and strongly<br />

supportive if it’s divisional. The body follows the head, so if the “head” has<br />

not fully “bought in” this will become apparent, and significantly weaken any<br />

transformation efforts.<br />

16. Q: Is it worth doing?<br />

A: If the outcome (e.g. dramatically improved customer centricity) is crucial to a<br />

corporate strategic priority, it’s very definitely worth doing. This foreseen<br />

impact must be kept top of mind, as the effort required for true culture<br />

transformation is substantial, and often demanding over a considerable period<br />

of time.<br />

17. Q: When is the best time to start?<br />

A: As soon as the need is recognized, the first steps should be undertaken. It’s<br />

often felt that something else “needs to be finished, or done, before we begin”.<br />

There is never an “easy” time to start, and since it’s a cultural shift being<br />

made, it will positively influence any ongoing initiatives, so the earlier that<br />

benefit is realized, the better.<br />

6


18. Q: Is there a finish line?<br />

A: Absolutely. Once the transformation has occurred (2-4 years), then it’s done,<br />

and the new culture is in place. The true test of any transformation is “Will<br />

the new behaviors remain after the senior champions of the transformation<br />

have moved on?” If yes, then the finish line has been crossed.<br />

19. Q: Will it be painful?<br />

A: If done right, any culture transformation will be embraced by the vast<br />

majority of employees. It will lead to a more fulfilling work environment, and<br />

improved shareholder value. Change is always challenging, but need not be<br />

“painful” if done properly. It should be rewarding.<br />

20. Q: Will everybody ultimately get here?<br />

A: There is typically a very small number who will not ever get there, for various<br />

reasons. They should be encouraged to leave to find a culture which matches<br />

their needs. However, the rest will, ultimately, get there. Like a just turned<br />

hourglass – some grains of sand will fall sooner than others; but eventually it<br />

all slides down.<br />

A quick “Quiz,” and my Suggested Answers<br />

1. In your opinion, whose role is most important in an enterprise wide culture<br />

transformation initiative?<br />

1. CEO and Executive Team…The body follows the head.<br />

2. Senior Management immediately below Executive level<br />

3. Middle Management<br />

4. Front Line Leaders<br />

2. In your opinion, who has the greatest influence in the culture transformation<br />

initiative?<br />

1. CEO and Executive Team<br />

2. Senior Management immediately below Executive level<br />

3. Middle Management<br />

4. Front Line Leaders…To the majority of employees this is the level<br />

where leadership in action is really seen.<br />

7


3. As the culture transformation begins to be implemented, regular “pulse checks”<br />

on progress are important. Whose input on progress is most important?<br />

1. Customers<br />

2. Employee Population…Each individual contributes to a<br />

company’s culture so the combined impact and attitude of that<br />

group determines what the culture really is. “Customers” is often<br />

a frequent response, but actually customers are the beneficiaries<br />

of a culture, rather than the authors.<br />

3. Shareholders<br />

4. Executive Management<br />

5. Company Leaders<br />

4. Which aspect of the culture transformation communication package is most<br />

crucial to get right?<br />

1. What it means to each person<br />

2. What’s coming next<br />

3. Why we’re doing this…Personal conviction is the most powerful<br />

influence on behaviors change; so the “why” is more important<br />

that the “what”.<br />

4. Progress made along the journey<br />

5. Which component to implementing a sustained culture transformation do you feel<br />

is the most essential?<br />

1. Top down leadership, consistently demonstrated<br />

2. Widespread training showing what to stop, start, and continue<br />

doing<br />

3. Equipping all line managers with the skills to lead the<br />

transformation at every level<br />

4. Gaining and maintaining employee conviction for the foreseen<br />

benefit of a successful transformation…All of these are important<br />

but this level of conviction will ensure the transformation<br />

remains for years to come.<br />

5. A measurement and communication strategy that shares progress<br />

and celebrates success<br />

8


A Summary of the Top 10 Specific Steps Required for Successful Culture<br />

<strong>Transformation</strong><br />

1. Engage Executive Management<br />

2. Involve every line manager<br />

3. Monitor progress frequently<br />

4. Regularly reinforce the anticipated benefit<br />

5. Provide specific training on what to stop, start, and continue<br />

6. Build conviction, no just skill and knowledge, around the transformation<br />

7. Celebrate success<br />

8. Maintain the focus and intensity for 2 to 3 years<br />

9. Include every employee, including any unionized groups<br />

10. Ensure HR practices are integrated and aligned<br />

The Result<br />

A Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> carefully thought out, with a clear goal or vision in<br />

mind, and one that accelerates performance, not disrupt it, can bring great value to<br />

the organization. However, it takes time. Usually a Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> is a<br />

two to four year process, depending on the desired outcome.<br />

Hence, patience, organizational will, clarity, and an experienced partner all need<br />

to be in place before beginning. If done well a Culture <strong>Transformation</strong> can<br />

harness corporate energy, individuals’ desire to help, and existing leadership<br />

strength; and then leverage these to accelerate organizational performance, and<br />

customer benefit.<br />

and © Eagle’s Flight Creative Training Excellence Inc. Not to be copied without written permission. 1013<br />

9


CONTACT US<br />

Eagle’s Flight has offices throughout the United States and Canada, and is represented by global<br />

licensees around the world. Within the United States, our main office is located in Minnesota.<br />

Within Canada our main office is located in Ontario.<br />

Global Head Office<br />

US Head Office<br />

489 Clair Road West 7600 Parklawn Avenue<br />

Guelph, ON<br />

Edina, MN<br />

CANADA N1L 0H7 USA 55435<br />

Phone: 1-800-567-8079<br />

Worldwide: 1-519-767-1747<br />

Fax: 1-519-767-2920<br />

Web: www.eaglesflight.com<br />

SOCIAL NETWORKING


Page 108 of 134


Attachment C<br />

Intercultural Interactions<br />

and <strong>Cultural</strong> <strong>Transformation</strong><br />

Page 109 of 134


s_bs_bannerAsian Journal<br />

of Social Psychology<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology (2014), 17, 100–103<br />

DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12047<br />

Intercultural interactions and cultural transformation<br />

Zhi Liu and Michael W. Morris<br />

Columbia University, New York, USA<br />

Increasingly psychologists are going beyond the study of<br />

cultural differences to study cultural dynamics, the formation,<br />

maintenance, and transformation of cultural representations<br />

over time (Kashima, 2008). At the vanguard of this<br />

movement, Kashima (2014, this issue) elucidates how communication<br />

pragmatics involving ‘common ground’ work<br />

to create and then perpetuate cultural representations and<br />

identities. Common ground is ‘a set of meanings that are<br />

mutually known, believed, presupposed, or taken for<br />

granted by the participants of a joint activity’ (Kashima,<br />

2014, this issue). People form common ground in an interaction<br />

not only through what is explicitly said but also<br />

through what they infer from the other’s affiliations and<br />

histories. In a conversational process called grounding, one<br />

interactant checks his or her interpretation of the other’s<br />

words and the other confirms this interpretation. As a result,<br />

both interactants are left with the perception that the other<br />

shares the belief.<br />

Similar results follow from broader forms of ingroup<br />

communications. When someone speaks in a way that<br />

assumes some belief (a comedian’s inside jokes, a politician’s<br />

use of coded rhetoric) and the audience acknowledges<br />

the point (laughs at the joke, claps at the political<br />

reference), everyone comes away with the sense that this<br />

belief is shared. Similarly, Anderson (2006) analyzed how<br />

mass media (nationally broadcast radio, widely circulated<br />

newspapers) contribute to citizens forming a notion of<br />

national common ground. In sum, perceptions of belief<br />

consensus (i.e. cultural representations) are the precipitate<br />

of ingroup interactions. These perceptions of belief consensus<br />

then contribute to people’s notions of what distinguishes<br />

their group from others (i.e. ingroup identities or<br />

prototypes).<br />

Speakers use assumed common ground to tailor communication<br />

and to craft statements that are easily interpretable<br />

but not obvious (Clark, 1996). Audiences also use it to<br />

disambiguate and interpret a speaker’s utterances (Sperber<br />

& Wilson, 1986). Every act of using common ground<br />

increases the sharedness of a given belief (and the mutual<br />

perception of its sharedness). In sum, everyday ingroup<br />

communication exploits shared beliefs and also perpetuates<br />

them.<br />

Correspondence: Michael W. Morris, Columbia University, 3022<br />

Broadway, 718 Uris, New York, NY 10027. Email: mwm82<br />

@columbia.edu<br />

Received 9 December 2013; accepted 19 January 2014.<br />

Although cultural formation and maintenance are<br />

underexplored problems, cultural transformation is an even<br />

greater challenge. Ultimately a science of cultural dynamics<br />

ought to strive to identify a common set of mechanisms<br />

that underlie both cultural persistence and cultural transformation.<br />

In this essay, we argue that some important forces<br />

for cultural transformation come from intercultural interactions<br />

– communicating, interacting and coping with other<br />

cultures. Kashima (2014, this issue) focuses on withinculture<br />

communication. This comes in a tradition of<br />

explaining a culture as internal to a discrete population, a<br />

view that Sperber (1996) summarized as ‘culture is the<br />

precipitate of cognition and communication in a human<br />

population.’ However, no culture is unaffected by its<br />

neighbors; cultures influence each other through the individuals<br />

who span them. So a given culture is shaped not<br />

only by within-group interactions but also by interactions<br />

across cultures.<br />

Here we analyze how some of the same processes that<br />

Kashima (2014, this issue) describes in within-group interactions<br />

play out in across-group interactions. The same<br />

communication processes that work toward cultural maintenance<br />

in within-group interactions become springs for<br />

cultural transformation in across-group interactions. First<br />

we focus on how motives relevant to common ground<br />

distort people’s retelling of stories. While they perpetuate<br />

stereotypes in communication within a cultural group, they<br />

produce stereotype-challenging messages in communication<br />

across cultural groups. Second, we review findings<br />

from several literatures on how cross-cultural interactions<br />

affect cultural identities. As social identity research has<br />

found, people construct an image of the prototypical<br />

ingroup member in part by contrasting against salient<br />

outgroups. Recent cultural psychology research explores<br />

how inflows of foreign groups give rise, under some<br />

conditions, to cultural closure – defensive psychological<br />

processes and social movements that narrow ingroup<br />

identities.<br />

Story transmission biases<br />

and outgroups<br />

The classic work of Bartlett (1932) on communication<br />

chains found that as students retell stories to their peers, the<br />

story content becomes increasingly conventionalized. If the<br />

original story corresponds to a culturally conventional plot<br />

© 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and the Japanese Group Dynamics Association


Story transmissions and cultural change 101<br />

in some respects but not in others, stereotype-inconsistent<br />

details drop out with each retelling. This conventionalization<br />

process contributes to the maintenance of cultural<br />

representations, as it converts a novel story into a conventional<br />

story.<br />

While the common ground within any given group is<br />

ever evolving, two kinds of communicated information<br />

can be defined according to their congruence with the<br />

current common ground. Kashima (2014, this issue) proposes<br />

that these two kinds of information differentially<br />

serve two goals of communication – social connectivity and<br />

informativeness. Common ground consistent information<br />

in a message helps with interpersonal bonding because it is<br />

easily comprehensible, but it does not help the message be<br />

informative or newsworthy. Common ground inconsistent<br />

information is less useful for bonding but it raises the newsworthiness<br />

of the message. Hence, speakers are faced with<br />

an informativeness-connectivity dilemma. They choose<br />

how much common ground consistent information to<br />

include based on how much bonding is needed. Much everyday<br />

communication among neighbours or acquaintances<br />

is done in service of building or maintaining relationships,<br />

suggesting that the connectivity goal looms large. Consistent<br />

with this, Kashima’s studies of communication chains<br />

find that as stories about a stereotyped character, such as a<br />

politician or football player, are retold, transmissions<br />

become more and more biased toward stereotype consistent<br />

detail (Clark & Kashima, 2007; Lyons & Kashima, 2006).<br />

The shared stereotype serves as common ground within<br />

ingroup audiences, and the goal of connectivity favours<br />

common ground consistent detail. Ultimately the stereotype<br />

is perpetuated by the conventionalized retellings that<br />

result. [Not only the audience but also the communicator<br />

would be more likely to believe the stereotype after the<br />

telling (Echterhoff, Higgins & Levine, 2009).]<br />

Kashima notes some findings about conditions under<br />

which common ground inconsistent detail is transmitted,<br />

conditions where communication transforms rather than<br />

maintains shared cultural representations. Not surprisingly,<br />

stereotype-inconsistencies survive when their informativeness<br />

is emphasized – when the theme of the story is the<br />

falsity of the stereotype (Goodman, Webb & Stewart, 2009)<br />

or when the inconsistencies are explained in the story<br />

(Simpson & Kashima, 2013). More interestingly, inconsistencies<br />

survive when interactants are connected by a strong<br />

tie (friendship) rather than a weak tie (acquaintanceship),<br />

presumably because less bonding is needed (Ruscher,<br />

Santuzzi & Hammer, 2003).<br />

We propose that communication with cultural outgroups<br />

is another condition that fosters the survival of stereotypeinconsistent<br />

details. With an outgroup audience, the goal of<br />

social connectivity does not imply a bias toward stereotypeconsistent<br />

detail, because the stereotype would not serve as<br />

common ground with this audience. The freedom and creativity<br />

made possible by outgroup interactions is a classic<br />

sociological theme. Simmel (1908/1950) observed that<br />

when interacting with a stranger as opposed to an acquaintance,<br />

people are more likely to shed their inhibitions and<br />

express unconventional ideas. Park (1931) described the<br />

immigrant or ethnic minority as a ‘marginal man’ whose<br />

communications and interactions span two cultural communities,<br />

potentially leading to ideas that transcend the<br />

conventions of these communities.<br />

Research in our lab (Liu & Morris, 2014) used<br />

Kashima’s story retelling paradigm to test this by comparing<br />

chains of story retellings to ingroup members and<br />

outgroup members. Participants were given a written story<br />

describing a typically image-conscious, instrumental politician<br />

(adapted from the politician story used by Lyons &<br />

Kashima, 2006). The story consisted of equal amounts of<br />

details consistent with the politician stereotype and details<br />

inconsistent with this stereotype. Upon finishing some filler<br />

tasks after reading the story, participants unexpectedly were<br />

asked to retell the story to a purported audience so that he<br />

or she would be able to retell it to someone else. Participants<br />

were randomly assigned to either an ingroup audience,<br />

who was described as ‘a person from Washington D.<br />

C., the capital of the United States. The person grew up,<br />

studied, and currently works in that city,’ or an outgroup<br />

audience, who was described as ‘a person from a tribe in<br />

KwaZulu-Natal, which is a province of South Africa under<br />

the reign of its own king. People there maintain the pastoral<br />

lifestyles of their ancestors, raising goats.’ The retellings of<br />

the original story were passed to participants in the second<br />

position of the chain to read, and their retellings were then<br />

passed to those in the third position of the chain to read.<br />

In the ingroup condition, results replicated Kashima’s<br />

standard finding. Participants included more stereotypeconsistent<br />

information than stereotype-inconsistent information<br />

in their retellings of the original story. In the<br />

outgroup condition, however, this bias disappeared; participants<br />

included equal proportions of stereotype-consistent<br />

and inconsistent information from the original story.<br />

This finding suggests that intercultural interfaces will be<br />

sites of cultural transformation. People who speak to<br />

outgroup audiences regularly will be less prone to the<br />

conventionalization bias in the serial transmission of<br />

information, a mechanism for cultural maintenance. These<br />

cosmopolitan circles, then, may be sites where observations<br />

that contradict conventional beliefs survive retellings<br />

and therefore have more chance of changing people’s<br />

stereotypes.<br />

Identities and outgroups<br />

While our argument that intergroup communication<br />

transforms cultural beliefs is relatively novel, the role of<br />

© 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and the Japanese Group Dynamics Association


102 Zhi Liu and Michael W. Morris<br />

intergroup interaction in the construction of ingroup identities<br />

has been explored in several literatures. Social identity<br />

research delineated how social perceivers make use of a<br />

salient outgroup in constructing perceptions of the<br />

ingroup’s characteristics (Brewer, 1991; Tajfel & Turner,<br />

1986; Turner, 1987). A particularly important representation<br />

of the ingroup identity is the image of the prototypical<br />

ingroup member, and this is determined in part by maximizing<br />

difference to members of the salient outgroup<br />

(Hogg & Terry, 2000). For instance, after Hong Kong was<br />

handed over as a territory from England to China, many of<br />

its denizens shifted from identifying as ‘Chinese’ (a contrast<br />

to the salient outgroup ‘English’) to identifying as<br />

‘HongKonger’ (a contrast to the new salient outgroup<br />

‘Mainlander’) (Chiu & Hong, 1999).<br />

While social identity research explores effects of<br />

changes in the salient outgroup category, other research<br />

programs examine how exposure to artefacts and ideas<br />

from other cultures can spark cultural change. Globalization<br />

refers to the increased flow of people, organizations,<br />

products and images across national lines in recent<br />

decades enabled by changes in communication and transportation<br />

technology (Appadurai, 1996). People respond<br />

to inflows of foreign cultures in several ways. One way<br />

involves learning the second culture as stock of frames and<br />

practices. This produces changes in behaviour as individuals<br />

blend practices from two cultures in creative hybrid<br />

behaviours (Chiu, Gries, Torelli & Cheng, 2011; Leung &<br />

Chiu, 2010). Or individuals who have become bicultural<br />

can respond to cues of the foreign culture by adhering to<br />

its norms (Hong, Morris, Chiu & Benet-Martinez, 2000)<br />

or defying them (Mok & Morris, 2013). Some of the novel<br />

practices borrowed from immigrant communities diffuse<br />

widely enough to become the new mainstream, changing<br />

the common ground and transforming the culture (e.g.<br />

curry in England).<br />

Another way that people respond to foreign cultural<br />

inflows that can change the ingroup culture is construing<br />

them as threats. Individuals low in openness to other cultures<br />

respond to displays of cultural mixing with heightened<br />

need for cognitive closure (Morris, Mok & Mor, 2011)<br />

as well as judgments that exaggerate cultural differences<br />

and stereotype individuals (Chiu, Mallorie, Keh & Law,<br />

2009). These changed judgments can become a new<br />

common ground. Political theorists have documented that<br />

reactionary social movements, whether fascist or fundamentalist,<br />

feature a narrowing of shared collective identities<br />

in response to perceived threats of foreign influx or contamination<br />

(see Morris et al., 2011).<br />

Conclusion<br />

In sum, the role of common ground in communication and<br />

identification provides a basis for a more dynamic cultural<br />

psychology. Here we have advocated extending Kashima’s<br />

analysis of these processes to communication and interaction<br />

across cultural groups as well as that within groups. In<br />

this era of globalization, cross-cultural interactions occur<br />

more frequently than ever before. Hence, cultures may also<br />

be transforming more than ever before, in ways that are<br />

important to understand. We have tried to sketch some<br />

mechanisms that can underlie both cultural maintenance<br />

and cultural transformation.<br />

References<br />

Anderson, B. (2006). Imagined Communities:<br />

Reflections on the Origin and Spread of<br />

Nationalism. London: Verso.<br />

Appadurai, A. (1996). Modernity at Large: <strong>Cultural</strong><br />

Dimensions of Globalization. Minneapolis,<br />

MN: University of Minnesota Press.<br />

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge,<br />

UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On<br />

being the same and different at the same<br />

time. Personality and Social Psychology<br />

Bulletin, 17, 475–482.<br />

Chiu, C., Mallorie, L., Keh, H. & Law, W.<br />

(2009). Perceptions of culture in multicultural<br />

space: Joint presentation of images<br />

from two cultures increases in-group attribution<br />

of culture-typical characteristics.<br />

Journal of Cross-<strong>Cultural</strong> Psychology, 40,<br />

282–300.<br />

Chiu, C. Y., Gries, P., Torelli, C. J. & Cheng, S.<br />

Y. Y. (2011). Toward a social psychology of<br />

globalization. Journal of Social Issues, 67,<br />

663–676.<br />

Chiu, C. Y. & Hong, Y. Y. (1999). Social identification<br />

in a political transition: The role of<br />

implicit beliefs. International Journal of<br />

Intercultural Relations, 23, 297–318.<br />

Clark, A. E. & Kashima, Y. (2007). Stereotypes<br />

help people connect with others in the community:<br />

A situated functional analysis of the<br />

stereotype consistency bias in communication.<br />

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,<br />

93, 1028–1039.<br />

Clark, H. H. (1996). Using Language. New<br />

York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T. & Levine, J. M.<br />

(2009). Shared reality: Experiencing commonality<br />

with others’ inner states about the<br />

world. Perspectives on Psychological<br />

Science, 4, 496–521.<br />

Goodman, R. L., Webb, T. L. & Stewart,<br />

A. J. (2009). Communicating stereotyperelevant<br />

information: Is factual information<br />

subject to the same communication<br />

biases as fictional information? Personality<br />

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 836–<br />

852.<br />

Hogg, M. A. & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social<br />

identity and self-categorization processes in<br />

organizational contexts. The Academy of<br />

Management Review, 25, 121–140.<br />

Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y. &<br />

Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural<br />

minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to<br />

culture and cognition. American Psychologist,<br />

55, 709–720.<br />

Kashima, Y. (2008). A social psychology of<br />

cultural dynamics: How cultures are formed,<br />

maintained, and transformed. Social and<br />

Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 107–<br />

120.<br />

© 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and the Japanese Group Dynamics Association


Story transmissions and cultural change 103<br />

Kashima, Y. (2014). Meaning, grounding, and<br />

the construction of social reality. Asian<br />

Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 81–<br />

95.<br />

Leung, A. K. Y. & Chiu, C. Y. (2010). Multicultural<br />

experiences, idea receptiveness, and<br />

creativity. Journal of Cross <strong>Cultural</strong> Psychology,<br />

41, 723–741.<br />

Liu, Z. & Morris, M. W. (2014). Speaking with<br />

strangers: Stories, stereotypes and change.<br />

Unpublished manuscript.<br />

Lyons, A. & Kashima, Y. (2006). Maintaining<br />

stereotypes in communication: Investigating<br />

memory biases and coherence-seeking in<br />

storytelling. Asian Journal of Social<br />

Psychology, 9, 59–71.<br />

Mok, A. & Morris, W. M. (2013). Bicultural<br />

self-defense in consumer contexts: Selfprotection<br />

motives are the basis for contrast<br />

versus assimilation to cultural cues. Journal<br />

of Consumer Psychology, 23, 175–188.<br />

Morris, W. M., Mok, A. & Mor, S. (2011).<br />

<strong>Cultural</strong> identity treat: The role of cultural<br />

identifications in moderating closure<br />

responses to foreign cultural inflow. Journal<br />

of Social Issues, 67, 760–773.<br />

Park, R. E. (1931). Mentality of racial hybrids.<br />

American Journal of Sociology, 36, 534–551.<br />

Ruscher, J. B., Santuzzi, A. M. & Hammer, E.<br />

Y. (2003). Shared impression formation<br />

in the cognitively interdependent dyad.<br />

British Journal of Social Psychology, 42,<br />

411–425.<br />

Simmel, G. (1950). The Sociology of Georg<br />

Simmel. (K. Wolff, Trans.). New York: Free<br />

Press. (Original work published 1908).<br />

Simpson, A. & Kashima, Y. (2013). How can a<br />

stereotype inconsistency bias be encouraged<br />

in communication? Asian Journal of Social<br />

Psychology, 16, 71–78.<br />

Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture:<br />

A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford:<br />

Blackwell.<br />

Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance:<br />

Communication and Cognition. Cambridge<br />

MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Tajfel, H. & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social<br />

identity theory of inter-group behavior. In:<br />

S. Worchel & L. W. Austin, eds. Psychology<br />

of Intergroup Relations, pp. 71–72. Chicago:<br />

Nelson-Hall.<br />

Turner, J. C. (1987). Rediscovering the Social<br />

Group: A Self-Categorization Theory.<br />

Oxford, UK: Blackwell.<br />

© 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and the Japanese Group Dynamics Association


Page 110 of 134


Advocacy Foundation Publishers<br />

Page 111 of 134


Advocacy Foundation Publishers<br />

The e-Advocate Quarterly<br />

Page 112 of 134


Issue Title Quarterly<br />

Vol. I 2015 The Fundamentals<br />

I<br />

The ComeUnity ReEngineering<br />

Project Initiative<br />

Q-1 2015<br />

II The Adolescent Law Group Q-2 2015<br />

III<br />

Landmark Cases in US<br />

Juvenile Justice (PA)<br />

Q-3 2015<br />

IV The First Amendment Project Q-4 2015<br />

Vol. II 2016 Strategic Development<br />

V The Fourth Amendment Project Q-1 2016<br />

VI<br />

Landmark Cases in US<br />

Juvenile Justice (NJ)<br />

Q-2 2016<br />

VII Youth Court Q-3 2016<br />

VIII<br />

The Economic Consequences of Legal<br />

Decision-Making<br />

Q-4 2016<br />

Vol. III 2017 Sustainability<br />

IX The Sixth Amendment Project Q-1 2017<br />

X<br />

The Theological Foundations of<br />

US Law & Government<br />

Q-2 2017<br />

XI The Eighth Amendment Project Q-3 2017<br />

XII<br />

The EB-5 Investor<br />

Immigration Project*<br />

Q-4 2017<br />

Vol. IV 2018 Collaboration<br />

XIII Strategic Planning Q-1 2018<br />

XIV<br />

The Juvenile Justice<br />

Legislative Reform Initiative<br />

Q-2 2018<br />

XV The Advocacy Foundation Coalition Q-3 2018<br />

Page 113 of 134


XVI<br />

for Drug-Free Communities<br />

Landmark Cases in US<br />

Juvenile Justice (GA)<br />

Q-4 2018<br />

Page 114 of 134


Issue Title Quarterly<br />

Vol. V 2019 Organizational Development<br />

XVII The Board of Directors Q-1 2019<br />

XVIII The Inner Circle Q-2 2019<br />

XIX Staff & Management Q-3 2019<br />

XX Succession Planning Q-4 2019<br />

XXI The Budget* Bonus #1<br />

XXII Data-Driven Resource Allocation* Bonus #2<br />

Vol. VI 2020 Missions<br />

XXIII Critical Thinking Q-1 2020<br />

XXIV<br />

The Advocacy Foundation<br />

Endowments Initiative Project<br />

Q-2 2020<br />

XXV International Labor Relations Q-3 2020<br />

XXVI Immigration Q-4 2020<br />

Vol. VII 2021 Community Engagement<br />

XXVII<br />

The 21 st Century Charter Schools<br />

Initiative<br />

Q-1 2021<br />

XXVIII The All-Sports Ministry @ ... Q-2 2021<br />

XXIX Lobbying for Nonprofits Q-3 2021<br />

XXX<br />

XXXI<br />

Advocacy Foundation Missions -<br />

Domestic<br />

Advocacy Foundation Missions -<br />

International<br />

Q-4 2021<br />

Bonus<br />

Page 115 of 134


Vol. VIII<br />

2022 ComeUnity ReEngineering<br />

XXXII<br />

The Creative & Fine Arts Ministry<br />

@ The Foundation<br />

Q-1 2022<br />

XXXIII The Advisory Council & Committees Q-2 2022<br />

XXXIV<br />

The Theological Origins<br />

of Contemporary Judicial Process<br />

Q-3 2022<br />

XXXV The Second Chance Ministry @ ... Q-4 2022<br />

Vol. IX 2023 Legal Reformation<br />

XXXVI The Fifth Amendment Project Q-1 2023<br />

XXXVII The Judicial Re-Engineering Initiative Q-2 2023<br />

XXXVIII<br />

The Inner-Cities Strategic<br />

Revitalization Initiative<br />

Q-3 2023<br />

XXXVIX Habeas Corpus Q-4 2023<br />

Vol. X 2024 ComeUnity Development<br />

XXXVX<br />

The Inner-City Strategic<br />

Revitalization Plan<br />

Q-1 2024<br />

XXXVXI The Mentoring Initiative Q-2 2024<br />

XXXVXII The Violence Prevention Framework Q-3 2024<br />

XXXVXIII The Fatherhood Initiative Q-4 2024<br />

Vol. XI 2025 Public Interest<br />

XXXVXIV Public Interest Law Q-1 2025<br />

L (50) Spiritual Resource Development Q-2 2025<br />

Page 116 of 134


LI<br />

Nonprofit Confidentiality<br />

In The Age of Big Data<br />

Q-3 2025<br />

LII Interpreting The Facts Q-4 2025<br />

Vol. XII 2026 Poverty In America<br />

LIII<br />

American Poverty<br />

In The New Millennium<br />

Q-1 2026<br />

LIV Outcome-Based Thinking Q-2 2026<br />

LV <strong>Transformation</strong>al Social Leadership Q-3 2026<br />

LVI The Cycle of Poverty Q-4 2026<br />

Vol. XIII 2027 Raising Awareness<br />

LVII ReEngineering Juvenile Justice Q-1 2027<br />

LVIII Corporations Q-2 2027<br />

LVIX The Prison Industrial Complex Q-3 2027<br />

LX Restoration of Rights Q-4 2027<br />

Vol. XIV 2028 <strong>Cultural</strong>ly Relevant Programming<br />

LXI Community Culture Q-1 2028<br />

LXII Corporate Culture Q-2 2028<br />

LXIII Strategic <strong>Cultural</strong> Planning Q-3 2028<br />

LXIV<br />

The Cross-Sector/ Coordinated<br />

Service Approach to Delinquency<br />

Prevention<br />

Q-4 2028<br />

Page 117 of 134


Vol. XV 2029 Inner-Cities Revitalization<br />

LXIV<br />

LXV<br />

LXVI<br />

Part I – Strategic Housing<br />

Revitalization<br />

(The Twenty Percent Profit Margin)<br />

Part II – Jobs Training, Educational<br />

Redevelopment<br />

and Economic Empowerment<br />

Part III - Financial Literacy<br />

and Sustainability<br />

Q-1 2029<br />

Q-2 2029<br />

Q-3 2029<br />

LXVII Part IV – Solutions for Homelessness Q-4 2029<br />

LXVIII<br />

The Strategic Home Mortgage<br />

Initiative<br />

Bonus<br />

Vol. XVI 2030 Sustainability<br />

LXVIII Social Program Sustainability Q-1 2030<br />

LXIX<br />

The Advocacy Foundation<br />

Endowments Initiative<br />

Q-2 2030<br />

LXX Capital Gains Q-3 2030<br />

LXXI Sustainability Investments Q-4 2030<br />

Vol. XVII 2031 The Justice Series<br />

LXXII Distributive Justice Q-1 2031<br />

LXXIII Retributive Justice Q-2 2031<br />

LXXIV Procedural Justice Q-3 2031<br />

LXXV (75) Restorative Justice Q-4 2031<br />

LXXVI Unjust Legal Reasoning Bonus<br />

Page 118 of 134


Vol. XVIII 2032 Public Policy<br />

LXXVII Public Interest Law Q-1 2032<br />

LXXVIII Reforming Public Policy Q-2 2032<br />

LXXVIX ... Q-3 2032<br />

LXXVX ... Q-4 2032<br />

Page 119 of 134


The e-Advocate Monthly Review<br />

2018<br />

<strong>Transformation</strong>al Problem Solving January 2018<br />

The Advocacy Foundation February 2018<br />

Opioid Initiative<br />

Native-American Youth March 2018<br />

In the Juvenile Justice System<br />

Barriers to Reducing Confinement April 2018<br />

Latino and Hispanic Youth May 2018<br />

In the Juvenile Justice System<br />

Social Entrepreneurship June 2018<br />

The Economic Consequences of<br />

Homelessness in America S.Ed – June 2018<br />

African-American Youth July 2018<br />

In the Juvenile Justice System<br />

Gang Deconstruction August 2018<br />

Social Impact Investing September 2018<br />

Opportunity Youth: October 2018<br />

Disenfranchised Young People<br />

The Economic Impact of Social November 2018<br />

of Social Programs Development<br />

Gun Control December 2018<br />

2019<br />

The U.S. Stock Market January 2019<br />

Prison-Based Gerrymandering February 2019<br />

Literacy-Based Prison Construction March 2019<br />

Children of Incarcerated Parents April 2019<br />

Page 120 of 134


African-American Youth in The May 2019<br />

Juvenile Justice System<br />

Racial Profiling June 2019<br />

Mass Collaboration July 2019<br />

Concentrated Poverty August 2019<br />

De-Industrialization September 2019<br />

Overcoming Dyslexia October 2019<br />

Overcoming Attention Deficit November 2019<br />

The Gift of Adversity December 2019<br />

2020<br />

The Gift of Hypersensitivity January 2020<br />

The Gift of Introspection February 2020<br />

The Gift of Introversion March 2020<br />

The Gift of Spirituality April 2020<br />

The Gift of <strong>Transformation</strong> May 2020<br />

…<br />

Page 121 of 134


The e-Advocate Quarterly<br />

Special Editions<br />

Crowdfunding Winter-Spring 2017<br />

Social Media for Nonprofits October 2017<br />

Mass Media for Nonprofits November 2017<br />

The Opioid Crisis in America: January 2018<br />

Issues in Pain Management<br />

The Opioid Crisis in America: February 2018<br />

The Drug Culture in the U.S.<br />

The Opioid Crisis in America: March 2018<br />

Drug Abuse Among Veterans<br />

The Opioid Crisis in America: April 2018<br />

Drug Abuse Among America’s<br />

Teens<br />

The Opioid Crisis in America: May 2018<br />

Alcoholism<br />

The Economic Consequences of June 2018<br />

Homelessness in The US<br />

The Economic Consequences of July 2018<br />

Opioid Addiction in America<br />

Page 122 of 134


The e-Advocate Journal<br />

of Theological Jurisprudence<br />

Vol. I - 2017<br />

The Theological Origins of Contemporary Judicial Process<br />

Scriptural Application to The Model Criminal Code<br />

Scriptural Application for Tort Reform<br />

Scriptural Application to Juvenile Justice Reformation<br />

Vol. II - 2018<br />

Scriptural Application for The Canons of Ethics<br />

Scriptural Application to Contracts Reform<br />

& The Uniform Commercial Code<br />

Scriptural Application to The Law of Property<br />

Scriptural Application to The Law of Evidence<br />

Page 123 of 134


Legal Missions International<br />

Page 124 of 134


Issue Title Quarterly<br />

Vol. I 2015<br />

I<br />

II<br />

God’s Will and The 21 st Century<br />

Democratic Process<br />

The Community<br />

Engagement Strategy<br />

Q-1 2015<br />

Q-2 2015<br />

III Foreign Policy Q-3 2015<br />

IV<br />

Public Interest Law<br />

in The New Millennium<br />

Q-4 2015<br />

Vol. II 2016<br />

V Ethiopia Q-1 2016<br />

VI Zimbabwe Q-2 2016<br />

VII Jamaica Q-3 2016<br />

VIII Brazil Q-4 2016<br />

Vol. III 2017<br />

IX India Q-1 2017<br />

X Suriname Q-2 2017<br />

XI The Caribbean Q-3 2017<br />

XII United States/ Estados Unidos Q-4 2017<br />

Vol. IV 2018<br />

XIII Cuba Q-1 2018<br />

XIV Guinea Q-2 2018<br />

XV Indonesia Q-3 2018<br />

XVI Sri Lanka Q-4 2018<br />

Page 125 of 134


Vol. V 2019<br />

XVII Russia Q-1 2019<br />

XVIII Australia Q-2 2019<br />

XIV South Korea Q-3 2019<br />

XV Puerto Rico Q-4 2019<br />

Issue Title Quarterly<br />

Vol. VI 2020<br />

XVI Trinidad & Tobago Q-1 2020<br />

XVII Egypt Q-2 2020<br />

XVIII Sierra Leone Q-3 2020<br />

XIX South Africa Q-4 2020<br />

XX Israel Bonus<br />

Vol. VII 2021<br />

XXI Haiti Q-1 2021<br />

XXII Peru Q-2 2021<br />

XXIII Costa Rica Q-3 2021<br />

XXIV China Q-4 2021<br />

XXV Japan Bonus<br />

Vol VIII 2022<br />

XXVI Chile Q-1 2022<br />

Page 126 of 134


The e-Advocate Juvenile Justice Report<br />

______<br />

Vol. I – Juvenile Delinquency in The US<br />

Vol. II. – The Prison Industrial Complex<br />

Vol. III – Restorative/ Transformative Justice<br />

Vol. IV – The Sixth Amendment Right to The Effective Assistance of Counsel<br />

Vol. V – The Theological Foundations of Juvenile Justice<br />

Vol. VI – Collaborating to Eradicate Juvenile Delinquency<br />

Page 127 of 134


The e-Advocate Newsletter<br />

Genesis of The Problem<br />

Family Structure<br />

Societal Influences<br />

Evidence-Based Programming<br />

Strengthening Assets v. Eliminating Deficits<br />

2012 - Juvenile Delinquency in The US<br />

Introduction/Ideology/Key Values<br />

Philosophy/Application & Practice<br />

Expungement & Pardons<br />

Pardons & Clemency<br />

Examples/Best Practices<br />

2013 - Restorative Justice in The US<br />

2014 - The Prison Industrial Complex<br />

25% of the World's Inmates Are In the US<br />

The Economics of Prison Enterprise<br />

The Federal Bureau of Prisons<br />

The After-Effects of Incarceration/Individual/Societal<br />

The Fourth Amendment Project<br />

The Sixth Amendment Project<br />

The Eighth Amendment Project<br />

The Adolescent Law Group<br />

2015 - US Constitutional Issues In The New Millennium<br />

Page 128 of 134


2018 - The Theological Law Firm Academy<br />

The Theological Foundations of US Law & Government<br />

The Economic Consequences of Legal Decision-Making<br />

The Juvenile Justice Legislative Reform Initiative<br />

The EB-5 International Investors Initiative<br />

2017 - Organizational Development<br />

The Board of Directors<br />

The Inner Circle<br />

Staff & Management<br />

Succession Planning<br />

Bonus #1 The Budget<br />

Bonus #2 Data-Driven Resource Allocation<br />

2018 - Sustainability<br />

The Data-Driven Resource Allocation Process<br />

The Quality Assurance Initiative<br />

The Advocacy Foundation Endowments Initiative<br />

The Community Engagement Strategy<br />

2019 - Collaboration<br />

Critical Thinking for Transformative Justice<br />

International Labor Relations<br />

Immigration<br />

God's Will & The 21st Century Democratic Process<br />

The Community Engagement Strategy<br />

The 21st Century Charter Schools Initiative<br />

2020 - Community Engagement<br />

Page 129 of 134


Extras<br />

The Nonprofit Advisors Group Newsletters<br />

The 501(c)(3) Acquisition Process<br />

The Board of Directors<br />

The Gladiator Mentality<br />

Strategic Planning<br />

Fundraising<br />

501(c)(3) Reinstatements<br />

The Collaborative US/ International Newsletters<br />

How You Think Is Everything<br />

The Reciprocal Nature of Business Relationships<br />

Accelerate Your Professional Development<br />

The Competitive Nature of Grant Writing<br />

Assessing The Risks<br />

Page 130 of 134


Page 131 of 134


About The Author<br />

John C (Jack) Johnson III<br />

Founder & CEO<br />

Jack was educated at Temple University, in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania and Rutgers<br />

Law School, in Camden, New Jersey. In 1999, he moved to Atlanta, Georgia to pursue<br />

greater opportunities to provide Advocacy and Preventive Programmatic services for atrisk/<br />

at-promise young persons, their families, and Justice Professionals embedded in the<br />

Juvenile Justice process in order to help facilitate its transcendence into the 21 st Century.<br />

There, along with a small group of community and faith-based professionals, “The Advocacy Foundation, Inc." was conceived<br />

and developed over roughly a thirteen year period, originally chartered as a Juvenile Delinquency Prevention and Educational<br />

Support Services organization consisting of Mentoring, Tutoring, Counseling, Character Development, Community Change<br />

Management, Practitioner Re-Education & Training, and a host of related components.<br />

The Foundation’s Overarching Mission is “To help Individuals, Organizations, & Communities Achieve Their Full Potential”, by<br />

implementing a wide array of evidence-based proactive multi-disciplinary "Restorative & Transformative Justice" programs &<br />

projects currently throughout the northeast, southeast, and western international-waters regions, providing prevention and support<br />

services to at-risk/ at-promise youth, to young adults, to their families, and to Social Service, Justice and Mental<br />

Health professionals” everywhere. The Foundation has since relocated its headquarters to Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, and been<br />

expanded to include a three-tier mission.<br />

In addition to his work with the Foundation, Jack also served as an Adjunct Professor of Law & Business at National-Louis<br />

University of Atlanta (where he taught Political Science, Business & Legal Ethics, Labor & Employment Relations, and Critical<br />

Thinking courses to undergraduate and graduate level students). Jack has also served as Board President for a host of wellestablished<br />

and up & coming nonprofit organizations throughout the region, including “Visions Unlimited Community<br />

Development Systems, Inc.”, a multi-million dollar, award-winning, Violence Prevention and Gang Intervention Social Service<br />

organization in Atlanta, as well as Vice-Chair of the Georgia/ Metropolitan Atlanta Violence Prevention Partnership, a state-wide<br />

300 organizational member, violence prevention group led by the Morehouse School of Medicine, Emory University and The<br />

Original, Atlanta-Based, Martin Luther King Center.<br />

Attorney Johnson’s prior accomplishments include a wide-array of Professional Legal practice areas, including Private Firm,<br />

Corporate and Government postings, just about all of which yielded significant professional awards & accolades, the history and<br />

chronology of which are available for review online. Throughout his career, Jack has served a wide variety of for-profit<br />

corporations, law firms, and nonprofit organizations as Board Chairman, Secretary, Associate, and General Counsel since 1990.<br />

www.TheAdvocacyFoundation.org<br />

Clayton County Youth Services Partnership, Inc. – Chair; Georgia Violence Prevention Partnership, Inc – Vice Chair; Fayette<br />

County NAACP - Legal Redress Committee Chairman; Clayton County Fatherhood Initiative Partnership – Principal<br />

Investigator; Morehouse School of Medicine School of Community Health Feasibility Study - Steering Committee; Atlanta<br />

Violence Prevention Capacity Building Project – Project Partner; Clayton County Minister’s Conference, President 2006-2007;<br />

Liberty In Life Ministries, Inc. – Board Secretary; Young Adults Talk, Inc. – Board of Directors; ROYAL, Inc - Board of<br />

Directors; Temple University Alumni Association; Rutgers Law School Alumni Association; Sertoma International; Our<br />

Common Welfare Board of Directors – President)2003-2005; River’s Edge Elementary School PTA (Co-President); Summerhill<br />

Community Ministries; Outstanding Young Men of America; Employee of the Year; Academic All-American - Basketball;<br />

Church Trustee.<br />

Page 132 of 134


www.TheAdvocacyFoundation.org<br />

Page 133 of 134


Page 134 of 134

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!