21.12.2012 Views

The Social Causes of Husband-Wife Violence - UNH IT - University ...

The Social Causes of Husband-Wife Violence - UNH IT - University ...

The Social Causes of Husband-Wife Violence - UNH IT - University ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>The</strong> John K. Fesler Memorial Fund provided<br />

assistance in the publication <strong>of</strong> this volume, for which<br />

the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press is grateful.


<strong>The</strong><br />

<strong>Social</strong> <strong>Causes</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Husband</strong>-<strong>Wife</strong><br />

<strong>Violence</strong><br />

Murray A. Straus<br />

and Gerald T. Hotaling<br />

Editors<br />

UNIVERS<strong>IT</strong>Y OF MINNESOTA PRESS OMINNEAPOLIS<br />

1786


Copyright a 1980 by the Vniverrity <strong>of</strong> Minnesota<br />

All rights reserved.<br />

Published by the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press,<br />

2037 <strong>University</strong> Avenue Southeast,<br />

Minneapolis, Minnesota 55414<br />

~ibrary <strong>of</strong> Congress Caralosng in Publication Data<br />

Srraus, Murray Arnold. 1926-<br />

<strong>The</strong> social causes <strong>of</strong> husbandwife violence.<br />

Bibliogiaphy: p.<br />

Includes indexes.<br />

1. Conjugal vioience-<strong>Social</strong> aspecrr-Unired States-<br />

Addresses, essays, lectures. I. Horaling, Gerald T.,<br />

joint aurhor. 11. Tide.<br />

HQ809.3.U5S88 306.8'7 79-27071<br />

ISBN 0-8166-0886-5<br />

ISBN 0-81664955-1 pbk.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Minnesota<br />

is an equal-opporurnity<br />

educator and employer.


Foreword<br />

Despite the lip service ritualisticallp paid to the<br />

need to integrate sociological theory and empirical<br />

research, too <strong>of</strong>ten research on the causes and consequences<br />

<strong>of</strong> social problems lacks thDoretiCa1 guidance. Much<br />

theorizing in such areas, on the other hand. procee9s in<br />

blissful ignorance <strong>of</strong> scientific aata. <strong>The</strong> virtue <strong>of</strong> this<br />

book is that it avoids these pitfalls. It presents research<br />

findings vithin a firmly held theoretical framework, at the<br />

same time that some <strong>of</strong> the empirical findirgs rifins<br />

existino theories. <strong>The</strong>se findinas. to borrow Robert K.<br />

nerton's formulation. help to initiare, refornulate,<br />

deflect. and clarify theories, rather than simply testing<br />

them; and thus contribute not only to empirical knowledge<br />

but also to the consolidation <strong>of</strong> tha theoretical<br />

propositions in the area <strong>of</strong> family relationships and in the<br />

general field <strong>of</strong> social conflict and violence.<br />

what I like particularly in the approach <strong>of</strong> flurray<br />

Straus and his associates is their commitment to an ironic<br />

perspective. <strong>The</strong>y have a fine sense <strong>of</strong> the incongruities<br />

between the public image <strong>of</strong> an institution, :n this case the<br />

family, and the underlying reality. Just as medical<br />

research has shown that hospitals, which are supposed to<br />

make people well, may make them sick and produce iatrogenic<br />

diseases, so the authors show that family living, supposedly<br />

predicated on consensus, integration. a3d harmony, may lead<br />

to forms <strong>of</strong> conflict and v;olence rarely found outside the<br />

family context. <strong>The</strong> very features <strong>of</strong> family life that<br />

contribnta to intimacy, it turns out, also facilitate high<br />

degrees <strong>of</strong> violmce between spouses.<br />

Murray Straus and his associates are finely attunea to<br />

the need to attend to the unanticipated as well as the<br />

anticipated consequences or social actions. <strong>The</strong>y are aware<br />

that although evil intents may lead to desirable<br />

conseqnences, good intentions may lead to undesirable ones.<br />

-.


we learn here, for example, that more egalitarian relations<br />

betveen husbands and uives may have the ironic consequence<br />

or itcreasing rather than decreasing conflict betveen them,<br />

at least in the short rur. <strong>The</strong> authors' orientation to the<br />

ironic perspective on human affairs yields significant<br />

insights that could probably could not have been reached<br />

without this stance.<br />

ThrOnghcUt this book, the authors eschew what Georg<br />

Siamel once called the "fallacy cf separateness." That is,<br />

they never succumb to the temptation to reg+rd family<br />

conflicts in terms <strong>of</strong> the personalities cf husbands and<br />

vives. <strong>The</strong>y are succ~ssful at conveying the idea that<br />

family conflicts, as all types <strong>of</strong> irteractions within the<br />

family, cannot be understood without the realization that<br />

they tend to derive irom social srructures and cultural<br />

norms. <strong>The</strong> high incidence <strong>of</strong> conflict and violence in<br />

conzemporary families, they argue persuasively, ousr b?<br />

nnderstood in terms <strong>of</strong> furdanental co2tradicti3;s buil; into<br />

the foundations <strong>of</strong> family life. <strong>The</strong>y argue, for example,<br />

that when the resources <strong>of</strong> a spouse are low--when that<br />

spouse has, for example. a low status position in the<br />

occopational world--the chances are higher that he or she<br />

w i l l resort to violence in marital quarrels. <strong>The</strong>y draw<br />

attention to the interfamilial consequences <strong>of</strong> the<br />

deprivatlon <strong>of</strong> valued status position with attendant losses<br />

<strong>of</strong> ego identity and symbolic reinforcement <strong>of</strong> self-worth.<br />

Bence, the Ironic tinding that working-class husbands, rho<br />

tend to cling to an ideology <strong>of</strong> male dominance more<br />

determinedly than middle class husbands. in fact possess<br />

fewer resources for exercising power in the family and thus<br />

resort to violence more frequently to compensate.<br />

An ironic perspective, alert to the ambivalence <strong>of</strong><br />

homan relationships, especially in intimats settings, has<br />

borne considerable fruit in this work. Aware that (to<br />

borrow from Bronislaw nalinouski) aggression like charity<br />

begins at home, they have documented with instructive<br />

thoroughness that, ccntrary to the prevailing image, family<br />

relations are the breeaing ground <strong>of</strong> both love ana<br />

hostzlity, <strong>of</strong> selfless devotior and <strong>of</strong> destructive violence.<br />

what is more, they have shown that to decrease rhe level <strong>of</strong><br />

violence in family settings ixvolves more than counseling<br />

and therapy. It involves EO less than a restructuring <strong>of</strong><br />

relarions between men and women, which, in its turn, is<br />

largely dependent on a iundamental restrncturirg <strong>of</strong> the<br />

allocation <strong>of</strong> power and status in the society at large. I<br />

hope that their semlnal contribution w i l l find an echo among<br />

scholarly investig+tors and sccial prsctltioners alike.<br />

Stony Brook. N.I. Lewis A. Coser


Acknowledgments<br />

This book is a product <strong>of</strong> the Family <strong>Violence</strong> Research<br />

Program at the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> New Sampshire. <strong>The</strong> program<br />

began as a result <strong>of</strong> the stimulus provided by the 1970<br />

annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the Batioral Council OL Pamilp Relations.<br />

<strong>The</strong> theme <strong>of</strong> that confererce was "<strong>Violence</strong> And <strong>The</strong> Family."<br />

Preparing a paper tor the conference (Straus, 1971) and the<br />

discussion that followed. made us see both the theoretical<br />

and the practical importance <strong>of</strong> research on physical<br />

ViOlSnce in families. RE are now even more convinced that<br />

such research w i l l increase our gelera1 undsrs;anding <strong>of</strong> thb<br />

human family and w i l l provide knowledge thar w i l l rsduc~ a<br />

Source <strong>of</strong> vast human misery.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Paaily VLolence Research Program also illustrates<br />

what can be accomplirhed when a group <strong>of</strong> graduate students<br />

and faculty focus on a single topic ores even a relatively<br />

short period <strong>of</strong> time. ur hope it confirms the faith ir our<br />

work Shown in the financial support provided by +.he<br />

<strong>University</strong> or new Hampshire and by the National Instttute <strong>of</strong><br />

nental Health (grants number nH27557. nR13050. and ME15161).<br />

A list <strong>of</strong> publications from the Paeily <strong>Violence</strong> Research is<br />

available on request.<br />

we also want to express our appreciation to the<br />

anonymous referees who reviewed the book. and '0 many<br />

colleagues, both at the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> New Sampshire ana<br />

elsewhere, whose comments, criticisms, and suggastions have<br />

been important to the development <strong>of</strong> the Family violence<br />

Research Program in general and/or the specific chapters in<br />

this book. A Special thanks to Sieglinde Fizz for her<br />

Conscientioos typing <strong>of</strong> this manuscript and for her patience<br />

acd good nature throughout this project.<br />

Durham. New Hampshlre nurray E. Skraus<br />

Gerald T. Eotaling<br />

vii


FOREWORD by Levis L. Coser<br />

ACKBOYLEDGBENTS<br />

Contents<br />

Ex: L eBisu<br />

Culture, <strong>Social</strong> Organization, and Ironp in rhe Study<br />

<strong>of</strong> Pamily <strong>Violence</strong>, by Gerald 1. Botaling and<br />

Kurray A. straus<br />

Uife-Beating: Bow Common and Why?<br />

bp tlurrap 8. Straus<br />

<strong>The</strong> fiarriage Llcense as a Bitting License: Evidence<br />

from Populer Culture. Law, and <strong>Social</strong> Science.<br />

by lurrap A. straur<br />

Vlolence and the <strong>Social</strong> Structure as Reflected in<br />

Children's Books from 1850 to 1970. by nartha D.<br />

uuggzns and Burray A. straus<br />

A Cultural-Consistency <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Family <strong>Violence</strong> ir<br />

lexican-American and Jewish-Ethnic Groups, by<br />

Joseph C. Carroll<br />

---- Part ---- 111. <strong>Social</strong> Qspization and Pq!&&y SL_a&ezE:<br />

Sexual Inequality and <strong>Wife</strong> Beating,<br />

by lurray A. Straus<br />

stress and Pamily <strong>Violence</strong>,<br />

by Keith n. Farrington<br />

<strong>The</strong> Paradoxical Nature <strong>of</strong> Pamily Relationships acd<br />

Family conflict. by Joyce E. Poss<br />

Attribution Processes in Eusbaxd-<strong>Wife</strong> <strong>Violence</strong>,<br />

by Gerald T. Botaling


--- Part -- IV. -- <strong>The</strong> InterDlax <strong>of</strong> culture a& Sccial Q.;q=gizgtior<br />

10. "And we Eaven't Bad Iry Problems Sincen: Conjugal<br />

Violerce and the Politics <strong>of</strong> marriage,<br />

by Ralph LaRossa<br />

11. Uife-Employment, narltal Equality, and <strong>Husband</strong>-Rife<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>, by Bruce u. Brovn<br />

12. Resources. POVBI, and Susband-<strong>Wife</strong> VioleLce, by<br />

Craig M. Lllen and Burray 1. Straus<br />

13. A Soc~ologlcal Perspective on the Preventian <strong>of</strong><br />

II~E-Beatl~g<br />

by Hurray E . Straus<br />

BEPEREBCES<br />

ADTBOR INDEX<br />

SUBJECT INDEX<br />

ABOUT TAE AUTHORS


Part I Overview


Chapter 1<br />

Culture, <strong>Social</strong> Organization, and<br />

Irony in the Study <strong>of</strong> Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Gerald T. Hotaling and Murray A. Straus<br />

That acts <strong>of</strong> physical violence are common--even<br />

typical--oi American DarriagEs has been well established<br />

(see Chapter 2 and Straus, Gelles, and Sreinnetz, 1979).<br />

Yhar is not known is why violence occurs, or what to do<br />

about it. <strong>The</strong> perspective <strong>of</strong> this book is simple: that<br />

physical violencs between husbands and wives is socially<br />

patterned. $1<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters are deliberately diverse. but all share<br />

the perspective that violence grows out <strong>of</strong> the ncture <strong>of</strong><br />

social arrangements. In part. the diversity is inevitable<br />

because the authors are different. <strong>The</strong> mejor differerces,<br />

however, are built into the plan <strong>of</strong> the book--to present<br />

major differences in vieupoin;. lost <strong>of</strong> the chapters<br />

present thwries to explain the prevalence <strong>of</strong> violence in<br />

the famly. Since the social causes <strong>of</strong> husband-wife<br />

violence are dlverse and complex, the different chapters<br />

seek to show how different sets <strong>of</strong> these factors might<br />

operate to produce violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters also differ because. no matter how cogent<br />

the theory, it must be supported by empirical evidence.<br />

Consequently, five <strong>of</strong> the chapters report such data. Aere<br />

also deliberate diversity exists. illustrating such<br />

aifferen? methoas as case studies, content analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

popular literature, brief questionnaire studies, and a<br />

survey <strong>of</strong> a nationally representative sample <strong>of</strong> couples.<br />

Each <strong>of</strong> rhese theories, and each <strong>of</strong> tbese methcds, has<br />

limit~tiohs and advantages.<br />

paradox <strong>of</strong> marital violence.<br />

Tcgether, they help unravel the


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 4<br />

MRR<strong>IT</strong>AL VIOLENCE AND THE SOCIAL STRUCTUSE<br />

Any attempt to understand uhg physical violencs occurs<br />

so <strong>of</strong>ten in American marriages must take into account the<br />

way society Structures the interaction <strong>of</strong> husbands and<br />

wives. Of course, a proposition that .simple is recessarily<br />

deceptive. Our intention is not to convrnce the reader that<br />

the aspects <strong>of</strong> social patterning to vhich this book gives<br />

attentlon are the only causes <strong>of</strong> marital violsncs. We<br />

acknowledge the complexity <strong>of</strong> married life, but temporarily<br />

narrow our focus so that we can gain a clearer understarding<br />

<strong>of</strong> that part <strong>of</strong> the complex vhcle <strong>of</strong> husband/vife corflict<br />

that is created by the very nature <strong>of</strong> our society.<br />

Physical violence among family members usually is<br />

considered an infregusnt occurrence; when violence does<br />

erupt, the husband or wife who resorts to it is thought to<br />

be dafective or abnormal. Neither vieu seems to he correct.<br />

Rars and riots aside, physical aggression occurs more <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

among family members than among any others. noreover, the<br />

family is the predominant setting for every form <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

violence from slaps to torture and murder. In fact, some<br />

form <strong>of</strong> physical violence in the life cycle <strong>of</strong> fzmily<br />

members is so likely that it can be said to be almost<br />

universal (see the section on "Family Sccialization in<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>" in Chapter 2). If this is indeed the case, then<br />

violence is as typical <strong>of</strong> family relationships as is lsre.<br />

<strong>The</strong> available evidence suggests that, with rare<br />

exceptions, family members using violence are not mentally<br />

ill. Instead. violent acts by one family member againsr<br />

another are the result <strong>of</strong> socially learned and socially<br />

patterned behavior.<br />

<strong>The</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> causation this book describes are what<br />

soc101ogiSts refer to as "social structure." <strong>The</strong> coccept <strong>of</strong><br />

social structure is almost as elusive as it is important for<br />

understanding how society works and how social life affects<br />

our relationships. A recent volume (Blzu, 1975) gives ample<br />

testimony to the vide variety <strong>of</strong> ways in vhich the concept<br />

is used. But amidst this diversity <strong>of</strong> definition, a gansral<br />

ihterpretation exists, as stated in the program <strong>of</strong> the 1974<br />

meeting <strong>of</strong> the American Sociological Association:<br />

Whatever t h ~ sp~cific orientation, the<br />

structural approach is designed to explain, no:<br />

the behavior <strong>of</strong> individuals, but the structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> relations among groups and individuals that<br />

finas expression in this behapior. (Blau,<br />

1975:2)<br />

our speciflc approach to social structure contairs two<br />

main elements. First, we use the term to mean those aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> society that are relatlvely enduring and that transcecd


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 5<br />

the individual and the particular moment. Rot that social<br />

structure is fixed: indeed, it constantly changes. But<br />

compared vith other factors being enamired, it is relatrvely<br />

stable. Second. we identify tuo different but interrelated<br />

processes <strong>of</strong> social patterning: culture and social<br />

organization. In reality, culture and social organization<br />

cannot be separated. but for aralytical porposes it is an<br />

important distinction.<br />

<strong>The</strong> following two sectiors sunmerize the concepts <strong>of</strong><br />

culture and <strong>of</strong> social organization. <strong>The</strong>se w i l l be familiar<br />

to many readers <strong>of</strong> this book, especially sociologists, and<br />

can be skipped over. Rowever, ue want to address th3se in<br />

other pr<strong>of</strong>essicns concerned with family violence, such as<br />

psychologists, social workers, psychiatrists, police<br />

Officers, and lawyers. <strong>The</strong>se sections sill help alert them<br />

to these crucial aspects <strong>of</strong> the sociological perspective.<br />

This is particularly importart because it contrasts sharply<br />

with the more usual psychclogical explanations for riolsnce.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Concept <strong>of</strong> Culture<br />

when we describe "cultural influence1' we are really<br />

talking about causes. <strong>The</strong> question addressed in Part I1 <strong>of</strong><br />

this book is the extent to which culture causes violence<br />

between family members. Before that question can be<br />

answered, one must first be clear about vhat culture is.<br />

<strong>The</strong> term culture means something roughly siailar to<br />

social heredity, that is, the total legacy <strong>of</strong> past human<br />

behavior effective in the present or vhat is available to be<br />

learned from others (Williams, 1970). That concept covers a<br />

vast domain, ranging from how to hold a spoon or say the<br />

old ~ - ~~ father to ~~ the ~~~ com~lezities . . <strong>of</strong> matrix aloebra. Thus a<br />

~~~~~<br />

mreat deal <strong>of</strong> the culture <strong>of</strong> a sociefv is not <strong>of</strong> direct<br />

2 -<br />

-<br />

lnterest to those concOrned with understanding the famrly.<br />

<strong>The</strong> aspect <strong>of</strong> culture <strong>of</strong> direct interest is what are<br />

called social norms. A social norm prescribes the correct<br />

thing to do *her interacticg vith another person. To be<br />

cultural norms, these norms must be prescriptions shared by<br />

the society cr sector <strong>of</strong> a society in which the b~havior<br />

takes place. <strong>The</strong>y also nust bc rules <strong>of</strong> behavior that are<br />

learned from others.<br />

Cultural norms in large part account for differences<br />

between the family patterns <strong>of</strong> people in different societies<br />

and in different subgroups within one society (for example,<br />

differences between social classes or betveen groups such as<br />

Prench-speaking and English-speaking Canadians).<br />

Cultural norms regulate almost all aspects <strong>of</strong> family<br />

life. <strong>The</strong>y provide a blueprint <strong>of</strong> the behavior appropriate<br />

for husbands, wives, children, grandfathers; in fact, for


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 6<br />

each <strong>of</strong> the relationships within a family. Thus. the<br />

culture contains norms specifying how marriages are to be<br />

arranged (and, if necessary. dissolved), who fs LO be<br />

regarded as a member <strong>of</strong> the family. what activities a<br />

husband should carry out in relation to the rife and vice<br />

versa, how children should be brought uo, ard so on.<br />

If ths idea <strong>of</strong> cultural norms as causes <strong>of</strong> family<br />

behavior were taksn liberally, all families within a giver<br />

society would be expected to act in the same w3y.<br />

Obviously, that is not the case: every family is in some<br />

ways urique. <strong>The</strong>refore. even thoogh culture does, on the<br />

average, define what family life is like, it cannot tell the<br />

whale story. Ue must round out the story somewhat by<br />

consid~ring what sociologists call social organization.<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Organizational Influences<br />

<strong>Social</strong> organization refers to the patte-n <strong>of</strong><br />

relationships amcng individuals and among groups--how the<br />

parts are related to each other and to the whole. Some<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> social organization are dictated by the culture,<br />

many are not. Whether or not it is prescribed hy culture.<br />

each aspect <strong>of</strong> social organization has consequences that are<br />

distinct from the cultural influences. For example, a<br />

family might contain ore, two, three, four, or eight<br />

children. Cultural "rules" specify how many children one<br />

should have: the middle-class rules <strong>of</strong> the recent past<br />

tended to specify two or three children. AnyonE who had ro<br />

children was under considerable cultural pressure. <strong>The</strong><br />

pressure is <strong>of</strong>ten subtle. but may be expressed openly: "Why<br />

donqt you hzve any childrsn?" or "Kow cons you have six<br />

children?' But subtle or cot, social prPssures to conform do<br />

exist and most <strong>of</strong> US follo~ the rules <strong>of</strong> the culture.<br />

Now the number <strong>of</strong> children in a family is an important<br />

aspect <strong>of</strong> its socigl organization and makes a difference to<br />

what goes on in that family, no matter whether the parents<br />

had that number <strong>of</strong> chlldren becauss <strong>of</strong> cultural rules,<br />

hiological limirs on fertility, or contraceptive failure.<br />

To take a simple example, if t he family eats the evening<br />

meal together, the number <strong>of</strong> children present rill influence<br />

how long any one child can, or the average, talk at the<br />

dinner table. Rssume that each child gets ar equal chance<br />

and that the meal lasts 30 minutes. If there are two<br />

children, each child can talk for ten minutes (allowing ten<br />

minutes for the parerts to say something). But if therf are<br />

four children, each child*s l i m i t is cut to fire minutes.


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 7<br />

TEE SOCIIL STRUCTURAL CAUSES OP BUSBABD-RIFE VIOLENCE<br />

<strong>The</strong> preceding two sections preface the shared ideas <strong>of</strong><br />

the authors <strong>of</strong> this volume. This is not to sly that they<br />

are in complete agreement as to the social structural causes<br />

Of husband-wife violence, but all focus on the cultural and<br />

Organizational features cf Bm-rican marriagss a.d their<br />

relatlon to conflict and vtolcnce.<br />

Each chapter in this book. whether it is a deduction<br />

from an existing theory, a case study, or a statistical<br />

analysis, attempts to clarify some aspect <strong>of</strong> the social<br />

causes Of husband-wife violence. <strong>The</strong> chapters are<br />

deliberately diverse in approach because re believe that<br />

case studies, statistical analyses, and theoretical<br />

deduction all are necessary in the search for an explanation<br />

<strong>of</strong> marital violence.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the difficulties in research on fanily violence<br />

has been too heavy an emphasis on fact-gathering relative to<br />

testing causal theories. Since this is partly because<br />

PrOmiSiPg theories have not been formulated, half <strong>of</strong> the<br />

chapters attempt to fill that void. 3ut in no case is the<br />

purpose <strong>of</strong> any chapter to set forth an integratea or<br />

complete sxplanation <strong>of</strong> husbacd-wife violence. That task is<br />

at the presen% stage <strong>of</strong> our knowledge clearly beyond what<br />

Can be accomplished, even though preliminarr steps have been<br />

taken (Gelles and Straus, 1979).<br />

with thls general understanding, the authors <strong>of</strong> the<br />

various chapters spare the reader a repetitious disclaimer<br />

about the partial nature <strong>of</strong> the theory examinea and, in the<br />

Case <strong>of</strong> the strictly theoretical chapters, do not repeat<br />

that the conclusions are intended to stimulafs empirical<br />

research. not to substitute far -~- such -.--<br />

~ data. --.-. Thlrs. .-... for -..<br />

example. in the first two chapters we try to show that some<br />

factor in the family system cot only produces a high level<br />

<strong>of</strong> aggression, but also makes wives the most frequent<br />

victims <strong>of</strong> that aggression. <strong>The</strong> third chapter, although<br />

admitting the organizational features <strong>of</strong> family life that<br />

contribute to this high level <strong>of</strong> aggression, examines<br />

through popular culture and other matezials the prevailing<br />

sexist attitudes about the role <strong>of</strong> violence in the faailv<br />

system. <strong>The</strong> result is to make explicit the implicit set ii<br />

cultural norms and values +hat legitimate, and at times<br />

encourage. violence betveen husbands and wives.<br />

Conversely, Parrington (Chapter 7) and Hotaling<br />

(Chapter 9) both recognize the exrstsnce <strong>of</strong> cultural norms,<br />

but focus on the organizational features <strong>of</strong> married life<br />

that contribute to violence. Parrington presents a theory<br />

<strong>of</strong> intrafamily violence based on the notion <strong>of</strong> optimum<br />

Stress level. He defines stress as an imbalance between the<br />

demands with which an individual or family is faced and the


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 8<br />

capability <strong>of</strong> responding appropriately to them. Be argues<br />

that all individuals and faailies derrlop persoral and<br />

unique optimum stress levels at which they function most<br />

comfortably. Similarlg. Hotaling specifies the particular<br />

combinations <strong>of</strong> family rules End family structural<br />

characteristics which produce a high probatility that a<br />

family member w i l l attribute malevolent intent to the rule<br />

violations <strong>of</strong> other faoily members.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Ristory <strong>of</strong> the Study <strong>of</strong> Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

DeScribir.g a "history" <strong>of</strong> the sociological study <strong>of</strong><br />

intrafamily violence may be premaure; it has beer a very<br />

short time since sociologists first turnea 2 critical eye on<br />

the phenomenon. But the period has been long enough to<br />

point out some differences between "earlier" work and the<br />

present volume. Previous efforts rere aimed at establishing<br />

the prevalence, the correlates. and, most important, the<br />

socially patterned nature <strong>of</strong> family violence (Steinmetz and<br />

Straus, 197'4; Gelles, 1974). Intrafamily violence was<br />

established as a widespread phenomenon, appearing in many<br />

forms besides the mars spectacular crimes <strong>of</strong> murder and<br />

child abuse. Studles <strong>of</strong> husbands and wives revealed<br />

varying, but suhstantial, aaounts <strong>of</strong> spousal violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> most accurate measure, to date. <strong>of</strong> the extont <strong>of</strong><br />

husband-wife violence comes from the analysis <strong>of</strong> data from<br />

the representative sample <strong>of</strong> American families presented in<br />

Chapter 2. Straus finds that, during the survey year, one<br />

<strong>of</strong> every six couples (16 percent) reported violence between<br />

spouses. If the reference period is the duration <strong>of</strong> the<br />

marriage, the figure is between one <strong>of</strong> four and one <strong>of</strong> three<br />

couples (27.8 percent). Straus warns, however, that these<br />

figures are probably affected by substantial underreporting.<br />

It is almost certain that not everyons "told all.' Pilot<br />

studies and informal evidence (where some <strong>of</strong> the factors<br />

leadirg to underreporticg were less) indicate that these<br />

figures could easily be twice as largs.<br />

Previous work on intrafamily violence also has negated<br />

the comfortable notio2 that family violance can be explainea<br />

SolEly hy psychopathology. <strong>The</strong> sheer amouc?. es well as the<br />

~~~~~~~~d variat:=c i? rlres at lrtritaeily VID~FPCE among<br />

various SDclal qroups, bol:?s ar expla2a:ioc a?.chcrel rr ?he<br />

abnormalities <strong>of</strong> individual members.<br />

Besides establishing the prevalence <strong>of</strong> violence in the<br />

home and the extent <strong>of</strong> 'sociological causes, earlier<br />

investigators also have paid attention to +he particular<br />

social factors related to family violence. For exlmple.<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the factors found to be associated with different<br />

rates <strong>of</strong> family violence were subcultural norms (Coser,<br />

1967; Wolfgang and Perracuti. 1971), social class<br />

(Levinger. 1966; Kohn. 1969). and a husband's lack <strong>of</strong> ths


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 9<br />

"resources" necessary to legitimate his positLon as family<br />

head (Goode, 1971: O'Brien, 1971).<br />

<strong>The</strong>sP writers point to factors that may make certain<br />

couples more or less likely to engag5 in physical<br />

aggression; the present volume focuses attention on the<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> married life i+self. Ths oriectirg qu?sticc<br />

becomes: vhat cultural and social organizatioral processes<br />

make the marital dyad a potentially violent social<br />

relationship?<br />

B few sociological investigators as well have<br />

recognized certain incongruities in marriage. For example,<br />

Cuber ar,d Har<strong>of</strong>f's study (1965) <strong>of</strong> middle-class marriages<br />

reveals a type characterized by arguments and fights. <strong>The</strong><br />

"conflict-habituated" marriage basically relies or hostility<br />

to bind the couple together and to lend stability to the<br />

marriage.<br />

Aicks and Platt (1970). in a review <strong>of</strong> studies <strong>of</strong><br />

marital happiness, find that low happiness <strong>of</strong>ten may be<br />

associated with marital stability. Similar findings that<br />

appear contrary to common sense are those <strong>of</strong> Blood and Rolfe<br />

(1960) and Piteo (1961). who stated that marriages over time<br />

experience a d=g& in companionship, affection, and common<br />

values and beliefs: as couples become more familiar vith<br />

each other, they become more estranged. Lastly, simmel's<br />

(1950) classlc analysis <strong>of</strong> the marital dyaa sees the<br />

two-person bond as the most imtimate and, at the same time,<br />

the most unstable social relationship.<br />

RereiC lies the important contribution <strong>of</strong> logically<br />

orgarlzel, probrrq :heore-zeal zrd ssp1:ical snalys?s: rtay<br />

ars rpprcally courrer:?turi:ve, :hat IS, tt.ay rovsal the<br />

existence ot phe~omen2 aid relatronsh-ps con'razy t3 zaaa?r<br />

sense.


Page<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters contained in this volume mphasize<br />

large part the ironic nature <strong>of</strong> married life and violent<br />

<strong>The</strong> intriguing question, which all thesn chapters aadrer<br />

is why the social group that society most cften lcoks to 1<br />

warmth, intimacy, help, and love. is also characterized<br />

cruelty and violence. It is to an examination <strong>of</strong> this a<br />

other ~ronies about husbard-wife violence that ue ccw turn<br />

IRCNP AND PABILY VIOLENCE<br />

<strong>The</strong> recognition and study <strong>of</strong> family violence hz<br />

blurred apparently simple ideas about the relationshi<br />

between the ramily and deviance. Once we recognize tha<br />

families are not easily classified as either normal o<br />

abnormal, healthy or sick, the picture becomes complicated<br />

<strong>The</strong>se complications, though, lead to a fuller appreciatio<br />

Of the complexity <strong>of</strong> family life. As David natza has argue<br />

in his interpretation <strong>of</strong> theories <strong>of</strong> deviance and thei<br />

handling <strong>of</strong> the distinction between conventional and a~vian,<br />

phenomena:<br />

once the distincticn hetween good and evil is<br />

made problematic, once their interpenetration is<br />

stressed, a similar insight map develop with<br />

respect to the relations between phenomena and<br />

their purportea causes. (1969:69)<br />

It we accept the nocion that the family is a social<br />

group capable <strong>of</strong> generating conflict and violsnce just as<br />

easily as intlnacy and love, we can accept the idea <strong>of</strong> irony<br />

in the relation <strong>of</strong> tamily life to violence.<br />

What do we mean by irony? nost simply, irony rPfers to<br />

a point <strong>of</strong> view, the ability to see Phenomena related to one<br />

another in curious ways. As fiatza notes:<br />

... irony refers to the complicated--and<br />

suprising--relations between good and evil<br />

phenomena in sequer.ce. Irony is a state <strong>of</strong><br />

affairs or a result opposite to, and as if in<br />

mockery <strong>of</strong>. the appropriate result. (1969:691<br />

In a sociological sense, irony is a point <strong>of</strong> view that<br />

recognizes the coexistence <strong>of</strong> incongruities in the culture<br />

and organization <strong>of</strong> social life. Thus, the marital<br />

relationship is organized according to certain cultural<br />

values that ere intended to maximize love, support, and<br />

happiness. Xouever. because <strong>of</strong> this very sane mode <strong>of</strong><br />

organization an6 because <strong>of</strong> the influence <strong>of</strong> thess sane<br />

cultural values, conflict and violence coexist with these<br />

more benign aspects <strong>of</strong> married life. Again, as NatZa tells<br />

us, "a key element <strong>of</strong> irony is latsncy" (p. 70). Qualities<br />

inherent in social norms end social organization, despite


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 11<br />

their hidden nature, lead tc unexpacted ZesuLts ic the<br />

family setting.+Z<br />

Irony also points to the existecc* <strong>of</strong> pa'terning in<br />

relationships where no patterning is obvious. For example,<br />

Reinhold Niebuhr has deficed irony as "appnrent.ly for?nFrous<br />

ircongruities in life rhich are discovere9, cpon closer<br />

examination, to be not merely fortuitous' l1952:viii). <strong>The</strong><br />

incongruities surrounding husband-vife violence are ironic<br />

in Niebuhr's sense cf the word, for the sxisting evidencs<br />

Supports the claim that husband-vifr violence is not the<br />

result <strong>of</strong> rardom events, but is patterned into the very<br />

structure <strong>of</strong> marital and family relatiocs.<br />

Before we specify the ironies apparent in husbacd-wife<br />

uiolence, it would be useful to point out why there is such<br />

a rssistance to viewing husband-wife violence as a patterned<br />

aspect cf married life.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Eyth <strong>of</strong> Pam~ly Nonviolence<br />

<strong>The</strong> family is usually thought to he a group committed<br />

to nonviolence between its members. Family msmbers are<br />

supposed to maintain benevolent and loving relationships.<br />

From rhat is known about the prevalence <strong>of</strong> family violence<br />

in American society, there seems to be a discrepancy between<br />

the idealized picture <strong>of</strong> the family and rhat actually goes<br />

on [Steinmete and Straur, 1974: Chapter 1; straus, 1974b).<br />

This idealization is a useful, perhaps necessary social<br />

myth. Its usefulness derives from the family's position as<br />

a tremendously important social institution. Elaborate<br />

precautions are taken to strengthen and support it. In<br />

Pestern Countries, one <strong>of</strong> these supportive devices is the<br />

myth <strong>of</strong> familial love and gectleness. <strong>The</strong> ideal exourages<br />

people to marry and stay married despite the stresses and<br />

strains <strong>of</strong> family life (Perreira, 1963). Thus fron the<br />

standpoint <strong>of</strong> preserving the integrity <strong>of</strong> a crucial<br />

institution. such a mgthology is highly uscful.<br />

This myth is transmitted through<br />

values as reflected in literature,<br />

cultural norms and<br />

motion pictures. and<br />

television. For example, Buggins and Straus (Chapter 4)<br />

find the myth cr family nonviolence a pervasive theme <strong>of</strong><br />

children,~ literature between 1850 and 1970. Uhlle .the<br />

authors find that the typical children's classic bock is<br />

marked by a number <strong>of</strong> violent acts, including killings,<br />

alaost no intrafaaily faailp violence is depicted. This<br />

seems remarkahle in llght <strong>of</strong> the fact that i n society<br />

generally, physical violence betvsen family members is more<br />

common than be:weer any other aggressor-victim relationship.<br />

AS the myth <strong>of</strong> family nonviolence makes clear, a set <strong>of</strong><br />

cultural norms promulgates an image <strong>of</strong> the family as a place<br />

<strong>of</strong> lose and gentleness. Bt the sane time, as already


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> structure and Irony Page 12<br />

mentioned, a set <strong>of</strong> norms exists that legitrmates, axd at<br />

times encourages, the use <strong>of</strong> violence on fenily members.<br />

This is an excellent example <strong>of</strong> a cultural contradiction.<br />

Cultural contradictions are found i? most, perhaps al:<br />

societies. <strong>The</strong>y are by no means entirely unaesirable. In<br />

fact. cultural contradictions help prevent sccieties from<br />

stagnating, open possibilities for social change, ard allow<br />

for a measure <strong>of</strong> individual iutonomy. without them, we<br />

night be slaves to the dictates <strong>of</strong> culture. Each individual<br />

and each family must manipulate many, <strong>of</strong>ten conflicting,<br />

norms and values to work out a strategy appropriate to their<br />

Own CirCUmStanC'3s and aspirations. <strong>The</strong> process <strong>of</strong> selectirg<br />

from and reconcillng the different aspects <strong>of</strong> a culture is<br />

one <strong>of</strong> the reasons why, despite cultural noras, families<br />

differ.<br />

<strong>The</strong> choice <strong>of</strong> norms for guiding one's life leads to<br />

diversity, but there is no guarantee that one w i l l recognize<br />

all <strong>of</strong> the implications <strong>of</strong> that choice. Certain unsxpected<br />

resuLtS. 01 latent features <strong>of</strong> these choices. make<br />

individual and family life a complicated matter.<br />

This complexity is especially true for violence in the<br />

faaily. Knowing that our family system is a violent system.<br />

rational people certainly woold not choose this situation.<br />

Yet over 90 per cent <strong>of</strong> Bmericans marry, and many marriages<br />

are marked by conflict and violence. Perhaps what is<br />

important for understanaing the relationship between<br />

vlolence and the family is not individual choice but the<br />

ironies that seem to underlie the norms and organization <strong>of</strong><br />

family life.<br />

Irony 1. Cultural norms that legitimate and, ar<br />

times. encourage violence between faaily<br />

members are instrumental in maintaining the<br />

family system; but these same Lorms<br />

perpetuate violence as an integral part <strong>of</strong><br />

family life.<br />

A set ot norms exists that legitimize the striking <strong>of</strong><br />

family members, at least under certain conditions. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

norms sometimes are used to justify the use <strong>of</strong> violence to<br />

maintain the family systen. In no way should this statement<br />

be interpreted to mean that we favor any type <strong>of</strong> family<br />

System Over another. Whether our present family structure<br />

should be mairtained or changed is nct at issue here. In<br />

most forms <strong>of</strong> social organization. whether it is a whole<br />

society. a bureaucracy, or the family system, there is an<br />

emphasis on maintaining the status quo. Even though social<br />

organization constantly changes, groop well-beicg is<br />

adversely affected if no provision is made for a relatively<br />

stable framework for action.


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 13<br />

Our interest hers is understanding the relationship<br />

betveec the norms that legitimize violecce erd the role<br />

these norms play in family life. Goode (1971) argues that<br />

those who desire to maintaiP the present family syst?m may<br />

USE force or its threat as a form <strong>of</strong> social control. In<br />

other words. norms legitimizirg violence maintair craer in<br />

the fanily group by imposing strong sanctions uh~l ar<br />

individual tries to play by other "rules." For many in<br />

American society, force or the threat <strong>of</strong> force in the family<br />

is seen as a permitted technique for preventing or<br />

controlling certain behaviors <strong>of</strong> family members. Is Goode<br />

notes:<br />

... the mother who abandons her children. the father<br />

who runs <strong>of</strong>f vith the children, the wife or husband<br />

who takes a second spouse, the child who beats up<br />

his mother. the adolescent girl rho wishes to sp~nd<br />

a ueekend with her boyfriend against the rill <strong>of</strong> her<br />

parents, the wife who wishes to change the family<br />

domicile without the consent <strong>of</strong> her husband, a11 can<br />

be and sometimes are restrained by either force or<br />

its threat, if not from family members then at their<br />

request by the community through its command over<br />

force (1971:626).<br />

nost Americans see a moral obligation for parents to<br />

use physical punishment as a means for controlling children<br />

if other means fall (Stark and McEvoy, 1970). and a good<br />

proportion see it as the most desirable means for<br />

Controlling children. Although the legal right <strong>of</strong> a husband<br />

to physically punish a wife no longer exists (Calrert,<br />

1974). the informal corms <strong>of</strong> certain social groups (and<br />

speolflc families in all segments <strong>of</strong> society) still<br />

legitimize the use <strong>of</strong> physical force to control an errant<br />

spouse.<br />

Of the modalities that ensure that the family functions<br />

as an efficient social group. violence is seen to have high<br />

instrumental value. It uorks--at least in the short run.<br />

This kind <strong>of</strong> violence is what Gelles an8 Straus (1978)<br />

call 'legitimate-instrumental** violence, violence that is<br />

permitted or required by the norms <strong>of</strong> society. Physical<br />

force 1s used to induce some desired act or to prevent some<br />

undesired behavior. Legitimate-instromental violence occurs<br />

in all role-relationships <strong>of</strong> the nuclear family vith greater<br />

or lesser frequency. <strong>The</strong> greatest frequency is in the<br />

parent-child relationship in the form <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

punlshment, but as Straus shows in Chapter 3, the marriage<br />

license also tends to- be a hitting license.<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> can also t+ke a ~~legitimate-expressiv€ls<br />

form.<br />

By expressive violence ue mean the use <strong>of</strong> physical force to<br />

cause pain or injury as an end in itself. Examples <strong>of</strong> this<br />

type <strong>of</strong> violence in the family include the videspread


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 14<br />

beliefs that it is better to spank a child than to "hold in"<br />

one's anger and better to let siblings "fight it out' than<br />

to interfere.<br />

R t tines violence in the family setting goes beyond<br />

sanctioning by cultural norms; it is illegitimate. This<br />

most widely recognized type <strong>of</strong> violence in ths family<br />

includes the most spectacular and extreme forms: child<br />

abuse and murder.<br />

Whenever physical force is used within the family, for<br />

vhatever reason. there are certain unexpected outcomes.<br />

Chapter 2 points 'to three such unexpected outcomes. <strong>The</strong><br />

first is the association <strong>of</strong> love vith violence. <strong>The</strong> child<br />

learns that those vho love him/her most are also those who<br />

hit and have the right to hit. <strong>The</strong> second unintended<br />

Consequence is the lesson that when so~ething is really<br />

important. it justifies the use <strong>of</strong> physical force. Pically,<br />

and most important. these indirect lessons becoms so<br />

fundamertal to the individual's personality and vorld view<br />

that they are gsneralizcd to social relationships, end<br />

especially to the relatiocship closest to that <strong>of</strong> parent and<br />

child-- that <strong>of</strong> husband and wife.<br />

This last point is examined in detail in Joseph<br />

Carroll's chapter on the differential role <strong>of</strong> violence in<br />

Mexican-lmerlcan and Jewish subculturer. He attempts to<br />

show how general family norms and values oparafing uithir<br />

these two divergent ethnic groups act to influence the<br />

actual use <strong>of</strong> violence as well as its perpetuation in future<br />

generations.<br />

<strong>The</strong> irony here is that these norms, seer. to be<br />

necessary in maintaining the family group, also gaarantee<br />

that violence w i l l become a regular feature <strong>of</strong> family life<br />

in the future. This guarantoe is ensured by simple<br />

membership in the family group. R s Gelles (1974) notss:<br />

"...the family serves as basic training for<br />

violence by Exposing children to violence, by<br />

maklng them victims <strong>of</strong> violence, and by<br />

providing them with learning contexts for the<br />

commission <strong>of</strong> violent actsn (p. 170).<br />

Indeed, the evidence is clear that early experiences with<br />

physical punrshment lay the groundvork for the normative<br />

legitimacy and actual use <strong>of</strong> intrafanily violence. Overs<br />

and straus (1975). in a study <strong>of</strong> exposure to violencs and<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> approval, show that the more violence experienced<br />

by a child, the greater the tendency to fzror the use <strong>of</strong><br />

violence as an adult. Gutsacher (1960) states that a common<br />

experience amorg a group <strong>of</strong> murderers he studied was the<br />

high level <strong>of</strong> violence their parents inflicted on them when<br />

they were growing up. Tansy's (1969) study <strong>of</strong> homicidal<br />

<strong>of</strong>fenders finds that 67 percent had histories <strong>of</strong> violent


ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure ard Irony Pilgs 15<br />

child-rea-mg. Gelles (1919) finds tha? respondrn%s who had<br />

seen their parents engaging in physic31 violence vers much<br />

o r likely to physically fight vith rhexr our spouses than<br />

were people uho never saw their parents physically fight<br />

(p. 173).<br />

Welsh (1976) explored the relationship betwsen severe<br />

parental punishment and juvenile delinquency. Be defines<br />

severe parental punishment as any type <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

discipline using an object capable <strong>of</strong> lnflicting physical<br />

injury. welshqs analysls <strong>of</strong> three samples <strong>of</strong> delinquent<br />

children ~ -- - led him to conclude that the recidivist - - - - male<br />

dsll-.qur?: ~ n o cad never beer exposed t C L bilL, blard,<br />

exte:5icn cord or ti?: wb5 COr.EIlSzer:. IC CddlI:>r, Ie1sh<br />

t1r.d~ amOCg ale ?elirlue~?s -<br />

c srrcco rElatlCnshio b2-weer.<br />

severe parental punlshnent and aggression.<br />

Increasicg evidence indicates that a high price is paid<br />

for aaintaining order in the family through violeoc?. <strong>The</strong><br />

norms that legltieate violence assure a family institution<br />

and a society characterized by violence for years to come.<br />

Irony 2. <strong>The</strong> social organizational features <strong>of</strong><br />

family life that contribute to intimacy also<br />

facilitate the occurrence <strong>of</strong> a high rate <strong>of</strong><br />

intrarpousal violence.<br />

Though the family shares certain characteristics vith<br />

other social groups, as a social group and as an institution<br />

the family has distinctive characteristics. Gellss and<br />

Straus (1978) have catalogued certain <strong>of</strong> these features <strong>of</strong><br />

the tamily to point out that a spscial theory <strong>of</strong> violence is<br />

necessary for the family group. Some <strong>of</strong> these<br />

characteristics, and others mentioned by authors in this<br />

volume, serve a dual role in the family. on the ore hand,<br />

they contribute toward making the family a warm, supportive,<br />

and mtimate environment: on the other hand. they suggest<br />

reasons why this social group may be especially prone to<br />

violence. we llst eleven such factors:<br />

1. ILmf RJ&. An elexentary characteristic<br />

accounting for the high incidecce <strong>of</strong> violence is that so<br />

many hours or the day are spent ints1actir.g with other<br />

family members. Bowever. although this factor is important,<br />

the ratio <strong>of</strong> intrafamily violence to violence experienced<br />

outside the family far exceeds the ratio <strong>of</strong> time spent in<br />

the family to time spent outside. Comparing the fariily with<br />

another group in which large amounts <strong>of</strong> time are spent, such<br />

as a work group, provides a concrete example that far more<br />

is involved than "time at risk."


ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Pege 16<br />

take place over which a dispute or a failurs to meet<br />

expectations can occur.<br />

3. in tens it^ <strong>of</strong> Involpg~Emyg;. Not only is there a vlder<br />

range <strong>of</strong> possibilities for disputes or d5ssatisfactione.<br />

but, in addition. the degree <strong>of</strong> injury felt in such<br />

instances is likely to be much greater than if the ssme<br />

issue were to arise ir relation to someone outside the<br />

family. Foss (Chapter 7) considers this factor especially<br />

important in generating hostility among family members as<br />

well as in creating the strategies for dealing vith<br />

conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest among family members.<br />

4. Infrmqinca Activities. nany family activities hzre<br />

a 'zero sum" aspect. Conflict arises from such decisions as<br />

whether to play Bach or neodelssohn on the family stsreo,<br />

vhother to go to a movie or bowling, or how to line up for<br />

the bathroom. Less obvious, but equally important, is the<br />

infringing <strong>of</strong> one's perscnal space or self-image by the life<br />

style and habits <strong>of</strong> others in the family, such as those who<br />

leave things around versus those who put everything away, or<br />

those who eat quickly and those who like leisurely meals.<br />

6. and a g~~~g?&?;&s. <strong>The</strong> differences in age<br />

and sex <strong>of</strong> family members (especially during the<br />

child-rearino ~ . vearsl . couoled vith the existence ~ . <strong>of</strong><br />

A ~ ~ ~ - - . . .--- -~ ~ -<br />

~~ ~<br />

oenerational and sex dlfferences in culture and outlook.<br />

make the famlly an arena <strong>of</strong> culture confllct. Thlr confllc+<br />

1s expressed zn such phrases as "the battle <strong>of</strong> the sexes"<br />

and "the generatron gap."<br />

7. Bscrzbed Roles. Compounding the problem <strong>of</strong> age and<br />

sex differences, family statuses and roles are assigned, to<br />

a considerable extent. on the basis <strong>of</strong> biological<br />

characteristics rather than on the basis <strong>of</strong> competence and<br />

interest. One aspect <strong>of</strong> this structuring has traditionally<br />

been a focus <strong>of</strong> contenrion -- socially structured sexual<br />

inequality or. in contemporary language, the sexist<br />

organization <strong>of</strong> the family. Straus (Chapter 6) argues that<br />

a male-dominated family has especially high confllct<br />

potential when it exists in a society with an egalitarian<br />

ideology. But, as Bllen and Straus point out (in Chapter<br />

12). even without such an ideologic inconsistency, the<br />

confllct potential is high, because inevitably not all<br />

husbands can fulfill the culturally prescribed leadership<br />

roles.


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 17<br />

8. PaptQ privm. In many societies the normative<br />

kinship and household structure insulates the family from<br />

social controls and social assistance in cnp?.cg with<br />

intratamily conflict. This characreristic is most typical<br />

<strong>of</strong> the conjugal family system <strong>of</strong> urban-industrial sociities<br />

(Laslett. 1973). Both Foss (Chapter 8) and ADtaling (Chapter<br />

9) mention that the nuclear structure <strong>of</strong> America? family<br />

life makes less likely the dampening effects <strong>of</strong> the presence<br />

<strong>of</strong> third parties in husband-wife arguments and disputes.<br />

9. Involuntary pembershiz. Birth relationships ars<br />

obviously irivOluztary, and under-age children cannot<br />

:hemselves terminate such relationships. In addition, Sprey<br />

(1969) shows that the conjugal relationship also has<br />

nonvoluntary aspects. First, the social expectatlon is that<br />

marrlage is a long-term commitment, as Expressad in the<br />

phrase "until death do us part.' In addition, emotional,<br />

material. and legal rewards and constraints <strong>of</strong>ten make<br />

membership in the family group inescapable, socially.<br />

physically, or legally. So. when conflicts and<br />

dissatisfactions arise, the alternative <strong>of</strong> resolving them by<br />

leavlng <strong>of</strong>ten aces not exist--at least in the perception <strong>of</strong><br />

what is practical or possible.<br />

L - ~ ---- -~ ~<br />

~ ~ ~~ -<br />

famiiy life cycle--the birth <strong>of</strong> children, naturation <strong>of</strong><br />

children. auina. and retireaert. ~ ~<br />

. - -. <strong>The</strong> crisis-like nature <strong>of</strong><br />

these changes has long been recognized (Lellasters,1957).<br />

A11 <strong>of</strong> this, combined with the huge emotional investment<br />

typical <strong>of</strong> family relatiorships, means that the family is<br />

likely to be the locus <strong>of</strong> more, and more serious. stresses<br />

than other groups. A reading <strong>of</strong> Chapter 7 emphasizes the<br />

important role <strong>of</strong> internal and external stresses on family<br />

life in the explanation <strong>of</strong> intrafamily violence.<br />

11. Extensive KnoYledue <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> BiO9raEhies. Because<br />

<strong>of</strong> the intimacy <strong>of</strong> the marital relationship, spouses usually<br />

have an in-depth kowladge <strong>of</strong> each other's social<br />

histories--their abilities and shortcomings, their strengths<br />

and vulnerabilities, their likes and dislikes (Hepburn<br />

1973). In effect, the members <strong>of</strong> marital relationships have<br />

at their disposal icformation that can be used to support<br />

and enhancs each other's identities because each knows about<br />

the things that matter tc the other. A t the same time, this<br />

information can be used to damage the identity <strong>of</strong> sither<br />

spouse. Goode (1971) suggests that intimates are able to<br />

launch verbal assaults on the partner's vulnerable points<br />

because the nature <strong>of</strong> marriage exposes each other's<br />

weaknesses. Hotaling (Chapter 9) specifically refers to the


Ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Eage 18<br />

extensive knowledge <strong>of</strong> partners as predisposing them to<br />

mistakenly attribute malevolent intent to the actions <strong>of</strong><br />

spouses.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se eleven factors, and no doubt there are others,<br />

have been posited by authors <strong>of</strong> this volume and others as<br />

important characteristics <strong>of</strong> the family group that<br />

contribute to its high rate <strong>of</strong> conflict and violencs. <strong>The</strong><br />

irony here is that many <strong>of</strong> these same features encourage<br />

wzrmth, ictimacy, and support; for example, large amounts<br />

<strong>of</strong> time spent fogether, deep emotional involvement, privacy.<br />

and in-depth knowledge <strong>of</strong> one another.<br />

<strong>The</strong> relationship <strong>of</strong> these unique characteristics <strong>of</strong> the<br />

family to violence as well as to iltimacy has not been<br />

verified anpirically. But the theoretical work on this<br />

issue suggests many intriguing questions for research. <strong>The</strong><br />

most ieportant is whether changes in thase unique<br />

characteristics can make the family a less violent group<br />

without Sacrificing the benefits <strong>of</strong> an intimate arvironment.<br />

For example, if we compare nuclear family str.ucturPs with<br />

various combinations <strong>of</strong> extended family structures, w i l l<br />

there be a higher level <strong>of</strong> support between members ic the<br />

latter form, as well as a lower level <strong>of</strong> stress? Straus'<br />

comparison <strong>of</strong> supportiveness between spouses iii cuclear and<br />

in joint households (1975) suggests that lack <strong>of</strong> stress may<br />

be gained at the expense <strong>of</strong> support. And vhat role do<br />

grandparents play in terms <strong>of</strong> the occurrence <strong>of</strong> intrafamily<br />

violence when they live in the same households as the:: sons<br />

and daughters and grandchildren?<br />

Investigations also could<br />

characteristics. For example,<br />

be focused on specific<br />

the affect <strong>of</strong> involuntary<br />

family membership on violence could be studied by comparing<br />

nonmarried couples who live together with married csuples,<br />

controlling<br />

involveoent<br />

for relevant<br />

among family<br />

factors. ~lso. does intense<br />

members facilitate or dampen the<br />

occurrence <strong>of</strong> violence? Do families vith extensive<br />

community interests or involvements vith nonfamily members<br />

experience less violent interaction at home? <strong>The</strong>se Examples<br />

suggest<br />

brcuqht<br />

c.lg a Lev<br />

tc bear on<br />

ct .any such questloris :hat c3uld h~<br />

*he specla1 socral ?rger:zaczcnal<br />

teecmre? acd yhe~r irtluerce on tuebana-vlfe s:olcnce.<br />

Irony 3. <strong>The</strong> change to an egalitarian structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> marriage leads to the destruction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sexist organization <strong>of</strong> the family but also<br />

seems to lead to higher rates <strong>of</strong> violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> two previous ironies could be called =cz_c ironies.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y were concerned vith the family as an institution an6<br />

social group es compared vith other institutions and social<br />

groups in terms <strong>of</strong> its proclivity to high rates <strong>of</strong> violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> last two ironies presented here are concerned with micro<br />

level processes, that is, with characteristics that


ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 19<br />

differeatiate families frcm each other.<br />

<strong>The</strong> tirst <strong>of</strong> these ironies concerns the sexisr<br />

orgalization <strong>of</strong> the family (Straus, Chapter 6). This<br />

chapter cites nine specific ways in which the male-dominated<br />

srructure <strong>of</strong> society and family creates and aaintairs a high<br />

level Of marital <strong>Violence</strong>. one <strong>of</strong> the many ways in which<br />

male domination can lead to family violence occurs when the<br />

male is threatened. In our society, where male-superiority<br />

norms are in the process <strong>of</strong> transition, and in vhich the<br />

presnmption <strong>of</strong> superiority must be validated hy "resources"<br />

such as valued personal traits and material goods and<br />

services, ascribing superlor authority to men is a potent<br />

force ic producing physical attacks on wives. R husband who<br />

wants to be the dominant person in his family, but who has<br />

little education, a joh that is low in prestige and income.<br />

and a lack ot interpersocel skills, may resort to physical<br />

violence to maintarn his position.<br />

Empirical evidence supports these notions. For<br />

example, in families where the husband's achieved status is<br />

lower than hls wife's. O'Brien (1971) found a greater<br />

tendency to USE force and viol~nce on family members than<br />

when the husband had the -resourcem <strong>of</strong> a higher prestige<br />

occupation. Also, Allen and Straus (Chapter 12) found that<br />

among working class husbands who were high in economic or<br />

personal resources, there vas no correlation between power<br />

in the family and violence. However, among those working<br />

class husbands who were low in resources, the correlation<br />

between male power and violence was 0.49.<br />

Tho expectation <strong>of</strong> male doninance is graphically<br />

illustrated in the case history <strong>of</strong> Joe and Jennifer reported<br />

by La Xossa (il Chapter 10). This couple depicrs the<br />

conflict between men and women ix the transition from male<br />

superlority to egalitarian family norms, most clear in the<br />

dialogue betweel Joe and Jennifer about Jennifer's<br />

employment outside <strong>of</strong> the home, which Joe perceives as a<br />

threat to his right to he the dominant asmber <strong>of</strong> the<br />

relationship.<br />

<strong>The</strong> issue <strong>of</strong> wife employment, perhaps the most direct<br />

threaz to male domination in the society as well as in the<br />

family, is investigated by Brown (Chapter 11) to determine<br />

its potential impact on husband-wife violerce. In a more<br />

general vein. Brown probes the conflict between emerging<br />

sexual equality and existing norms that promote male<br />

domination.<br />

Logically, we would assume that the increasing<br />

breakdown <strong>of</strong> the sexist organization <strong>of</strong> the family would<br />

also lead to a decrease in husband-wife violence. But this<br />

decrease may not be the case, at least in the short run.<br />

During this transition period, as the family restructures<br />

its power distrrbution, conflict and violence may actually


Ch.1. Soclal Structure and Irony Pagp 20<br />

be increased as men feel threatened by their loss <strong>of</strong> power.<br />

Ironically. attempts bq women to increase their power =n<br />

society and the family map serve to victimize vomen further.<br />

at least temporarily.<br />

In the long run, the egalitarian ethos should lead to a<br />

decrease in husband-wife violence, hut it w i l l not decrease<br />

until men begin to accept egalitarian norms as legitimate.<br />

Irony 4. <strong>The</strong> suppression <strong>of</strong> conflict, widely<br />

felt to decrease violence, may actually<br />

increase it.<br />

Most people fear conflict and try to aroid it.<br />

sociologists and psychologists do research to find out why<br />

conflict occurs, ostensibly to be able to provide<br />

information that w i l l enable people to aroid conflict.<br />

narriage counselors and other pr<strong>of</strong>essionals concerned with<br />

the family, with a few exceptions such as Bach and Uyden<br />

(1968) and ShOstroB and Ravanaugh (1971), focus much <strong>of</strong><br />

their efforts on helping families to avoid conflict. <strong>The</strong><br />

implicit assumption hera is that the suppression <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

w i l l lead to the avoidance <strong>of</strong> hostility and violence.<br />

Houever, conflict theorists have presentea a convincing<br />

case for exactly the opposite assumption; they argue that<br />

conflict is an inevitable part <strong>of</strong> all human associations<br />

(coser, 1956; Dahrendorf, 1959; Sprey, 1969).<br />

Furthermore. they hold that any social unit that attempts to<br />

suppress conflict runs a high risk <strong>of</strong> collapsing, either<br />

because it falls to adapt to changing circu~stances or<br />

hecause hostility accumulates, eroding group solidarity.<br />

<strong>The</strong> application <strong>of</strong> ccnfllct theory to famlly vrolence<br />

has assumed that conflict 1s central to. or even 2<br />

prerequlslte for, vlolencc. Hoverer, a dellneation <strong>of</strong> how<br />

conillct processes and violence are related has not received<br />

adequate attentlon. Poss takes on thls Issue ln Chapter 8.<br />

Foss contends that certain unique features <strong>of</strong> the<br />

husband-wife relationship, that is. the high frequency <strong>of</strong><br />

interaction betveen spouses. total personality involvement.<br />

and the difficulty in simply leaving the family setting.<br />

vlll produce hostility and generate attsmpts to suppress or<br />

ignore hostility. Foss proposEs that the avoidance <strong>of</strong><br />

conflict SiTuationS ironically tends to increase hostility<br />

as well as the probability <strong>of</strong> violence.<br />

If suppressing conflict leads to violence does this<br />

imply the more conflict the better for marital<br />

relatiocships? <strong>The</strong> question <strong>of</strong> how much conflict is<br />

desirable in the family setting is an uninvestigated and<br />

important Empirical challenge. Straus (1978) has suggested<br />

that rhere is a curvilinear relation between the amount <strong>of</strong><br />

conflict and group vell-being. That is. the absence <strong>of</strong>


ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 21<br />

conflict in the sense <strong>of</strong> conflicts <strong>of</strong> isterest is<br />

theoretically impcssible; Ever if it could be brought<br />

about, suppression <strong>of</strong> conflict vould he fatal for group<br />

vell-being. A t the same time, very high levels <strong>of</strong> cocflict<br />

can create such a high level <strong>of</strong> strass and/or such rapid<br />

chazqe that group welfare is adversely affected.<br />

If Strzus' hypothesis is correct, that a certain level<br />

<strong>of</strong> conflict is indeed healthy for the marital relationship,<br />

how could the appropriate level be determined? Certain<br />

the~retical deductions made by authors in this voluae and<br />

Others suggest that the level <strong>of</strong> intimacy in marital<br />

relationships may be a key factor in the amount <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

experienced. Several writers have suggested that the more<br />

intimate the relationship, the more likely the occurrence <strong>of</strong><br />

conflict (Simme1.1950; Sprey, 1969; Poss. Chapter 8:<br />

Hotaling, Chapter 9: Brown, Chapter 11). Pertaps, then,<br />

the lessening <strong>of</strong> those factors that contribute to intimacy<br />

also would reduce the amount <strong>of</strong> conflict in the family.<br />

Possc chapter points ont that in some patterns <strong>of</strong><br />

family life a less intimate or intensivs involvement is<br />

characteristic. Rn excellent case in point is dsscribed by<br />

CubPr and Har<strong>of</strong>f (1965) as the "passive-congenial' marriage.<br />

a llfe-style in vhich all emotional and irstrusental<br />

satisfaction does not derive from the marriage. In<br />

marriages in which creative energies and the satisfaction <strong>of</strong><br />

interests and emotional needs are nlt exclusively directed<br />

to the marital relationship, less intensive patterns <strong>of</strong><br />

invclvement w i l l occur. whether this type <strong>of</strong> marriaqs also<br />

has less conflict and violence is a crucial question for the<br />

study <strong>of</strong> family violence.<br />

TRE IRONIC NATURE OF INTRAPAIILY VIOLEBCE<br />

when this book was planned, the key ideas to be<br />

presented were those <strong>of</strong> culture and social organization. As<br />

the book began to come together, what might be called the<br />

"ironic nature cf intrafamily violence" Kept cropping up.<br />

This in no way undercuts the importance <strong>of</strong> culture and<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Organization as interpretive tools. <strong>The</strong> dual<br />

influence <strong>of</strong> culture and social organization is described<br />

throughout this volume as revealing the socially patterned<br />

nature cf marital violence. <strong>The</strong> individual personalities <strong>of</strong><br />

married ccuples, without considering ths socially<br />

constructed nature <strong>of</strong> marriage itself. cannot explain why<br />

marriage as a social institution is characterized by a high<br />

incidence <strong>of</strong> strife and violerce.<br />

But culture a d social organization are somewhat static<br />

notions and do not force one to face up to the dynamic and<br />

emergent nature <strong>of</strong> fzmily interaction. In the context <strong>of</strong><br />

this book, it is largely the notion <strong>of</strong> irony that provides<br />

the needed dynamic. we have pointed to four such ironias.


ch.1. <strong>Social</strong> Structure and Irony Page 22<br />

~t first glance, these ronies nay seem to b? only amusing<br />

oddities. <strong>The</strong>y are more than this. Recognizing the<br />

existence <strong>of</strong> these Counterintuitive processes Sensitizes US<br />

to the complexity <strong>of</strong> family life. Ironies highlight the<br />

fundamental contradictions that reside ir the cultural rorms<br />

and social organization <strong>of</strong> family life. <strong>The</strong>y force us to be<br />

wary <strong>of</strong> accepting the conmon-sense dichotomy between vice<br />

and virtue, good and evil. love and conflict.<br />

Lastly, the exploration <strong>of</strong> ironic rel+tiooships<br />

naturally leads to the opening up <strong>of</strong> new and interosting<br />

research questions on family violence. Rather than viewirg<br />

irony as residing in nature or in the universe, a<br />

sociological perspective cn irony leads us to focus on the<br />

complex relationship betueen culture and social organization<br />

and its role in gereratirg the contradictions <strong>of</strong> family<br />

life.<br />

NOTES<br />

*Part <strong>of</strong> the first section <strong>of</strong> this chapter is a revised<br />

v~TSi0n <strong>of</strong> part <strong>of</strong> "cnltural and <strong>Social</strong> Organizational<br />

Influences on violence Eetveen Family nembers" (straus<br />

1974b).<br />

1. For the purposes <strong>of</strong> this chapter, violence is<br />

defined. following Gelles and Straus (1978). as "sn act<br />

carried out with +he intection <strong>of</strong>, or perceived as having<br />

the intention <strong>of</strong>, physically hurting another person." <strong>The</strong><br />

"physical hurt" can range from slight pain, as in a slap, to<br />

murdsr. Blthough this is the basic definition <strong>of</strong> violence<br />

used in Our research, it is usually also necessary -0 take<br />

into account a number <strong>of</strong>. othar charactcristlcs <strong>of</strong> the<br />

viol5nt act. such as whether it is ~instrumeotal" *o some<br />

other purpose or "expressive," that is, an 2nd in itself;<br />

apd whether it is a culturally permitted or require3 act<br />

versus one that runs counter tc cultural norms (legitimate<br />

versus illegitimate violence). Thus, the blsis fcr th3<br />

*sintext to hurt" may range from a concerr for a child's<br />

safety (as when a child is spanked for going into the<br />

street) to hostility so intense that the death <strong>of</strong> ths other<br />

is desired. <strong>The</strong> former woold be an example <strong>of</strong> "legitimate<br />

instrunental violence" and the latter <strong>of</strong> "illegitisats<br />

expressive violecce."<br />

2. Certain aothors in this volume. e-g.. Po55 (Chapter<br />

7). use the term paradox rather than irony. As natza (1969)<br />

notes: there are two meanings <strong>of</strong> paradox-the general and<br />

the technical. Ir the general meaning <strong>of</strong> paradox, soneChing<br />

can be both paradox (a texet contrary t3 commcn sense) end<br />

irony (an outcome <strong>of</strong> evlnts that mocks the fitness <strong>of</strong><br />

things). Poss uses the tern paradox ir the general an3 20:<br />

the technrcal sense <strong>of</strong> ar apparrnt Internal ccntradict;on.


Chapter 2<br />

<strong>Wife</strong>-Beating: How Common and Why?<br />

Murray A. Straus<br />

Chapter 1 made frequent references to th;<br />

high rates <strong>of</strong> physical violence that<br />

characterize Rmericzn marriages. until<br />

recently. sucb assertions had to be based on<br />

impressionistic evidence, or on studies <strong>of</strong> small<br />

or perhaps unrepresentative samples, f3r<br />

example, couples seeking divorce or couples<br />

involved in "domestic disturbance" police calls.<br />

Th=s chapter. however. reports the results <strong>of</strong> 3<br />

study <strong>of</strong> a large and nationally representative<br />

Sample <strong>of</strong> couples. <strong>The</strong> rates <strong>of</strong> violencs found<br />

in this study were somewhat lower than expectad<br />

for "ordiParyn pushing, slapping, and shoring,<br />

but astoundingly high for "wife-briting." In<br />

addition to documenting the high incidence <strong>of</strong><br />

wife-beating, data on "husband-beating" is also<br />

presented and "sed to illustrate a recurring<br />

thene <strong>of</strong> this volume: that marital violencc<br />

cannot be ucderstood in terms <strong>of</strong> a single factor<br />

such as sexrsm. aggressiveness, lack <strong>of</strong><br />

self-control, or mental illness <strong>of</strong> husbands who<br />

beat fhelr wives. Each <strong>of</strong> these factors is<br />

importan: but does not acc0ur.t f3r the rate 3f<br />

assault by vives OP husbands.<br />

<strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> first objectlve <strong>of</strong> this chapter is to present some<br />

t h findings ~ on violence between spouses from a recently<br />

completed study <strong>of</strong> American couples. <strong>The</strong>se findicgs are<br />

unique. being the first such data on a natiolally<br />

represectative sample. Although the findings have<br />

limitatiocs, they give a+ least some indication af the<br />

extent to vhlch wife-beatirg is part <strong>of</strong> the way <strong>of</strong> life <strong>of</strong><br />

American fanilies.*l


Ch.2. if e-~eatir.9 Page 24<br />

<strong>The</strong> secord objective is to explain furthsr the irony<br />

that the group to which most people look for love and<br />

gentl~n~ss is also the most violent civilian<br />

society.<br />

group in our<br />

he first <strong>of</strong> these objectivas poses tremendous<br />

technzcal problems. <strong>The</strong> secora objective, in addition to<br />

the technical probless, poses theoretical problems<br />

fundamental to our understanding <strong>of</strong> human society.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, what follows should be taken as highly teLtative.<br />

beginning ansvers to these questions.<br />

Dat+ w i l l be presented on a sample <strong>of</strong> 2.143 couples.<br />

This sample was chose? in a way that makes it exrremely<br />

likely that they are representative <strong>of</strong> all American couples.<br />

<strong>The</strong> age, race, and socioeconomic status <strong>of</strong> the couples in<br />

the sampl? correspond quite closely with census data for' the<br />

nation as a whole. So fer 50 good. But what about the data<br />

on wife-beating?<br />

WEAT IS RIPE-BEATING?<br />

TO do research on the incidence <strong>of</strong> wife-beating, one<br />

must be able to defire it in a way that can be measured<br />

objectively. one soon realizss that "wife-heating' is a<br />

political rather than a scientific term. For most people.<br />

wlfe-beating refers only to those instances In which severe<br />

damage is inflicted. Othor violence is treated as normal or<br />

laughed <strong>of</strong>f with remarks such as "Hornen should ba struck<br />

regularly, like gorgs." or take the following:<br />

concord, N.R. (AP) <strong>The</strong> New Rampshire CDmmission<br />

on the Status <strong>of</strong> Women has rejpcted a plan '0<br />

help battered wises, saying that wife-bsatinq is<br />

caused by the rise <strong>of</strong> feminism.<br />

"Those women libbers irritate the hell out <strong>of</strong><br />

thelr husbands,' said Commissioner Gloria Belzil<br />

<strong>of</strong> Aashua.<br />

A t a meeting acnday, commission members,<br />

appointed by GOV. neldrim Thornson, said any<br />

program to help battered wives would be "an<br />

ilrasion <strong>of</strong> privacy.*# (Portsaouth Berala, Sept.<br />

13. 1977)<br />

This statement suggssts that a certain amount <strong>of</strong><br />

vlolence in the family is "normal violerce" in the sense<br />

that it is deserved (for example by "irritating the hell"<br />

out <strong>of</strong> one's spouse) and that, contrary to its pcsltion on<br />

ViolencE outsid4 the family, the state should not intarfsre.


Ch.2. Rite-Beating Psge 25<br />

A rscEnt conversation with a student who had deciaed to<br />

do a term paper on violence in the family suggests the same<br />

conclusion. She came to see me tor help on how to rirrow<br />

the toplc ?o something manageable. I suggested chat she<br />

could choOR tc concentrate Or husband-wife violence,<br />

parent-child violence, or riolenca between th= children in a<br />

tamily. She was astoulded at the last possibility and said<br />

"Well, I never thought <strong>of</strong> my brother hirting me as<br />

violenC4.' <strong>The</strong>re seems to be an rmplicit, taken-for-granted<br />

cultural norm that makes zt legitimate far family members to<br />

hit each other.<br />

A t what point does one exceed the bounds <strong>of</strong> "ordinary"<br />

marital violence? When does it become "wife-beating?" To<br />

solre this problem that Richard Gelles and I took for our<br />

research, we gathered data on a continuum <strong>of</strong> violent acts,<br />

ranging from a push to usi2g a kLifs or gun. This allows<br />

readers to draw the line at whatever place seems most<br />

appropriate for their purpose.<br />

EEASURING WIPE-BEATIBG<br />

But this "solution" can alsobe a means <strong>of</strong> avoiding the<br />

issue. So besides data or each violent act, tie combinEd the<br />

most severe acts into what can be called a "severe siolsnce<br />

index" or, for purposes <strong>of</strong> this chapter. a "<strong>Wife</strong>-Bs~ting<br />

Icdex."<br />

<strong>The</strong> Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS) were used to gather<br />

these data (Straus. 1979). <strong>The</strong>se scales provide data on how<br />

family menbers attempt to deal with conflicts between<br />

themselves. <strong>The</strong> Physlcal violence Index <strong>of</strong> the CTS contains<br />

the following eight items:<br />

K. Throwing things at the spouss<br />

I. Pushing, shoving, or grabbing<br />

8. Slapping<br />

N. Kicking, biting, or htttirg with the fist<br />

0. R i t or tried to hit with something<br />

. P- . Beat uo<br />

Q. Threatened wzth a kclfe or gun<br />

R. Used a knlfe or gun<br />

<strong>The</strong> overall <strong>Violence</strong> Index consists <strong>of</strong> the extenr to<br />

which aLy <strong>of</strong> these acts were carried out during the prroious<br />

12 months. <strong>The</strong> <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating Index consists <strong>of</strong> the extfnt to<br />

which acts N through R occurred.


~h.2, <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating Page 27<br />

<strong>The</strong> choice <strong>of</strong> acts N throogh R as ths <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating<br />

1ndeZ does not reflect cur conception <strong>of</strong> what is permirsibl4<br />

~iolence. I find nsne <strong>of</strong> these to be acceptablr for<br />

batueen any human beings, ircluding parent acd<br />

child, brother and sister, husband and wife, student and<br />

t~acber. minister and parishioner, or collaaguss in a<br />

depattment. In short, I follow the maxim coined by Jchn<br />

valusex: "Peopl~ sre 10t for hittiog.'<br />

What, then, is the basis for selecting items R through<br />

R to make up the <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating Index? 1tia simply the'.<br />

thes9 are all acts that cerry vith them a high' risk <strong>of</strong><br />

serious physical injury to the victim. With thesD<br />

consideratiors in mind. WE can turn to the questioc <strong>of</strong><br />

tryiog to estimate the extert <strong>of</strong> uifs-beating in the united<br />

states.<br />

TEE SXTENT OF YIPP-BEATIAG<br />

<strong>The</strong> procedures for epasuring violsnce just described<br />

were used i~ a study <strong>of</strong> a nationally representative sampls<br />

<strong>of</strong> American families, made possible by a grant from NIMs. ?.<br />

probability sample <strong>of</strong> 2.1Y3 families was studiea. In<br />

approximately half the cases the person providing the<br />

informati<strong>of</strong> was a woman and ir half a mar. To be eligible<br />

for iPcluSion in the study, the respondent had to b? cra<br />

member <strong>of</strong> a male-female coupla, aged 18 to 70. <strong>The</strong> ccuple<br />

did not have to hare children, nor did they have to be<br />

legally married. Our sample contains couples vith and<br />

wiThout children, ar.d married and unmarried couples in about<br />

the sane proportion as are found in the population.<br />

---<br />

YEarlp Incidence. <strong>The</strong> most direct, but in some ways<br />

also a misleading, statistic emerging from ths data an the<br />

Z.lU3 couples ir our sample is thet. for the 12-morth period<br />

preceding the ixterview. 3.8 percent <strong>of</strong> the respondents<br />

reported one or mcre physical attacks that fall ucd?r our<br />

operational dstinition <strong>of</strong> wife-beating. Applying this<br />

iCcidenCe rate to the appraxieately 47 millicn couples in<br />

the united States means that, in any one year. approximately<br />

1.8 million wives are beaten by their hushanas.<br />

I mentioned that this can be a misleading figure; two<br />

other facts must be considered: how <strong>of</strong>ten thes? beatings<br />

Occur. and how they fit in with thn overall pattern <strong>of</strong><br />

violence in the family.


~h.2. <strong>Wife</strong>-Beatirg Page 29<br />

-- FreMgEy @rlX ths Ygs. Among those couples in<br />

which a beating occurred, it vas typically not an isolated<br />

instance, as can be seen from the "Frequency* colums <strong>of</strong><br />

able 1. Kovever,<br />

overstates the case;<br />

the mean frequency <strong>of</strong> occurrence<br />

ir a few cases violence was almost a<br />

daily Or weekly event. For this reason, the median gives a<br />

more realistic picture <strong>of</strong> the typical frequency <strong>of</strong> vi~lence<br />

in the violent families. This is 2.4, that. is, th? typ%ca1<br />

pattern is cver two sericus assaults per year. But <strong>of</strong><br />

course there is great variarior. For about a third 3f +he<br />

couples who reported an act that falls in our categxy cf<br />

wife-beating, beating occurred cnly once during tho year.<br />

A t the other E X ~ ~ E Bwere P cases in which it Occurred once a<br />

week Or more <strong>of</strong>ten. About 19 percent reported two beatings<br />

during the year. 16 percan' repcrted three or four, and 32<br />

percent reported five or more.<br />

A more literal interpretation <strong>of</strong> the data can he<br />

Obtalned frcm looking at the figures in Table 1 for each<br />

type <strong>of</strong> violent act. Whec the category <strong>of</strong> "wife-beating" is<br />

restrlcted to those who used the term *'beat up" to describe<br />

vhat happmed (item P), the figure is 1.1 percent, with a<br />

median <strong>of</strong> 1.7 beatings per year. while this is much lower<br />

than the 3.8 percent figure that takes into account all the<br />

severe violent acts, it still represents over half a million<br />

families.<br />

Duration <strong>of</strong> fiarriaae Rates. Inother aspect <strong>of</strong><br />

vife-heating that must be considered is the proporlio~ <strong>of</strong><br />

families in which a beatirg occurred. Onfortunltely,<br />

our data for events before the year <strong>of</strong> the survey aa not<br />

identify the assailent and the victim. 111 that can be<br />

reported is that 28 percent <strong>of</strong> the couples in the study<br />

experienced at least ore violent incident and 5.3 percent<br />

experienced violence that we consider a beating.<br />

IG some <strong>of</strong> there cases. it was a single slap or a<br />

Single beatlng. Bouever. there are several reasons vhy even<br />

a single beating is important. First, in my values, even<br />

or.€ such event is intrinsically a debasement <strong>of</strong> human life.<br />

S~cond. physical darger is involved. Third, many, if not<br />

most. such beatings are part cf a famlly power struggle.<br />

Often only one or two slaps fix the balance <strong>of</strong> paver in a<br />

family for many years--or perhaps for a lifetime.<br />

Physical force is the ultimate resource that most <strong>of</strong> us<br />

learn as children to rely on if a11 else fails and the issue<br />

15 crucial. As the husband in the family describea in<br />

Chapter ID said when asked why he hit his wife during an<br />

argument:<br />

... She more or less tried to run me and I said<br />

no. and she got hysterical and said. "1 conld<br />

kill you!" Lnd I got rather angry ar.d slapped<br />

he- in the face three or four times and I said


Ch.2. wife-Beating Page 30<br />

-Don't you ever say ?hat to me again!" And ur<br />

haven't had any problem slcce.<br />

Later in the interview, the husband evaluated his use<br />

<strong>of</strong> physical force as follows:<br />

You dor.lt use it until you are forced to it. A t<br />

that point I felt I had to do something physical<br />

to stop t h bad ~ progression <strong>of</strong> events. I took<br />

rn? chances vith that and it worked. In those<br />

circumstances my judgment was correct 3rd it<br />

worked.<br />

Since superior strength and size gives the advantage to men<br />

in such situations. the single beating may be an sxtremely<br />

important factor :n maictainicg male doainance in the family<br />

system.<br />

kccurax <strong>of</strong> Estimates. How much confidence ca' be<br />

placed in these figures? I am reasonably confident that the<br />

sample is representative <strong>of</strong> Rmerican cooples gen€rally. But<br />

that is only one aspect <strong>of</strong> the accuracy question. Tha other<br />

main aspect is whether our respondents "told all." BEre I<br />

have doubts tor the following reasons:<br />

1. Und~rrepOrti~g <strong>of</strong> domestic violence is likely to<br />

occur among two gzoups <strong>of</strong> people, for opposite reasons. For<br />

a large group, viclence is so much a normal part <strong>of</strong> the<br />

family system that a slap, push, or shove (and sometimes<br />

even more severs acts) is simply not noteworthy or dramatic<br />

enough to be remembered. Such omissions are Especially<br />

likely when we ask about evects over the entire length <strong>of</strong> a<br />

aarriage.<br />

2. Paradoxically, there is also underrepart:ng at the<br />

Other end <strong>of</strong> the vlolence cortinuum--those who ~xpsrlenced<br />

such severe violent acts as being bitten, hit vith objects.<br />

beaten up. or attacked with a knife or gun. ThEse acts go<br />

beyond the "normal vlolence' <strong>of</strong> family life. Such acts are<br />

admitted reluctantly, because <strong>of</strong> the shame involved if one<br />

is the victim, or the guilt if one is the attacker.<br />

3. I final reason for regarding thsse figusrs as<br />

drastic underestinates lies in the naturE <strong>of</strong> our sample.<br />

Since a major purpose <strong>of</strong> the study was to investigate the<br />

~xtePt to which violence is related to other aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

hushand-wife interaction. we sampled only couples living<br />

together. Divorced persons were asked only about the<br />

current marrlage (again because <strong>of</strong> inte:riew tims limits and<br />

rrcall accuracy problems). Since "excessive' violence is a<br />

major cause <strong>of</strong> divorce, and since our sample is limitEd to<br />

couples living together, these data probably oait many <strong>of</strong><br />

the high violence cases.


Ch.2. wife-Beating Page 31<br />

<strong>The</strong>se Corsideration~, plus the higher rates in our<br />

pilot studies and informal evidence (where scne <strong>of</strong> the<br />

factors lsading to underreporting were less) suggest that<br />

the true incidence for violence in a marriage is probably<br />

closer to 50 91 60 wrcert <strong>of</strong> g.&& c=&~ than it is to the<br />

28 percent who were willing to describe violent acts in a<br />

Bass interview survey.<br />

WIPE-BELTING IS NOT RESTRICTED TO WIVES<br />

Although this chapter is concerned primarily with<br />

vife-beating, an adequate understanding <strong>of</strong> the phenomsron<br />

requires that we cocsider it ir a wider context. We must<br />

reCogPize that one does not have to be married to be the<br />

victin <strong>of</strong> physical violence by a partner. Our national<br />

survey (Pllo and Straos, 1980) a stody by iiennon (1976) <strong>of</strong><br />

students living together, and much informal evidence suggest<br />

that couples who are no? married have rates <strong>of</strong> viclonc? that<br />

are as high or hlqher than those married. In fact, cloplee<br />

do not have to live together. Once a step is taker touerd a<br />

aarriage-like arrangement. as in a boyfriana-girlfriend<br />

relationship, and Especially if regular sex is involved. the<br />

violence rate jumps dramatically. <strong>Violence</strong> can no longer be<br />

figured i~ the rates per 100,000 characteristic <strong>of</strong> assaults<br />

ir general. Instead, simple percentages, that is, rates per<br />

hundred, are more logical. Why this happens is imporlant in<br />

itself and also because it throws a great deal <strong>of</strong> light on<br />

the situation <strong>of</strong> wives.<br />

ADSBAND BEATING<br />

NOW we come to findings that may be surprising to some<br />

readers. <strong>The</strong> rational sample data confirm what all <strong>of</strong> our<br />

pilot studies have shown (Gelles. 1974; Steinmetz, 1977;<br />

Straus, 197Y): that violence betveeo husband ar.d vife is<br />

not a one-way street. Thr old cartoons <strong>of</strong> the vife chasing<br />

a husband with a rolling pin or throwing pots and pans are<br />

closer to reality than most <strong>of</strong> us (especially those <strong>of</strong> us<br />

with feminist sympathies) realize. This can be seen froa an<br />

inspection <strong>of</strong> the wifc columns in Table 1.


Ch.2. Slfe-Beating<br />

the VOfd VlOleDCe.<br />

- - -- -- --<br />

Page 32<br />

Sg'cific piclent Acts. If we look at the specific<br />

typss <strong>of</strong> violent acts sampled by the CRT, there is eviaence<br />

-.- for +he .~~. ont and Dan L - ~ throvino ~ - ~ ~ stereatvoe. 2<br />

since the number <strong>of</strong><br />

L - -<br />

~ 2 . ~ .<br />

wives who threw things at their husbanas is almost twice as<br />

larqe as the number <strong>of</strong> husbazds who threv thinqs at the:=<br />

wives. For half <strong>of</strong> the violent acts, however, the rate is<br />

hioher far the husband: +he freouencv is hisher for the<br />

d~ -- A<br />

husbands in all but tva <strong>of</strong> the items. <strong>The</strong> biggest<br />

discrepancy in favor <strong>of</strong> wives occurs in kickirg and hittinq<br />

vith objects. Such acts are less depenaent on superior<br />

physical strength. This Eggma to support the viev<br />

important diff~rEncE between male and female<br />

that an<br />

domestic<br />

violence stens from the smaller size, weight, and muscle<br />

development <strong>of</strong> most women, rather than from any greater<br />

rejection <strong>of</strong> physlcal force on moral or normativ: grourias.<br />

Pollcy mulications. Althooqh these findings show high<br />

rates <strong>of</strong> violence uiq~, this fact shcula not divert<br />

attention from the need to aive urimarv attertion to wives<br />

as the imlaedlate focus <strong>of</strong> social policy.<br />

are a number <strong>of</strong> reasons tor this:<br />

<strong>The</strong>re<br />

(1) P oalidity study carried out in preparatiori for<br />

this research (Bulcr<strong>of</strong>t and straus, 1975) shows that<br />

underreporting <strong>of</strong> violence is greater for violecce by<br />

husbands than ir is for violence by wives. This is probably<br />

because the use <strong>of</strong> physical force is so much a part <strong>of</strong> the<br />

male way <strong>of</strong> llfe that it is typically rot the dramatic and<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten traumatic evect that the same act <strong>of</strong> violence is for a<br />

voman. To b~ violent is not unmasculine. But to be<br />

physically violent. & unfsminine according to contemporary<br />

Raericac standards. Consequently, if it were possible to<br />

allow for this difference in reporting rates, even ic simple<br />

~umerica.1 terms, wife-beating probably would be the more<br />

severe problem.<br />

(2) Wen if one does not take into accourt this<br />

differencs in unaerreporting, the data in Table 1 show that<br />

husbands have hlgher rates <strong>of</strong> the most dangerous and<br />

injurious forms <strong>of</strong> violence (beating-up and using 6 knife or<br />

gun).<br />

(3) Table 1 also shews that when violent acts are<br />

committed by a husband, they are repeated more <strong>of</strong>ten ?ha> is<br />

the case for wives.<br />

(4) <strong>The</strong>se data do not tell us what proportion <strong>of</strong> the<br />

violent acts by rives were in response to blows initiatea by<br />

husbands. Wolfgang's data on hosba~d-wife homicides (1957)<br />

suggest thet this is an important factor.


ch.2. uif %-Beating Page 33<br />

(5) <strong>The</strong> greater physical strength <strong>of</strong> men makes it more<br />

likely that a woman WLll be seriously injured when beeton up<br />

by her husbacd than the reverse.<br />

(6) A disproportionately large number <strong>of</strong> attacks by<br />

hnsbards seem to occur vhec the wife is pregnant (Gslles.<br />

1976). thus posing e danger to the as yet unborn child.<br />

(7) aomec are locked into marriage to a such greater<br />

extent than men. Because <strong>of</strong> a variety <strong>of</strong> economic and<br />

social constraicts, they <strong>of</strong>tor have no altarnative to<br />

putting up with beatings by their hnsbands (Gelles. 1976;<br />

Bartin, 1976; S?raus, 1976a. 1977b).<br />

In short, wives are victimized by violence in the<br />

family zo a ouch greater extert than are husbands and should<br />

therefore be the focus <strong>of</strong> the most immediate rsmedial steps.<br />

Bowever, these data also indicate that a fundamental<br />

solution to wife-beating cannot be restricted to she<br />

immediate problem <strong>of</strong> the assaulting husbands. Rather,<br />

violence is embeadPd in the structure <strong>of</strong> the society an6 the<br />

family system itself (straus, 1976~). Ths particularly<br />

brutal form <strong>of</strong> viclence known as wife-beating is likely to<br />

end only with a change in the cultural and sscial<br />

orgatizational facCors underlying parent-to-child,<br />

child-to-child, and wife-to-husband violence as ~€11. Some<br />

<strong>of</strong> the spacitic steps to accomplish this change are outlined<br />

in Chapter 13.<br />

TEE CRUSES OF RIPE-BEATING<br />

ROY I turn to the proposition that the causes <strong>of</strong><br />

vife-beating ars to be fonnd in the structure <strong>of</strong> American<br />

Society and its family system. Demonstrating this, svsn in<br />

. --- . .<br />

principle. is a vast undertaking. Indeed, ?ha: is whni this<br />

book is abcut.<br />

seven <strong>of</strong> the<br />

In - this chaoter. . I w i l l sim~lv idertifv<br />

maln fectors and *he aensrel tenor <strong>of</strong> the<br />

argoment. Plgure 1 glves an overvlsw <strong>of</strong> these factors and<br />

some <strong>of</strong> thelr rnterrelatlonships.<br />

3 combination <strong>of</strong> these factors (plus others cot<br />

diagrammea for lack <strong>of</strong> space) makes the family the most<br />

violent <strong>of</strong> all civilien institu5ions and accounts for that<br />

aspect <strong>of</strong> family violence which we call wife-beating. Let<br />

US look briefly at each <strong>of</strong> these factors.<br />

--<br />

1. <strong>of</strong> Ea&x s~9fLLgi. An essential<br />

starting point for any understanding <strong>of</strong> fasily violence is<br />

the hlgh level <strong>of</strong> conflict characteristic <strong>of</strong> families. In<br />

Chapter 1, eleven reascns for the typical high level <strong>of</strong><br />

coqflict within the family ware idsntlfied and briefly<br />

explained. <strong>The</strong>se reasors included the broad rarpa <strong>of</strong><br />

activities shared by family members, with consequert greater


Ch.2. Wifa-Beating Pag~ 34<br />

opportunity for cocflic's <strong>of</strong> interest than in more narrowly<br />

focused groups; the age and sex differences built irto the<br />

structure <strong>of</strong> the family: and the assigcment <strong>of</strong> fanily roles<br />

on the basis <strong>of</strong> age and sex rather than interest ard<br />

COP~S~~CCE. A l l four chapters in Part I11 consider varicus<br />

aspscts <strong>of</strong> intrafamily cocflict. Chapter 8 csnsiders thess<br />

co~flicts in the light <strong>of</strong> conflict thecry and the paradox <strong>of</strong><br />

'he love-hate relationship so <strong>of</strong>ten found il families.<br />

2% yZq& Level <strong>of</strong> <strong>Violence</strong> in f& socie; et <strong>The</strong> high<br />

level <strong>of</strong> conflict inherert in the family, coebrned vith the<br />

huge emotional investment typrcal <strong>of</strong> family reletionships.<br />

means that the family is likely to be the locus <strong>of</strong> acre, and<br />

more serious, conflicts than other groups. But confli-t and<br />

violence are not the same. Violcnce is only one olpans <strong>of</strong><br />

dealing with conflict. What accounts for the use <strong>of</strong><br />

violence to deal vith conflicts within the f~mily? A<br />

fundaaeotal starting place is that we are part <strong>of</strong> a violent<br />

society. <strong>The</strong>re is a carry-over from one sphEre <strong>of</strong> life to<br />

another. as I have triad to show in a paper comparing levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> family violence in different societies (Straus, 197la).<br />

However, granting the carry-over principle, this explanation<br />

is by no means sufficient. Conflict is 31sO high, fox<br />

example; in academic departmects. Eut there has never been<br />

an incident <strong>of</strong> physical violence ia any 3f the six<br />

departments in which I hare taught during the past 25 years.<br />

In fact. I have only heard <strong>of</strong> one such inciaant occurring<br />

anywhere. Clearly, other factors must be present.<br />

3, sad 4. Fzmil~ SgCii&@si~~ in Zio&ens%. One 3f the<br />

most significant <strong>of</strong> these other factors is that the family<br />

is t h ~ settino in which most DeODle first exoerlence<br />

&<br />

physical violecce, and the setting that establishes the<br />

emotional context and meaning <strong>of</strong> violenc~.<br />

Learning about violence starts with physical<br />

punishment, which is nearly universal (steinmetz and Straus,<br />

1974). When physical punishment is used. several<br />

consequences can be expected. First, most obviously, is<br />

learning to do or not do whatever the punishment is ictendea<br />

to teach. Less obvious, but equally or more important, are<br />

three other lessons that are so deeply learned that they<br />

become an integral part <strong>of</strong> one's personality and world view.<br />

<strong>The</strong> first <strong>of</strong> these unintended consegusnces is the<br />

CssoclatioC Of love with violence. PhysLcal punishment<br />

typically begins in infancy with slaps to correct and teach.<br />

Iomay and Daddy are the first and usually the mly ones to<br />

hit an icfaLt. and for mast children this continues<br />

throughout childhcod. <strong>The</strong> child therefore learns that those<br />

who love him or her the most are also thYse vh3 hir.<br />

Second. since physical punistmmt is used to train the<br />

child or to teach about danqa;ous things to be avoided, it<br />

establishes the moral rightness <strong>of</strong> hitting other family


T ~ E third unintended conseguence is the lesson that<br />

when something is really important, it justifies the usa <strong>of</strong><br />

p&ysical force.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se indirect lessons are cot confined to providirg a<br />

model for later treatment <strong>of</strong> one's own chilaxen. Sathet.<br />

they bECOme such a fundamental part <strong>of</strong> the icdiviaual's<br />

personality and outlook that they are generalized to other<br />

social relationships, especially to the relationship closest<br />

to that <strong>of</strong> paren? and child-that <strong>of</strong> husband and wife.<br />

Thus, early experiences with physlcal punishment lay<br />

the groundwork for the ncrmative legitisacy <strong>of</strong> all typss <strong>of</strong><br />

violence but especially intrafamily violence. Rs suggested<br />

by box 4, it provide: a role aodel-indeed. a spzcific<br />

nscriptn (see Chapter 4 ar:d Gagnon and Simon, 1973)--for<br />

such actions. nany children do not even nesd to gensralize<br />

this socially scripted pattern <strong>of</strong> behavior from the<br />

parent-child nexus in which it was learned to other family<br />

relationships: it our estimates are correct, millions <strong>of</strong><br />

children can directly observe physical violence between<br />

husbands and vives (see also Ovens and Straus. 1975).<br />

5, Cgl_sg=& E m . <strong>The</strong> preceding discussion has<br />

focusea .on the way ir. which violence becomes built into the<br />

behavioral repertory <strong>of</strong> husbands and wives. Though<br />

important, early experience could not account for the high<br />

level <strong>of</strong> family violence, were it not also supportpa by<br />

Cultural norms 1Egitinizing such violenr predispositions.<br />

Since most <strong>of</strong> us tend to think <strong>of</strong> norms that call for love<br />

and gentleness within the fas~ly, it is difficult to<br />

perceive that there are bcth @ jgg? and & fa&? cultural<br />

notms legitimizing the use <strong>of</strong> violence between family<br />

members. Chapter 3 documerts the evidence for the existerce<br />

<strong>of</strong> such norns and Chapter 5 illustrates the indirect effect<br />

<strong>of</strong> cultural norms.<br />

6, agfi Z, =pal Ineaualt~~ 2 s C2g1=3 !9g52gg~sg 2s<br />

Women. <strong>The</strong> last two causal factors, boxes 6 and 7 <strong>of</strong> Pigure<br />

-----<br />

1, can be considered together and summarized in the<br />

pToposition that the sexist organizaticn <strong>of</strong> the society and<br />

Its family system ;s one <strong>of</strong> the most tundamental factors in<br />

the high level <strong>of</strong> uife-beating. Chapter 6 and Plrt IV<br />

demonstrate this proposition. Policy recommendations aimea<br />

at preventing vife-beating arE examined in Chaptsr 13. Some<br />

aspects also have been presented earlier in this chapter.<br />

Slnce these issues are discussed ir depth in other chapte:s,<br />

I w i l l note only that boxes 6 and 7 <strong>of</strong> Figure 1 summarize<br />

the main elements <strong>of</strong> sexism that lead $5 wife-beating.<br />

Perhaps devoting an inappropriately small part <strong>of</strong> the<br />

text Of this chapter to sexual inequality--one <strong>of</strong> the most<br />

important causal factors in uife-beating,--will dranetize


that sexlsm is orly one part <strong>of</strong> the complex causal matrix <strong>of</strong><br />

family violence (cutllned in Figure 1). That male domixance<br />

does not protect men from violence by other men also<br />

illustrates this complexity.<br />

'1f true equality between the sexes were somehow to he<br />

achleved tomorrou, all forms <strong>of</strong> family violence (including<br />

wife-beatiog) would continue to exist--though prohably at a<br />

Somewhat lower ircidenc~--~nl~ss step5 also are taken to<br />

alter the factors identified in boxes 2. 3, 4. and 5 <strong>of</strong><br />

Figure 1. <strong>The</strong> level cf nor-family violence also must be<br />

lowered to end the training in violence that is part <strong>of</strong><br />

growing up in a typical American family. <strong>Violence</strong> is truly<br />

woven into the fabric <strong>of</strong> American society. and int3 the<br />

personality, beliefs, vilues. and behavi3ral scripts <strong>of</strong> most<br />

<strong>of</strong> our populatioo. Ellmination <strong>of</strong> wife-beating depends not<br />

only on sliminating sexual inequality. but also on altering<br />

the system <strong>of</strong> violence on which so much <strong>of</strong> American<br />

depends.<br />

society<br />

ROTE<br />

1. This chapter, originally presented at th? conference<br />

on "Battered wires: DEiicing the Issues,n Center for<br />

R962aroh on women. Stanford <strong>University</strong>. Hay 20, 1977; and<br />

at the Second World Congress, Internetional Society on<br />

Family Law. flontreal, June 14. 1977, is a shortened and<br />

revised version <strong>of</strong> "wife-Beating: Bow Common and why."<br />

reprinted vith permission from Victimologp sol, 2. Number<br />

3, 1977. (c) 1977 Visage Press. Inc. <strong>The</strong> materials in this<br />

chapter w i l l be presented more fully in a forthc3rnirg book.<br />

- Behjn_& ==a& mz <strong>Violence</strong> ;gali_s=g c+_g&&~<br />

(straus. Gelles. and steinmetz, 1980).


<strong>The</strong> cultural corms and values psrmittigg<br />

husband-wife violence resemble what Bern 3 ~ Ben d<br />

(1970) call "noncorscious ideology." This ides<br />

points out that on some issues (the Bess<br />

examined the role <strong>of</strong> women in America!? society)<br />

our per,spective is so deeply held and so subtle<br />

that we cannot Even imagine alternative beliefs<br />

and attitudes. Only when lsnonconscious<br />

ideologiesn are challerged are people apt t5<br />

display the depth <strong>of</strong> such a belief system. Orly<br />

under challenge is the rationale <strong>of</strong> the ia?ology<br />

likely to surface.<br />

Bs the chapters ix the following section<br />

aake clear, our society fails to recognize the<br />

massive cultural "scripting" (Gagnon and Simon.<br />

1973) that makes it more permissible to strike 3<br />

family member than to hit a friend, co-worker.<br />

or stralger. One <strong>of</strong> the reasons these cultural<br />

norms OK scripts are ignored is 2 deeply held<br />

belief that violence is an integral part <strong>of</strong><br />

human nature. This belief blinds psople to<br />

seeing that cultural norms specify who may ba<br />

hit and uhc may not, ard how hard, and under<br />

what circumstances.<br />

Another factor blinds people to the<br />

Culturally patterned nature <strong>of</strong> family viclencs<br />

(548 Chapter 1): the irony tha: the manifest<br />

ideology (as contrasted with the "ncnconscious<br />

ideology") emphasizes peace, harmony, and<br />

gentleress amorg family members. <strong>The</strong> reality to<br />

which the chapters ir Part I1 testify is that<br />

norms legitimizing intrafamily violence are<br />

present ard powerful. <strong>The</strong>y nay not be easily<br />

recognized, but *nonconscious ideologies* seldom<br />

are. Beccgnizing these taken-for-granted<br />

cultural norms is critical to understanding the<br />

<strong>Social</strong> causes <strong>of</strong> family violence: family<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> must be seen as s mode <strong>of</strong> behavior<br />

acquired through years <strong>of</strong> cultural and family<br />

socialization.<br />

Page 38


Chapter 3<br />

<strong>The</strong> Marriage License as a Hitting License:<br />

Evidence from Popular Culture, Law,<br />

and <strong>Social</strong> Science<br />

Murray A. Stmus<br />

This chapter emphasizes a thene that<br />

underlies many <strong>of</strong> the other chapters. It is the<br />

ability to see through or probe behind the<br />

<strong>of</strong>ficial version <strong>of</strong> reality. <strong>of</strong>ficial norms nsy<br />

exalt the family as a group devoted to<br />

gentleness and lore, but this chapter provides<br />

evidence that these norms coexist with a more<br />

Subtle set <strong>of</strong> norms that legitimate the nse <strong>of</strong><br />

force and violence in the family setting.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapter also raises a number <strong>of</strong><br />

critically important questions for UnderstandIcg<br />

violence in the family system. Although it<br />

demonstrates the existence <strong>of</strong> pro-violence<br />

norms. the questior <strong>of</strong> how such norms come inta<br />

existence in the first place is not ansvered.<br />

Why are they being maintained, sonetiaes with<br />

great fervor, by men and women alike?<br />

Throughout this volume there are hints to<br />

answers to these questions. For exa~ple, the<br />

chapter on violerce in children's books (Chapter<br />

4) illustrates ore <strong>of</strong> the ways that cultural<br />

norms are<br />

generation.<br />

transmitted from generation to<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters in Part III show some<br />

<strong>of</strong> the ways that the nature <strong>of</strong> families creates<br />

a high level <strong>of</strong> conflict, and the conaitions<br />

under which conflict turns into violence.<br />

Finally, Part I V helps us avoid the trap <strong>of</strong><br />

fixing an any one factor as $ks cause, or evec<br />

the main cause, <strong>of</strong> husband-wife violence. Each<br />

<strong>of</strong> those chapters shows rhat neither cultural<br />

norms nor family szructure alone can account for<br />

the high rate <strong>of</strong> husband-wife violence.<br />

together they form an explosive conbination.<br />

........................<br />

But


Ch.3. narriage License Page 40<br />

In Chapters 1 and 2, and in a series <strong>of</strong> other papers<br />

and books (Gslles. 1974, 1977: Steinmetz and Straus. 1974;<br />

Straus, 1973). evidence was presented that the family is<br />

~leeminrnt ~~~ in - all lvDes ~- <strong>of</strong> ~- ohvsical .~.-- ~- violence. - from slaDS to<br />

~<br />

torture ~ and murder. ~~~ -~ <strong>The</strong>se ~~~ studies ~ suaapst > thar the first<br />

-- ~..<br />

2<br />

priority for those corcerned with the level <strong>of</strong> violence in<br />

hmerican society should not be violence in the streets, but<br />

violence in the home, and the factors that lead to it.<br />

<strong>The</strong> most appropriate place to begin is vith a<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> the physical punishment <strong>of</strong> children. Ye<br />

believe that physical punishment serves as the primary<br />

mechacism by vhich members <strong>of</strong> our socisty learn to us? both<br />

verbal and physical aggression. Parental use <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

punishment is remarkably similar to many experiments on<br />

aggrsssion using electric shocks. Even more important.<br />

physlcal ponishment provides a powerful role model through<br />

which the child learns that if one truly vishes to influence<br />

another, physical force is effective and socially<br />

acceptable: that it is legitimate, <strong>of</strong>ten morally necessary<br />

to use physical violence on those one presumably loves: and<br />

that one should respond to aggression with aggression.<br />

Since studies in the United States and Great Britain<br />

show that at least 93 percent <strong>of</strong> all parents use physical<br />

punishment (Steinmetz and Straus, 1974). and that at least<br />

half contlnue to use it through the senior year in high<br />

school (Straus, 1971; Steinmetz, 1974). it is an almost<br />

universal social learning experience. noreover, this role<br />

modeling is supplemented for an amazingly large number <strong>of</strong><br />

children by explicit instlgation to aggression. Exhortation<br />

to violence is not restricted to slum families. Parents<br />

typically psreit or urge a child to fight back if a sibling<br />

is aggressive. noreover, 70 percent <strong>of</strong> the respondents in<br />

one national sample survey felt that "when a boy is growing<br />

up, it is very important for him to hare a few fist fights"<br />

(Stark and ticEvoy, 1970).<br />

IS for husband-vife aggression, this chapter vill<br />

attempt to shov that a marriage license is an implicit<br />

hitting license. <strong>The</strong> available evidence shovs that the<br />

typical adult is more likely to be attacked--verbally,<br />

physically, or even morally--by his or her own spouse than<br />

by any other persor. Data on homicides in at least a dozen<br />

different ccuctries (Curtis. 1974; wolfgang, 1956:<br />

Bohannan, 1960) show that murderers and their victims were<br />

more <strong>of</strong>ten members <strong>of</strong> the same family than <strong>of</strong> any other<br />

murder-victim relationship. In fact. when woman are<br />

murdered, it is overuhelmingly by their husbands. End. to<br />

complete the picture, when husbands nurder wives thep tend<br />

to do so with great brutality. as indicated by such things<br />

as multiple stab wounds or multiple gun sh3ts. as compared<br />

vith the single srab or single shot typical when wives<br />

nurder husbands (Yolfgang, 1956).


Ch. 3. flarriage License Page 41<br />

<strong>The</strong>retore, something in the family system not only<br />

produces a high level <strong>of</strong> physical aggression. but also makes<br />

wlres the victim <strong>of</strong> that aggression. Chapter 6 examines<br />

this high rate <strong>of</strong> aqgression agains- women in terms <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sexist Organization <strong>of</strong> society and the family, and<br />

identifies nine <strong>of</strong> the processes that link sexual ireguality<br />

to physical aggression against women. starting with the most<br />

obvious linkage: the use or implicit threat <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

intimrdatioP to keep voaer subordinate.<br />

TEE PARIDOX OF FABILY VIOLEBCE XORnS<br />

AS noted in Chapters 1 and 2, the norms and values<br />

relating to intrafamily violence pose a paradox. On the one<br />

hand, there is the "myth <strong>of</strong> family nonviolerce" (Steinmetz<br />

and Straus. 1974; Straus. 1974b) that reflacts cultural<br />

nor86 and aspirations for the family to be characterized by<br />

love, gentleness, and harmony. on the other hand, social<br />

norms exist that imply the right <strong>of</strong> family members to strike<br />

each other, theretare legitimizlng intrafamily assaults. at<br />

least under certain conditions.<br />

as nlted in our discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>The</strong> nyth <strong>of</strong> Family<br />

Nonviolence in Chapter 1, cultural contradictions and<br />

discontinuities <strong>of</strong> this type are pres9nt in Every sociatp to<br />

a greater or lesser ext€nt.(Benedict, 1938; Eabree, 1950:<br />

Ry+n ard Straus, 1954). Physical aggression is a prime<br />

example <strong>of</strong> this in Bmsrican society. Although thzro are<br />

clear norms and values restricting violence an3 emphasizing<br />

the value <strong>of</strong> peace and harmony, especially between family<br />

members, simultaneously a high level <strong>of</strong> actual violence<br />

exists, along with rorms glorifying aggressior and violence.<br />

In respect to the family, the legitimation <strong>of</strong> violence is<br />

sometimes explicit or even mandatory--as in the case <strong>of</strong> the<br />

right and obligation <strong>of</strong> parents to use an appropriate level<br />

<strong>of</strong> physical force to train and control a child. Ir fact.<br />

parents are permitted to use z level <strong>of</strong> physical force that<br />

is denied prison authorities in conrrolling innares. Ir the<br />

case <strong>of</strong> husband-wife relations, similar norms are prFsent<br />

and powerful. but largely implicit, unrecoyniz?d, or covert.<br />

what is the eviderce that such norms exist?


Ch.3. tlarri~ge License Page 42<br />

IBPORilAL nANIPESTETIONS OF CULTURAL NORMS<br />

PERM<strong>IT</strong>TING OR APPROVING lAAlThL VIOLENCE<br />

Ingehorg Dedichen, who lived vith hristotle Onassis for<br />

12 years, describer an incident in which 0c.assis beat. her<br />

severely until he quit frcm exhaustion:<br />

<strong>The</strong> fol1ow:ng day instead <strong>of</strong> apologizing,<br />

Onassis explained, "A11 Greek husbands, I tell<br />

you, all Greek men without exceptioc, beat their<br />

wives. It's good tor them." And then hz laughed<br />

(Shearer. 197S:q).<br />

nosc <strong>of</strong> the American or English public reading this might<br />

dismiss it as a Greek peculiarity. But, just as cn3ssis'<br />

statemect is an exaggeration for Greek men, our denial <strong>of</strong><br />

this norm Exaggerates in the other directioc. crce one is<br />

aware that there are norms legitimizing marital (and<br />

especially husband-tc-wife) violence, irstancss such as the<br />

above pop up constantly. One amazing examplE is the arcient<br />

(and flagrantly sexist) joke told on ths BBC women's program<br />

"Pettycoat Lane' in the spring <strong>of</strong> 197'4. One woman asked<br />

another why she felt her husband didnlt love her anymore.<br />

Rer answer: "Ha hasn't hashed me in a fortnight."<br />

ht a higher literary level, plays provide many ~xamples<br />

<strong>of</strong> the marriagE license as a hLtting license, including<br />

Several by George Bernard Shaw, 3rd t h recent ~ play about a<br />

Lesbian Coupl~, "<strong>The</strong> Killing 0f.Sister George," in which<br />

June makes threatening motions toward Alice:<br />

Slice: Don'? touch me. ~ o u ~ got r e no right.<br />

June: I've got every right.<br />

Alice: 1.m cot married to you. you knov.<br />

<strong>The</strong> above are, <strong>of</strong> course, only literary rsflectioas <strong>of</strong><br />

the cultural norms that can be observed in everyday life.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se examples racge fcom casual remarks such as that cf th%<br />

railway conduc?or who, vhen asked by a voman for help with a<br />

stobborn seat, did so and remzrked, "sowe <strong>of</strong> these seats are<br />

just like women: yon have to kick them to maki thae work"<br />

(protest letter to Th€ Nev York Times, July 19. 1974:5).<br />

Other examples appear in the meaia with at least tacit<br />

approval <strong>of</strong> their contents. as in the following section <strong>of</strong><br />

the videly read column by Ann Lanaers (October 29, 1973):<br />

Dear Ann Landers: Come out <strong>of</strong> the clouds, far<br />

Lord's sake. and gat down heze vith us humans.<br />

I an sick to death <strong>of</strong> ycur holier-than-thau<br />

attitude toward women whose husbands give them a<br />

well-deserved helt in the mouth.


Ch.3. narriage License Page 43<br />

Don't you know that a man can be pushed to the<br />

brink and sone:hing's gat to give? h crack ir!<br />

the tseth can be a vocderful tensLon-breaker.<br />

It's also a lot healthier than keeping all that<br />

anger bottled up.<br />

By husband hauls <strong>of</strong>f end slugs me every Eer<br />

months and I don't nind. Be feels better and so<br />

do I because he never hits me unless I deserve<br />

it. So why son'?. you cone <strong>of</strong>f it?-Peal mppy<br />

Dear R.H.: If you don't mind a crack ir~ the<br />

teeth every few months, it's all right with ma.<br />

I hope you hare a good dentist.<br />

I number <strong>of</strong> husbands and wives interviewed by Celles<br />

expressed similar attitudes, so that Gellss developed a<br />

classification <strong>of</strong> types <strong>of</strong> "normal violence" 'hat includes<br />

such categories as "1 asked for it." "I deserved it,,* "Shp<br />

needed to be brought to her senses," 4tc. (G2llos.<br />

1974:58). Other examples occur in connection with family<br />

disturbance polic~ calls, with wives as well as husbsnds<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten asserting their right to hi5 each other bicaus~ they<br />

are sarriea (Parras, 1967; Yorkshire Post. Bay 23, 1979:9).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se sane attitudes are widely shared by <strong>of</strong>ficials <strong>of</strong> the<br />

criaical justice system. scastimes this presumea right to<br />

hit is linked to the race or social class <strong>of</strong> the couple, as<br />

shown in Deny <strong>of</strong> Pamas' examplss and in an English judge's<br />

remark, 'if he had bean a miner in South wales I might have<br />

overlooked it" (London Daily Birror, January 29. 1974:l).<br />

This remark made headlines, but only because welsh miners<br />

PrOteStEd.<br />

E final example is provided by a marriage counseling<br />

case (Straus, 1973:120) ir which the husband hit his wife on<br />

numerous occasions. Re ard his mlfs felt that he coula not<br />

help hiaself because, ir the heat <strong>of</strong> ths trem?ndous<br />

arguments, he "last control." <strong>The</strong> counselor, however, tried<br />

to persuade the couple that the husband's behavior was not<br />

simply a reversion to "primitiven levels, but in fact was<br />

under normative control. He did so by asking the husband<br />

"Why didn't you stab her?' This conversa5ion brought out the<br />

implicit. unrecognized, but core5heless operating noro that<br />

peraitted the husband to hit his wife but not to stab her.<br />

This unrecognized norm legitimizing intrafamily violsace.<br />

unless it producss severe injury, parallels the Califoraia<br />

"wife-beatingm statute cited below.


Ch.3. narriage License Page 94<br />

Legltlmation <strong>of</strong> <strong>Husband</strong>-Wlf- Vlol~nce<br />

by the Courts and Pollcs<br />

<strong>The</strong>re 1s considerable evidence that Even though laws<br />

giving husbands the right to "chastise" ar erring vife are<br />

no longer with us. the underlying spirit 3f such laws<br />

lingers. That spirit is now primarily extralegal, but in<br />

important ways it is still embodied in the legal system.<br />

------<br />

Immunilp<br />

legitimisations<br />

$o&r. one <strong>of</strong> the most important <strong>of</strong> these<br />

<strong>of</strong> husband-wife violence is to be fou~d in<br />

the doctrine <strong>of</strong> "spousal immunity' that, to this day, in<br />

many jurisdictiors prevents a wife from suing her husband<br />

for assault and battery. In other jurisdictions, the law<br />

has been changed ccly recently. Truninger (1971:269) cites<br />

the following example:<br />

In Self vs. Self (1962) th+ wife alleged that<br />

the defendant husband ..." unlawfully assault4<br />

plaintiff and beat upon, scratched and abused<br />

the person <strong>of</strong> plaintiff," and that as a result<br />

plaintif* 'sustained physical injury to her<br />

person and emotional distress, and among other<br />

icjuries did receive a broken arm.' <strong>The</strong><br />

hosband's motion for a summary jodqmsnt was<br />

granted by the trial court.<br />

on appeal. the California Supremp court reverssa<br />

the trial court's judgment, thus overruling<br />

eeveral older California cases supportirg<br />

interspousal immunity. <strong>The</strong> raCiocale <strong>of</strong> courts<br />

retalning the common law sp3usal inmucity<br />

doctrine fear was that allowing the tort action<br />

"would destroy the peace and harmony <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

hone, and thus would be contrary to the policy<br />

or the 1 2 ~ . ~ ~<br />

PGar ~f fhe Police i~ P$. As Truninger also points<br />

out, it is doubtful Whether a vife gains much, other than<br />

the principle, from ability to sue her husband for assault<br />

acd battery: the actual operation <strong>of</strong> both the civil and<br />

criminal justice systems puts up snormaus obstlcles and, in<br />

any case, it typically does nothing to prevent imeEdiate<br />

repetition <strong>of</strong> the <strong>of</strong>fense while the case is bsing<br />

adjudicafed. Usually the only way in which a riol€nt spouse<br />

can be removed from the hcae is by arrest, but the police<br />

make such arrests rarely. In fsc', until th- 1976 revision.<br />

the International Association <strong>of</strong> Police Chiefs' *r%ining<br />

manual recommends that arrest,s gpi be made ic such cases.<br />

<strong>The</strong> co-arrest guideline probably is more clcsely<br />

followed than any other ir the training manozl bscaose it so<br />

clearly fits the experience end values 5f the polic*. In<br />

gereral. the police see. to st-are the belief i n the<br />

legitimacy <strong>of</strong> spousal vioLence. prsvided tha rEsulting


Ch.3. larriage License Page 115<br />

injuries or destruc:ion are within llmits. sore policp<br />

dEpartmEPtS have informal "stitch rules- whereby the wouna<br />

requires a certain (high) number <strong>of</strong> stitches befsre an<br />

<strong>of</strong>ficer makes a2 arrest (Field and Field, 1973:229). Parnas<br />

(1967) cltfs case after case <strong>of</strong> the police avoiding arrasts<br />

in s;tuacions aandaticg ore were the parties not husband and<br />

wife. Almost any polrcsnac can cite nuwprous eramplas <strong>of</strong><br />

husbands' claiming the right to strike their wivss. and many<br />

police themselves believe this to be the law (TLunirger,<br />

2972: 272: Coote. 2979) .*1<br />

obtaining even basic physical protection is <strong>of</strong>ter<br />

difficult as is graphically shown in the following instance<br />

(REV York Times. June 14. 1976) :<br />

It was about 4 o*clock in the afternoon "ten a<br />

call came into the 103rd Precinct station h~use<br />

in Jamaica, Queecs, from a woman vho said her<br />

husband had beaten her. that her face was<br />

plesding and bruised. She thought some <strong>of</strong> h?r<br />

ribs had been broken.<br />

Wan yon help me?- she pleeded to the pclice<br />

<strong>of</strong>ficer who answered the phona. 'By husbard's<br />

gone now, but he said he vould come back and<br />

kill me.' She wes also frightened, she said.<br />

thlt he would start heating the children rbr te<br />

returned.<br />

'It's not a Pollcs Department thrng," +he<br />

<strong>of</strong>flcer told her. "I+'s really a family thing.<br />

You'll have to go to Famlly Court tomorrow.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re's nothrrg that I can do."<br />

<strong>of</strong> Prosecutors to &i. Dsspite the repeatsd<br />

nature and frequent severity <strong>of</strong> marital violerce, it is<br />

erdured for long periods--<strong>of</strong>ten mary years--by large Gumhers<br />

ot women. Some <strong>of</strong> the reasons that so many wives tolerate<br />

this situation follov from the variables analyzed in this<br />

paper. other factors hare been identified in Gslles' paper<br />

"Pbused Hives: why Do <strong>The</strong>y Stay?" (1976). sooner or later,<br />

however, the situation brings large nuabers <strong>of</strong> ween to the<br />

pcint ot desperation. Soma rsspord to this by leaving. or<br />

even by killing the husbacd; others attempt to secure a<br />

warrant for the arrest <strong>of</strong> their husbands. Por obvi3usly<br />

differert reasocs, each <strong>of</strong> these alternetives is typically<br />

unsatistactory. If the wife etteapts to bring charges, she<br />

faces being "cooled out* at every step by <strong>of</strong>ficials sf the<br />

criminal justice system. This prccsss is illustrated by<br />

Pield and Field's tabulation <strong>of</strong> the approximately 7,500 such<br />

attempts ir. Washington, D.C. in 1966:<br />

Invariably, the police had told +hem that, in<br />

order to protect themselves, they had tc "get =<br />

warrant from the district attorney.' <strong>The</strong>y


Ch.3. marriage License Page 46<br />

announced typically, "I have come to get one."<br />

TO them this implied an automatic process, like<br />

dropping nickels into a verding machine, and<br />

they expected a routine procedure culminating i n<br />

the issuance <strong>of</strong> a warrant for their husband's<br />

arrest. <strong>The</strong>ir heightened feeling <strong>of</strong> precipitats<br />

danger reinforced this expectation. and thsir<br />

eense <strong>of</strong> grievance and desperation was further<br />

solidified bv the lono vait thev endured before<br />

talking with the ir-itial screening policeman or<br />

the district attcrney. Of these 7,500 women,<br />

fewer than 200 left having secured their<br />

objective (1973:232).<br />

Finally, ever when the circumstancas are such that the<br />

pclic~ and district attorney cannot avoid bringing charges.<br />

few such cases get to trial.<br />

R survey cf the assaalt cases in the District <strong>of</strong><br />

Columbia showed that over three-fourths <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

cases !!ct,involving husbanas and wives went to a<br />

disposition <strong>of</strong> the merits <strong>of</strong> guilt or innscence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> enforcement pattern was reversea in<br />

husband-wife CZSES. Orly about one-sixth <strong>of</strong> all<br />

arrests involving marital violence ultimately<br />

ended at trial cr vith a guilty plea. 3cd the<br />

crime charged by that time vas invarisbly a<br />

misdemeanor rather than a felony (Field and<br />

Field. 1973: 224).<br />

------<br />

Victim Com~ensaticr. :rother vay ir which th? law<br />

continue5. 5.2 effect, to legitimize husbans-wife assault<br />

crops up in connection with the workings <strong>of</strong> boards ana<br />

commission^ that have besn set up in England and a feu<br />

Aaericaa states to compensate vicfims <strong>of</strong> crimes. <strong>The</strong><br />

English board explicitly ruled against ccmpersation when ths<br />

victim is a spouse (Williams. 1979) and this also seems to<br />

be the case vith the California lsv (Edelhertz and Geis,<br />

1979:278; Trunioger, 1971:270).<br />

T& Les=& astern aFd cultural &E~;PQ 2f IPtrafalllE<br />

- violence. -- - - -- <strong>The</strong> situatior described in this section is vell<br />

summarized by the phrase used as the subtitle <strong>of</strong> Pisla and<br />

Field's article (1973) cn the criminal process ir cases <strong>of</strong><br />

marital violence: "Neither Justice Nor Peace." 4 situation<br />

so pervasive is not llkely te he a result <strong>of</strong> hisflrical<br />

accident. nor is it likely to b? a consequence 3f the many<br />

difficulties in dealing legally with marital violence and<br />

the low rate <strong>of</strong> success achieved by invckicg criminal law.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se difficulties and uncertainties, after all, l o not<br />

deter the ~~~ oalice A and ceurts -~ --~- from ~- ~ ~ rnvokino - criminal<br />

adjudication processes for many crimes--such 35<br />

prostitution--with an even love? rate <strong>of</strong> success in ccntrol.<br />

Rather, the failure to invoke criminal penalties rafleczs<br />

historical continuities in the cultural norms that make the


Ch.3. marriage License Page 47<br />

marriage license a hitting license. This is elaost clcar in<br />

the California Penal Code section on wife-beating, which<br />

prohibits an assault only if it results in severe physical<br />

injury. But the most cleer conremporary legal expression <strong>of</strong><br />

the right <strong>of</strong> husbands to use physical force is found in the<br />

immunity <strong>of</strong> husbands from prosecution for rape <strong>of</strong> their<br />

wives.<br />

Expermental and SUTVE~ EVZdenCe on<br />

Approval <strong>of</strong> laTital Vlolence<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a slowly growing body <strong>of</strong> Empirical research on<br />

intrafamily aggression and violence, some <strong>of</strong> which provides<br />

evidence on the cultural norms we are considering. <strong>The</strong><br />

snrvey conducted for the United States Rational Commission<br />

<strong>of</strong> the <strong>Causes</strong> and Prevention <strong>of</strong> <strong>Violence</strong> foand that about<br />

one quarter <strong>of</strong> the persons interviewed said they could<br />

approve a husband or wife hitting each other under certain<br />

circums;ances (Stark and 8cEvoy. 1970). That figuze is<br />

probably a considerable underestimate because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

existence <strong>of</strong> opposite corms--the more socially acceptable<br />

ac3-siclence n3rms and the implicit or covert pro-violence<br />

norms.<br />

<strong>The</strong> contradictcry and covert narure <strong>of</strong> the corms<br />

approving marital viclecce makes expsrinental and<br />

ob~ervational studies particularly approprilte, because<br />

these stodies do pot depend on the willingness or ability to<br />

verbalize norms and values. Urfortunately. for practical<br />

reasons. all the observational studies have beer <strong>of</strong><br />

parent-child violence (Rellak and intell. 1974). But there<br />

have been experimental studies <strong>of</strong> marital aggression or<br />

studies that bear OL marital aggression.<br />

he first <strong>of</strong> these studies also reflects ths mora<br />

geneyal phenomenon <strong>of</strong> male hostility to women:<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the leist recognized indices <strong>of</strong> mals<br />

hostility to females is the reaction <strong>of</strong> men who<br />

watch a violent act against women, rather than<br />

coamitting or initiating it themselves. Three<br />

psychclogists from Richigan State university<br />

staged a ssries <strong>of</strong> fights that were to be<br />

witnessed by UnSuspectiCg passersby. <strong>The</strong><br />

researchers found. to their anazement, that ma15<br />

witnesses r~shed to the aid <strong>of</strong> men being<br />

as~aultsd by either women or o4n, and thct man<br />

helped women being hit by othpr women. Rut not<br />

one male bystander interfered when a male actor<br />

apparently beat up a woman (Pogrebin, 1974:49-55<br />

and 80).


Ch.3. narriage License Page 48<br />

In addition to the icterpretation <strong>of</strong> thess ficdings as<br />

reflecting male hostility to women, it also seems likely<br />

that they reflect the rorm permitting assaults betwsen<br />

spouses. That is, the male bystanders did not come to the<br />

aid <strong>of</strong> a female victim <strong>of</strong> a male assailant because they<br />

inferred that he was the womac's husband. This, in fa=%, is<br />

the reason a number <strong>of</strong> bystanders gave for nor intsrvening<br />

as Kitty Genovese was murdered (Rosenthal. 1964). This<br />

corclusion is further given credence because it agrees vith<br />

experimental studies <strong>of</strong> "bystarder intervention,' such as<br />

the experiments reported and summarized by Bickman (1975).<br />

Bickman concludes that the social definition <strong>of</strong> what acticns<br />

are right for the bystander is a more powsrful determinant<br />

<strong>of</strong> intervention than the severity <strong>of</strong> the crime or concern<br />

for the welfare <strong>of</strong> the victim.<br />

Closely related to these findings is an unpublished<br />

experiment by Churchill and Straus in which the subjects<br />

were given a description <strong>of</strong> an assault and asked to irdicate<br />

what punishment they felt was appropriate. In the course <strong>of</strong><br />

the assault, the victim was kcocked unconscious. In half<br />

the descriptions the assailant was described ss the vomanvs<br />

husband. ~ ~ ~~ ~~ ~ In the ~~~ other ~~ - half. ~- the descriotion ~ ~ . - ~<br />

was ~~ .~~ identical<br />

~~ ~~<br />

except that the couple was described as "going together" for<br />

a year. <strong>The</strong> mean punishment score when the victim was not<br />

married to the assailant was 4.15, compared vith 2.65 when<br />

the victim was the wife. Boreover. this experiment probably<br />

understates the differerce, in that it specified thst the<br />

unmarried cou~le had been ooiro toaether for a vear. <strong>The</strong><br />

dltference pkobably would have -beep much greater if the<br />

unmarried couple had not been descrlbed as havzcg a<br />

quaslmarltal relatlorshlp.<br />

In a final set <strong>of</strong> experiments bearing on this issue.<br />

couples interacted in a standardized laboratory task<br />

involving conflict. <strong>The</strong> data for married ccuplss was<br />

compared vith the data for urmarried couples in the Sam9<br />

task situation. E study by Ryder (1968) found that<br />

strangers were treated more gently than were spouses.<br />

Similarly. using an experimental task that required the<br />

couple to reach a decision, Winter, Perreira, ard Bowers<br />

(1973) found the unzelated couples listened respectfully to<br />

one another whereas married couples were <strong>of</strong>ten rude to ane<br />

another. Although thEre is a long distance betveen ruaeness<br />

and violence. it seems likely that what Ls marifested in<br />

these two experiments is the beglnning <strong>of</strong> the journ3y that<br />

for many couples ultimately ends in violence (Straus,<br />

1974a).


ch.3. larriage License Page 49<br />

TRZ CYCLE OF INTRAPABILY VIOLENCE<br />

In Chapter 2 the role-modeling function <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

punishment was described, and especially the implici: lesson<br />

that it is permissible, even mandatory, to ose physical<br />

violence on those one loves most. But the learning <strong>of</strong><br />

social scripts for aggression betveen family members takes<br />

place in many other ways, and I w i l l conclude by briefly<br />

menticning just one <strong>of</strong> these: observation by childrsn <strong>of</strong><br />

aggressivs behavior between the parents theaselvss, ana<br />

physical violence in particular.<br />

Whenthe Family <strong>Violence</strong> Research Program began, we<br />

were under the impression that violence betveen spouses is<br />

rare. and that middle class parents take pains to avoid<br />

physical fights in the presence <strong>of</strong> their children. <strong>The</strong> idea<br />

that physical violence between spousgs is rare was the first<br />

<strong>of</strong> the myths about intrafamily violence that the data from<br />

the program forced us to abandon. Subsequently, a study by<br />

BulCrOft and Straus (1975) suggested that the itea <strong>of</strong><br />

parents' being able to hide physical fights may also be<br />

iccorrect.<br />

In this study, 121 university students and thsir<br />

parents each separately completed parallel qusstionnaires,<br />

including one section that dealt with conflicts betweec the<br />

parents. <strong>The</strong>re was a series <strong>of</strong> items concernirg modes <strong>of</strong><br />

coping with these conflicts (the Conflict Tactics Scales<br />

deSCrlbEd in Strans, 1979 and used in Chapt?rs 2 and 12).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se items were arranged in order <strong>of</strong> coerciveness. starting<br />

with discussing things calmly, and endirg with hitting the<br />

other with a hard object. A violence index uas computed<br />

from the latter questions.<br />

we found a surprisingly high correlatior. between the<br />

SCOrJS computed on the basis <strong>of</strong> data obtained frsa the<br />

student and ~~- the ~~. scsrec -....- ohtainnn on the basis <strong>of</strong> +he<br />

~ ~ ~~~~ ~~ ~~.<br />

questionnaire completed by each parsnt: the Correlation<br />

brtween the parent-report data and the child-r?port data for<br />

husDand9s violence was .64, ard for the wife's violence<br />

index the correlation was .32. <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> this<br />

finding in the present context is that it indicates the<br />

degree to which children know about acts <strong>of</strong> physical<br />

a49Z9ssion between their - - Darents. at lrast durinq the Tear<br />

th; child is a senior in high schooi. coosequsntl$. par;nts<br />

seem again to be serving as role models for lntrafamily<br />

physical aggression and for learning t h ~ social corms<br />

which--to repeat the opening statement 2f this chapter-aaks<br />

the family the most frequent setting for aggression <strong>of</strong> all<br />

types. ranging from insults, to slaps. $0 baiting, torture.<br />

and murder.


ch.3. narriage License Page 50<br />

NOTES<br />

Part <strong>of</strong> this chapter is reprinted with permission from<br />

nurray R. Straus, "Sexual Inequality. Cultural Warms, and<br />

<strong>Wife</strong>-Beating," originally published in YtztLE&zy. 1976: 1<br />

(Spri~g):5(1-76. (c) 1976 Visage Pzess, 1r.c.; a ~ in d Emilio<br />

c. Viano. editor, Victims and So~ie$x, Washington, D.C.,<br />

Visage Press, 1976.<br />

1. Programs to lcform and train p3lice to deal more<br />

effectively with family disturbances have bpen initiated in<br />

several cities. see Bard. 1969, 1971, 3nd Chapter 13.


Chapter 4<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> and the <strong>Social</strong> Structure<br />

as Reflected in Children's Books<br />

from 1850 to 1970<br />

Martha D. Huggins and Murray A. Straus<br />

<strong>The</strong> previous chapter showed that cultural<br />

norms make the marriage license a hitting<br />

license. <strong>The</strong> research reported in the present<br />

chapter vas designed to see if this approval <strong>of</strong><br />

violence between family members is a theme in<br />

books written for children. <strong>The</strong> resalts show<br />

that childrer's books do depict a great deal <strong>of</strong><br />

riolence. including killings. In addition, they<br />

reinforce the lesson <strong>of</strong> physical punishment by<br />

depicting riolence as an effective means <strong>of</strong><br />

secoring justice or <strong>of</strong> achieving some valued<br />

end.<br />

Aowever, althongh this chapter shows that<br />

children's books describe and justify violence.<br />

for violence the findings are not what we<br />

expected. Giver the high rates <strong>of</strong> familp<br />

violence pointed cut in Chapters 1 and 2. it is<br />

iro~ic that in these stories little <strong>of</strong> the<br />

violence takes place between members <strong>of</strong> the same<br />

iamlly. Thsre is a ride discrepancy between the<br />

reality <strong>of</strong> family life and the idsalized picture<br />

implied by the lack <strong>of</strong> family violence in<br />

children's books. Bs in television, physical<br />

aggression is largely depicted as something that<br />

occurs between strangers. <strong>The</strong> chapter concludes<br />

with a discussion <strong>of</strong> the discrepancy between the<br />

violent reality <strong>of</strong> family life and the media's<br />

avoidance <strong>of</strong> shoving violence between family<br />

members.<br />

........................


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 52<br />

<strong>of</strong> the highly industrialized nations <strong>of</strong> the vorld, the<br />

united States is clearly one <strong>of</strong> the aost violent (Palmer,<br />

1972:15). Nany explanations have been <strong>of</strong>fered for this<br />

phenomenon (Graham and Gurr, 1969). and undoubtedly a cumber<br />

<strong>of</strong> factors operate to maintain physical violence as a<br />

continuing aspect <strong>of</strong> American social structure. On2 factor<br />

that has been a subject <strong>of</strong> considerable controversy is the<br />

mass media.<br />

Some investigators argue that violence in the media<br />

reflects the violence <strong>of</strong> the society (discussed in Lynn,<br />

1969). Others maintain that violence in the mass meaia and<br />

in sports serves as a safety valve, permitting aggressive<br />

drlves to be drained <strong>of</strong>f--the "drive discharge" and<br />

"catharsis" models (BEttelheim, 1967; Freud, 1959:<br />

Peshbach and Singer, 1971; Lorenz, 1966). Both the<br />

"reflection" and the "catharsis" theories see violence in<br />

the media and in sports as having either a neutral or a<br />

neutralizing role. <strong>The</strong>y therefore contrast sharply with<br />

theories that hold that violence in the media is part <strong>of</strong> the<br />

process <strong>of</strong> transmitting and encouraging violance. Among the<br />

latter are the "cultural pattern" theory <strong>of</strong> Sipes (1973).<br />

.social learning" theory (Bandura. 1973). and "general<br />

systems theoryn (Straus, 1973).<br />

<strong>The</strong> theoretical and methodological issues underly+ng<br />

this COntrOverSy are so complex that an eventual resolution<br />

w i l l require, at the mimimum, an accuaulation and<br />

*triangulation' <strong>of</strong> evidence from a variety <strong>of</strong><br />

investigations. Aistorical studies <strong>of</strong> a variety <strong>of</strong> cultural<br />

forms are particularly needed. A study <strong>of</strong> chilaren's books<br />

therefore seemed desirable because (1) most <strong>of</strong> the available<br />

research on the mass media and violence focuses on<br />

television. Childrsn's books, however, may be just as<br />

important or more important. Our informal observation is<br />

that the impact <strong>of</strong> a book read by a child (or to a young<br />

child by a significant person such as a parent) is extremely<br />

powerfol. (2) <strong>The</strong> availability <strong>of</strong> children's books for over<br />

one hundred years enables a degree <strong>of</strong> historical depth not<br />

possible for any <strong>of</strong> the other mass media.<br />

Thgoretical Perspective. We believe that the relation<br />

between literature and society is "dialectic": literary and<br />

cther artistic productions reflect the culture and social<br />

organizatlcn <strong>of</strong> the society. especially its dominant strata.<br />

However, once it is in existence, literature serves to<br />

control and mold that culture and social structure. If the<br />

artist's work is to be accepted, he or she must draw on the<br />

Cnltural heritage <strong>of</strong> society and appeal to important<br />

elements in the lives <strong>of</strong> members <strong>of</strong> society. It the same<br />

time, the work <strong>of</strong> ac artist--once accepted--becomes s part<br />

<strong>of</strong> that cultural heritage and is one <strong>of</strong> many elements<br />

influencing and controlling what goes on in the society.


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 53<br />

Previous content analysis studies have given some<br />

indication <strong>of</strong> the relationship between changes in society<br />

and changes in literary contents. straus and Boughton<br />

A--- ---- -- --<br />

cross-lagged correlation. Since we w i l l not be presenting<br />

such data, the present chapter is not <strong>of</strong>fered as a test <strong>of</strong><br />

these assumptions. Our aim is more modest; simply to<br />

present the results <strong>of</strong> our historical analysis, together<br />

with our interpretation <strong>of</strong> the trends.<br />

-<br />

specific Obiectives. One <strong>of</strong> the purposes <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

is to determine if the level <strong>of</strong> interpersonal physical<br />

violence depicted in children's books has been increasing or<br />

decreaslng during the 120-year span from 1850 to 1970. He<br />

pose no hypothesis about the direction <strong>of</strong> change, because<br />

the available evidence does not suggest any overall '<br />

or decrease in the level <strong>of</strong> violence in the Unitea<br />

during this period (Graham and Gurr, 1969).<br />

States<br />

<strong>The</strong> second objective is to gain information on the way<br />

society defines and labels physical violence for its next<br />

geEBration. <strong>The</strong> study exanines the extent to which violence<br />

in literature is depicted as an nexpressive" act (carried<br />

out to cause pain or injury as an snd in itself) or an<br />

"instrumenzal'* act (carried out to achieve some extrinsic<br />

purpose). Sznilarly, the proportion <strong>of</strong> violent acts<br />

presented by the authors as "legitimateu and "illegitimate*<br />

suggests how society evaluates and labels physical violence.<br />

Finally, the content analysis was designed to obtain<br />

information on rhe statuses, roles, motives, and emotions <strong>of</strong><br />

the characters involved in violence. azd the precipitating<br />

conditions, outcomes. and consequences <strong>of</strong> violence. To the<br />

extent that violence in literature mirrors violence in the<br />

society, such information provides insight into this<br />

important aspect <strong>of</strong> social structure. To the extent that<br />

literature influences society, such information gives<br />

important clues to the "scriptn (Gagnon and Simon, 1973) for<br />

violent behavior presented to children.*l


ch.a. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 54<br />

SAIIPLE BND SETHOD<br />

g~mle. R three-step samplirg process was used. <strong>The</strong><br />

first step was the identification <strong>of</strong> what, for want <strong>of</strong> a<br />

better tar.. can be called "children's classics,' books<br />

recognized by a literary elite <strong>of</strong> the society. we focusea<br />

on this type <strong>of</strong> literature because, as Marx suggested<br />

(1964). the ideas <strong>of</strong> the elite strata tena to be the<br />

dominant and influential ideas in the society. Prom .this<br />

perspective, it is not the moral evaluations <strong>of</strong> the<br />

popula3.on at large that give rise to a group's definitions<br />

<strong>of</strong> reality, but mainly the evaluations <strong>of</strong> the aominant class<br />

(Parkin. 1971:92). On the. basis <strong>of</strong> these assumptions, we<br />

sought out lists <strong>of</strong> recomme~ded an3 esteemed children's<br />

books, for example, the nuotable Children's Books:<br />

1965-1972' prepared by the Book Evaluation Committee <strong>of</strong> the<br />

American Library Associatiou.*2 <strong>The</strong> body <strong>of</strong> work compiled by<br />

this method is a chronologically ordered list <strong>of</strong> all books<br />

published between 1850 and 1970 that were included in any <strong>of</strong><br />

the lists <strong>of</strong> recommended books.<br />

<strong>The</strong> second step <strong>of</strong> the sampling process was designed to<br />

yield five books published in 1850. and five published every<br />

fifth year thereafter. in 1855. 1860. and so on, up to and<br />

including 1970. for a total <strong>of</strong> 125 books. For those rears<br />

in which many books appnared, the sample <strong>of</strong> five was drawn<br />

by random numbers. If there rere fnuer than five books in<br />

the sample year (as sometimes happened i n the early years).<br />

books from the closest adjacent year were included, for<br />

example, a book published in 1856 is included in the sample<br />

for 1855. Since these are all "classic" or "recommended"<br />

books we were able to find 115 <strong>of</strong> the originally selected<br />

125 boots in nearby libraries. <strong>The</strong> missing tsn books were<br />

replaced by a random selection from among the other books<br />

published during the appropriate years.<br />

<strong>The</strong> third step in the sampling process coesisted <strong>of</strong><br />

using a *.able <strong>of</strong> random numbers to select fifteen different<br />

pages from sach book. We followed this procedure to prevent<br />

longer books from disproportionately influencing the<br />

resnlts. Our data than describe any act <strong>of</strong> interpersonal<br />

physical violence that occurred on one <strong>of</strong> the sample pages<br />

1s 125 "recommendedn chFldrenvs books published from 1850<br />

through 1970.13<br />

goding p=$&&. <strong>The</strong> basic unit <strong>of</strong> analysis consists <strong>of</strong><br />

an act <strong>of</strong> interDersona1 violence, which ue define as the<br />

identifying the book in -which it occurred and providing<br />

space to code the type <strong>of</strong> information identified a few<br />

paragraphs back. Bore speczfic information on ?ach <strong>of</strong> these<br />

variables w i l l be given when the relevant data are


Ch.4. Tiolence in ChildreL's Books Page 55<br />

presented. *4<br />

FREQUENCY AND TRENDS IN VIOLENCE<br />

nary observers <strong>of</strong> the American scene have suggested<br />

that America is a violent society. Palmer (l972:l5), for<br />

example, contends that:<br />

"Since its inception, the United States has been<br />

in the front ranks <strong>of</strong> violent societies. Born<br />

in revolution, vracked by civil war, involved in<br />

numerous vars, it has also the tradition <strong>of</strong><br />

bloody rioting, homicide and arrest."<br />

According to the statistics cited by Palmer, each year<br />

there are 15.000 criminal homicides, 35.000 suicides.<br />

300,000 Serious assaults, and 50,000 forcible rapes, and<br />

these are minimum estimates. <strong>The</strong>sr more extreme forms <strong>of</strong><br />

physical force only partially illustrate a more widespread<br />

pattern <strong>of</strong> violence in the United States. Por example.<br />

physical fights between husband and wife may occur in half<br />

to three-quarters <strong>of</strong> all marriages (see Chapter 2). and<br />

physical fights between siblings are so common as to be<br />

almost universal (Straus. Gelles, ald Steinmetz, 1979). Is<br />

the violence that is so much a part <strong>of</strong> American life found<br />

in the literature for children?<br />

Ths answer to this rhetorical question is a clear<br />

"yes." lore than three-quarters <strong>of</strong> the 15-page<br />

'book-segments" had one or more violent episodes, with a<br />

total <strong>of</strong> 264 such episodes. <strong>The</strong> largest number <strong>of</strong> violent<br />

episodes in a single book was 10 (in &IS_: &gf&g).<br />

<strong>The</strong> mean number <strong>of</strong> violent acts per book-segment uas 2.1.<br />

<strong>The</strong> flgura <strong>of</strong> 2.1 violent episodes per book-segment means<br />

that a 50-page book is likely to include about seven violent<br />

acts and a 150-page book. about 21 violent acts.<br />

- aowities oz &enEp. <strong>The</strong>,variety <strong>of</strong> methods used in<br />

these books0 cause physical pair or injury to another<br />

Covers most <strong>of</strong> those knom to the human race. <strong>The</strong>ss ranged<br />

flom merelv shakino someone trvo such incidents\. tc hittino<br />

. . - ~<br />

and kicking (39 incidents), torture (6 incidents), hurning<br />

(11 incidents), stabbing (43 incidents), ard shooting (40<br />

incidents). <strong>The</strong> most frequent type <strong>of</strong> violence involved<br />

pouncing on someone, grabbing then forcefully or causing<br />

them to fall (66 inciaents or 25 percent <strong>of</strong> the tstal).<br />

Boverer. such relatively mild forms vere outnumbered by<br />

about two to one by more severe forms such as stabbing.<br />

shooting. torture, and burnirg.<br />

This fact is also reflected in the classification <strong>of</strong><br />

acts on the hasis <strong>of</strong> the resulting physical injury: 22<br />

percent <strong>of</strong> the 264 cases described a physical injury, and an


Ch.9. Violen~e in Children's Books Page 56<br />

additional 33 percent described a violent death. Clearly,<br />

YE are not dealing with "kid stuff." <strong>The</strong> essentially adult<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> the violence portrayed in these books w i l l be<br />

shovn at greater length leter.<br />

0<br />

55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 g 05<br />

10 15 20 75 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65<br />

- - -<br />

Time Period


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Psge 57<br />

Historical Trends. Figure 1 gives the mein number <strong>of</strong><br />

violent acts per book-segment for each five-year time<br />

period. This chart reveals no long-tern "secular" trend.<br />

This finding is consistent vith the conclusions <strong>of</strong> Graham<br />

and Gurr (1969:628) concerning studies <strong>of</strong> actual rather than<br />

fictional violence. A t the same time, there is a pattern to<br />

the highs +nd lows in fictional violence. <strong>The</strong> highs tend to<br />

occur when the society is engaged in war. Thus, the highest<br />

points in Figure 1 occurred during ths Bmerican Civil war<br />

(1865). World sar I (1915). world War 11 (1945). and during<br />

the peak <strong>of</strong> the Viet Nam denorstrations in the united States<br />

(1970) .*5<br />

<strong>The</strong>se data. shoring that violence in children's fiction<br />

is associzted with the collective violsnce <strong>of</strong> the society,<br />

are remarkably close to the findings <strong>of</strong> studies that show an<br />

increase in actual violence ~ U g a g society during periods<br />

<strong>of</strong> war (Ercher and Gartner. 1976: Aenry and Short.<br />

1954:102). <strong>The</strong> archer and Gartner findings are particularly<br />

impressive because they are based on data for 110 different<br />

nations. <strong>The</strong> correspondence betwepn the findings <strong>of</strong> our<br />

content analysis vith analyses based on actual<br />

violence is also <strong>of</strong> methodologicaL interest<br />

levels <strong>of</strong><br />

in that it<br />

provides additional support for using<br />

understand the operation <strong>of</strong> a society.<br />

content analysis to<br />

FICTIONAL VIOLENCE AND SOCIAL CONTROL OF DSVIANCF<br />

<strong>The</strong> data just presented suggest that violence is an<br />

integral part <strong>of</strong> ~recomended" children's books. Housver.<br />

Since Our interest is not in children's literature 2% ge,<br />

but in using children*~ books to gain a greater<br />

Understanding <strong>of</strong> the role <strong>of</strong> violence in American society.<br />

our focus will be on the nature <strong>of</strong> violence and its<br />

correlates. Re have already presented one such<br />

correlate--the association <strong>of</strong> fictional violsnce with<br />

periods <strong>of</strong> national collective violence. This can be<br />

lnterpreted as manifesting the pricciple that artistic<br />

p:oductions rsflect the sociocultural matrix <strong>of</strong> the artist.<br />

Rs suggested that fiction nay serve to mold and control<br />

SOc2ety. acd it is to this issue that we now turn. Durkheim<br />

(1950) and Erikson (1966) hold that moral violations are<br />

Singled out for punishment and public disapproval as a means<br />

<strong>of</strong> strengthening the commitment <strong>of</strong> the society to its moral<br />

norms. Thsoretically, the high incidence <strong>of</strong> violence in<br />

these books could be a vehicle to expose an6 puzish those<br />

who US8 violence. Several <strong>of</strong> our findings. however, suggest<br />

thar this is .ot the case. In fact, the implicit message is<br />

that violence is effective in resolving seemingly ins3luble<br />

problems.


Ch.9. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 58<br />

<strong>The</strong> first Evidence for this interpretation is the large<br />

proportron <strong>of</strong> the violent incidents classified as<br />

"instrumental violence" (defined as the use <strong>of</strong> violence to<br />

forc? another to carry out, or to hinder mother from<br />

carrying out. some act). Some 72 percent <strong>of</strong> acts were<br />

classified as instrumental violence. compared vith 28<br />

percent classified as "expressive violence" (acts carried<br />

out to cause pain or injury as an end in itself).%<br />

violence in these books is portrayed overwhelmingly as<br />

usef "1.<br />

E second type <strong>of</strong> evidence against the theory that the<br />

violence is a vehicle for conveying moral disapproval <strong>of</strong><br />

violsnce is shown in the outcomes. Of the 171 instrumental<br />

violent acts that could be coded for outcome, 60 percent<br />

were depicted as achieving the desired outcome.<br />

Third, WE classified each act according to whether the<br />

book's author portrayed it as legitimate or illegitimate.<br />

Of the 261 acts that could be coded 03 this dimensioc. 48<br />

percmt were presented as socially legitimate acts.<br />

Pinslly, additional insight is gained when the<br />

instrumentality and the legitimacy dimensions are<br />

cross-classified, revealing that most <strong>of</strong> the acts <strong>of</strong><br />

instrumental violence (55 perc9nt) were depicted as socially<br />

legitimate whereas "orly" 28 percent <strong>of</strong> the expressive acts<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence were depicted as legitimate. Thus. when<br />

violence is portrayed as a means <strong>of</strong> achievement, it tends to<br />

be giren the stamp <strong>of</strong> social approval by the authors <strong>of</strong><br />

these books. Bnt when it is portrayed as an expression <strong>of</strong><br />

emotion, it is depicted as illegitimate. We suggest that<br />

this relationship represents the combination <strong>of</strong> the<br />

historically important emphasis on achievement in Rmsrican<br />

soclety coming tcgether vith the nations1 heritage <strong>of</strong><br />

violence.<br />

Overall, the eridsncc suggests tha't the high frequency<br />

<strong>of</strong> VL~~EDCI 51 children's books is not part <strong>of</strong> a social<br />

control process restrictirg violence.<br />

storiss is typically carried out<br />

<strong>The</strong> violence in these<br />

to achieve some ecd or<br />

solve scme prcblen: it is usoally successful: end, when<br />

used for such lnstrunertal purposes. it is most <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

depicted as <strong>Social</strong>ly legitimate. Thus, to the extent that<br />

children's books are a means <strong>of</strong> social control and<br />

socialization, thep contribute to the<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence in American society.<br />

institutionalization<br />

- a vocabnlm <strong>of</strong> notives. Even if these books are not<br />

part <strong>of</strong> a process by which the society exposes and labels<br />

violence as a deviant act. other social control - and ~~~~ -<br />

<strong>Social</strong>ization functions in relation to violence are not<br />

ruled out. Ue have suggested just the opposite--that these<br />

hooks play an important role in labeling violence as<br />

legitimate and in teaching the socially appropriate


ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> ir Children's Books Page 59<br />

occasions for its use.<br />

As Bandura (1973) shows. aggression and violence are,<br />

for the nos7 part, socially scripted behavior. Among the<br />

nost important elements <strong>of</strong> the script for violence taughr in<br />

these children's books are "motives" or reasons that<br />

communicate to the child society's definition <strong>of</strong> the<br />

occasions on vhich violence may be used. Our initial<br />

analysis made use <strong>of</strong> 34 categories, some <strong>of</strong> which sere<br />

predetermined and the remainder added as we came across<br />

reasons that did not fit the categories. <strong>The</strong>se 34<br />

categories then were groupea unaer six major heaaings.<br />

In designing the study, we f~lt that violence in<br />

children's books Often would be presented as a msans <strong>of</strong><br />

punishing or preventing socially disapproved behavior,<br />

especially on the part <strong>of</strong> children. To allow this hunch a<br />

fair opportunity to be proved or disproved, we combined all<br />

codirg categories that could be considered violence used to<br />

enforce social norms or values. we included any indication<br />

that the purpose <strong>of</strong> violence was to enforce a legitimate<br />

authority: to punish violations <strong>of</strong> aesthetic norms (table<br />

manners, etc.). Jack <strong>of</strong> thrift, lying, stupidity,<br />

wickedness, or greed; cr to promote the general triumph <strong>of</strong><br />

good over evil. <strong>The</strong> "<strong>Social</strong> Controlw category in the<br />

accompanying tabulation shows that all these instances came<br />

to Only 18 percent <strong>of</strong> the total number <strong>of</strong> violent acts.<br />

TABLE 1. NOST COMEON REASONS FOR II<strong>IT</strong>IATIBG VIOLENCE<br />

-----<br />

Reason X<br />

(N=264)<br />

Goal Blockage or Frustration 22<br />

Emotional States 22<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Csntrcl<br />

Self-Def ense<br />

18<br />

18<br />

War . .<br />

10<br />

No Apparent<br />

Other<br />

<strong>The</strong> low percentage <strong>of</strong> violence for social control does<br />

not come about because any other rationale dominates the<br />

portrayal <strong>of</strong> violence in these books. In fact. the two<br />

categories that share top place, "Goal Blockage or<br />

Prustration" ana "Emotional States,' inclnae only about 22<br />

percent each <strong>of</strong> the total Cases. <strong>The</strong> Goal Blockage category<br />

includes using violence to remove an obstacle: for example,<br />

to femove a barrier to the satisfaction <strong>of</strong> hunger, to attack<br />

a person blocking attainment <strong>of</strong> a goal. to assert one's


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in child re^'^ Books Page 60<br />

pouer in general, <strong>The</strong> other top category, "Enctioral<br />

States." includes violence motivated by some strong emotion<br />

such as shame or humiliation. revenge, or rage over having<br />

been insulted.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Control. self-Defense, and War categories<br />

combined come to 46 percent <strong>of</strong> the motives Jr reasons for<br />

v~olence. Thus, the types <strong>of</strong> violence for which a moral<br />

case can be argued are slightly less frequent than the<br />

combined frequency <strong>of</strong> violence to attain some other end,<br />

gratuitous violence. or violence as a result <strong>of</strong> an esotional<br />

state. Clearly, if these are morality Tales, an important<br />

part <strong>of</strong> the moral code being communicated is the "eye for an<br />

eye, tooth for a tooth' aspect <strong>of</strong> the Old Testament. By and<br />

large, these conclosions apply over the entire perioa from<br />

1850 to 1970- Roserer, the <strong>Social</strong> Control category tended<br />

to be more common prior to 1930. In addition. one <strong>of</strong> the<br />

subcategories occurred only in the period 1955 to<br />

1970--violence used to punish the "stupidity" <strong>of</strong> othsrs.<br />

Perhaps the increasing burraucratization <strong>of</strong> modarn society<br />

and the attendant denards for rationality, so well described<br />

by Weber (1964). leads to depicting "sheer stupidity"<br />

(Irrationality) as one <strong>of</strong> the more serious moral<br />

transgressions <strong>of</strong> cur time.<br />

C ~ i t a Punishment. l<br />

Pnother aspect <strong>of</strong> the vocabulary<br />

<strong>of</strong> motives contained in these books concerns vinlent aeath.<br />

<strong>The</strong> fact that someone was killed in 33 percent <strong>of</strong> the<br />

book-segments provides an opportunity to gain insight into<br />

the social definition <strong>of</strong> killing and death that is presented<br />

to children. Rn important aspect <strong>of</strong> this depiction comes to<br />

light because the death <strong>of</strong>ten occurs because the victim has<br />

committed some moral wrccg or crime. That is, although the<br />

terms "capital punishment" and "death penalty" are not used,<br />

these hooks graphically describe use <strong>of</strong> the death penalty.<br />

And, as Bruno Bettelhoiin (1973) says about "...those great<br />

American folk heroes, <strong>The</strong> Three Lirsp P=gsn (in uhtch the<br />

big bad wolf is boiled alive for blowing the house down):<br />

'Children lore the story ... But the important lesson<br />

underlying the e'joyment and<br />

captures their attcntion."*l<br />

drama <strong>of</strong> the story equally<br />

we felt that the books sampled contained many such<br />

examples <strong>of</strong> ths iuplicit use <strong>of</strong> the death penalty. To check<br />

this, re crosstabulated the variable indicating deatt or<br />

other injory with the act that precipitated the violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results show that capital punish men^ (the killing <strong>of</strong> a<br />

character who committed a moral transgression or crime)<br />

occurred in 22 percent <strong>of</strong> the instances ir which a character<br />

died. Since these are happenings <strong>of</strong> gzeat dramatic<br />

intensity, what Bettelheim calls "the important lesson<br />

underlying the Enjoyment and drama <strong>of</strong> the story" is likely<br />

to make e strong impression on the child's mird. It is not<br />

at all far-fetched to suggest that this literary background<br />

is part <strong>of</strong> the basis for the widespread, seemingly


ch. 4. Vlolence in Children's Books Page 61<br />

irrational commitment to the death penalty Og so nany<br />

mericans (see Gelles and Straus. 1975).<br />

RRCE, SEX, hND PBUILY<br />

Race. <strong>The</strong> racial identification <strong>of</strong> violent characters<br />

----<br />

in these books and their victiss does not shov any striking<br />

devzation from the composition <strong>of</strong> the United States<br />

population. Of the initiators <strong>of</strong> aggrassire acts, 80<br />

percent were white. 9 percent black, 1 percent oriental, 7<br />

percent Indian, and 8 percent "other.' <strong>The</strong> distribution for<br />

victims <strong>of</strong> these zggressive acts is approximately the sane<br />

(79, 7, 2. 5, and 6 percent).


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Eooks Page 62<br />

Sex. nost <strong>of</strong> the violence in this sample <strong>of</strong> books took<br />

place--~etveen men. uinety-one percent <strong>of</strong> the aggressors<br />

were male, as were 86 percent <strong>of</strong> the victims. Thus.<br />

violence in these hooks is overwhelmingly depicted as a male<br />

actlvity. If the period covered by this study is one<br />

showing a gradual movement toward sexual equality, this<br />

trend should be reflected ir a gradual increase in the<br />

proportion <strong>of</strong> female characters who engage in "masculine"<br />

acts <strong>of</strong> all types, including aggression. <strong>The</strong> lower line <strong>of</strong><br />

Figure 2 does indicate just such a trend.<br />

Although the proportion <strong>of</strong> vomeu aggressors is growing,<br />

there are many ups and downs in Figure 2. Gecas (1972) did<br />

not find any such trend over time in a study <strong>of</strong> adult<br />

magazine fiction, and it may be that the "trend" re observed<br />

reflects a coinciderce cf random fluctuations. Arguing<br />

against this possibility is the fact that the correlation <strong>of</strong><br />

0.45 has a probability <strong>of</strong> chance occurrence <strong>of</strong> less than<br />

0.05 when measured over 25 time periods. In addition.<br />

Pigure 2 suggests a cyclical pattern within the general<br />

trend. A spectral analysis (Dixon, 1965) was therefore<br />

carried out to determine if a dependable cyclical pattern<br />

could be seen in the time series; this analysis revealed a<br />

cycle <strong>of</strong> four time periods (20 years), accounting for 38<br />

percent <strong>of</strong> the variance.<br />

In the absance <strong>of</strong> other information, it is difficult to<br />

interpret this 20-year cycle. One possibility is t hat a<br />

child reading these books at age 10 would have reached<br />

maturity and perhaps vould he writing books <strong>of</strong> his 3r her<br />

ovn in 20 years. That generation's authors might then tend<br />

to produce works <strong>of</strong> fiction influenced by the depiction <strong>of</strong><br />

female chlzactsrs in the books they had read as a child.<br />

But whatever the explanation. the fact that this cyclical<br />

pattern. in combination vith the upward secular trend,<br />

accourts for 58 percent <strong>of</strong> the variance in the proportion <strong>of</strong><br />

female aggressors suggests that more than chance factors are<br />

shown in Figure 2. In addition. the increase in female<br />

aggressors is consistent vith an increase in the proportion<br />

<strong>of</strong> wcmen arrested for various crimes, especially violent<br />

crimes (Roberts. 1971) .*8<br />

Adults --<br />

Versus Children. On the basis <strong>of</strong> actual and<br />

pctential injuriousness, and in terms <strong>of</strong> the purposes<br />

depicted, we suggested that the violent incidents in these<br />

books are not "kid stuff.' <strong>The</strong> most direct evidence is that<br />

80 percent <strong>of</strong> the initiators <strong>of</strong> violent acts vere cdults.<br />

HOr was this figure <strong>of</strong>ten mitigated by ase <strong>of</strong> an animal or<br />

other nonhuman characters: 80 percent <strong>of</strong> the aggressors<br />

vere humzn characters. <strong>The</strong> image presented to children in<br />

these hooks is <strong>of</strong> adults being physically violent.<br />

Boreover, the percentage <strong>of</strong> acts in which the aggressor is<br />

an adult has gradually been increasing over the 120-year<br />

span <strong>of</strong> this study, as the upper line shows in Pigure 2.


ch.4. violence in Children-s Books Page 63<br />

<strong>The</strong> preponderance <strong>of</strong> adult figures as physical<br />

aggresso:s is an instance <strong>of</strong> fiction that does cot mirror<br />

reality, not because adults in our society are not--;iolent.<br />

bnt because children zg. Pushing, shoving, hitting, and<br />

pbysical fighting are more common among children than among<br />

adolts, rather than the reverse.89 This reversal may<br />

represent one <strong>of</strong> the mp-hs concerning violeuce in Rmerican<br />

middle-class society. namely, that violence is approved only<br />

when its end is seen as socially worthy or valusd, as<br />

punishment <strong>of</strong> wrongdoers or npreventative' air raids.*lO<br />

-<br />

P&Ly <strong>Violence</strong>. Another myth that these books<br />

transmit is the notion that physical violence between family<br />

members is rare. <strong>The</strong>re is a wide discrepancy betveen the<br />

-- idealized victure - <strong>of</strong> ~- the ~ -. familr -- a as -- a - orauo ..- .= committea ..-- -. ..- tn ..<br />

nonviolence hetween its members and "hat actually goes on.<br />

<strong>The</strong> available evidence suggests that violence is typical <strong>of</strong><br />

family relations (Steinmetz and Straus. 1973: Straus,<br />

1974b: Straus. Gelles, and Steinmetz. 1979). In childhood.<br />

the persons most likely to strike a child are siblings and<br />

parents. In adulthood. the victim <strong>of</strong> assault or murder is<br />

most likely a family member. As shovn in Chapter 3.<br />

informal norms. largely unverbalized, make a marriage<br />

license a hitting license (see Gelles, 1974; Schulz, 1969).<br />

Nevertheless, in these children's books, 91 percent <strong>of</strong><br />

the vlclent incidents take place betveen persons who are ggt<br />

related. Two percert <strong>of</strong> the violence was by fathers and two<br />

percent by mothers. In only a single incident did a husband<br />

hit a wife and in none was the aggressor a wife or<br />

grandparent. <strong>The</strong>se findings are parallel to those revealed<br />

ic an informal search for instances <strong>of</strong> husband-wife violence<br />

in 20 r~orels for adults (steinmetz and straus, 1974).<br />

<strong>The</strong> absence cf husband-vife violence in adult fiction<br />

and the virtual absence <strong>of</strong> any intrafamily violence in<br />

children's flction calls for an explanation, especially<br />

since so much contemporary fiction attempts to show reality<br />

in grim detail. although these data do not permit a<br />

z cf~nltive answer, re suggest that the following processes<br />

may be at work.<br />

First is a process <strong>of</strong> social control. <strong>The</strong> society does<br />

have a commitment to familial nonviolence, even though it<br />

exists side by side vith more covert norms permitting and<br />

encouraging intrafanily vlolence (Chapter 3). Thus, the<br />

cultural representations <strong>of</strong> society tend to portray families<br />

in a way that vill not encourage peoplE to violate this<br />

norm.<br />

second is a process <strong>of</strong> s%G& coQsfzEi&g f Eg&&&.<br />

<strong>The</strong> society must have its members define the fanily as a<br />

place <strong>of</strong> love and gentleness rather than a place <strong>of</strong> violence<br />

because <strong>of</strong> the tremendous importance <strong>of</strong> securing conaitnent<br />

to the family as a social group. <strong>The</strong> ayth <strong>of</strong> family


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Eooks Page 6U<br />

nonviolence is one <strong>of</strong> the many ways that the institution <strong>of</strong><br />

the family is strengthened and supported. It helps<br />

encourage people to marry and to stay married despite thc<br />

actual stresses <strong>of</strong> family interaction.<br />

Third, most speculatively. the ~pf;& a_f fagu<br />

nonviolence discourages members <strong>of</strong> the intellectoal elite,<br />

whether novelists or sociologists, from probing into thir<br />

aspect <strong>of</strong> the family. We have all been brought up on thir<br />

literature and even read it as adults. Apparently. havinc<br />

accepted the literature's basic premises, novelists avoie<br />

writing about physical violence between family nsmbers, and<br />

sociologists have practiced "selective inattention" tt<br />

research on this aspect <strong>of</strong> the family (Steinnetz and Straus,<br />

19741.<br />

SUBBARY BND CONCLUSIONS<br />

Our study <strong>of</strong> 125 'classic" Or "recommended' children's<br />

books pnblished from 1850 to 1970 revealed an extremely high<br />

incidence <strong>of</strong> physical violence. Almost all the books<br />

described one or more acts <strong>of</strong> actual or threatened violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> typical children's book can be expected to have one<br />

violent incident, a third <strong>of</strong> which is lethal, for every<br />

seven pages <strong>of</strong> text. <strong>The</strong>re was no general increase or<br />

decr9ase over the 120 years studied. However, the portrayal<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence tended to be h~gh during periods in which the<br />

Society was engaged in war, and to bs low during periods <strong>of</strong><br />

economic difficulty.<br />

if the depiction <strong>of</strong> violence were construed as a<br />

vehicle to express societal disapproval <strong>of</strong> violence. it<br />

would be presected as evil. and the psrpetuators <strong>of</strong> violence<br />

uculd be :ariahcC. Tte opposlte serms TO be the care. For<br />

exlmpl*. 'r:strumertaln v:oleicr was frsqusnt, 3rd 7yp;callp<br />

zerulied LL tte a:tllr.m€~t 3f the aggressor's >,lrpos?.<br />

Nevertheless, the evidence suggests that these books do<br />

have important functions <strong>of</strong> socialization and social<br />

control. <strong>The</strong>y provide scripts and role models through which<br />

generations <strong>of</strong> young Bmericans have learned how to behave<br />

violently. Among the elements <strong>of</strong> these complex scripts thzt<br />

must be learned are the motives that one can legitimately<br />

invoke to justify violence, the kinds <strong>of</strong> persons who can be<br />

violent and against whoa violence is permissible, the level<br />

<strong>of</strong> socially acceptable injury, and the Emotions that are<br />

approprlate or required or the part <strong>of</strong> the aggressor (for<br />

example, joy or remorse) and the victim (for examFle, rage,<br />

tears, or humiliation). A l l <strong>of</strong> these elements and their<br />

COnPlex interrelations are depicted for the child in this<br />

sample <strong>of</strong> books.


Ch.4, <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 65<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> between family members is a major exception to<br />

the conclusion that children's books provide a script for<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>. Vlolence within the family was rarely p<strong>of</strong>trayed,<br />

reflecting the social mythology <strong>of</strong> familial nonviolence.<br />

he myth <strong>of</strong> family nonviolence may ;n turn reflect the high<br />

stake that society has in securing ana maintaining<br />

comeitment to the family as a social group.<br />

NOTES<br />

*Paper presented at the 1975 annual msoticg <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Eastern Sociological Society. This rese=rch was partly<br />

supported by National Ir~stitute <strong>of</strong> flental Sealth grant<br />

nomber 18-15521 am3 by a summer fellowship awarded to the<br />

senior author by the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> New Eampshire Graduate<br />

school. We would like to express our appreciation to Paul<br />

Kaplac for his work on coding the 125 books, to Loren Cobb<br />

for assistance with the spectral analysis, and to Paul Drew,<br />

Arnold Linsky. Stuart Palmer, and Donna Peltz for valuable<br />

comments and criticisms on an earlier draft.<br />

1. Assuming that literature does influence society, a<br />

content analysis hy itself can only indicate the nature <strong>of</strong><br />

the message. It does not provide data on the intensity <strong>of</strong><br />

the influence or on the specific sectors <strong>of</strong> the population<br />

that are most, least, or not at all influenced. In a<br />

complex modern society, both the intensity and the extent <strong>of</strong><br />

irfluance are highly problematic, for the same reasons that<br />

"funcYiOna1 iotegratiol" in general is problematic in such<br />

socisties (Cohen, iY69: 151-156).<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> folloving supplenentary material is available on<br />

request: (1) List <strong>of</strong> books analyzed. (2) List <strong>of</strong> book<br />

lists used to locate books for each <strong>of</strong> the fire year<br />

periods. (3) ccdf and cote sheet used in the content<br />

analysis.<br />

3. For 193 <strong>of</strong> the books, we were able to obtain<br />

information on the apprcximate age-<strong>of</strong>-child range for which<br />

the book was considered suitable. We coqed the aidpoint <strong>of</strong><br />

th9 age range for each book. <strong>The</strong>se median ages rangsd from<br />

five books with a recoamerded age <strong>of</strong> six years to one book<br />

for 16 year 016s. <strong>The</strong> mean <strong>of</strong> the median ayes was 10.8<br />

years and t h mode ~ was 10 years (26 percont <strong>of</strong> the cases).<br />

Y. Although ue coded only acts <strong>of</strong> interpersonal<br />

violence that occurred on the pages drawn in the sample, we<br />

read as much <strong>of</strong> the rest <strong>of</strong> the book as nec3sssry to<br />

determine such things as the social characteristics <strong>of</strong> the<br />

actors and their motives. E copy <strong>of</strong> the detailed content<br />

analysis code may be obtained from the National Auxiliary<br />

Publications Service. See footnote 1.


Ch.9. Vlolence in Childrec's Books Page 66<br />

TWO different coders made the content analysis. E test<br />

<strong>of</strong> the reliability <strong>of</strong> the content analysis procedure was<br />

carried out three days after the actual coding had begun.<br />

A l l books coded that day were done by both coders. For the<br />

360 coaings compared (10 books, 36 variables per book),<br />

there was an 87 percent agreement.<br />

TO prevent differences between coders from influencing<br />

the trend analysis. each coder analyzed only two or three <strong>of</strong><br />

the hooks for a given year. '<strong>The</strong>refore, possible "drift" or<br />

changes in coding standards that might have occurred as the<br />

coding proceeded would not bias the time series analysis.<br />

5. None <strong>of</strong> the hooks published during the peak years<br />

were "mar stories." In addition, little <strong>of</strong> the violence<br />

portrayed in any <strong>of</strong> these books is the killing or wounding<br />

<strong>of</strong> an enemy soldier.<br />

6. Our coding categories were y erpr=sslve.<br />

versus =*I;& rr.strums%tll. srr.ce b ~ t h coap0ler.ts nay be<br />

orassnc. SEE - .- S-laus. Fellas. a ~ d Ster?.ae-z 11973) for 3<br />

~ ..--.. -. -.~- ~ - - ~ - . ~. -<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> this and related issues in identifying types<br />

<strong>of</strong> ~ ~ violence. <strong>The</strong> ~~. cadino <strong>of</strong> esch act as either ~ ~<br />

> orimarilp<br />

~~<br />

icstruasctal or exprsssrse was carried out sspara:ely from<br />

the codrcg ot such variables as ttn specifrc raasors for<br />

lLl?ia:ina V:O~E~CP (SEE Table 11 and there 1s iherPf3re 2<br />

~A -~ ~ ,~ ~<br />

small discrepancy between the two variables. If the tvo<br />

noninstrumental categories aresubtracted from Tahle 1, this<br />

produces 71 rather than 72 percent instrumental acts.<br />

7. Rettelheim was referring to teaching the work-ethic<br />

i this quotatior, but we feel it is equally applicable to<br />

the violence-ethic that is also presented.<br />

e. Of course. as those familiar with crime statistics<br />

realize, this does not necessarily mean that women have<br />

engeged in more violent +cts. Changes in the social<br />

3afinition <strong>of</strong> women siailar to those occurring in these<br />

children's hooks may also characterize the perception <strong>of</strong><br />

women by the pclice and public prosecutors, leading to an<br />

increase in arrest rate rather than an increase in incidence<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence.<br />

9. Eowever. if the unit <strong>of</strong> violence is homicide or<br />

those assaults that enter the <strong>of</strong>ficial statistics, then the<br />

peak age is the middle to late twenties.<br />

10. Esssrting that this is one way in which children's<br />

literature reflects ideal rather than actual social patterns<br />

points up the weakness <strong>of</strong> the "dialectical interplay"<br />

theory, namely. that it is untestable: nothing can refute<br />

it. Correspondence can be claimed as an instance <strong>of</strong> support<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 'freflection'l process and a discrepancy can he claimed<br />

as part <strong>of</strong> the *influence" process. Neverthaless, as Cohen<br />

(1969:6) notes. untestable theories can have hsuristic


Ch.4. <strong>Violence</strong> in Children's Books Page 67<br />

value. In the present caso it sensitizes us to finding<br />

instances that, in our judgment, reflect one or the other <strong>of</strong><br />

thase two processes and to speculate abwut the underlying<br />

reasons. If these speculations point to isportant social<br />

processes, It can be said that the theory has heuristic<br />

value, especially so if it leads to sobseqoent research to<br />

test these speculations.


Chapter 5<br />

A Cultural-Consistent! <strong>The</strong>oy <strong>of</strong><br />

Family <strong>Violence</strong> in Mexican-American<br />

and Jewish-Ethnic Groups<br />

Joseph C. Carroll<br />

up tc this point we have been co3sidering<br />

cultoral norms that deal directly with violexce.<br />

Rowever, this chapter suggests that pven<br />

cilltural corms that do 2% have a marifest<br />

reference to violerce also affect the level <strong>of</strong><br />

violence. ?or example, norms Day structure<br />

family rolcs in a way that iccrezsss tsnsion an4<br />

hostility in the family, even though that is cot<br />

what is intended. Carroll argues that the<br />

elements 02 a culture tend to be interaspendent.<br />

Hs applies this "cultural consistercy" theory t3<br />

nexican-Saerican and Jewish-American families.<br />

For example, Carroll concludes that i n<br />

mexican-lm~rican families, norms call for male<br />

dominance in husband-wife r9la:ionships and<br />

father dominance in pareat-child relatiocships,<br />

whereby it is not legitimate for a wife or child<br />

to contest the husband or fsther, are<br />

systematically 1:IkeZ to a high lsvsl <strong>of</strong><br />

violence. In Jewish families, it is nct<br />

illegitimate to argue with one's husband, uifs.<br />

or father. Conflicts arc not settled on the<br />

basis <strong>of</strong> ascribed power, but or. the basis oE<br />

discussion and krovledge (either scriptural or<br />

scientific). To the extent that this ideal is<br />

folloved, corflicts can be settled without<br />

resorting to violecce.<br />

while this chapter examines only two ethnic<br />

Snbcultures, it is a promising beginning to the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> a typology <strong>of</strong> family subcultursl<br />

norms and their role in permitting or<br />

discouragir.g the use <strong>of</strong> violence as a means <strong>of</strong><br />

coxflict resolutioc.


~h.5. Cultural Consistency Page 69<br />

<strong>The</strong> coxcept ci the subcultore <strong>of</strong> violence rpfprs to<br />

norms that deal directly with the extent to vhich violpnce<br />

may be used era the conditions under vhich violence is<br />

permissible. Eowever, this conceptualization does no* deal<br />

with the questlan <strong>of</strong> how such norms and values con= into<br />

beicg and why they persist as cultural patterns. One<br />

explanation <strong>of</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> these cul+ural patLerns<br />

asserts that specific cultural elements reflect the<br />

cperation <strong>of</strong> the culture 3s = '~=fgg. White suggests that<br />

cultnre may have systemic properties, in that the structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> culture consists <strong>of</strong> integrated components rela'sd by<br />

cause and effect (1975:36). Thus. norms concerning ~F?lence<br />

tend to reflect and he cgnsistent with tke values<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong> a group.<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this chapter is to develop such a<br />

cultlral-consist=rcy theory <strong>of</strong> violence and to apply this<br />

theory to Mexican-Bmericar and Jewish ethnic groups. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

groups were selected because they seemed to have markedly<br />

different values and norms, allowing for examination <strong>of</strong> the<br />

th40ry from two diffsrect standpoints. <strong>The</strong> two groups may<br />

be Seen as extremes on a cultural value continuum on which<br />

other ethric groups may be placed and compared. Dther<br />

theories besides cultoral consistsncy assert that a colture<br />

reflects the typical pers0r.ality <strong>of</strong> z society's menbers or<br />

that a cultnre reflects the pressurPs and liaitations<br />

inherent in the organization <strong>of</strong> a society. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

explanations are also important for a full uraerstanding <strong>of</strong><br />

a cultur;, but they are omitted from the present chapter to<br />

allow room to adequately develop a cultural-coosistency<br />

theory.<br />

A CULTURBL-CORSISTEICY TEEOBY OF VIOLEBCB<br />

Sicce the goal <strong>of</strong> this chapter is ta axplaio the causs<br />

<strong>of</strong> family violecce in certain ethnic groups in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

cultural consistency, we must first lefine what we mrar by a<br />

subculture. Rccordirg to Wolfgang, subcolturss arisr vhan<br />

not all <strong>of</strong> the valu~s, beliefs, or norms in a society are<br />

given equal status by all groups. Subcultural groups may<br />

partially accept or deny elements <strong>of</strong> the central or dominant<br />

values. yet remain within the cultursl systam (1967:99).<br />

Thus a group <strong>of</strong> people, for example, an ethnic group, may<br />

share values and norms regarding family life tkat ars not<br />

identical to those gererally accepted by the wider so-iety.<br />

In our definition, values for family life refer to hasic<br />

values <strong>of</strong> the athcic group rather than to those that deal<br />

explicitly with violerce. Examples <strong>of</strong> more basic ValuSs are<br />

those <strong>of</strong> power relations assigned by age and sex, or the<br />

practice <strong>of</strong> religion.


Ch.5. Cultural Consistency Page 70<br />

llthough they are not concerned with specific eLhric<br />

groups. Parsons and Shils' discussion <strong>of</strong> the consistency <strong>of</strong><br />

cultural patterns is relevant. <strong>The</strong>y state:<br />

Cultural patterns tend to become organize3 into<br />

Systems. <strong>The</strong> peculiar feature <strong>of</strong> this<br />

systematization is a type <strong>of</strong> integration which<br />

we may call consistency <strong>of</strong> pat?ern (1953:Zl).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Corsistency <strong>of</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> such a system w i l l<br />

exsst to the extent to vhich the same<br />

combination <strong>of</strong> value judgments.. .runs<br />

Ccnsistentlp throughont the actors' responses to<br />

different situatiors: that is, to a differen?<br />

class <strong>of</strong> objects, different objects in the sane<br />

class, and the same Objects on different<br />

occasions (1953: 172).<br />

In a cultural system, as the 5erm is used above.<br />

Orientations toward a particular ronsocial or social object<br />

are interdependent. <strong>Social</strong> objects may be individuals or<br />

collectivities (1953:s). Culcural patterns refer to the<br />

extent to which systems <strong>of</strong> ideas or bsliefs, and systems <strong>of</strong><br />

value oriertations are shared by members <strong>of</strong> a culture.<br />

According to Parsons and Shils, the problem for the<br />

studsnt <strong>of</strong> culture is to determine the value judgments that<br />

lead to cmsistency <strong>of</strong> patterns.<br />

In order therefore to determine the existence <strong>of</strong><br />

Systematic coherence where there has not been<br />

explicit systeaatizatiol, if is necessary frr<br />

the student <strong>of</strong> cultare tc nzcover the *xylicit<br />

Culture 2nd to detect whatever common premises<br />

may underlie apparently divers? 3r.d unconlectea<br />

Items <strong>of</strong> orientatiol (1953:22).<br />

<strong>The</strong> positior taken in this chapter ie that each sthric<br />

group shares a set <strong>of</strong> "commoa premises" thaL constitute the<br />

source <strong>of</strong> mcmbers' actiorl ir all areas <strong>of</strong> life. <strong>The</strong>s? are<br />

the basic values <strong>of</strong> the group. Thns, to arrive at the<br />

actual level <strong>of</strong> family violence, we must examine the basic<br />

family values <strong>of</strong> the ethnic group.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is little doubt that norms for falily viol~ncs<br />

do<br />

exist in some ethric groups. Lewis (1960) found th+t severe<br />

punishment was traditional in Tepoztlan. Parents believed<br />

in early punishment that began about the time s child<br />

learned to walk, although the most severe punishmen: <strong>of</strong><br />

childrel occurred between the ages <strong>of</strong> five and twslrs.<br />

Lewis reported that although puxishment in this village had<br />

become less severe. it was not uncommon for fathers to beat<br />

their children with a stick or rcpe.


Ch.5. Cultural Corsistency<br />

In years past family<br />

bounds oi the law.<br />

Page 71<br />

violence was even within 'he<br />

canon Law in years past accepted wife-beating as<br />

a fair means <strong>of</strong> keeping a spouse in ordsr. I<br />

hundred Years aao ~~< 5% -~ was an unouertioned - --~- ~ oattern<br />

in many familles, due in pert to the lack <strong>of</strong><br />

Status <strong>of</strong> a woman. and in Dart to their - - chattel<br />

value in a marriage [Pennell, l974:lS).<br />

<strong>The</strong> final question to be discussed in the development<br />

<strong>of</strong> a theory <strong>of</strong> eChnic family viclsnce is how the norms <strong>of</strong> a<br />

group remain strorg from generation to generation. wolfgaxg<br />

believes that learning processes perpetuate the snbcultural<br />

norms for violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> development cf favorable attitudes tcvard,<br />

and the use <strong>of</strong>. violence in a subculture usually<br />

involve learned behavJor and a process <strong>of</strong><br />

differential learning, association. or<br />

identification (1967: 160).<br />

Since imitation is a powerful learning process (Baadura,<br />

1973). one could say that the greater the amount <strong>of</strong> violence<br />

in parents' behavior toward a child and toward each other.<br />

the greater the chance that the child w i l l sct violently<br />

late; in life (see chapter 12, section D).<br />

A t this point it may be appropriate to assess the idea<br />

that one's culture has a great deal to d3 with the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> personality. Perhaps in viewing culturr as a<br />

cause <strong>of</strong> persorality, we see only half cf the picture.<br />

Personality may have a lot to do with the developmene <strong>of</strong><br />

culr'lrs. Yicqer (1965) has stressed ttls p-lzr i n .n<br />

rxamlnarlor ot vherher -he sociocultural system aftocts<br />

:~dis:duaI actlor cr wherher :r.divi?lal act_?> iffscrs +h*<br />

SOoiOcultural system. or both. Be calls for a more careful<br />

specificaclor <strong>of</strong> causal relatiorships in studies <strong>of</strong> culture<br />

and states that tke relationship between cultural nxms and<br />

personality is reciprocal. In other woras, "individual<br />

responses to and interpretations <strong>of</strong> culture lea3 to<br />

normative variation" (1965:82). Also, Kardiner (1963) has<br />

reported that although culture and persorility changa each<br />

other in a cyclic process, institutional charge tends 50 be<br />

followed by personality change. <strong>The</strong> follomirg diagram<br />

illustrates the reciprccal nature <strong>of</strong> the relationship<br />

between culture1 values, norms. and personality.


Ch.5. Cultural consistency Page 72<br />

Exircme male domi- Old over young ihc micrcr the dii- Chiidicn 5houId icdrn<br />

nance (Caiiilo-Bcron, (Penaioid) ciplinc the be!!^ the to hc iubmiirive and<br />

Fermndei-Marina hii id (cnrilIo.Beron, obedient to rhc father<br />

el ai., Lewis, Madscn Ramirez) (Fcinnndez-Marina ci a$.)<br />

Penaloral<br />

Norms:<br />

I<br />

Mother iuppoits<br />

Fathci tends to<br />

father's authority -.c-----favordaughter<br />

(Fernandei-Marina (Penalom, Lewis)<br />

el a!.. Lewis]<br />

Uistsnce and severity in rather-ion icla!ioni<br />

[Pcnalosa, Lewis)<br />

I<br />

+<br />

.1<br />

coercive power<br />

I<br />

Low degiec <strong>of</strong> referent power (Lewis1<br />

4<br />

Son's fear <strong>of</strong> the father (Fernandcr-Marina cr al., Madsen)<br />

I<br />

i'erpetuoiian: I. Majority oi hoyi deqire 4 to be itim . thcir . lather, even<br />

though they fear him (Feinundci-Marina el ail<br />

1.<br />

2.Child ai aduit treats his wrc and children thcramc<br />

way his father treated him IPenaIod<br />

Figure 1. Mexican-American paicnt-child violence


ch.5. Cultural Consistency Page 73<br />

In movins erom these general prrnciples to specific<br />

ethnic groups. Our first step was to collect an invectarv <strong>of</strong><br />

family values and noras from-the literature or each sthcic<br />

group. Family values were defined as "generally tccspted<br />

esplrations and ideals which are publicly sanctisned"<br />

(Kardiner, 1963). and which are concerned with intrafsmily<br />

relatioFshipS. Norm axe see2 as more specific rules<br />

governing behavior. arising from ths values 3nd more closely<br />

licked to violence. For example, a basic value in<br />

Ksxican-American ethric groups would be the dominaocs <strong>of</strong> old<br />

over young, whereas a norm would be the severity <strong>of</strong><br />

parent-child relations.<br />

Nsxt, WE attempted to develop hppotheses concerning<br />

causal links betveen the family values and the aorns for<br />

ViolencE or nonviolence. <strong>The</strong> final step was to find<br />

child-rearirg norms in ths literature that seem to<br />

perpetuate behavior in Each ethnic gronp.*l<br />

A11 <strong>of</strong> the basic family values in Figure 1 are<br />

concer2ea with the all-encompassing authoritarian rale which<br />

the older male possesses in nexican-lrncrican families.*? <strong>The</strong><br />

fundamental incompatibility betveen great zuthority and<br />

intimacy suggests that the four basic family values prcaote<br />

distant and severs father-chila relations. Yirazae (1977)<br />

reported that father-child relations do tend r3 b5 so~e~hat<br />

distant. Lewis (1960) suggested that distance may be<br />

manifested through a gen+ral lack <strong>of</strong> affective relations as<br />

fathsrs pay lass attention to children as they grow older.<br />

An example <strong>of</strong> severlty would be that children are <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

severely pnrished for an <strong>of</strong>fense such as lying (Penalosa,<br />

lY68:685). Penalosa and Levis both report that relations<br />

between fathers and daughters tend not to be as distact or<br />

Severe as father-son rolatiocs. Fathers map show a mild<br />

form <strong>of</strong> favoritism to daaghters and be more protective and<br />

possessive.<br />

Implicit in the above parrgraph is the follaving<br />

proposition relatizg any cne <strong>of</strong> these basic family values to<br />

more speclfic norms.


Ch.5. Cultural Cons-stency Page 79<br />

1. TO the extent that ths nale poss~sses s great<br />

amount ot authorrty in faally llfe, the sor?<br />

w l l l that culture be characterrzed by %=start<br />

and severe father-son relations.<br />

In the cultural-consistency theory for nexican-American<br />

families an important relationship exists betveen ths norm<br />

<strong>of</strong> distant. severs father-son relations and the actual ose<br />

Of violence. <strong>The</strong> severe father-son relations may lead to<br />

the son being afraid <strong>of</strong> his father; the son's fear<br />

contributes to tha father's exercising <strong>of</strong> control through<br />

his temper and violent outbursts.<br />

How does the SOP'S fear <strong>of</strong> the father lead t3 the<br />

display <strong>of</strong> temper and violent outbursts as a meacs <strong>of</strong><br />

control? Apparertly, the presence <strong>of</strong> rear in a rElstiDnship<br />

inhibits the degree <strong>of</strong> communication betwesc two p=rt!es.<br />

Possibly the distance and emotional alo<strong>of</strong>ness between father<br />

and son indicates low "referent pover" and therefore the<br />

need to use "coercive power.' Referent power is the degrep<br />

to which one person's identification or onenrss with another<br />

allows th9 latter person to influence the former (French and<br />

Raven, 1959:1611. Coercive power aoes not derivs from<br />

attraction or identification but from the threat <strong>of</strong><br />

punishment as a means by which one person influences another<br />

(French and Raven. 1959:157). In other words. the child<br />

COntOrmS to the father's wishes not because <strong>of</strong> a mutual<br />

attraction but because <strong>of</strong> the threat <strong>of</strong> force an3 thz high<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> punishment. <strong>The</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> a great degree<br />

<strong>of</strong> coercive power may contribute to the fear <strong>of</strong> the fathez<br />

that Fernanaez-Earire t & (1958) acd Badsen (1969)<br />

report as prevalent in ~uerto-~ican and uexican-~msrican<br />

families. Eadsen stated that Mexican-American college<br />

students asked to evaluate the father's role in the Latin


ch.5. cultural Consistency Page 75<br />

fanily felt that it was too authoritarian. Studerts<br />

reported that "it is true that the children have a great<br />

respect for the father but it is a respect based oc fear*<br />

and that "[the fa-her] should be a friend to his chilaren,<br />

cot a dictatorn (Badsen, 1964:SZ).<br />

This fear may impair a child's understandirg <strong>of</strong> the<br />

father's desires r%la%ing to a certain matter. <strong>The</strong><br />

psychological distance and minimal comnunicaticn may lead to<br />

the child acting in a way which the father feels is wrong<br />

and may increase the chance that the flther w i l l act<br />

violertly.<br />

Five .propositions lirking severe and distant father-son<br />

relations and the actual use <strong>of</strong> violence are implicit in the<br />

paragraphs above.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> more severe and distant the father-son<br />

relations, the lower the referent power it<br />

that relationship.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong> lower the referent power, the greater the<br />

Coercive power that the father needs to bring<br />

to bear on the son.<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> great-r the coercive pover, the greater<br />

the son's fear <strong>of</strong> the father.<br />

5. <strong>The</strong> more the son fears the father, the more<br />

difticult it may be for that son to<br />

undsrstand his father's desires and the<br />

greater the chance that the sor w i l l not act<br />

according to those aesires.<br />

6. TO the extent that &he son does not act<br />

accordlng to the fatter's desires ard a<br />

COerclvE power relatzonship exLsts betvesr<br />

father and son, rhe greater the chance that<br />

Control "111 be based cn temppr and violent<br />

Outburs+s.<br />

usicg deductive logk, these five propositions can be<br />

merged to show the relatiocship between the norm <strong>of</strong> severe<br />

and distant father-son relations and the actual use <strong>of</strong><br />

violence as follows:<br />

Ccmbining 2, 3, and 4:<br />

7. <strong>The</strong> more severe and distant the father-son<br />

relationship, the greater the. son's fear <strong>of</strong><br />

the father.<br />

<strong>The</strong>n coablning the above with 5:


Ch.5. Cultural Consistency Plga 76<br />

8. <strong>The</strong> nore severe and distant the father-son<br />

relationship, t h greater ~ the chance that the<br />

SOL w i l l not act according to the fathsr's<br />

desires.<br />

Finally, combining 7 with 6, the link between the norm for<br />

violence and the actual use <strong>of</strong> violence is made.<br />

9. <strong>The</strong> more severe and distant the father-son<br />

relatlOnShlp, the greater the chance that<br />

cortrol w i l l be based on temper and violent<br />

Outbursts.<br />

It deductive logic is usea to combine prlpositions 1<br />

aLd 9, the following proposition is yieldea.<br />

10. TO the extent that the male possesses 2<br />

great deal <strong>of</strong> authority in family life, the<br />

greater the chance that control rill be based<br />

on the use <strong>of</strong> temper and violent outbursts.<br />

Thus, according to the cultural-consistency theory, in<br />

nexican-American ethnic groups, a value related to family<br />

lifs that has nothing directly to do with the use <strong>of</strong><br />

violence acts to increase the actual lerel <strong>of</strong> violence. <strong>The</strong><br />

method <strong>of</strong> linkiJg propositiocs used above can be used to<br />

relate the other three family values to the actual 12ve1 <strong>of</strong><br />

family violence.<br />

This pattern <strong>of</strong> violence is probably perpsruated<br />

through social learning processes (3andura. 1973). Owens<br />

and Straus (1975). for example. show ?hat +he morc vialence<br />

experienced by a child, th- greater the tcndancy to favor<br />

violence as an adult. Apparently the child comes to believe<br />

that the best ray to have his own children act obsdiently is<br />

to use physical force, even if he was afraid <strong>of</strong> it as a<br />

child. ringer's statemert that :ndividual pcrsanality has a<br />

great deal to do with the development <strong>of</strong> culture also<br />

applies here. If the child feels that the use <strong>of</strong> force is<br />

the best way at controlling other members if his flaily,<br />

cultural norms that may support this vie= are reinforced.<br />

<strong>The</strong> discussion up to this point has centered on the<br />

formal normative system found in Mexican-8morican<br />

famIlieS.*3 nirande (1977) reports that this formal<br />

st-ucture does exist bat has been interpretEd bolh as a<br />

source <strong>of</strong> pathology and as a source <strong>of</strong> warmth and security.<br />

llirande (1977) also suggests that while the formal structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> norms nay exist, familial and sexual roles are being<br />

modified as Mexican-lmericans are assimilated, in incrsasirg<br />

numbnrs, into Aaerical society. Eaukes and Taylor's 1975<br />

study <strong>of</strong> the power structure <strong>of</strong> nexican families supports<br />

nirande's point. Thus. it is possible that changing roles<br />

may be manifest in less support <strong>of</strong> the father's fcrnal<br />

aothority and less distant an$ less severe fathsr-son


Ch.5. Cultural consistency Page 77<br />

relations. As a rssult, the proposed link betvcsr th=<br />

fcrmal normative structure and family violence may be weaker<br />

than predicted above.<br />

On the other hand, recent research on family violence<br />

among Anglo-Americans suggests that as tha traditional male<br />

dominated power structure becomes undermined, thsre map be a<br />

tendency for family violence to increase in the short run<br />

(see Brown, Chapter 11 and Allen and Straus. Chapter 12).<br />

This may be especially true <strong>of</strong> families which become female<br />

centered (Straus & &.. 1979).<br />

Jews traditionally hava been characterizsa as havirg a<br />

lov rate <strong>of</strong> family violence. most Jevs would be considered<br />

in or above the middle class today, and this stetus might<br />

contrzbote to their lov rates <strong>of</strong> family violrnce.*q Souever.<br />

even a? the turn <strong>of</strong> the century. when most Jews war?<br />

working-class immigraCtS, they probahly also had lover rates<br />

<strong>of</strong> famlly violence than other poor ethnic groups. Jewish<br />

family values may be related to the low level <strong>of</strong> family<br />

violence.<br />

Figure 2 applies the cultural-consistency theory <strong>of</strong><br />

family violence to Jewish-Americans.<br />

1. To the extent that intellectuality is<br />

stressed, the emphasis w i l l be on rational<br />

means at sclvirg family problems. rather than<br />

on verbal or physrcal coercion.<br />

B function <strong>of</strong> intellectuality in Jewish families. which<br />

may be more closely related to nonviolence, is the fact th+t<br />

ncrms for articulateness, argumentativeness, and<br />

parect-child bargaining are prasent. <strong>The</strong>se thres norms<br />

w012ld seen to sanction conflict but to channel it <strong>of</strong>f into<br />

di~cussion rather than physical action. Yaffe reported


Ch.5. Cultural Consistency Page<br />

that:<br />

4<br />

Anything far the<br />

EOBe discipline is far less strict among Jews<br />

than among other groups. Jewish parents arr<br />

naggers, screamers, nudgers--but not hitters.<br />

<strong>The</strong> child has to go pretty far before he'll get<br />

the back <strong>of</strong> his father's hand. And a lot more<br />

ConverSational freedom is tolerated. Jewish<br />

children are allowed to interrupt, cantradict<br />

the grown-ups, be "fresh* (1968: 312).<br />

Importance <strong>of</strong> family in Religiosity <strong>of</strong>maie Pursuit <strong>of</strong> Knowledge:<br />

Jewish identincarion (Shapiro and Daihefrky, the mind is a great tool.<br />

(Shapiio and Daihefiky) Bemrrein) (Beinstein, Gordon,<br />

1<br />

Intense parentchild<br />

involvomeni (Shupiia<br />

and Darhefrky, Yrffe)<br />

-<br />

4<br />

Cornpaision<br />

4<br />

lnlellecruality<br />

Children (Shapiio (Shapira and Shapiio and Darhefrky)<br />

and Dashefiky) Dashefrkyj<br />

Pwtootion: Bays perceive ihemielvei as like their father (Shapiio and Dahefrky)<br />

Figure 2. lewirh.American parent-child violence<br />

Shapiio and Darhefrky,<br />

Siiodtbeck, Yuffe)<br />

Daiheiiky, Yaffe) Bargaining<br />

Non-<strong>Violence</strong>


cultural Ccrsistency Page 79<br />

Similarly. Zukls study <strong>of</strong> 300 Jevish families (1978)<br />

d a strorg current <strong>of</strong> egalitarianism running through<br />

sh families. particularly in more assimilated, less<br />

O ~ O X ones. This feeling, combined with ths channeling<br />

ggressien into verbal rather than physical expression,<br />

s to "the high level <strong>of</strong> bickering that occurs between<br />

and and wife, between parents and children. and betweer<br />

.... Teasicg, sarcasm and ridicule =re reqularly<br />

ov~d br -. Darents to set limits on childrel's behavior."<br />

Eternally ln competltion. the Jewlsh fsmlly 1s a<br />

aerltocracy.<br />

Thus. lt is posslble that frrquent ccnflrct may be a<br />

characterlstlc <strong>of</strong> Jewlsh famrlles. If that is the case, the<br />

frequency <strong>of</strong> ccnflict may be a slgn that &wish famllres are<br />

very stable.<br />

when close relationships are characterized by<br />

frequent Conflicts rather than by the<br />

accumulation <strong>of</strong> hestile and ambivalent feelings,<br />

we may be jnstified, given that such conflic?~<br />

are not likely to concern basic consensus, in<br />

taking these fregoent conflicts as an index <strong>of</strong><br />

the stabilixy <strong>of</strong> these relationships (Coslr.<br />

1956:SS).<br />

<strong>The</strong> following prcpcsition can be developed from the<br />

above discussion:<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> greater the emphasis on articulateness,<br />

argumentativeness. and parent-chill<br />

bargaining. the greater the chacce that<br />

conflicts rill be resolved through discussion<br />

ra+her than viols3l cr coercive action.<br />

Pically, the basis <strong>of</strong> a cultural-consistency analysis<br />

can be shovn by combining propositions 1 ard 2 to pr?sent<br />

one relationship between a basic family norm and the actual<br />

use <strong>of</strong> violent or nonviolent action to resolve a conflict.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong> greater the extent that the pursuit <strong>of</strong><br />

knowledge is stressed, and debete and<br />

conflict are regarded as legitimate, the<br />

greater the chance that conflicts w i l l be<br />

resolved through discussion rather than<br />

violent or coercive action.


Ch. 5. Cultural Consistency Page 80<br />

CONCLOSIOA<br />

In this chapter we have attempted to construct a<br />

cultural-consistency theory <strong>of</strong> family violence. This theory<br />

is based on the idea thet cultural values having Po msnifsst<br />

reference to violence act either to increase or to hold down<br />

the actual level <strong>of</strong> family violence ir csrtain ethnic<br />

groups. Values and norms were linked together, and the<br />

Corms were ;n turn linked to the actoal level <strong>of</strong> violecce in<br />

an ethnic group. <strong>The</strong> perpetuation <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> vialence<br />

from generatloc to generatior was also examined.<br />

<strong>The</strong> flexican-American and Jevish-aaerican ethnic groups<br />

were used to illustrate this theory. A higher lave1 <strong>of</strong><br />

family violence was assumed to bf present in<br />

n~xican-American than in Jewish-lmerican fsnilies. <strong>The</strong><br />

higher level <strong>of</strong> violence in Mexican-American families was<br />

proposed to be associated with the values <strong>of</strong> severe male<br />

dominance, strict discipline. and submission to tte fither.<br />

severe and distant father-son relations were seen to be ths<br />

result <strong>of</strong> these values, leading to fear <strong>of</strong> the father, poor<br />

ccmmunicatiol, and a resulting high level <strong>of</strong> parent-child<br />

violence. Perpetuation <strong>of</strong> this subculture is accomplished<br />

through the desire for boys to be like thsir fathers even<br />

though they fear then, ard because a child turned adult<br />

treats his wife and children the same way his father treated<br />

his family. Modifications in the formal normative structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> lerican-American f am:lies<br />

husband-father's formal authorrty vere<br />

challnnging the<br />

noted and seen as<br />

possibly<br />

run.<br />

increasing family violence, at 1~2st Ln the short<br />

Values <strong>of</strong> the Jewish ethnic group also were examined in<br />

this theoretical framevork. <strong>The</strong> basic famzly v=lues<br />

emphasized vere the pursuit <strong>of</strong> krowlsdga and the use cf the<br />

mind rather than the body. <strong>The</strong> value <strong>of</strong> intellectuelity<br />

resulting from these values was proposea t3 lead fo the<br />

favoring <strong>of</strong> articulateness. argumertstiveness, and<br />

bargaining as a way to solve family aisputes. Thus, debate<br />

and rat physical coercion is used and there is less family<br />

violence. Values are perpetuated in that boys perceive<br />

themselves as like their fathers.<br />

Finally. as noted ir Chapter 1, the conclusions <strong>of</strong> this<br />

chapter, like the conclusions <strong>of</strong> the other theoretical<br />

chapters. are not put forward as established facts. Rather.<br />

they are intended as stimuli ard hypotheses for empirical<br />

research.


ch.5- Cultural Corsistency Pege 81<br />

NOTES<br />

*A revision <strong>of</strong> a paper presented at the amoal meeting<br />

<strong>of</strong> the National Council on Family Relations. August 20-23,<br />

1915. <strong>The</strong> preparation <strong>of</strong> the chapter vas supported bp<br />

Aational Institute <strong>of</strong> mental Realth grant number 13050. i<br />

would like to thank Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Dorothy Pinnegan <strong>of</strong><br />

Colby-Sawyer College fcr many helpful soggestions and<br />

conments through the develapment <strong>of</strong> this paper.<br />

1. Another source <strong>of</strong> perpetuation could be fesaback<br />

processes that occur ir the proposed causal chain beiveer<br />

broad family values not related to violecca, norms <strong>of</strong><br />

violence, and the actual use <strong>of</strong> violence. <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong><br />

violence to control a situation, resulting in the label <strong>of</strong><br />

violence. might strengthen the norm <strong>of</strong> violence through a<br />

self-fulfilling prophecy. in fact, the perceived<br />

effectiveness <strong>of</strong> a norm <strong>of</strong> violence or nonviolence might<br />

even strengthen the more basic value from vhich the violent<br />

or ncnviolent norm has criginated. I!! other words, if the<br />

norm <strong>of</strong> violence or nonviolecce is al effective means <strong>of</strong><br />

control. then the vay <strong>of</strong> life that sanctions this norm would<br />

be further reinforced.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> Grebler e_t_ a_&: (19701 study <strong>of</strong> urban<br />

Llexican-Aeericans found that the value placed on the man as<br />

authority figure varies by income lev21 and neiphbarhood.<br />

Rowever, nearly tvo-thirds <strong>of</strong> those vith medium or lower<br />

income living in areas vith a high psrcentage <strong>of</strong><br />

mexican-lmericans felt that the hustand shculd have complete<br />

control over the family income. This findirg suggests that<br />

the value <strong>of</strong> male authority is quite strong in ethnic<br />

enclaves.<br />

3. I would like to thank Prcfessor Alfred0 Mirande <strong>of</strong><br />

+he Universi:y <strong>of</strong> California, Siverside, for brirging to mg<br />

attention a view <strong>of</strong> the Mexican-American farily which<br />

differs from the view which is more prevalent in the<br />

literature. This view implies that K~xizan-Rmrrican<br />

families would have a lover degree <strong>of</strong> family violence than<br />

is suggested in this article.<br />

4. Erlazger (1979). in a review <strong>of</strong> studies Examining<br />

the relation betweer social class acd family violence,<br />

rsported differences betveaa social classes, with physical<br />

punishment increasing as one goes down the ststos ladaer.<br />

HOveVer, the diffsrences zrs not as large as many would


Part 111 <strong>Social</strong> Organization<br />

and Family <strong>Violence</strong>


Important as are the cultural horns tha<br />

make it leglcimate for family members to us<br />

physical force on each other. thsy do not full<br />

explain family violence. First, these norms zr<br />

guidelines more for culturally y=gi$&b<br />

actions than for colturally rs&=$$ actions<br />

So the question resains as to why sow* fsailie<br />

do and others do not engage in such permissibl<br />

violence. We must also examine how or why norm<br />

legitimizing violence within the fanily ca<br />

into being, and why such norms coctinue<br />

exist.<br />

A s With most aspects <strong>of</strong> society, ro sing1<br />

factor provides the answer to these questions.<br />

Rather a complex interweaving <strong>of</strong> factors exists<br />

that ve are just beginning to unravel. X3verer.<br />

Even at this early stage in the scientific study<br />

<strong>of</strong> family violence. it is clear that nzny <strong>of</strong> the<br />

threads making up the fabric <strong>of</strong> violence in the<br />

family are those identified in Chapter 1 as<br />

social Organizational factors. 9 r.umbfr oE<br />

these were briefly discussed ;a that chapter.<br />

for example, the involuntary nature <strong>of</strong><br />

aembership in e faaily, the privacy <strong>of</strong> the<br />

family as a social insti+utioa, ald assignment.<br />

Of roles within the Easily by age and sex rather<br />

than by interest or competence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters in Part 111 treat some <strong>of</strong><br />

these social Organizatiocal factors in aetail,<br />

indicating the types <strong>of</strong> social arrangements that<br />

give rise to situations in which people fEna ta<br />

resort to the use <strong>of</strong> violence. Sioce thesi<br />

arrangesents are regularities <strong>of</strong> social life.<br />

most <strong>of</strong> which have been part <strong>of</strong> :he structure <strong>of</strong><br />

the human family for perhaps thousands 3f years,<br />

we can specula+e that they are also at the root<br />

<strong>of</strong> the persistence <strong>of</strong> norms that permit violercs<br />

in the family. Cultural norms tend to evolve i n<br />

a vag that reflects, regularizas, and<br />

legitiaizes the typical behavior <strong>of</strong> members <strong>of</strong> a<br />

society. In shorf, as was noted in Chapter 1,<br />

culture and social organization are tvs facets<br />

<strong>of</strong> a single reality. Even so, they are far fros<br />

harlng a fixed and immutable relationship to<br />

each other. <strong>The</strong> emergent nature <strong>of</strong> all homar<br />

life dictates that cul5ure and social<br />

organization w i l l constantly get out <strong>of</strong> Lins


y~th each other, with a resulting pressure tc<br />

restore the synchronizat~on.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters in Par? 111 has3 at least four<br />

things in common. First, they share what could<br />

be called an emphasis on structural<br />

~onduci~eness. Each chapter seeks to explain<br />

the high rate <strong>of</strong> conflict and violence in the<br />

family by focusing on some zspect <strong>of</strong> th2<br />

structnre <strong>of</strong> relations between family nenbsrs<br />

and on how the nature <strong>of</strong> these relationships<br />

contributes to or dampens violence.<br />

Secocd. each cf the authors, 5n his Jr her<br />

own way. highlights the irony that th?<br />

organizational structure <strong>of</strong> family life creates<br />

conditions for warmth and intimacy as well as<br />

for conflict and violecce.<br />

Third. these chapters do not present nau<br />

data, but instead summarize and extend a large<br />

number <strong>of</strong> existing empirical studies ana<br />

theoretical arguments. Th~y organize and make<br />

deductims from existing thecry and rssearch on<br />

violence and family violence. Nct that they<br />

feel collecting new data is unnecessarp, but<br />

organizicg and icterpreting. from a scciological<br />

perspective, what is already kcown abcut family<br />

violence can give new insights and a sounder<br />

basis for future empirical research.<br />

Last, posslbly the mcst zmpoztant aspect <strong>of</strong><br />

the format <strong>of</strong> tke chaptsrs in Part 111 is their<br />

exlmtra+.iar ot . ways in which Eccial<br />

sarrables ere :n-errelated. Po: uc-il r?<br />

uzdersTarC hou a nanbDr cr r-les2n' sariehlc;<br />

fit together, explanations <strong>of</strong> family conflict<br />

and violence vFll be incomplete.<br />

Page 85


Chapter 6<br />

Sexual Inequality and <strong>Wife</strong> Bea<br />

Murray A. Straus<br />

......................<br />

This chapter describes a major aspect o<br />

the social structure underpinning husband-wif<br />

violence: the sexist organization <strong>of</strong> the famil<br />

and the society in general. It argues tha<br />

sexism produces violence because men us<br />

violence to marntain their position as "head <strong>of</strong><br />

the household." nore is involved in sexism than<br />

the "rightn to be the head <strong>of</strong> thr faaily as<br />

supported by cultural values end beliefs;<br />

sexism is also grounded in i~stitutional<br />

arrangements--such as the expectation that men<br />

w i l l marry ycungsr wonen and the segregated<br />

labor market ix vbich women's jobs are lpss well<br />

paid--that maKe male dominance a reality. A<br />

society, or groups within a society, may favor<br />

equalitarian relationships, but unless these<br />

notions go beyond beliefs and become rsflectea<br />

in societal arrangements, the domination <strong>of</strong><br />

women by men is bound to continue.<br />

Ban7 cf the issues examined in this chapter<br />

crop up again and again throughout t.he vclume.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se issues are critical in the discussion <strong>of</strong><br />

practical steps to reduce and eventually<br />

eliminate husband-wife violence, a thrm?<br />

examined fully in Chapter 13.<br />

This Chapter describes some <strong>of</strong> the ways in which rh<br />

male-dominant paver structure <strong>of</strong> the family gives rise t<br />

violence. This implies that, as our society msv?s tovsra<br />

nore egalitarian family system, both the actual levels o<br />

violence and the norms implicitly permittfng such vi3l=ncs<br />

w i l l decline. Al'hougb this decrease is the like1<br />

low-term outcome, it is far from a certainty. First. man<br />

cther factors affect the level <strong>of</strong> violence in the famil


- 1. - Defense - cf Bale P.uth01it~. Ir the c>nL.DxL <strong>of</strong> an<br />

vidualistically oriented urban-industrial society, the<br />

rlption <strong>of</strong> superior authority to husbands is a poteEt<br />

CE producing physical attacks on wives. In such a<br />

iety, male-superiority norms are not clearly understood<br />

are in the process <strong>of</strong> trarei+icn, and the pz=sgs~;:Qe if<br />

e superiority anst be validzte4 by supprior 'r~sources"<br />

h as valued<br />

vlcos (Eodman,<br />

pfrsonal<br />

1972).<br />

tralts and material<br />

If every nan wsrs,<br />

gools and<br />

in fact.<br />

Derior to his wife in such resources 3s intellisence.<br />

.owledge, occupational prestige. and income, there would be<br />

concordance between the authority ascribsd an3 the<br />

dividual achievements ex~octed to acconDanP that authoritv<br />

. .<br />

ach:evement-oriented societies. clelrly, such<br />

p9ricrity is not zlways the case, dSspitE the societal<br />

ruoture that gives men tremendohs advantages in access to<br />

€58 traits and resources. Consequently, many men feel<br />

nost compelled to fall back on tho "ul+.ima?a resource** <strong>of</strong><br />

ysical force to Paintain their superior pcsition (Goode.<br />

71; Seraus, 1974b:bb-57). P graphic illustratici! <strong>of</strong> just<br />

is process is the case <strong>of</strong> Joe and Jennifer reportsd<br />

Rossa (Chapter 10). Bllec and Straus gire st~tistica, b?<br />

idence in Chapter 12.<br />

-- ---<br />

2. Com&a:p nasculinit~. Talcott Parsons (1997) has<br />

Zrgued that in modern industrial societies, the s~p3rztion<br />

<strong>of</strong> the male occupa2ional role from the family and ?he<br />

predominance <strong>of</strong> the mother in child rearing crectes a<br />

fundamental difficulty for men in resppct to achieving a<br />

masculine sexual identity:


Ch.6. sexual Inequali'y<br />

<strong>The</strong> bcp has a tendency to fcra a dirsc<br />

teminine identLfication. sicce his mother is th<br />

model most readily available and significant<br />

h i But he is not destined to become an adu<br />

woman. noreovsr he soon discovers that<br />

certain vital respects Women are considera<br />

inferior to mer. that it would hence bs shamefu<br />

for him to grow up to be like a "omin. Rex<br />

when boys emerge in%o what Freudians call t<br />

"latency period," their behavior tends to be<br />

marked by a kind <strong>of</strong> cog~gllpg $s;q&&&te<br />

hqgression toward women vho "after all are<br />

blamc," is an essertial concomitant (p.305).<br />

llthough Pazsons' emphasis is on the particular<br />

constellation just described as partial explanatior I<br />

gsneraLly bigh level <strong>of</strong> male aggressiveness i? H-<br />

sOCie?les. it also seems lLkely to be part <strong>of</strong> the<br />

that so much male aggressiveness is diroctad agairst<br />

wives in particular. Similarly, Parsons' analy<br />

shows the origins <strong>of</strong> female aggressive<br />

perticular structure <strong>of</strong> the family in industrial sc<br />

much <strong>of</strong> this aggressiveness is focused specifically aq<br />

men, especially husbands. as the agents <strong>of</strong> women's repre<br />

position in society.*2 This climate if mnlual artagn<br />

bctvsen the sexes provides a coltext :hi? is not o<br />

conducive to attacks by husba~ds on uirss but probably<br />

underlies a number <strong>of</strong> other related pheriomeaa, snch a<br />

growing evidence that in mar7 insrences, "rape is a p,<br />

trip, not a passion tripD (Eezt, 1?75:40: Borgzss<br />

Aolmstrom. 19741. loreover. as in the typical homose<br />

tape in prisons (Davis. 1910). the degradatior<br />

humiliation <strong>of</strong> the victim is <strong>of</strong>ten a eajor mstivating for<br />

3- Economic Co~strairts ,?a giscriminatiQQ. Thp ss<br />

ecO?.omic and occupational structure <strong>of</strong> society allows u<br />

few alternatives. Tbe jobs open to women ire 13uer<br />

status and. despite antidiscrimination legislation. vo<br />

continue to earn less than men in the same occupatfo<br />

Without access to good jobs, women are depfndent cn thhusbands.<br />

If a divorce takes place, almas? all hnsban<br />

default c?. support payments after a sh3rt time, ev<br />

assuming they could afford thsm in the first plac<br />

Corsequently, many women continue to endure physical attac<br />

trom their husbapd because the<br />

pcverty (Gelles, 1976).<br />

alternative tn divsrce<br />

children, but at the same time society eoss rot pror<br />

elther economic recompense for her doing so, or chila car<br />

Centprs that take over part <strong>of</strong> the burden s3 that she ca<br />

earn ecough to support her children. Occupations<br />

discrimination, lack <strong>of</strong> child-care facilitiss, inadsquat


port from either the aovernmsnt or the father--all<br />

omm into remairing married even whec they are the<br />

f violence.<br />

fir$& or i&g SWe-?ar€nt FEpppla. Another <strong>of</strong> the<br />

norms that helps to maintain the subordination <strong>of</strong><br />

the id?a that childrer carrot be adsquatsly brought<br />

e parent. Thus, if a woman is to have children, she<br />

o have a man. TO the limited extsnt that research<br />

supports this view. the situation comes about only<br />

<strong>of</strong> the confounding <strong>of</strong> poverty acd social ostricisn<br />

ngle parenthood. Blthough it seems likely that if<br />

ressure and constraints were removed, most women<br />

want to live with a man, an importart ninority does<br />

nd lives, i~ effect, in a state <strong>of</strong> fcrced<br />

ation. Thus. innumerable and. under present<br />

ons, unnecessary social and acoxoaic constraints<br />

the sicgle-parent family from bsing a viable social<br />

nd forces nary women to accept or ccntinue a<br />

Pate, StrSSSfnl relationship.<br />

1. Eps~agce <strong>of</strong> ~jfg<br />

-t system, being a wife<br />

rg= Xpggp. undsr the<br />

ard mother is the most<br />

rtant single role for a woman. IndEed, Emerican<br />

ural noras<br />

ss married.<br />

sting much<br />

are such that one CEnfor be i full voman<br />

E mar, OR the other hand, hcs tbp option <strong>of</strong><br />

or little <strong>of</strong> himself in the husband-father<br />

depending on his interest, abllity, ard c:zcumst.arces.<br />

Short, *he stigma <strong>of</strong> being a divorced aaz is saall<br />

ared with that <strong>of</strong> being a divorced womac--to whlcn a<br />

cia1 term, with overtones <strong>of</strong> immorality, ha5 been<br />

ached: divorcee. This forced dependence on the wife<br />

E as the basis for a respected positior ir society aakes<br />

difficult for a womal to rsfusf to tolerate mile viclence<br />

d end the marriage.<br />

7. Aega+jx Self-Image. Wnder the present social<br />

rncture, women tend to develop negatir~ self-images,<br />

pecially in relation to the crucial trait <strong>of</strong> achissesent<br />

orner, 1972: Truxisger. 1971:260). As a conraquence,<br />

-clings <strong>of</strong> guilt ard masochism develop, which parmit women<br />

0 tolerate male aggression acd violence and, in some<br />

xtreme cases, to seek it. Pull sexual equality would<br />

liminate this as a sexually structured pattern <strong>of</strong> bshavior.<br />

ven though it may remair or an individual-to-indisiaual<br />

asis. Only 2 de-emphasis on individual competitive<br />

ChiovBment w i l l fnlly eliminate this problem.<br />

--<br />

8. Women as Children. <strong>The</strong> conception <strong>of</strong> women as the<br />

roperty <strong>of</strong> men is no longer part <strong>of</strong> the legal system <strong>of</strong><br />

ndustrial countries. Aowever, Elements <strong>of</strong> this outlook<br />

linger in the folk culturc and also survive in certain<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> +he law, such as in 'he statotes that declare the<br />

husband the head <strong>of</strong> the bousehould and give him vlrious<br />

rights over his wife, like the right to choose thf place <strong>of</strong>


Ch.6. Sexual Inequality<br />

abode, to which the wife must conform.*3 In additio<br />

is the related coccepticn <strong>of</strong> women as uchildli<br />

combination, these aspects <strong>of</strong> the saxist orgaciz<br />

Society give husbands a covert moral right to use<br />

force OP their wives andogous to the overt legal --<br />

parsnts to use physlcal force on their children (see G<br />

1979:58).<br />

9, &l_e orientation sE $25 QhB:n_al ~nsticf<br />

Not only is much male violence against wires attributa<br />

the sexist organization <strong>of</strong> society, but the final tnd<br />

is that th€ male-oriented organization <strong>of</strong> the cr<br />

justice system virtually guarante4s that few vcmaa ui<br />

able to secure legal relief. To begin vith, the long<br />

in obtaining court orders and "peace bonds- make<br />

useless in securicg immediate relief from the dan<br />

another assault. Even without. thGse delays. many<br />

cannot attend court because <strong>of</strong> the lack <strong>of</strong> chi1<br />

arrangements during the long hours <strong>of</strong> waiting for a ca<br />

Come UP Or because, zs OfteP happezs, the cas<br />

rescheduled. P-mong ather impediments to securin? pro<br />

against assaults by a husbard a=€ +hose dsscribed<br />

section <strong>of</strong> Chapter 3 or *'Legitimation <strong>of</strong> Rusban<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> by the Courts and Police": immurily from sui<br />

failure <strong>of</strong> police to act against hUsSands, :hs "cooling<br />

by police, prosecuting attcrreys, and judges <strong>of</strong> wive<br />

attempt to bring complaints, and the denial <strong>of</strong> coapsns<br />

by public compensation review boards.<br />

SEXUAL LIBERATION AWD TPE REDUCTION OF NRR<strong>IT</strong>AL RSSRO<br />

Alrhough Goode believes that force or its threat<br />

ultimately necessary for the existenca <strong>of</strong> society, h~<br />

concedes that *'...the amount <strong>of</strong> force now appliea in th<br />

various areas <strong>of</strong> family life ...[ is not] ... either necess<br />

or desirablen (Gocde, 1971:92]. One <strong>of</strong> the ways in uh<br />

the amount <strong>of</strong> fcrce [necessary to maintain a viabl~ patt<br />

<strong>of</strong> family llfe] can be reduced is to reduce the degree<br />

inequality found withlc the family. Children's immatn<br />

imposes a l i m i t on the extent to which they can be gr<br />

equality vith their parents.<br />

and physical conditinns that<br />

But the particular econo<br />

may have justified<br />

subordinete position for women in earlier periods <strong>of</strong> hist<br />

are Clearly no'lccger present.<br />

<strong>The</strong> goals <strong>of</strong> the voaenls movement are cents~rd<br />

elimlnating each <strong>of</strong> the violence-producing lnequit<br />

discussed in this chapter. Since these factors account<br />

the higb level <strong>of</strong> physical assaults on women by th<br />

husbands, achievement <strong>of</strong> the goals <strong>of</strong> the feminist movem<br />

is tremendously important for any reduction <strong>of</strong> mari<br />

assault.


Pege 91<br />

the process <strong>of</strong> advocating these fundamental<br />

1 changes the women's movement has mads vsrious<br />

coctrihutions. For example, the ideology <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

movement itself encourages women t3 resist all<br />

oppression. especially physical violence. Thsro<br />

n an explosive growth <strong>of</strong> "battered-wife shelters."<br />

g in England about 1972 and in the nnitea S'ates<br />

1975. <strong>The</strong>se provide imoedia'e physical escape,<br />

arly for those with young children who night<br />

e have xo alternative to being victimiz+d by their<br />

vomen's movement has been perceptive in recognizing<br />

perior male physical strength and skill are important<br />

f the process by which male dominance is aaintainad.<br />

recognitior pertly accounts for the emphasis on karate<br />

her self-defensive training. Enwover, it is uclikely<br />

karats will, in fact, protect womeo frcm assault,<br />

e than the ability to respond physically protects men<br />

assault by other men. Boreover. the karate approach<br />

utionaltzes the role <strong>of</strong> physical violence ir social<br />

ction and hence incrraszs the likellhooa <strong>of</strong> further<br />

ce. Ieverthelsss, the ehphasis on physical<br />

~fense trainirg is an important spu& step towards<br />

entoal elimination <strong>of</strong> violent rsp:essiDn <strong>of</strong> rsaen.<br />

his even?uality w i l l Only Clme aboat if the more<br />

enfal problems <strong>of</strong> sexual insquality car be 0v;:coms.<br />

ately, emphasis has shifted from training in the us9<br />

sical force to trairing in "assertiveness," which is<br />

ortant Step in the direction <strong>of</strong> sexual equality.*u<br />

Bevsrtheless, the difficulties <strong>of</strong> the period <strong>of</strong><br />

sition cannot be overlooksd. <strong>The</strong> lonu-run consequences<br />

a more equalitarian society may be t3 lesss~ the<br />

uency with which wives are victims <strong>of</strong> assault by<br />

bz~.ds. Eut, we suggested earlier (see also Kolb and<br />

aus, 1974: Whitehurst, 197U). the &&~~=EY~ Conseqnences<br />

y be the opposite, because a sizable number <strong>of</strong> mec w i l l<br />

easily give up their sex-stereotyped roles. A 1 1 three<br />

ters in Part <strong>IT</strong> document this in different ways: a case<br />

<strong>of</strong> a specific couple, logical deduction, and<br />

stical data on a large sample <strong>of</strong> cauples. Like<br />

aditionally oriented women, men are cocditioned by their<br />

lrure to perceive only the prerogativ?s and advantages <strong>of</strong><br />

e traditional male role, and to ignore its bu~denr,<br />

straints, 2nd disedv3ntages. Thus, a less violent world<br />

d less vi~lence in th4 fzmily require men's as wall as<br />

men's liberation.<br />

Ocfortunately, progress toward sexual equality end<br />

reedom from sexually stereotyped roles has rot basn as<br />

great as seemed possible in the early 1970s. R t present<br />

(1978-74). there is even considerable doubt that th? Fqual<br />

Rights Bmendaent w i l l pass. B state-hy-state tally by the<br />

International Women's Year Commission found that stat- lavs


Ch.6. Sexual Inequality<br />

(which govsrn most domestic matters) continue to gi<br />

substantially fewer ziqhts than their husbands in r<br />

inheritance, divorc5, property ownership, domestic<br />

and adultery (united Press, October 30. 7977). Kan<br />

show that traditionel sex roles remair entrench<br />

among the ycung. For example, a survey <strong>of</strong> 17-par-<br />

the Wational Bssessment <strong>of</strong> Eaucatioral Progrsse foun<br />

nnly half <strong>of</strong> this age group thinks all women should b<br />

to pursue careqrs Outsids the hone (Be* Fork Tirnss, 2<br />

1977: 826: see also Reinhold. 1977).<br />

8.5 a recent coementaror put it: "Scrakch alm3s<br />

man, and you'll find wistful memories nf his a<strong>of</strong>her a<br />

socks and cooking Sunday lunch...." (Prarckn. 1977;<br />

also Stapp and Pines. 1976). <strong>The</strong> detlils vary, <strong>of</strong> co<br />

with the setting and the socioeconomic level, but the<br />

<strong>of</strong> male superiority reeairs the same. In that same ar<br />

Pzancke tells abou' a frierd:<br />

... who has made a name for herself i<br />

educational films in new Ycrk, had aicner in LJ<br />

Anqeles recently with another old friend. uh<br />

has made a much bigger nans for himself in<br />

televisior. Settling down over late-night<br />

brandies, she was stunned vhsn he turned to her<br />

andsaid, Vcu krow, I can't imagine being<br />

married to you. I d panic 2nd run." "Why?"<br />

Sally askea, hurt that Fheir fri-~dshio vhich<br />

had mver even touched on the subject <strong>of</strong><br />

-.<br />

marriaae. seemed suddrnlv flawed. "You're 1<br />

star," he said. But. Sally pointed out, h l was<br />

the one getting qucted in Tine and Bevsreek and<br />

was probably earring $100,000 a year to her<br />

820,000. "Eaybe so." he said declarativ~ly.<br />

"but underneath !t all I think you're smarter<br />

than I am."<br />

what lurking terrors possess men when their<br />

womln achieve success on their own? Ths truth<br />

<strong>of</strong> the matter is, that in spite <strong>of</strong> Virginia<br />

slims, ve haven't come such a long ray. babies<br />

(Prancke, 1977:uU).


xual Inequality Page 93<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are, <strong>of</strong> course, many other factors which<br />

te to the existence and mairtenancs 5f norms<br />

ng intrafaaily violence. Owens a d Strsus (7975),<br />

ample, present data on the correlatior <strong>of</strong> childhood<br />

nces <strong>of</strong> viclence (including victimization) with<br />

ence attitudes and velues. See alss the discossiac<br />

e influence <strong>of</strong> sociely's positive Evaloation <strong>of</strong><br />

E in Chapter Y; ar.d in Straus. 1979a. 19146.<br />

. See whiting (1965:137) ard the discussior. <strong>of</strong> "the<br />

yth" =a Stsinaetz acd Straus (1979:lO-13) fs: other<br />

n which the pattern <strong>of</strong> male-female relationships built<br />

the society helps to create antagonism bEtw~En rhe<br />

and hence strecgthens tho associatiJn between<br />

y and violence.<br />

. It is pertinent that. EvEn in a state known for its<br />

and familial experimentation, as recently as 1971,<br />

lifornia State Bar Issociatian voted ~ p t to repeal<br />

egislation (Trucinger, 1971:216).<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> combir.ation <strong>of</strong> sexual equality, ffaals<br />

rtiveness, and sexual libefaticr mioh' also contribute<br />

educing rape. If women were to escape the culturally<br />

sotyped role <strong>of</strong> disinterest in and resistarc* '3 sex.<br />

were to take on an zssertive role in expr-ssina +heir<br />

sexuality. rather thar. lea~lng such sxprassion tc ths<br />

ertiveness <strong>of</strong> men, womsn's LEV status mig?t csntributa to<br />

reducticr in rape in several nays. Pxrst, many rapes<br />

an illegitimte extersior. <strong>of</strong> sanctiored techriqu?s <strong>of</strong><br />

ercoming" the culturally prescribed resistance <strong>of</strong> women<br />

EX (Kirkpatrick and Kanin. 1951). Seccnd. the<br />

unding <strong>of</strong> SEX and aggression that is built into our<br />

rure could be reduced (Steinmetz and Straus, 1974: 70-13).<br />

rd, to the extent thet sharply differentiated sex roles<br />

responsible for ths , phenomena <strong>of</strong> "compulsive<br />

culinity' and structured antagonism between the sexes,<br />

elimination <strong>of</strong> sexual inequality woold reduce the Dumber<br />

"power trip" and "degradation ceremonyl motivated rapes<br />

ovnmiller, 1975).


Chapter 7<br />

Stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Keith M. Fanington<br />

This chapter emphasizes the effect <strong>of</strong><br />

family structural characteristics on the amount<br />

Of Stress experienced. Parricgton suggests that<br />

compared with most other groups, th? family<br />

Experiences more stress and may b~ less capabls<br />

Of coping with it. <strong>The</strong> family, because <strong>of</strong> its<br />

urique structure, elso may be less efficiert in<br />

dealing vich str~ssfnl situ~tims.<br />

It is interesting to contrast this<br />

sociological perspective with the implications<br />

<strong>of</strong> a "medical" model for the cortr31 <strong>of</strong><br />

violence. "Curzngw violence through drugs snd<br />

surgery has a powerful appeal to laymen ana<br />

SciEntiSts alike. If a day does arrive when it<br />

becomes routine to take a pill when we feel ths<br />

Urge to kick a co-worker or thrsv a punch at our<br />

mate, intrzfamily violence w i l l probably still<br />

exist. If the family runs out <strong>of</strong> nonaggression<br />

pills. one spouse could blame the other for<br />

being lax, and might feel justified in hittin3<br />

the Other so that he or she won't forget the<br />

next time. or perhaps a mother w i l l slap her<br />

child because the child refuses ro swallow ths<br />

nonaggression pill.<br />

FaSringCOn's point <strong>of</strong> view is that<br />

regardless <strong>of</strong> medical technology, the structure<br />

ot relationstips betwen family members must<br />

change it vzolerca is to be redoze*. If<br />

conflicts ot interest and stress in family lifs<br />

are inevitable, thsn tho family must develop a<br />

Structure for resolving thase conflicts<br />

nonviolently. Blthough Chapter 13 <strong>of</strong>fers some<br />

suggestions. achievirg thrse ends w i l l be<br />

difficult. since, as Farrington points out.<br />

family structure works against the nonviolect<br />

resolotior <strong>of</strong> stressful situations.


P GENERAL STRESS FBhlEQOEK<br />

<strong>The</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> stress has had a histary markPd by<br />

considerable conceptual ambzguity and dissension (nechanic,<br />

1962: Chaptsr 1; 1968:296-300; Lazarus, lQ66:Ct.ap%?; 1:<br />

Levzne and Scotch. 1967:163-165; Scott and Sowazd, 1970;<br />

acGrath. 1970). Stress has been defined ana studied as (1)<br />

a threatening or disruptive stimulus (Grinker and Spiegsl,<br />

1945; Basowltz & g., 1955:7; Hill, 1958: Janis.<br />

1958:13); (2) 3 particular response or patter3 <strong>of</strong> respenses<br />

in ?he face <strong>of</strong> such a stimulus (Eas0wit.z er a_&.. 1955:289:<br />

Janis, 1958:13; Eechanic, 1962:7): or (3) a skate <strong>of</strong> the<br />

organzsm as it is exp~rieccing such a stimulus (Selye. 1956:<br />

Dohrecrend. 1961; Wolff. 7968). <strong>The</strong> ?roblems srisicg from<br />

this lack <strong>of</strong> a common definition have led a number <strong>of</strong><br />

theorists to abandon the attempt to achieve specificity, and<br />

instead to use the term "stress" to refer to a general field<br />

<strong>of</strong> study that encompasses all <strong>of</strong> these more specific<br />

phenomena (Janis, 1958:ll-13: Lazarus, 1966:27; Levine ana<br />

Scotch. 1967:169: BcGrath. 1970:16). This is the approach<br />

taken in the "general stress madel,* accordirg to which<br />

family violeccs w i l l be analyzed in this chapter.<br />

Components Of Stress<br />

<strong>The</strong> phenomenon <strong>of</strong> stress can be seen as compossa <strong>of</strong><br />

several distinct elements. Drawing fcam schemes devslopsd<br />

by Eill (1958). Dohrenvend (1961). Levine and Scotch (1967)<br />

and BcGrath (1970). the general stress model treats the<br />

follo~irg as the compon~nt parts <strong>of</strong> any stress situatioc:<br />

-- ---<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> Stressor Stimulus. This refers to any situation<br />

or condition, encountered by an individual or group, that is<br />

capable <strong>of</strong> producing disroption or threat for that<br />

lndividoal or group. When this condition occurs, it can be


Ch.7. Stress and Family Violerce<br />

said that the stimulus is imposing a "denand" on<br />

ir-vclved, by regulrirg that Some response be a<br />

alleviate the stressor situation and remove the thr<br />

danger or disruption.<br />

Some theorists havs viewed StrPssor s'im<br />

necessarily synorymoos with major tragedi<br />

catastrophes. This idsa is ucfortunate, for it igno<br />

subtle effects <strong>of</strong> less dramatic stimuli. Re S<br />

Eoward rote:<br />

<strong>The</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> stress has besn t3o clos<br />

equated with extreme trauma acd duress. T<br />

association has hed the effect <strong>of</strong> divert:<br />

attention away from the study <strong>of</strong> stimuli th<br />

are wearing to tte organism, and that hav<br />

important physlological acd psychologicl<br />

COnSsquences for it, but which are neithc<br />

dramatlc or especially ur.usual...both trauaati<br />

and nontraumatic<br />

StTeSSfUl in ?he<br />

but wearing events ar<br />

SeCSE that th~y both produc<br />

the same types cf ptysiological<br />

psychological responses (1970:266-267).<br />

ac<br />

IC fact, it is not even necessary that the. stimol<br />

viGved in nsgaCive terms. Hany ceutrally or even posi<br />

defined rtimoli axe capable <strong>of</strong> placing great demands<br />

those experiencing them (Kolaes and Bahe, 1967). B.<br />

people respond as much tc their perception <strong>of</strong> a situati<br />

to the actual situation, virtually all stimuli in<br />

<strong>Social</strong> environmezt have the potential to produce stress<br />

some individuals.<br />

2. O ~ j ~ g DEmand. v e This refers<br />

realit? <strong>of</strong> a given situatior. To<br />

to the "object<br />

the extent tha<br />

strsssor StimU1uS poses demands that are independent <strong>of</strong><br />

cognitive procosses <strong>of</strong> definition and percoption,<br />

Individual Or group involved can be said to be experienc<br />

objective demand. While perhaps quite csmmon aT<br />

physiological or Siochenical levels <strong>of</strong> hnnan Eristen<br />

stressors that cornpletsly bypass cognitive auarsness<br />

evaluation are probably somewhat rare at the social lev<br />

Rowever. this is no? meant to imply that objective dsmana<br />

never an importanC aspect <strong>of</strong> "sociolzglcal" str<br />

situations. For example, facing the thought <strong>of</strong> parachut<br />

fxom a plans. 0; being confronted by whit appears to be<br />

armed criminal, are situations that pose a rery real thr<br />

to the unfortunate victim, irdependent <strong>of</strong> that pars3<br />

specific evaluation and definitton <strong>of</strong> the situation.<br />

-- ---<br />

3. Subj'=i&E Qg-=d. ?.ppley and ?rumbull h<br />

Observed that. "With the exception <strong>of</strong> EXtreBs and sud<br />

life-threatening situatfors. it is reasonable to say that<br />

stimulus is a stressor to all individuals txposed t3 i<br />

(1967:7). This variation in what is viewed as stressful


145s and Family Violecce Page 97<br />

individual diffcrezcfs in learning and experisace.<br />

has aptly noted, "Ran is vulnerable [ta stross]<br />

he reacts not only to the actual JXIS~CCS <strong>of</strong><br />

but to threats and symbols <strong>of</strong> danger erperieccsa in<br />

t" (1968:3). therefor^, ii frequently happens that a<br />

5 posing 10 real "Objective" danger is nonerhsless<br />

ed and defined +s probl~matic. 12 soch an irstance,<br />

es litae diff~relcf how mnch objective demand<br />

g exists: what :p irportazt is that the definition<br />

er or threat has been made, and ths indiviaualfs)<br />

d w i l l be acting in accordance with this definiiion.<br />

the terminology <strong>of</strong> the general stress sodel. this can<br />

rred to as "subjec+ive' demana.<br />

RPsBzse SaDabilitLss. This category includes all<br />

a skilis, attributes. and resources that an individual<br />

up has at its disposal in dsali~g vith stressor<br />

i. RlthOUgh the list <strong>of</strong> pOssibl9 T%spDnse<br />

llties is Fcdless, some <strong>of</strong> the more important oms<br />

a include past experience Ln asaling with similar<br />

1886, various skills that hare been learned or acquired,<br />

lligence and creativity, the motivation ro deal with the<br />

ation, support and encouragement from others in *he<br />

-a1 system, the asoort <strong>of</strong> energy availahlc, 325 th?<br />

ity to ccctrol orets emcticnal state. ill <strong>of</strong> these<br />

se elements are vieved by the general stress ao?el as<br />

ning to form a reservoir <strong>of</strong> possibl~ reepocsas to<br />

ssful stimuli.<br />

5, thgice <strong>of</strong> 2eazss. Rhen an individual or group is<br />

ronted by a stressor stimulus, one or more responses<br />

be selected from those availabla and applied 'cward<br />

s stressor. Obviously, just as individuals ana groups<br />

y substantially vith rEgard to what :hey sez as posing a<br />

eat or problem. so also w i l l they vary in their capacity<br />

deal with various eituatians acd in the repocses that<br />

ey w i l l make 11 a given sztuation. Bousver, it seems<br />

asonable to assume that, ir the great ~ajority <strong>of</strong> cases,<br />

effort w i l l be mde to meet the inpandtng deaands to the<br />

atest degree possible. This does not imply that ths best<br />

ice "ill aluays be made, but it does assone ths: the<br />

ividual or group w i l l he notioetsa to act in its best<br />

erest by making what it believes to be an appr3priate<br />

6, Stress Lev1.2. If t h response ~ made to a st:esror<br />

stimulus is Sutfxient to meet and sliminate the dpnands<br />

being posed by that stimulus, it can be said that nastery,<br />

or ~ucc~ssful rEso1utioP <strong>of</strong> the problem, has taken place.<br />

Under these circumstances, the stressor situation should no<br />

longer be problematic. In fact, ths individual or group<br />

w i l l probably be more effective in dealing with similar<br />

problems in the future because <strong>of</strong> this experience (Scott and<br />

aovard. 1970:272). In this serse, it can bt arguad 'tat<br />

stress has certain positfve consequences, provided it


Ch.7. Stress and Family BLoleece<br />

remairs within reasonable limits.<br />

Eowever, ?he rasponse made to a stressor sti<br />

not guaranree that demand w i l l be eliminated<br />

insLances, the responsefs) rill b~ insuffi<br />

inappropriate. and mastery w i l l not occur. Th<br />

such failure on the part <strong>of</strong> response copaDi<br />

according to tbe general stress model, an increa<br />

"stress lavel' <strong>of</strong> those Lnv~lved.<br />

Viewing "stress" as the discrepancy betw€en d<br />

response capabilities is cot a nev iaea:<br />

canceptualization is suggested by Mechanic (1962:<br />

1968:301) and XcGrath (1970:17-211, and is inplici<br />

and soward's eprohlen-solvingw stress model (1970<br />

and Scott, 1965). Eccolding to this view. the g<br />

discrepancy between impending demands an* 4aistilg<br />

capabilities. the greater the stress level <strong>of</strong> the i<br />

or group in question. In using the term "stress<br />

rather than simply 'stress," the grneral stre<br />

assumes that man and the groups he forms are in ?. c<br />

state <strong>of</strong> stress, and that a s-&Qt_e congru?rce<br />

demands and resporse c2pzhil;t=es for acy indLoi<br />

group is impossible.<br />

In accordance vith this view that stress<br />

"constant" in huaan experience, the general stress<br />

Suggests that individuals ana groups adapt to this fac<br />

life by developing a sLress level at vhich they fee<br />

comfortable ard function most Effectively. ThPsa flop<br />

stress levels develop over time, and they represe<br />

combined product <strong>of</strong> a number <strong>of</strong> social, psychological<br />

StruCturaL factors. Scot'. and Howard suggest that ih=<br />

wide Individual variations in these optimum stress 1s<br />

with some persors acd groups deaandi3g more congruence<br />

others:<br />

<strong>The</strong> cature <strong>of</strong> this lsvel variss tremendously<br />

BmOng individuals: <strong>The</strong>re ars persons xho<br />

reguire high and sustained levels <strong>of</strong> stimulatior<br />

in order to feel comfortable and satifled:<br />

there are others vho require camparatirely law<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> srlmulatlcn, and vho fsel most<br />

Comfortable when demands made on them arr<br />

tightly dispersed arourd very low activity<br />

levels (1970:270).<br />

In additlon to purely individual differences in optim<br />

str%ss level. substantial macroscopic variation betvsEn a<br />

among societies is also liXsly, with sems gr2ups accurtam<br />

to a much higher level <strong>of</strong> stress than others.<br />

Because these optimum stress levels are, in effect,<br />

cumulative product <strong>of</strong> all past experience, thay are lik<br />

to be firsly ectrenchsd ard quite resistant tc rapid


s~ and Fanily <strong>Violence</strong> Pilge 99<br />

nge. On the <strong>of</strong>her hard, an inaividual or group's<br />

+&& stress level is likely to be subje-? to<br />

fluctuation, deperding on the partLcular stimuli<br />

d. For the most part, these changes should be<br />

811nor. 211OY%Dg the stress level to remain within<br />

le, or at least tolerable. limits. In those<br />

in which changes are not minor, where stress level<br />

exceeded, the situation becones problematic.<br />

ding to the gecsral stress model, changss in<br />

eve1 occur whether the discrepancy between demands<br />

nse capabilities is "positiven or "negativeo in<br />

. Just as the demands being placed ??. an<br />

1 o r group nay be coo great, so too can they be too<br />

Grath uses the terms "overload" and uunderloaam to<br />

his disticction, and states:<br />

re is ncw a substantial body <strong>of</strong><br />

it~ratDre...whlch suggests t h ~ t tress-like<br />

ffects may result from an env:ror.ment that<br />

places too little asnand on...tha focal organism<br />

works such as Paliner's theory linking "t?nsi~n' and<br />

ce (197U; 1972). Klausner's studies <strong>of</strong> stress-seekirg<br />

, Aeroc's inv=s+igation <strong>of</strong> the 'lpathology <strong>of</strong> boreaoa"<br />

and seid~rberg's clinical descrip%ioc <strong>of</strong> the "trauma<br />

tl9ssness1' (1972) seem to support the co9tention ?hat<br />

tle demand can be just as dangerous 2s too much.<br />

o sumn~rize the notion <strong>of</strong> stress level, it can be saia<br />

"Stress not only rcvolves a *state' <strong>of</strong> the focal<br />

SP and a 'state' <strong>of</strong> the environment, but also involver<br />

.latlonship between the two11 (McGrath. 1970:16). <strong>The</strong><br />

ter the discrepancy betvefn existing demanas and<br />

lable respocse capabilities, the greater vill be the<br />

s5 level <strong>of</strong> that individual or grcup. T3 the extent<br />

this imbalacce is not presently, and does 09t suddenly<br />

me, too great: as lorg as mast existing demands cac bs<br />

t satisfactorily from within these response capabilities;<br />

d as long as enough demand exists to challenge the<br />

dividual or group sufficiently. fhe stress level cln be<br />

id to he at a minimal. nonproblematic level. iioverer. if<br />

increase in the actual stress level <strong>of</strong> ar individual or<br />

roup is a significant departure from the optimum stress<br />

~vel <strong>of</strong> tha; individual or group, this change is likely to<br />

ve overall negative consequences for those icvolvea.


Ch.7. Stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

unresolved Stress Sltuatlons<br />

In the discussion thus far, it has been argued th<br />

variables interact to determine an individual's or g<br />

stress level. From this proposition, it follows tha<br />

tailure to achieve mestery over a particular st<br />

stimulus can be ~ttributed to (1) the nature <strong>of</strong> tt.~ st<br />

situation, and/or (2) the response(s) made by<br />

Experiencing this stressor situation. As ve shall<br />

characteristics cf each <strong>of</strong> these elements <strong>of</strong> the s<br />

experlefics can preclude successful resolution 3f the p<br />

at hand.<br />

First, in considering stressor stinuli, it is o<br />

that problems vary ir terms <strong>of</strong> their potentia<br />

response. no matter what they atteapt. Sc?rt and Pouara<br />

(1970:271; Yoward and Scott, 1965:146-147) bavs identified<br />

three types <strong>of</strong> problem srtuatlons that prelude successful<br />

. .<br />

map:=ry: (11 prctl+ms *tk- jcsc deaacls rr FIC?:s :f ?he<br />

org??:sa.n reporre c?p=.b:lil:=s. (2) probl=lr ikat taw* co<br />

demands exist. so that it is imp3ssible to mse<br />

s~cce~~Fu11y. IP any <strong>of</strong> these situations, no natts<br />

response(s) are chosen, they simply w i l l not be sufL<br />

to achieve maszerp aver the stressor stimulus.<br />

Concerning the resporses made to stress sitoat-<br />

Scott and Howard (1970:272: Eoward and Scott. 1965:147have<br />

r.icely deliceated a typology <strong>of</strong> possLble resp3c<br />

<strong>The</strong>se include (a) the assertive response, in which t<br />

involved meet the problem "head-on," and attempt to solv<br />

in a direct and meafiirgful fashior., (b) the div~<br />

response. in which resources and eEergy are applied to<br />

problem but are ~1sdirected. t.t;us leaving the actual pr<br />

virtually untouched, and (c) the inert response, in eff<br />

a %on-response.' in which there is no meaningful att4<br />

made to resolve the problem at hald.<br />

Even rn stress situations in which an individual<br />

group faces a stresscr stimulus that is potecti<br />

solvable--in thar they possess response cepabili<br />

sufficient ro meet its demands--only by zhoosing<br />

assertive response w i l l they actually achieve msster<br />

Divergent or irert responses are not actullly directed<br />

the problem ir question; thus, these responses are unab<br />

to Contribute directly to a resolution <strong>of</strong> that problem.


stress and Panily <strong>Violence</strong> Psge 101<br />

~ences <strong>of</strong> the Failure to Achieve nastery<br />

r the above reasons, there are ruaerous stress<br />

tions in whlch the demands posed by a stresscr stisulus<br />

ot be met successfully. Responses to these szrPssors<br />

e made, yat mastery <strong>of</strong> the situetion floss not result.<br />

plained earlier, 'his discrepancy between demand and<br />

e capabilities represents the stress level <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

ual or group in question. And, to the axtent that<br />

iscrepancy represccts a drastic or major change 2n<br />

Stress level--in the direction away from one's optimal<br />

s level--it is to be expected rhat this unresolved<br />

situation may become even more problematic aver time.<br />

or sxample, ?he continued presencs <strong>of</strong> ths still onmet<br />

s nay have a serious impact npon those inrcl~e3. For<br />

e, the failure to resolve a problpm <strong>of</strong> f=~lty<br />

nication betweex a husband acd wife car easily lsaa to<br />

, mor~ serious, marital problems.<br />

1 second possible consequ-nce 3f a? iniffmcLive<br />

onse to stress may be that, not only has this respozse<br />

ed to deal with the crigi2al problem, but it pay have<br />

vertently introduced new stressor stimuli. 1s w~lff<br />

8:3) suggesrs, the consequences <strong>of</strong> a respons? mede to a<br />

55 situaticn may turn out tc he mare d~maglrg thin the<br />

cts <strong>of</strong> the stress Situation itself. Par zxsmple,<br />

ereas a husband hitting his wifo might appear to i?rminata<br />

fectively a current nari'al argument, this divergent<br />

sponse could, on a lorg-term basis, csuss r?scntmerf and<br />

stility to build up within the wife, thus posing<br />

re serioos problem for the future.<br />

an even<br />

Elso, thsre is what Scott ard Rovard have termed the<br />

cord order problem" <strong>of</strong> handlicg the unres3lved t=nsions<br />

t have arisen during the attempt to resolve the current<br />

45.5 situation. <strong>The</strong>se "tensicnsn refer ts a stite <strong>of</strong><br />

rgy arousal within the individual. In the words <strong>of</strong> Sco't<br />

EOwBrd:<br />

... evec when problems Ere successfully solvea. 3<br />

time gap exists between the initial prcvocation<br />

and the ultimate resolution. During the time ir<br />

which the problem is being dsalt with, the<br />

organism 1s i n a st+te <strong>of</strong> qrsater ar lESS=r<br />

Bobilization, a state in uhich Energy and<br />

resources are bound up so that ths organism<br />

experiences tension. In cases <strong>of</strong> successful<br />

problem salving. tonsicns are evantually<br />

dissipated and the organism returns to its ususl<br />

level <strong>of</strong> functioning. when problems are cot<br />

Solved, however. tensions persist until<br />

mechanisms are found to cope with them. <strong>The</strong><br />

failure to master threats therefore gives rise<br />

to a second-order problem: ... that <strong>of</strong> dealing


Ch.7. Stress ard Pamllp Vrolence<br />

Page 102


----<br />

~CT~SSO~<br />

~~~<br />

Page 103<br />

-~<br />

should ~resent no further difficult~. - ~ .~ Rcuever. . ir<br />

instances, the stressor situation will not be resalved<br />

he responses made. Ir these situations, the disczspancy<br />

een demand and responsa capabilities rspresents an<br />

ease in stress level. This increase in stress leo.1 has<br />

potenfial to produce additional problems for those<br />

Ired. Let us now apply this framework to the stuay <strong>of</strong><br />

ss and %he Family<br />

APPLYING TRE GWFRAL STRESS MODEL<br />

TO InTBAPAnILP BIOL37CY<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> theorists have already begun to irdicite<br />

importance <strong>of</strong> "stress" for explaining a major portion <strong>of</strong><br />

afaeily violence, especially in the area <strong>of</strong> chila sbuse.<br />

example, Blumberg (1964-1965) views overz~alons physical<br />

shment <strong>of</strong> children by parects as being as much s result<br />

the various 5treSSe~ experienced by *he child's pareots<br />

t is a result <strong>of</strong> the actual desire or neea to coctrol<br />

child's behavior. In Plumberg's words, "When everything<br />

getting On top <strong>of</strong> 2 no+her she smacks more"<br />

64-1965:19Y). Gil (1971) explains . socioec3nomic<br />

fferences in child abose as resulting at least partially<br />

m the difterential number and saverity ~f strassful<br />

eriances characterizing family life at different social<br />

ss levels. In explaining violence directea toward<br />

laren, Gelles' "social psychological' model <strong>of</strong> child<br />

use (1973) attributes primacy to a variety <strong>of</strong> "situational<br />

<strong>The</strong> ccnmon thread rurnicg through these statements is<br />

at any theory that attempts to explain irtrafamily<br />

olence nust give primary consideration to the structural<br />

biables likelp to determine a family's predisposition to<br />

olence. This is precisely what the stress fraseuork<br />

110~s US to do. since it views - ~ these structural - ~- variables<br />

StrPsSOr SIinuli thac impose demands on individuals and<br />

milies, and to which scme response must be made if stress<br />

VelS are to remain within tolerable limits.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re can be little doubt that the modern family, both<br />

a social unit and through its indiviaual members,<br />

cour.ters numerous stressor stimuli in the course <strong>of</strong> its<br />

y-to-day existence. As Campbell statss,


[Modern] changes [in family solidarity] ao 30<br />

necessarily signify a decllne in the impnrtanc<br />

Of the family. <strong>The</strong>y do reflect the increasin<br />

pressures which the fzmily is under--but t h ~ s<br />

stresses frequently stem precisely from the fac<br />

that more is baing drmanded <strong>of</strong> fanily life than<br />

ever before (1969: 251) -<br />

<strong>The</strong> stressor stimuli that corfront fasilies and<br />

members stem from many different scurces. For example<br />

are relatsd to the functions that the famil7 is "entr<br />

uith performing, both with regard to its individual me<br />

and to the larger social structure. Others are the<br />

<strong>of</strong> PErtiCUlar characteristics <strong>of</strong> i3dividual family<br />

such as their status in the scc'el stratification syst<br />

their present position ir the "family life cycle." Eo<br />

regardless <strong>of</strong> the Specific origins <strong>of</strong> the stresscr s<br />

that confront any particular family. the point tn b<br />

here is that these confrontations are facts <strong>of</strong><br />

life--not isolated and out-<strong>of</strong>-the-ozdinary CccurrencEs<br />

In addrtion to these external stresses, the f<br />

itself is <strong>of</strong>ten a source <strong>of</strong> stress. in that many pro<br />

can be traced back tc particular family sitoatim<br />

stroctures. For example, Croog (1970) notes that<br />

problems as intrafamily value conflicts, the parti<br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> the various stages <strong>of</strong> the family<br />

cycle, and problems <strong>of</strong> role conflict are potential soo<br />

Of Stress that arise from directly vithin the Easily u<br />

IP addition to those stresses that emanlte from withi<br />

family are numerous events experienced by individual me<br />

m the "external world,* vhich are then brought back<br />

the family setting. Rlthcugh the family is comnsnly Lho<br />

<strong>of</strong> as a place vhich family members can bring these ex<br />

Pressures and "let <strong>of</strong>f steam," this action may have o<br />

negative consequences for the family as a uhsle. A s<br />

states, "<strong>The</strong> release <strong>of</strong> these emotiscs nay in thsms-<br />

create tension situations that have the effects <strong>of</strong> strss<br />

as far as other family members are concern&" (1970:<br />

Even in situatiors in vhich the family itself is not<br />

source <strong>of</strong> a particular problem, the family may oererthe<br />

amplify the effects <strong>of</strong> that problem on its members,<br />

operating as a "crystalizing entity vithin vhich t<br />

stressors emerge and exert their impact" (Crsog. 1910:25<br />

Vlolence as a Response to Stress<br />

If th9 family does experience numezous stres<br />

situations. it Eollcus that individual family mcmbsrs,<br />

families as social units, <strong>of</strong>ten w i l l be called on to itts<br />

to meet and eliminate the demands posed by vari<br />

stressors. 3.36, as discussed earlier, when confronted w<br />

a stressful situation, these people w i l l look '0 th<br />

repertoires <strong>of</strong> response cepabilities as they try to come


stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong> Page 105<br />

response that produces mastery.<br />

learly. as Goode (1971:629) has suggested in his<br />

urce theory" <strong>of</strong> family violence, the potential to use<br />

ce can be regarded as an importart resource <strong>of</strong><br />

duals or groups. Pnt into the terms <strong>of</strong> the general<br />

5 model, the capacity for violence represents a portion<br />

individual's or group's response capabilities that can<br />

plied to the solutioc <strong>of</strong> varFous problems.<br />

st only is violence a Qpss&&f response t3 stressful<br />

.lors: in our society, it <strong>of</strong>ten represents an<br />

t& response. Xs stein me?^ and Straus note.<br />

icans have alvays had a propensiry to use violence to<br />

re natioral and personal goals4* 197:1Ul) B major<br />

n for the prevalence <strong>of</strong> violence thr2ughout our<br />

ty's history has been that 5ur social norms and<br />

ral values actually sancticr and legitimize t h use ~ <strong>of</strong><br />

nce as appropriate behavior in many situations (see<br />

er 3). This Cormative Epproval ?f violence seems to be<br />

ially true within th= context <strong>of</strong> ths family, wherr, for<br />

le, it is acceptable, or at least permissible, to spank<br />

sbehavlng child or slap a nagging rife when<br />

unstances "call for" such behavior.<br />

IS Viole~ce Used as a Response to Stress?<br />

<strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> violelce as a possible response to stress<br />

uations represents an interesting paradox. <strong>The</strong><br />

ilability <strong>of</strong> nost resources for any particular individual<br />

family is likely to depend or a number <strong>of</strong> structural acd<br />

rsonal features, including socioeconcnic status, 39%. and<br />

x. Borever, virtually everyone possesses th+ resource <strong>of</strong><br />

sntial violence. It is true that different inaiYiaucls<br />

1 require vastly different situations to provcke then to<br />

ertent that they will commit an act <strong>of</strong> violecca or<br />

ression. Ilso, the actual means <strong>of</strong> expressing violence,<br />

the resulting msuccessn <strong>of</strong> the act, rill vary greatly.<br />

ever, the point to be made here is that, whereas the<br />

session <strong>of</strong> most other resoorces varies from pers3n to<br />

erson and family to family, the potential for violence is a<br />

constact" part <strong>of</strong> all individual and family respocse<br />

pabilltles, no matter how extensive or how minimal the<br />

malrder <strong>of</strong> the response capabilities.<br />

on the other hand, althoogh everyone cqn use violence,<br />

at everyone do=. <strong>Violence</strong> is not almys the response<br />

hosen to a stressful situation. In fact. it is used<br />

Elatively infrequently. Thus. o ~ e question <strong>of</strong> major<br />

mportance corceras the issue <strong>of</strong> ~gqp violence is usEd as an<br />

SSErtiV0 response to a stress situation. and Kg1 it is used<br />

n those instances.


Ch-7. Stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> Availability <strong>of</strong> Rlternatire Responses to Strsss<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the most important considerations in ans<br />

this question conce?ns the nature <strong>of</strong> th? other re<br />

capabilities. A s Bettelheim suggests. "whether or n<br />

w i l l use violence or avoid it depends entirely 0.<br />

alternative solution5 are known to a person facin<br />

problem" (1967:301). Similarly. Steinmetz and Straus a<br />

We believe that the willingness and ability to<br />

us6 physical violence is a 'resource'.... E<br />

family member car use this resource tc<br />

CompePSate for lack <strong>of</strong> otter such rssouzces ir<br />

money, knouledge, and resFsct (1974:9).<br />

Goode has developed this general idsa in b:s rtsg<br />

theory <strong>of</strong> family violence (1971). Goode identifies "fo<br />

and its threat" as on9 <strong>of</strong> four major sets <strong>of</strong> ressurces t<br />

an individual can use in c?Lcmptirg +o achiere desrrsd i<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> can be used as z resource uhen other alterpa<br />

resources, i.e.. econcmic, prestige, and trait resoor<br />

are ulavailable or have proved ineffective. RDusver, G<br />

emphasizes thac the actual use <strong>of</strong> violence is likely t<br />

dependent upon the state <strong>of</strong> the individual~s<br />

resources. Thus. lf other appropriate resources do e<br />

it is likely that one or more <strong>of</strong> these w i l l be triad<br />

solution to the problem instead <strong>of</strong> violence. as<br />

states:<br />

most people do cot willingly choose overt<br />

vlolence vhen they command other means because<br />

the costs <strong>of</strong> using force are high in ary social<br />

system, but especially in the family....<br />

Consequently, it is a general rule that the<br />

greater the other resources an indiviaual can<br />

command, the more force he can muster. hut the<br />

less he w i l l actually deploy or use force in 3n<br />

overt manner (1971:628).<br />

Thus, we see that violence and aggression are 'substitut<br />

for other types <strong>of</strong> resources vhen these other resources a<br />

rot a par' <strong>of</strong> existing response capabilities.<br />

SUbc~ltural or Family Norms Regarding <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Ox the oce hand, it has been argued that our soci<br />

has norms and values that positively sanction the use<br />

vlOlePCe in the family setting. Rnd this legitimization<br />

aggressivs behavior is likely to increase the extont<br />

which violence is ~ctually used as a "problem-solo<br />

technique" within the family.


h.7. Stress and Pamily <strong>Violence</strong> Page 107<br />

Aouever, as Wolfgang and Perracuti suggest in their<br />

subculture <strong>of</strong> violencP thecry (1967), it is alsc likrlg<br />

* the norms governing the appropriateness <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong><br />

olance w i l l also vary somewhat wirhiq society. Thus, in<br />

cme segments <strong>of</strong> society, violence nay be regarded as mars<br />

ulturally acceptable than it is in other po~tions <strong>of</strong><br />

ociety. A s this 1eg:tiaation <strong>of</strong> violence vanes, the<br />

redisposition to use violence as a response to stress<br />

tuations should vary accordingly. Thus, in those<br />

ubcultures.' and in those particular family ur:ts, vhere<br />

istirg norms most explicitly sanction violence and<br />

grfssion, we should most <strong>of</strong>ten ~xppct ToSpOn95 <strong>of</strong> a<br />

olent nature.<br />

st Experience with <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Empirical evidecce suggests that persocs who behave<br />

ggressiv~ly withic their families tend to have family<br />

ackgrounds <strong>of</strong> viclence (Xeinpe el 2.. 1962: Stcsl? and<br />

ollock. 1968: Gil. 1971). This sugges5s that. no5 only is<br />

iolenc~ a learned behavior. but in addition, is taught end<br />

rznsnitted by the family as part <strong>of</strong> the socialization<br />

recess. Thus. it can be argued that the family operates as<br />

"training ground" fcr violence (Steinmetz and Straus,<br />

974: Pa?? IV: Gellss ard Straus, 1978).<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>. Tension. and Frustration<br />

Thus far, discussion has centered on the use <strong>of</strong><br />

iolePce as an assertive response to a stress3r s:imulus.<br />

he implicaticn has been that violent?, wher used in this<br />

ashior. represents a rational attempt on the park <strong>of</strong> a<br />

erson to deal with a probleaatic situation in what that<br />

erson believes is an appropriate and effective aacner to<br />

hieve a desired end. This coacepticn <strong>of</strong> stress-induced<br />

olerce seems conparable to what Gelles and Straus have<br />

raed instrumental violerce--"the use <strong>of</strong> pair or injury as<br />

punishsent to isduce another person to carry out some act


Ch.7. Stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

or refrain from ar act11 (1'378).<br />

Aovever, there is another place in the general<br />

model where violence is expected to occur-as a conseq<br />

<strong>of</strong> the secord-oreer problems <strong>of</strong> tension and frustratis<br />

can result from unresolved stress situations. In fa<br />

15 here that intrafamily violence is likely t3 occu<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten--not as an assertive, prcblem-oriectsd at+e<br />

achieve mastery <strong>of</strong> a situation, but rathor a<br />

"irrational.' lashing-out behavior spawned by see<br />

unresolvable problems. And, just as the concep<br />

stress-i~duced violence appear; to corresp3nd cl3sa<br />

Gelles and Straus* definition ot "instrumental" violenc<br />

does the idea <strong>of</strong> violence as a reactim to frus'ratio<br />

tension seem roughly compsrable to "exprassiva" Violec<br />

defined by these authors---the us- <strong>of</strong> physical fa<br />

cause pain or injury as ar. end itself" (Gelles and s<br />

1978) .<br />

Frustration and Stress<br />

AS used by <strong>Social</strong> scientists. the concfp<br />

frustr3tion seams to r9fer most <strong>of</strong>ten t3 in emotional s<br />

accompanied frequently by anger and anxiety, that may a<br />

vhen an individual is prevented from attaining a ass<br />

gnal. We suggest that this affective state <strong>of</strong> frustra<br />

can, like tension, be viewed as a 'second-order prob<br />

Springing from the failure to satisfactorily resolve<br />

stress situation--in effect, ar "emotioral counterpart<br />

the unexpended energy that Scott and Bovard term "tensio<br />

Tb* incorporation <strong>of</strong> stress-produced "frustration"<br />

the general stress modal appears to bs coisistent w i t<br />

rest <strong>of</strong> the framework. Implicit in this trsatmen<br />

frustratioC are all <strong>of</strong> the necessary elements <strong>of</strong> a s<br />

situation discussed earlier: (a) a stressor stimulus,<br />

the form <strong>of</strong> a desired goal, and pressurss. both internal<br />

external. to attain that aoal: lbl . . obiective scd<br />

Sublective demand to achieve that Goal: . tcl .. the response<br />

capabilities <strong>of</strong> the person from which an assertivb,<br />

divergent. or ar tuert response, w i l l be ch3sen: and (a<br />

discrepancy between demand and response, implying that.<br />

ole recson or another, the desired goal has nct be<br />

attaiced.<br />

Vlewing frustration in this manner. the general stre<br />

model is able to treat a vide range <strong>of</strong> specific situatio<br />

and conditions as nfrustraticg." For exaaple, a mother m<br />

beccme ufrUstratEd' by the fact that her young ch<br />

continually cries and misbehaves. e husband and f%t<br />

whose racial and educatiocal background prevents hLm f<br />

obtaining a stable job through vhich he can support<br />

family may well be "frusirated*. by this situation. a no<br />

may find the housewife-mother role "frustrat=ngql in tcrss


Ths Qeneral stress model would not denv the important<br />

een as arising from an unresolvsd stress situation.<br />

7s. the aeneral stress model sees all frustration as<br />

lying the existence <strong>of</strong> Stress, ln. t h ~ t it ultimately<br />

s from the discrepancy betveen a desired goal and the<br />

ity to aChieVE that gcal.<br />

.ration and the Family<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> inves?igatoFs have lrckee fTuS+r?.ti3E with<br />

afamily violence. For example, Gil's the?ry <strong>of</strong> child<br />

= (1971) holds that the poverty existing in the lover<br />

econonic levels cf society creates frustrstions<br />

sed 1c child abuse. O'Brien explains violence between<br />

nds and wives by suggesting that the inability to<br />

hieve adequa$e economic revards might result in "f anily<br />

tered venting <strong>of</strong> the aggressiveness on the par? <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sband which had its antecedence in frusL.rstions<br />

ountered in the larger structures <strong>of</strong> the social and<br />

nomic world" (1971:696). shitehurst (1974) predicts that<br />

trend toward greater equality for women may us11 pose a<br />

jor source <strong>of</strong> frustration, and hence <strong>of</strong> violence, f3r men<br />

cannot cope with their new status.<br />

BUT. vhat is it abour the family that has promptea these<br />

d other frustration-based theories <strong>of</strong> family violence? Is<br />

ere a special connec'ion betveen frustrakion and the<br />

mily? Is the family a more likely setting than other<br />

cia1 groups for the geceration. and acting sut. <strong>of</strong><br />

ustration?<br />

<strong>The</strong> family can be regarded as a urique social entity in<br />

eras <strong>of</strong> its potential for frustration. Part <strong>of</strong> the reason<br />

or this special positioz, as discussed earlier, is that the<br />

amily routinely encounters numerous strsssor stimuli in the<br />

curse <strong>of</strong> its daily Existence. <strong>The</strong> sheer number <strong>of</strong> Stresses<br />

aced probably ensures that et least some <strong>of</strong> these cannot be<br />

esolved.<br />

Ic additicn,<br />

ociety and the<br />

the family owing to its positinn in<br />

mechanics <strong>of</strong> its int~rnal structure, is<br />

ikely to bp confronted with a number <strong>of</strong> stressors that have<br />

lov probability <strong>of</strong> successful resolution sca hence s high<br />

otentLal fcr generating frustrat:on. Some <strong>of</strong> th? more<br />

ommon "insolublen demands families <strong>of</strong>ten face include the


Ch.7. Stress and Family <strong>Violence</strong><br />

the s~~~essful regulation <strong>of</strong> the conflicts that s<br />

occur among intimates, +nd the supervis'on <strong>of</strong> the act<br />

<strong>of</strong> all taaily members ir the external world.<br />

Thus, the family seems to engendsr frustratior<br />

both quantitatively and quali?atively, it faces<br />

problems in trying to counter stressor stiauli.<br />

Gelles and Straus probably do not overstate th<br />

saying that "the family, by virtue <strong>of</strong> its struc<br />

functions, can be viewed as an inherently fru<br />

institution for its members* (1978:28).<br />

Response Capabilities <strong>of</strong> Families<br />

However, the large number and vide variety <strong>of</strong> d<br />

stress experiences aIe only part <strong>of</strong> the reas9<br />

frustration is typical <strong>of</strong> families. Enothor sst <strong>of</strong><br />

InCIEaSeS the likelihood <strong>of</strong> frustration withln -he<br />

<strong>The</strong>se tactors derive from the ability <strong>of</strong> families to<br />

thE stres~es they do face.<br />

Although the family may be relatively "stress-p<br />

it does not necessarily follow that the fam<br />

well-equipped to cope with these stressors and the a<br />

that they impose. on the contrary, apparently the<br />

urban family is "a less than successful group dealin<br />

stresses and problems" (Gelles and Straus. 1978).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are many reasons for this inadequacy<br />

example, the status positions represented within the<br />

generate diverse, <strong>of</strong>ten conflicting, interests, and<br />

conflicts nay interfere vith optimal decision-making.<br />

because the typical moderr faally is relativaly smal<br />

limited rumber <strong>of</strong> people can be used as resour-<br />

attempting to cope vith a difficult situation. In add<br />

the intimate and intense nature <strong>of</strong> family relatic<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten means that decisions are made emotionally rather<br />

rationally.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se and other structural characteristics are 1<br />

to render the family a relatively inefficient sccial<br />

with regard to both the decision-waking and the perf,<br />

<strong>of</strong> tasks based upon these decisions. Thls ineffic<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten makes it difficult to mest the demands <strong>of</strong> such<br />

family rasponsibilitias as socialization <strong>of</strong> the youo<br />

fulfillaen? <strong>of</strong> the emotioral needs <strong>of</strong> all family aembez<br />

say nothing <strong>of</strong> the more dramatic crises that frsqu<br />

befall inaividoal families.


tress and Family Violsnce Page 111<br />

posed hy these stimuli. Bnd, according to the<br />

stress model, the result <strong>of</strong> this unfortunate<br />

for the generation <strong>of</strong> frustration.<br />

as a Reaction to Frustration and Tension<br />

n if we agree that the likelihood <strong>of</strong> frustration<br />

g in the family 1s high, the eruption <strong>of</strong> xip&pgce is<br />

aiced. R causal linkage is necessary to detzrnine<br />

how frustration in the fam'ly translates into<br />

ive behavior.<br />

OaUSE the general stress model equatfs the<br />

no?s <strong>of</strong> tension and frustration, it is useful to<br />

to Scott and Howard's treatment ~f tension<br />

272-273) in attoepticg to detersice tta relatiocship<br />

.n frustration and violence. <strong>The</strong>se aufhors argue that.<br />

an icdividual is faced with the "second-ordsr problem"<br />

-tempting to resolve the tension created by a failure at<br />

y, the only rpal course <strong>of</strong> action svailable is to<br />

ate this accumulated tension through some mechanism <strong>of</strong><br />

e. Tha; such a line <strong>of</strong> reasoning is applicable to the<br />

nal state <strong>of</strong> frustr~tioc is shorn by what EEndura<br />

:31-39) has referred to as the "aggressive drive<br />

lesn <strong>of</strong> violent behavior, most notable <strong>of</strong> which is<br />

d gt alL's "frustration-aggression hypothesis'' 11939).<br />

hether one prefers to attribute this behavior to<br />

ses <strong>of</strong> social learning (Bandura and Falters. 1963;<br />

,a, 1973; Steicmetz and Straus, 197U) or to innate<br />

gical potertial (Dollazd pi gL.. 1939; ardrey, 1966:<br />

z. 1966). there can be little denying that ViolencE is<br />

inly one <strong>of</strong> the more popular means <strong>of</strong> "blowicg <strong>of</strong>f<br />

in our society. ffcr only is violence a fig_ousg:<br />

nism <strong>of</strong> tersion release, but, at least in emerica.<br />

ression is defined as a Erg& response to frustrition"<br />

inmetz and Straus. 1974:9). Thus, to the extent that<br />

family has an Rinherently frustrating" chanctsr,<br />

ressive violence should be expected to occur <strong>of</strong>ten within<br />

family setting.<br />

It should be emphasized that a distinct, qualitative<br />

rence exists bitwsen violence that ts an asser:ive.<br />

umental reponse to a stressor situation, and vi3lence<br />

is an expressive reactioE to frustratior and tersion.<br />

difference holds even though the same stressor<br />

ation can be the source <strong>of</strong> violence in bsth oases. and<br />

. thouqh these two types <strong>of</strong> violence can be outwardly<br />

istinguishahle.<br />

<strong>The</strong> reason fcr making this distinction is that very<br />

ferect cognitive processes appear to be involved in these<br />

types <strong>of</strong> violent act. on thc one hand, strass-induced,


Ch.7. Stress and Family Violerce Page 112<br />

instrumental violence is problem-oriented and<br />

goal-directed--an attempt to directly meet and resolve the<br />

demands posed by a stressor stimulus.<br />

On the other hand, frustration-produced, expressive<br />

violence lacks this ra?ioral, problem-solving dimension. It<br />

is not an assertive response, and it is not really dirsctea<br />

toward the solution <strong>of</strong> a problem. llthough this kind <strong>of</strong><br />

violent behavior is goverred, at least to some extent, by<br />

internalized norms that specify spproprilts types <strong>of</strong> corduct<br />

in given situatiocs (Gelles and Straus, 1978:21)--+nn thus<br />

is not really "unconscicus" or "beyond the control" <strong>of</strong> the<br />

actor--the fact remains that expressivs violence doss not<br />

represent the rational, carefully chosrn rssponss to a<br />

Str4ssor SltuatioL that instrumental violence doss.<br />

Expressive Vlolence and Cctharsx<br />

<strong>The</strong> general stress model's treatment <strong>of</strong> .expressive<br />

violence does not imply adherence to vhst Steinmetz and<br />

Strans have termed the "cathars%s ~vthn IlqlU!lu-~K.<br />

~ .~~.<br />

Straus. 1974). Proponents <strong>of</strong> t<br />

(Dollard ey G.. 1939: Bett<br />

1968) have argue,~~ ~~~<br />

violence tends to -. .- . , - - - - - . -<br />

feelings to the point where more serious aggression results;<br />

hence. they regard the limited Expression <strong>of</strong> aggression as<br />

desirable and beneficial. Acwersr. the aeneral stress model<br />

~ ~- ~~<br />

does not consider frustration-produced violence in such<br />

functionalist terns: rather. it expresses an empirical fact<br />

regarding intrafamily violence--that it is <strong>of</strong>ten a<br />

consequence <strong>of</strong> unresolved stress situations, an8 that the<br />

"intervening* process is frustration.<br />

Possibly, expressive violence doas improve a stress<br />

situation temporarily. Even Straus, a severe critic <strong>of</strong> the<br />

catharsis viewpoirt, concedes, '<strong>The</strong>re can be little d<br />

that an outburst <strong>of</strong> aggressive activity is <strong>of</strong>ten follove<br />

a sharp reduction in tension, an emotional release, and<br />

a feeling <strong>of</strong> quiescence. Thus, there is <strong>of</strong>ten ar =meed3<br />

cathartic effect" (1979a:25). However. even if viol<br />

does dissipate accumulated frustration and tension, t<br />

outcomes are probably only temporary and superficial. P<br />

a long-term rievpoirt, it is doubtful t<br />

trustration-caused violence w i l l have acy mfaningf<br />

posltive affect on a problem: Expressive violenze rare1<br />

directed at the geriuine causes <strong>of</strong> a problem. <strong>The</strong>refore.<br />

is unlikely to change the cocditions that caused the tens<br />

and rrustratlon in the first place. In fact, because <strong>of</strong><br />

potential for reinforcing future violent behsvior (Feshb<br />

1970; Bandura. 1973:31-39: Steinmet2 and Str<br />

1974:14-1.5)- expressive violence not only fails to impro<br />

present troablesome family situation, but also can c<br />

unintended and undesirable long-term consequences.


ch.7. Stress and Family Violerca Page 113<br />

vwerload" versus "Underload" Stress Situations<br />

E SOUTC~ <strong>of</strong> Some difficulty ir the scheme proposed here<br />

whether any qualitative differences exist between<br />

erload" and "underlOzd*- stress situations ic their<br />

trntial for generating expressive violent;. Rs discussed<br />

arllsr. the general stress model deflnes stress as sgp<br />

~gnificant discrepancy between demand and respor.se<br />

apabilities, whether demand exceeds response capebiliti~s<br />

r vice versa. <strong>The</strong> connectiors between stress, frustraticn,<br />

rd violence may be fairly clear in the case <strong>of</strong> "overload"<br />

tress situations: what about the cornectiocs between<br />

xpressive violence and "underload" stress sifuations marked<br />

y excessive rootine, lack <strong>of</strong> challenge. and e general<br />

bsence <strong>of</strong> stimulation?<br />

gsgarding the comparative effects af '*ovnrlnsd* rsrsus<br />

ndsrlcad" stress situations as causes cf violence and<br />

gression, the presect inclination <strong>of</strong> the general stress<br />

del is to assume that they are. in effect. the same. This<br />

consistent with the "Clockwork Orange" theory <strong>of</strong> family<br />

lence (GElles and Straus. 1978:30-32). vhich suggests<br />

t aggressive behavior <strong>of</strong>ten occurs in situations where<br />

e glove fits too smoothly and family members try to. 'stir<br />

ngs upv jusr to maks things interesting." On the strength<br />

this +hecry, then, one would expsct psrsnnr to sttack<br />

thers VhEn their lives are marked by boredom and lack <strong>of</strong><br />

citcment. just as t h ~ y do when faced by unsolvable<br />

oblems.<br />

uesticn<br />

Aovever, there are those who would probably<br />

this assumption (Palmer, 1970; 1972: Gelles and<br />

traus, 1978:31), sugges5icg that this aspect <strong>of</strong> the<br />

elationship between stress acd violence needs<br />

nvestigalian before this question can be resolved.<br />

much


Ch.7. Stress and Family Biolecce<br />

If Sound, the geceral stress mods1 would seam to<br />

a strong position to contribute sobstentially tow<br />

integrated theory <strong>of</strong> family violatce. It allows fo<br />

incorporation <strong>of</strong><br />

social-psychological<br />

elements<br />

approaches<br />

fron several imp<br />

to family violenca<br />

example. frustration-aqgression theory and social le<br />

theory. Et the same time, the general stress model<br />

into account the various structural factors that de?s<br />

which families w i l l be most likely to Experience s<br />

what r3~ources they w i l l have at the3 disposal io ae<br />

stress, and, most important <strong>of</strong> all, the likelihoo<br />

stress H i l l result in violence (Fzrrinqton, 1975:37-45<br />

NOTE<br />

*Portions <strong>of</strong> sections <strong>of</strong> this paper appeir in nTow<br />

General <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Stress and Family violrncs.* by<br />

Farrington, a paper presented at the 1975 annual reeti<br />

the Rational Council on Family Rslatioos.


Chapter 8<br />

<strong>The</strong> Paradoxical Nature <strong>of</strong> Family<br />

Relationships and Family Conflict<br />

Joyce E. Foss<br />

Not all violence betveen family members is<br />

the result <strong>of</strong> a conflict. Ind cot all conflicts<br />

end in violence. whether or not the inevitable<br />

confllcts <strong>of</strong> family life eventuate in viclence<br />

depends partly on vhether there are alternative<br />

methods for resolving the corflict. Bowever, in<br />

Chapter 7 Parrington proposed that families may<br />

be structured in a vay that adversely affects<br />

their capabilities for resolving confllcts. In<br />

this chapter. Poss examines this contention in<br />

more detail and also concludes that in intimate<br />

groups like the family, structural constraints<br />

work against effective conflict resolution.<br />

rmong +he factors working agaixst families'<br />

using conflict ~esclution techniques that have a<br />

low probability <strong>of</strong> producing violence are the<br />

high emotional investment betveer family<br />

members. their total personality involvement.<br />

and the knowledgs that members cannot simply<br />

pick up and leave when a conflict develops.<br />

Simple avoidance is a widely used technique to<br />

prevent an escalation into violent encour.ters.<br />

but it is a very difficult strategp in groups<br />

that are structured to generats high levals <strong>of</strong><br />

interactio~, ecoromic interdependence. and<br />

smotional commitment. Besides. as Poss notes.<br />

the avoidance <strong>of</strong> conflict in in+imate groups<br />

uill lead only to more conflict.


Ch.8. Paradoxical Family Relationships Page<br />

interpersonal violence and aggression. <strong>The</strong> issue cf thl<br />

relationship between family structura and conflict anr<br />

violence 1s important for several reasons. Firs?, neithel<br />

family sociologists nor conflict theorists gDnerally ha.,!<br />

taken full advantage or the family arena as one in which<br />

conflict approach might be usefully applied (Parrington an(<br />

POss. 1977: Skolnick, 1973: Spre?, 1969). Second. more<br />

specifically, conflict theory has bear iaentifisa as r<br />

potentially fruitful approech to the study <strong>of</strong> violecce ir<br />

families (Gelles and Straus, 1974; Steirmetz snd Strams,<br />

1974:5-6). Further, several other frameworks i3sntifiea aa<br />

valuable in the area, such as resource, structura,<br />

exchange, and frustraticn-aggression theoriss (Gellps an[<br />

Straus, 1374). also include confl2ct as a c+ntral coccept.<br />

Yet, thoogh conflict <strong>of</strong>ttn is preselted as celtral tc, or a<br />

precondition for, violence. a specific theory <strong>of</strong> hor<br />

conflict processes and violence are related has not been<br />

adequately delineated.<br />

Lewis Coser's work or cocflic: provides a found<br />

for this analysis: his extensive attention to inti<br />

groups makes his work readily applicable to the fsm<br />

This chapter presmts a number <strong>of</strong> propositions conce<br />

family Conflict and violencs and summarizes the formul<br />

In diagrammatic form. A major emphasis in the chapt<br />

carefnl specification <strong>of</strong> key concepts in the area.<br />

TKX F91ILY PS A STRUCTURAL PERADOX<br />

COSerls analysis <strong>of</strong> conflict in intimate groups hi<br />

on a recognition <strong>of</strong> the paradoxical nature <strong>of</strong> t<br />

Structure. Intimate groups are paradoxical in that ths<br />

Strnctural elements that make hostility likely also crea<br />

high probability that hostility w i l l be suppressed. <strong>The</strong><br />

key characteristics <strong>of</strong> intimate groups ara the<br />

freguency <strong>of</strong> interaction and the total persona<br />

involvement among group membsrs. <strong>The</strong> following propositi<br />

illustrate the paradoxical relationship <strong>of</strong> these struct<br />

features with hostility and the suppression <strong>of</strong> hostility<br />

TWO focal variables 1r thls part <strong>of</strong> the analysis are<br />

confllct <strong>of</strong> Interest asd hostlllty. wblch are deflred as<br />

rollo~s:<br />

Conflict sf lpterest: hr. nobjsctrv€a sztuatlsr.<br />

In which two or more partles hold<br />

contradictory saloes and clalms over scarce<br />

status. power, and resources.


8. Paradoxical Paaily Relationships Pags 117<br />

This definition <strong>of</strong> conflict <strong>of</strong> interest concurs vith<br />

er's use <strong>of</strong> phrases like "conflict situation" or<br />

casions for conflict' to refer to a situation in which<br />

interests <strong>of</strong> the participants are objectively at odds.<br />

r sharply distinguishes between such a situation and<br />

llct itself, which refers to a particular kind <strong>of</strong> 5 g f 6 ~ ~<br />

ehavioral strategy. This distixction w i l l be fu?ther<br />

ored when cocflict itsElf is discussed. Ros'ility also<br />

no? a behavior or action, but is a ~cQj~~fiv3 feeling <strong>of</strong><br />

ositioc, that may<br />

flicting interests.<br />

or may so? occur when peopl= havs<br />

ily Structure and Hosxllitp<br />

Intimate groups are by definition characterized by<br />

al, rather thar segmented. personality involvament 2nd bp<br />

igh frequsncy <strong>of</strong> interaction (Coser, 1956:611). Following<br />

work <strong>of</strong> Simmel and Freud, Coser argues that habitusl or<br />

tense interactior "furnishes frequent occasions for<br />

nflict' (1956:62)--or frequent conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest, in<br />

rms <strong>of</strong> the present argument. In relationships with 3 high<br />

equnncy <strong>of</strong> interaction, conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest w i l l be<br />

ed and numerous. In cther words. frequect interaction<br />

uces both a high number or ESZ~B~P <strong>of</strong> conflicting<br />

rests and a high frequency with which those conflicts <strong>of</strong><br />

.rest w i l l arise.<br />

<strong>The</strong> relationship be?ween degren 9f personality<br />

Olvement and conflict <strong>of</strong> interest is similar. "<strong>The</strong> more<br />

relationship is basnd on the participation <strong>of</strong> the total<br />

ocality--as distinct from segmental participation"<br />

er. 1956:62), or the more bases on which persons are<br />

racting vith Ens arothsr, the greater the var:DkY and<br />

nency <strong>of</strong> the conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest that arise. Two<br />

ositions can then be stated:<br />

P,o~~br&op 1. <strong>The</strong> more frequent :he<br />

interaction, the more freguent and varied are<br />

Conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest.<br />

-- Pro~ogz%g 2. <strong>The</strong> more total the personality<br />

involvement in a relationship. the more<br />

frequent and variea are conflicts <strong>of</strong><br />

interest.<br />

coser further asserts (1956:62):<br />

---<br />

Prog%ir;im 3. Conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest tend to<br />

prcduca hostility.<br />

This proposition states that when people are face3 with<br />

onflicts <strong>of</strong> interest, snbjective feelings <strong>of</strong> ill w i l l or<br />

position are likely to arise. Bowever, this rule is not<br />

thout rxcepticn. For example, subjective hostility map


--- not occur when 'he participants define as legitimate an<br />

Aobjectivelyn unequal distribution <strong>of</strong> resources. Yet even<br />

though conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest do not always laed to<br />

hostility, zt is appropriate to stata przposition 3 as a<br />

general tendency.<br />

Proeosltlon 4. Preqosnt lnteractror tads to<br />

-- - create hostxllty.<br />

Propos-?lo% 5. Total prrsonallty involvement<br />

tezds tc creat- hostlllty.<br />

To turther snmnarize the argument thus far. if lrtrmate<br />

groups are those characterized by frequent lnteractron and<br />

total personality Involvement. then:<br />

a=~s&&?; 5. Intzmate relatlonshlps tend t><br />

create hostlllty.<br />

Pamllr Structure and Suppression <strong>of</strong> Aostzlity<br />

Intimate relationships tend not only to<br />

hostility, but also to cause a high affective or expre<br />

investment in the relationship that in turn gene<br />

efforts to suppress hostility (Coser, 1956:EO-6<br />

affective investment seems to involvs tv3 elements:<br />

positive liking or attraction to the relationship. ana<br />

replaceability <strong>of</strong> the members <strong>of</strong> the relationship*l. Th<br />

two concepts are roughly equivalent to the concepts<br />

Conparison Level and Comparison Level <strong>of</strong> Rlternativss<br />

Thibaut and Kelley's exchange theory (1959).<br />

In accordance vl+h Aomans' hypcthssis that iocr~ase<br />

irteraction 1s related to '*mutual sentlments <strong>of</strong> llklng,"<br />

Coser suggests (1956:62) :<br />

~~=posltlon 1. <strong>The</strong> more frequent the<br />

interactlon. the greater the posltlve affect<br />

among group members.<br />

<strong>The</strong> specific effects <strong>of</strong> persocality involvement are<br />

entirely clear in Caser's discussion, but it s..<br />

reasonabls to posit that total personality involvemeni a0<br />

not have an important direct effect on positive affect, b<br />

influences the perception <strong>of</strong> the replaceability <strong>of</strong> gr<br />

members. E relationship characterized by total persona<br />

involvement is largely built on the unique cmtributions<br />

each personality. Bs Simmel points out, this is especia<br />

true in dyadic relationships like marriage (1950:122-1<br />

when this occurs. the replacement <strong>of</strong> one group member<br />

at the very least seriously alter the quality <strong>of</strong> g<br />

relationships. Thus:


8. Paradoxical Family Felationships ?age 119<br />

-- P r o . <strong>The</strong> more total the personality<br />

involvelert ir a relationship, the lower the<br />

replaceibility <strong>of</strong> group members.<br />

Ps suggested above, positive affect and low<br />

laceability <strong>of</strong> group meabsrs cln be further relatea in a<br />

necting proposition.<br />

--- Proeg&r&ip 9. <strong>The</strong> greater the positivs affect<br />

ard the lower :he replac-ability <strong>of</strong> group<br />

members, the greater the cff~ctive or<br />

expressive inv=?:mert 1n the group.<br />

coser further suggesrs (1956, 1962, 1968):<br />

--- ProEoslfion 10. <strong>The</strong> greater the affectivs<br />

investment in a relationship, the greater the<br />

fear <strong>of</strong> dissolution <strong>of</strong> the relationship.<br />

p=oEosi;t_Zor 11. <strong>The</strong> greater the fear if<br />

dissolution or a relationship, the greatmr<br />

the tecdency to suppress hostility.<br />

Rnd finally, to summarize. we can state:<br />

Propositio~<br />

12. Irtimate relationships tend t3<br />

produce suppressiox <strong>of</strong> hostility.<br />

rumental DEpe2dency ard Suppression <strong>of</strong> Eastility<br />

<strong>The</strong> preceding discussior treats only the high aflgEfiEp<br />

stment in intimate groups acd its subsequent effect on<br />

ression <strong>of</strong> hostility. <strong>The</strong>re are also significant<br />

rueectal icvestmects in relationships. Cos3rvs Dsphasis<br />

ntiRatE g~oups that are based on voluntary association<br />

havs influfnced his lack <strong>of</strong> attention t3 the<br />

rumental aspects <strong>of</strong> irtimate relatiorships. This is not<br />

ar that volurtary relationships are never based a: least<br />

ially on irstrumantal concezcs. Rather, we suggest that<br />

relationships are rocvoluntary their instrunental bases<br />

me more importart and nore appzrent. This seems ?a be<br />

c+se with the family.<br />

rs sprey points out, "participation ir the fsmily is<br />

a troly voluntary matter" (1969:702). In the first<br />

e. children have no choice in participating in family<br />

tionships. Secard, for adults, marriags is essertially<br />

oluntary in that there is "no real alternatire...3 the<br />

ied state as a lif- career" (Sprey, 1969:702). Sprey<br />

€5 that individuals have little or no cho:ce as to<br />

her to enter family reletionships in general. Yet, +he<br />

mmertal dependencies ir family relatiorships ere<br />

-recognized (sse Scaczoni. 1970; 1972:62-66): hsving<br />

2 entersd a particular family relationship. I? is at best


difficult to 19ave it. Thus, family relationships ar<br />

relatively nonvaluctary or cosxive at this levsl.<br />

difficulty in leaving family relationships say b4 due<br />

to the affective insestmert in intimate groups discuss<br />

Coser, but it i5 also due '0 the instrumental inverfne<br />

aependencies <strong>of</strong> faaily members.<br />

That the high inaLrumenta1 investmsct tn fa<br />

suppresses hostility may acCually he cortradictory.<br />

zffsctire investment, high instrumental lnvesment i<br />

relationship may increase fear <strong>of</strong> the relationsh<br />

dissolving, and ir tom prodace attempts to supp<br />

hostility. Thus. wa stete a ptrallel to Propcsitivn 10:<br />

Propsa:loy 13. Th? hlgher Lhe ms:rumental<br />

lrvestnsn* IP a relatlonshzp, the hlgher +he<br />

pear if dlssolutlon <strong>of</strong> tbs relatlonship.<br />

S ~ C S Propos~tlon 11 furLher assarts tha* fear I<br />

dlssolutlon produces suppression <strong>of</strong> hostlllty, we can th,<br />

s!lggest that ths greater +he lnstrumental lnvestment in<br />

relatlonshlp, the greater the snpprasslon <strong>of</strong> bostilzty.<br />

on the other hand, a family member may be aware <strong>of</strong> tt<br />

irstruoental deperamce <strong>of</strong> the o_t_hsy on the relationshZp a,<br />

thus may recognize the law probability <strong>of</strong> the other'<br />

leaving. <strong>The</strong> person may then have less faar <strong>of</strong> tl<br />

relationship's terminating (perhaps rightly s ~ ) acd mc<br />

therefore be &== likely to suppress hostility. Thr6<br />

factors mitigate against this possibility. First<br />

instrumental investment may increase the fear <strong>of</strong> dissolutrr<br />

indirectly, by increasirg affective investmen'.<br />

Suggested i!! Scanzoni's exchange modol <strong>of</strong><br />

This j<br />

marria:<br />

(1970:16-25).<br />

instrumental<br />

Second, family members may not rezognize tt<br />

bhses ot thEir relationships. f hi<br />

nonrecognition may ever be the norm for aarite<br />

relationships in which cultural Emphasis on romantic lsva e<br />

the basls for the relationship may obscure the icstrumentz<br />

aspects. Third, evan when the person does recognize t E<br />

ipstrumental bases <strong>of</strong> the relationship, a pertnzr voul<br />

probably recognize his or her own dependencies befor? thoe<br />

<strong>of</strong> the other, if oaly because one is more sensitive to as<br />

iamillar with one's own predicament. Consider the positic<br />

<strong>of</strong> a young woman uirh several yourg children and feu skill<br />

relevant to employment outside the home. Eer or<br />

instrumental dependencies on the marital relationship see<br />

so obvio~s and extreme<br />

husband's dependency nay<br />

that she may not realize th3c he<br />

be equal to hers. PErceirFn<br />

herself as havicg the most to lose should th? marriage brea<br />

up, she w i l l fear its dissolution and thsrsfore<br />

suppress hostllity.rZ<br />

attempt t


Page 121


Thus, to the extent that both (or ~11) msmbers <strong>of</strong><br />

group have an instrumental investment, each has something t<br />

105~ if the group dissolves and so each may be fearful c<br />

expressing hostility. For the above rPasons<br />

Proposition 13, that ins:rumental icrastment incrsasPs fez<br />

<strong>of</strong> dzssolution, stands--at least for the PreSsPt. 01<br />

further commecf is in order. Propositions 10 an8 13 sugger<br />

that both atfsctive ard instruepntal inrrstments in<br />

relationship lead group members to feer tte dissolution c<br />

the relationship. Yet, objectively speaking, it is possibl<br />

tc argue that those very lrsostmects that lead grrup slsber<br />

to fear dissolution <strong>of</strong> the grGup are zldrsd tts strpn_gEQs c<br />

that relationship--8<br />

intimate groups.<br />

curious contradictior i n the life c<br />

Figure 1 summarizss ic diegrammatic form the mai<br />

outlines <strong>of</strong> the model presented thus far. Intimate groups<br />

characterizes by frequent interaction and total personalit<br />

involvesent, are paradoxical in structure, ir that the<br />

promote both subjective hcstility and attempts to suppres<br />

hostility. <strong>The</strong> instrumental investmeots which CharactEriz<br />

famly relationships also contriDute to the suppressioc o<br />

hostility.<br />

<strong>The</strong> processes 1% irtimate grcups considered thus fa<br />

place the actox in a state <strong>of</strong> a~bivalence. On th€ one hzna<br />

he or she is taced vlth a namber <strong>of</strong> conflicts <strong>of</strong> intsres<br />

and feels resulting hostility toward another. Dn the othe<br />

hand, ha fears thc relationship's dissolution and attempt<br />

to suppress<br />

paradoxlcsl<br />

hostility.<br />

structure<br />

<strong>The</strong> ir.timata group repres-nts<br />

likely to produce subjectiv<br />

ambivalence or tension.<br />

a+ . - this point it is appropriate to shift 3ur focus fro<br />

causal chains to the options or choices available scd th<br />

conditions ~nfluencing these cholces.<br />

options examined are avoidance<br />

<strong>The</strong> three bshiviora<br />

and sxpressivs an<br />

instrumental conflict. Coser does not as clearly prssen<br />

this stage in<br />

whzch a number<br />

the cocflict process as z dacision poln: a<br />

<strong>of</strong> options ere open to group membsrs<br />

Rather, his analysis lnplies an alnas: icevitabl'<br />

progressLoK toward expressive conflict in intimate groups,<br />

Pur'her, Ccser does rot explicitly include avoidacce as i<br />

pos~~ble choice. although he aces suggsst some possihll<br />

consequences <strong>of</strong> avoidance strategies.


8. Paradoxical Family Fslaticnships p3gc 123<br />

ms <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

EefOrE examinirg the condltions influencing the choice<br />

strategy, we need to ccnsider ths meaning <strong>of</strong> som= key<br />

~epts. Conflicts <strong>of</strong> inferss: usre dsficed earlier is an<br />

ective situa?ion ir which the participants hold<br />

tradictory values and claims, aid uc not-3 thii Coser<br />

*fully diStiCgUlShJS between such an ChjFctivf situation<br />

behavioral strafsgies l i k ~ c0r.f lict. ThLs distinction<br />

maintained here.<br />

-- Conflict: - Active oppositron betweao parti?s<br />

holding contradictory values and cllias over<br />

scarce status, power, and resources.<br />

This definition underscores the very real possibility<br />

t persons with objectively artagonis3c interests lo rot<br />

essarily engage in actegocistic behavior. It must hO<br />

ressed that the term conflict refers to the actual<br />

hapipr or group .embers. No matter how Eerions ths<br />

nflicts <strong>of</strong> icterest amorg group members appear to be.<br />

nflict itself has not occurred unless the participants<br />

tually engage in some torn <strong>of</strong> antagonistic bshavior.<br />

This definition ot conflict is equivalent to thst <strong>of</strong><br />

ser. as a "struggl~ over values and claims to scarce<br />

.us, power. and ~ESCU~CFS" (1956:8). H3wever. we<br />

5 inguish between two forms <strong>of</strong> conflict.<br />

Instrument21 conflict: Task-orianted csnflict.<br />

he distinction between ~xpressive and ins?rumen+al<br />

onflict is based cn Coser's modification during the course<br />

f his analysis ct key concepts and definitions. nost<br />

nportant is his discussion <strong>of</strong> "r~alilti~' and<br />

con-reallsiic" conflict:<br />

Conflicts which arise from frustretion <strong>of</strong><br />

specific demands within the relationship and<br />

from estimates <strong>of</strong> gairs <strong>of</strong> the participants, acd<br />

which aIE directFd at the presumed frustratirg<br />

object, can be called ze_aMs:ig confgcts<br />

inscfar as they are means toward a specific<br />

result. 4p-realistic g~~flZ:=--. are a3t<br />

occasioned by the rival ends <strong>of</strong> the ahtagorists,<br />

but by ?he need for tersion release <strong>of</strong> at Least<br />

one ot them (1956:49).<br />

C35er goes OE to state that "henceforth,<br />

'conflict' w i l l apply to realistic conflict only"<br />

ths term<br />

(1956:60).


&qzfs&c&: A spscrflc stra-egy with the intsrt<br />

or goal <strong>of</strong> lnjurlng the other.<br />

"Afc~: Speclfxally physrcal aggresslon.<br />

Consistent wl?h +he theories at Steinnetz an< Strar<br />

(1974:U) and Coser (1956:SO-51). aggressron can be<br />

sunstrategy <strong>of</strong> either instromental cr expressrvS ccnflict<br />

In oLher ~ords, aggression i+self can be charact-rized a<br />

l~strumental or expresslre.<br />

Dlstrrgulshlng Irstrumental and Expressrve Csnflict<br />

Instrumental and expressive conflict a;? pors ty,pe<br />

that, in reality, <strong>of</strong>len are mixed ic the same act (Coser<br />

1956:53-54). Still, criferia by which we should disticguis<br />

these two forms <strong>of</strong> conflict need examining. Coser is co<br />

altogether consisrent ox this matter. On the sne hand, h<br />

argues Chat exprsssive and instrumental conflict csn h<br />

dlstirguished ir , terms cf th-ir consegusPces: "wherea<br />

[instruaentzl] conflict necessarily changes the prsv'<br />

tsms <strong>of</strong> the relationship <strong>of</strong> the participants,<br />

hostility [nxpres5ive conflict] has no such neczss<br />

effects and may leave the terms <strong>of</strong> the ;Elation<br />

unchangedn (1956:QO).<br />

<strong>The</strong> flaw in this analysis is that instrunPntal corf<br />

is essentially *figs as thac kind <strong>of</strong> behavioral expzes<br />

at hostility that results ic change or is instrument


8. Peraaoxical Pamily Belatiorships Page 125<br />

ccessful. One difficulty is that whether a strategy was<br />

eed instrum4ntal Or expressive co~fll~: must be<br />

irically assessed 22~2 facts as tc whettsr il was<br />

cessful (for a discussion cf prohleas with PZ ~2%; fago<br />

~YSBS 12 a related ccnnection, see Skinner, 1953:31-35:<br />

erwood, 1957: 195-233). Second, the anelysis implies that<br />

ever an instruaental strategy is ussd, a 5uccEs~ful<br />

one is inevitable. This contradicts Caser's own<br />

pition that even wber ec icstrunsntal ccnfl:cL sta'egy<br />

sea, the specific m?zns adoptpa 3ra not nrcissarily<br />

uate for reaching the intended goal (1956:54). Third,<br />

approach implies that f-xgresgr_ve conflic: is h q ~ ~<br />

essful in fostericg the attainnant <strong>of</strong> irstrumental<br />

5. Pinally, confounding the potential consequences <strong>of</strong> a<br />

vioral strategy vith the very definftion <strong>of</strong> the strategy<br />

ludes empirically examicing relationships between<br />

ous strategies and outcomes.<br />

A second possibility for distinguishing between<br />

strumental and expressive conflict is to examine the<br />

ality <strong>of</strong> the acts performed. <strong>The</strong> major difficulty vith<br />

s possibility has already been suggsstea: that<br />

rticular strategi~s can <strong>of</strong>ten be used in both icstrum?ntal<br />

expressive Conflict. For example, aggression is prcbably<br />

t <strong>of</strong>ten indicative <strong>of</strong> purely expressive behavior, but can<br />

a ratianal chcice <strong>of</strong> behavior in pursuit <strong>of</strong> son2<br />

trumental goal. Ttus, the aggressive quality <strong>of</strong> so act<br />

oat sufficient evldenoe that the act necessarily falls<br />

o one or the other types <strong>of</strong> conflict.<br />

<strong>The</strong> third approach, "sad here, is to differestiate<br />

trumental and Expressive conflict according to the ictent<br />

goals <strong>of</strong> tke participants. This approach is cocsistenr<br />

th the portions <strong>of</strong> CosEr's analysis that do not base the<br />

stinction CL success or outcome. as in: n...conflicl is<br />

ewed by the participants as a means toward the achievement<br />

rea11stic ends, a means which night be ahsodonad if other<br />

ans appear to be more effective for raaching the ssea end"<br />

956:54). In contrast, expressive conflict has as its goal<br />

he mere expre~sior ot diffuse hostilities" (1956:51). Th?<br />

me epproach is also used here in defining aggression in<br />

rms <strong>of</strong> one party's goal <strong>of</strong> inflicting some injury on the<br />

her. This goal can ir +urn be a means toward achieving a<br />

re general instrumeotal or expressive goal.<br />

<strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> goals is itself a rather shaky basis for<br />

ese importart distinc+ions. Still. it is consistert with<br />

e <strong>of</strong> Coser's approaches and avoids the problems <strong>of</strong> his<br />

her, outcome-based. approach. Pinally, using,gosls in<br />

fining aggression is internally consisten: vlth our<br />

proach to instrumental ard expressive conflict, scd is<br />

mpatible vith current literatore on family violence.*3


discussion, and it sesas 1egi:iaate to include thi<br />

theoretical link in the conflict process. Thu<br />

state:<br />

prQgEsltQr x. If the tendency tow+<br />

bostilit~ does not outweigh the tendsn<br />

toward suppression <strong>of</strong> hostility, avoidarce i<br />

the most likely outcome.<br />

Pvoldarce say be deflned as:<br />

- avoidance: -- Any<br />

nonongagenent<br />

tactic oriented tovar<br />

or dis=ngagament, inCluK<br />

physical, emotional, and intsllEctu<br />

nonecgagement or withdrawal (adapted fro<br />

Rctaling in Chapter 9).<br />

Coser suggests that in intimate grnups 2 strat2<br />

avoidance il cor:flicts <strong>of</strong> interest is dEsticed i<br />

(1956:62). In rhs first placs, if no attsmpt is ma<br />

6911 with contradictory interests, the original co<br />

w i l l remain uzresclved. Et the same time, new cecflic<br />

interest wlll arise out <strong>of</strong> the structure c<br />

relationship. Thus.<br />

Pro~oslibo~ 5. When an avoidance stratsgy is<br />

used. Conflicts <strong>of</strong> Internst W l l l aCCUmU1aAF.<br />

Porther, slncF Proposition 3 suggested that conflict<br />

ct interest create hostility, us can stste that t1<br />

accumulation <strong>of</strong> conflicts <strong>of</strong> intsrest indicate8 !<br />

Proposition 15 w i l l cause further hastility. Combining ti<br />

two propositions, vs can state in summary:<br />

P,!os1t~o3 ". When ar avcldancs strategy 1s<br />

used, host=lr+y Increases.<br />

P s this strategy <strong>of</strong> avoxdcnce continues, snd hostllit<br />

rncraases, subjective hostrllty w r l l eventually be too grea<br />

+o be suppressed, and a mor9 actlve confllct strategy nil


~ p 3 ~ 12- : ~ Tte & grsater ~ ~ +hz h0st;:;ty. L.hs<br />

more llkely is bost~llty to outweigh<br />

suppression <strong>of</strong> hostllity.<br />

End, the corollary <strong>of</strong> Proposition 14, which stated that<br />

if hostility does not outweigh suppressior <strong>of</strong> kostil%ty *hen<br />

avoidance is the outcome, follows:<br />

Propositior 38. If hoctility outweighs tbs<br />

suppresslor. <strong>of</strong> hostility, conflict--eithsr<br />

expressive or instrumsntal--is the most<br />

likely outcome.<br />

<strong>The</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> Coser's analysis suggss~s that any kind <strong>of</strong><br />

avoidance in an intimate =elationship w i l l eventually fail<br />

in this manner. eovevsr. in contrast to Caser, some kirds<br />

<strong>of</strong> avoidance nay actually provide a fairly stabla resolution<br />

<strong>of</strong> conflxcts <strong>of</strong> interesr, under certain circumstancFs.<br />

Avoidance car. slnply mean avoiding recognition or discussion<br />

<strong>of</strong> the specific conflicting interests that has€ created<br />

hostility. 0: avoidance can involve m31f general strstegias<br />

<strong>of</strong> reducing the aaonnt <strong>of</strong> Lnteraction or personality<br />

involvement in the relationship.<br />

Resolvicg Conflicts <strong>of</strong> Ir+erest Through Avoidance<br />

Reducing the amount <strong>of</strong> perr0r;ality icvolvement and/or<br />

icteraction in a relationship may resolvz a specific<br />

cozflict <strong>of</strong> ixterest; perhaps more important, if we accep*<br />

PrOpOsition~ 1 and 2 that frequent inter3ction and total<br />

personality involvement sngender conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest, then<br />

reducing the level these two elements should lower <strong>The</strong><br />

possibility that new conflicts <strong>of</strong> interast w i l l develap.<br />

Even though the family terds to be 3 cssrcive<br />

institution in that it is relativ?ly difficult to "lesve the<br />

field," regular family patterns do<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> interpersonal withdrawal.<br />

provide for a cartain<br />

For example, one spouse<br />

may become imaersed in work or in child care, thereby<br />

1esseCing interaction a%d involvEment with :he other. Such<br />

accoanodatiors involving personal withdrawal are represented<br />

in Cuber and Hareff's (1965) typology <strong>of</strong> marriages. Ic ths<br />

"devitalized marriage" couples gradually drift away from<br />

th~ir inlfial clcseness, and ir the "psssi7e-congenial<br />

marrieae' - -~ .~ the couols views their marriase as 3 "cscveriert<br />

~ -.and<br />

comfortable way to livs while directLng ens's true<br />

interests and creative energies elsewhere" (Skolnick,<br />

1973:239). In gsneral, marriages that hars an openly<br />

recognized instrumsntal rather than expressivE emphasis show<br />

less intense patterns <strong>of</strong> involvement.


Ch.8. Paradoxical Family Relatiorships<br />

Whether such marriages always result from an a<br />

Strategy in conflicts <strong>of</strong> int=rest is noc :he point. B<br />

we simply wish to note that family patrerns comaonl<br />

that allow for reducing the intensity <strong>of</strong> ictera<br />

therefore lowering conflicts <strong>of</strong> irterert and ho<br />

Still, C-rtain conditiors need to 51 net if in a<br />

strategy is to actually reduce, or at lsast not exa<br />

conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest ard hostility. First:<br />

<strong>of</strong> irterest. "4<br />

This proposition is relevant both to the<br />

strategies <strong>of</strong> withdrawal just discussed and to<br />

Specific avoidance <strong>of</strong> particular issues that d<br />

involve general in+srpersonal withdrawal. When con<br />

legitlmata, this specific kind <strong>of</strong> avoidance may ta<br />

form <strong>of</strong> "agreeing to disagre€.v As Sprey pcint<br />

"families may live together in motual rsspect in the<br />

great dlffErenCes in beliefs or values' (1969:704).<br />

-- Propositior 20. I£ an avoidance strategy is<br />

considered 1eg:timate and if it actually<br />

reduces involvement in areas where conflicts<br />

<strong>of</strong> interest tend to develop, then the number<br />

<strong>of</strong> conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest w i l l decrease.<br />

<strong>The</strong> basic argument is that although families in ge<br />

may be intimate Ervironaerts, some faoilies are<br />

intinate than others. moreover, in sons patterns <strong>of</strong> f<br />

life legitimacy is corferrea on less intimate or in<br />

involvement, such 25 the American marriages idsntifie<br />

Cuber and Ear<strong>of</strong>i, and many <strong>of</strong> the English vorkia<br />

middle-class couples Bott stmaied (1957).*5 TJ the e<br />

that an intlmzte relationship becomes lagitimatelg<br />

iCtimate, then the pressures toward hostility inheren<br />

intimate relatioEships are lessened.


8. Paradoxical Family Relationships P'iqe 129<br />

BOYeVer. ve reeaphasized thit, as ?zop=sirl3ns 14<br />

15 suggest, an avoidance strategy in intima'? faaily<br />

ationships in general vill not resolve existing corflicts<br />

iCtereSt, and conflicts <strong>of</strong> intczrs? and hostility w i l l<br />

mulate. In addition, avoidance s?rateg;ss, Even under<br />

ditione <strong>of</strong> legitimacy and specifycity, may have only<br />

mited usefulness in resolving conflicts cf inter=+ a-a<br />

y exacerbate problems over the long ruz. In th* firs?<br />

ce, avoidance uspd repeatedly and indiscriminetely may<br />

d to the "corrosion" <strong>of</strong> relalionships described by Blood<br />

wolfe (1960:87-88). Second. avoidancE used frequently<br />

become the characteris+ic style for asalicg with ggy<br />

flict <strong>of</strong> interest. Thus, when inpartant issuas con? op<br />

which one or more parties consider aroidancs an<br />

egitieate straLegy, cortinued avoidance w i l l lead to tho<br />

era1 +ccumulation <strong>of</strong> hostility described above. Perhaps<br />

.mportmt, the lack <strong>of</strong> previous practice in working out<br />

r issues w i l l make attempts to dsal directly with the<br />

-.ect ones more difficult.<br />

ACTIVE ANTAGOAISE: EXPBESSIVF ANTAGONISE IND COBPLIC?<br />

TO the exten? that persorality involvement in a<br />

ationship is total, expressive antagonism is likely to<br />

ur, partly because greater knowledge <strong>of</strong> the other makes<br />

rg personal attack possible. Bs Eotaling poiatsd out.<br />

..intimates know how to support the idertities <strong>of</strong> each<br />

ther becanse each knovs about the things thet matter cr are<br />

mportart to the other. While this extsnsive klovled~? can<br />

E used to Support and enhance identities. st the 59Be t i m 8<br />

t can be used to damage the identity. ..." (see Chapter 9).<br />

11~0, where ties are "diffuse and affec?.ivem IC3ser.<br />

use a common example, one cannot easily distinguish between<br />

an action being "a mear thing to do" and the actor beirg "a<br />

mean person,. vher there is total personality icvol~ement.<br />

In another sense, +he expressive and icstrueental asp~cts <strong>of</strong>


Ch.8. Paradoxical Family Fllatio?ships<br />

the relationship ars so irtortvired that they are<br />

to SEparate. Thus. for example, raising the .<br />

reassiqoing ins?rUaental responsibilities may be int<br />

by rhe Other as an accusation <strong>of</strong> lazin~ss- 9ased<br />

considerations:<br />

proeositi<strong>of</strong> 21. 'be greater the persorals<br />

involvement, the more ltkely is cc~flict<br />

takl the form <strong>of</strong> expressive conflict.<br />

SECODB, if much hostility has accumulated. conf<br />

nore llXely to be expressive. Proposition 16<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> avoidance suggests that hostility 1<br />

accumulate in istimate groups. Increased hostili'<br />

great personality Lnvalvement, may make it diffic<br />

examine issues oh their merits. Thus,<br />

--- ProEc&r&on 22. <strong>The</strong> greater the accusulation<br />

hostility. the more. likely is cmflict f<br />

take the expressive form.<br />

Effects or Express~ve a?d Iostrusmtal Confllct<br />

Certain effects <strong>of</strong> an avoidance outcome h3ve a<br />

been considered. To sum.marize, unless an avoidance s<br />

is used in actual areas <strong>of</strong> conflicting interests<br />

viexed as legitimate. it vill fail to resolve the<br />

Conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest: this failure <strong>of</strong> rssolution. con<br />

with the additior <strong>of</strong> the avoidance itself as an is<br />

with the ihevitable occurrence <strong>of</strong> rew issues, vill ca<br />

aCcumUlatiOr: 05 conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest acd an incr<br />

hostility. <strong>The</strong> implications for succsss <strong>of</strong> expressi<br />

issibumental conflict need similar consideration.<br />

conflict. Unfortunately, as &ser argues, the u<br />

exprDSsire COnflicL 1PSsecs 'he likelihood thst the or'<br />

Conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest w i l l he resolved. First,<br />

suggests that exprassivs co<strong>of</strong>lict terds to increase<br />

in'ensity <strong>of</strong> the encounter. although Coser does<br />

FXpliCitly dffins "intensitp." it seems roughly equivale<br />

to aggression and/or vtolesce (l956:69). BggresrFcn he<br />

been defined hera as 'a specific strategy with the intent o<br />

goal <strong>of</strong> injuricg the other," and violeoce is a sabtyps<br />

aggressior. In line with coser's discussion <strong>of</strong> express<br />

Conflict and intensity, it is suggesied that:<br />

PEPEOS~L-IOL 23. Expressive Confllct tends ts<br />

lncreasP aggress~on.*6


aradoxical Pamlly Relationships Page 131<br />

er further s~ggssts ?I!+: vhCn aggression is ssed,<br />

ellhood decreases tbat cocflicts <strong>of</strong> icterest will he<br />

fully resolved.<br />

er interfere<br />

Ths<br />

with<br />

"zggress5Ve overtonas" 3?<br />

the participaots' ability<br />

an<br />

to<br />

er the original cl+ias <strong>of</strong> conflicting interest<br />

-- -<br />

ProEsitior: 2. Aggrssslon tends to rsduc? ths<br />

likelihood <strong>of</strong> SUCCE~S~U~ resolution <strong>of</strong><br />

c~r~flicts <strong>of</strong> intsrest.<br />

y combining Prcpositiocs 23 and 24, it c3n be further<br />

that expressive conflict tends to reduce the<br />

ood that conflicts w i l l be resolve6 successfully.<br />

converse shculd also be noted, that instrumental<br />

~ct tends to increase<br />

lict lesolution.<br />

the likelihood <strong>of</strong> successful<br />

--<br />

Leqitimacn =qa s&&sz Besau+apg. Proposition 19<br />

rts Chat if a strategy is considered illegitimais. the<br />

ategy w l l l becoms e nev issue, thereby iocreasing<br />

fllcts <strong>of</strong> isterest and hostility. This propcsitisr was<br />

anced in refereccs to avoidance, but it seams =qually<br />

licable '0 iCStr~menta1 and expr2ssive conflict<br />

ategies. Exp~e~sive conflict seams more likely to be<br />

idered illegitimate than instrumectel cocflict, and thus<br />

likely to increase conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest. Bowever. as<br />

us polnts OUT (1974a:442), the participants Ray view as<br />

tlmate expressive co~flict that milltatss against<br />

easing Conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest, or may see instrumental<br />

fllct as illfg5tima+E, in which case<br />

.flict wonla increaseccnflicts <strong>of</strong> intsrest.<br />

instromental<br />

Yet even if expressive conflict is considsred<br />

itimate and doer not add to future cosflicF; <strong>of</strong> ictzrest,<br />

may still be relatively ineffective in resolvirg Present<br />

conflicts ot in?eres?. 85 Propositions 23 Bad 29 suggest.<br />

expTessive cozflicx introduces elements into an encountsr<br />

that interfere vith racing the initial conflicts <strong>of</strong><br />

interest. Put another way, expressive conflict interferes<br />

wlth tte possibility at task-oriented conflict Occurring.<br />

Under most circumstances, instrumental conflict is mast<br />

likely to resolve contlicts <strong>of</strong> interest, rhrle expressive<br />

cOnf11ct and avoidance are generally less ~ffoctive.<br />

EOV~VET. any <strong>of</strong> the three strategies could be effsctive<br />

under certaln conditions. <strong>The</strong> fical prop3siCiun asserts<br />

that regardless cf the strategy ussd, if conflict is<br />

resolved successfully, then ths strategy employed wiXl he<br />

reinforced.<br />

gpr3s&pTcg 25. Successful rssolution <strong>of</strong><br />

initial conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest reinforces ths<br />

strategy used.


Ch.8. Paradoxical Family Relationships


ch.8. Paradoxical Family Relationships ?age 133<br />

Pignre 2 sunmarizes in diagrammatic form the overall<br />

model.*7 In*-inate groups, like the family. charsctarized by<br />

frequent interaction and total personality involvemect are<br />

paradoxical in structure in that they pronote bozh hostility<br />

and the snppr€ssion <strong>of</strong> hostility. If the tendency toward<br />

hostility does PO? cu+w~igh th2 tender.cy to suppress<br />

hostility. than the most probrtle outcome is avoidaccr. In<br />

general, avoidance is cot an effec'ive means <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

resolution and is likely te increase hostility. If<br />

hostility c~tneighs the tendeecy to suppress hostility, the<br />

outcone w i l l be one <strong>of</strong> two forms <strong>of</strong> conflict. Of these,<br />

expressive conflict is more likely than instrumsntal<br />

conflict. However, expressive conflict is more lik?ly to<br />

produce a99ression and as a result is less likely to i2831ve<br />

lnitial conflicts <strong>of</strong> interest thar is ins~U*:X <strong>of</strong><br />

r"12?i3r50:~5 all sro :rca:rrc?blp llnkid ra *h? s:r=.'%gics<br />

cczs:der%d here ~ 2 ccr:r:bu:e a<br />

+o a tulle: u~3?rs?3r3:~.7 <strong>of</strong><br />

conflict and violence in the family.<br />

Relevant to the prescnt discussion is the <strong>of</strong>ter cited<br />

basic tecet and contribution <strong>of</strong> conflict theory: that<br />

conflict is an integral. inevitable part <strong>of</strong> all social life.<br />

Examination <strong>of</strong> this essertion requires rsturning ts the<br />

distinction between conflicts <strong>of</strong> interDst and conflict. 1%<br />

seems qoiee reasocable to assert that c_2~f&igtz af ir_+pzgst.,<br />

ir the sense <strong>of</strong> occasior2s gereratirg contradictory claims.<br />

are irevitable- Yet cpzf&zc_i, especially task-crisn'.ed or<br />

instrumental conflicl, is certainly not inevitablr. In<br />

fact, the thrust cf this chapter is that in intinate groups<br />

like the family, conflict in this specific senss is very<br />

difficnlt to carry out and is thus highly likely to bs<br />

absent.


Ch.8. Paradoxical Family Eelationships Pag<br />

Plnally, we trust the attention to terminolsgy<br />

helped keep conflzct ard violerce concep5unlly dis<br />

Populer conceptions <strong>of</strong>ten sesm to eqnata<br />

canflzct and vlole~cc, cz ?c v:ew ccnfllct as prcducLnq<br />

violerce. In contrast to soch a view, ws argue th<br />

is the absence <strong>of</strong> specifically instromertal confli<br />

families that leads to a high level <strong>of</strong> interpcr<br />

violence and aggression.<br />

NOTES<br />

*I would like to espscially thank Dennis Poss<br />

GSrald Botaling for their cornmeit on earlier drafts <strong>of</strong><br />

chapter.<br />

1. Personality icoolvenent and affective investment<br />

b~ related (see Propositions 8 and 9). but 5h.y<br />

coxeptoally distinct. <strong>The</strong> important feature <strong>of</strong> persona<br />

involvement is its totelity. Affective investment iaoo<br />

both the strength <strong>of</strong> at:raction or liking and<br />

replaceability <strong>of</strong> menbers.<br />

2. This is not an entirely satisfactory resclutio<br />

the coctradiction, particularly since it ignores un<br />

exchange. Yet to rescloe this question fully vould req<br />

ar intricate series <strong>of</strong> propositions about cxch<br />

pro~~sses, which would take us far afield and would bs<br />

beyond the scope ard focus <strong>of</strong> this chapter.<br />

3. Rotaling defines aggression as "a physical<br />

psychclogical act or acts, occurring in an interper<br />

situation, which are judged to be intentio<br />

harm-producirg" (Chapter 9). Since he focus5s<br />

---- attrjbutio~ cf aggressive intFrt in families, the emp<br />

In dstiaitior is on tha perception by gse~s <strong>of</strong> the per5<br />

intest. While the focus here is not on percaptiors<br />

others, re suggest thct these approaches to d~fin<br />

aggression are similar in emphasizing inter~t or qoals <strong>of</strong><br />

actors. <strong>The</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> using intent or goals a<br />

definitional basis is alsc recognized in the suggestion<br />

"...to the extert that actors define such behavior<br />

noreal ana not inrended to irjure. they are cot aggrss<br />

acts. ..* (straus, 1974.3: 942).<br />

4. This proposition is stated in general tarns, ni<br />

it applies not only to avoidance, but also to expressive<br />

instrUmEnta1 conflict, which w i l l be discussed later.


Chapter 9<br />

Attribution Processes<br />

in <strong>Husband</strong>-<strong>Wife</strong> <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Gerald T. Hotaling<br />

.....................<br />

B c?nt~al tenet <strong>of</strong> this chapter is<br />

"intent" is imputed rather than obse<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, an action is intentionally agqrs<br />

Or violent only when the imputation is<br />

Rttribution theory alerts ns to the imp3<br />

Of undsrstandinq why. and under what condi<br />

on? perceives an action as aggressive ra<br />

thaP as onintentioral or accidental.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapter attempts to shov that cert<br />

Structural cbaracteristics <strong>of</strong> the family prod<br />

conditions that ircrease the probability that<br />

family membsr w i l l attribute malevolent inte<br />

to the acts <strong>of</strong> another faaily member. <strong>The</strong> act<br />

we refer to are those that violate family rule<br />

Hotaling pciltr to what he feels is a cectr<br />

irony in fanily life: ths factors th<br />

Contribute to the warmth and intimacy <strong>of</strong> famil<br />

reletionship5. such as the sharing <strong>of</strong> secret<br />

and personal information. also flcilitat<br />

perceiving rule violations as int?ntion+ll<br />

malevolent.<br />

A fir!al section <strong>of</strong> the chapter examines the<br />

Yay in which attribution <strong>of</strong> aggression or<br />

violence serves to stabilize violence as a<br />

regular feature or family life.<br />

........................<br />

Violent behavior results from a process that<br />

participants construct within a situation. Struct<br />

theories <strong>of</strong> violence have tended to nPglec?<br />

interpersonal developnent <strong>of</strong> violent behavior acd f<br />

136


<strong>The</strong> rrezet0rne:lor cf ":rrr:al' '2-7 "s=rr?urqS nlr-ors<br />

:":mate r?l~+.io:sC1p~. 1:ke tt.e farn:ly, is ssen is tei-q<br />

I<br />

tac=l:tat=d by certai? 0rcar.lzaCicral or ~truc:uzal l=n'urcs<br />

:ne aar::al Scrd.<br />

I<br />

logical startlng point in this kind <strong>of</strong> social<br />

ogical analysis is the examination <strong>of</strong> the contsntior<br />

he "meaning' <strong>of</strong> aggression*l and viclance is<br />

atic. <strong>The</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> an act as intentional<br />

ion or as accidental herm-doins is an tapartant<br />

,=tion in analyzing violence (e-g., Tedeschi, $2 +i.,<br />

In many instances, the perception <strong>of</strong> aggressive actions<br />

onproblematic. Cultural values and beliefs greatly<br />

t the identification <strong>of</strong> aggressive acts.<br />

ictensity, <strong>of</strong>fensive, and pain-producing actions are<br />

ly taken for granted as signs <strong>of</strong> +ggrEssion or<br />

ace. But haaan aggression need not take the form <strong>of</strong><br />

t physical damage; aggression can be nanifsst in<br />

al, indirect, passive, and subtle forms <strong>of</strong> psychological<br />

. It is Especially when harm-doing actions take these<br />

s that judgmental controversies are likely to arise.<br />

human aggression is <strong>of</strong> this type and there are good<br />

ons why this is so. A s Bandura (1973:Y) states:<br />

People ordrnarily refrain from direct persons1<br />

assaults because such obvious actions carry hiqh<br />

ris1s <strong>of</strong> retaliation. Rather, they favor aisguis~a<br />

modes <strong>of</strong> agqression that, being difficult to<br />

interpret or to consider blameworthy, afford<br />

prot~ction agai~st counterattack.<br />

=-.-.---- r.-'-~ --- -~ .----<br />

~~ ~ ~<br />

:ive and nocaggressive actions. To this end,<br />

-wife violence w i l l be examined within the co~tsxt <strong>of</strong><br />

tior theory.


Ch.9. lttribution Processes<br />

LTTXIBUTION THEOBY<br />

In i*s simplest fore, attribution theory describe<br />

process by which people attempt to explain and prpdict<br />

behavior. In other words, it inusstigates people's<br />

for the nearing or behavior.<br />

LS Kelley and Thibauc (1969) have defined it:<br />

Attribution refers to the process <strong>of</strong> inferr<br />

or perceiving the dispositiocal properties 3<br />

ePtitieS....Bttributicn theory describes th<br />

PrOCESS by which the indiviaual seeks an<br />

attairs CcncepLiors <strong>of</strong> the stable disposition<br />

or attributes (p.7).<br />

While there are at least three distirct attributio<br />

approaches (see Aeider. 1958: Jones and Davis. 1965: an<br />

8. H. Kellep, 1967, 1971). all three perspectives hav<br />

common elements. shaver (1975) has identified some <strong>of</strong> th<br />

commonalities <strong>of</strong> the above theorists in terms <strong>of</strong> thre<br />

stages in the attribution process. <strong>The</strong> first stage involve<br />

the observation <strong>of</strong> an actlon, either directly or indirsotl<br />

through the reports <strong>of</strong> others. <strong>The</strong> secona stsge. and th,<br />

one that is tmportazt to the theme <strong>of</strong> this chapter, involve;<br />

the attribution <strong>of</strong> inrention. Ona attampts to interprpt th,<br />

actions Of others as in+entiocal ard goal-directsd or as +b,<br />

result <strong>of</strong> accident. reflex, or habit. <strong>The</strong> final stag<br />

concerns the imputatic1 a "ceusem and searches for thl<br />

ansver to "why the person acted as he/she did." <strong>The</strong> 3nswe:<br />

USna1ly takes one <strong>of</strong> two forms: the action is attributed t i<br />

<strong>Causes</strong> in the environment or the situation ("Re's under i<br />

lot <strong>of</strong> pressure latelyn) Or to ths undef1y:ng aisposltion o:<br />

the person ("He's just in aggreSSIve person"). This<br />

sequsnce <strong>of</strong> events nay require only seconds to complete 01<br />

In some cases, such as jury deliberations, several days o!<br />

weeks-<br />

<strong>The</strong> concern with attributional processes, especiall)<br />

the attribution <strong>of</strong> intention. has bDen evidr-nt in th? work<br />

or psychologists in work an aggression and violence. 0ve1<br />

the years, researchers have argued ahout how they wouli<br />

decide when to label a response or set <strong>of</strong> responses a:<br />

aggressive or violent (Bass, 1971; Eandura, 1973: ant<br />

Tedeschi, a., 1974). <strong>The</strong> emphasis is these vritings i:<br />

on the expsrimenter-subject relationship. 1.. what<br />

criteria ar ExperimEnter would use to chara-terize subject<br />

responses as aggressive. Unfortunately. the question <strong>of</strong> hor<br />

a person imputes intentional aggressiveness in a variety <strong>of</strong><br />

relational contexts has rot been fully investigated.<br />

In this chapt~r, the attribution <strong>of</strong> aggression ard its<br />

role in the production cf family violscce w i l l be examined<br />

via the rule system and structure <strong>of</strong> tho husbaca-wife


tribution Proce~ses Page 139<br />

ship. A najQr question w i l l be: are there<br />

tional rfaturss pecalier to the hushand-wife<br />

ship tha* facilitate t h atfributicn <strong>of</strong> melev?lent<br />

THE NATUEF OF BELATIOIRL RULES<br />

groups have rules and regulations,<br />

- -<br />

v~riation in the extect to which -<br />

~,h.r,nr 1s con'rolled hr rules. In ~ ~~ * --- orher - words. . orou~s ,~ .<br />

'.heir rules: (1) their substentise<br />

behavior they control: and (2)<br />

ange. that is. where a3d when thsy<br />

axamole. workers in a steel plant<br />

. .<br />

err- mles for wte? to show UD f< )r wsrk. when to 50 home.<br />

uFh work to get docs, and hbv to get. the work done:<br />

arly, in graduate school dapartments, rules (at times<br />

cit) concern the appropriate amount <strong>of</strong> work to b9 done<br />

n given periods. <strong>The</strong>se sorts <strong>of</strong> rules are <strong>of</strong> limited<br />

active and spatiotemporal range, in that they are<br />

rced primarily with inettumental behavior an3 are<br />

cable ocly durirg specified times and in specified<br />

3s. <strong>The</strong>ir scope is not all-embracing.<br />

In the husband-wife relationship, rules are <strong>of</strong> broad<br />

stantive scope. Rere, rules ere designed to control both<br />

trumental and expressive behavior. Furthermore, they<br />

r a vids spatiotemporal range: they are applicable<br />

ss Situations and in and out cf one another's presence.<br />

ions differ as to why ralational rules are <strong>of</strong> such broad<br />

pa, but various authors agree that these rules attempt to<br />

trol a large amount <strong>of</strong> behavior.<br />

Both D. Cooper (1971) and R. D. Laing (1972) have<br />

ressea th* nature <strong>of</strong> relational rules in families and<br />

.ir import for individual members. <strong>The</strong>se a*~thors<br />

phasize the political nature <strong>of</strong> family rules: holding<br />

t the family as an extension <strong>of</strong> capitalist ssciety<br />

ates rules through which "dehumanization" <strong>of</strong> the<br />

ividual tekes place. Prom their perspective, faxily<br />

es are all-inclusive and restrict individual growth and<br />

velopnent. Through rules. family menbars ere forced into<br />

les in which a major portior <strong>of</strong> reality is kept beyond<br />

evezyday experience. while their ideslogical oriectation<br />

map differ, these authors both highlight the fact that the<br />

family attempts to coxtrol a vids rangP <strong>of</strong> behavior.<br />

From a somewhat different perspective, Denzin (1970)<br />

2nd Turnar (1968) outline a set <strong>of</strong> rules, applicable to the<br />

huaband-wife relaticnship, vhoss function it is to protect<br />

and echance icdividual identi?ies.*Z Denzin argues that<br />

"zules <strong>of</strong> rslatioDShiD" are generated in intimate relations


<strong>The</strong>se rules are: (1) task mlrs, -0 specify who dc?? vhak<br />

when, and with whoa; (2) rules specrfying daference an<br />

demeanor; (3) rules for regulating knowledge, secrecy, a<br />

personal problems; and (4) rules specifying proper csndn<br />

<strong>of</strong> ego and other vhen not in each other's presenc<br />

Similarly. Turner (1968) suggests two rules that vill<br />

developed between ictimates to protect the identities <strong>of</strong> o<br />

arother. <strong>The</strong>y are: (1) the avoidance <strong>of</strong> one another<br />

sensitive zones; and (2) the declaration by the winner ?ha<br />

his/her victory is attributable to luck or exparierce rathe<br />

than the ineptness <strong>of</strong> the vanquished.<br />

Takicg Denzin and Turcer together, we could say that<br />

relational rules are agreamerts between intimates that each<br />

vill avoid behavior that threatens the situated id€ntities*3<br />

<strong>of</strong> each. <strong>The</strong>se rales are necessary because each has<br />

revealed to the other intimate knowledge. sscrets, desires<br />

and persocal problems. important to identity, that are not<br />

available to the public at large.<br />

TO summarize the discussion thus far we can cocceivs <strong>of</strong><br />

relational ruler as being somewhat ironic. For people to<br />

reman in physical and emotional proximity, rules must be<br />

developed that protect and enhance situated identities, and<br />

that at the same time restrict and control behavior.<br />

TEE NRTORE OF RELATIONAL ROLE VIOL1TIONS<br />

ncst relational rules betveen intimates are gna$fig&,<br />

-- ing&&$t, and pbiquous. In addition, they are constantly<br />

bsing ncdified according to different situatFcna1 demands.<br />

For example, one spouse may begin to interpret rules<br />

differently withcut inforaicg the othsr <strong>of</strong> the changed<br />

perspective. In such a situaticn, violations <strong>of</strong> rules are<br />

easier since the boundaries between rule conformity end<br />

violation become quite precarious. For example. a husband<br />

tzlkinu to another woman at a Dartv ma7 be COEsidared a rule<br />

> .<br />

violator by his wife accoriing to his distance from the<br />

woman, the exprnssicr on his face, and the contert <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

COn~PrsatiCn. This say be so even though the husbacd never<br />

Intended a violation <strong>of</strong> relational rules. <strong>The</strong> point is that<br />

the ambiguous, undefined and implicit nature <strong>of</strong> relational<br />

rules raises the probability that rule violatiors w i l l<br />

occur.<br />

Since rule violations may be either intentional or<br />

accidental, the nearing <strong>of</strong>. the violation that t h other ~<br />

attribotes to the violating actor is significent (Scott and<br />

Lyman, 1970). <strong>The</strong> violation <strong>of</strong> relational rules leads<br />

intimates tc feel embarrassment, irritation, annoyance, and,<br />

at the extreme, self-threat and publicly designated dsviance<br />

(Denzin. 1970).


T ~ E mannfr in which a violaticn 1s perceive3 hss import<br />

for the conseguences. A perscn w i l l give the rerporse<br />

appropropriate to a viclation, according tc his/her<br />

percepti<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong> the violation as "accidental" harm-doing or as<br />

aggression. Most interaction between family membsrs is<br />

harmOniouS and cooperattve. Indeed, most rule viol3tions<br />

are sf seen as aggressive. but rather as accidents, as<br />

responses tc situational demands, or as respsnses to<br />

extraneous influences, such as alcohol or drugs. Actually.<br />

relative to the rate <strong>of</strong> relational rule violitions pfrceivea<br />

as aggressive, most rule vio1atior.s are denied or dismissed.<br />

If this were not so. ntima'e relationships would be very<br />

difficult to maintain.<br />

THE RELATION BETWEEN TEE BTTPIBUTION OF RULE VIOLATIONS AS<br />

AGGRESSIVE BND INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE<br />

Bs mer.tLor.Ed previously, the attribution <strong>of</strong> rule<br />

viola?ions as intertionally aggressive is an important step<br />

in the occurrence <strong>of</strong> interpersonal violence. R s Turner<br />

(1968) puts it:<br />

when ego perceives that alter is attempting fo<br />

cegatively affect his interests.. .the chazact=r<br />

<strong>of</strong> the interaction is pervasively altered by<br />

heightened preoccupation with sslf-images.<br />

incressed use <strong>of</strong> ~mphatic and diagnostic<br />

interpretatlocs ot gestures. ..and constant<br />

efforts to assign credit and responsibility<br />

(p. 106).<br />

Attempts to locate the "intent' <strong>of</strong> the rule violation<br />

w i l l determine the subsequent action taken to reduce the<br />

harm-doing <strong>of</strong> the rule violation. If malevolent intact is<br />

attributed to the rule violator, the pr3bzbility <strong>of</strong> vi3lence<br />

is increased. lany experimental studies have found that the<br />

perception <strong>of</strong> oppcnent's intent is a more important variable<br />

in the instigatior <strong>of</strong> aggression than actual physical<br />

attack. If a given act is seen as aggressive, it<br />

dramatically escalates the probability <strong>of</strong> counteraggression<br />

(Bandura, 1973: Epstein and Taylor. 1967: Easelli and<br />

altrochi, 1969).


RESPONSE TO RULE VIFLFTIONS 9?TQIBUTFD YO 9GSRISSI3R<br />

<strong>The</strong> attribution <strong>of</strong> aggression alons is not sufficient<br />

tor the irltiaticn ot interpersocal violsnce. PJllowicg ths<br />

imputaaon, the ettributor w i l l evaluate the situatioc and<br />

the possible alt.err+tive responses he/she can make. 8spburn<br />

(1973) has suggfsted three possible tactics the atleibutor<br />

can choose to reduce the harm-dolng caused by th? rule<br />

violation (see Elcuchart) .<br />

&pp&ggd!c=. ore tactic that can be used to deal uLth<br />

violatiors is avoidalce. <strong>The</strong> tactic <strong>of</strong> avoidance<br />

~8cessitat~s e physLcal withdrawal from the location or an<br />

eieotlonal disengagement trcm t h encounter. ~<br />

<strong>The</strong> Lactic <strong>of</strong><br />

avoFdance 1s difficult in rntimate relaxionships vh-rs there<br />

are extorral carstraints on the participarts tc rsmsin in<br />

the sltuatior (SSP Foss, Chapter 8).<br />

Rcc??g$lQ~~. Another tactic is acceptance <strong>of</strong> the rule<br />

vio1a:ioc. Acceptance <strong>of</strong> rule vioktion is also difficult<br />

ZP ln+imate relationstlps, where 'ha rnles serve to protecr<br />

the laentitles ot the participants. Acc~ptir.g tb.e rule<br />

violation 2s appropxiat~ behavior r?c?ssarily alters the<br />

e ~ p ~ c t ~ t leach o n ~ has toward the other. DisruptLng these<br />

stable and orderly irteractior patterns makss vulnerable<br />

OPE'S identity.<br />

If either accept?.nce or avoidanc~ is sslsctsd is a<br />

tactic by which to respond to aggressive rule viols'ions,<br />

the likelihood that the encounter vill end in violfrcs is<br />

grcitly dialnlshed.<br />

It is the tactic <strong>of</strong> retaliatior that is<br />

l~ad tc interpezsonal violenca. Whnr ago<br />

cf retaliation helshe risks retaliation by<br />

other. Initially, ego vill label the rul? violation as<br />

aggressi're, that is, tracsmit tc the violator tha' hP/she<br />

attributes inrsc+ianal harm-doing to him/her. Once the rule<br />

violation is labeled, ego can legitimatsly seek re+ribution<br />

f r c ~ the rule violator. If the rule violator cannot<br />

aaequately negotiate the aggressive label, he/sh? may<br />

counterre'aliate rc an att=spt to rid hia/h?rselt <strong>of</strong> chs<br />

aggressive<br />

contertion<br />

labnl. This process accords with SpEey's<br />

(1971) +hat successful management <strong>of</strong> conflict i~<br />

tamlies rEqUirfS the ability to negotia+e, bargain, acd<br />

cooperate--to manipulate a range <strong>of</strong> bahaviaral skills. If<br />

the process <strong>of</strong> r~talia~io~-~~~nfe~re?aIiatio~<br />

continuas, the<br />

p~obabllity <strong>of</strong> Y F O ~ E is ~ C increased. ~<br />

TC snmmarlze the<br />

tom:<br />

precsdlng sectim i n propositional<br />

P r o ~ o s i t ~ ~ ~<br />

2. if 2 rule violation is<br />

ZtTrlhuted as agqressivs, thet is, as


Ch.9. Rttributiox PTOC~ESES Page 144<br />

intentional harm-dorng, rhe probability <strong>of</strong><br />

interpersonal vialerce is ircreased.<br />

If 'he attribution <strong>of</strong> aggrnssiveness is eade, ar<br />

evaluatiOP <strong>of</strong> the situation w i l l determine an appropriate<br />

resporse by which to reduce the harm-doing <strong>of</strong> the ruls<br />

violation. It is posited that:<br />

--- Propositioii 2. <strong>The</strong> mare intinete 'hi<br />

relationship, 'he less liksly w i l l ecceptacce<br />

Dr aVOld2nCE be used as a tactic t? reducs<br />

the harm-doing cf rule violations.<br />

Since acceptance or zvoldance is difficuit for thosc<br />

iPvolPed in enaurlcg intZaate relationships:<br />

-- .b?<br />

ProposP~or 2. Th9 Bore intimate<br />

relatiorship, ?he more likely w i l l<br />

retaliation be used as a tactic to ?educe th?<br />

harm-doirg <strong>of</strong> rule viola?ions.<br />

If the ractic <strong>of</strong> reLaliation is ch?sen, chances<br />

increase that violence vlll ensue; thp norm <strong>of</strong> reciprocity<br />

will legitimize r~tallaticn.<br />

-- Prlp.oslti0n u. If the attribution <strong>of</strong> aggression<br />

1s communicated to the rule violator, thr<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> courterretallaticn iccreases.<br />

--- Propositioz 5. <strong>The</strong> lover the negoiiatiol<br />

abilities <strong>of</strong> the actors involved, the higher<br />

the prcbabiliry 3f comterretallation.<br />

-- Proposition 6. <strong>The</strong> mOZE the ZDt3112tiOPcounterretaliation,<br />

:ha hTgher ih8<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> violfrxe.<br />

CE2BRCTEPISTICS OF BOLES END ROLF VIDLBTIONS<br />

TB<strong>IT</strong> ENERBCE AGGPESSIVE ATTPISOTIOA<br />

Whether a rule violation is aggressive is typically<br />

icferred from, amocg other things, charactsristics sf the<br />

rule vlolator (e.g., power 3rd status), charactsristics 3f<br />

the situation (prEssrce cf alcohol or csarcioc), and<br />

cheracteristics or tho ruls violation itsclf. This lz'rsr<br />

category is <strong>of</strong> Interest hare. Certail charscteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

rules and types <strong>of</strong> ruls violations do sesm to lead L3 the<br />

v~olaior's being SEEP as e ggressi~e. <strong>The</strong> ch3r3cteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

rules to be cahrldsred in this cornecrion are thlse that:<br />

(1) control or restrict. behavior, (2) thzeite?. situated<br />

idonaties, (3) arc hlghly cchnecied 'a other rules. ica (9)<br />

disrupt pstablished claims. In the discussion <strong>of</strong> each <strong>of</strong><br />

these Charactfristlcs ~e w i l l attempt to demonstrat? how


ch.9. Rttrlbutlon Pracessrs Pao= 7115<br />

ccr'air structural features <strong>of</strong> ths husbind-uif? relationship<br />

may echancs the attributior or aggression for rule<br />

v101ations.<br />

Experlme~tel stndies have found that actions ths?<br />

control t h ~ behavtor <strong>of</strong> another, or corstrair anath?rrs<br />

beha~ioral al?smatiVFS Or OUtCOmeS, 2r? mOrC likely '3 be<br />

at%ributed as aggr~ssive (Brown and Tedsschi, 1974; Cimezon<br />

end Janky, 1972: Tedeschi. ct a&., 197Y). Wh?n an ictioa<br />

forces a person ~ n t o a position in which alternative csuzses<br />

<strong>of</strong> action are sevsrely ltmited, the actor probably w i l l<br />

eualoate the ac+ion as gaal-dirscted. Ere rull rialiticns<br />

that occur between Intimates more likely to be parceivea as<br />

ont trolling thaP rule viola+ions ic other scctal<br />

rdatiorshlps? St has already been nentionfd that a viao<br />

variety <strong>of</strong> rules betveel intinaies are und<strong>of</strong>insd and<br />

axSigiion~. Eu?, ir addition, ac argan~zetiocal Eeature <strong>of</strong><br />

the husband-wife rel~tionship say ilcreasc the dsqree to<br />

which z zule<br />

paTt7~FT.<br />

niclatinn <strong>of</strong> one spouse impinges oc the<br />

In nary ins*i


-- Pr~PCsitioL 8. <strong>The</strong> lover


Two people by no means reckless <strong>of</strong> eact othsz's<br />

righfs and teelings, hut ever tender cf them for<br />

thc 805t part, may tcar at each other's<br />

heartstring^ in this sacred bond with impunity:<br />

though if +hey uerc acy other twa they uould not<br />

Sp28k or look at each other again after th?<br />

ootrages -hey exchange....If the hushnd a23<br />

wife 32E blunt, outspoker people Like 'h?<br />

Laphams, they do lot weigh their words, if they<br />

ars more refined, they wsigh them rsry<br />

carefully, srd krov accuratsly just tow fnr thlv<br />

"111 vary 3rd in what most s?ns~tiue spot they<br />

may be plarted ulth the mast effect (1964:SO).<br />

Bssides possessirg kraviedge <strong>of</strong> other nemh?rs that can<br />

be "sed to do harm, intlaetes are predisposed to inpux; harm<br />

where DO nalevoler,ce 15 nFCESSarily irtendcd. For ~xnmple.<br />

lf 1n the pressice ot cthers, 2 wife sxprssses tha lesire<br />

tor certain material goods th+t are bayond her aezns. a<br />

hushand vho is sersltive about his aarring p3tential may<br />

teil threatened by such a statement. while her ints2t nay<br />

have been to share her feelings about 'hings she uould likq<br />

to hav2, the COmeFxt may be threatening to tte kushand<br />

because he knows :hit she knows that te is sensitivs about<br />

such thlngs. Fecause she knlvs ho is sensitive about such<br />

things, he :s likely to feel, she would not have szld ir<br />

unless sh? meant to commenr on his earning capscity. Lf 2<br />

PonlntimatF had exp:essfd :he same desire for azrerial<br />

goods, +her? would have heen no reason far the hushan3 to<br />

p~~ceive such i statenent 2s threatening t5 his<br />

self-ldfLtlty.<br />

If a norlntlmate asks, "Why dcnqc you ~vsr taka your<br />

wlfe aut Lo drr?er?qs ths question may cat be percsived as<br />

thriazening even when it ==s meant salevolfntly. <strong>The</strong><br />

perceiver cannot he crrtail that rhe nonintinate kn3xs he is<br />

~FTsiriVe about such ramazks. Because he knovs :hat his<br />

wife COPS he 1s sensiti~e t3 such thl?gsr the sa.4 remark<br />

by her vlll be more lik~ly perceived as threztaning.<br />

Bttr~botion theory <strong>of</strong>fers scne empirical evidence that<br />

this sort <strong>of</strong> situatlor say indeed occur. Jonss in6 Davis<br />

(1965) pr3vide ~xp~rimental ~fsezrch 02 the knowl~dge pe9ple<br />

use In deciding wish certainty an acr's intsr.tion+liiy. Two<br />

<strong>of</strong> the:= COCC~US:OIS are 3f direct corcern to intlmate<br />

TJlatlODShipS:<br />

Rnovledgf end ahility are preconditions for th2<br />

esslgrment cf ~~~~~~~~~s. Each plays a rcl? ic<br />

enablirg the perceiver tc decide uhe'hcr en<br />

effect or CO~SE~USCCF <strong>of</strong> action was accidsatsl<br />

(1965:221-222).


Ch.9. Attribution Processes<br />

-- Pro&gZ& 11. <strong>The</strong> aore extensive the<br />

knovledge <strong>of</strong> social biographies, the higher<br />

the certainty that other's ruls violation was<br />

intentional-<br />

Highly Connected Pules<br />

<strong>The</strong> violation <strong>of</strong> a rule that is highly connected to<br />

other rules iccreases the probability that th2 rule<br />

violation w i l l be perceived as aggressive.<br />

When a highly cornectfd rule is violated, it has the<br />

potential to disrupt the entire functianirg <strong>of</strong> the<br />

organization. For example, ir tbe military as ir many<br />

bureaucracies, rules are clearly specifiel and highly<br />

dlfferentiated. <strong>The</strong> violation cf a rule is hardlad an a<br />

statute basis. <strong>The</strong> violation is coded. 3na "suitable<br />

punishment' is predefined. <strong>The</strong> rules are so fragmented that<br />

it is difficult to violate highly connected rules: though<br />

the rules are connectPd, each rule is also disticct from<br />

others.<br />

In intimate relationships, relational rules 3:s not<br />

disxlnct from one another, but vague, undefinea, and Ear the<br />

nost part highly cornected tc ore another. As an example, a<br />

TUlE violation in which e husband refuses to fulfill 2 task<br />

tunctioc. by not helping around the house may generats +r<br />

argument in which the generalization is made that he is lazy<br />

and doesn't care whax happens around the hosa. 92 may<br />

retort that his wife dces cot always please him with what<br />

she does around the house and, besides, he works and she<br />

doesn't. Reference to past evects may ba br3ught in, as<br />

ve11 as recourse to dispcsitioral traits tc bolster<br />

argumentive points. To summarize:<br />

P z E E L : ~ ~ ~ . <strong>The</strong> more inximate the<br />

relationship, the more vague and undefFnsd


ch.9. Pttrlbotlon P~CCCES~S Page 149<br />

s i n . Th* more vagu* ard undsfined<br />

the rules, the higher the connectedness <strong>of</strong><br />

the rules.<br />

-- -- - --- -<br />

ProESitiOn 19. <strong>The</strong> more intimate the<br />

rslaticnship. the higher the probability that<br />

rule riclatiors w l l l bs violations <strong>of</strong><br />

connected rules.<br />

highly<br />

o i o 2 . <strong>The</strong> violation <strong>of</strong> a highly<br />

cccnected rule heightens the probability that<br />

the rule violator w i l l b? perceived 3s<br />

aggressive.<br />

Disruption <strong>of</strong> Claims<br />

StablR interaction patterns, developed over the course<br />

<strong>of</strong> a reletionship, order and make predictable the<br />

explctations cf ezch participant toward the other. Patterns<br />

<strong>of</strong> "claiming behavior' develop and become stahilizad.<br />

Claiming behavior, as the phrase implies, refers to Raking<br />

claims or demands on another because <strong>of</strong> your social position<br />

relative :O the other. Intimate relationships, as ccnpared<br />

vith ConTn+im~te relaticnships, car be characterized by<br />

E&ai&ggg CoBDlacence, wherein patterns <strong>of</strong> claiming<br />

behavior become taken fcr grarted. RelatiDcal complacence<br />

is 1.55 likely to occur in groups ia which pettfrns <strong>of</strong><br />

claiming behavior are based solely on secondary or exchange<br />

considerations; for example, in employ2r-employee or<br />

landlord-tenant relations.<br />

Relational rule violations that disrupt established<br />

patt~r~s ot claiming behavior have a high probabili+y <strong>of</strong><br />

being perceived as aggressive.<br />

<strong>The</strong> inauguration <strong>of</strong> asymmetrical claims into ar<br />

initially defined egalitarian (symmetricsl)<br />

relationship, or <strong>of</strong> symmetrical claims into an<br />

initially defied authoritarian (asymmetrical)<br />

relaticnship w i l l disrupt the harmony <strong>of</strong> the<br />

intsraction and threaten the id9ntities <strong>of</strong> the<br />

participants (Eepburn, $773: 27).<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature illustrates the effect <strong>of</strong> iltroaucing<br />

symetry Lnto initially defined asymmetrical relatiocships.<br />

Reiss (1968) points out that almost half <strong>of</strong> the cases <strong>of</strong><br />

police brutaltty involve an open defiance <strong>of</strong> the cffic?x's<br />

authority. That is, the arrested party sough? to disrupt<br />

taken-for-granted claies--in this case, a rejectioc <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>of</strong>ficer's authority- Westley (1953) reports chat whDn asked<br />

to indicate those circumstances in which th-y deem violent<br />

techniques appropriate, the police most <strong>of</strong>tsc cited those<br />

instances i T ~ which their claims were defied by uncooperative<br />

person5 SO that coercion had to be used to maintain their


ch.9. Bttrlbotio~ Processes Page 150<br />

self-intsrest. Tedeschi, gi =l. (197U) 2rgllE thii or=<br />

tactor that leads pcrsms to label ects as aggrssivc is tho<br />

advancernext <strong>of</strong> self-intezest. I disiuptio? 3f established<br />

claims is likely to be perceived as the advancsmsnt 3f the<br />

Orher's selfish int~xests by the party whose interests arm<br />

being negatively ettacted.<br />

In intimat? relationships, relaIlona1 coaplaceccs ray<br />

pradisposn one p+rty to view disruptions <strong>of</strong> c1ai.s as<br />

attemp's to further self-ipterest <strong>of</strong> the othfr. For<br />

example, a previoosly lonvorklng wife who decid?s to get 3<br />

job could be perceived es attempting ts disrupt previausly<br />

established claims. Her inIent nay be sssertive. ro+<br />

aggressive, but it may be percsived as 2ggrP55ivE by hex<br />

hnsband who sees such an actran as negatively aff€cLirg his<br />

self-intsrest. Thls process is illustrated iir ChaptFr 10 <strong>of</strong><br />

'his volume in Ralph IaRossa's cass study cf J3c afid<br />

Jennit?~.<br />

to<br />

<strong>The</strong>se four charactfrlstics <strong>of</strong> rule violations arr held<br />

facilitate 'he perception <strong>of</strong> rule via1atior.s as<br />

malevolent. If rule vialaticrs occur that have one 2r more<br />

ot these CharaccerisIics, the probability is high that such<br />

r n l ~ violations<br />

hzrm-producing.<br />

w t l l be perceived as intentionally<br />

Up to now, WP have been primarily concerned virh thc<br />

inpotation <strong>of</strong> irten:icoality to the actions <strong>of</strong> 0th~:~ ecd<br />

it5 role ir facilitating violence. ?.nother larg?<br />

ccnslderatian in attribution theory c0ncerr.s locabinq the<br />

"cause'. <strong>of</strong> the ac-ion. Tt:s perc~ivs: w i l l sOmczin?s 3'tsmpt<br />

to lrterpret an actilr by linking it to the =nvirmn+r: or<br />

the personal dispositior <strong>of</strong> the actor.<br />

A good deal <strong>of</strong> evidezcs frcm attribution theory shows<br />

that pressures =xis% to attribute :esponsibility to th-<br />

person, that is, to his/her persocality or disposition, thr<br />

more times aggressive Ections occur or are percsivad as<br />

occurr:rig. Reletional rule violations that result i~<br />

interpersonel violence reir.force the perceLv2r in vi?wing<br />

the violator 3s an aggressive person. Experiaantal sviaence<br />

from Kelley, si =A. (1962) supports this c0rten:ion:<br />

En effect is attributed to ore <strong>of</strong> its psssibl?<br />

CBUSPS with which, over tine, it covaries.<br />

A s t h ~<br />

assccia?ion between rule siols?ions 3rd<br />

ir:erpe~~Onal violence tocoEes sironger, and occurs under<br />

difterect conditions, so does the certainty rhat the rule<br />

vlolator is an aggr-ssive person.


other proc?sses also are a? work that irfluence the<br />

pe~c?l~er to view the rule Violator a5 ax aggre5sive p2rsOn<br />

rather than 2s 2 victim <strong>of</strong> his/hez envizonment. Far<br />

extmple, an experiaert by KarousD snd Pansor (1971)<br />

dpmonstra+ed that people are gfrsrally cost-orienrcd in<br />

tOrairg Overall eYBluationS. When an objsct 3r avect has<br />

bath posifiv€ and r?ga-ive characteristics, p~ople v3igh tho<br />

nore thai 'he posltive. Ir the fsmily situsticn,<br />

preVlOuS rul? viole'ions have caused i n t ~ ~ p e ~ s o ~ a l<br />

V;~len~~, subsequent ac+ions taksn by :he rule violatcr.<br />

containirg both posltive end lqative faalurss, nap<br />

be mgr~ 1:kely to be see? as aggressive.<br />

nore important to our discussion <strong>of</strong> the Lapn*ation <strong>of</strong><br />

aggression to the personal dispositio~ <strong>of</strong> the sctar is th=<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> violence itself. v:olence is usually s high<br />

intensity. paic-producing act. If in th= past a+t=~.pts to<br />

~eeolve rule violatiocs have escalated to vislenc-, the<br />

prlbability is greater <strong>of</strong> seeiog the other as agg:crsive.<br />

Exper;aental ev;dcsc€ from both KElley (1967) snd Palstsr<br />

(1966) polnts out 'hat the perceiver has an increesicp leed<br />

to attrxbute rssponsibllify as the outcomes <strong>of</strong> sctiocs<br />

become nore severe.<br />

In a social-psychological sanso. the consequeccas for<br />

the role vxalator car be SEV~~P, in tha* changes ir<br />

self-concep' may occur as attributions to 'he personal<br />

~hazz~ter~stic~ are trarsaitted. <strong>The</strong> rule viola's; say<br />

begin to see hia/hers~lf 8s aggressFvI or violent an0 ic: In<br />

toxs <strong>of</strong> the larqer cultoral meacing 3f beiog an aggr;ssive<br />

OT violent person.<br />

lffect me's ittribntior.. <strong>The</strong> action nbservsd has air-cL<br />

positive or lagativs corsequecces for 'he<br />

hedoristically relevant. also, perceiver<br />

pcrcsiver--r' is<br />

bias may occur<br />

becauss <strong>of</strong> the relationship <strong>of</strong> actor :a perceivsr. whar the<br />

pErCeirer belLeVes that ths action he/sh? cbserrss hss been<br />

conditroned by his/her presence, pe:sonalisa w i l l affect<br />

dispositioral a?:rlbufions.<br />

Eecause <strong>of</strong> the<br />

these two factors<br />

high interdspendence cf tts c3aple.<br />

*re especially relevant to intimate<br />

relationships. Rflati~nal rule viol%?iors are


Ch.9. Bttrlbutio~ Process~s Page 152<br />

hadonistically relevant aria personalistic to intimates and<br />

miy bias the attribution process. Inciaates may be<br />

reluctant to lebel a spouse rs aggressivs or vlolent b?causs<br />

t h attributioc ~<br />

hes laplications for both. T3 lab?l an<br />

intlmate as an aggressxva cr viols-t persln may be too<br />

diffScult to reconcile ulth the image <strong>of</strong> spouse 3s a freely<br />

cholqn, close emctianal partcer. <strong>The</strong> perceiv9r may tend to<br />

err LP tho dxrectioc <strong>of</strong> nonrecognition <strong>of</strong> the spuss~s<br />

Xe~pOXSiD~lity =Pa ac'ribute ruls violsti3r.s $3 the<br />

er~ironment or situaticr rather than tc tho partcerqr<br />

per~onali*y. ?indings from a study by Ylrrow, 2: gi.<br />

[1955), <strong>of</strong> wives' definitions cf husbands'+m;tional prabl4ms<br />

can be thus irterpreted. <strong>The</strong>y repcrf a grsst raluct3cc? on<br />

+he part ot wives to labs1 their husbands as msntally ill.<br />

Not wtil a11 p9ssible in+.erpz=tations or their hushafids*<br />

behav10r are Examired and found wanting is the attribution<br />

to psrson~l disposition made. In othsz words, the evidecce<br />

has to b? orsr~helming before the husbecd ls seen as a<br />

mentally 111 person in need <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essional help.<br />

<strong>The</strong> other concep; relevant to attribution bias amcng<br />

Irtinates is what Xelleg (1971) calls plurrlistic ignorance.<br />

This v&rlahlP TF~?XS to the lack <strong>of</strong> consensus an an<br />

attribution through either the absence <strong>of</strong> conparisoo 3r the<br />

lacK <strong>of</strong> other observers. <strong>The</strong> private nature <strong>of</strong> th? frmily<br />

institution (Laslett, 1373). i? vhich most activities take<br />

plaCB0~t 05 visu ct onf family menh?:s, height=ns the<br />

likellhcad ot pluralistic ignorance. Ittributions a2ds ic<br />

the family setrirg usually 'ak? place wrthout feedback from<br />

others. <strong>The</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> 'bird parry observers and the inshility<br />

to observe Other sets <strong>of</strong> =ntimatcs during the resolution if<br />

rule violations leads to e lack <strong>of</strong> certainty as to 'ha<br />

proper at'rzbutian. Because <strong>of</strong> this lack <strong>of</strong> certainty and<br />

the possibiljtp <strong>of</strong> errcr. dispositioral a+tr;bntions sap be<br />

made in + conservative direction, blaming th? situation<br />

rather thrn the spouse.<br />

Bs was me~tion~d i? referents to lahelico mental<br />

illness. attribut:~~ bias C C U ~ bc ~ an inportant variebl? il<br />

ac~oufiting for the reluctrnce <strong>of</strong> many married persons to<br />

seek help when i~volved in violert rslationships, unril the<br />

outcomes becon? severe tcr everyone involved.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> relatlorshlp between husband-wife violence and two<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> attributicn theory were examined in this chlpter.<br />

<strong>The</strong> first or these aspects, the attributio~. <strong>of</strong> ilfOP:i3nal<br />

agg1?ssion, was seen as ac rmportart variable in the<br />

occurrencs <strong>of</strong> interpersoral violence. It was lrgued that<br />

the violatlor <strong>of</strong> relational rulss, vhich ars design?& to<br />

protect the ide~tities <strong>of</strong> pir:icFpacts in Lntimate<br />

relationships, can tnit-ate + process wherein rhe vllidity


~h.9. Rttrlbutlon PTOCGS~ES Page 153<br />

Of the rule is negotiated, and attempts to Toduca the<br />

harm-doing <strong>of</strong> zhs violation are bsgun. l'hs siclation <strong>of</strong><br />

iela;:onal rulss may be iccid9ntal or deliberate, the<br />

slg~~tiCan? factor beizg the msaning <strong>of</strong> the visiafinr th+t<br />

the oxher attrlbutes -a ths violatcr.<br />

FJUr kirds <strong>of</strong> rul= vlolaiioos most likely ts be<br />

perceived 25 aggressive are (1) rule violaticcs ?tat c3rtrol<br />

behanor, (2) rule violations seer as thraats tc sltuazsd<br />

identirres, (3) violations <strong>of</strong> rules that 2re highly<br />

to other rules, and (9) rule vi311tior.s tkat<br />

dlsznpt. established claims.<br />

<strong>The</strong> second aspect oE af!ribution theory examioaa was<br />

that concezned with the process <strong>of</strong> locaiinq the "cause" <strong>of</strong><br />

the actioc. 09ce a perceiver attrlbutes ir.tentlonali2y to<br />

anothsr's acticns, he/she atturnpis to interpret tbe cause <strong>of</strong><br />

th4 actio?. Pttributions are made to the envircca-st or<br />

slruation, or to the persnrality sf the actor. Certain<br />

feizures <strong>of</strong> lntxmste re1ztio~~h:ps were seen as promacicg<br />

attrrbutro~al bias, wherei~. sltuat~ocal ai3 e?.vir~cmental<br />

tac10~5 IFF blamed tor vlolent actions rather thin the<br />

personal drsposltlor <strong>of</strong> +he actor.<br />

NOTES<br />

+A V ~ T S ~ O X ~f th+s chaptqr was preserted st rhe <strong>The</strong>ory<br />

Constrnctio~ Workshop at the ZCXUZ~ meeti~g <strong>of</strong> tk4 BlfioPal<br />

Conncll on Family Bela+lors, August 13-20, 1975. <strong>The</strong><br />

prepazarror <strong>of</strong> this chipter was supportsa by N3tion21<br />

I~stitutr <strong>of</strong> n e ~ t nealth ~ l grant No. 13050. I would like<br />

to rhank Es. Joyce E. FCSS B P ~ 85- Siu~dra L. Etw-11 for<br />

many helpful suggestions throughcut the formulstiox <strong>of</strong> this<br />

chapter.<br />

1. RggrEssiCn 55 here defined as a physical Or<br />

p~y~hological act. or acts, occurrirg in 3r icterp-rsaral<br />

situation, that are judpcd to he irt=c?i3rally


harm-producing. <strong>The</strong> ddsticctinn betv<strong>of</strong>r:<br />

violrrce is so prcblematlc +hat it would<br />

aggressior and<br />

take an rctirc<br />

chapter to deal with thr eaetsr (sse G~lles era Sfraus,<br />

1978). his chapter focuses ox hcu attribution processes<br />

faclliiate violence by producirg a cycle <strong>of</strong> escals:inq<br />

aggressior.<br />

2. Iden?.ity is seer hFre 85 a socicl pheccmenon.<br />

Through OUT posit~~n-SB+S, iz the social structure in<br />

rElatlOh to other positrcn-sets, it is established where and<br />

what we are. Mas? basically, iaeirity is the answar ts thp<br />

question, "Who am I?"<br />

3. Sltu~ted ~dentity to the lcc3tion <strong>of</strong> ace's<br />

social self in rslatlon to e sp~ciric social pos


art IV <strong>The</strong> Interplay <strong>of</strong> Culture<br />

and <strong>Social</strong> Organization


Tht chapcers in the prec?ding seciior<br />

emphasize that cerizin aspacts <strong>of</strong> fasily<br />

Organization increass the p=ohability a£<br />

C03flict. But ccnflict 1s not ths same is<br />

violence: the:? a x many nanviolen+ usys 3f<br />

sertling conflict. EOVBV~T, in a group<br />

charac-erized by a high degree <strong>of</strong> conflict,<br />

where P O ~ S legiLimizs the use <strong>of</strong> physical force<br />

(as shown in Part 11). the *?age is set far 3<br />

high ~ F V E <strong>of</strong> ~ <strong>Violence</strong>. This coablnatioc <strong>of</strong><br />

social Orgarizatio~al and cultural factors forms<br />

the ~OCUS ot all three chepters in Part <strong>IT</strong>.<br />

<strong>The</strong> three chapters have something alse io<br />

COmmoC. A l l illustrate what All?n and straus in<br />

Chapter 12 call the "ultimate resource theory <strong>of</strong><br />

vi01~r.ce." TMs theory statas that vislence w i l l<br />

be used as a resource when other resouzcss srr<br />

lacking. Thus 2 tanily member with little<br />

power, mor-y, or status is nore likely to resort<br />

to vlolerce as a means <strong>of</strong> winning a c~cflict.<br />

<strong>The</strong> chapters in ihls section usi vastly<br />

dittsrent rn+%hods, but each comss to gzips "5th<br />

the type <strong>of</strong> questicns just listed. ?


e Haven't<br />

Since": Conjugal <strong>Violence</strong><br />

olitics <strong>of</strong> Marriage<br />

Ralph LaRossa<br />

JOE and Jennifer, the couple whose story we<br />

follow in this chapter, is not the stuiy <strong>of</strong> a<br />

"disturbed" couple.<br />

varying degrees,<br />

<strong>The</strong>y, like other csuples in<br />

have certain conflicts <strong>of</strong><br />

interest--some <strong>of</strong> which may end in vi3lence.<br />

Thslr story gives us an inside view <strong>of</strong> the<br />

interplay <strong>of</strong> cultural and social organizntional<br />

factors in producing warits1 violencl. Is<br />

pointed out in earlier chapters, the fact that<br />

cultural norms permit violencs amsrg family<br />

membqrs does not automatically mean that<br />

viol~nce w i l l be used to resolv? c3nflict<br />

situations. we must also understand the social<br />

organizatioc <strong>of</strong> the marriage. This is vividly<br />

illusfrated vhen Joe relates the time he slapped<br />

Jenflfer<br />

during a<br />

"three<br />

pariod<br />

or<br />

in<br />

four times." This o=currea<br />

their narriage when Joa<br />

perceived Jennifer as attempting to doricate<br />

the~r relationship. Joe responded to this<br />

attempted coup with violerce because he felt hs<br />

"had to do something to stop the bad progression<br />

<strong>of</strong> events." In short, violenca was us~d vhen<br />

other factors that in his mind voull validate<br />

his claim to dominant power uers lacking.<br />

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .<br />

In an attempt to help fill the need for ethrsgraphiss<br />

3f Rn~~ican marriages, I recently co2ductld a qualrtative<br />

study Of Sixteen married cJuples expecting their first child<br />

(LaBossa. 1977).t1 By conversations with those couples led<br />

me to c3nclud9 that, contrary to popular opinion,<br />

confrontatios azd strugglo as well as conssnsus ard<br />

equilibrinm are at the root <strong>of</strong> the marital union. <strong>The</strong> idea<br />

thlt marriage is a ccrflLct system is hsrdly nova1 (see


Sprey, 1969. for example). what evidsntly his been lickicg<br />

however is empirical suppaet for this contention.*2<br />

he purpose <strong>of</strong> this chapter is to present a.<br />

illustration <strong>of</strong> the cocflict theme. <strong>The</strong> casa study that<br />

follows tells the story <strong>of</strong> Joe an& Jennifer (Pseudonyms), a<br />

couple whose power struggls is manifested in 1 varisty <strong>of</strong><br />

ways. heir story is particularly sigcificsot bacauss <strong>of</strong><br />

the violent side to their relationship. Through ;heir<br />

experience, the cold war metaphor, iaplicit in tho conflict<br />

approach, truly comes to llfe.<br />

JOE AND JENNIFER<br />

Th? first pregnancy meant essentially two zhings to Jo+<br />

and Jennifer. It meant that after having waited Pearly four<br />

years they were finally starting a family, sonsthing they<br />

had aluays wanted to do. It also signaled a chaogf in their<br />

work strncture. For the first time since they ware married<br />

JenLifsr would not be uorkino. nor? inoort.int. for the<br />

firs: :=me smce they sere marri;d Joe would be ;he sole<br />

wage earner. <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> this latter point is that<br />

Joe intended to ase his new position to m+ke a claim for<br />

dominance in the marriage.<br />

Joe was a metaphorical speaker. DftEn, vhile<br />

discussing an issue, he uould (to "clarifya a point) brlnp<br />

in anecdotes from hls personal experiences or relats the<br />

issue to the in'ernationzl state <strong>of</strong> affairs. <strong>The</strong>re were<br />

tmss during my irterviews with this couple. I aust ccnfess,<br />

when I wished he vould have been more specific in his<br />

answers. I learned however that I had to accept, as others<br />

had, that Joe uas just "deep."<br />

Jennifer: Joe is a very deep thlnker, ard hs<br />

always has soeethlng on hls m~nd. Pe car.<br />

drlve you right up a wall!<br />

Though Joe did most <strong>of</strong> the talking, Jennifer was not ar<br />

a 1055 for words. Scmetimes she found it difficult to get a<br />

word in etgeu~se or remember what question I had asked after<br />

Joe had plcked i? up and ruz with iz for a uhils, but then<br />

SO dFd I. Phen JenEifer did speak. she said %hat was on her<br />

mind--as dld Joe: but, as she once said, what it took Joe<br />

to say in a paragraph, she said in a sentence. On a number<br />

<strong>of</strong> cccasions durine ~ X F interviews vhile Joe was building an<br />

argument (and this vas particularly true if 33e's argument<br />

mas an attempt to justify why he should be in charge) a feu<br />

well placsd ucrdr by Jennifer and Joe's edifice uould come<br />

tumbling dew.


Ch.(~- Eavcn't 5ad any Prcblems Since Page 159<br />

JOE ard Jennifer had knoun each other since high<br />

Sch~ol. <strong>The</strong>ir first raactioc to each other was, as Jencifer<br />

put ~ t, 'mntnal disgust." Both attribute this ts the fact<br />

?hat they are each honest types--if they asn't like you,<br />

they tell you-ard, in the beginning, they told each other<br />

more <strong>of</strong> vhat 'hey didn't like than <strong>of</strong> vhat they liPe3. In<br />

tloe, their hatred turned ro love. #ha: attraztad thpe to<br />

each other was thsir similarities--their opeaness, their<br />

aggzessiveness, and, interostirgly, what they saw as the<br />

inability <strong>of</strong> either to dominate the other. it was a<br />

relationship bullt explicitly or. corflict and honesty. (Joe<br />

once described Jennifar as his "confessor,' +nd he hers.)<br />

T ~ E couple could not recall any specific paint at vhich<br />

the7 decided to gst marrled. "Someplace a1or.g the lice,' it<br />

"as just assumed. Though they nay not have gone through the<br />

rarr~age proposal ritual, the transition to the married<br />

state was 0x3 they took very seriously. Joe and Jecnifer<br />

yere a religions couple. Joe. In particular, prided hinself<br />

on hls interpretations <strong>of</strong> ths Bible. <strong>The</strong>y did not believe<br />

iz di~orcs. <strong>The</strong>y f~lt it reflected ueakn~ss--a couple's<br />

inability to face llfe's problems.<br />

when they got married, they lived solely on Jencifer's<br />

income. Bavitg graduated from high school with a business<br />

alploma, she worked as a bookkeeper. Joe was just beginning<br />

hls thlrd year <strong>of</strong> college, studying to be an accouotart.<br />

Actually. for rhE tirst three and a half years <strong>of</strong> their<br />

marriage, Jencifer would be the primary wage earner. This<br />

yas because after Joe was awarded his bachelor's degree, he<br />

vent on ;o attad a postgraduate business schml which took<br />

hlm a year and a half to complete.<br />

~oth believed that the vay they sere aach raised<br />

explained their personalities and uhy ,hay complemectel each<br />

other. Jenn~fsr described hcr prs-marriage family life as<br />

ore in vhich shs was the primary decisi3n-maker.<br />

Jennifer: I uas always very inaependect bsfore<br />

I got BaXrlEd. BS a matter <strong>of</strong> fact, my<br />

parents were never my rule. I was the 1812<br />

<strong>of</strong> my parents.<br />

Joe, on the ether haad, was brought up in i patriarchal<br />

home--all decisions Mere made by his father.<br />

502: ... Bnd at my house it %as just the<br />

opposite. ny father was a very strong father<br />

Image. ?radltlocal. "Come to him, your<br />

fath~r w i l l decide for YOU ."... Es rsuldn%<br />

g i v ~ mr any responsibility.<br />

Jennifer's iidependerce, they felt, was a Euncticc <strong>of</strong><br />

her tanng been forced to be independent all alcng. 309's<br />

was a aanifcstatior <strong>of</strong> his rebellion against his father's


Ch.10. Kaven't Had Any ProDlems Since Page 16,<br />

autocratic style ("I had to sort <strong>of</strong> assert nyszlf*),<br />

According to Joe, Jennifer caae to the marriage wanting t,<br />

"ge+, rid <strong>of</strong>" some <strong>of</strong> her power, and he caae "wanting s:re,l<br />

so their relatlocship "worked out all right." Neither voul,<br />

try to dominate the other.<br />

Rlthough Joe claimed that Jennifer wanted less pare:<br />

and ;ha? she would not try to dominate him, this evidentl:<br />

was &sf the czse. Soaet5me during the first t u ~ years oj<br />

their marriage, Joe and Jennifer got into in argum2nt thai<br />

ended in violence--Joe struck Jennifer. <strong>The</strong> conflict was I<br />

power struggle: Joe supposedly hit Jennifer becaust<br />

Jernifer was trying to dominate. HE responded uith forcr<br />

because he telt he "had to do something physical to st3p th'<br />

bad progression <strong>of</strong> evente." <strong>The</strong> sequence opens uith mj<br />

asklng Jennirer whether she thought she ran things now--that<br />

15, does she Delieve she is "the rulef <strong>of</strong> JOE 35 shs was<br />

"the rule" at her parents and sistors.<br />

Intervieuer: DO you think you run things now?<br />

JeCnlter: NO, I tried hard, though!<br />

Joe: She tries. One day we had a conflict and<br />

she mare or less tried to run me and I told<br />

her n3, ard she got hystsrical and said, "I<br />

could kill YOU!" And I got rather angry ana<br />

slappad her in the face rhrfe or four times<br />

and I said "Don't you 2V91 5+y that to me<br />

again." And we haven't had any problems<br />

since. so she's sort <strong>of</strong> learned that she<br />

~sn't gomg to dominate.<br />

Jernitsr: Yes, aT.6 1 krnd <strong>of</strong> llke 'he idea,<br />

too.<br />

Joe: She threw a temper rantrum when she<br />

realized that she couldn't dominate ae, and<br />

"her sha started getting hysterical, ... that's<br />

the last time, kid! Yeah that's th? worst<br />

argument ue ever had! That uas a drawn out<br />

bang out fight. It lasted about four hours.<br />

It sort <strong>of</strong> built and built...<br />

Interviewer: Were you surprised uheo Joe hit<br />

you?<br />

Jo?: Oh, bey, was she.<br />

Je~cifez: Yeah.<br />

Joe: She started crying not because I hur; her<br />

but becaase she vas shocked--"How lare you!'<br />

Ittarvlewer: uhg d-d you hxt he:?


10. Ea~er't Bad A c ~ Problems Since Page 161<br />

jennlfer: That was a long time ago.<br />

~oe: That was a real long time ago. It's just<br />

Like if you want to do som?thing like tear<br />

down a house, what do you use? Do you use ao<br />

atom bomb. or do you os? a crane snd hammers<br />

acd stuff like that? It's just like physical<br />

force. You dor't use it until you're forcG3<br />

to use it. A t thet point, I felt I had to a3<br />

something physical to stop thi bad<br />

progression <strong>of</strong> events. I took my chances<br />

with that and it uorkad. In those<br />

circnnstanc9s, my judgment was correct and it<br />

W C ~ ~ E I .<br />

~ecnifer: Joe aoesn't usually use force. That<br />

Ua5 the fzrst and the last tin€ he'll ?VEX a3<br />

that. It was my fault. I was tryicg $2<br />

dominate him, that's for sure. But I was<br />

always that type <strong>of</strong> person. Chzt's vhy. I<br />

always had to be that typs <strong>of</strong> person, becausE<br />

I always had to naXe my ovr decisions. I<br />

never had anybody else make my decisions.<br />

Joe: I'm a very dominating psrson, t30. $3<br />

there was a conilict there.<br />

Jenlifer: I think rhat's one <strong>of</strong> the reasons v?<br />

got along so w i l l , because he was the first<br />

person I vent out with that I couldn't<br />

dominate. So he was a challenge.<br />

J09: That was a severe conflict. I don't know<br />

Lf we hadn't solvea that problem, if wa w~ula<br />

still be married, because <strong>of</strong> the tension.<br />

I ' m not the ki~d <strong>of</strong> person that's going to b?<br />

aomlnated.<br />

Je~nlfer: R c ~ I'm not either.<br />

JOB: 50 ve'w had to agree, thrmgh s process<br />

<strong>of</strong> compromise, and talking this 3ut. We'ra<br />

l:ving on reconcLliation and compromise and<br />

understendirg.<br />

Though lengthy, the Sequence is important. It is<br />

Important not orly for what is said, but for how as vsll.<br />

For example, the tenor <strong>of</strong> Joe's comnents--he speaks 2s if<br />

~ennifer were auilty <strong>of</strong> disrespect or even insuborainstion<br />

("Dm't you Ever ssy that to me again!".. ."Thai's the last<br />

time. kid!"). ?"en more, there is ucdoubt2dly a csrtain<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> pride expressed--he Gsx he had won 7h.t arguasrt.<br />

Jenrlfer, on the other haid, is quick to point out "that was<br />

the first a_= the 68t time feel1 erer do thet [hit h?r]."<br />

She wanted to make it perfectly clear to me, but more


ch.10. Havez't Had sny Problens since Page 16<br />

importantly tc JOE, that she too had no intention <strong>of</strong> beit<br />

dominated--and. perhaps. that shs considered Joa's glcalin<br />

a2 attempt to do just that! She goes on to admit that sb<br />

married Joe because "he was a challenqe.' Does she mean b<br />

this that she cozsiders Joe her opponent? Ths conflic<br />

Patnrs <strong>of</strong> thelr marriage rs explicitly acknowledged whsn jo<br />

concludes by saying that their relationship works<br />

"reconci1ia:ion and compromise" (words which connot,<br />

"conflict' as well as "ucdsrstandlng."<br />

BS noted, Joe was a stude<strong>of</strong> for the first three and<br />

half years <strong>of</strong> their marriage. When he graduated, h,<br />

accepted an <strong>of</strong>fer to work as an accountant with a lo=*:<br />

firm. Ole month after Joe tcok :he job, the couple begal<br />

trying to conceive a child. Four months later they founr<br />

out that Jencifer was pregnant. <strong>The</strong>y uer2 wideray jus:<br />

biding their time, waiting for Joe to finish school--the<br />

point at vhich +hey felt it would be time to start a family.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y always intended to have children. R s Jennifer put it,<br />

they never "really seriously' cocsiderrd n3t hevin:<br />

children.<br />

Joe: I think having children is a<br />

f l f l l e t People that are narrisd and<br />

don't have children tend to get more selfish<br />

as they get older. And I think there's a lot<br />

<strong>of</strong> truth in that.<br />

Jenciter: If you see people without children,<br />

they tend to be vezy selfish, self-centered<br />

people.<br />

JOB: I thlnk people uho have children tead to<br />

he more outgoing, acd have a healthier<br />

attitude toward life.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y also bellered the chlld would brllg them closer '3 each<br />

cther . Joe: I thick it's going to pull us tsgether<br />

more... Each and every little item that you<br />

do together or cac discuss together or have<br />

il Conmon brings you clossr togethsr.<br />

In anditlon to these reasors, the couple <strong>of</strong>fered yet<br />

%other reasor vhg they opted to have s child. <strong>The</strong>y wanted<br />

to star* 3 family befor? Jennifer got "ioo ambitious" in her<br />

jcb. <strong>The</strong> fact is that while Joe may have just been starting<br />

h:~ czreer, Jz~nifer had become quite established in hers.<br />

She bad becans tt.e superv;sor <strong>of</strong> the bzokkesping dapartment<br />

in the company she had bees<br />

married.<br />

working for SLnce they were<br />

J~lzif~;: I fignrfd I better have one baror* I<br />

gat too ambitious in my job. I was getting i


lot <strong>of</strong> proeotioLs and I decided if I got too<br />

ambitious I may not want children. I might<br />

get too involved 1n material things.<br />

ind in another intervie*--<br />

Jennifer: Tters's a point in your life "her you<br />

shonld have 3 family-.. If ysu wait too<br />

long, you start to believe that money is m3re<br />

importan; thsn family life. I've seer. rh+t<br />

happen to some other people.<br />

508 and Jennifer's deemphasis <strong>of</strong> "saterial things" and<br />

the:r positlvr regard for "family life' was, to a large<br />

degree, an Outgrowth <strong>of</strong> their religious beliefs. Bt the<br />

COrs <strong>of</strong> these beliefs is the notion that working is for<br />

pei~onal fulfillment and cot for the monetary rewards it may<br />

br=ag. Jennlfer spoke <strong>of</strong> beiLg a full-time mother, so I<br />

asked her whether she believed she would ever return to<br />

wort. She assured me she would, that she would like to work<br />

zs a consultact eventually, if orly part-time. She felt it<br />

was important for her to pursue her career, that "in this<br />

day and age. you need more then just the family." It was<br />

apparent that Jcnclfer's corcept <strong>of</strong> s ~ l f was related to her<br />

career as well as to her family. So ues Joe's. While<br />

533c:rer's ambitions ware being stifled, Jae's ambitions<br />

"era being ralged. Jennifer once said that when her<br />

"quiet," "subdued' family first met Jos, they were "sh3ckedt'<br />

by hi5 ffank3ess. <strong>The</strong> impression the couple give hswerer<br />

When they speak <strong>of</strong> the effect uhich moving fros stndsnt to<br />

vorkPr had on Joe is that he had lost some <strong>of</strong> his<br />

a~~ertlve~ess i C the interim. Through his work he 115s<br />

eridintly regaining his indepecdence 3nd self-confidence.<br />

JeoriSer: I think JOE is getting more<br />

independent ... He's been working well with<br />

all the business people he's beEa dealing<br />

w i t h lately. RS'S gstti~g more<br />

self-confident...<br />

Intervrewr: Do you feel that Joe lacked<br />

self-conf~dence?<br />

Jenciier: I thlnk that "hen you first get out<br />

<strong>of</strong> school YOU do. You're not ussd to being<br />

-11-h hasines5 people. You're used to be=zg<br />

with s~ude~ts.<br />

I D ~ ~ ~ Y I E W Fhat ~ ~ : do you think about your s?lfcorfSderce,<br />

Joe?<br />

Joe: I thick I'm gairiig more self-confidsnce.<br />

With more experience you know what ts do.<br />

another self-confidence builder which Joe was rnrclvfd with


Ch.10. Haven" Had Eny Problems Since Page 16Y<br />

was studying for the Certified Public Bccountant (CPA)<br />

exams. Ee dianqt went to be a CPB. BE jnst wa~ted to pass<br />

the exam and. as he said, "stick my tongue out." (.st whoa?<br />

He didn't say.) Jernifer also warted him to take the exam so<br />

that he would be more flexible. If he didn't like one job,<br />

he would. be able to move to another with more ease.<br />

<strong>The</strong> fact that both Joe and Jannifer's individual<br />

concepts <strong>of</strong> self were so related to th~ir rsspective careers<br />

is particularly interesting. When the subject <strong>of</strong> arguing<br />

came up (I asked all the cauples vhar they usually argued<br />

ibout), Joe ard Jenrifer said that ths thing they argued.<br />

about the most was accounting and bookkeeping. When I asked<br />

them why they argued so much about accounting and<br />

bookkeeping, it became apparent that they considsr<br />

themSE1Ves. sore 01 less, in the same business--the business<br />

<strong>of</strong> handling money--and that in this business thsy bath hare<br />

their own ideas. Actually, they seam to approech the<br />

bnsiness f~om two airferDnt ---.-. mints <strong>of</strong> view. ~ o e . as an<br />

~ A - ~ - ~, ~~accDuntant,<br />

represenzs ~ h e abstract or rhsorotical<br />

viewpoint. Jennifer, as a<br />

concrete (down to earth) vieu.<br />

bookkecpsr, represents the<br />

R L later ~ on,<br />

Interviewer: Why do you think you end up<br />

arguing ahout it?<br />

Jennifer: I thi~k rhatls something we both have<br />

our own ideas or.<br />

Joe: Sometimes I ' m inconsistent and shs points<br />

it oat. E t Other times her knJwlsdge about<br />

the subject is Lor as high as mine, so I have<br />

to sort Of Educate her.<br />

Jenrifer: I ' m more accurate and he's sore<br />

knovlsdgeable. Put it that way.<br />

Jennifer: HE'S an accountant, ana I'm a<br />

bookkeeper-<br />

Joe: Yeah. she's + bookkeeper. Bookkeepers can<br />

fLnd errors, and accountants can make up<br />

systems 2nd can decide hou the systels can<br />

zur or why, and the bookkeepers can figti<br />

errors.<br />

Jan~:fer: Eookkeeper~ can correct accountarts'<br />

mistakes.<br />

Tt~' classic ~oztfli~t--~ducati~n<br />

VB~SUS sxperie~ce--<br />

seems to be at the root <strong>of</strong> their discussioc. Despite what<br />

may appear to most <strong>of</strong> us as an intellectual ?xsrcise, the<br />

fact that Joe and Jennifer's individual concepts <strong>of</strong> self are


ch.10. Har~n't Had Any Problems Since Page 165<br />

so related to the money handling business makes their<br />

con2rontations more than a dirersias. <strong>The</strong>y "era, I believe,<br />

man2festatLons <strong>of</strong> the same conflict that had bean going on<br />

be'vsen them sicce they net in high school--who doainstes<br />

whom?<br />

Given that the onset <strong>of</strong> pregnancy signaled a change in<br />

the work structure <strong>of</strong> the couple's marriage, on? might also<br />

suspect that thslr perennial conflict vould develsp into<br />

some interesting power plays and parries. This is, in fact,<br />

essentially whet happened. Joe may heve rebelled agsirst<br />

his father's attempt to exE:t control ss t he husbacd-father.<br />

<strong>The</strong>74 rere, however, lndicztions that Joe too would have<br />

lik~d to command Jernifer's respect an3 suboramation<br />

because te too vas no* the aan <strong>of</strong> the house.<br />

Joe: Hell, I'm a pure male chauvinist pig. and<br />

I'll admit it.<br />

Jennifer: Yeah.<br />

JOE'S chznvinism, or more precisely his belief that he<br />

should doninste Jennifer bscausf that's the way it should<br />

be. was <strong>of</strong>ten not es explicit as the above adaiss:on, but<br />

thsre was no mistaking its Existence in some <strong>of</strong> Joe's other<br />

comments. Por exaeple:<br />

Joe: 1 doo't really discuss the pregnancy that<br />

much with others. I let Jennifer do all ths<br />

aiscussing... my backgrourd with the people<br />

In this area: %he nsn just don't discuss<br />

pregnancy...ve let the women take care <strong>of</strong><br />

that.*3<br />

Joe: It seems like there's a breakdown in<br />

TO~ES, if you know what I'm gftticg at. It<br />

seems like zll the women "ant to ba coal<br />

alners zll <strong>of</strong> a sudden. It seems ro be th?<br />

thi~g to do. Ey theory is: that the vomEc<br />

would be better <strong>of</strong>f to star home and take<br />

care <strong>of</strong> the kids and take care <strong>of</strong> tha social<br />

clubs and that sort <strong>of</strong> stuff. And the men 93<br />

out znd earn the money... I think the basic<br />

problem rith juverile delinquency and the<br />

*hole mess that this country is in is that<br />

ths mar goes out and works, and ths woman<br />

~ O E S OE* and works, and the children are left<br />

Dome...<br />

Unfcrtunately for Joe, Jennifer wouldn't buy his<br />

ideological (the culture says that's :he way it's SUDPOSEB<br />

to be) theory or who doainates whom. To be a master, one<br />

must have a slave. Sat the byproawct <strong>of</strong>


Ch.10. Aaveh'i Had Eny Problems Since Page 166<br />

~ > -.--- ~.~ ~ ~- ~ ~- ~ ~ -- - -. ~<br />

cali& the nsource theory <strong>of</strong> pouer. his theory argues<br />

that the allocation <strong>of</strong> tasks and power is based (or should<br />

be basod. it you're using it as a maxim which Joe was) or<br />

the comparative resources <strong>of</strong> the members <strong>of</strong> a society and by<br />

the life circuwstancss within - ~~~ which thev live. In more<br />

sznple terms, what thi; means for the 'micro society.<br />

marriage, is that the division <strong>of</strong> work ma, most<br />

importantly. power is deesrmined not by ideology but by who<br />

brings in more resources. R resource is deficsd as anythirg<br />

that one partner may make available to the othan, helping<br />

the latter satisfy his or her needs or attain hls :r her<br />

goals (Blood and Xalfe. 1960:12). Boney and expertise.<br />

example, might qualify as resoures.<br />

for<br />

Rithin the resource theory system. Joe's claim to pouer<br />

would be structurally based on the asssrticn that hf was<br />

bringing in what some ccuples consider thc most iapsrtant<br />

resource--money. Perhaps Jennifer tried to use this<br />

justification to domilate Joe auring the first three acd a<br />

half years <strong>of</strong> t heir marriage. She was then th% breadwinser.<br />

Rod perhaps Joe, though he (literally) fought her attempts<br />

durlng the early years <strong>of</strong> their marriage to make such a<br />

claim, eventually was convinced <strong>of</strong> h?r deficition <strong>of</strong> the<br />

situation. would this explain his loss <strong>of</strong> self-confidence<br />

that getting a job (resources?) helped him to regail?<br />

Fherher or not Joe was making a claim basea on rules which<br />

had existed all alorg, it was obvious he anticipai,ed using<br />

uhat he saw as his caeperatively greater resources to<br />

support his domination. Wi*h the transitiox to parenthood<br />

he would become the breadwinner, he would have the<br />

responsibilities, and he would be ia charge...cr sa he<br />

hoped.<br />

Joe: I'm sort <strong>of</strong> proud and happy now that my<br />

wife's pregnart and we're going to have a<br />

child. and it was the motivating farce in<br />

terms <strong>of</strong> me thinking about being the<br />

breadwinner, assuming a specific role. She's<br />

going to be staying home. Before, I was just<br />

another person going out and WorKing and nav<br />

I'm going to be the breadwinner...<br />

Icfervisver: Do ycu lite that?<br />

JOF: I thlnk it's nice to feel that you'rp<br />

raking charge. .. V h ~ n you have<br />

reSpOnEibil~ties, you end up being in charge.<br />

OPCS agaln, to Jos's frus~ration. Jennifer vouldn't buy<br />

his theory on who dominates whom. She mads it clear a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> times during the interviews that she had no<br />

intention <strong>of</strong> endorsing Joe*s claim. <strong>The</strong> sequence which


ch.10. Haven't Bad Any Problems Since Page 167<br />

follows illustrates Joe's moves and Jennifer" counntzroves<br />

in thelr negotiation <strong>of</strong> power.<br />

Intervlever: In the organization <strong>of</strong> your<br />

marriage, are you the boss?<br />

Joe: In the circumstances here, in the uay<br />

we're dividing the authority, now she's g<strong>of</strong>ng<br />

to be the housewife and I'n going to b€ th2<br />

principal breadwinner. That mores me up 2<br />

notch in terms <strong>of</strong> baing the breadwinner ana<br />

having the say in financial matters. Sh3's<br />

going to be in control <strong>of</strong> the house<br />

ex~losively. She's going to have more say in<br />

what goes on vith it, even mare so vith the<br />

furnishings <strong>of</strong> the house.<br />

Jenclter: I don't thick he's the boss, b?canse<br />

I never<br />

either.<br />

thought <strong>of</strong> ~~yself as being the boss<br />

Interviewer: What do you think <strong>of</strong> 502'5 notion<br />

that if he's making the aoney, he's a notch<br />

up on you?<br />

Jennifer: Oh, that's his idea.<br />

Joe: Well. I think when. ..anybcdy does<br />

something to assume responsibility in a<br />

specific area, there is sort <strong>of</strong> a raising <strong>of</strong><br />

him there in euthority in that area. ?ha:'s<br />

211 I'm trying to get at. Because I w i l l be<br />

the sole breadvinner, my authority w i l l go up<br />

slightly.<br />

Irterviever: So your authority is going up hsre<br />

and Jenrifer's is going dovn here. [I<br />

motioned vith my hands to indicate two<br />

aitterent levels. 3<br />

Jecnifsr: I'd still work on that one... It's<br />

still going to work th+t mine vill go up<br />

there. [Translation: I "ill still have 3s<br />

much authorzty as he has.] Re thinks that ray<br />

[bur I know better].<br />

InfErvLevar: Do you believe he's the boss<br />

b4C2~59 hers the breadvrrrer?<br />

Jennifer: 60 can tslisue it if he wants.<br />

Interviever: What do you think he believes?<br />

Jennifer: I thlnk he's more <strong>of</strong> a hsusehclder.<br />

Thaz's a better word.


Ch.10. Havec't Bad Any Problems Since page 168<br />

Joe: Yeah. I get stepped upon!<br />

[Laughter].. .for example, it there shoula be<br />

a prowler in the house and they had a gun, I<br />

would probably assume responsibility in that<br />

circumstance because I'm in charge sf the<br />

weapons. and I'm the more physical, violect<br />

personality? [Laughzer] So I would take mars<br />

rfsponsibili~y 1n that circumstance bscause I<br />

am more knovledgeable. Now if she was<br />

gung-ho on guns, I'd say. "Here, you go<br />

downstairs..." [Laughter] That sort <strong>of</strong> thing.<br />

In times <strong>of</strong> emergency I take over... We each<br />

assume our own responsibilities<br />

area.<br />

i C our our<br />

Intervlever: But you're going to be head <strong>of</strong> the<br />

household.<br />

Joe: Yeah. [Laughter] I like the way you said<br />

that!<br />

Inter~iewer: what does it mean to be head <strong>of</strong><br />

the household?<br />

Jennifer: It neens nothicg. [Laughter]<br />

Joe: This is what it means. It means nsthirg,<br />

but when a job is botched up, the buck stops<br />

here. That's what it means! [Laughter] ...<br />

What I'm saying is if I the sole<br />

breadwinner. I think over a period <strong>of</strong> time<br />

I'll be feeling more authority in specific<br />

areas doe to the Circumstance that I'm<br />

familiar with. If she should get a job, my<br />

responsibility as sole breadwinner woold have<br />

to go down. And hers w i l l start t3 rise.<br />

She's the on9 who's going to be in contact<br />

with the kid more time than I an, so 1'8<br />

going to have tc lean over and say. "OK,<br />

she's the boss when it comes TO taking care<br />

<strong>of</strong> and making decisions about this little<br />

kid." See what I'm gstting at? Because this<br />

turns the area <strong>of</strong> responsibility, bscause<br />

she's more in touch with it. and s3 Jn and s3<br />

forth. So what happens is that there are<br />

many areas <strong>of</strong> responsibility. So at any<br />

given pclnt in time, you assuse "boss if the<br />

caz.'> "boss <strong>of</strong> the ueapons," "boss for hone<br />

defense." "boss for beirg breadwinner.' 'boss<br />

tor heavy manna1 labor," "boss for repairs<br />

and rep1aceaents.n "boss over the tool^.^<br />

Her--"boss for childbearing, childcariny,<br />

food, shopping, household decisions*...I was<br />

just trying to explain that because I an<br />

earning the money solsly that I probably w i l l


ch.10. Aaven't Bad Any Problems Since PagE 169<br />

end up having nore decision making pouer in<br />

that area.<br />

Jennlter: Yeah. but I know hov to handle it<br />

more.<br />

<strong>The</strong>rE 2re a number <strong>of</strong> things worth noting in the above<br />

sequence. Plrst <strong>of</strong> all, Joe's claim is bound to run irto<br />

trouble from the start. Ee is atteapting to argue that<br />

since he is the breadwinner, he is to hare "the say" in<br />

financial Batters. Given their sensitivity to money<br />

handling. Jennifer's fical reply, "Yeah, but I know how to<br />

handle it [money] more [because I'm a bookkeeper and you're<br />

an abstract accountant]," is predictable. SBcordly,<br />

although Joe elaborates on his claim by arguing that<br />

responsibility Implies authority, and that Jennifer w i l l ,<br />

because <strong>of</strong> her responsibilities, be "boss" <strong>of</strong> Some areas<br />

too, the tact is soae areas have more veight than others.<br />

<strong>The</strong> area in this household which carries the most veight is<br />

the flnancial area. Joe knovs this, but tben so does<br />

J~rnlfer. She retusfs to give Joe's claim validity by<br />

derying rt access to tCEir world <strong>of</strong> conseosull rules ("Oh.<br />

that's his idea..." "Re cen believe it if he wants..." "It<br />

means nothing") .<br />

Toward the end <strong>of</strong> the pregnancy, it appeared Chat Joe<br />

had not given up on an ideological claim to power, that he<br />

in fact uould resort to both ideology and resources to<br />

support his po~er play. By the fourth intsrvieu, Jennifer<br />

had quit work and was trying to adapt to being a housewife.<br />

It was difficult for her. She took a great deal <strong>of</strong> pride in<br />

th9 work she had dons, the books she had set<br />

department she supposedly had straightened out.<br />

up, the<br />

when she<br />

left, everything in the department started to "fsll apart."<br />

<strong>The</strong> person who took JennifErqs place didn't want to learn<br />

what to do, so they claimed, and as a consequence Jennifer<br />

had been called a number <strong>of</strong> times to give sssistarce over<br />

the phone. <strong>The</strong> whole affair provokfd a conflict betveen Joe<br />

and Jsnnifer. In spite <strong>of</strong> Jennifer's attachment to her<br />

previous job, Joe wanzed her to "let go." He was actually<br />

quite vehemen; about it. H i s threat <strong>of</strong> rhat he uould do "if<br />

they [Jennifez's former co-uorksrs] call up" is intsresting.<br />

A9 says he is goi3g tc "act like a father" when he tells<br />

:hem t3 stop call:,g. Is it his father he is going to act<br />

like. his Either<br />

for Jce? Erd for<br />

the patriarch who maae all the decisions<br />

vhon is he acting--JenciferVs former<br />

co-workers or Jenr~ifer, herself?<br />

Joe: ... If they call up here, I ' m going to get<br />

on the phone and act llke a father. Bnd 1's<br />

golng 70 tell them, "Hey, you'd better hold<br />

up now, and if you call once acre, I'm going<br />

to punch you lr the mouth." and I'n goirg to<br />

hang up an them. End I know they are going<br />

to bother her. I don't want that to happen.


Ch.10. Haven't Had Iny Prcblems Since Pags 170<br />

Joe once said that he felt the pregnancy made him more<br />

<strong>of</strong> a man and Jernifsr more <strong>of</strong> a mornan. Perhaps what he<br />

neat by this is that fir:ally he can draw that line as his<br />

father drew the line. whether Jennifer w i l l be able to<br />

continue to resist remains to be seen.<br />

CONJUGAL VIOLENCE AND THE POL<strong>IT</strong>ICS OF NARRIAGE<br />

Joe and JenEifer's relationship appears to be bullt on<br />

rntarpersonal conflict--"an inconparible 8iffersccE <strong>of</strong><br />

objective . . . a desire cn the part <strong>of</strong> both Contestants to<br />

attain Ghat is available only to one, or only in partw<br />

(Dahrendori, 1959:135). <strong>The</strong> conflict between them may be<br />

viewed in essentially oce <strong>of</strong> two ways-as a sign <strong>of</strong> a<br />

"disturbed" or '#sick" relationship, or as a normal<br />

COnSEquenCe cf the marital bond. <strong>The</strong> first conception, the<br />

consensus approach. assumes that the husband-wlfe union is a<br />

r;lationship based an agreement and homeostasis. Thf second<br />

concepticn, the conflict approach, assumes that dissensus<br />

and struggles for power are inherent qualities <strong>of</strong> a<br />

marriage. <strong>The</strong> first conception is the popular one.<br />

muever, the validity <strong>of</strong> this conception has been questioned<br />

in many <strong>of</strong> the chapters <strong>of</strong> this book and by sociologists<br />

such as sprey. sprey (1969). for example, argues that the<br />

~o~sensus approach is based on two fallacies: (1) the<br />

bellEf that participation in the family is a voluntary<br />

matter. and (2) the notior that the family is a buffer<br />

berween the individual and society.<br />

TO dispnte these claims, Sprep notes first that<br />

membership in one's rats1 family is obviously not by choice,<br />

and that there is no real normative alternarive t~ the<br />

marhied state as a life career in our society, and secondly<br />

that conceiving the family as a world into vhich one may<br />

withdraw from the conflicts <strong>of</strong> everyday life erronsously<br />

assumes that individuals are somehow apart froa rathsr than<br />

involved in socisty. A conflict approach is <strong>of</strong> course not<br />

new to social scierce (cf.,Robbes. narx, Simmel). For some<br />

reason, however, it has traditionally been resezved for<br />

explainma macro level relations (for example the class<br />

struggle) end norfamilla1 oicro encounters (the presectetion<br />

<strong>of</strong> self in everyday life). <strong>The</strong> approach is just b?ginning<br />

to gain slgnificart suppcrt among family researchers.<br />

Presenred here as an isolated case, Jos and Jennifer's<br />

story may cams across as a docoasnt <strong>of</strong> a "disLurbsd"<br />

relationship. Constdered ic conjunction with the other<br />

conple~ i= the largar study froa which this case study was<br />

taken, however, Joe and Jennifers's marital experience does<br />

not seem that orusnzl. <strong>The</strong> fact is all the sanple couples,<br />

in one way or another and in varying degrees, were involved<br />

in an ongoing interpersonal confrontation. What about Joe's<br />

violence toward Jennlfer dnrirg the first two years <strong>of</strong> their


ch.10. Raven't Bad hny Problems Since Page 171<br />

,,,arriago? Is this "usual?" It is true that <strong>of</strong> the sixteen<br />

couples in my sample only two disclcsed a Violent ~ncounter<br />

(the other couple also said the vife uas the victim;<br />

ssesed to be a precipitator). Not t3 be overl~oked<br />

boxever is the fact that recent mquiries into the incidence<br />

~f taaily violence suggest that conjugal violence may irdeea<br />

be a usual occurrezce (see Chzptsrs 1 and 2). <strong>The</strong> research<br />

indicates that Joe ard Jsnniferrs marriage may be sore<br />

typtcal than we realize, or would like to believa.<br />

Recen? attsapts to Explain family violence have. in<br />

fact. resulted in the dsvelopment <strong>of</strong> theories uhich<br />

coapleaenf the conflict approach. <strong>The</strong> conceptualizations<br />

proposed by Allsr. and stzaus (Chapter 1%) and by Brown<br />

(Chaprer 11) are cases in point. Each <strong>of</strong> these theories<br />

sees husband-wife violence as a consequence <strong>of</strong> an intsrplap<br />

bBtYeen cultural and social structural variables rather than<br />

psychopathological factors. Implicit in eich is the<br />

assertion that social conflict is t9$2 the<br />

organization <strong>of</strong> the corjugal system and <strong>of</strong> the larger<br />

society vith uhich that system transacts.<br />

Drawing from the work <strong>of</strong> Goode (lY7l), Rodman (19721.<br />

and RO~ETS (1379).<br />

nUlt5rnate Resource"<br />

Blleo ard Straus' chapter proposes th4<br />

theory <strong>of</strong> conjugal violsnce.<br />

Esse~tially the theory suggests that in an<br />

individUallsticallp oriented urban-industrial society (for<br />

example the United States) where male superiority norms are<br />

weak and somewhat ambiguous and where the pr?sumption <strong>of</strong><br />

male saperiority must be validated by<br />

"resources" (such as material goods and<br />

sup?riari:y ic<br />

valued person51<br />

traits). violence w i l l be invoked by an indiviaual who lacks<br />

other resources to serve as a basis for power. In other<br />

words, 9iolence may be understood as ths "ultimate ressurce"<br />

for sustaining a power claim. Ellen arc7 Straus' theory<br />

<strong>of</strong>fsrs a cogent explanation <strong>of</strong> why Joe resorted to violenco<br />

to stop what he saw as "the bad progression <strong>of</strong> events." &:<br />

the time <strong>of</strong> the encounter, Joe lacked the resources to<br />

ground his patriarchal ideology ("I'm a pure male chauvinist<br />

pig, and 1111 admit it"]. Economically supported by<br />

Jennifer, he could not rely on any "extrinsic" ressurces<br />

(sccnomic acd prestige conffrricg characteristics) is a<br />

basis for power. Purthezmore, Jennifer implies that Joe's<br />

..-ansic" resources (valued personal traits) mre also low<br />

during thli time when she remarks later on that Joe seemed<br />

to be "getting more ind~pendent...sore self-confident* as a<br />

result <strong>of</strong> his (now) being with "business people" rath4r than<br />

"sf~fi4nls."fU G ~ Y E<br />

ideology v%rsus vife<br />

~ ttis iebalance--husband d0minar.t<br />

dominant resourcp structure (st<br />

mbalance that Allsn and Straus see as characteristic <strong>of</strong> t h<br />

worklng class)--ope would expect (assuming the validity <strong>of</strong><br />

the "Ultimate Resourcen theory) that Joe weald resort to 3<br />

Slap in the face "three or four times" to show Jennifer that<br />

desplte his lack <strong>of</strong> "extrinsic' and "irtrirsic" resources,<br />

hr is "not the kind <strong>of</strong> person that's [sic] going to be<br />

~


Ch.10. Haven't Aad hny Problems Since Page 172<br />

dominated."<br />

Joe's threat <strong>of</strong> violence during the final reeks <strong>of</strong> the<br />

pregnancy may perhaps also be interpreted within the Rlles<br />

and Straos framework. Was Joe threatening Jenniferqs<br />

co-workers ("If you call once more, I'm going to punch you<br />

in the mouth") 31. was he in fact indirectly threatening<br />

Jennifer? A s long as Jennifer held on to her job, Joe could<br />

not base a claim for power on his greater economic position.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Education versus experience debats makas it clear that<br />

Jennifer did not see her occupational status as below Joe's.<br />

Was he commuLicating to Jennifer that if her career was<br />

goirg to contilue to interfere he would have n2 choice but<br />

to fall back on the "ultimate resource" to show her he<br />

intEnded to be "boss?"<br />

Joe and Jennifer also illustrate the processes<br />

describes in Brown's chapter on "8ife Bnplopmant, flarital<br />

Equality, and Rusband-<strong>Wife</strong> <strong>Violence</strong>" (Chapter 11). Brov~<br />

notes the increase in the number <strong>of</strong> working wives in Ao~rica<br />

and the corresponding decrease in husband pou%r that this<br />

change implies ("Since money is a resource in marriage, ve<br />

may assume that as more wives contributed ts the family<br />

income the paver <strong>of</strong> the husband was affected inversaly").<br />

- Brown - devsloDs - ~ a thearv that "traces out ~ . the known and<br />

~ - - ~ ~ ~ - - L<br />

- ~~~ -<br />

theorstical consequences <strong>of</strong> vife employment and seeks to<br />

answer the question or whether one <strong>of</strong> the consequences <strong>of</strong><br />

uife ~mplopment is an increased level af husband-wife<br />

conflict and violence."<br />

<strong>The</strong> argument is made that one consequence <strong>of</strong> the<br />

increased economic ildspandencs <strong>of</strong> the uife is a change in<br />

her authority<br />

equalitarian.<br />

expectations<br />

Since the<br />

from<br />

male<br />

husband dominant to<br />

has traaitionally been<br />

ascribed superior statas in our society, this attitude shift<br />

leads to a conflict between the wife's equalitarian<br />

authority expectatiocs and the husband's male superiority<br />

norms. <strong>The</strong> critical question from Brounls point <strong>of</strong> view is<br />

"Does the<br />

strocture?'<br />

husband accept<br />

If the answer<br />

the equalitarian<br />

to this question<br />

authority<br />

is n2, the<br />

conflict w l l l be seen as illegitimate and the husband vill<br />

revert to trzditio~al domicating techniques increasing the<br />

likelihood <strong>of</strong> violent conflict. If the answer to the<br />

question is yes, the intimacy in the couple's relationship<br />

w i l l increasn (owing to shared aspects <strong>of</strong> life) a2d this<br />

increase w i l l in turn iscrease the conflict in the marriage.<br />

("Increasing intimacy brings with it an increasing awareness<br />

cf, and confronta%on urth, the uniqueness <strong>of</strong> the othsr."<br />

sprey, 1971r729.) 59vevar. the conflict brought on by the<br />

Increased irtimacy w i l l be seer as legitimate-that is,<br />

Constructive and highly desirable--decreasing ths likslihood<br />

<strong>of</strong> violent conflict. Whether Jennifer's economic<br />

independence prompted a change in her authority expectations<br />

is not clear. She may have held equalitarian or vife<br />

dominant expectations Epye she became a bsokkeeper. Be


ch.10. Haven't Had Iny Problems Since Page 173<br />

that as it may. th~re is no doubt that Jennifer's ideas on<br />

who should be in charge conflicted with Joe's traditional<br />

thoughts on the subject. Confronted with the corflict<br />

between Jennifer's authority expectations, and his own male<br />

superiority norms, how does Joe react? 9e continually<br />

refuses to accept the authority structure which Jennifer<br />

proposes. It could be said (assuming the validity <strong>of</strong><br />

nraun's thearvl ~ -- ~- that Joe's slao was one manifestation <strong>of</strong> his<br />

~ . ~~- ~ ~ . - ~ ~- -~<br />

~~<br />

resorting to traditional dominating techniques vhen his<br />

superiority was threatened.<br />

Parricularly significant is the neaning which Joz and<br />

Jennifer impute to the violent incident. Both see the<br />

encounter as an act that was appropriate, given the<br />

situation (Joe: "1 had to do something physical ..."<br />

Jencifer: "It was my fault.") <strong>The</strong>ir responses provide an<br />

example ot what Gelles (1974:59) calls "normal<br />

~io1encE"--violence that is accepted, approved, and even<br />

mandated in family interaction. <strong>The</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> this type<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence is yet another demonstration <strong>of</strong> how social<br />

conflict can be built into the orgacizatiox <strong>of</strong> a Barrisge.<br />

It is Lotevorthy that marital violence is being<br />

conceptualized by these researchers as an act that has both<br />

ideological and social structural antecedents. <strong>The</strong>ir dual<br />

~CCUS 1s ir accord vlth sy conclusions ao the conflict<br />

approach. Specifically, the conflict approach suggested by<br />

the intezvieus does not imply simply an exchange framework.<br />

Rather, the conflicr process emerges as a system*5 in which<br />

the marital symbol structure (convertional sign structure)<br />

a5 well as the marital exchange structure (rssource<br />

structure) influence. ar.d are influenced by, the marital<br />

pover srrucrure (the ability <strong>of</strong> the husband to affect<br />

marital life versus the zbility <strong>of</strong> the wife to affect<br />

marltal life). In other words, marital politics (the<br />

distribution +nd exercise <strong>of</strong> power) is not based simply on<br />

ideology (for example. ths husband is in charge b~cause<br />

that's "the vay it's supposed to be") or on exchange (the<br />

husband is ir charge because he is bringing to the marriage<br />

rewards--money. status--that satisfy his vifa), but on a<br />

sythesis <strong>of</strong> the tuo.*6


Ch.10. ilaven't Had Rny Problems Since Page 174<br />

NOTES<br />

*I am indebted to Baureen LaRossa and Howard 8.<br />

Shapiro for their helpful comments or an earlier draft <strong>of</strong><br />

this chapter. This excerpt is reprinted from Conflict and<br />

pQvEr La earrraae: asc$Lrg f& p=rz$ BLLP by Ralph<br />

Laxossa. Sage Library <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Research, Vol. 50. (c)<br />

1977, pp. 69-82 by pfrmission <strong>of</strong> the Publisher. Sage<br />

Publrcations. Inc. (Beverly Eills/London) .<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> research is essentially two studips il one.<br />

manifestly, it is a study <strong>of</strong> haw married couplas respond to<br />

the first pregnancy--to the transition to parenthood. nore<br />

important, and at a higher level <strong>of</strong> abstraction, it is a<br />

study <strong>of</strong> the structure and pheoomenologp <strong>of</strong> the husband-wife<br />

relationship. Sixteen married couples vere interviewed<br />

during the twelfth, trentieth, tvecty-eighth, ttirty-sixth<br />

weeks ot their respective first pregnancies. <strong>The</strong> interviews<br />

vere conjolnt (husband and wife together) and unstructured<br />

(nonstacdardized) . <strong>The</strong>y were conducted in the couples'<br />

homes, were taped and later transcribed. Analysis <strong>of</strong> the<br />

interne" transcipts was qualitative (the conceptual<br />

components ot explanation vere developed. for the most part.<br />

from the data).<br />

2. Sprey (1969) cites research that he feels<br />

"chronicles" or "illustrates" the conflict framework (Bach<br />

and Qydsc, 1968; Brim a&.. 1961; xaukins, 1968: Levis,<br />

1967; Scanzonl. 1968). To these one could add Larson<br />

(1974) and Rausch g . (1974). Each <strong>of</strong> these<br />

investigators does <strong>of</strong>fer some finding(s) that may be<br />

interpretEd as support for the conflict approach. None <strong>of</strong><br />

these studies. however. confronts the major assumptions <strong>of</strong><br />

the conflict approach. In other words, none addresses the<br />

question <strong>of</strong> how conflict is intrinsic to family life, or how<br />

families manage rather than resolve conflicts. lone <strong>of</strong><br />

these studies focuses on the political dimension, the<br />

nucleus. ir. my opinion, <strong>of</strong> the family as a conflict system.<br />

<strong>of</strong> the studies cited, Bach and uyden (1968) perhaps comes<br />

the closest to addressing these issues. <strong>The</strong>ir rssearch is,<br />

howevEr, based or clinlcal impressions.<br />

3. Joe's reference to "the people in this area" is<br />

irteresting. Bosn and raised in rural New England. Joe<br />

seems to be claiming that there exists a geagraphically<br />

based subculture and that he is part <strong>of</strong> it.<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> idsntiticatio~ <strong>of</strong> 'extrinsic' ar~a "intrinsic'<br />

rssources is from Blau (1964:20-22).<br />

5. <strong>The</strong> definition <strong>of</strong> a system subscribed t3 is<br />

Buckley's (1968:493). "Re define a system in general as a<br />

complex <strong>of</strong> elements or components directly or irairectly<br />

related in a causal network, such that at least soma 3f the<br />

components are related to some others in a nore or 155s


Ch.70. Xaven't Had Any Problems since Page 175<br />

stable way at af.p 23 GE, <strong>The</strong> interrelations may be<br />

matual or unidirectional, linear, non-linear O r<br />

in?-rrniAtent. and - ~ ~ varvixo - . , in d64rees <strong>of</strong> causal efficacy or<br />

prisrizy. <strong>The</strong> particular kinds -<strong>of</strong> more or less stable<br />

interrelationships or components that become esrablLshed at<br />

any time constitute the particular s=us;s"f<br />

at that time."<br />

the systel<br />

6. Sprey (1972:237) makes the point that a conflic?<br />

approach "implies a framework <strong>of</strong> exchange." He does not<br />

explain rhat he meaxs by this. If however he is saying that<br />

cognitive sociology (symbolic interaction, phanoaanslogy)<br />

has no place within a conflict approach, then I must<br />

disagree. In my opinion (and I believe family violence<br />

r~searchers would concur with me on this), a more<br />

appropriate way o? stating the cass is that a conflict<br />

approach implies politics (the distribution and arercise <strong>of</strong><br />

power), and polit~cs entails not only the ability to affect<br />

reilfoscenent contingencies (exchange) but also the ability<br />

to affect the defirltio7. <strong>of</strong> the situation (symbols). For a<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> this issue as it applies to the stuay <strong>of</strong><br />

marital power, see Safilias-Rothschild (1971).


ife-Employment, Marital Equality,<br />

and <strong>Husband</strong>-<strong>Wife</strong> <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Bruce W. Brown<br />

A theme in the previous chapter that<br />

pervaded the dialogue between Joe and Jecnifer<br />

was the issue cf wife employment. In this<br />

chapter. Frown suggests that on2 consequence <strong>of</strong><br />

V~IES VOrkiPq is an increased level <strong>of</strong><br />

husband-wife c<strong>of</strong>flic? and violence. <strong>The</strong><br />

increased econDmic independence <strong>of</strong> the married<br />

woman changes her auChority expectations from<br />

husb3nd dominant to equalitarian, ana leads to s<br />

conrllct betUEPn the wife's FEY authori'y<br />

expectations md *he husband's male superilriry<br />

norms.<br />

A crucial cocticgency in Brown's<br />

formulation is whether the husband accepts the<br />

equalitariac authority sTructure. If not,<br />

attempts to disrupt his power claims may be mst<br />

with violent rssistzncs, especially if he is<br />

lacking in other resources. Ic the case <strong>of</strong> Joe<br />

+nd Jennifer, Jsnnlfer's actempt to alter the<br />

zuthority structure <strong>of</strong> tb=ir marTiZgE caus~d J?E<br />

to use violecce.<br />

1 forther con+ribution <strong>of</strong> Brcun's chaptsr<br />

15 his irvestigatlon <strong>of</strong> the factzrs ?haL<br />

~nfluence the husband's acceptance or rejectiob<br />

<strong>of</strong> equelitarian 2uAh3r5'y pafzerns. la ?b.=<br />

reader w i l l aots. many <strong>of</strong> ths factors he list5<br />

are bourd up in aais sex role Expectarions that<br />

le3Ve men unprepared far dovelopin3<br />

relatiocstips the? call for shared pnvEr an3<br />

equal task allocation wiLh women.


Research on family pover has emphasized :he hscsficial<br />

effects <strong>of</strong> the equalitaria? marriage style. It has been<br />

heralded in the popalar culture as a cure-all for *h3<br />

ailments <strong>of</strong> contemporary sarriages. Aovever, others such as<br />

straus in Chapter 6. Kolb and Straus (1974). and whitshurst<br />

(1974) who have studied the Emerging equalit3rizn sarriag-<br />

style concluded that, vhcterer the ultimate benefits. Suricg<br />

the transition period, the move toward equality betveen ths<br />

sexes may cause many problms.<br />

Such a chanae toward "re sexual equality in the<br />

fcmily, like any other drastic charge ir th?<br />

soclal structure, poses problems, at least<br />

duzicg the trarsrtion periaa. Inaividuals<br />

socialized +.o operate in 028 systsm <strong>of</strong> family<br />

organization m3y have difficulty opsrst5ng under<br />

new stazd~rds (Rclb and Stnaus. 1974:756).<br />

<strong>The</strong> focus <strong>of</strong> this chepter is en explanatisn <strong>of</strong> c7.e c<br />

the factors that led Whitehurst (197U:76) to sugger<br />

"...that the cocflict bstueen the emsrging equalitarFe<br />

social strncture znd the cortinuing male-sup9ricrity noro<br />

w i l l tend tc increase rather thac dccreass conflict a1<br />

violence betyeen husbands End wives." This chspt-r ?rat€<br />

out the known and theorezical consequences c<br />

wife-saploymsnt and ccnsiders the question <strong>of</strong> whether one c<br />

the consequences <strong>of</strong> wife-cmploympct is an increased level c<br />

husband-ulEf corflict ard violence.<br />

LEG<strong>IT</strong>IBRTION OF SER<strong>IT</strong>BL POWER<br />

Blood and Wolfe (1960:ll) think that no change ic the<br />

American faaily is more significant thac the shift from<br />

ore-sided male authority to the. sharing <strong>of</strong> pover Prd/or<br />

authority by the husband aad uife. P~YCT and aothoritp are<br />

i~volved in przctically ?very aspect <strong>of</strong> merriage.*l PS Kolb<br />

and straur; (1974:757) state:<br />

... the degrse <strong>of</strong> control ~xercised by family<br />

memb~rs over ace arather is an Element <strong>of</strong> family<br />

s:=ucture affected by 8x6 ir turn effacting macy<br />

other aspects cf family integrstiln aod<br />

~nterzct~cr.<br />

Blood and Wolfe's (1960:12) analysis c9ntras:i tv?<br />

so~rces <strong>of</strong> power ir 'ha oarital r%l3+ionship: "culture ana<br />

conpetnncs." Briefly. their cultural Explanation <strong>of</strong> pacer<br />

stetss that Dover lies in the ha-ds <strong>of</strong> the partner "ha the<br />

culture dictit;s should have that pouPr. Io this case,<br />

Elood and Uolfe zpphrently are specking <strong>of</strong> iuthority, the<br />

right to exercise power. In our own culture, the popular<br />

vifv has beer that "the men shoula be the hes3 <strong>of</strong> th?<br />

house."


Ch. 11. Ulfe-Fmploymert Psg= 178<br />

FnPLOYnENT BND POWER<br />

flacy researchers think thet no other factor has<br />

affected the balance <strong>of</strong> pcuer in Amsricar marriages mcrc<br />

than the increase in ?he number <strong>of</strong> vorkicg viv%s. klaous<br />

(1974:232), for example. says that:<br />

... with 65.7 percent <strong>of</strong> aarrind vomec presecrly<br />

engaged ir gainful employment. incluaing 20.6<br />

percent <strong>of</strong> uown with childrec undcr six years<br />

<strong>of</strong> age, people nc longer are willing to accapi<br />

the rorms legitimating and prrscribirq s<br />

segregated conjugal role organizstior.<br />

AcCordiLg to the rescurce th~ory <strong>of</strong> pcvEr ?he increased<br />

res3uxes <strong>of</strong> working wivss hav~ important implicatiors for<br />

marital power and for ideology concerning marital au'h3riiy.<br />

Sicce socey is a resource in marriage, as the wife's<br />

resources increase. her pow== prssueably also increases and<br />

she is in a better bargaining position to suggest or demand<br />

a more equalitarian +u?hori+y strocture.<br />

LS the casa <strong>of</strong> Joe and Jennifer illustrates, this<br />

transition to a more FqU3l sharing <strong>of</strong> anthority apps3rs to<br />

be particularly difficult for husbacds. PC Icubt, many<br />

COPt~mpOrery husbands ard wires vi?v 'he wife's rrplcymert<br />

es a necessary income supplement. This, however, do?s no?<br />

guaEartee thet husbends w i l l find some <strong>of</strong> the l'u:.ictirded<br />

corsequances" <strong>of</strong> w~fe-employment easy '3 %&just to. Purks<br />

and W21r (1976:28Q). for example, t3ur.d that husbands <strong>of</strong><br />

working wives were less s?tiefiad w i t h =hair n3rriZ"ns *tr2<br />

husbands <strong>of</strong> nonworking uivss. FurT.hermore, a?er pr<strong>of</strong>cmirist<br />

me? seem to prefer 3rfamily woman" to 'career womer." (Stapp<br />

and Pines, 1976). <strong>The</strong> reasor for husbacds' difficulty wiih<br />

a2 equal sharirg <strong>of</strong> authority is essentially a csrflicl<br />

betueec +he two bases <strong>of</strong> pover just discussed--cultural<br />

norms 2nd individual resources. In terns <strong>of</strong> individual<br />

reso~rces, employed Y~VEC shonld share in the au'horitv<br />

structure at ?h+ mirriage. ~ovever, th= populsr culture and<br />

the legal system (veitzman, 1975) tall husbands just <strong>The</strong>


Opposite. Kclb and S+raus (197r1:761) cite a recent Barris<br />

Poll to supporr their statement that "both in law ?nd in<br />

popular opilioc, the husbcnd is still expactsa to bs the<br />

tamrly 'head' or leader."<br />

WIPE-EBPLOYBZNT AND TEE CAELLSAGE TO XSCRIBSD<br />

EELE BUTAOR<strong>IT</strong>Y<br />

<strong>The</strong> remalrder ot this chapter is concerned vtth en<br />

~xplaratior <strong>of</strong> the hypothesized relationship bsLueen<br />

wife-employment ard husbard-wife niolencs. "Violent-," as<br />

=sea in this chapter, refers to @?ys:cg&<br />

h~5b2nd6 and wives.<br />

violence bgtweer<br />

Ue begx vitk the prcpasitian rhat smplaymant <strong>of</strong> the<br />

wrfe provides her with increased resourc9s. <strong>The</strong>se ranaurces<br />

12 turn lead to increased power. In the majority <strong>of</strong><br />

stndies, power 232% been corstrued as decisior-aakirg and +he<br />

two ccncepts have come to bs UP& interchangeably<br />

(satilias-Pothschild, 1970). B numbex <strong>of</strong> these studFes<br />

tomd that xarklng wives exert nore influence in<br />

de~is:o~-rn+kl~g.*2 With =his shift toward scre equal pover<br />

between husbard icd vife, the wit? comes to expect a change<br />

to squaiitarian authority (Blood and Bamblir, 1958). To the<br />

extent thar this change occurs, wife-saplcyment may lsid to<br />

an urdermrning <strong>of</strong> ascribed male authority pltrercs. RF<br />

scznzor.i (19'lo:lss) statss, "<strong>The</strong> wife is more m0tivat.a ?o<br />

'go along' with him, to 'give in* to him, to let 'his hare<br />

hLs way' to the extent that he provraes maximue zc3nonic<br />

rewards.' If the husband 1.5 c3 laager provisinq nzximum<br />

economic reuerds, because the vife is now involved tc %His<br />

funct:~~. she 1s cot i5 irclined "'5 go zlonq with him."<br />

Eomans (1961:287) put this idea more ger.frally in '.he<br />

propo~itior that "the most important single 53ctoz in rn8kir.g<br />

a man a laader rs...the ability to provide rare an8 valued<br />

rewards for his followers." If a husband c0 lcnger p ~ 3 0 F d ~ ~<br />

rara and valued rewards for his "tollouer," Sectus- she<br />

herself DOV pmv:des %hem. the husband no longer is in a<br />

position tc be the ucquestionsd leader. <strong>The</strong> result is an<br />

attempt to shift from a dominant-subrissive to an<br />

equalitariac authority structurs within the marriage.


Ch.11. w~fe-zmploymepi Page 180<br />

norms. Empirical data shcv the existence <strong>of</strong> such conflic's.<br />

Specifically, Gianopulos and aitchell (1957). Bye (1958 and<br />

1963). acd Glilzer-Balbir (1975:Table 7) found thzt rn-rital<br />

conflict is more frequent emcrg couples in uhich the vlfe is<br />

employed. As stated e~rlifr, Whitehurst thinks 'hst ths<br />

conflict betweex the emerging equalitarian authority<br />

strncture and male superiority norms uiLl inzreasa<br />

husband-wife violence. he remainder <strong>of</strong> this chapt~r aeals<br />

with the conditions under uhich Uhitehurst's asserfi?n may<br />

be correct.<br />

FACTORS INFLUENCING EUSBAND ACCEPTANCE OF<br />

THE EQUAL<strong>IT</strong>PRIAN AUTHOR<strong>IT</strong>Y STRUCTURE<br />

<strong>The</strong> husband's acceptaxce or rejection <strong>of</strong> 'he<br />

equalltar-an authcrlty structure is clntingect upon a cumber<br />

<strong>of</strong> variables. <strong>The</strong> PX&SFP? thecry deals with f~ur <strong>of</strong> those<br />

variables:<br />

1. degree cf compulsive masculinity<br />

2. degree <strong>of</strong> anticipafory socializatioc<br />

3. degr49 <strong>of</strong> role clarity<br />

4. degree +C which the transition facilit3tes g3al<br />

attainment.<br />

C&"E~&~XB nasculinit~. Pars3ns (1947) sees<br />

"~O~pulsi'r~ masculinity" as the result <strong>of</strong> the effort 2n the<br />

pert <strong>of</strong> man to assert their masculinity and x~pudiate a<br />

~ainr8.l ider:ification with their mothers.<br />

ccmpuisive miscolirlty syrdro~e have bsen<br />

Eleaents sf the<br />

analyzid by a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> other sociolcgists. For example, Selsuick and<br />

Peek (1971) think that Ewericar msa<br />

tough aPd to avoid shovirg emotions.<br />

are scci3lized to be<br />

Evidence f3r the ideal<br />

<strong>of</strong> toughness Tn men is supplied in a Canadiac survey<br />

conducted by Goldfarb (1970. as cited in Uhit?hurst,<br />

1974:80). ir which 61 percent <strong>of</strong> ths rpspondeots felt men<br />

should ba tough and nct back away from a ftght. Ross<br />

(19721, in a top-sslectior study, colfirsed the erarmous<br />

pressures by family and society fo: boys to behave lr. the<br />

accepted masculine way. Uhen bops were giver. s toy<br />

appropriate for the opposiCe sex, they shoved acxiety,<br />

l~dicatlng that an inappropriate toy u3uld mean<br />

embarrassment and possibly pur.ishment.<br />

One aspect cf the wacc~p~ed mssc~li~? my" is fcr<br />

hn~b~cds to be domznant cver their wives. Fsr many<br />

husbards, aL equal sharirg <strong>of</strong> authority vlthin the marriags<br />

may be viewed 2s a "lack <strong>of</strong> masculinity#* on their part.<br />

are moving toward an age o: eouality between the sex?=,<br />

We<br />

and<br />

cur difficulty in aeklng this tzarsirlox depelds in part on<br />

the degres cf compulsive masculinity exhibited<br />

in our society.<br />

by husbands


Ch.11. Wlfe-E~ployment Pagr 181<br />

------ B?.ticiEtm so&cii;=gy:op. Anticipatory s3cializz?ion<br />

is defined as thl adoption <strong>of</strong> the norms and salups <strong>of</strong> ? roli<br />

before being in a social situztion "hers it is apprcpriats<br />

(Berton, 1957:265 and Burr, 1973:125). <strong>Husband</strong>s ir Rm?rican<br />

soci9ty have not received much anticipatory s3cializ2tian<br />

for an equal shening <strong>of</strong> mazilal auth3rity. In fact. just<br />

the opposite seems to be trus <strong>of</strong> Smerican boys, who hold<br />

~ ~ ~ attitudes 1 s t by t h ~ t01?d.fr age <strong>of</strong> two (Kuhn, 1976).<br />

Eovever, tc the extsnt that a reu and more egualLtarian<br />

marital ideology permeates the culture through ths mass<br />

media, t h temlnist ~ movem~nt, and family ard sea aducatioc<br />

in the schccls, men w i l l find it easier to accapt this new<br />

marital authority structure uher faced with the Lssu4 il<br />

thFiT 3 % marriage. ~<br />

- Role -- - Clara. ---- Cattrell (1992:6181 defircs rcle clarity<br />

as 'the degree to which thara :s a set <strong>of</strong> explicit<br />

definl~ions <strong>of</strong> t h ~ reciprocal behavi3r expected." Bs<br />

mentioned preuiously, t h trarsition ~<br />

to equalitarim marital<br />

authority structures is a difficult one. espscially for<br />

husbands. Komarovsky (1973) tound that althouqh males aqres<br />

verbally vith equality betveen tho sexes, *hiy are still<br />

attached emotionally to treditianal attitudes and p3ttercs<br />

<strong>of</strong> behavior. Since the amouct <strong>of</strong> role clarrty influences<br />

thc EasE <strong>of</strong> transition into roles (Burr, 1973:127), one <strong>of</strong><br />

the reasoxs husbands ere experiencing difficulty nay be th?<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> clarity concerning their roles es meritel partners<br />

in an equali+.ariar aurharity structure.<br />

R role-model <strong>of</strong> an equalitarian husbard is hardly to be<br />

found in our culture and, therefore, no explicit s=t <strong>of</strong><br />

definitions exists for +kc husband to follow. nszitsl role<br />

prescriptiocs. although moving in ths direction sf iccr+asea<br />

EqUalitaTianism since 1900, still tend to relegate husbands<br />

to the provider role and vines to the hoaernak?~ acd<br />

child-rearer roles (Brcwn, 1978). <strong>The</strong> mass asdia, in<br />

particular, adverttsing during sports evec;s, emphasize tho<br />

"real man" image (Brovr, 1973). <strong>The</strong> "real msr" is<br />

physically tough, is dcniRant over wonen, axd has the<br />

freedom ?O do what he Dlea~es when hs 019a565. <strong>The</strong>58<br />

Chara~teristics are in direct opposition to chsracteristics<br />

nfeded tor e viebls relationship with vtres who subsczibe to<br />

an equalitarian marital authority stroctor.?.<br />

- -- - - --- - - - -- -<br />

FacilltatioF <strong>of</strong> Go21 :


Ch.11. <strong>Wife</strong>-Smployeent Page 182<br />

confused (Lenasters, 1971). <strong>The</strong> comics cat only rrf:ect,<br />

but also Shape husbands' attitudes toward sharing marital<br />

euthority.<br />

Dagwocd Rumstead :epresocts an importaut<br />

archetype in :he American psyche-th? irrslevast<br />

male. DagwOOd LS browbeaten by a doninearicg<br />

and assertive wlfe. is abused by his boss. and<br />

15 generally a fallure in everything he tries<br />

(Berger, 1979:lOS).<br />

Failure and ronartalraec:, thel. are belisvfd fo be +he<br />

OU~COQO fcr husbands wh3 let their wives shsre ~guslly in<br />

marltal authority. nnfil men come to psrceive 53me <strong>of</strong> ?he<br />

advactages to ;QP_~&&J%~ <strong>of</strong> an equalitari~n relationship<br />

+hey w i l l reslst change ir that 6irec:ior.<br />

--<br />

S:!S&:ty S~;~_!&UE$.<br />

Eeiecrinc or +4$ Esg$l_~r;~~~+s<br />

<strong>The</strong>se four var~ables--degree <strong>of</strong> compulsive nasculILity.<br />

degree <strong>of</strong> anticipatcry socializatioc, dfgran <strong>of</strong> role<br />

cla~ity, and dsgree to which the tracsition facilitit-s goal<br />

atfa~rment--all play a part ic the husband's 3ecision to<br />

accspt or<br />

5tr"Ct"re.<br />

reject the equalitmian marital au'hsrity<br />

Een are trying to cape with the changes through<br />

the traditio~al attitudes. Rh4~ %he 511<br />

dominating techniques no longer prove ~ffsctive.<br />

men becomc Insecure and anxious bscauss <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

ominous specter <strong>of</strong> 'failure.'<br />

what arc these "old dominating techniques?" Grsatar<br />

monetary resources is one, but ths ever-increasing<br />

proportion ct employed vivas makes this rescurc? less and<br />

less available. 9 higher level <strong>of</strong> education is sno'her, but<br />

with the incrsasirg ecrollm-?t <strong>of</strong> women in callog~, tt~t<br />

also is increasingly less true. In fact, ircrsisss ir<br />

educational attainment may be 02.2 cf th? causal factors <strong>of</strong><br />

the emergence <strong>of</strong> presciptiocs for joint airital<br />

decision-maklsg (Brovr, 1977). If nothicg else, at least<br />

men nsed to have the normative presciprlms cocc=rning<br />

merital authority cn their side, but that also has been<br />

slowly but surely charging (Brown, 1978). Hovevrr, one<br />

adVaPtagE that husbands still have over their wires is their<br />

greater physical strergth. As Whitehurst (1974:78) notes:


ch.11. <strong>Wife</strong>-Employment Pige 183<br />

when all other resourc-s <strong>of</strong> aasculins iflentit7<br />

feil. mer can aluays rely on being 'tough' as a<br />

sign <strong>of</strong> manhood. Hardlicg uivss in aggressivs<br />

ways is 12 some respects an ?xtension <strong>of</strong> 'he<br />

ncrmal ways men learn lo handle a variety <strong>of</strong><br />

problems.. ..<br />

Similarly, Koolaravsky's (1962:227) study <strong>of</strong> blue collar<br />

families led her '0 coCClUde that the threat <strong>of</strong> violscce is<br />

ancther ground <strong>of</strong> RasculinC pcver. Pically. Rolfgacg (1969)<br />

points out that "violerce :s 2 msans <strong>of</strong> seekiCq pow-r and<br />

may be defined as a? act <strong>of</strong> dcspair committed whsa the aoor<br />

1s closed to alteraatlve resolutio~s." No Soubl. miry<br />

contemporary husbands feel that the do324 not only hive bsoc<br />

shut, but actually slaaned in thsir faces.<br />

VIOLENCE I N THE FQOAL<strong>IT</strong>BRIEN AUTHOR<strong>IT</strong>Y SlRUCTUPE<br />

up to this point, the chapter has been concern~3 with<br />

the corfllcc ever th9 transition to equalitarian marital<br />

authority structures. Bowever, this is not to suggest that<br />

the husband's rejection <strong>of</strong> an equalitarian authority<br />

structure is the or17 source <strong>of</strong> conflict +Ed violarce.<br />

eusba3ds who aCCBpt Fq~ality in authari'y find that this<br />

leads to ircreased intimacy and coepaniocship in their<br />

marital relationship awing to the lncrease ir shared ispects<br />

ot iits. Blood End Aolfe (1960:160) fOursd that the sore<br />

decisions were shared by the husband ana wife, ths acrp<br />

satlsfactioc was expressed with marital compaoionship.<br />

This increased iatieacy would appsar to b% a plus for<br />

equalitariac marriages, but Coser (1969) an6 Sprey (1971)<br />

cautlon that the more intiaate social relations ere, the<br />

more l ikely thay are to give rise tc conflict. ireer (1941)<br />

found that in ethnic groups ic which there are lswer<br />

~xpBctations or ~motioral attichnsnts between hnshaaa and<br />

if%, marriages terd LO be mote stable. Goode (1962)<br />

concludes that marital strain is likely be rsduced when<br />

*he partners laver thFir expect3tiops 3f cmotFocal<br />

performance and comply vith minisel role 3bligatto3s.<br />

<strong>The</strong>:~tore (azd someuhat peradoxically) marital conflict car<br />

he expected io as th4 narital psrlnrrs shirf nor?<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> life.


Ch. 11. @FEE-Employmln~


~h.11. Wlfe-Employm+~+ Pege 185<br />

APOther aspcct at this paradox is ths* if an<br />

~qualltarlan authority structare is acceptid by both<br />

~pouses. ccrfllc? "111 b' riew~d a5 leg~rime~~, bacause both<br />

pertnirs have equal riohxs ro h~ve thrir viavs prevail.<br />

Bach and Wyden (1968:98) suggest :h+t "vrzbal ccc.flict<br />

betwes~ intimatgs 1s not only accspxabl?, especially between<br />

husba~ds ~ r d wires; lf is construc


Reverting tc traditional doolirating<br />

techniques by the husbatd leids to an<br />

ircreased 1ikolLhocd <strong>of</strong> husband-wife<br />

violence.<br />

Acceptance <strong>of</strong> the equalitarian authority<br />

structurs by the husband leads to increas=3<br />

intimacy betveer. ths husband acd wife.<br />

Increased intimacy between the tusband aca<br />

w i t s le+ds to inCTeas~d leqilr~8fe corflrct<br />

~E~YEJP the husband and vlfe.<br />

Legitimate cocflicr leads to a lecrsassd<br />

llkelihcod <strong>of</strong> husband-wife viollnce<br />

meas ot co~flict ~esol~tion.<br />

as 3<br />

NOTES<br />

*Revls~On <strong>of</strong> 2 paper prfsent~d at the znnoal oeoticg <strong>of</strong><br />

the National Councll or Family Relations, Salt Lake City,<br />

Utah. august 20-23, 1975. <strong>The</strong> pr~paratio~. <strong>of</strong> this papsr was<br />

supported by Naticnal Instirute <strong>of</strong> neatal eeilth grant No.<br />

13050. <strong>The</strong> carmerts and criticisms <strong>of</strong> Lee Crandell an4 Deac<br />

Knudson ardsd ir the revlsioa <strong>of</strong> the piper.<br />

1. As used LT this chapter, "paver" rsfers t3 the<br />

p0tsfit:al ability to coc?rol another's b?havior, ald<br />

"authority'<br />

1960:ll).<br />

retsrs to legitimized power (Blcod 2 ~ d W31fe.<br />

2. 3ahr. Boverman and Gecas, 1974:366; Plood,<br />

1363:294; Blood, 1965:lYS: Blood<br />

tee:, 1958:397; EcKlnley, 1964:lQY;<br />

inif R5lfa.<br />

Scaazsni,<br />

1960: 40:<br />

1970:160;<br />

Wolfs, 1959:109. <strong>The</strong> studies just Ci-.?d zr1 all<br />

inve5tig~t10~5 <strong>of</strong> Rmerlcan fanlliss. Elth3uqh Eimilar<br />

results have befn fo~nd in some<br />

(xbbatt, 1976:llz; ~looe, 1361:159;<br />

cross-culzural studies<br />

>uric ina Jecsvic.<br />

1967:328; Lupri, 1969:144; ilichel, 1970:159; Oppoxg,<br />

1970:678; Pichmond, 1976:262; Safilios-Rothschild.<br />

1967:346; Welle~, 1968:439). cortradictory ~esults wefa<br />

found by Certers. Raven. and Radrigues, 1971:276; B~ffea.<br />

1960:224; Randfl and lesser, 1972: 134; nildleon and<br />

Pu-nsy, 1960:608; and Strzus. 1977.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong> valldlty <strong>of</strong> this assertion depends an<br />

dlszmguishing betvesn conflict (in the sense <strong>of</strong> "ccnflic?<br />

<strong>of</strong> luterest" as described in Chaptef 8) and agqressisn (in<br />

the sense ot a malevolect act). If Each ard Wyain's


assertion is rcad as suggesting that vprbal aggresstan helps<br />

avcid phyS:Cal aggressron, the cvidenCF is overvhrlsingly<br />

the oppasite (Straus, 197ua).


Chapter 12<br />

Resources, Power,<br />

and <strong>Husband</strong>-<strong>Wife</strong> <strong>Violence</strong><br />

Craig M. Allen and Murray A. Straus<br />

Both LaRossa's case study and Brown's<br />

theoretical analysis suggest that "resources"<br />

like the ircoms prcvidod by a wife's emplcymect<br />

outside *he home incresses her power, gpg<br />

increases the likelihood that the husband<br />

w i l l us9 violence to "stop the bad progression<br />

<strong>of</strong> events," as Joe puts it in Chapter 10. But<br />

neither a case study nor a logical dednction are<br />

sufficient evidence for a scientific conclusioc.<br />

Snch a coaclusior depends on additional data.<br />

and that is what Rllen and Straus's study <strong>of</strong><br />

approximately 400 couples provides.<br />

In addition to the statis'ical fiodicgs.<br />

this chapter takes the theoretical 3nalysis ~f<br />

LaRossa and Rrowr one step further. It puts<br />

forward the "ultimaC3 resource t tpory" *5<br />

explain the relationship between power and<br />

violerce by takirg into consider?+icn cultural<br />

norms regarding family power. Tha theory holes<br />

that in individualistic, achisvsme~t-oriecttd<br />

s0ciet:es. ~0r.s suppo~ti~g male superiority are<br />

weak or ambiguous. For ?. hasbznd to be ocrg<br />

than a census *illy in th? sCatistics cc<br />

"household heads." he has td validate his<br />

position by superior "resources" such as oonsy,<br />

occupational status, or valued persoral traits.<br />

If the husband lacks such resources, and feals<br />

entitled to a position <strong>of</strong> dominance, viclPnc?<br />

may be used as the "ultimaCe resource" to back<br />

up reelings <strong>of</strong> power entitlement. Data frcn<br />

approximately 400 couples support these ideas.<br />

but only tor Workjng class couplss.


ch.12. Resources and Power Page 189<br />

E nmber <strong>of</strong> soclalogical theoriss <strong>of</strong> family violence.<br />

such as those by G i l (1975). Goods (1971), ard Streus<br />

(Chapter 6). hold ?hat the use <strong>of</strong> physical fosce is the<br />

ultinate sanctior that underlies the maintenanc? <strong>of</strong> the<br />

presect male-dominan' pattern <strong>of</strong> family organization.<br />

Although the analyses <strong>of</strong> these authors may seem plausible,<br />

they rest on piecing together scattered, <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

impressioni5t:c evidence. We reed specific tests <strong>of</strong> the<br />

idea that the threat <strong>of</strong> physical violence ondarliss the<br />

existing family structure. <strong>The</strong>se data, although linit=d in<br />

several ways, are a step in that direction.<br />

THEORIES OF RESOURCES AND POWER<br />

Pollowlng the work <strong>of</strong> French acd Raven (1959). Blood<br />

and Volfe introducsd the "resource theory" <strong>of</strong> family power.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y defined a resource 2s "...anything that one partnnr may<br />

make available to the other, helping the latter satisfy his<br />

needs or attain his goals" (1960:lZ). <strong>The</strong> Blood 2nd WOlfe<br />

study found that the spouse with the greater cumber <strong>of</strong><br />

resources tends to have more power over his or her<br />

parrner.*l However, Radmin's (1967) snalysis 3f data from<br />

several countries fouxd the theory to be suppsrted in the<br />

Wnited States and Prance. but not confirmed by Safa from<br />

Greece and Yugoslavia. Ir the lattes two countries. the<br />

correlations <strong>of</strong> father's education and occupation with power<br />

were in t h opposite ~ direction from that predicted by the<br />

resource theory. Rodmar. concluded that, to explain the<br />

distribution <strong>of</strong> maritel pouer, the culture1 cortext from<br />

which norms defiring appropriate resources are d?terained<br />

must be takrr. into account (see also Pox. 1973). In a<br />

subsequent comprehensive review <strong>of</strong> findicgs relzted to the<br />

resource theory, Rodman (1972) developed a th-ory <strong>of</strong><br />

nrssources in u cultural context" in which he suggests that<br />

the dynamics <strong>of</strong> excharge in the marital dyad car be<br />

understood only in fhh cor.tfxt Of the cnltare <strong>of</strong> ~h:cb the<br />

dyad is a part:<br />

<strong>The</strong> balance <strong>of</strong> marltal power is influezc3d by<br />

the interacfion or (1) the canpararive rescurc%s<br />

<strong>of</strong> husband and wife ard (2) the cultural or<br />

subcultural expectations about fhe distribution<br />

<strong>of</strong> marital pouer (1972:60).<br />

TRE ULTIMATE RESCWRCE TEEORY<br />

Goode (1971). Podmar (1972). and Rogers (1974) suggest<br />

that violerce is a resource irvoked when individusls lack<br />

other legltimz?e resources to serve as bases for their<br />

power.*Z Bccording to ~oode (1971). a spouse who lacks the<br />

prestige, money, or skill necessary to induce the other


Ch.12. AeSoUrces and Power Page 190<br />

spouse to perform scme behavior might resort '0 violence as<br />

a final resource, even though the exercise <strong>of</strong> violencx is an<br />

illegitimate cr negetively sanctioned mode <strong>of</strong> exercising<br />

paver. Rodman (1972) asserts. however, that this pattern<br />

w i l l b2 found only in a soci€:y in which (1) the norms which<br />

legifimatize the cxsrcise <strong>of</strong> power are weak and somewhat<br />

ambiguous. (2) the ac+uel exercise <strong>of</strong> legitimst? psxzr must<br />

be validatsd by the rescurcGs in the form <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

qualities acd material possession. <strong>The</strong> Onited States<br />

tppifies this kind <strong>of</strong> scclety, because (1) equ~litarian<br />

norms are rep1ac:cg petriarchal ocrms in the marital dyad,<br />

(2) as 2 cOns2que~ce. there is norma1ivs ambiguity about :he<br />

dis


ch. 12. Resources end Power<br />

phys1cal force as a basis for power.<br />

the<br />

--- saneis. <strong>The</strong> data to be reporfed are from a study <strong>of</strong><br />

taeilies <strong>of</strong> studerts in ictroductory s3ciolcgy and<br />

anthropology classes at tke Univ~rsity <strong>of</strong> New Hanpshire in<br />

the fall <strong>of</strong> 1972. <strong>The</strong> data war€ obtained by qu?stior.naires<br />

completed on a volurtary basis during a class period. Of<br />

the 583 questionnaires distributed. 555 or 92 perclct were<br />

completed. Since the ar-alyses to be presentid 3+pended on<br />

both ths studer.tqs and both parent's living at toms luring<br />

the student's serlor year in high school, further cases vere<br />

105t because + parect or the child vas not living et home<br />

tha' year. In additlcn. there was the ireviiable lass <strong>of</strong><br />

some cases because ceztair qu~stioes vere not snsverld. <strong>The</strong><br />

resulting flnal sample size ranges fxm 324 3 437,<br />

dependlng or the variables ircludsd in a given t3ble- Of<br />

these Cases, 26 psrcent are famllies in vhicb the husbacd is<br />

a manual worker.<br />

--<br />

KeggE Z I ~ Viole~ce. Data 03 husband-uife violerc? was<br />

securzd from on* <strong>of</strong> the ccuple's children becaus* <strong>of</strong> (1) ths<br />

dlfflculty Of directly Observi~g the occurre>re <strong>of</strong> vislent<br />

behavior bstveen spouses, particularly the more extreme<br />

forms: (2) the ethical problems entailed Lr. devisi~g an<br />

experin€ntal situatio~ in which violent behavior between<br />

husbards and wives could be precipitated; and (3) the fear<br />

that the strong cegatire senctians in our sociery against<br />

the u:actic~ and EVSP the edaission <strong>of</strong> violence would lead<br />

to refusals and/or UnderreportiPo by husbendr a23 wive=.<br />

<strong>The</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> interview data on family pattprrs is<br />

always open to question ones one goss beyon? such snbjscts<br />

as 'he ages ard sex <strong>of</strong> children, and son; rsseazchlrs fssl<br />

that this procedure is even more guss:ionable when a child<br />

is the source <strong>of</strong> the data. Bowever. Calonico and Thomas<br />

(1973) round that children caE predict<br />

behavior under certax. cixums'arces<br />

their parents'<br />

slightly BPE~<br />

acc~rately thac thelr perents car <strong>of</strong> cne another. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

attributed t<br />

with the11<br />

h firding ~ to the familiarity cf the childrec<br />

parertsq past behevior. Bahr, 39vcrmar, axd<br />

Gecas (1974) fourd that adolescents across various age<br />

categcri9s vere quite consistent in reporting th?ir parents'<br />

behavior. <strong>The</strong>y suggest 'hat it would be extremely difficult<br />

tor parmts to hide thrir behavior froa adolescent children<br />

over a long period <strong>of</strong> time. R more comp:ehsnsive<br />

exawitbtior <strong>of</strong> tbls iss~9, togethzr with data 32 the<br />

validity <strong>of</strong> the vialerce measure used in this paper. is<br />

glren in Eulcraft and Straus (1975).<br />

high degree <strong>of</strong> agreamect between<br />

That eralysis shows a<br />

student reports <strong>of</strong><br />

husband-wife V~O~ELCB aPd dat2 providod indepsnd?ntly by the<br />

spouses themselren.


Ch.12. Resources and power Pa3e 192<br />

<strong>The</strong> speclfic technique used to measur? physical<br />

violePCe 1s the <strong>Violence</strong> scale <strong>of</strong> the family Cpgfll~<br />

------- ------<br />

Tzctics Scales (cTs) instrument (Straus. 1974a:15; 1979).<br />

In this tschrlgue. the respondsnt is first asked to idsc!ify<br />

the major sources <strong>of</strong> disagreement and conflict during the<br />

referent year. Pollavirg this, a series <strong>of</strong> questions are<br />

asked concercicg partner's responses to conflicts 3urirg<br />

that year. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

increzsiCg order <strong>of</strong><br />

qnesti~~s are arranged in gradually<br />

coerciveness <strong>of</strong> respocses, bPgir.ning<br />

with disc~ssi~g things calmly, and ?ndicg with hitting the<br />

other person ~ i t h sowethiPg hard.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Violence</strong> index ccnsists <strong>of</strong> the final fire items. E<br />

co~p1et.e list <strong>of</strong> the CTS questions is givar in Straus<br />

(1974a: 1979). togsther with data cn ths ini~rna<br />

consistency rsliability <strong>of</strong> each index. Validity data ere<br />

qiven ic Bulcr<strong>of</strong>t and Straus (1975).*4<br />

pxgr neasurg. <strong>The</strong> cnnceptuallzatisc ard measurement<br />

<strong>of</strong> ccnjugal powerhas posed many difficultiss, as avidanced<br />

by several critical studies (Cromwell and Olson, 1975;<br />

R~er, 1963: Olson, 1969: Olson and Rabunsky, 1972;<br />

Safillos-Rothschild. 1970: and Turk and Ball. 1972). Our<br />

d~flnition 1s sxplained in Straus, 1977 and Straus and<br />

Ta11m2n. 1971. <strong>The</strong> specific n%asurs used in this chap:=r is<br />

the Decisior Power Index <strong>of</strong> Elcod end WYlfe (196D).*5<br />

Rlthoogh it has been widely criticized (in part because<br />

it is the most widely used measure). Blood and w?lfe's<br />

measure seemed best sulted to this reso3rch fnr the<br />

follovrrg reasocs: (1) It uses a convenient list <strong>of</strong><br />

indicators in which power ls theoreticelly most likely<br />

eeide~t--who has thr final s+y in importent 2nd typical<br />

family de~isions. (2) Its wide USE in the past dscade 2nd a<br />

halt p?rmits coep2riscn with many previous s+udi=s. (3) <strong>The</strong><br />

many correlates <strong>of</strong> paver as measured by this s~riod provide<br />

vhat is, ir sftect, "ccr:struct validaticn" av~denca, and a<br />

recent paper by Straus (1977) provides eutdence <strong>of</strong><br />

concurrent validity. (4) Research on power lo tilniliss t.35<br />

incr~zsingly demonstrated the aultidlmensior.al nnture <strong>of</strong> the<br />

CCP.COPI..<br />

<strong>The</strong> thecry bFirg tes+ed requiras a mfesure res7ricted<br />

to what Cromwell and Olson (1975) call rhe "paver OutcYnes"<br />

dteenslon. This, <strong>of</strong> course, deliberately onits othcr<br />

crucial dimersiors such as tho processes by which decisions<br />

are reached. Although for convenience in expositi2r ws<br />

follow ths colvention <strong>of</strong> using the word eaE.ez Esr the<br />

particular aspect measured, fhe reader must bear in micd<br />

that this ass refers Only to who makes certain som:vhat<br />

arbitrarily selected dec~sions. In particular. tte ne%sure<br />

explicitly excludes vhat French and Rarer (1959) call the<br />

"basis" <strong>of</strong> power, such as "reward power" (the ability to<br />

provid? rewards because <strong>of</strong> coctrol <strong>of</strong> mn€y or 02h?r valued<br />

gooas sr services). "expert poserv8 based on psrcepticc cf


superior knowledge, exc. 4 'power ourcome" typ? measui- was<br />

cecessary because :he theory b-in? tested cnrcerns tho<br />

relatioCShipS between csr'air <strong>of</strong> French and Raven's "basas"<br />

<strong>of</strong> power (which we prefer -3 iden'ify as wresourc=sn) and<br />

pOW4X outcomes, in the ser~se <strong>of</strong> who makes critical<br />

d~cisions.<br />

Since the Flood and Uolfe technique examines answers<br />

that indicate "who has thl final say" in respect to thc six<br />

6e~lS10n~. and sirce the measure is scored so that high<br />

scores show that ? h ~ balarce <strong>of</strong> poue? is or th-. side 3f the<br />

husband and lov scores shcv that the xi*? his r~lstively<br />

more decisioc power, up w i l l sosetimes use the tern "Final<br />

Say Power Index" or Velativ1 Powei Index' ?a idsntify<br />

instrument.<br />

this<br />

- ReSOUrCE - - -- fleasure. -- - - POT each S~JUSF, a2 Rbsalute<br />

RBSOUICE Index and a R?lative REscurca Index %#?re<br />

c~nstz~cled. <strong>The</strong> Bbsolute Resource Indzx cansists <strong>of</strong> the<br />

snm <strong>of</strong> four items referrirq to 2san_ogir Ezes;ao<br />

conf=rr_&r,g charac?eristics (education, occupatin. incams.<br />

satisfaction with income) and four items refsrring to valuea<br />

r,~~~gg&& '~_Q+;s (high self-esteem, achievement ori-nts'ion<br />

and soc1ab;llty. ard lcv anxisiy). <strong>The</strong> former cec be<br />

thought <strong>of</strong> as corresponding to what 31au (39hu:ZO-22) would<br />

call 'm~xtrinsic'z resources, and the latt-z es "hat he<br />

identifies as "intrznsic" resources. A parallel P-lntivs<br />

Re50DTC9 Index vas also computed for F2Ch Sp3U5EI by first<br />

calculating the exten? to which a paziicular J ~ U S P h2a E<br />

hLgter score on each r25onrc~ and thsn sumnic.g ?ktf positive<br />

scores.*6<br />

POUFP AND VIOLENCE<br />

<strong>The</strong> most elemsntary empirical data or the issus <strong>of</strong> this<br />

papsr is the correlation Optveer the balarce <strong>of</strong> power in the<br />

ramlly (+s measured by our versilr. <strong>of</strong> the 31036 ana W>lf=<br />

'*Decision Power Index") ac3 :he frequency 3E physical<br />

violewe bezwsen spcuses. Howerer, as was explai~ra i~ the<br />

iniroductior, ard as we nore agai3. bolov, snch correl+'ions<br />

are not an adeguate ?@s% <strong>of</strong> ultimate resource 'hacry b-cause<br />

they do cot take ~n-o account ths resoorcss th?i give<br />

lfgiCloicy to each ac"orTs pcvfr positioc. Neverthelass,<br />

thsS4 simple ~o~r~lations are a~ 2ppropriate poi~i a? which<br />

to bagin the data anelysls, ir for co athfr rsason that to<br />

demonsfrate their inadaquacy.


Ch.12. ResO~rce6 and Power Page 19I1<br />

T ~ E uppe; arrow <strong>of</strong> Figure 1 give5 the orr re let ions<br />

between the relative pow7 index acd ths us? <strong>of</strong> violence by<br />

tho total sznplr, by the working class par? 3f +t.e sample,<br />

and by fhe mlddle cless part <strong>of</strong> th; sample. <strong>The</strong><br />

~orrelatio?~ betweer the Re1er:vs Power Irdox and the wife's<br />

violenc~ are show in the lover arrow. In gecsrsl, Figure 1<br />

6hox5 CP~Y low and variable correlations between pcusr and<br />

violence. However. to the extext that Cocclusions can be<br />

drawn, Flguze 1 suggests that in the middle class. ch=re is<br />

ro relationship betweer relative power and the PASbBgCP<br />

VIOI~PCF, but a5 Lhe husband's paver increases, i& x:EQL~<br />

violence decreases (-.16). <strong>The</strong> pattern is reverse* t3r ?he<br />

vorklng class, "here there is a slight tendency Elr the<br />

husbz~d's Violexce to increase as his power incr~asCS. but<br />

nc relatiocship is shown between his porsr and violence.<br />

<strong>The</strong> pattern for the Latal sample is 'he same as ihc p3ttern<br />

to= ?he middle class, largely because the preponaersnce <strong>of</strong><br />

Case5 are middle class (75 to 80 percent, depending 2n the<br />

variables ic a qiven set <strong>of</strong> tabulations).*7


ch.1~. Resources and Power<br />

Page 195


Ch.12. Resources and Power Page 196<br />

RFSOURCZS PWD POWER<br />

g&gcs9= R9sourc~s- Par? A <strong>of</strong> Figure 2 shows that fcr<br />

all SaBplss, as the husb2xdls recaurcsr iccrease, th~r? is a<br />

tecdsncy for his pouer to increase (r = 0.19). Eaealy any<br />

relatlor is seep between Che vifglj rescurcss and the<br />

balancs <strong>of</strong> power in ?he family. 30% to the extPct -hit z<br />

re1a':lonship exlsts, the negztive correlations suggest %he<br />

other sid= <strong>of</strong> the coin shoun by the fixdings for the<br />

husband: that is, the nore the wife's resouxcrs tha less<br />

the hUSband'5 POWET.<br />

-------- -----<br />

Pflativs Resources. Corrrlatiors betwesn r$&gd&lg<br />

resOUrCes ax6 the husband*~ balance <strong>of</strong> pouer are givsr in<br />

Par': B ot F;gure 2. <strong>The</strong>se correla?ions rrveal ths same<br />

pattern as was tound far absolute resources €xc?pt Zh3t the<br />

correlations are larger (sn average <strong>of</strong> 73 percsnt greater).<br />

<strong>The</strong>r9tore Chese data show that bath absolute and relative<br />

~ESOUTC~S affect tt.~ balalce cf powzr in fhe fimilies<br />

studied. Acuever, Evan within the co>t=xt <strong>of</strong> the lor<br />

ccrrelaticrs that ore EXPFC~S in analysas <strong>of</strong> a sr~gle causal<br />

factor, ocly ths husbacd.s relative resources se?m to havs a<br />

subs:znti+l relatio~ahip to who has the final say.<br />

F~rth~rmoze. for both husbands scd wives. the ccrr+lation<br />

between resources and power is stionger ~Lthic the working<br />

Class thar ul+hin the middle class part <strong>of</strong> +ha ssnple.<br />

Despite ihesi qualifications, ths correlations il each<br />

sample, for both husba~as and wives, and for b3th types <strong>of</strong><br />

resonrces. are ir the same direction as That predictPd by<br />

the resource theary.*8<br />

TAEORIES OF RESOUSCES AND VIOLElCF<br />

np to :his point ve have shown only a us~k and =rraric<br />

rElat=O~.~h~p betweep nolsnce ard t.h? halarc? <strong>of</strong> paver in<br />

family decisloni. We suggesied that th? sinple correlation<br />

between power B S ~ violence falls '0 ~ake into iccooat the<br />

dimension <strong>of</strong> legitimacy ;r, exercising power. in a modern<br />

~ndusraal soclety, as in aosz other societies, pousr is<br />

usualiy ascribed '0 ths husband. But the husband mus? also<br />

posssss the persccal ald macerial r;souzces that ar3 valued<br />

in such a soclety. In short, it was arqu~d that dsspi


ch.12. Resanrces and power PagE 197


Cb.12. Resources and Power Page 198<br />

absolute Resources. <strong>The</strong> correlations ir the top line<br />

<strong>of</strong> Figure 3, part A, show that ?he greater the husband's<br />

absclute resources. the less his use <strong>of</strong> physical violence.<br />

For wives, the correlations shown in the lower arrow <strong>of</strong><br />

part A are small but in ?he same direction 3s those for<br />

husbands: the greatEr her abso1nte ~esources, tb.e less her<br />

violence. For both spouses, but especially for th?<br />

husbands. the correlations are stronger for the wrklng<br />

class than for rha middle class.<br />

Turning to the correlations between husband's resouzces<br />

and the life15 viclerce, the upper diagonal arrow <strong>of</strong> par? P<br />

indicates the same trecd: as ?he husband's absolute<br />

resOurcEs increase, the wife's violence decreases. This<br />

same tendeccy is evldent from the correlations <strong>of</strong> wife's<br />

r~soucces with 'he husband's violence, except th3t the<br />

ccrrelations. although all negative, are also a11 near zero<br />

iT magnitude. It appears from these data thst the husband's<br />

absolute resources are more releted to ths wifa's ana his<br />

own violence, than the wife's absolute resources are related<br />

to her husband's ard her cwn violence.<br />

- -<br />

Relative Resources. A t first glance it seems as thong~h<br />

part B <strong>of</strong> Figure 3 shows cnly low and erratic relat


Ch.12. RasO%nces and Pouer Page 199<br />

creating a dual set <strong>of</strong> rag0 ireme1<br />

Satisty: to exercise 1s ,gitimt<br />

family, one must both have super:<br />

Obviously only husbands can meet<br />

BESOUPCES BND TEE LEG<strong>IT</strong>IMlCY OF POVEB<br />

llthaugh the firdings in the precsdicg three sections<br />

are interesting in their own right, they were pr?sentrd<br />

primarily 70 lay thr groundwork for a axe specific test <strong>of</strong><br />

the two main hypotheses derived from the ultimste rsssurce<br />

theory. Specifically, in en icdividual-achievemart oriented<br />

SCCiEXY such 15 the United States, the pattern <strong>of</strong><br />

rormativsly prescribed male family leaaership that<br />

characterrzes so many societies is suppleasn'sd by ths<br />

forther requirement that the husband deaons're%r his<br />

eligibility to occupy a lsadsrship p3sitigr in posrzssicq<br />

valued personal charectfristics and providing ecocomic<br />

goods. If he does not, then the legitinacy <strong>of</strong> his povrr is<br />

undermined. under these circunstacces, we suggest, aale<br />

POW47 w i l l be asSOCia?ed with violence.


ch.12. Resources and over Psge 200<br />

Th4 findings can be summarized under two hsadicgs: e<br />

high resource group and a low resource graup-fl0<br />

Ccrrelations between POYI~ and violecce vere thsc computed<br />

within each <strong>of</strong> the resource groups. This procPss was<br />

repeated for each <strong>of</strong>


Ch.12. Resources and Pover ~ a 201 g ~<br />

within the middlE class (top four correlatisns ir. th.<br />

right panel) the regecive correlations shcv that ths more<br />

the balance <strong>of</strong> power is in favor <strong>of</strong> the husbsnd, ?he less<br />

the wife's violence. This terdarcy is present bath whe? the<br />

husbana'5 XESOUrCE5 are high and when they 2rE lows but the<br />

correlation is stro~ge1 in the latter case. Withir the<br />

~orklng Class part <strong>of</strong> the sample, ContrDllLng far the<br />

3usbaod's resources (second block <strong>of</strong> four correlatlocs in<br />

the right panel <strong>of</strong> Table 1) produces a much more coinplicafed<br />

set <strong>of</strong> corrslaticrs: Wher the husband's rssaurces ir= high,<br />

there is a slight tecdercy for high husband p>wsr to he<br />

associaTed u:th low xire riolsncs (as ir tha findings for<br />

the m:ddle class)- But when the husband's absalate<br />

resOUICES are low, rF.en "he greater hls power* tLe maze ~ P T<br />

ulolerce (r =0.31). Both these firdings, and especially the<br />

difrer2nca between the Correlations for low and high<br />

rE50UrCO husbandsv are consistfxt with the theory that<br />

sp~czties that violence w i l l occur when husbands uh3 lack<br />

the ~PSOU~CSE to legitimize their ascribed power try to<br />

EX~TCLSE that power. On the other hand. we have no? as yet<br />

form~lated any satisfactory explanation tor the finding that<br />

vhec the ccntrol for resources is on the b25i6 <strong>of</strong> the<br />

rf&=$hpg Egerss <strong>of</strong> husband znd vife, the corrslatians aye<br />

Pear zero, eves though they show the same shift from<br />

negative for the high rescurce group to positivs for ?h? low<br />

resoorco group.<br />

<strong>The</strong> two right columns in P+rt B <strong>of</strong> Tabl~ 7 shsw ths<br />

ccrrelations befweec the hwsba~d's pozer i ~ 5te d WL~S'S<br />

violence V ~ E P cortrols are introduced for the wife's level<br />

cf resou:cE+. <strong>The</strong> aiddl? clsss blnck cf f2ur correlatiors<br />

shows a pattern slmilar tc that tourd wher ?he ccrtrCL was<br />

on the basis or fhe husband's resoorces: csmely, f3r 211<br />

resOnrce groups1 the nore the Lusbz?d's p3vsz. *kC less<br />

violence on the part c2 ?he wife. eut "he? we lo3k at the<br />

125: four correlations in the lowe: right corrar <strong>of</strong> Table 1,<br />

still a d:fterent pattern is r?vealed. Firs:. the<br />

CCrrelatioPs ars all very low. Aouevsr, the pattprn <strong>of</strong><br />

ttese corrslatiocs tits our theory exactly: whan th5 wives<br />

are hlgh in resources. high power on %he par: 3f the husband<br />

1s associated with a tezdeccy towards oiolanca on ihs part<br />

<strong>of</strong> the wife--porh+ps as a means <strong>of</strong> rssisting msls aosinacce<br />

or the part <strong>of</strong> uoner. whose resources lead thsm to '39sire a<br />

greater share <strong>of</strong> the pover. Conversely, whec resourcns are<br />

low, high pcvnr on ths par- <strong>of</strong> the husbard is associated<br />

with less ~iolfnc~ an the par: <strong>of</strong> his wife (althouah +hs<br />

~egatzve cor~fletion~ hetween male power and ths wife's<br />

violence are low). we car speculate t h ~ t this corrslation<br />

represents an acceptance <strong>of</strong> t h<br />

hlErarChy an the part <strong>of</strong> women who<br />

claia greater equality.<br />

traditional ~<br />

family power<br />

lack the resources to<br />

Overall, the date on violence initiated by wives are<br />

like those for male violancn. In both inst2ncas. thz data<br />

axe consis+~rt with the ~OBOU~CE theory only vithio the


Ch.lZ. Rescurces and Pouer PagP 202<br />

working class part or the sample. Specifically, when the<br />

husbacd's rsscafces arn high, high male pover is no+ net by<br />

violence on the part <strong>of</strong> the wife; whereas if the husband's<br />

IESOUZCES are low, male power texas tc be met by female<br />

ViolOLCe. <strong>The</strong> other side <strong>of</strong> the coin is shout xhen the<br />

------<br />

wife's resources are conirolled, but the correlations ire<br />

all very low. Nevertheless, ve icterpret the positive<br />

corrslation between malv paver and wife's violence as<br />

reflecting resistance to male domination on the psrt <strong>of</strong><br />

working class wivfs vith high resources, and the low<br />

negative correlations as reflecting acceptance sf male<br />

doalnance on the part <strong>of</strong> working class wives vith low<br />

reSOUrCOS~<br />

SUnflRRY BND CONCLUSIONS<br />

<strong>The</strong> research presectsd in this paper vis designed tc<br />

explore two clos~ly related issues corcerning physical<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> in marriage. <strong>The</strong> first <strong>of</strong> these issues is the<br />

extent to vhich the use cf physlcal force is associated vith<br />

the maictenance <strong>of</strong> male dcminance in tha family. <strong>The</strong> sscond<br />

issue is a test <strong>of</strong> -he idea that husbands who lack certain<br />

valued persoral traits and materm1 possessisrs (called<br />

"resources") tecd tc substitute physical violencs to<br />

mzintain a poslrlon <strong>of</strong> superiority. Together ;hsse two<br />

ideas form ths core af what ve call the "ultimste rssource<br />

theory" <strong>of</strong> intrafamily violence.<br />

Since this is crcss-sectioral ressarch, 'he causal<br />

relatiocships inherent in the ultins'e rcsource theory<br />

cannot be tested in any definitive way. Rather, we coaputea<br />

correlations betveen pcvs: +Ed v'ole2ce a7d simu153~?3usly<br />

held constant certain otner variablas as + mnacs <strong>of</strong><br />

discovering if the resulting patrern <strong>of</strong> ralatlccships<br />

zccords with the idea thai violence is the ultimate resource<br />

underlying male dcminance in lmericah families.<br />

<strong>The</strong> resulting corralations were uenerelly low.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore. what conriderce cre can have ic these results<br />

derives trom the cansistercy vith vhich we found pattercs 3f<br />

positlre and negative correlations concordsnt with the<br />

theory, and also from the fact tkt the largsst corr=litio?.s<br />

(0.49 and 0.31) were found for those relationships that<br />

constituted the most crucial test <strong>of</strong> rhe theory. With these<br />

qualifications in mind, the findings caa be suamarizcd as<br />

fellows:<br />

1. Llttle or co relationship exists betveen the<br />

conjugal balance <strong>of</strong> power and the use 3f violence by either<br />

spouse. This firdlsg is consistect with the ultimate<br />

resource theory in that the theory specifies that male<br />

dominants w i l l lead to ulclsnce only if the husband cannot<br />

legitimize his SUperior p ~ e r position throuqh the


ch.11. PPSOU~CBS and Power Page 203<br />

possession <strong>of</strong> va1:dstlng resources.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> greater the husband's rssources, the less his<br />

USE <strong>of</strong> physical violence. Similirly, ths greater the v~fe's<br />

resources, the less her use <strong>of</strong> violence. <strong>The</strong>s2 findir!gs are<br />

consistent with the ultimata resource theory ic that they<br />

suggest that those who car call on resources that do not<br />

have the costly side effects <strong>of</strong> violent= "ill use less<br />

vrolence.<br />

3. superiorl?y in resources cqdgf&~p f& peegap Was<br />

also examined far possible association with violence. This<br />

aralysis revealed that the Extert to which a husband's<br />

resouncencnceed ...~---~-<br />

~~~~~~~ those <strong>of</strong> ~ hie ~ ~ wife has little or no r=lation<br />

to violence bp either spouse. Rorever, the sore the ghfplp<br />

rrsoorcss --- sXceFd ~~ ~~~ those ~ cf her husband. the rore likely Che<br />

~~ hnsbard - ~ 1s to have used . ohvsical . force durino the rsferen'<br />

year. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggsr that the wife's poss?ssion <strong>of</strong><br />

SUpEr1OT resources can undsrmine the ability <strong>of</strong> the<br />

hasband's resources to validate superior poif~r, thus l~ading<br />

to the substitution <strong>of</strong> 2 resource in which wives cen rarely<br />

be s~pe~lcr to thelr husbands: physical ViolencJ.<br />

4. Finally, the specific hypothesss derivea frne the<br />

ultimate resource theory predicted a tendency for male power<br />

to be assaciated with violence onlp when th* husbacd lacks<br />

validating resources. R? found this hypothesis true, but<br />

only tor working class husbands. Po: husbanls higt in<br />

TFSDU~CES. as pr~dict~d, there was no correlation b?tveen<br />

power and use <strong>of</strong> violence. But for those vorkicg class<br />

husbands low in resources. the grea'er ?he husband's p3uer.<br />

the more liten he uspa physical force on his spouse ic<br />

situations oi conflic?.<br />

5. In almost all the analyses rsport-l in this papez.<br />

ard especially in the most direct test <strong>of</strong> the ultimate<br />

:ISOUTC~ fheory summarized in the precedirg psragraph. 'he<br />

correlatiols were stronger in working class familizs, or<br />

sometimes present only in vorking class families.<br />

what could account for the fact that the ultimate<br />

rescurce theory se?as to apply only within working class<br />

families? TWO po~sibilities ccme io mind. <strong>The</strong> firs? is<br />

based on social<br />

and can be call96<br />

class differenc~s in mals authority roras<br />

the "middle class equalitarian norms"<br />

explanation. <strong>The</strong> sfcord<br />

types <strong>of</strong> violence and can<br />

is based on class diEforenzes in<br />

be called the "uorking class<br />

instzumental violence" explanation.<br />

------ ----- -----<br />

Brddle Class ?over Aores. This explanarioc lssumfs<br />

that wlthlr the middle class a significant dsgree <strong>of</strong> support<br />

no longer exists tor lorms that give superior<br />

husbacds. If this is correct, then few<br />

authority to<br />

middle class<br />

husbands w i l l a+tempt to claim the triditional Bale<br />

authority. <strong>The</strong>refore, physical farce is not needea t9 back


up ac ascribed posiilal <strong>of</strong> authority if the husband lacks<br />

orher resources to validate his ascribed authortty.<br />

U~fortunat~l~. vs find this explanation difficult to iccept.<br />

becanse our personal observation <strong>of</strong> fa~ilies, tngeth?r with<br />

much other evidence (Bahr, Bowerman, and Gecas, 1974; Blood<br />

and Yolfe, 1960; Kolb and Straus, 1979; Scanzoni. 1970;<br />

Skolnick and Skolnick. 197Y: Chapter 6) suggests that<br />

despite Considerable lip service to sexual equality, the<br />

actual structure <strong>of</strong> middle class families remains basically<br />

male-dominant.<br />

Rorkinq class I~struacgtal xz~&gc=. This explanition<br />

begins from the assumpfion that what we have called<br />

ninstrumental Tioler.cc*l (SSP Chapter 1. footnote 2) is more<br />

acceptable within the working class, vhnre there is a<br />

ccntinurng tradlticnal saphasis on the 'macho17 or 'he-man."<br />

By cortrast, the rejection or weakness <strong>of</strong> such norms ma<br />

values in the middlc class may lead middle class husbands to<br />

restrict the instrumental use <strong>of</strong> violence to a significant<br />

degree.<br />

This does rot mean that physical violence between<br />

husband and wife is absent in the middle class. <strong>The</strong>re is,<br />

in fact. 3 growing body <strong>of</strong> svidercs that cocjugll violence<br />

is part or the reality <strong>of</strong> American slddls class family<br />

StrOCture. although less frequent than in the working<br />

Class.*ll But the Px3+ure <strong>of</strong> tb.is violence may bs differsct.<br />

It nay be larg=ly what we hare ccllpd "expressis2 vi;l&nce."<br />

In tact, the current popularity <strong>of</strong> "creative aggression,"<br />

"catharsis," and the idea that it is h?ilthy for th3<br />

ind~oldual and good for the marriage to "let it all hang<br />

out.' tends to legitimize expressive violence as something<br />

that must be go' out <strong>of</strong> one's nys+en (S+raus. 1974s). If<br />

the foregoing is correct, ?her merit31 riolence in the<br />

aiddle class vould be primarily expressive. uhrrsas nariral<br />

vlOlQnce in the wozkirg class would be primarily<br />

instrumental. <strong>The</strong>refore. sirce this sxplanation <strong>of</strong><br />

instrumental violence is the focus <strong>of</strong> the ultimate resource<br />

theory, It tollo~s that this theory should apply ozly within<br />

the a0rk:P.g class. because fhat is the ~ 1 2 ~ 5<br />

i which<br />

Instrumental violence is presumed to be the predominaat type<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence.<br />

Unrortunatelp, this second explanation for the firdlrg<br />

that the ultimate resource theory applies only io the<br />

working class is also difficult to accept because our<br />

personal impression is that uhile there aa? bs less<br />

instrumental violence ir middle class families, it is far<br />

trm Completely absent. Eoueorr, although nsither miadle<br />

Class Equalitarian norms nor working class inatruwental<br />

vLo1e~ce by themselves 50Em suffic~znt to sxplain ths<br />

supp<strong>of</strong>t <strong>of</strong> the ultimate rfsoarce theory only for w?rkmg<br />

class tamilies, the combined ~ffects <strong>of</strong> class diff=rsrce in<br />

power norms and in rorms cor.cerning instrumental violence<br />

nay be s<strong>of</strong>ficiert. Specifically, because <strong>of</strong> the weakening


Ch.12. R~SOU~CPS and Power P3ge 205<br />

cf male-dominar.ce norms in the middle class, a challenge to<br />

2 husband's superior authority may be less <strong>of</strong> a threat to<br />

the identity and masculinity <strong>of</strong> middle class husbands than<br />

<strong>of</strong> the working class husbands. Biddle class husbirds may be<br />

able to adapt to what they formerly paid lip ssrvic?, and<br />

still retain their self-esteem. If this is the case.<br />

husbands ic the middle class have less need to d?fac& an<br />

+scribed position <strong>of</strong> au-hority. In addition, t3 ths exterr<br />

that instruasntal violwcs is subject to more r?vsre<br />

limitations in the middle class, it vill be a hrgher "costw<br />

resource because <strong>of</strong> mcr4 xegative side effects. Thsz~fore,<br />

'h2 lEsSer need to Use this ultimate resource canbinpa with<br />

the greats= cost <strong>of</strong> using violence nay account fsr the<br />

tizding that the ulfiaate resourcs theory ipplie3 orly<br />

within ths working class sample.<br />

*we arc irdebted to Johc 8. Scanzoni for comments and<br />

critlcism thlt aided in the revision <strong>of</strong> this plper.<br />

1. "Resource theoryw is a specific instaccs <strong>of</strong><br />

"exchange theory" (Fahr, 1974; Resr, 1963).<br />

?henry terms, the rfasorrng just presented<br />

In excharge<br />

1s thtt ac<br />

irdlvidual<br />

renard at<br />

w l l l attenpt<br />

mlninum cost.<br />

to abtein a dssired outcoms or<br />

Thus, if one spouse desires<br />

comp1:ance tron the other in a particular eackange sequaccl,<br />

the less costly the use <strong>of</strong> the particulaz resourcs sorving<br />

as a basls for his power, the mo:e likely it is to be<br />

iPvoked, directly or indirsc~ly. Eovevsr, In ths abscccE cf<br />

"minimum cost" ~ ~ S O U Z C ~ S ,<br />

it is LRCBSEBZY +O Invoke more<br />

costly rBsourceS to achieve a goal. Ths use <strong>of</strong> violenc? (as<br />

illegi?inate rssource] tcrclsg another to comply vith ore's<br />

requests is probably much more costly to +n ir.dividual than<br />

the USE <strong>of</strong> mare legltimats resources such es occupatloral<br />

sta+us, prestige, educatioral attainmexts, personal<br />

attributes, etc., vhich enrlce the other to colply woes<br />

VOluDtanily.<br />

2. For ease or ccmaucrcatior. from this point on, when<br />

the term "'resources" is used it should be taksn as waning<br />

"valued materral possessions and personal characteristics<br />

-- other - - - - than -- - physlcal sfrergth," that is, resources othfr than<br />

+he actual or potectial use <strong>of</strong> physical force.<br />

3. Slmilar ideas ~+VE been expressed many time5 before<br />

and keep reemerging. For example, in Parsons' 1947 sssay on<br />

"Certain erimary SourCPs and Patterns <strong>of</strong> Rggression ic the<br />

Soc131 Structure or the Restern world," in Pollo nay's eeYg=<br />

---<br />

and Inrocerce (19721, ir research on "locus af cartral"<br />

(Goodstad? and Rjelle, 1973), an& in van den serghe<br />

(1974:778-787). P2rsons' analysis not only asserts the


Ch.12. Resources and Pover Pa3e 206<br />

basic proposition that aggressive acts ars carri?d out<br />

"...mare out <strong>of</strong> weakness and handicsp thar out<br />

<strong>of</strong>. ..strength," but also mentions several <strong>of</strong> tha fsctors<br />

included in the measure <strong>of</strong> individual resourcss usel for<br />

this study. Sp2cificz11y. he discuss~s the uss <strong>of</strong><br />

aggression as a node <strong>of</strong> response to anxiety, arguir.9 that<br />

the secure and nonanxious persor is less likely to aggrlss.<br />

He 8150 asserts that in a society valuing individual<br />

achievement, *allure to +chieve is a po'ent source <strong>of</strong><br />

frustratloc and hence aggrsssicn.<br />

9. <strong>The</strong> items indsxirg vi3lerce in the versror <strong>of</strong> the<br />

CTS used vrth this sample are: I. Threv soa~thing (but cot<br />

at the other) or smashed something, J. Threatened ts hit or<br />

throw something. K. Threv something at the othor, L.<br />

Pushed, grabbed, or shoved, 8. H i t or tried to hlt the<br />

other person, but rot with anything. N. H i t or tried t3 hit<br />

the other with scaethirg hard. <strong>The</strong> following rDsponse<br />

categories and score weights ars used for ill ifsss:<br />

Never = 0, Once that year = 1. Two or three tiass = 2,<br />

Often, but less than occe a month = 3. About once a month<br />

= 9, Mor? than once a mocth = 5. <strong>The</strong> <strong>Violence</strong> Index<br />

consists <strong>of</strong> the sun <strong>of</strong> these response category weights.<br />

This lndex has a racge <strong>of</strong> 0 to 25. However, to avoid giving<br />

Excessive wight to a rev outlying cases with exzremPly high<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> violence, ths T ~ Y ECOf9S were rmc01+d int3 a<br />

modification <strong>of</strong> the "ggr(* score nlrmzlizing ~DC~.P:~U? vith<br />

a range <strong>of</strong> 0 to 8 (see Straus. 1974a:footcot~ 6).<br />

5. Because oi t ine Limitations 13 *.!is quastion~aire,<br />

only six items were used, rather than the eight usld by<br />

Blood and Wolf€: what car to ge+? How fani.19 irrcns is<br />

spent in general? Rherc tc gc on a racatior? Which house<br />

or apartment to take? whether mother should work or quir<br />

working? ThiLgs concerring the children's activities? <strong>The</strong><br />

response categories used were: mother always = 1, nother<br />

mar? than father = 2. Father and mcther exectly the<br />

sans = 3, Father more than mother = 9. Father alvsys = 5.<br />

6. <strong>The</strong>se four irdexes (i-e., Bbsolute and Rslativs for<br />

husbands and for wires) were arrived at on the basis sf an<br />

empirical searchicg procedure. Specifically, we first<br />

Searchzd the questionnaire to identify everything that<br />

seemed as though it would fit the idea <strong>of</strong> an lntricsic or<br />

extrinsic resource. Sixteen such itlns or scares were<br />

identified. We then carried out an "sxternal criterion item<br />

analysis" (Straus. 1964:354) to find thcse items that Were<br />

in fact correlatsd vith our paver oeasure. Each sf ths<br />

eight items makrng up thcse indexes was select?d because it<br />

was found to be correlated vith power for the husband 3r the<br />

vlfe, or both. <strong>The</strong> itens were stardardized to a rang5 <strong>of</strong> 0<br />

to 100. Each respondent's score on a given index ther<br />

consists <strong>of</strong> the mean <strong>of</strong> the eight standardized items and can<br />

therefore range from 0 to 100.


ch.12. Resources and power Psqe 207<br />

another way <strong>of</strong> thinking ebaut these scor~s rs chat each<br />

is expressed as percentage <strong>of</strong> the maxiaum passibl~ score.<br />

Thns. a respordent with an Absolute Resource index <strong>of</strong> 28 has<br />

an ararage <strong>of</strong> 28 pErcent <strong>of</strong> 'he maximum possible points for<br />

the sight resources. B =espondcrt vith a sc3re 3f 28 oc the<br />

R~latLVe Resources index has 28 percent 3f the raw scors<br />

obtained by the respondent whose resources excse5ed that<br />

his or her spouse by the greatest amouct.<br />

<strong>of</strong><br />

Finally, it is rnpartant to ncts that because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

w~y in vhlch these indexes were arrived at, they are simply<br />

a way <strong>of</strong> summarizing into one convenient measure what would<br />

otherwise require flght correlations or eight table rows.<br />

In particular, these index-s cacnct be used to "test' the<br />

resource theory hecause they include only itens zlrsady<br />

knOwP to be assuciated vith pcver.<br />

7. For PUIPOSES <strong>of</strong> this study we dlvided +he sample<br />

rnto Occupational class groups based on the husband's<br />

occupation. If the husband was ergaged in macual vork, ve<br />

classified the family as vorking class. If the husb3nd was<br />

ergaged in nonnanual vork, the family was classified as<br />

middle class.<br />

Rsaders should he evare <strong>of</strong> the folloving two criticisms<br />

<strong>of</strong> thls procedure. (1) Some would fir.d it prefsrable to use<br />

the concepts <strong>of</strong> "blue collar" versos "uhite collar*, bscause<br />

<strong>of</strong> the controversy over what cocstite*?s a sncill class and<br />

the basis for assignmert to classes. (2) Besing the<br />

classification <strong>of</strong> the cpmelg OP a characterstic <strong>of</strong> the<br />

husband has a bullt-in sexual bias (Steinwetz and Straus,<br />

1973). Our use <strong>of</strong> this pzcced3n4 leflects ??.a lov<br />

percentage or vives who vere employed cutride th? homo. ?ad<br />

we tried to use bcth occupetiocs, the number <strong>of</strong><br />

un~las~zf~able cases would have resnlted i" an excessive<br />

1055 <strong>of</strong> Cases *roo ths aralysis.<br />

8. <strong>The</strong> product moment correletions given in Figure 2<br />

und~rstzts the extent to which these data ara cocsistent<br />

vlth the resource theory. is 2 check to see if curvilinear<br />

patterns vere prssert (which <strong>of</strong> ccursE vcull sh2v up as<br />

correlations rear zero), we plotted the mean pover sccr? for<br />

each decile cf the reSOUrce SCOrES. <strong>The</strong> resulting gr3phsr<br />

vlth the exceptions to ha nlted later, did not indicate<br />

curvlllnear relationships. However, they did suggest a<br />

closer assoclatlon <strong>of</strong> reSourcEs with vi31enc~ thlf 1s shown<br />

by the correlation coefficients. We think that correlations<br />

Ere low because <strong>of</strong> the large variance associated with the<br />

means we plotted. Ir addition. occasional nonlinear<br />

relitions ars seer. For example. the correlations <strong>of</strong> the<br />

husbacd's power vith the wife's relative resourcss is<br />

Only -0.09 for the mldale class sample. while this is in<br />

the directior the resource theory preaicts, it is alss no:<br />

Statistically significant and very low coefficient.<br />

Houcver, the plo- for this rslationship shows a graauai


Ch.12. Resources and Power ?age 208<br />

d~creaS9 in husbard's pouer as the wife's resourzes increase<br />

relative to his, until the ninth aec:le, at which point<br />

there is a precipitous drop in husband's power. Thus, the<br />

lox corr~latior. reported in Figure 2 is due to this<br />

threshold effect, acd also variance around the ten means.<br />

<strong>The</strong> only other nonlineer relationship we were sble to<br />

discern is that b e t ~ powsr ~ n 2nd the husband's vi3lsnce<br />

within the middle class sample. Busband's vial~ccc was<br />

found to he greatest ir wife dominant faniliss, hut also<br />

tends to be high in extremely husband dominant families.<br />

So. it Seems 5s thmgh middle class husbards resort to<br />

physlcal force bath when their ascribed superior position<br />

has been taken away, 2r.d also when they have 3 power<br />

position that goes beyord mere Superiority to one ir which<br />

then wivss have +lmost no decision pouer. See Straus,<br />

1973, Figure 2, for a plor <strong>of</strong> this relationship and further<br />

discussion.<br />

9. It is important to recognize that rhe superior<br />

reSOUrCFS <strong>of</strong> huSbaPdS were n3t achieved in open comp~citian.<br />

because <strong>of</strong> the ic~.umerable impediments to achievemect for<br />

Yomen in nost societies. <strong>The</strong>refore, Even if ons accepts<br />

superlor resources as providing a moral justification for<br />

superior power. the serist structuring <strong>of</strong> opporturity Fr~ the<br />

soclety renders such an ethlcal justificatio~ inrsXd.<br />

10. Those b~lov the median on each resource ixdsx vere<br />

classified as low and those above, as high in resources.<br />

Working class ard middle class faniliss Vera classified<br />

separately to reflect the large class difference in these<br />

resonrces, and on the assongtion +hat beirg "high" 2r "low"<br />

in resources is rolativo to the average l?vel 3f res3u:cns<br />

in the sector <strong>of</strong> society in vhich one participaias.<br />

11. A graphic illustration vas recently provided is the<br />

Bergman fllm, P'gn a &rrL+qq. Numerous<br />

illustrations, together with stetlstical data, are in cslles<br />

(1979). In the prsssnt sample, the mean violence scare is<br />

0.90 tor worklng class and 0.33 for middle class husbends,<br />

3rd 0.65 for working class and 0.39 for middle class wives.<br />

Expressed as a percentage vhc used niolencs in rhs referent<br />

ysar. the fzgures are 20.5 percept for working cless +Ed 9.6<br />

percent for middle class couples.


art V an icy<br />

ns


<strong>The</strong> ideas, research nethods, and rssearch<br />

results in this book could be summarized in 2<br />

number <strong>of</strong> ways. Poi example, we could poict out<br />

and further illustrate certain themes which ruc<br />

through almost all the chapters, such as the<br />

cultural norms legitimizing violence, or th?<br />

conflicts inherent In the<br />

famlly. 01 the 1019 <strong>of</strong><br />

"aintalnlng male dominance.<br />

organizatior <strong>of</strong> tha<br />

phystc~l viol3nce in<br />

<strong>The</strong> method tirally chosen ts highlight an6<br />

summarize Some if the main themes is to ask what<br />

the practical implications are <strong>of</strong> the theori?~<br />

and facts car,teined in this book. Consequently.<br />

the tinal chapter identifies six charecteristics<br />

<strong>of</strong> Bmsriczn social structure that, togethsr.<br />

brirg about the high lcvel <strong>of</strong> husb3nd-"if=<br />

violepce. For each <strong>of</strong> these factors Lhsre is =<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> what can be dons to changs things<br />

to reduce *he level <strong>of</strong> rarFtal viclrnse. In<br />

all. 21 different "policy implicatisrs" wpre<br />

deduced. Considerable evidence ?xrsts thst som?<br />

<strong>of</strong> these ?oL?cias would reduce viol%?.c?: the<br />

other policl~s ire proposed on the strength <strong>of</strong><br />

reasonable speculaLloc. Fortunately fs: those<br />

seeking to rsduca the level <strong>of</strong> marital violencl,<br />

the question <strong>of</strong> vhether pro<strong>of</strong> can be given that<br />

the policies w i l l actually achxeve a reauction<br />

is almost beside the point; sost <strong>of</strong> the 21<br />

policy recommerdetions ars steps that arr<br />

socially desirable in their orr right. Psr<br />

example, we do not know how much a lou?r level<br />

<strong>of</strong> economic insecurity and un?nploy@ent w i l l<br />

reduce assaults by husbands on their wives. But<br />

since full aeploymen: and a basic alnimun incone<br />

is a social good. we "111 has* gained ever if<br />

movement toward that social good has nJ Effect<br />

on the level <strong>of</strong> marital violence.


Chapter 13<br />

A Sociological Perspective<br />

on the Prevention <strong>of</strong> <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating<br />

Murray A. Straus<br />

Our ideas about the causes <strong>of</strong> wife-beating obviously<br />

influence the steps ve take to prevect it. If wife-b'sters<br />

are thought to be mentally ill. thsn psychotherapy is<br />

clearly needed. If husbands hit their wives because moderr<br />

society puts excessive atrairs on the nuclear f%mrly, then<br />

some reorgazization is needed <strong>of</strong> the roles in ths family and<br />

the family's relation to the society. 1'. is importsnt to<br />

recogn:ze that this chapter is co?c?rnea only vith the<br />

social causes <strong>of</strong> husbard-vlfe violencs, and only vith<br />

chznges in socie?y 'hat might prevent marital riolercc.<br />

With this in mind, the chapter w i l l summarize some <strong>of</strong><br />

the ways in which vife-beating is produced by the very<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> our socisty and its family system, and at tho same<br />

time attempt to foraulata policies aimed at reducing tho<br />

level Ot husband-wife violence.<br />

Some recommendations below advocate general social<br />

charge5 rather than tho more specific aspects <strong>of</strong> "Fieily<br />

Policy Analysis.' as dsfined by Wilson and BcDOnala (1977).<br />

In most cases, however. the suggestiocs listed below are<br />

clearly Daripulable by law or practice in the public realm,<br />

Or by the policies acd decisions af businsss and other<br />

private orgar.xzations.<br />

CULTURAL NORBS PERB<strong>IT</strong>TING WIPE-BE<strong>IT</strong>ING<br />

E fundamental zspect <strong>of</strong> Eaerican sacial structure that<br />

must be understood and corfronied is the existprce <strong>of</strong> the<br />

cultural norm that, as rotsd in Chapcer 3 and elsewhere.<br />

Bakes the marriage license also a hitting licecse. But C ~ P<br />

coccept <strong>of</strong> marriage license as hitting license is contained<br />

rot oniy in the folk culture. Nore important, es noted in<br />

Chapter 3 and 6. it remairs embedded in the legal system


desplte many reforms tavcring uomen.<br />

A great deal <strong>of</strong> other evidezce supports tha existPncs<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 'hittlng license* norm. I vill have to assume rhat<br />

the i7:tormation pr~s~ntsd in Chapter 3 md 6 makes thc case<br />

plauslbie. What then are tb.e implications for prev+otion?<br />

<strong>The</strong>re seem to be et least two parallel "policy implications"<br />

(PIS).<br />

PI 1. Bake the public aware sf this lerg=ly<br />

unperceived norm.<br />

This policy implication has a paradoxical quallty, but<br />

it is proposed on the +ssueption that awareness can<br />

contribute to the demise <strong>of</strong> the "hitting licaxsc n9re."<br />

because S U C ~ 3 norm is contrary to other norms and values<br />

about the faxiiy. It w i l l pave th9 uay tovara + sscond<br />

policy impllcation, <strong>of</strong> specific benpfit to husbards and<br />

wives, but Especially to the latter.<br />

PI 2. Redefine the oarital relatiolship as one<br />

in which ary use <strong>of</strong> physical fcrce is as<br />

unacceptable as it is between oreself and<br />

those with whom one works, bowls, or play?<br />

tennis.<br />

<strong>The</strong> speclfic policy charges lmplied by PI 2 are<br />

illnsrrated in the consfrt decree recently siqnnd by *be N-w<br />

York City Police Department (New York Times. 27 Jurs<br />

1978:l). This dccree cblfgates the Poiica DapartInsrt to<br />

arrest men who commlt feloni~us assaults cr other f3l$nies<br />

against their wires when there is reascrable cause to<br />

believe the husband committed the czime. In aaditiar, the<br />

agreement provides that the police n3tify ?ha wife sf her<br />

rights. including the right to make a cztizen's arrest with<br />

the aid <strong>of</strong> the police. <strong>The</strong> police also w i l l be rfqUirEd to<br />

remain at the scene <strong>of</strong> the crime temporarily to protect the<br />

yife Or to assist her in obtaining medical assistaxce if she<br />

requests it. If the husband hcs left the scene, the polic?<br />

must try to locate him an they would anp other suspect.<br />

B more detailed discussion <strong>of</strong> changes in the criminal<br />

justice system vill be found in the discussioc <strong>of</strong> PI 19.<br />

POT the individual wife. this means making clear to her<br />

husband that physical force simply vill not bz tolerated.<br />

In an unklovn, but perhaps oat insigniflcart proportior <strong>of</strong><br />

csses. this a1028 conld serve to alter the situation bscause<br />

the "h!ttino 1icsPse~f es~ect <strong>of</strong> marriaoe is so nuch ar<br />

2 &~~~<br />

n~perceilred "taken-for-granted" norm, ind is 50 ccntrsry to<br />

Other widely acknowledged and valued<br />

marr;age relationship.<br />

norms concerning the<br />

By th~ms~lves,<br />

such icdividual attempts 2t redcficicg<br />

the marital relaticnship to render violence illegitimate are<br />

unllkely ro be sufficient. In the first place, normative


Ch. 13. <strong>Wife</strong>-Eeating Page 213<br />

rules are only one structnral determinant <strong>of</strong> hahavisr, afid<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten a minor aetsrminant. In the second place, such rules<br />

do not arise from thin air. Rether, they reflsct, ana tend<br />

to be integrated with, a network <strong>of</strong> other cultursl alrepnts.<br />

Perhaps ever more, they rsflect tha reelities <strong>of</strong> dsily<br />

llvlng.<br />

problem<br />

C<strong>of</strong>sequently. I truly useful approach to the<br />

<strong>of</strong> wife-beating must eddress these more fanaam~ntal<br />

causes. <strong>The</strong>se e l e m ~ are ~ t ~ so closely interwoven that it is<br />

n~arly as difficult to discuss them separately as it w i l l he<br />

to chafgs then. Eonever. they can at lsast be groupFd irtc<br />

somewhat neaningtul patterns.<br />

WIFE-BEATING 1.5 R FEFLECTION OF SOCIETeL VIOLENCE<br />

<strong>The</strong>ss examples <strong>of</strong> goserfnsntal violence provide<br />

pow~rful mod~ls for the behavior <strong>of</strong> icdividual cirizens.<br />

Thsy form er important pert <strong>of</strong> a mors geraral norszt:vp<br />

sys?am that hllds that uiol€rc= can and should he use3 to<br />

attai~ <strong>Social</strong>ly desirable ends (Blumsc?hal c: $1.. 1972,<br />

1975). Of COUTSE, it is sxtr~mEly<br />

difficult to prova that<br />

go~arnmental violerce provides a role model for icdiviaual<br />

~iclefcs, but one typs <strong>of</strong> evidence supporting this<br />

conclusion is exEmplified in Chapter 4 and in Archsr and<br />

Gartcer (1976).<br />

suggins and Straus' study (see Chapter 4) locked at a<br />

sample <strong>of</strong> English language children's bolks covering the<br />

period 1850 to 1970. <strong>The</strong> original purpose v%s t3 see if the<br />

~ E P B ~<br />

<strong>of</strong> inwrpersonal violence depicted in these books<br />

shoved an upward or doucward trend over this 120-year<br />

periJd. <strong>The</strong> resnlts showed no trena <strong>of</strong> this type. e3vever.<br />

even :bough there were cc "war stories' in the sanplr <strong>of</strong><br />

hooks, durifg 2rd immediately follouing szch msjor var the<br />

f1Eg~Fncy <strong>of</strong> Lct~rpexsoral violence rose dramatically.<br />

Similarly, archer and Sartner (1976) found a p3stwar<br />

increase ir homiclda rates for a large sample <strong>of</strong> cations.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y concluded that the increase in murder rates was due to<br />

a carry over <strong>of</strong> the xer-time authorized or sasctioned


Ch. 13. Rife-Beating Page 214<br />

killing. <strong>The</strong>refore:<br />

PI 3. Reduce to the maximum extent possible the<br />

use <strong>of</strong> physical force as an instrument <strong>of</strong><br />

government.<br />

tlegra <strong>Violence</strong>. violence 11 the mass media both<br />

reflects the existing hioh level <strong>of</strong> aggression and rislence<br />

if American society and helps perpetuate that pattern. <strong>The</strong><br />

typical citizen watches "prime time' TV in which more than<br />

half <strong>of</strong> allcharacters are icvolved in some violencr, and<br />

one <strong>of</strong> ten in killing (Gerbilsr and Gross. 1976). <strong>The</strong> aoouni<br />

<strong>of</strong> gratoitous violence in current motion picturss is alsc<br />

extremely high. <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> these facts has beer<br />

explored by intensive research, incloding a number <strong>of</strong><br />

excellent longitudinal acd experimental stuatss, during 'he<br />

past 10 years. <strong>The</strong>se studies have led almost all scientific<br />

reviewers <strong>of</strong> the accumulated evidence to cocclude that<br />

violerce in the media is part <strong>of</strong> i societal patterc that<br />

keeps America a high-violerce society (Surgron Geceral.<br />

1972).<br />

<strong>The</strong> message <strong>of</strong> the mass aedia is clearly that physical<br />

force can and should be usod to secure sccially desirable<br />

eras, not just in the "vild west." but in allcsi all aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> contemporary life. llthough it is rare tor the media to<br />

depict husbands using physical force on wives, th? more<br />

general message is easily ?ransferred to ?te marit31<br />

relationship. A t presan+, I know <strong>of</strong> r3 direct sridsnce %tat<br />

the high valus inpliciCly placed 02 both instrumenlsl and<br />

expressiw violence in the mass media is tracsferred ts the<br />

marital relationship: however, this transferral seams so<br />

likely in view at the extEnsire evidence <strong>of</strong> the phPnomenon<br />

that psychologists call '*transfer <strong>of</strong> training' thi' *he<br />

fcllouing pollcy implication seems warzactea:<br />

PI 4. Limit violecce in the mass media as much<br />

as is consistect with preservicg fre?dcn <strong>of</strong><br />

expressioP and artistic integrity.<br />

Essentially, PI 4 calls for reaucicg the extert to<br />

which television and flctian and nonfiction works "exploit"<br />

v~ol


Ch.13. Wlfe-Beatl~g Page 215<br />

half ot a11 American households coltax a gun, most <strong>of</strong> which<br />

are "hand guns%$ rather +han "sporting guns." <strong>The</strong> following<br />

proposal is add:assaa to +his sbtuation:<br />

PI 5. Enzct strlrgent gun control l;gialatio?,<br />

particularly restricticg hacd guns, hut alsc<br />

requirirg thar all guns he kept 13cked acd<br />

unloaded.<br />

PI 5 has hsen ep'ly termed "domestic aisarmamprt" by<br />

Reatai Etzlori. This policy could go a l2ng way toward<br />

reducing the most extreme aspect <strong>of</strong> domestic viol-nce:<br />

aurder. Of course, domestFc disarmament w i l l 3at rsduce<br />

violerce ES s:, sirce ore can still punch, kick, choke, or<br />

kmfe. But an attack with t gun is much m3r? liksly to be<br />

fatal than are other modes <strong>of</strong> attack.<br />

".<br />

.HE FRETLP PS TWINING GROUND FOR VIOLENCB<br />

What has befc said 50 fer ~nphasrzEs the extant to<br />

which riolerce ir thb fanly reflects the leva1 <strong>of</strong> vi=lerce<br />

i!! the society. But the cther side <strong>of</strong> thc coin is at least<br />

equally important; the level <strong>of</strong> violence in all aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

the socis'y, including the family itself, reflect3 what is<br />

learned and grrerallzed frQm vhat happens ;?. tte ?z%ily.<br />

beginring with icfancy.<br />

- Pfisis& PUTiShnex. <strong>The</strong> implicit models for hfhavioz<br />

provided by actions <strong>of</strong> the gavfrnmsrt ar.d dapic-ed ic the<br />

mass 6ed1a form two legs <strong>of</strong> the stool supporting R~ericaI!<br />

viclerce. <strong>The</strong> third leg is the family itself. In fact, the<br />

family may play the most crucial role, b=cause the family is<br />

the ssttirg in vhlch most people first experience physical<br />

OiOlencD, and hecause that. violence is expsri?nced ir a<br />

strong emotional contexc. Specifically, at least 90 psrcent<br />

<strong>of</strong> parents USP physical punishment in their chrlarsr'? early<br />

years. Noreover, far thou* half <strong>of</strong> all children. this<br />

continues through the end <strong>of</strong> high school--essentially until<br />

ths child leaves hone (Bachman. 1967: Steicmetz. 1974:<br />

streus, 1971).<br />

<strong>The</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> physical punishment ic tra5nir.g the<br />

next generation <strong>of</strong> violent citizens was dsscrihsd in<br />

Chaptsr 2. I2 the forthc~ming bcok giving the results <strong>of</strong><br />

OUT catlonal survey <strong>of</strong> violence lc families (Straus. G~llcs.<br />

a ~ s+.einaetz. d . 19791. . . 0r.e cha~ter sives deteilea soidenc?<br />

supporti~g this relationship. We found *hat th= mare<br />

physical punishment a child experienced, the m3r% ~ i 3 l ~ ~ t<br />

his/her marriage years later. This correlation is present<br />

for "ordinary' physical punislment, but it is particularly<br />

strong when physical purishment is used haavily.<br />

punishment, then, lags the groundwork for ths<br />

Physical<br />

Darnatire<br />

lsgitieacy <strong>of</strong> intrafzaily violence ara provi3es a


Ch. 13. <strong>Wife</strong>-Bsatirg PSgc 216<br />

roiP-rnodel--ir~d8~d a speczfic "scrip?" (Gagno3 ard Simon,<br />

1973: Buggins and Straus, Chaptsz 4)--for bath tho<br />

perpetrators a s :& q&=:i~s <strong>of</strong> such actiocs. Gelles<br />

(1376). for Fxampls, found tha' One Of the thr== main<br />

tactozs linked Lo a urfe's tolera?ing abuse from her husbacd<br />

in the exiert to which her parents hit her as a child (see<br />

also Lefkoxitz pi.. 1976). an impartsct policy<br />

Impl~cztlon <strong>of</strong> the fects just pr~sfrred should be:<br />

PI 6. Gradually sliminaie physical punishmert<br />

2s a mode <strong>of</strong> child rearirg.<br />

I have used +hs +erm *~graduallyW in farmulatlng this<br />

policy implicatior even though my own v3lues fsvsr ?mm=diats<br />

cessation <strong>of</strong> physical punishment. nsny prsctical<br />

drfficul?les<br />

diftrcultles<br />

sfand<br />

that,<br />

in<br />

if<br />

the way <strong>of</strong><br />

disregarded,<br />

lomediets csssation,<br />

cculd have ssricus<br />

Consequences. Specificelly, ve csuld not exp-c? to<br />

elinrrate physical punishment until paT3ntS 530 be pz~vided<br />

al?ernati~e tech~iques for carirolling the behavi2r <strong>of</strong><br />

children, to prot~ct thsm from dtngqr and Zo teach the<br />

pract:c21 skrlls ard sthtcal values for which society holds<br />

parents r?sponsibls. That a feu parPn*s n+rlg? to bzicg up<br />

children without physicel punishasr* does ro: imply that<br />

most parents can do so. Such methoas must bs validat+d<br />

befcre VB risk undsrmirirg the vital tssks sf s3cialization<br />

carried out by parents. Forturatrly, sora sffsct~re<br />

techazques are begirning ts emerge (see r=fer=nces follcwing<br />

PI 8).<br />

slQ&go xi&ggc_q. Elaost as uciversal ss physical<br />

punishment is physical fightirg betvasn children ir the<br />

family. Perhzps such fighting is icevitabl? in early<br />

childhood. But it is rot lneritable that iitacks or each<br />

Other by brothers asd sisters be regarded as much less<br />

repreh??!Sible thac attacks or or by ucrelated children.<br />

This diff?rerce zr. 'he way iden'ical acts <strong>of</strong> violence are<br />

evaluated and handled syabolizes and ruinfcrc?~ the<br />

leglfimacy <strong>of</strong> riolenc~ betweer fzmily eemb?.rs. 4s a rssult,<br />

V~O~PPC? toward sihlings coc+5rues lang aft?r vi9lance<br />

toward peers has disappeared. For sxample, amsng .he sample<br />

studied by Straus (197Ua). almost tuo-thirds hsd hi: or been<br />

hit by a brothsr or sister during 'he year they Her? skgLgg~<br />

1h high school, canpared with th- one-thira who r?p>rt;d<br />

hittr~g or being h ~ by t someme outs id^ the family. Thus,<br />

right through high school, many youcg people ?xperi?nc9 a<br />

5EC3nd aspect <strong>of</strong> irtrzfamily violercs that implies that<br />

nothrng 15 ierrtbly wrong about the use <strong>of</strong> physical force<br />

bstween family asnbsrs.<br />

then:<br />

To the sx'ent that this occurs,<br />

PI 7. Fncaurage pirepts to control acts <strong>of</strong><br />

physical force between their children and to<br />

avoid erplicitlg ?r implicitly defrning such<br />

2CtS 26 p?~.1S5ible.


RS in the cans or physical pulishaent, ispl?m=ctirg<br />

PI 7 is not wersly a mat'er <strong>of</strong> ceas5ng Fo do something. One<br />

<strong>of</strong> the circumstances highlighted by the social~gical<br />

pers:ective 1s that Elements <strong>of</strong> the social structure are<br />

int~rwc~en. and rh~refore one <strong>of</strong> them Cannot hi triaiEa in<br />

iS01+TiO~. In thls case, we must ask: "What is *her? about<br />

the s:tua:ior. <strong>of</strong> children in a family thet gErerates such a<br />

hlgh level <strong>of</strong> ~iolencs?~ znd "Row can chlldrer resolve their<br />

di5agree89nts withcut physical fights?'l Ortil children are<br />

sgulpped vith the skllls ta do that, it<br />

.<br />

is<br />

.<br />

as urrealistic<br />

for pare~ts to implcre "dccqt tight" 3s ;r IS for Eaa:ly<br />

life educztors to implore parents rot to spank.<br />

Co~s~guently:<br />

PI 8. Provide paref:s ald children with<br />

tochrlques other then force and caerciar Ear<br />

ccplcg with and resolving the inevitlble<br />

conflicts <strong>of</strong> fesily life.<br />

nany obsraclfs bar the way fcr iap1ansn:ing PT 8, one<br />

<strong>of</strong> the most inportic+.oo: which is Eiscuased i n s&ctior E <strong>of</strong><br />

this chapter--the feilure to recognize the iccvitabili'y <strong>of</strong><br />

ictrafaeily cenflict, and hence to take sfeps far copicg<br />

w:th conflict n9nv:olantly. Eut even LE that vers rot a<br />

zactor, what techriques are available? Rlthough still i<br />

matter <strong>of</strong> research +Ed cortrcv-rsy, promisin? mc2t.sls fcr<br />

resolving parent-child ard sibllng-sibli'g corflic: havr<br />

bee!! developed over the last feu years (8lechman f: la.,<br />

19762, b: O'Dell, 1974; Pattersor pi 31.. 1975).<br />

"~~3+c-SensozjIII Depiivatio~. Barry Aarlov orcD<br />

ep~tomized the resulls <strong>of</strong> his classic ~xperimer?~ xith<br />

monkeys rsared xr. iso1at:cn by saying that mockeys daprived<br />

ot warn sac~al contact in infancy z*...~ould rether fight<br />

than love.' <strong>The</strong> sane idea has surfaced ic a rumbar <strong>of</strong><br />

different ways 11 the h5story <strong>of</strong> social science: for<br />

example. ln the work OD -he autharitsrian p?rsoneliCy <strong>of</strong><br />

Fdorzo gr a&. (1950). Par' <strong>of</strong> what Edorco's "P %=alenT<br />

rneasnres is the propensity to use physical niclsnc4 for<br />

socially desirable ends. People who get high 'F %c+len<br />

SCOT~S, f3r fXamplt, fezd to fa~or the d?ath psnalty 2hd to<br />

feel that sex crimilals should bs bath imprisocSd and<br />

". ..publicly whipped, or worse.8r ldorno $$ 81. found thet<br />

+~CSI sane psopl3 also received ralatively isss lave ard<br />

affection from their parents than did those lcv cr the "P<br />

scale."<br />

Blst rec~ntly, Prsscott (1975) has pcinta8 to<br />

neurophysiological and crnss-cul'ural zvider:~? shoving that<br />

the more a persor is deprived cf a'soeatcJlr.scry<br />

gratification," such as irtimets physical contact, love, and<br />

affectioc, the greater the level <strong>of</strong> aggression, Fnclu3icg<br />

physical aggressior. For Example, a tabulation 3f 2++a for<br />

99 socleties rev~aled thst thi societies that do no2 plovlae<br />

much physical affectlor to chsir children also tend t3 show


Ch.13. uife-Beating Page<br />

a high level <strong>of</strong> violence between adults. Sircs 3 lcvina<br />

affectionate ctlldhood tecds to inoculat~ persocs<br />

societies against violenc4, it s ~ e n likely ~ that t<br />

background would be<br />

violence in +he tanily.<br />

particularly efflcicious agai<br />

<strong>The</strong> pclicy implicatlan that fallows from this find<br />

15 not that parents "should be" warm and affsction<br />

because by nov that has PEcoae part <strong>of</strong> the standard Ameri<br />

child-reerilg ideolcgy (as compared with the "school <strong>of</strong> h<br />

knccks" and the "don't spoil the child" ca?c-ptian,<br />

Ratt.er. the policy iepllcatior rscoqnires that, d66pit~<br />

ideology <strong>of</strong> wermth ard affectioc, millions <strong>of</strong> childrpn<br />

ir fact deprloca <strong>of</strong> just that (idorno t 31.. 1950; Pee<br />

1963; Levis. 1971). Consequently:<br />

PI 9. sponsor research to determine ths social<br />

and psychclogical conditions that cause soml<br />

parents :o be cold and distant rsthar thxn<br />

warm and lavirg. and translate the rFsults<br />

into programs to assist such parents.<br />

TEE INEV<strong>IT</strong>ABIL<strong>IT</strong>Y OF CONFLICT I N FABILIZS<br />

Contlic:, ir the serse <strong>of</strong> differerc?s ir =bj=ctiv~<br />

"interests* Detvssp persons and between groups. is<br />

leeritable part cf all human association (Coser, 19:<br />

DihrE~do~f. 7959: Simmel. 79081. Some srcuos tend zn<br />

groups, :be level <strong>of</strong> conflict is particularly hi<<br />

Chapter 1 describes same <strong>of</strong> the charactsristics <strong>of</strong> :<br />

familythat give rise to its rypicslly high 1evc.l<br />

contllct.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se 51elET charact~~i~tics <strong>of</strong> the family are by<br />

means a ccnplet~ list <strong>of</strong> factors that produce confli<<br />

Ro~ever, they should be suffici~nt to indrcats that :<br />

family is typ~cally the locus <strong>of</strong> a high lsvel <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

th9 5a.s time :hat it :s the locus af a high Lsv;l<br />

ictsrpersonal support 3rd love. noderr society dces 1<br />

provide adequate eechanlsms for convi3Lent resolution<br />

these conflicts. First, the fimilyTs pzivacy and soparat5<br />

from close ties with neighbors and relatives cuts it r<br />

from the asszstacc= in solving conflicfs thst such gin5<br />

can p~ovlde. <strong>The</strong>r? IS PO one to turr. to for h51p. SBCO?<br />

this same privacy and isola+.ian frcs kin and ceiqhbors ssr<br />

that feu or cc agents <strong>of</strong> s3cial control exist to bl3ck i<br />

use <strong>of</strong> physical force. consequertlp:


Ch.13. u~te-Beat:vg Page 219<br />

PI 10. Fedace the iapect <strong>of</strong> government progrars<br />

and regulatiols that, directly or indirsctly,<br />

enconrage geographic mnhili'y or reduca ties<br />

to the extended family.<br />

T h x pclicy w i l l be Even more difficult $3 implsment<br />

than a+ny <strong>of</strong> the <strong>of</strong>hcrs suggested in this chapter, tot a<br />

nunbsx <strong>of</strong> r5250~~. First. the ar* acd sciencs<br />

impact analysism is only now bsglnning to<br />

<strong>of</strong> "fzmily<br />

hs explnrsd<br />

(8inncsota Family S*uay csnter, 1976). Rsiae from a few<br />

obvious programs (such as those policies that giu= more<br />

encour3geme~t to buildirg rev neighborholds than ro<br />

preserving ?he quality <strong>of</strong> existicg neiqhhorhoods), simply<br />

identityl:y the rel5vant programs and governsent regulations<br />

w i l l be a slow, uncertain process. Second, those pr3grzci<br />

that are ide~tlfifa w i l l usually be ssrvirg sons import?.<br />

purpose. Corsequently, the policy vould be not ~errly<br />

latter <strong>of</strong> ending so~efhlng, but even more a mattsr - ~<br />

firdllg alternatives that do not pncooraga mobility and *b'<br />

redoction <strong>of</strong> eatsndsd family ties. Finally, the aid an?<br />

support that intiaate commnnities aaa kir provids are no? an<br />

Onmixed blessicg: they c2r be stif1ic.g st ths Sam- tiap<br />

that they are helpful.<br />

We hare already sugq~s'ed :'a? 3ur cccillingnrss to<br />

recogxize the high level nf cosfllc: in families is irsplf a<br />

source <strong>of</strong> ~iol~cce. 95 long as ccnfllct within the family<br />

is sieved as urorq, abnormal, or illegitimate, people w i l l<br />

be reluctart tc learn techniques for engaging ir conflict<br />

nonviole~tlp.<br />

PI 11. P B C O ~ ~ ~ ZihP E ine~ltabillty and<br />

ieqitiazcy <strong>of</strong> conflict within :ha family<br />

rn-her than<br />

dfvlat:cn.<br />

corsider conflic' an abn~raal<br />

Once the inevitability end legitimacy <strong>of</strong> conflict<br />

within families is recognized, th9 way is open for the<br />

media. marrlege counselors, churches, and hnna. service<br />

agencies to help families rind cocstructive ways <strong>of</strong><br />

resolvirg conflicr~. Many <strong>of</strong> the methods cited in the<br />

refsrs~ce~ followirg PI 8, and those described in secti3ns A<br />

ald I, are dasigred to do just that.<br />

en important aspec-f these methods is that thEy are<br />

ifitsneed for normal farilies; they make no assump*ions<br />

2bout psychop8thclngy. Irstead, these neihods assume :hat<br />

the tasily mmbers need tc learn more efficient vsys <strong>of</strong><br />

solving irterperscral problems ind proca?d to tsacb those<br />

tecnniques by rovel, nonaorallstic behavioral mlthods. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

focus on teaching people &p= t o solve problems. not on !&t<br />

the solotion to the problem is.


Although the inevitability <strong>of</strong> mayital coaflict stauld<br />

b= rscoqnizfd, it is also inportact to av3Fd the trap<br />

represent?d by ore uing <strong>of</strong> ?he 'encounter grcup' aoo€Bert,<br />

which has i:s parallel anolg a number ?f marriage cours~lors<br />

an3 writers <strong>of</strong> marital advico books,<br />

vyasn's ihp Ipsptf: ==rap ('1968).<br />

such as Each and<br />

Each and Wvlr-n urge<br />

their readers to drop +*outaod~d ~otions <strong>of</strong> etiquett?" ard<br />

vertilate their Enger. Duricg oce group ssssion, one 3f the<br />

authozs urged the xnm=n participants, "Don't be afraid to be<br />

a rsal shrsu, z real bitch! Get rid cf ycur pent up<br />

hosrilities! Tell *hem whore you're really it! Let it be<br />

total, vicious, exaggerated, hyperbol?!" (8ouard. 1970:54).<br />

llthough Each ard nyden3s book cortaics 3sserti3rs to<br />

the coctrary. the overall aessags <strong>of</strong> the bock, as I re36 it,<br />

urgES wives to do just what the quotation suggssts. This<br />

advice is based cn a "catharsis," or "vsntilition' thecry <strong>of</strong><br />

agqrsssinn cortrol. That thearr bnyins with the assumptior<br />

that all <strong>of</strong> us hava in cur natnr4 a greater or lesser<br />

tfnde~cy toward aggression that soneh~v must find<br />

expression. If we attsnpt to repress +Pis dpep hiologicallp<br />

based motivatior, our innate aggression w i l l only cause an<br />

9xplosion at some later tims.<br />

3it---ur?+=iy ror ~trr? h;:<br />

21.?E? TC FT":::CB? T"sC::3"\-t.<br />

.ir- 2r-c~ -: .:uc? '!VICP,<br />

ILY )Ti-':" " C7:C1-:!1C<br />

r:o?r +onnrns -t= -borrv. 3-c T U C ~ OI :- E$CYC 7t.e :=Y-YSO:<br />

. &<br />

.<br />

. that opporturitirs to ohserre aggression or to be aggz,ssivs<br />

tena to produce grea,f:z suhsequnr.t levels <strong>of</strong> aggression and<br />

viclence (serkovifz. 1973: EaKarson, 1970; Steinlstz and<br />

Strans. 1974; Straus, 197U). In genersl, aggression<br />

against another (either verbally or physically) tends Io (a)<br />

produce courter aggrsssior. (b) Impsass getting tc the real<br />

problem, and (c) 2f it does succfad in soue1chir.g the cthrr<br />

pefson, reiPforces -he use if aggression as a mode <strong>of</strong><br />

:nteractlon.<br />

'let<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is. havsver, a ksrnel <strong>of</strong> truth underlyina 'hs<br />

it a11 hang out" and "ventilation" approacbPs to<br />

marriage. <strong>The</strong> difference must b? percsived between<br />

assertion (s~anding up for cne's intsr2sts) and aggr?ssion<br />

(acts carrisd out with the ictezfian <strong>of</strong> hurting tte other].<br />

A~serli~enE.55 is essential: but one cac De assertive<br />

vlthout beilg aggressi~~ (although always with the risk cf<br />

aggression being imputed). For example, the critical first<br />

step5 <strong>of</strong> 'gstti~g help,' "canceling the hitting license,"<br />

aca "makiT~g clear that ane is prepared to 12aVe,' ire all<br />

highly 25SFTti~2, hat nocaggressive zc's. Sscond.<br />

25~5rtiver~ess is rec~ssary if there is tl be 2ny h~pe <strong>of</strong><br />

resol~~ng the co~tlict ~ver which the vi3lsn~~ occurs. If<br />

c02tl:ct arises over the children. ssx, noney, or running<br />

the household, then these issues must 50 faced.


Ch.13. Wrfe-Beating Pzge 221<br />

Procedures for ratronal conflict rescluticf, <strong>of</strong>ter<br />

comblned wlth systems tcr revardllg occurrences <strong>of</strong> desired<br />

beha~iOT, are cextrel concerns cf tho recent *'marriage<br />

encounter' movement (Koch ar.d Kcch. 1976: nace 2r.d Eace,<br />

1974) and <strong>of</strong> behavioral^ th~rapists llke PlFChnan st zL,<br />

(19762, b). Patterson (1975). and a nulnbfr <strong>of</strong> 0-hers who are<br />

r~pres2nt~d ic the chapters ot an important new b~ok or<br />

'TTpazi Elaxranships (Olsor. 1976; see also Jacnbscr and<br />

nartir, 1976). Ore can say that a prircipal goal af +hese<br />

approaches to "treaclng relationships" is ths inprovemirt <strong>of</strong><br />

i~terperso~al skills, inclndirg ass~rtlveness, so thlr the<br />

legitimate iLtsrFs*s 3f 111 partics can be furthered. This<br />

kind ot therapy say also have the zdvantsge <strong>of</strong> being less<br />

threateclng and more attzactive to husbands. If they rccora<br />

with prev~lllng masculins role models, een are mare<br />

rel~lctant t h a ~ UOmer to hesh ovsr their chilahool or prssent<br />

emorlcns 2 ~ their 6 psy~hologi~al status, as required ic the<br />

tradltionil 'insight" therapy. <strong>The</strong>y pre"2r tc 2ns%ils<br />

actions 2nd results DOZE than histo:? ald p2rsoxil5ty. and<br />

these are precisely the foci <strong>of</strong> the lev marriage elcountor,<br />

oarrlage snrichrner.?, and marriage counseltog approachss.<br />

Perhaps ;he most pervasive set <strong>of</strong> factors bringicg<br />

about wife-beaticg<br />

structure <strong>of</strong> ths<br />

are those<br />

family acd<br />

cocnpcted<br />

society.<br />

with the s=xist<br />

In fact. to a<br />

considerable exrent. *he cultural norms ard valurs<br />

permrtting and sometimss encouraging 'usbard-CJ-wife<br />

v101erce retl~ct the hierarchical and male-dominart +yp= <strong>of</strong><br />

SCCiEty rhzt characte~i~es the western world. <strong>The</strong> ilght '0<br />

use force exists, as Goode (1971) co?.cludes, to p1ovi3~ the<br />

ultimate support fcr the exisrirg povez structure 2f the<br />

tamily. in case thoso low ir the hierarchy rsfuss to accept<br />

*heir place 2nd roles. Nine ct the specific usys i2 which<br />

the male-domitant structure <strong>of</strong> the society and <strong>of</strong> ths family<br />

create 3rd mi:nta:n a high level <strong>of</strong> marital violscct were<br />

described ir ChaptEr 6.<br />

Since many mer musr fall back or the "ultimate<br />

~DSOU~CE' <strong>of</strong> physical tlrce to aairiaic their authority (see<br />

Chaptsrs 10 and 12 and Goode, 1971). it fcll3ws that a<br />

pclicy thrt reduced this peed would be helpful:<br />

?I 12. ElimiCaiE the husband as "hela <strong>of</strong> :ha<br />

familyfs from its continuicg presence il tha<br />

lav, ir reltgiol, in admicistrativ~<br />

prccPdare, and 35 a take3-for-grznted aSpECt<br />

<strong>of</strong> family life.


Ch. 13. Hlfs-Beating Peg= 222<br />

Alth3ugh progress is being made in respect t5 the<br />

achievement <strong>of</strong> husband-wife equelity, the idea thit the<br />

husband 1s head ot the family remains firmly rootcd in<br />

American culture (see the survey :sported ir. Pxza_$p. 1971;<br />

also ROlb and Straus. 197u and the conclusion to Chapfpr 6).<br />

In United Stated governsen? stalistics, the only time a<br />

VomaP can he classified as the head <strong>of</strong> a household is when<br />

no husband is physically present. No praviston is made for<br />

lisfirg joir~t head <strong>of</strong> household. Only through thc activa<br />

pursuit <strong>of</strong> the goals <strong>of</strong> the fnairist nlveaent is signiticaor<br />

change llkely. noreover, the importance <strong>of</strong> the feminist<br />

movemePt goes well beyond husband-vrfe equality, ard it w i l l<br />

be impossible +o eliminate sexism :n the fanily urtil it is<br />

also eliminated ir the society at large.<br />

Although the elininatior <strong>of</strong> sexism in the family is a<br />

historical Charge at vast magnitude, aspects <strong>of</strong> sexism are<br />

Ulthln th? immediat~ control ot Individuals. Por example,<br />

both tor her OW protection ar.d as a contribution to the<br />

overall policy objective, no voaan sh~nld Entsr marriage<br />

without it being firmly and pxg&lyit&p underszocd thaf the<br />

husband is ~ o t the hezd <strong>of</strong> ths family. Unl~ss stated<br />

Othsr~~se. the irpliclt marriage contract includes the<br />

"standardw clause about male leadership. Chirgtng ftis<br />

cortract after marrlage is roT c ~ ~ 6iff;cult. l y<br />

but gives<br />

rlse to feflirgs c: having been misled or chsated.<br />

Althgugh objections nay he made to intr3ducing these<br />

ideas ir junicr and senlor high school classes on the family<br />

(as lcdicated by recent congressional pressure on the<br />

natiocal SclEnce FouLdatiox that rssolted in en3ing support<br />

fcr curriculum projects ic rr?hropology and Psychllogy) ,<br />

nary local school districts w i l l find such content<br />

appropriata. Ir addltlan, the feminist movement can<br />

Contirue to chall+age the implicit support cf male dominant<br />

faally relations ir Eagazines for young vonpn sucb 25<br />

sexQT,zser.. 9~&&, and GagogK.<br />

Ecotomic Constraints and Dlscrimlcation<br />

L+ck <strong>of</strong> .?CO~OE?~C ~l?srratirs8 to depscdence 02 the<br />

husband is ancther ore <strong>of</strong> the three aain factors thit 6elles<br />

(1976) found associeted vith beaten wives' remaining vith<br />

thelr hosb~~ds. Thus. tor womsn to be in s p0s:tion ir<br />

which they can refuse to tolerata physical cosrcicn by their<br />

husbands, ccc~pational ard economic equality are absolutely<br />

essent121. Consequently, one <strong>of</strong> the sost fucdsaental policy<br />

lmpllcati3ns is:<br />

PI 13. ElimiPatE the pervasive systsm <strong>of</strong><br />

sex-typed occupations in which "womsn's<br />

9cCUpatioT.S" t9r.d tc be poorly paid. and the<br />

equally psrvaslve difference between the pay<br />

ot Een and womer in the same occupation.


~urder.~ <strong>of</strong> Child Cars<br />

S~xnally based divisio~ <strong>of</strong> labor thahasiqns<br />

child-r~aring rssporsibilty to th? wife, occupational<br />

discrlminatlon. lzck ct child-care fzciliti~s, icadequate<br />

child support from either the gcvernaent or the father-all<br />

coerce women to remain married even when the victims <strong>of</strong><br />

vi0ltPCE. R busbard aoes not need to fear that if he beats<br />

h:s wits ara th? uif- lezv~s, hs w i l l be rssporsible for<br />

both tht care ot the child ard the need to earn sufficient<br />

ir,came. so, 2 hushaid car hit (acd oth?rvise oppress) his<br />

wlf4 with relative impnnity. Re can be reas3nably confidsn+<br />

th2i if she does leave, he w i l l not hav4 the children ucless<br />

he insists on it; courts are reluctact to award childrin to<br />

fathers in ary circuastancss. No shame is icvolved for a<br />

rather who clzims that the child w i l l be best <strong>of</strong>f with the<br />

mc?her, but for a mothez to say +he chila is he?-cr cff with<br />

the father is cot only shaneful, hut :n many c%s?s vill<br />

cause +he chi= to be institutiocalizsd or placed in a<br />

f~stsr home. Child care responsibility is cnly one <strong>of</strong> many<br />

examples <strong>of</strong> SEX-typed r~los that hind wossn to violent<br />

marriages. Sherefo?f:<br />

PI 14. Reduce nr ellmicare the sex-tppel<br />

pattern <strong>of</strong> faslly role risponsibilities.<br />

Es in the care <strong>of</strong> sexual stersctyping ir the paid labor<br />

force, lnterest ard ability rathsr than sex need to be the<br />

primary crl*eria for who does what. aoreover, this is a<br />

policy zmplica?ion 'hat, like that in respect t3 paid<br />

employment, is dssirable regardless <strong>of</strong> its Effect on<br />

wife-beating. Just as many (but not all) woman w i l l find<br />

greater tulfillme~t through eguzl participation ic the paid<br />

12bOr torce, many (hut agaic not all) non vill find grsater<br />

fulfillment than they cow eapsrience in equal participation<br />

It the hcosehola labor force. That possibility is now<br />

denied to me2 because <strong>of</strong> the shame actached to men's shoving<br />

major irterest ir househola work a d child cars.<br />

PI 14 is a long-range type cf social ch319~. 20d we<br />

need not wait for it LO cnae about. In the meantime. other<br />

steps say be taker to aid vomex :rapped in a riolsnt<br />

marriage by the necessity cf assunxng responsibility for<br />

child care if the mar:lage breaks up:<br />

PI 15. Estsblish or subsidize a comprehensive<br />

ard hlgh qualiiy system 3f day-care centers<br />

£Or pr~SCh0Ol children.<br />

Egail. thls policy is long ovszdue in its own<br />

cat just for its potential ir preventing wife-beating.<br />

right.<br />

Such<br />

ta~iiitiE~ are needed by millions <strong>of</strong> vomec who<br />

sstlstactory marriages.<br />

erjoy fully


<strong>The</strong> thEEe BSpSCts Of th9 sfXiST StrUC?UTe Of the<br />

soclety are listed below; thei: imFlicatiocs for<br />

wife-boating Wers described ic Chapter 6 and need ajt be<br />

repeated here.<br />

myth <strong>of</strong> the Sixgle Parent Bousohold<br />

Preeminence or Wlfe Bole for Wonen<br />

<strong>The</strong>se aspects and the nary other ways in which women<br />

are disadvantaged (and tterefore less able to 2nd a vi3lent<br />

marriage) suggest thet fur.darneltel charges shculd be made.<br />

PI 16, 17, and 18 are xot policy analyses in the specific<br />

sense used by Wilsor. and t3cDorald (1972:l-2). but are<br />

guidelines tor needcd chzcges in public laws and private<br />

practlcss. <strong>The</strong> mcst iaportact pollcy implication <strong>of</strong> all<br />

those put forth ir. this chapter is th3t:<br />

PI 16. Pull sexual equality is essfrtial fcr<br />

prevention <strong>of</strong> wife-beating.<br />

A t +hls poi~t we must make clear an important<br />

liaititlon to much cf what has be?n said. Sexual equality<br />

ir ;tsslf is almosr ceetainly gnf going to er.8 cmflict and<br />

viol~nce between husbardr and wives. Equality vill reducs<br />

or eliminate =*+air, klnas <strong>of</strong> carflict, but, at the same<br />

time. it w i l l crfata new conflicts. Issues that are rat now<br />

the . ~~ ~ subiJCt - ~~ <strong>of</strong> dlsameemert i~ million5 <strong>of</strong> families--such as<br />

2~~ ~<br />

who w i i l work tor wages and who vill vork in the hcas?, or<br />

more saecific issues. such as who vill do the launarv--can<br />

no longer be determined by referring to the tradi~ional<br />

pattern ot tamily roles. Rathcr, these issuss becOmE open<br />

gu"sticn~. OV4r which severe conflict can arise. It is by<br />

no means incor.ceivablc that nsif&x partner w i l l Wact to be<br />

In the paid labor force, or that npzf,hg; vill want to d~ th.<br />

laundry. Consequectly, a rsduction in the level <strong>of</strong> vl3lerce<br />

also requires tha? couples have interpirsonal and<br />

contlict-ma~egenert skills needed to cope with, ard realize<br />

the befiefits <strong>of</strong>, a less rlgid family systea. Millions <strong>of</strong><br />

p-ople lack these skxlls:<br />

then.<br />

almost all <strong>of</strong> us can rnprove<br />

In addition. I? would be shcrtsighted. evsn dsrg=rous,<br />

to ov~rlook t h cos;s ~ <strong>of</strong> freedom ard fleribilizy. Pr~edom<br />

ard f l ~ ~ i b l l ir. l ? faeily ~ pztterrs and sex roles remove sons<br />

<strong>of</strong> the foundatiors <strong>of</strong> stability and security in life. Not<br />

eVarycCe<br />

Promm's<br />

flnds rhe berefits worth these :os?s.<br />

(1941) classic book gscg~l fre! E~e:ppg<br />

Prich<br />

was<br />

co~cerned with far more than -he reasons thi? fascism had<br />

such wide support. A t +he Other end <strong>of</strong> ths continuum. the


Ch. 13. W~te-Beating ?age 225<br />

mllliors <strong>of</strong> women cppcsed to the Equal Rights emccdmpct and<br />

the ternmist novenen: reflect the anxisty that mazy uc~sc<br />

feel over the possible loss <strong>of</strong> familiar and stable gui3es to<br />

lits. Tbsrotare:<br />

PI 17. 36 the EOC~C


Ch. 13. Vlfs-Esatlng Paq~ 226<br />

Male O:len+.ation <strong>of</strong> the Criminal Justice syszem<br />

Not only is much malr violence agairst wives<br />

at'ributable ?o the sexlet organization <strong>of</strong> society, hut the<br />

male-criented orgarizatior. <strong>of</strong> <strong>The</strong> crilinal justics system<br />

v1rt3elly quazactees that fsv women w i l l be able to secure<br />

legal relief (Fields. 1978).<br />

But even if a woman gars to Family Court, unless she<br />

has unusual und~rstandicg cf axd abilliy L3 menipulate +ho<br />

System, there is <strong>of</strong>ten a three-weak delay before her r=?u9st<br />

for 3 "peace bord" or ec Oforder <strong>of</strong> pzotection" comas bsfore<br />

the judge. Such orders ire therefor9 <strong>of</strong> 10 gre3ter help ir<br />

secnring imeediate pratectior from alath~r zssa2lt th3n the<br />

police <strong>of</strong>ficers described in ChapLer 3. men ~ithout these<br />

delays, many voaan carcc+ sttfnri court beczusa <strong>of</strong> -ha lack<br />

<strong>of</strong> child-carp arrangensrcs dnriig tb~e long h?urs <strong>of</strong> vaitir~g<br />

for a casa to corns up, frequently repeated when the case is<br />

rescheduled.<br />

Chapters 3 ard 6 detail mary impediesnts ta securing<br />

legal pro*ecticn agiirst essault by a husband, incluaing (a)<br />

legal delays, especially in ohtainicg a "peace bond" or an<br />

"ard?r <strong>of</strong> protec?ion"; (b) iminuriiy from suit by one's<br />

spouse; (c) ths requirsnent that, despite aburdint physical<br />

e~idenc~, the police ctticer must witness the atCack before<br />

ar, arrest can be made: (0) the frequent failure <strong>of</strong> police<br />

to arrest ere2 W ~ E P thsy do witness an asssult: (E) ths<br />

"coalinq out" by police, prosscn:ing a5torneys. and lodges<br />

<strong>of</strong> wives who attempt to bring complaints: end [f) ths<br />

refusal to maka an award b~ public conpe~sation review<br />

boards (Even ir cases <strong>of</strong> psrmanprt disability) if the irjorr<br />

"3.5 inflicted by the hnsbard. A l l <strong>of</strong> these fzct3rs indicate<br />

the nEed for thn following policy:<br />

PI 19. El:mixaLe from the criminal justics<br />

system the implicit tcleration <strong>of</strong><br />

vife-beating shcvn in sia'utary an6 common<br />

law; a-i'udes <strong>of</strong> tbe police. pflse~utor~,<br />

and iudoes: and cumbersome and insffec?iv?<br />

Same movement in the dircctior <strong>of</strong> PI 19 is ncv takinq<br />

plats. but ir 1s far fror a general trena. B chargs Lr the<br />

1Sgal system zequires a priority activity by vsll ozg+nlzea<br />

feminist g:oups. 8s in the "Now wife Assault Pr2gramn ic Enn<br />

B.rb31, fiichigan (FOLjik, 1976; Resnik, 1976) or in the<br />

cccaslotal enlightere6 polic~ departacnt that rsccgnizes the


Ch.13. <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating Psge 227<br />

need to rrcrient its mode <strong>of</strong> copicg viih 'family<br />

dlsturbarce' calls (Bard ~2 81.. 1975).<br />

ECONONIC PROSTRBTION BND VIOLEACE<br />

PmEriCan society, liks most socie:ies, is one in which.<br />

trom early childhood on. people learn '3 TESPOP~ to<br />

trustrat:on aid 5tress by aggression. This r?Spons? is not<br />

inevitable biologically<br />

since ID a few societies people learn to. acd typically<br />

do. respord to frustratior in other ways. N=v?rthel-sa.<br />

aggressive response 2s typical <strong>of</strong> this roclsty and is<br />

1rkeIy to reaair so ir the foreseeable futo:?. For +his<br />

reason, and because a policy dLrec:ed to rsducing<br />

frus:ratlon would be desirible in irs own right, we shoula<br />

gLvs high pziarrty to enable as may psrsons as possibl~ to<br />

avold slCuatlons cf oxtrems frustration cE important llfs<br />

goals. This proposal is by 30 mfars tha sams as a%temFtirg<br />

to crea&.o e life vithou? frustration. Such a life, even if<br />

it were possible, would be empty, and w3uld probably be a<br />

source <strong>of</strong> violence in rtself (see the discussion 5f the<br />

TlncksorX Orangew theory <strong>of</strong> violerce in Gelles and straus.<br />

1978). Aouever. a major blockage ot a critical life gaal is<br />

quite another th1r.g.<br />

a+ny critical lite gcals are (or perhaps<br />

beyond the realm or social policy to facilitate.<br />

on which thcre is high cocsensus, 35 well<br />

should be)<br />

But a goal<br />

as a high<br />

possibility <strong>of</strong> a ~ h i ~ ~ ichange, r g is the provisior <strong>of</strong> a<br />

meaningto1 sccupatioral rcle and an adequate love1 <strong>of</strong> ircomo<br />

for all faailies.<br />

in rndusrrisl societies, tho husbacd's positicr <strong>of</strong><br />

le~dership depsnds cn tho prestige and earning power 3f his<br />

Occupetion. Consequcn+ly, it the husband is unaeplcyed or<br />

does cot earn 2n amourt consistent with other men in rhe<br />

fanlyvs nftvozk <strong>of</strong> associates, his leadsrship positisn is<br />

n~dermin~d. Data from a srudy by O'Brierr (1971) shov '.hat,<br />

when this happens. husbards tecd -0 t ry to maintain their<br />

superior position through the use <strong>of</strong> physical forc-. Dat3<br />

from my study <strong>of</strong> the parerts <strong>of</strong> universizy studeots shov<br />

that ihs percentage <strong>of</strong> husbands rho struck thair wises in<br />

the last year rarges from a law <strong>of</strong> 9 and 7 percent for 'hose<br />

whose U:VBS are alm05t completely or completely sa:isf:ed<br />

with theLr family income, op to 16 and 18 percent for those<br />

whose xives are slxghtly satisfied or nlt at 311 satisfied.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is 2150 e~idelce that asseults on wives<br />

onemploymen: (Parade. 1971:13: Straus,<br />

Steinnetz, 1979).<br />

go up<br />

Gelles,<br />

with<br />

and


a<br />

In discussing the sexist organizatian <strong>of</strong> the faaily as<br />

cause <strong>of</strong> uife-beatlng, we poixted ont that if husbsrds no<br />

longer had the burden <strong>of</strong> being the "head <strong>of</strong> th? family" and<br />

the a3in "breadwinner," they vould no' ~ e to ~ resort d to<br />

~iclence to maintair that position in situations where the<br />

w:fe is mare cornpeter;, earrs more, or has a Nore<br />

prest:gious occupation. <strong>The</strong> same reasoning ipplies, pirhaps<br />

even nor6 strongly, vher the husband is unamployed.<br />

Clea~ly, the nost furdam?ntal change needed is male<br />

liberation trom the bcnds <strong>of</strong> fraditiocel sex roles. Pt the<br />

sam5 time, ve can pursue e policy that, aside from its<br />

int:~?.sic worth. 15 likely to TE~UCS wife-beatirg. It is<br />

stark in its simplici%y and powerful in its €ffE:t on human<br />

welfare:<br />

PI 20. Pull employment for all ser and women in<br />

the l+bor force at wage levels consistsnr<br />

w:th the standards <strong>of</strong> tte society, and s<br />

guaranteed incorns for those o?.ab19 to work.<br />

hsld~ from its impac' nr uifs-beating, ir. avoiding ma<br />

or the most severe frustratiocs thsL a,ps:soo can exparLance<br />

In en rndustrlal society end in bypassrng issues <strong>of</strong> power<br />

within the family, full employeent exerts a powerful effect<br />

an ssli-esteem. Kaplan (1975) has shown that the lnu?r an<br />

adolescent's self-esteem, the greater the likelihood <strong>of</strong> his<br />

being violent; Kaplan's data suggest that bops 13w in<br />

self-esteem 59ek to aChlEVe r~cognition from others thzongh<br />

rialeace. This bshavlor, sf course, is tied to the oquation<br />

<strong>of</strong> mascu1:~ity with aggresslvensss. Consequartly, when<br />

recogrition through achievemect ir school. sports, 3r an<br />

occupation is lickirg, sales car and do demonstrate their<br />

"manhoodv through uiolercs. Again, the more fundae~ntal<br />

policy abjsctlvs is to chenge the definition cf nasculirity.<br />

But as Long as ;hat definitioc continues to be a part <strong>of</strong> our<br />

culrure, full employmPnt can help avoid invokrng this aspect<br />

<strong>of</strong> "eachood" through providing meaningful employment as a<br />

bas15 fOT SSlr-8Steel.<br />

B more radical approzch to this relatiocship bstveen<br />

economic frustration and vife-beating fccuses on what<br />

Critics <strong>of</strong> Americen sccisty see as the inhueen occupafional<br />

and economic system. Such czitrcs ar? not opposed to full<br />

enployment, but they do oppose the scorosic and social<br />

sys.e.2 that judges humai worth by ~arcings and competitive<br />

cccupatlo~al achle~~me~t. 85 long as such a systpm<br />

pTeV3.115, the bulk <strong>of</strong> the popularion is deoied a!? adsguate<br />

level <strong>of</strong> selt-est.sem because only + mirority, by definition.<br />

can be it the top in occupa+ioral prestige ana incorn-. I?<br />

additlor, the strivirg to get to the top pushes the more<br />

humac valuss '0 subcrdin3re position. Ties JE frierdahip,<br />

kin, and community. for example, are regularly sacrifices in<br />

moving to get a better jch or accrpting a promotion.<br />

Consequently, the folloulng policy is proposed:


ch. 13. Rife-Beating Pag? 229<br />

PI 21. Reduce the extent to which scci?ty<br />

evaluates pecpls or the basis <strong>of</strong> their<br />

eccnonic achievemeres and redvcn thi<br />

occupation+l and economic competition 'hit<br />

this ectails.<br />

<strong>The</strong> implicaticn <strong>of</strong> PI 21 is 33: the end t3 all<br />

competition. Coapetitior can be plsasurable, if on? can<br />

rbno=e tha arena -- <strong>of</strong> ~- ccaoptition - ~~. and if there is a rt=asacable<br />

~ ~ --<br />

chalce <strong>of</strong> vinlirg. Bather, the policy suggssts the need to<br />

end ;he forced and (for most <strong>of</strong> the papolsticn) ro-win<br />

competition that now characterizes our 3ccup+tional-sc3nomic<br />

SUnillRY AND CORCLUSIONS<br />

Physical violencs betveer hushand and wife 1s a c9mnor<br />

an inevitable part <strong>of</strong> human na'orp. Ocly i? rare<br />

but C D ~<br />

i155anc=s is it ?.P OUZgrOYth <strong>of</strong> p~thn13gical BZlE<br />

aqgressiueiess, or <strong>of</strong> fenale masochism. Parhe=, the typical<br />

pattern <strong>of</strong> hushand-wife oiolence, and its extreme Fn the<br />

form <strong>of</strong> 'wife-bsating." is largely a rpflection 3f the<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> the socisty, its family system, ard its typlcg <strong>of</strong><br />

sex roles znd male/fenalG pernonalirylraits. C3r.segusrtly.<br />

the focus <strong>of</strong> this chap+er has been ro deduce from these<br />

social factors ?he policies that, if adoptaa, would reduce<br />

the level <strong>of</strong> ~lf~-b~a


Ch. 13. <strong>Wife</strong>-Baatlng page 230<br />

------ Factor ZIJ. THE PBCILY IS TAE PRIMABY SETTING IA WHICR<br />

VIOLENCE IS LEBRNED<br />

6. Gradually sliminate physical punishment es a mcle <strong>of</strong><br />

child rearing.<br />

7. EncCUZage parents to control acts <strong>of</strong> physFcal force<br />

between thei: childrer and to avoid pxplichtly or<br />

implic:tly detining such acts as psreissible.<br />

8. Provide parents and children with tschniques other than<br />

torcn and coercion for coping with azd resolving the<br />

inevltahl~ conflicts af faailp lifs.<br />

9. Sponsor rs~earcb ta detsrmine th? sccial and<br />

psychological c~ndltions that cause some parects to be<br />

cold and distant rather than warm a2d l3vinq. +I&<br />

translate the results into programs :o assrsc such<br />

parents.<br />

&r;er 21. THE INEV<strong>IT</strong>ABIL<strong>IT</strong>Y OF CONPLICT IF TE9 FRflILY<br />

10. Reduce the impact <strong>of</strong> government proarams end<br />

regulations that, directly or indirscLly, anc3crige<br />

geographic mobility or reduce ties to the extended<br />

Family.<br />

11. ReCOgniZE the lr.evitability <strong>of</strong> conflict within the<br />

family rather 'heo corsider cmflict ar abccrmal<br />

deviation.<br />

Factor 1. SEXUALLY STSREOTYPFD POLES RND SEIISil i N IRE<br />

PRnILY PND TRE SOCIETY.<br />

IliaiPEte the husband as 'head <strong>of</strong> the family" froe its<br />

co"tinUFPq presence i~ the law.<br />

administratrve procedure, and as a<br />

in religioc. in<br />

'sksl-for-gre<strong>of</strong>ed<br />

aspect <strong>of</strong> family lifs.<br />

Eliminate the pervasive system <strong>of</strong> sex-typad occupations<br />

in whlch "women's occupations" 'end to be poorly paid.<br />

and the equally pETV351Pf difference between<br />

<strong>of</strong> men and woeen in the same occupation.<br />

the pay<br />

Reduce or eliminate the sex-typed pattern <strong>of</strong> family<br />

role responsibilities.<br />

Esfab1:sh or subsidize a comprebensivs end high quality<br />

system <strong>of</strong> day-care ce-ters for praschool childr?n.<br />

~ u l sexual l equality is essential<br />

u~te-beatirg.<br />

for prevention <strong>of</strong><br />

AS the S O C ~ E ~ ~<br />

~limi~ates flx7d sex-roles. ~ltern3riv~<br />

sources <strong>of</strong> stability and security in sslf-deficition<br />

"ill be needed.<br />

Parent-chLld interaction, parcntal expectations, an6<br />

all othfr aspects <strong>of</strong> socialization should not be<br />

diffe:ent:ared accordilg to the SEX <strong>of</strong> the chilS.<br />

Eliminate froe tha criminal justice system the implicit<br />

toleration <strong>of</strong> wife-bea+ing shown in statutory and<br />

common law; the attitudes <strong>of</strong> the police, prosecut.ors,<br />

and judges; 2nd cumberscne and in~ffectise proc4dure.s<br />

tha+ make even the available legal remedies and


---<br />

protection ineffective.<br />

PPCDR !I. PRDSTRATIORS BUILT INTO TEE ECONDBIC SYSIPE<br />

20- Fell employment for all men and women in ths lsbor<br />

rorce a+ wage levels consistent vizh the standards <strong>of</strong><br />

the society, and a guaracteed income for thos~ unahle<br />

tc work.<br />

21. Beduce the extent to which society evaluetes people on<br />

the hasis <strong>of</strong> their economic achievemints, ana reducs<br />

the Occupational acd eccnosic coapetiti3n th%t this<br />

entails.<br />

Elthoughthe primary focus cf the chipter uss on<br />

pclicles that w i l l Eexez wife-beeting, *.he desperate<br />

imsadlate sztuaticn <strong>of</strong> millions <strong>of</strong> wives must also be<br />

addressed. ConsequeCtly, the last ihird nf tb* loroar pcyr<br />

frcm which this cha?ter was exc?rpt=d (straus, 1977) is<br />

devoted to steps that individual wives mszried to an<br />

assaultive husband can take, and to actions fsaiFist and<br />

community groups, police. end human service ageacl?~ can<br />

take. to cops vixh tte immediate problem.<br />

Changing a pheronener as deeply %bedded in the sacial<br />

system as wife-beating is a vast undertaking. Ss many<br />

actims are needed that one almost does cot know vhcre to<br />

start. Ir fact, a realistic zpproach recognizes that there<br />

is no one place to start. Rather, a broad publiz awareness<br />

a ~ d comnitm~nt to change is necessary. so that individuals<br />

and groups in all spheres <strong>of</strong> life can attend tc changes in<br />

each af these spheres. For example, change in the legal and<br />

law e~for~ement system w i l l cot in itself end vife-bsa'irg.<br />

Rut, :he police, lawyers, judges, and legislators can act to<br />

remov~ some ot the aaly barrlers that now prsvent vaasn from<br />

~Cc~iving legal protection from<br />

stat"^, u~less the assailant USES<br />

beatings. Thus, in most<br />

a weapon, the police<br />

cinrat make an arrest, even if the rife is obviously injured<br />

and the husband makes nc attempt to decy her charges. (She<br />

cal, however, make a "citizePqs arrest" and icsist that the<br />

police help her--provided she has sufficisr? preserce <strong>of</strong><br />

mind, self-confidence. ard determinatim. acd some plsce to<br />

hide when the husband is zeleased frnn jail an hour or two<br />

later!.) <strong>The</strong> law regulating the evidence needed tc mske an<br />

arxest f3r wife-beating can be changed, just as lavs<br />

regarding ths evidence needed for a rape conriction havn<br />

recently changed. Sirnilerly, putting a husbald in jail<br />

deprives the wife <strong>of</strong> her aeacs <strong>of</strong> support: this E3ct is<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten pointed out to women and is one reason s3 few SroPrely<br />

beatsn wives press Chargss. BOW EVE^, i C SO.€ states, 3<br />

pri.53~81 can be released for employment during working<br />

hours, and soch lavs could be enacted in other states if the<br />

socisty were tzuly determined to end vife-bs2ting.


Ch.13. <strong>Wife</strong>-Beating P39e 232<br />

This firal chapter has attempted to trace cut the<br />

policy implications <strong>of</strong> on1 knowledge <strong>of</strong> one aspect <strong>of</strong> family<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>: ulfe-beating.. <strong>The</strong>re is good riasJn for the<br />

emphasis an vife-beating. Womer are under greater risk than<br />

men <strong>of</strong> serious injury from physical attack, and they hare<br />

few alternatives for putting up with beatings by their<br />

husbands. In this ard ir previous chapters we pninted out a<br />

variety <strong>of</strong> social and economic cocstraints that lock vomer<br />

into marriage to a such greatnr extent than men. In short,<br />

as sltea in Chapter 2, women are victimize3 by violecce in<br />

the family to a much greater Extsnt than are husbands, and<br />

should therefore be t h focus ~ <strong>of</strong> the most inmediate rseedial<br />

embedded in the very structure <strong>of</strong> s0c:ety acd the family<br />

system itself. <strong>The</strong> solution to wife-bsating, like other<br />

forms <strong>of</strong> intrafamily violence, lies in the complex interplay<br />

<strong>of</strong> cultural and eoclal org+rizatioral fsctcrs surrxn3ing<br />

famlly life.<br />

NOTES<br />

*Revised version <strong>of</strong> part <strong>of</strong> "A Sociological Perspactive<br />

and Treatment <strong>of</strong> Uite-Eeating.' Reprinted with pernissicn<br />

from Bsria Roy, (€d.) Battered women. New Fork: Van<br />

Nostrand-Reinhold, 1977. This paper was first presented at<br />

the 1977 annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the American Psychiatric<br />

Association.<br />

I am grateful +o naria Roy and tl Pr<strong>of</strong>essors Richsrd J.<br />

Gel195 and Boward n. Shapiro, and Jean Giles-Sims for<br />

comm2ntS and criticisms <strong>of</strong> the first draft.


References


RBEOTT. SUSAN 1976 "Pull-ti~e farmers ard week-end wives:<br />

An analysis <strong>of</strong> al+erilg conjugal roles.,' JDurral <strong>of</strong><br />

Narriage and tho Family 38 (February):765-174.<br />

ADOSNO, T. W. E. FRENKEL-EFCNSVIR, D. J. LEVINSCIA, and<br />

N. SLNPORD 1950 Ths Authoritarian Personality. New<br />

York: Rarpfr ard Raw.<br />

ALDOUS. JOIN 1974 "<strong>The</strong> making <strong>of</strong> faally roles and family<br />

change." <strong>The</strong> Family Coardica?or 23 (July) : 231-235.<br />

EPPLEY. fiORTlnER H. and RJCEARD TRUMBOLL (?as.) 1967<br />

Paycholoq~cal Stress: Issu-s in RPsairch. ?trv Y2rk:<br />

~ppleton-ce~tury-cr<strong>of</strong>ts.<br />

ARCBER. DINE and BOSENASY GBRTNER 1976 "Violen? acts and<br />

violenf 'imss: A contemporativ9 approach +o pastvar<br />

h0aicid-e rates.' American Socio13gical SEV~PW 41<br />

(Decsnbsr) ~937-963.<br />

PRDREY, ROBERT 1966 <strong>The</strong> Terriforial Iaperative. Nsv York:<br />

Atheceum Publishers.<br />

EACH. GEORGE R. and PETEF WYDZN 1968 <strong>The</strong> Intina+e Fr~eRy.<br />

New York: Borrow. Pp.17-33. Blso reprictsa in<br />

Steinnetz and straus. 197U.<br />

PBCEflRN. JERELD G. 1967 Youth ir Transition. Ann Arbor,<br />

michigan: Institute for Sociel Rssearch. <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

nichigan.<br />

BEER. STEPNEN 3. 1974 opFffects on pOvar and tiiVisioL Of<br />

labor in the family." Chap. 7 ic Lois V. E<strong>of</strong>faan and<br />

P. Ivac Nye (eds.). <strong>The</strong> Employed nother il Essrica<br />

(seccrd Edition). Chicago: Rand ncbally.<br />

EAHR. STEPAEN J., CBAFLES 3. EOREBBLb, and YIKTOB GECAS 1974<br />

wAdalescant perceptions <strong>of</strong> conjugal power.' social<br />

Forces 52 (narch) :357-367.<br />

BRLSWICK, JZCK 0. ar. d CEBRLSS 1. PEEK 1971 "<strong>The</strong><br />

inexpressiv~ male: E trEgedy <strong>of</strong> Rmerica~ s3ciety." <strong>The</strong><br />

Family Coordirator (0ctaber):363-368. Elso rcpriated<br />

in Rrlsne and 3ero.s Skalrick (eds.) , Ictiaacy, Family,<br />

and Society. Boston: Little, Brown and Co., 197'4, PP.<br />

237-244.<br />

EANDUBA, ALBERT 1973 Eggression: E Soclal Learning<br />

Analysis. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall<br />

BANDURA A. and F. B. WlLTERS 1959 Adnlssc=nt Aggr=sslon.<br />

New Y~rk: Ronald Press.<br />

BANDURA, ALBERT 2Td P. 9. WELTETS 1963 <strong>Social</strong> Learning and<br />

Personality D€velooment. New Yark: Eoll, Rlnsharf an8<br />

Winston.<br />

BAaD, nORTON 1969 "Family intervention polica teems is +<br />

cOmm~City lental health resource." <strong>The</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Crimin~l Law, Criminology and Polics Scicr.cr 60<br />

(21 :247-250.


References Psge 236<br />

BARD, nORTON 1971 "<strong>The</strong> study and nodificatioc <strong>of</strong><br />

intra-familial violence.*+ Pp.159-169 in Jeraoe I.<br />

singer (ed.), <strong>The</strong> Control <strong>of</strong> hggrsssion ana Vi~lence.<br />

New York: Academic Press. Elso reprinted in Steirmetz<br />

and Straus, 1974.<br />

BARD, BORTON. t al. 1975 <strong>The</strong> Function <strong>of</strong> ths .orlice 52<br />

Crisis IPterventioc and Conflict nsnlgemsct: I<br />

Training Gulde. Hesbington, D.C.: U.S. Dspartmect <strong>of</strong><br />

Jnstice.<br />

~ -- ~~--.<br />

BART, PAULINE 5. 1975 ,,Rape daesr't end xiti? a ki~s." VIVA<br />

(June) :39-42 ard 100--102.<br />

BPSOP<strong>IT</strong>Z, HlROLD f$= gL. 1955 Enxiety and Stress: An<br />

Interdisciplinary Study <strong>of</strong> a Llfe Situa'f3n. Nsw York:<br />

RcGraw-Hill. - ~<br />

BELLAK. LEOPOLD and MAXINE ANTELL 1979 'An intercultural<br />

Study Of aggr1SRi~e bsharicr sl chi13zenqs<br />

playgrounds." hmericen Journal <strong>of</strong> Or?hopsychiatry u9<br />

191:503-511. . .~<br />

BEn, S. L. and D. J. BEn 1970 "Training the woman to krov<br />

her place: <strong>The</strong> power <strong>of</strong> a ron-conscious in<br />

D. J. Bern, Beliefs, Attitudes, a ~ d Humn Affairs.<br />

sslmon+.. California: Sroaks/Cola.<br />

BENEDICT. RUTH<br />

cultural<br />

1938 "Coitinuities and discontinuities in<br />

condltiaaing." . Psychiatry 1:161-167.<br />

Reprinted ix Clyde Kluckhohr and Esnry 9. Burray<br />

(sds.), Personality in Nature. Society, and Culture.<br />

New York: Alfred 1. Knopf.<br />

BERSER, AETBU8 A. 1974 <strong>The</strong> Comic-stripped :m?rican.<br />

Baltimore: Penguin Eooks.<br />

BERGER, PETE3 2nd HRnSPRIED KELLNEB 1969 "Barriage and the<br />

construction <strong>of</strong> raali+y." Diaganes 46:l-25.<br />

BFPKOU<strong>IT</strong>Z. LEONPRD 1973 "<strong>The</strong> case for bot*lirg<br />

PSyChOlOgp Today 7 (July):24-31.<br />

up rsge."<br />

BERNSTEIN, PSILIP 5. 1950 nhat the Jews Eelieve. New Yoxk:<br />

Farrar. stzaus ard Young.<br />

BETTELHEIB. BRUNO 1967 'Children should lssrn about<br />

vlolePc5." Saturday Evening Posz 240 (flarch):lO-12.<br />

A150 reprinted in Steinmetz and Straus. 1974.<br />

BETTELAEIfl, BRUNO 1973 'Bringing up childrec." Laaies Borne<br />

Journal lo (October) :32 if.<br />

BICXEAN, LEONlRD 1975 "Bystander intervention ic a<br />

CfimB.' Paper presentea +t International Advanced Study<br />

Institat? On Victimology and the NFeds <strong>of</strong> ;ontemporary<br />

Society, Bellagio, Italy. July 1-12,<br />

BLIU. PET% n. 1969 Exchange ar.6 Power<br />

1975.<br />

in <strong>Social</strong> Life.<br />

New York: John Rilsy and Sons.<br />

BLAO. PETER 1975 Bpproaches t0 the Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

StrnCtUre. New York: Free Press.<br />

BLECHMBN, ELAINE B., DAVID 8. I. OLSON, 3rd I. D.<br />

BELLSIN 1976 'Stimulus control over family<br />

problem-solvi~g behavior." Behavior <strong>The</strong>rapy.<br />

BLECRBAN. ELXIRE I., DAVID H. L. OLSON, C. Y.<br />

SCEORNAGEL, fl J. RRLSDOBD, and A. J. TURNEB 1976<br />

"<strong>The</strong> farily contract game:<br />

J3urlal <strong>of</strong> Corsulting<br />

u4:949-455.<br />

TEchniqne and case stu3y."<br />

and clinical Psychclogy.


References Page 237<br />

BLOOD. ROBERT 0. 1963 "<strong>The</strong> husband-vif9 relationship." Pp.<br />

282-308 in F. Iven xye and Lois W. E<strong>of</strong>fmac (rds.),<br />

<strong>The</strong> Employed Bother ir Emerica. Chicago: Rand<br />

McNally.<br />

BLOOD, ROBERT 0. 1965 "Long-range causes and consaqusnces<br />

<strong>of</strong> the employment <strong>of</strong> married womsn." Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Marriage ard tte Family 27 (Pebruary):83-'47.<br />

BLOOD, ROBERT 0. 1967 Lcve natch and Arranged Uarriage.<br />

New York: Free Press.<br />

BLOOD. ROBERT 0. aca ROBERT L. RERDLIN 1958 "~hc <strong>of</strong>fec-s<br />

ot the wife's e~ployment on ths family power<br />

structure." social Fcrces 36 (nay):347-352.<br />

BLOOD, ROBERT 0. and DONELD 8. QOLPE 1960 Eosbands 3nd<br />

U~YPS: Ths Dynamlcs <strong>of</strong> narrizd Living. New York:<br />

Free Press.<br />

BLUEBERG. BYRNA 1964-65 'when pe:snts hit out." 20tb<br />

Certury (Yinter):39-41. llso reprinta3 Ln Stsirmet2<br />

and Strzus. 19lU.<br />

BLUNENTfiEL, MONICB. D., POBERT L. KBBN. PRBBK n. ANDREWS,<br />

and KENDRB E. HEED 1972 Justifying <strong>Violence</strong>: <strong>The</strong><br />

Bttitodes <strong>of</strong> RmerzCan nen. Ann Erbor, Uichigan:<br />

Institute for <strong>Social</strong> Research, <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Michigan.<br />

ELUMENTHBL, flONICA D., ZETBA 8. CEADIRB, G?RALD A. COLE,<br />

2nd TOBY EPSTEIN JRYBRBTINE 1975 nore absut Jnstlfyisg<br />

Viol~rce: nethodclogiczl S'udies <strong>of</strong> Bttitudes and<br />

Behavior. Brn Irbar. Michigan: Institute for <strong>Social</strong><br />

Research. <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> nichigan.<br />

EoRBNNBN. PIUL 1960 "Patterns <strong>of</strong> murder +ad suicide."<br />

Chapter 9 in Peul Eahannan (ed.), 3-frican Homicias and<br />

Suicide. NEW Yozk: EtheneUI.<br />

BOTT, ELIZRBETR 1957 Pamily and social Network. London:<br />

TBVIS~OC~.<br />

BRIB, ORVILLE G., JR., ROY FAIRCBTLD, and EDGER P. BOBGOTTP<br />

1961 "Relbtlon~ between family problems." narriags ard<br />

Paslly Llvlng 23:219-226.<br />

BROWN, BRUCE W. 1973 "Tte image <strong>of</strong> the coctsmporary sale<br />

as depicted in advertisicg during sports ivents."<br />

Unpublished paper, Wilkss college, Uilkss-Barre.<br />

Pennsylvania<br />

BROVN, BRUCE 8. 1917 "Educatioc. employment, and<br />

prescriptions for marital decision-making: 1900-1978."<br />

PapPr presented at the arnual meeting <strong>of</strong> the Bmericar<br />

socrological Issociation. Chicago, Septsmh~r 1977.<br />

BROWN, BRUCE W. 1978 "wife-employment ard ths emergence <strong>of</strong><br />

equalitarian marital role prescriptiocs: 1900-7978."<br />

Journal cf Comparatrve Family Studiss (in press).<br />

BROWN. R. C. and J. T. TEDESCBI 1974 'Determinants <strong>of</strong><br />

perceived aggressior." unpublished manuscript. Gssrgia<br />

State <strong>University</strong>.<br />

BRoWN~ILLER. SUSEN 1975 Bgai~st Our will. New York: Simon<br />

and Schuster.<br />

BOCKLEY WALTEP (€6.) 1968 nodern Systems Research for the<br />

Behavioral Scioc?ist. Chicago: Eldine.


Referenc~s P3ge 238<br />

BULC 'ROPT. RICHAPD and RURRBY 2. STRRUS 1975 "Validity <strong>of</strong><br />

hosbaxd, wire, end child rspor" <strong>of</strong> iirtrfamily<br />

v~clence and power." nimsographed Paper.<br />

ED46 iESS. la8 W. and LYNDP LYTLE XOL?ISTROE 1974 Rape:<br />

Victlms <strong>of</strong> Crisis. - -- Bouie. nd.: Robert J. Brady Co.<br />

FURIC. OLIVERA and ~~ ~~ ANDJELKR ZECSVIC 1967 "Family<br />

authority. marital satisfaction, and the social ns?vork<br />

in Xuooslavia." Jourral nf narriags and F ~ s Family 29<br />

BURKE. RONBLD J. and TBUBRL W3IB 1976 "Relationshtp <strong>of</strong><br />

wives1 EmplcymeCt statos to husband, wife. and pair<br />

Satisfaction and PerFOmance."<br />

the Family 38 (nay) :279-287.<br />

Journzl <strong>of</strong> narriage and<br />

BORR, WESLEY R. 1973 <strong>The</strong>ory Corstruction and the Ssciology<br />

cf the Family. New ?ark: ~ o t viiey r a ~ a 832s.<br />

BUSS, R. H. 1971<br />

leds.) , <strong>The</strong><br />

"RggrPsSion pays.' In 3. L S:ng*r<br />

Control <strong>of</strong> Aggression and <strong>Violence</strong>. New<br />

YOEk: Riley and Sons.<br />

CALONICO, JPUES m. 2 ~ d DRRHIN L. TBOnRS 1973 "Role-taking<br />

as a function <strong>of</strong> value similarity acd affect in the<br />

nuclear family.'<br />

(4) :655-665.<br />

Journal cf Earriage and the F3mily 35<br />

CILVEBT, ROBERT 1974 'Criminal and civil lilhility in<br />

husband-wife assaults." elso reprinted in Steinmetz and<br />

Straus, 1974.<br />

CRUERON. P. an& C. JRNKY 1972 "<strong>The</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> sisuirg<br />

'Violent' T.7. upon children's at-home and ir-school<br />

behavior.'<br />

Kentucky.<br />

Ucpublished manuscripf. <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

CAMPBELL, JABIS S. 1969 '<strong>The</strong> family acd violence." Pp.<br />

251-261 in Steireptz and Straus. 197U.<br />

CARILLO-ESROB, CBPEEN 1974 A<br />

Women. San Prancisco:<br />

Comparisal <strong>of</strong> Chicano and Anglo<br />

R and r Research Associates.<br />

CENTERS, BICHASD. B3RTRP.B 8. Baven, a2d AROLDO RODfiIGD3S<br />

1971 "Conjugel paver structure: A rn-s~aminatior.~<br />

Americar. SOClOlogiCBl Revlew 36 (Bpril):263-278.<br />

COEEN, PERCY S. 1969 ncde'r~ <strong>Social</strong> <strong>The</strong>ory. L3ndon:<br />

Heinsmann Educational Books Ltd.<br />

COOPEB, DRVID 1971 <strong>The</strong> Death <strong>of</strong> th3 Paaily. Rev York:<br />

Random House.<br />

CCOT3, RSNB 1974 "Police, the lax, and hsttered wires.n<br />

Macchsster Guardian (Nay 23) : 11.<br />

COSER. LEWIS E. 1956 Tb.e Punctiocs <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Conflict. New<br />

York: Pree Press.<br />

COSfB, LEWIS A. 1967 Continuities in ths Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Conflict. New Pork: Pree Press.<br />

COSER, LEWIS A. 1969 <strong>The</strong> functions <strong>of</strong> social ~onflict.~<br />

Pp.218-219 in L. 2. Cosfr acd B. RosenbErg (eds.),<br />

s0~101ogiCal <strong>The</strong>ory: A Saok <strong>of</strong> Reaaings. New York:<br />

sacmilla*. --- ~<br />

COTTRELL, L. 5. 1942 "<strong>The</strong> adjustment <strong>of</strong> the ildividual to<br />

his age and SEX roles." Bmerican Sociological Review 7<br />

r0ctober1:617-620.<br />

CROBWGLL, RONALD E. and DAVID 9.<br />

Power In Famllles. AEV York:<br />

L. OLSON<br />

sags.<br />

(eds.) 1975


References Page 239<br />

CROOG, SYDNEY A. 1970 "<strong>The</strong> family as a source <strong>of</strong> stress."<br />

Pp. 19-53 in Sol Levinf ard Normar! A. Sc3tch (sds.),<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Stress. Chicago: Bldine.<br />

CUBER, JOAN F. and PEGGY BEEOFP 1965 sax and the<br />

Significanr Americans. Ealtimore: Penguin.<br />

COBTIS, LYNN 8. 1974 Criminal Violencs: Nationel Pattsrcs<br />

2nd Behavior. Lexington. 0355-: Lexington Books.<br />

DIBRENDORP. RLLP 1959 Cl2~5 a ~ Class d C~nflict in Sn3ustrial<br />

Society. Lordon: Boutledge ard Regan Paui.<br />

DAVIS, K E N J. 1970<br />

prison system."<br />

"Sexuel assaults In the Philadelphia<br />

rn John B. Gagnon +nd William Sieon<br />

(ids.), <strong>The</strong> Sexual Scene. Chicaqo: Ildice.<br />

DESZIN, NOSHAN K. 1970<br />

deviant behavior:<br />

"Rule5 Of COZdliCt azd the stuay Of<br />

some >ores on the social<br />

relatrosship.n Pp. 120-159 in J. D3uglas (~3.).<br />

Deviance and Respect?biliCv. wev Ycrk: B3sic Eo-ts.<br />

DIXON, 1. J. (sd.) 1965 EBD--Eiomedlcal Compurar Program.<br />

Pp. 459-481. 1.0s Bngoles: U.C.L.A.<br />

DOXRENUEBD. BRUCE P. 1961 "<strong>The</strong> social psychological nsture<br />

<strong>of</strong> stress: R tramEwCrk for causal icquiry." Joum91 <strong>of</strong><br />

Abnormal and social Psychology 62 (narch] :294-302.<br />

DOLLBRD. JOBN C. et. a. 1939 Frustration and Eggression.<br />

NCY 89vera: Yale <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

DURKHEIN, EMILE 1950 Rules <strong>of</strong> sociological Mpthod.<br />

ERIRSON, KRI 1966 Wayuard Puritans. New York: Wilep.<br />

ERLRNGEA, HOWARD S. 1974 "<strong>Social</strong> class diffsrerc9s in<br />

parents' use <strong>of</strong> physical punishnnnt." Pp. 150-158 in<br />

Steinmetz ard Straua, 197U.<br />

ETZIONI. BU<strong>IT</strong>bI 1971 "<strong>Violence</strong>." Chapter 14 in Robsrt K.<br />

nertoc and Robert I. Nishet (eds.) . Contemporary<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Problems. Third Edition. xeu Park: earcourt<br />

Brace Jovanovich.<br />

FERRINGTOY, KE<strong>IT</strong>H 1975 '<strong>IT</strong>cuard a general stress theory <strong>of</strong><br />

xntra-famlly violence." A paper presented at the 1975<br />

ancual neating <strong>of</strong> the Nazional Council oc Family<br />

Belslions. Salt Laks City, Utah.<br />

FARRINGTON, KE<strong>IT</strong>B and JOYCE E. POSS 1977 "In search <strong>of</strong> the<br />

'missing' conceptual framework in family soci~logy:<br />

t.ha soclal conflict traxework." Pap?r present& at the<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory Developwrt ard nethods Workshop, annual meeting<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Na?iocal couccil an Family Relafioas, October,<br />

1977, Sar Difgc, California.<br />

PENNSLL, NUBLA 1974 Irish marriage. Dublin: <strong>The</strong> nercier<br />

Press Ltd.


Ref~rences Page 240<br />

FERNBNDEZ-BBRINA, RARON. EDUlXDO P. BRLDONIDO, and RICHERD<br />

D. TRENT 1958 "Three basic themes in Puerzo Rican and<br />

Mexicap-lmerlcan family ~alues." Jourral <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Psychology 48:167-187.<br />

PERREIRA, RNTONIO J. 1963 "Family myth and homeos:etis."<br />

?.rchlv95 cf General Psychiatry 9:457-463.<br />

FESBBPCH. SEYnOUR 1970 "Aggre~sian.~~ Chapter 22 i n Paul E.<br />

nussen fed.).<br />

Third Edltlox.<br />

Carnichael's Barns1 <strong>of</strong> CktLd ?sych9logy.<br />

N9U York: JChl Viley a3a SOXs.<br />

FESEBBCB, SEYMOUR acd ROBERT D. SINGE?, 1971 Talfviriln acd<br />

aggrsssinn. San Frarcisco: Jossey-aass.<br />

FIELD. ER9TRR B. and AENRY 7. FIELC 1973 "narital<br />

violence and +he criminal process: Neithsr justics or<br />

peace." <strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong> SE~V~CF RCV~EY 47 (June):221-240.<br />

FIELDS, UARJORY D.<br />

intervention<br />

1978<br />

policies<br />

"wife bszting g3V9rDmeFt<br />

and practicis." Testtmo-y at<br />

heaflngs on<br />

Commi%tee oc<br />

"Research Into Dmestic violence,"<br />

scierce and Technslagy, D.S. Bouse <strong>of</strong><br />

Representatives. Pebzuary 14-16.<br />

FOTJIK. KRTSLEEN R. 1976 "Rife bezting: BOV i0 dEVplJp 3<br />

vita assault task force and project." En? Lrbor.<br />

filchigaf: Rnn Brbor-Uashtenzv Courty NOW <strong>Wife</strong> Rssault<br />

Task Force.<br />

POX, GREER L<strong>IT</strong>TON 1973<br />

resources model:<br />

"Another look at thE CCnparEtiVe<br />

lssesslrg the balance <strong>of</strong> power in<br />

Tnrkish aarriages.Qy Journal <strong>of</strong> narriage and the<br />

35 (4) :718-729.<br />

Femily<br />

FBBICKE, LINDA BIBD 1977 "Tho body Couct in the battle Of<br />

the sexes." New York Times (Septsmbar 29) :44.<br />

FRENCH JOHN R. P. a ~ BERTRAM d<br />

R. RIVEN 1959 "<strong>The</strong> bases Of<br />

social power." Pp. 150-167 ir. Dorwin Cartwriqht (ed.),<br />

studies 1r Sccial Power. Arn Brhor: Fessarch Center<br />

for Group Dynamics Ins'iture far SJcial Research,<br />

unlverslty cf Richigen.<br />

FREUD, S. 1959 Why War? Letter to Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Albert<br />

Einstein. 1932. Ir colleciea Pap?rs. Tolun~~ 5. New<br />

York: Basic Books.<br />

PROBM, ERICA 1941 Sscapc from<br />

Rinehart acd wlnstan.<br />

Fre@dom. New York: Holt,<br />

FURSTENBERG, FSRNK F. JR. 1966 "<strong>The</strong> American family: 1<br />

look backward."<br />

(June) :326-337.<br />

Rnerlcan Sociological Feview 31<br />

GRGNON. JOHN A. and HILLiRB SIMON 1973 Sexual COcdUCt:<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Sources <strong>of</strong> Human Sexual~ty. Chtcago:<br />

Rldlne.<br />

GECRS, VIKTOR 1972 "Botivos and aggressive acts in popular<br />

fictio5: 5-x and class diff~rencss." bmsricar J-urnal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Sociologg 77 (January) : 680-696.<br />

GELLES, RICABRD J. 1973 'VChild abuse as psy~hopathology:<br />

I so~tologlca1 cri+iqu9 and reformulstion." Pearicsn<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> orthopsychiatry 43 (July):611-621. Also<br />

reprlcted in Stsirme+z and Straos. 1974.<br />

GELLES, RICEBRD J. 1974 <strong>The</strong> violent Home: A Stu?y <strong>of</strong><br />

Physical Aggression Between Husbacds and Wives.<br />

B~verly Hills. California: Sage Publications.


References Page 291<br />

GELLES.BICHARD<br />

Jouraal<br />

J. 1976 "hhused wlvss:<br />

ot Marriage and<br />

ahy do ttey stay?**<br />

the Family 38<br />

INoVenber! :659-668.<br />

GELLES, RICHARD J. 1977 "viol9nca Towards Children in the<br />

United States.' Paper prasonted at the amsricac<br />

RSSOclatlo~ fcr the idvancement <strong>of</strong> Science, Denver.<br />

GELLES, RICBARD 3. and MUREAY A. STRRUS 1974 "Toverd 21<br />

integrated thsory <strong>of</strong> intrafanily violent?." Fapnr read<br />

at the 1974 <strong>The</strong>cry Constructicx aorkshop it the Annual<br />

neeting <strong>of</strong> the National Council on Fanilr Selatiois.<br />

St. Lcuis, Missouri.<br />

GELLES, RICEBSD J. 2nd tlUPRAY 1. STFAUS 1975 'F3n:ly<br />

experience 2nd pnblic support <strong>of</strong> the death pecalty.'*<br />

Pmerican Journel <strong>of</strong> Orthopnychiafzy 45 (Joiy) :596-613.<br />

GELLES, RICHARD J. and nURRE? ?. STRAUS 1978 "Detpr~lirarts<br />

Of <strong>Violence</strong> in the famlly: Toward a theoretical<br />

iCtEgration." chapter in wasley E. Burr. Ruebe?. H i l l ,<br />

F. Ivan Nye, snd Ira L. Reiss (eds.), Contempsrary<br />

<strong>The</strong>oriCs About tho Family. NPU York:<br />

G3RBNER. GBRGS ard LARR? GROSS 1976 "<strong>The</strong><br />

Fre? Press.<br />

scazv world <strong>of</strong><br />

TVTS heavy viewe:." Psychology Today 9<br />

(April) :91-49.89.<br />

GIRNOPULOS. ARTIZ and POWARD E. B<strong>IT</strong>CR3IS 1957 "Msrital<br />

disagreements ix vorking wife marriages as a function<br />

Of husband*^ attLtudss toward wife's a~ployasnt."<br />

aarrrage and Fam11y Living 17 (nos?mb~r) :373-378.<br />

GI?., DAVID 0. 1971 '*Violescs agairst children." Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

narriage and the Family 33 (?lovenberj:637-648.<br />

GIL, DAVID G. 1975 "Unraveling child abuse." Bm~rican<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> orthopsychiatry U5 (April):346-356.<br />

GLPZER-RALBIN, NONh 1975 Old Family, Nev Family. Pp.<br />

27-65 "Ban and vosan: IntPrpcrsmal relationshrps ir<br />

+he marital pair: New York: D. Van gostrand.<br />

GOLDFARE 1970 "We'zs acre violent than we think."<br />

BacLean's nagazine (Rugust):25-28.<br />

GOODE, UILLIEn 3. 1962 "aaritel satisfaction and<br />

instability: A cross-cultural anelysis <strong>of</strong> airorcs<br />

rat9s." Intermtioral <strong>Social</strong> Science Journal 14<br />

131 '507-526. ~-~~<br />

GOODZ,' UILLIA~ J. 1971 'Force ard violsnce in the family."<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Barriaqe and the<br />

(Nov~nber):624-636. Also reprinted in<br />

straus. 1979.<br />

Family 33<br />

Stai~~etz aEd<br />

GOODSTADT. B.<br />

LOCUS <strong>of</strong><br />

and L. BJEILE<br />

control and<br />

1973 "Power to the povsrless:<br />

the use <strong>of</strong> power.g' Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Personali'y and sociel Psychology 27 (~uly):190-196.<br />

GORDON, WE<strong>IT</strong>NEY E. 1964 B Community iP stress. New York:<br />

Livxng Eooks.<br />

GRiEB8, HUGE DAVIS 2rd TED ROBERT GURR (eds.) 1969 ViOl~Pce<br />

ID Eserica: Ris?arical and Comparative Psrspectivss.<br />

A3tional Commission On the Cznses and Provention <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Violence</strong>: Rsparts. Vol. I 3rd II. Washington, D.C.:<br />

U.S. Govs7neent Printing <strong>of</strong>flca.<br />

GREBLZR, LEO. JOAN P. NOOB!. ~2nd RALPB C. GUZRBN 1970 Th?<br />

fiexican-American People. Nev York: Free Press.


Peferences Page 2i)2<br />

GREEN, 9RNOI.D W. 1941 "3e cult <strong>of</strong> perscnali'q ara ssxual<br />

relations." Psychiatry 4:393-398.<br />

GRINKER, ROY R. and JOHA P. SPIEGEL 1995 Bell Under<br />

Stress. Philadelphia: <strong>The</strong> Slakiszon C3mpany.<br />

GUTBACEER, fi1NPRED 1960 Th4 nird <strong>of</strong> th4 nurder?r. New York:<br />

REWKES, GLENN R. and NIAN4 TAYLOS 1975 "PoVPr s"ruc's?S in<br />

Mexican and a;xican-lmerica:~ fern Earilins." Jourral <strong>of</strong><br />

narriaos and the faeilv 37.807-811.<br />

~ -~~. ' - -~~<br />

HARKINS. JAuES L. 1968 ~r8ssaciatioc between cmpanionship.<br />

hosti1i:y. azd nazital satisEactioc." Jourcal <strong>of</strong><br />

Narriage and the Family 30:647-650.<br />

BEEB, DAVID n. 1958 "Domin3ore 3rd tho vcrking<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Forces 36 (May) : 341-397.<br />

BEER. . DIVID 8. 1963 "<strong>The</strong> measuremen= as6 bases <strong>of</strong><br />

power:An ~v~rview." narriage and Family<br />

25-133-139.<br />

*if-."<br />

faail7<br />

Living<br />

REIDER. FR<strong>IT</strong>Z 1958 <strong>The</strong> Psychology <strong>of</strong> Interp?rsonal<br />

Relations. Nsu York: wiley and Sons.<br />

EENNoN, CEARLES E. 1976 "Inrezpersocal violence snd its<br />

management by cohabiting couples." Papex presents$ at<br />

1976 western Soclal Science fieeticgs, Tzmpe, ?.rizona.<br />

BENRY, ANDREW and JIflES SEORT 1959 Suicide an9 Homicide.<br />

G~CCCCE. Ill.: Free Press.<br />

RENRY, JULES 1963 Culture Bgainst Man. New Yorh: Random<br />

ROUSE.<br />

HEPEURN. JOEN R. 1971 "<strong>The</strong> subculture <strong>of</strong> violecc-."<br />

Criininology 9 (nay) :87-98.<br />

HEPSURN, JOeN R. 1973 "Pi~lent behavior ir interpersonal<br />

?~l?.tionShips."<br />

(summer) : $19-427.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Socioloqical Qulrterly 19<br />

BERON, HOODBDSN 1957 "<strong>The</strong> pathology <strong>of</strong> bor9d3m."<br />

scientific Rmerican 196 (Januzry) :52-56.<br />

RICKS, SARY W. and KARILYN PLATT 1970 '*Rari:al happiness<br />

and stability: a review <strong>of</strong> the research io the<br />

sixtiss." In Carlfred 5. Brodarick (ed.). A DEcad? <strong>of</strong><br />

Family Reselrch and Action. niroespolis, ninn:<br />

Natlonal Conncil on Family Xela:ions.<br />

BILL, SEUBEN 1958 "Generic features <strong>of</strong> families under<br />

stress." Scciel Casework 39 (F~bruary-fl3rch) : 139-150-<br />

AOFP~AN, LOIS 1. 1960 "Effects <strong>of</strong> emplnymez+. <strong>of</strong> mothers an<br />

parental power relations acd the division <strong>of</strong> hOusEhola<br />

t.asPs." flarriagf and Fanily Living 22 (Februaryj:Z7-35.<br />

ROPPBBN. LOIS U. 1960 "Parental power xel3tlons aPd the<br />

~ZVISIOC <strong>of</strong> ho-u~ehold tasks.' Pp. 215-230 in F. Ivan<br />

Yya and Lois w. R<strong>of</strong>fman (eds.). <strong>The</strong> Employed flo:h?r in<br />

America. Chicago: Rand nc8ally.<br />

RORANSOH. J. 3. 1970 "Psychophysiological evaluatioo <strong>of</strong><br />

the catharsis hypcthesds." In 3. I. M+gargee acd J.<br />

. sckanson (sds.), <strong>The</strong> Dynamics 3f Rggression. New<br />

York: Rarper and Sow.<br />

BOLSES, TZONAS 8. ana PICHRRD X. RASE 1967 "<strong>The</strong> social<br />

readjustment rating scale." Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychos3matic<br />

Research 11: 312 ff..


~eferences Page 243<br />

ROMANS, GEORGE C. 1961 <strong>Social</strong> aehanior, Its El~mrntarg<br />

Forms. A ~ w York: AarCoUrr, Brace and Yorld.<br />

PORNER, RATINA S. 1972 'Tovard an understandicg <strong>of</strong><br />

achievement-releted ccnflicts in womec.' Journzl <strong>of</strong><br />

Socl=l Issues 28:157-175.<br />

AOWARD. ALBN and ROBERT R. SCOTT 1965 "A propose3 framework<br />

trom the aralysis <strong>of</strong> stress i- the human orgccism."<br />

Behavioral Science 10:lUl-160.<br />

HOWARD. JANE 1970 Please Teach: 1 Guided Tour <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Potential novemeni. new York: Dell Publishirg.<br />

ROUELLS, WILLIAI DEAN 1960 <strong>The</strong> Rise <strong>of</strong> Silas Lapham.<br />

BnmeP<br />

New<br />

York: Holt, Ricehart and Winston.<br />

JECOBSON, NEIL S. and ShRCLAY NBETIN 1976 'Bebaviorel<br />

marrlage therapy: Current slates." Psychological<br />

Eulletln 83 (July) :5UO-556.<br />

JBNIS, IRVING L. 1958 Psychological Str~ss: Psychoaoalytic<br />

and Behavioral Studiss cf Surgical Pstiants. New York:<br />

John Riley ard Sons.<br />

JONES, E. E. acd K. E. DAVIS 1965 "From acts to<br />

disposit1ocs.- In L. Berkovitz (ed.) . P.dvaccss in<br />

Experimental <strong>Social</strong> Psychology. 1 2. Nev York:<br />

Acad?mic Press.<br />

KARDEL, DENISE B. and GERALD 5. LESSER 1972 "X2rital<br />

decision-making in American +nd Danish urban families:<br />

R research note.' Journal <strong>of</strong> Barriage +ad the Family 34<br />

(9ebruary) :134-138.<br />

KANOUSE. DlVID E.<br />

evaluations."<br />

and L.<br />

In<br />

REID BlNSON 1971<br />

. E. Jon3<br />

"NegeLivity in<br />

gl. (eas.).<br />

Bttributicn: Perceiving the <strong>Causes</strong> <strong>of</strong> Bsharlor.<br />

I%orrFStoXn, N.J.: General Learning Prass. <strong>University</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> Nebraska Press.<br />

KRPLAN, HORPRD 73. 1975 Self Lttitudes and Devlant Behivior.<br />

Pacitlc Pslisades, California: Goodyear.<br />

KEADINLR, BERAa 1963 <strong>The</strong> Psychological Frontiers <strong>of</strong> society.<br />

New York: Columbia <strong>University</strong> Prass.<br />

KELLEY, BAROLD H. 1967 *'httribution theory in social<br />

psychclogy." In D. Zerins (ed.), Nebraska Symposium on<br />

notlvaiior, 15. Lircoln, Neb.: <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Nebraska<br />

Press.<br />

KELLEY. HEROLD e.<br />

In E. E.<br />

1971 "Ettributioc in social<br />

Jones ga. (eds.).<br />

interaction.<br />

Bttrihution:<br />

Perceiving the <strong>Causes</strong> at Behavia:.<br />

General Lsarnirg Press.<br />

Uorristour, R.J.:<br />

KELLEY, ARROLD 8. and JOl?N W. TBIBBUT 1969 "Group<br />

problem-solving." Chapter 29 in Gardrer Lindzsy and<br />

Eliot Aronson (eds.), <strong>The</strong> eandbook <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Psychalcgy. 2nd Edition, volnme 0. Reading. nzss.:<br />

4ddiso~-Wesley.<br />

KELLEY, SAROLD I!., J. If. TRIBADT. S. RBDLOFF, and D. EONDY<br />

1962 '<strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> cooperaiian ic the 'micirnal'<br />

social situetion." Psychclogical nonogrlphs 76 (19)<br />

(Whole ro. 538).<br />

RENPE, C. HENRY sL al. 1962 "<strong>The</strong> battered-child syndrome."<br />

Journal<br />

71 :17-24.<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Raericac asdical Associatior 181 (July


References page 211<br />

KOCR, 30ENME and LEU KOCR 1976 ""he ozgsnt *rive ts aak<br />

good marriages batter.7s Psychology Today 1<br />

(September): 33-35 ff.<br />

KORN, BELVIb L. 1969 Class and Ccnformity: E Study i<br />

Values. Aomeuood, 1lllno:s: Dorrsy Press.<br />

KOLB. TRUDY D. ncd BURPPY P. STRRUS 1974 "l$arit%l pow<br />

and mariial happrness in ralstion to problea-solvir<br />

ability.' Jonrnal<br />

(November) : 756-766.<br />

cf narriage and thr ?Emily 3<br />

KOi3BROVSKY. MIRRE 1962 Blue Collar narriage. New York<br />

Random House.<br />

KOMAROVSKY, 8IRR1 1973 "Cultural ContPadictiOns and se.<br />

roles: <strong>The</strong> masculrne cise." eeerican Jourral o<br />

Socrology (Jar.):873-884.<br />

Jerome Skol~lcZ (eds.),<br />

Also reprints6 in Erlens ant<br />

I~?imzcy. Paoily, aad Society.<br />

Sostor.: Little. Brown and Co., 1974, Pp. 245-257.<br />

KURN, DEBNNL 1976 "SEX-role coccfpts <strong>of</strong> two- and thras-yea1<br />

old=." Paper preserted at the anxual meeting sf tht<br />

Western Psychological Association, Los Eagelss,<br />

1976.<br />

bpzil,<br />

LAING, R. D. 1972 <strong>The</strong> Pclitics cf the Panily. New York:<br />

Vantage.<br />

IBROSSA, RALPN 1977 Conflict and Power in Bazri~ge:<br />

Zxpectl~g i.hc First child. Bsverly Rills: sage.<br />

LARSON, LYLE '2. lY74 msystes and subsystem perception <strong>of</strong><br />

family roles." Journal <strong>of</strong> narriage and the Pimilj<br />

36 (February) ~123- 138.<br />

LESLETT, BARBARA 7973 "Ths family as a public acd privat~<br />

icstitution: R historical perspective." Journsl <strong>of</strong><br />

narriage and the Family 35 (Eugust):480-692.<br />

LAZARUS, RICRhRD 5. 1966 Psychological Stress snd the<br />

coping Process.<br />

LEFKOW<strong>IT</strong>Z. HOWROE P.,<br />

New York: ncGrav-Eill.<br />

LEOPCLD 0. WILDER, L. ROWPLL<br />

HOUSEflENB, ard LFONERO D. ERON 1976 'Parental<br />

punishmhnt: E longitudinal analysis <strong>of</strong> ~ffects.~t Pspsr<br />

rEad at th€ Ictercationzl Sociezy For Research or<br />

eggression conicrance, Paris.<br />

LEBASTERS, ERSEL E. 1957 "Parecthood as crisis." Marriage<br />

and Panlly Living 19 (Norember):352-355.<br />

LERESTERS, ERSZL E. 1971 '<strong>The</strong> passing <strong>of</strong> the 6cminant<br />

husband-tather." lapact <strong>of</strong> Science or Socie'y<br />

(Januiry-Darch) : 2 1-30.<br />

LEVINE, SOL and BORuAN R. SCOTCA 1967 "Toward the<br />

developnent <strong>of</strong> theoretical modsls: 11." 8Flbank<br />

nei3oria.l Pund Quarterly US<br />

LEVINGER, GEOFGE 1966 "Sources<br />

(2):163-179.<br />

cf marital dissatisfaction<br />

among appllcacts for divorce." American Joornal <strong>of</strong><br />

Orthopsychiatzy 26 (Cctobef):803-807.


~fferences Pag; 2115<br />

LEWIS, OSCER 1959 "Family dynanics in a nariciln illa age.^<br />

Barriage and Famiiy Livixg 21:218-226.<br />

LEWIS, OSCRB 1950 TFpoztlan: Village in Eexics. New York:<br />

Ealt. RlPehart ard winstor.<br />

LEWIS, OSCAR 1963 <strong>The</strong> Children <strong>of</strong> Sanchez. New York:<br />

Vintsgs Books.<br />

LEWIS, OXaP 1967 kg !rip=. Nev York: Random Bouse.<br />

LEUIS. PORERT R. 1971 "<strong>Social</strong>iz3tior into national<br />

ViolSCce: Famil.ia1 ~Crr9latSE cf hawkish attitodes<br />

toward war.' Journal <strong>of</strong> riarriags a.3 =he Pamily 33<br />

(November) :6?9-708.<br />

Straus, 1979.<br />

Also reprinted in Steinm?:~ an3<br />

LOPRTE. RELTNA 2. 1971 occupatiac: Elus?vif;. NPW York:<br />

Oxtord <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

LOBFNZ, KONRBD 1966 Oc Bggresrion. New York: Barcourt<br />

3race Jovenorich.<br />

LUPRI, EUGENE 1969 "Co~>tenporary aotharity patterns ic the<br />

West German family: A study in cross-natioral<br />

validation.' Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

(February] :139-149.<br />

narriaqe and the Familv 31<br />

LYNN, KENNETA 1969 "<strong>Violence</strong> in American literature and<br />

tolklorf." Chapter 6 in Augh D. Graham and rta 8.<br />

Gurr (eds.1. <strong>Violence</strong> in Emerica: Eistorical acd<br />

Comparati~e Perspectiv~s Vol. I. Uashirgtor D. C. :<br />

U.S. ~ovemment PriPting <strong>of</strong>fice.<br />

BRCE, DAVID and<br />

narriages--If<br />

VFAR<br />

We<br />

nACE 197Y We<br />

Really Want<br />

Can Have Sstter<br />

<strong>The</strong>m. Nashoille,<br />

Ternessee: Bbinqdo~ Press.<br />

RADSEN. YILLIRR 196u Bexlcan-Americans <strong>of</strong> Sooth Texas. New<br />

YOrk: Holt. Sinehart acd Wics;on.<br />

MARTIN, DEL 1976 Battered Wives. San Francisco: Glide<br />

Publications.<br />

BRBX, KARL 1969 <strong>The</strong> German Ideology. Moscow: Prsgrcss<br />

Publishers.<br />

SASELLI. D. D. acd J. RLTROCEI 1969 "Ettribution <strong>of</strong><br />

intent.' Psychological Snllstin 71:445-454.<br />

RRTZE. DEYID 1969 RecomiPg DPviaxt.<br />

Prentice-Fall.<br />

Englewooa Cliffs, N.J.:<br />

BPUREE, RDEA 1979 "Corporal punishment." Assrican<br />

Psychologis:<br />

HPY, ROLL0 1972<br />

29 (Rugust):619-626.<br />

Power ard irnoceace: A SElrch for the<br />

Sources <strong>of</strong> VZalence. New Yark: Norio3.<br />

nCGARTB, JoSEPE E. (~d.) 1970 <strong>Social</strong> and Psychcl3gical<br />

Factors in strass. Pp. 10-21 "A conceptual formulation<br />

for research on stress." New York: Rolt, Rinehart and<br />

Winston.<br />

ECKINLEY, DONRLD GILBERT 1964 <strong>Social</strong> Class aad Pamily<br />

Life. NOW Ycrk: Free Press.<br />

UECERNIC, DRVID 1962 Students Under Stress: R Stuly in<br />

the <strong>Social</strong> Psycholcgy <strong>of</strong> Adapt~tion. Nex York: Free<br />

Press.<br />

NECFLNIC, DAVID 1968 nedical Sociology. v=u York: Free<br />

Press<br />

nERCORI0, JOE 1972 Caning: Educational Rite and Tradition.<br />

Spracuse: Syracuse <strong>University</strong> Press.


~eferelces Page 246<br />

BERTON, ROBERT K. 1957 <strong>Social</strong> <strong>The</strong>ory and <strong>Social</strong> S


References Page 247<br />

PBRBAS. RRYBOND I. 1967 "<strong>The</strong><br />

donPstic disturbance.'<br />

(Fall) :914-960.<br />

police response to<br />

wiscansir Law Review<br />

the<br />

91'4<br />

PRRSONS, TALCOTT 1947 "Certain primary sources and<br />

pattorrs <strong>of</strong> aggression in the social stzuc:ura o:<br />

WestPrn world." Psychiatry 10 (ilay):167-181. Also<br />

ths<br />

PD.<br />

298-322 In T. Parsons (eds.). Essays in Sociological<br />

<strong>The</strong>ozy, Revised Editiox. new YorK: Tt.e Free Press,<br />

1966.<br />

PLaSONS, TALCOTT and EDWARD 1. SHILS 1953 Toward a<br />

Generzl <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> ?c'.icr.. Cambridge, nass.: 3zrvard<br />

<strong>University</strong> Prrss.<br />

PATTERSON, GERALD R. 1975 Families: Lpplicstions <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Learning tn Family Life. Revised Edition. Eugsre,<br />

Oregon: Castalia.<br />

PATTERSON. GERBLD R., J. B. REID, R. 8. JONES, and R.<br />

E. CONGER 1975 B <strong>Social</strong> Learning Epproach to Faelly<br />

Interv~n*:~~. Volume I: Fanilips vith tggressire<br />

ChildrFn. Eugene. Oreqoc: Castalia.<br />

PENPLOSR. FERNANDO 1968 "Hexicar family rolss." Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

narriage and ths PaaiLy 30:680-689.<br />

PINEO. A. C. 1961 "Disorchartmext in the later years <strong>of</strong><br />

marriage." narriage and Family Living 23:3-11.<br />

POGREBIN. LETTY COTTIN 197'4 "DO vomec make men violent?"<br />

Es 3 (Noveaber):49-55. 80.<br />

PRESCOTT, JAIES W. 1975 "9ody pleasure ar.6 ths origics <strong>of</strong><br />

violence." <strong>The</strong> Futurist (April):64-74.<br />

RBIIREZ. BRNDEL 1967 "Idectificatlon vith Eaaicao family<br />

Values and authoritarianism in flsxican-Rmericacs." <strong>The</strong><br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology 73:3-11.<br />

RPUSCE, RRROLD L., WILLIRLI A. BERRY, RICHaRD K. REPTEL, end<br />

ERRY ANN SWRIN 1974 Communication, conflict, and<br />

flarriage. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

REINBOLD, ROBERT 1977 "<strong>The</strong> trend toward sexual squality:<br />

depth <strong>of</strong> transtormatior uncertain." New York Times<br />

(November 30) : 1.<br />

BEISS, RLBE9T J.. JR. 1968 "Police brutality--answers to<br />

key qusstions." Transaction 5 (July-August);lO-19.<br />

RESNIK, IINDY 1976 'wife beating: Couns~lcr trairing manual<br />

~o.1.' Ann Arbor: AB ~o~/wife Assault.<br />

RICHBOND. MARIE L. 1976 "Beyond resources thsory: Pnother<br />

look at factors enabling women to affect fanily<br />

interaction.' Journal <strong>of</strong> narriage<br />

(nay) :257-266.<br />

and the Family 38<br />

FOBERTS, STEVEN 7. 1971 'Crime rates <strong>of</strong> wcmGn up sharply<br />

over<br />

RODNIA, HYflAN<br />

New York Times (June 13) :I, 72.<br />

1967 "narital power in France, Grnece.<br />

Yugoslavia and the United States: R cross-national<br />

discussion." Journal <strong>of</strong> Barriags and the Pamily 39<br />

(nay) : 320-32U.<br />

RODnEN, BYUAN 1972 "Barital power<br />

resources in cultural context."<br />

Family Studies 111 (Spring):50-69.<br />

and the theory <strong>of</strong><br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Comparative


aeferences Pige 248<br />

ROGERS. nlRY F. 1979 "1rstrumen:al and infra-resources:<br />

<strong>The</strong> bases <strong>of</strong> po~er.~~ P~erican Journal <strong>of</strong> Socicl3gy 79<br />

(my) :1918-1~33.<br />

FOSENTHAL, A. 8. 196U Thirty-Right vi?n-=ss-s. New York:<br />

ROSS,<br />

3cGraw-Hill.<br />

DOROTHEA and SHEILA FOSS 1972 "Resistant, by<br />

pre-school bcgs to sex-inappropriat? behavior.'<br />

<strong>of</strong> Educatioral Psychclogy 63 (Angurt):342-346.<br />

Jaurnal<br />

RUEGER. ROSS 1973 "SPeking freedom from the ma19 myth."<br />

Buman Behavior 2 (April) :75-77.<br />

RYRN. SRYCE P. and BURRBY A. STR>.nS 195U '-<strong>The</strong> int~grlt5on<br />

<strong>of</strong> Sinhalese socizty.*v S€scarch StudLaS <strong>of</strong> tha State<br />

collegf <strong>of</strong> Uashicg%ar 22 (December) : 179-227.<br />

BYDER, R. G. 1968 "Eusband-wife dyads versus married<br />

S?~aIl?ErS." F28ilY PrOCeSS 7:233-237.<br />

SRPILIOS-EOTHSCHILD. COBSTANTINA 19h7 "1 conparison <strong>of</strong> power<br />

structnre sad marital satisfaction in urban GrrPk and<br />

French families." Journal <strong>of</strong> uarriage and the Family 29<br />

(hay) :345-352.<br />

SRFILIOS-ROTASCHILD. CONSTANTINE 1970 "<strong>The</strong> study <strong>of</strong> family<br />

powfi ~tructu~e: a reoiev 1960-1969." Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Narriage and the Family 32 (Novsmbar):539-552.<br />

SEFILIOS-BOTHSCHILD, CONSTANTINB (ed.) 1972 Tovard a<br />

Sociology <strong>of</strong> women. Pp. 63-70 "1ns:ead sf a<br />

discussion: co~parionate marriages and sexual<br />

quality: are they compatible?" Lexington, Mass.:<br />

KEroX COrpora%iOn.<br />

SIIILIOS-SOTESCHILD. CONSTANTINR 1976 "R macroexamination<br />

<strong>of</strong> family power and lors:<br />

and micro-<br />

+r exchange<br />

model.' Journal <strong>of</strong> narriage and the Fmily 38<br />

(nay] :355-362.<br />

SCRNZONI. JOHN H. 1468 "B social system analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

dissolved and existrng famil:e~.~V Jcurrsl <strong>of</strong> marriage<br />

and the Family 30 (lugust) : 452-461.<br />

SCANZONI, JOBN H. 1970<br />

YoT~: Frf? TTFSE.<br />

Opportuiity and tho Paaily. New<br />

SCANZONI, JOBN H. 1972 Ssxual Bargaining. Engl~vood<br />

Cliffs, R.J.: Prec+ice-Rall.<br />

SCPNZONI, JOHN A. 1975 "Sex roles,<br />

marital solidarlty 15 black<br />

economic factsrs and<br />

and white marriages.''<br />

Jourlal <strong>of</strong> Narriage<br />

(February) : 130-149.<br />

and the Pamily 37<br />

SCBULZ. DRVID 1969 Coming up Black: Patterns <strong>of</strong> ahptto<br />

Soci?.lizaticn. Englevood Cliffs, N.J.: Prec'ics-Pall.<br />

SCOTT, KIRVIN B. +nd STAAFORD LYNAN 1970 "Lccoucts,<br />

deviazce a ~ d social order." Pp. 89-119 in J. Douglas<br />

(ed.) , Deviincs and Respectability. New York: Basic<br />

Books.<br />

SCDTT, ROBERT A. and ALAN HOWARD 1970 "NOae1.5 Of strsss."<br />

Pp. 259-278 in Scl Levine and Norman L. Scotch<br />

(eds.) , Sociel Stress. Chicago: Aldine.<br />

SEIDZNBERG. ROBERT 1972 "<strong>The</strong> trauma <strong>of</strong> evsntlessnsss."<br />

Psychoanalytic Review 59 (spring) : 95-109.<br />

SELYE, AANS 1956 <strong>The</strong> Stress <strong>of</strong> life. Yew York:<br />

NCGraw-Bill.


SBAPIRO, SOWARD and RRNOLD DASHSFSKY 9 7 EthXiC<br />

Identification<br />

Lexingtoz moks.<br />

Rmong American Jews. Lexington. 835s.:<br />

SHAVEP. XELLEY G. 1975 Rn Introduc


References Page 250<br />

STEINUETZ, SOZANNE K. and BURRAY A. STR?.WS 1973 "Ths<br />

family as cradle <strong>of</strong> violence." Society (formerly<br />

Transac3on) lo (spptember-October) : 50-56. (8<br />

ore-oublicntion * - ~ adaot?t;or cf -- Dart af the iniroductior<br />

~<br />

to vlolenc~ in the Pamily.)<br />

STEINDETZ, SUZANNE K. and BURRAY A. STREWS (Eds.) 197u<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> in thc Family. New York: Hlrpfr sna Rol<br />

(originally published by Dodd. H&ad and Co.) .<br />

STRKUS. MURRAY A. 1969 'neasurino families.' Chaw. 10 ir<br />

2<br />

Harold T. Christerren (ed.), Pandbook <strong>of</strong> flerrleg~ aci<br />

the famly. Chicago: Bans BcNally.<br />

STRAUS. MURREY A. 1971 "Some social antecedents 31<br />

physical purishmen*: a linkage theory interpretation.'<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Barriage and the Pamily 3:<br />

(8ovember):658-663. Rlso reprinted in Steinmatz ani<br />

Straus, 1974.<br />

STRRUS, nURRRY A. 1973 "A general systems theory approad<br />

to a theory <strong>of</strong> violence between family members."<br />

Science Information 12 (3une):lOS-125.<br />

<strong>Social</strong><br />

STRAUS. MURRAY A. 1979a "Leveling, civility, and riolencr<br />

Ln the faally." Journal <strong>of</strong> Barriage and the family 31<br />

(February) : 13-29, plus addendum in August 1974 issue.<br />

Rl~o reprinted in Nursing Education, 1979: and il<br />

Richard Y. Cactrell ald David F. Schraaer (~ds.),<br />

Dynamics <strong>of</strong> flarital Interaction, Kendall/Punt. 1974,<br />

and in Kerneth C. W. Kammeyer, (ed.) , confronticg tb'<br />

Issues: Sex Roles, flarriage and the Family. Bssron.<br />

Lllyn and Bacon, 1976.<br />

STRRUS, BURRRY A. 1979h Vultural and social organizations:<br />

Lnfluences on violence between family members." I1<br />

Raymond Prirca and Dorothy BarriPr (aas.)<br />

CoCfigUraticns: Biological and Cultural Fact>rs i!<br />

Sexuality and Family. Rew York: D.C. Aeath.<br />

STRAUS. BORRAY A. 19'75 "<strong>Husband</strong>-wife interaction in nuclea'<br />

and joint households." Pp. 13s-135 in D. Narai'<br />

(ed.). Explcrations i n the Family and Other<br />

Pr<strong>of</strong>essor K. n. Kapadia nsmorial volume.<br />

Thzcker.<br />

Essays<br />

Bombay<br />

STRAWS, ?IURRAY A. 1976 "Sexual inequality. cultural norms<br />

and wife-beating." Victimology 1 (Spricg) :54-76. A l s<br />

reprinted in Enilio C. viano (ed.). Victims an<br />

Society. Uashirgton, D.C.: Visage cress, 1976, and i<br />

JaPe Rober=s Chapman and Margaret Gates (!?is.). Wane<br />

Into Wlvos: <strong>The</strong> Legal and Economic Impact on narriage<br />

Sage Yearbooks In Women Policy Studies. VOlumP 2<br />

Beverly Fills: Sage, 1977.<br />

STPBUS, KURRAY R. 1977.3 "A sociological perspective on th<br />

prerentior and treatment <strong>of</strong> wife-beatirg." In Maria Ro<br />

(ed.), Battered women. New York: Va<br />

Nostrand-Reinhold.


References Page 251<br />

STRAUS, ~URRRY E. 1977b "Sccietal morphogenesis zed<br />

inrrafamily violence in cross-cultural perspective." Ic<br />

Leonore ~oeb Adler<br />

Research. New York:<br />

(ed.). issues in Cross-CulL.ura1<br />

Rctals Of the HEY York ZcadEmy Of<br />

Sciences 285:717-730. Elso reprinted in Karac<br />

ovals ski, (ed.), aomeres Health Care. Vakefield,<br />

nass.: Nursing Dinecsior, Vol. 7, 1968, Pp. 05-63.<br />

STRRWS, EWRRAY A. 1977~ 'qNoraative and behavioral aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence betveer spcuses: PrelimiPlrp 36%~ <strong>of</strong> a<br />

natioially representative OSB sample." Papor read at<br />

the<br />

12.<br />

Symposium on Violeccf in Canadian Sociezy, Earch<br />

sTRAUS, nURRAY A. 1917d "Exchange end power in aerriage ic<br />

cultural context: a multiwethnd ard multivariate<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong> Bombay and Nicneapalis families." Paper<br />

read at the t977 meetlng <strong>of</strong> th8 Association for Bsiar<br />

Studies, New York.<br />

STRROS. EURRAY A. 1979 "measuring intrafamilp conflict and<br />

violence: the Conflict Tac5ics (CT) scales." Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> narriage and the Family. 41:75-88.<br />

STR105, MORRAY I., RICHARD J. GELLES, and SUZANNF K.<br />

STEINBET2 1980 Behind Closed DOOIS: ViOlePCE in the<br />

American Family. New York: Qnchor/Doubleaay. In<br />

press.<br />

STRAUS. fiORR3.Y A.<br />

"Rchievement,<br />

a ~ d<br />

affiliation.<br />

LBRRENCE J. ROUGHTON 1960<br />

and co-operation values as<br />

clues to trends in Rmerican rural society. 1924-1958."<br />

Rural Sociology 25 (December) :399-903.<br />

STRAUS, BURRAY A. and IRVING TALLBAN 1971 "SIfiFAU: E<br />

technig"~ for ob~ervational measnr?ment and<br />

erperinental study <strong>of</strong> families." In Joac Blaous 21.<br />

reds.1, . - Family Problem Solving. Einsdale. Illirois:<br />

Dryden.<br />

STSODTBECK, FRED L. 1958 "Family ioterzction. values and<br />

achievement.+- Pp. 135-194 in David C. Scclelland,<br />

Alfred L. Baldwin. Uris Brcnfenbrenner and Prld L.<br />

Strodtbeck [eds.), Talent and Society. New Jersey: D.<br />

V+C NOstIanC?.<br />

SURGEON GEXFRAL'S SCIENTIFIC ADVISORY COEil<strong>IT</strong>TFB ON<br />

TELEVISION END SOCIAL BEBhVIOR 1972 Report Of th?<br />

surgeon Geceral*s scientific Adrisory Committee 3o<br />

Television and <strong>Social</strong> Behavior. Washington. D.C.:<br />

O.S. Government Printing Office.<br />

TANEY, C. 1969 "A psychiatric study <strong>of</strong> homicide."<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry 725 (9) : 1252-1258.<br />

?.m?rican<br />

TFDBSCAI, J. T., P. SUTH and R. BROWN 1974 "E<br />

reintarpretation cf research on aggression.'<br />

PsyChOlOgical Bulletin 81r540-562.<br />

TEIBAUT. JOHN W. and EXBOLD H. KELLEP 1959 <strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Psychalogp <strong>of</strong> Groups. NEW Pork: John Vilsy end Sons.<br />

TPUNINGER, ELIZ?.BETH 1977 nfiarital violence: T ~ E legal<br />

501utio1. " <strong>The</strong> Bastings IBY Journal 23<br />

(november) :259-276.


References Page 252<br />

.<br />

WELSH, RALPH S. ' i976 "Severe parental pur.ishnect acd<br />

delinquency: a developasntal theory." Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Clilical Child Psychology 5 (Spring) :17-21.<br />

YESTLEY. sILLIAn R. 1953 '*violence and the police."<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Ssciology 59 (July1 : 3U-41.<br />

WH<strong>IT</strong>E. LESLIE 1. 1975 <strong>The</strong> Concept <strong>of</strong> Cultural Systems. New<br />

nork: Columbia university Prass.<br />

WH<strong>IT</strong>ERURST, ROBERT N. 1979 "<strong>Violence</strong> in husbaua-wife<br />

interaction." Pp.<br />

BR<strong>IT</strong>EHURST. BOSERT N.<br />

75-82 in stcinmatz and Straus, 1974.<br />

197U 'Rlternative family structures<br />

and ~iclecce-reduction." Pp. 315-320 in Steinmetz acd<br />

Straus, 1973.<br />

YE<strong>IT</strong>ING, SEFTRICE B. 1965 "Sex identity coaflic* and<br />

physical violence: 1 comparative study." :nerican<br />

Rnthroplcgist 67 (December) : 123-140.<br />

wILLIA~S, DOBALD B. 1979 "conpensaticq victims <strong>of</strong> crimes<br />

<strong>of</strong> violence: Bnather look at the schsns.' Pp. 147-153<br />

;a I. Drapkin acd E. Viaco (sds.), victimology: R<br />

Aev FOCUS. volume 11. Sccie!y's REaction<br />

Vict~mizatio~. Lsxingt.on. $lass.: Lexington Books.<br />

to<br />

WILLIAES. ROBIN E. 1970 Rnsricar SociEty: E Sociological<br />

Interpretatior.. New York: Alfrsd A. Krspf.<br />

WILSON, LANCE 8. and GERALD U. MCDONALD 1977 Faailp Impact<br />

Analysis and Family Policy Advocate: <strong>The</strong> PzOCess <strong>of</strong><br />

Inalysis. Family Impact Series. miaessota Family<br />

study center, Report BO. 4.<br />

UINTEP. 8. D.. FERREIRA. 2. J., End N. BOREPS 1973<br />

'D~cision-maki-g in marries and unrelated couples."<br />

Family Process 12:83-94.


Refezeaces Page 253<br />

XOLFE. DONALD n. 1959 *uPover and authority in the family."<br />

Pp. 99-117 ia D. Cartwrigt: (nd.), Studies in S2cial<br />

Power. Bnn Arbor. aichigac: U2lvszsity <strong>of</strong> E:chigan<br />

Instituta far <strong>Social</strong> BesEarch.<br />

uOLFP, EARoLD G. 1968 Scress and Discase. second Edi-ion.<br />

EdLted by St~wart wolf ard Helen Goodell. Springfield,<br />

Ill.: Charles C. Thomas.<br />

WOLFGANG, EARVIN E. 7956 "Susbald-wif- hornicifis~.~*<br />

COrTeCtive<br />

2:263-271.<br />

Psychietry an3 Journll cf <strong>Social</strong> Thsrzpy<br />

WOLFGANG, RBRVIN 3. 1957 "victim-precipitate& criminal<br />

homicide." Jouznal <strong>of</strong> Criminal Law, Criminology end<br />

Police Science 98 (June): 7-11. Also Pp. 72-87 ic<br />

Earvin 3. Wolfgang (ed.), Studies in Aoeicide. New<br />

York: Harper ard Row.<br />

UOLPGANG. l5BRVIN E. 1969 "violence and human behavior."<br />

Psper preserted at the ennual meeting <strong>of</strong> the imerican<br />

Psychological 1ssociition. Washington, D.C., Bugust 30.<br />

UOLPGbNG, nABVIN and FRANC0 PERBACUTI 1967 <strong>The</strong> Subculture Of<br />

Vlclence. Nev Ycrk: aarces and Noble.<br />

YIFPE, JANES 1968 <strong>The</strong> Amerrcar Jew. New York: Papsrback<br />

Library.<br />

YARROW. MARIAN RADKE, CBERLOTTE GREEN SCIWPRTZ. RBRRIET S.<br />

ilURP!iY, ma LEILB CALBOUN DEASY 1955 "<strong>The</strong> psychological<br />

meaning <strong>of</strong> msrtal illness ic the family." Jourrrl <strong>of</strong><br />

social Issues 11 (4) : 12-ZU.<br />

YINGER. JOHN n. 1965 Tawara a Fiela <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Behavior:<br />

Personali:~ and <strong>Social</strong> structure. New York:<br />

RcGraw-H111.<br />

YLLO. KERSTI 8. and EURRRY A. STRlUS 1980 "Interpsrssnal<br />

violence among married and cchabiting couples." Family<br />

Coordinator. Epril, in press.<br />

YOUNG, IICHPEL and PETEF WILLBOTT 7973 <strong>The</strong> Syer8strical<br />

Family: B Study cf Work and Leisure in ths Londoc<br />

Region. London: Routledge and Xegan Paul.<br />

ZOK. GERALD H. 1978 "A therapist's p4rspectlve on Jewish<br />

family veluss." Joornal <strong>of</strong> narrisgs and Family<br />

Counseling 9 (Jzcuary) :103-110.


Indexes


Abbort. S., 186"<br />

Adorno, T. W., 217, 218<br />

Aldous, J., 178<br />

Allen, C. hl., 16,19,77,87,137,156,171,<br />

188<br />

Altrochi, 1.. 141<br />

Aniiiews, F. zW., 237<br />

Antell, M., 47<br />

Applry, ,\I. H., 96<br />

Archer. D., 57. 213<br />

Ardrey, R., 111<br />

Bach, G. R., 20, 112, 174n, 185, 186-87%<br />

220<br />

Bachman. J. G., 215<br />

Bah~ S. J., 186n. 191. 204,205<br />

Balswick, J. O., 180<br />

Bard, M., 50n, 226-27<br />

Baii, P. B., 88<br />

Bawwitz, H., 95<br />

Bcll. N. W.. 192<br />

Beilak, L.. 47<br />

Bern, D. J., 38<br />

Bern. S. L., 38<br />

Benedici, R., 41<br />

Author Index<br />

Berger, P., 9<br />

Berger, A. A., 182<br />

Brrkowitz. L.. 220<br />

Beins~rin. P. S.. 77, 78<br />

Berry, W. A., 247<br />

Bertelheim, R., 52, 60, 66n, 106. 112<br />

Bickman, L., 48<br />

Blau, P.. 4, 174". 193<br />

Blechman. 6:. A,, 217. 221<br />

Blood, K. 0.. 9, 129, 166, 177, 178, 179,<br />

183, 186". 189, 192, 193, 204. 206<br />

Blumberg, M.. 103<br />

Blurnenthal. M., 213<br />

Bohannan, P.. 40<br />

Boigorta, E. F., 237<br />

Boti, F.. 128<br />

Bowerman.C. E., 186n, 191. 204. 235<br />

Bower. N., 48<br />

Brim. 0. G. Jr., 174"<br />

Brown, B. W., 19. 21, 77, 171, 172, 181,<br />

182.188<br />

Brown, K. C., 145<br />

Brownmiller, S.. 93<br />

Buckley, W.. 174n<br />

Bulci<strong>of</strong>t, K., 32, 49, 191-92<br />

Burgers, A. W., 88<br />

Buric, 0, 186"


Author Index<br />

Burke. R. J., 178<br />

Burr, W. R., 181<br />

Buss, A. H., 138<br />

Edelheitz, H., 46<br />

Emhree, j. F.. 41<br />

Epsiein, S., 141<br />

Eiikson, I


Author Index<br />

Mundy, D , 243<br />

Murphey, H. S., 253<br />

Niebuhr, R.. 11<br />

Nye. F. I., 180, 235<br />

O'Brien, I. E., 9, 19, 109, 137, 227<br />

O'Dell, S., 217<br />

Olson, D. H. L., 192, 221. 236<br />

Oppong, C., 186"<br />

Owens, D.M., 14, 35, 76, 93<br />

Palmer, S., 52, 55, 99, 113<br />

Parade Mapine, 227<br />

Parkin, P., 54<br />

Parnas, R. I., 43, 45<br />

Parsons, T., 70, 87-88, 180, 205-6<br />

Parterson. G. R., 217, 221<br />

Peek, C. W., 180. 235<br />

Penilosa. P.. 72. 73<br />

Pineo. A. C.. 9<br />

Pines, A., 92, 178<br />

Plarr, M., 9<br />

Pogrebin, L. C.. 47<br />

Pollock, C. B., 107<br />

Prescotr, J. W., 217<br />

Purney, S. W., 186"<br />

Rabunsky, C., 192<br />

Radl<strong>of</strong>f, R.. 243<br />

Rahe. R. H., 96<br />

Ramirez, M., 72, 73<br />

Rsusch. H. L.. 174"<br />

Raven, B, H., 74, 18611 189, 192-93<br />

Reid, J. B., 247<br />

Reinhold. R., 92<br />

Reiss, A. J. Jr., 149<br />

Reinik, M., 226<br />

Richmond, M. L., 186"<br />

Robercs, S. V., 62<br />

Rodman, 14.. 87, 171, 189-90<br />

Rodripe& A, 186"<br />

Rogers, M. F.. 171, 189<br />

Rosrnihal. A. M., 48<br />

ROSS, D., 180<br />

Ross. S.. 180<br />

Roy, M., 232"<br />

Rueger. R.. 182<br />

Ryan, B. F., 41<br />

Ryder. R. G.. 48<br />

Safilios-Rothrchild, C.. 174". 178, 179.<br />

186". 192<br />

Sanford, N., 235<br />

Scanzoni, J. H., 119, 120, 174% 186%<br />

190, 204<br />

Schoinagel. C. Y., 236<br />

Schulz. D., 63<br />

Schwarrr, C. G., 253<br />

Sc<strong>of</strong>ch. N. A., 95<br />

Scott, ,M. B.. 140<br />

Scott, R. A.. 95, 96, 97, 98, 100, 101-2,<br />

108,111<br />

Seidcnbrrg, R., 99<br />

Selye, H.. 95<br />

Shapiio, H., 77, 78<br />

Shaver, K. G., 138<br />

Shearer. L.. 42<br />

Shils, E. A,, 70<br />

Shorr. 1.. 57<br />

Shosrrom, E. L., 20<br />

Simme1.G.. 9, 17, 21, 118, 170, 218<br />

Simon, W., 35, 38, 53, 216<br />

Singer, R. D., 52<br />

Sipcs, R. G., 52<br />

Skinner, B. F.. 125<br />

Skolnick, A,, 9, 116, 127, 204, 235<br />

Skolnick, J. 1-1.. 204, 235<br />

Smith, R., 251<br />

Spiegel. J. P.. 95<br />

Spiey, J., 17, 20, 21, 116, 119, 128, 143,<br />

145, 157-58, 170, 174". 175". 183<br />

Srapp, J., 92, 178<br />

Siaik, R , 13. 40. 47<br />

Steele. B. F.. 107<br />

Srrinmerz, S. K., 3, 8, 11, 31, 34, 36n, 40,<br />

41. 55. 63, 64, 66n, 93, 105, 106. 107,<br />

111.112.116.124,207.215,220,227<br />

Siraus, M. A., 3, 7, 8, 11, 13, 14, 15, 16,<br />

18. 19. 20, 22n, 25, 31. 32, 33, 34, 35,<br />

36". 40, 41. 43, 48.49. 50". 52, 53, 55,<br />

61, 63, 64. 6611, 76, 77, 87, 91, 93,<br />

105-8, passim, 110-13pasrim. 116,124,<br />

131,134n,135n.137,153-54,156,171,<br />

177, 179. 186.87". 188. 189. 191-92,<br />

204, 206,207, 208,213-16parsim, 220,<br />

222,227,231<br />

Sirodrbeck. V. L., 77, 78


Author Index<br />

Surgeon General's Scirntific Advisory'<br />

Cornmiriee on Television and <strong>Social</strong><br />

Behavior. 214<br />

Swain, M. A,, 247<br />

Tallman, I., 192<br />

Tanay, C.. 14<br />

'Taylor, M., 74. 76<br />

Taylor, S. P., 141<br />

Tedeschi, J. T., 137, 138, 145, 150<br />

Thibaut, I. W.. 118, 138<br />

Thomas, D. L., 191<br />

Trenr. R. D.. 240<br />

Tiurnbull, R., 96, 235<br />

Truningei, E., 44, 45, 46, 89, 93<br />

Turk, J. L.. 192<br />

Turner, A. I., 236<br />

Turner, R., 139-40. 141<br />

Underwood, B. J.. 125<br />

Van den Brighe, P. L., 205<br />

Walder, 1. O., 244<br />

Walster. E., 151<br />

Walters, R. H.. 107. 111, 235<br />

Weber, M., 60<br />

Weir, T.. 178<br />

Wcitzman. L. I., 178<br />

Wellei, R. H., 18611<br />

Welsh, R. S., 15<br />

Wesfley. W. A,. 149<br />

Whire, L. A,, 69<br />

Whitehurst, R. N., 91, 109, 133, 177. 180,<br />

182-83<br />

Whiting. B. B.. 93<br />

Williams. D. B.. 46<br />

Williams, R. M., 5<br />

Wiilmott, P., 190<br />

Wilson. L. R.. 211, 224<br />

Winter. W. D.. 48<br />

Wolfe. D. M., 9. 129, 166, 177. 178, 183,<br />

186n. 189,192. 193, 204, 206<br />

Wdff, H. G.. 95. 97, 101<br />

Wolfgang, M. E., 8, 32, 40, 69, 71, 107,<br />

137,183<br />

Wyden, P., 20, 112, 174% 185, 186-87%<br />

220, 235<br />

Yaffc, J., 77, 78<br />

Yarrow, M. R., 152<br />

Yinger. J. M., 71<br />

Ylio, K. A,, 31<br />

Young, M., 190. 236<br />

Zccevic. A.. 186"<br />

Zub, G. H., 79


Subject Index<br />

Absolure Resource Index, 193 Aegrcssive .. person, cultural meaning <strong>of</strong>.<br />

Absolute resources, 196. 198, 201<br />

151<br />

Acccpinnce: and rule violations, 143. in Aging, and family iifc cycle, 17<br />

intimate rclurionrhips, 144<br />

Alcohol, 141, 171<br />

Achievement: and conflicr. 58, 125; and Anger: holding in, 14; venting <strong>of</strong>. 220<br />

women, 89; orienration. 188: and ag- Antagonisric behavior, 123, 129-33<br />

gression, 206": and manhood. 228; md Anricipatory socialirarion. 181<br />

human worth. 228, 229, 231<br />

Assertiveness 100, 111-12, 150, 220<br />

Adolercenrr, as source <strong>of</strong> dara, 191 Attiibuiion iheory. 138-39. 147-48. 150<br />

Adultery, 145<br />

Auihoritarian personality, 217<br />

Adults: in children's books, 62; as aggres- Authoriraiianism, 73. 76, 81". 149<br />

sois, 63; as vicrims, 63<br />

Avoidance: to prevent violence, 115: and<br />

Affection: decline <strong>of</strong> in marriage, 9; and contlicr, 122-25, 129, 144; definition<br />

violence, 217, 218<br />

Affecrive invrsrmenr, in iniimate groups,<br />

<strong>of</strong>. 126; as behavioral outcome, 126-29<br />

118, 119. 120. 122, 134"<br />

Balance <strong>of</strong> power: in family, 156, 193:and<br />

Age differences, related to violence. 34 violence, 196. 200. 201, 202<br />

Aggression, 23, 111, 124, 130, 134n, 137, Bartered-wife shelters, 91<br />

153.54": wives subjecr <strong>of</strong>, 7; in family. Behavioral therapists, 221<br />

7, 41; in male delinquents, 15; learned. Blue-collar families, 183<br />

10; against women, 88; female, 88; Boredom: and screw, 99: and violence, 113<br />

male, 88: as normal, 111: in conflicis,<br />

125; resolution, 131; disguised, 137;<br />

Boys, and sexism. 181<br />

imputation <strong>of</strong>, 151; crearive, 204: and Cniifornia Penal Code, 47<br />

weakness. 206n; results <strong>of</strong>, 220. paiho- California "wife-beaiing" statute. 43-44<br />

logical. 229<br />

Canon Law, 71<br />

Aggressive drive rheaiies, 111<br />

Capital punishment, see Death pcnalry


Subject Index<br />

Catharsis as legirimizer <strong>of</strong> violence, 112,<br />

204, 220<br />

Causal theories, in family violence research,<br />

7<br />

Child abuse, 8. 14. 103. 109. 216<br />

Child care: and violence, 179; rcrponsihility<br />

for, 223; facilities. 223<br />

Child rearing: norms in eihnic groups. 73:<br />

responsibility for. 88. 181; and physi-<br />

cal punishment. 216,217.230; ideology<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 218; programsfor. 218<br />

Child support, 223<br />

Childhood experiences <strong>of</strong> violence. 93"<br />

Children: control <strong>of</strong>, 41; awareness <strong>of</strong><br />

physical aggression, 49: as aggressors.<br />

63; as nonvoiuniary family members,<br />

119: andworking women, 162: respon-<br />

sibiiiry for. 168; sr source <strong>of</strong> data, 191<br />

Childrcn's litcratuir: violence in, 11, 51-67<br />

Claiming behavior, in iniimaie relation-<br />

ships, 149<br />

Clockwork Oianee rheorv <strong>of</strong>violence. 113.<br />

227<br />

Companionship: decline <strong>of</strong> in marriage. 9;<br />

and equaliry. 183<br />

Comparison Level, 118<br />

Competition, and human worth, 229<br />

Conflict: in faniily, 10, 49, 218-21;avoid-<br />

ance <strong>of</strong>, 20; suppression <strong>of</strong>, 20; and<br />

violence, 20, 116, 134; as inevitable,<br />

20, 133, 218, 219,220, 230;ashealthy.<br />

21; and intimacy, 21;in Jewish families,<br />

79; resolution <strong>of</strong>, 115. 127; expressive,<br />

123-26; realistic, 123: insrrumenral,<br />

123-26. 129-33<br />

Conflict approach, 170, 171, 173, 174n,<br />

175n<br />

Conflicr <strong>of</strong> inrerest, 16, 94. 116. 117, 123,<br />

124. 127, 129-30. 133, 157<br />

Conflict Tacrics Scales, 25, 192<br />

Coping: with conflict. 49; with sties 101<br />

through violence. 107<br />

Criminal justice system: artituder toward<br />

family violence. 43.46-47, 211-12.231:<br />

fdurr to act, 44, 45: male orientation<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 90. 226, 230<br />

Cultural-consistency theory af violence,<br />

69-73: and Menican-American families,<br />

73-77; and Jewish-American fmdier.<br />

77-79<br />

Cultural norms: definition <strong>of</strong>, 5-6; and<br />

family behavior, 6; legitimizing vio-<br />

lence, 7, 12, 35. 38. 41. 68, 84; and<br />

myth <strong>of</strong> nonviolence. 11; de fact", 35;<br />

de jwe, 35; and power, 178: and wife-<br />

beating, 229<br />

Cultural valucr, in interpreting violence,<br />

117<br />

L.<br />

Culture: definition <strong>of</strong>, 5; and family life. 6;<br />

as a system, 69<br />

Day-care cenrers, 223, 230<br />

Death penalty,60-61, 213, 217<br />

Decision making: in family. 110, 193; by<br />

woman. 159; and employment <strong>of</strong> wife,<br />

179; sharing<strong>of</strong>. 183<br />

Decision Power Index. 192, 193<br />

Deference rules, 140<br />

Demands: and stress. 98. 99, 101, 113; in-<br />

soluble, 109<br />

Dcmranor rules. 140<br />

De~endency, in inrimsie relacionships,<br />

119, 120<br />

Divorce, 30, 89, 159, 185<br />

Dominance, re? Female dominance, Male<br />

dominance<br />

Dominant-submissive relationship. 179<br />

Drugs: cure for violence. 94; and rule vio-<br />

lstions. 141<br />

Economic difficulry, and violence, 64,227-<br />

3 1<br />

Economic resources, 193, 199<br />

Education: and dominance. 182; as re-<br />

source, 190<br />

Employmmr. and self-esteem, 228. 231<br />

Encounter groups, 220<br />

Equal Righis Amendment, 91,225<br />

Equditariunism: in marriage. 18. 19-20.<br />

156, 172, 177, 181, 183; in ethnic<br />

groups, 74, 79; and violence, 86-87.90,<br />

91, 183-85; and frusriation, 109; a d<br />

intimacy, 149, 172; and conflici, 176;<br />

occupational and economic, 179, 222,<br />

224. 228. 230: and males. 181, 182; re-<br />

jecrion <strong>of</strong>, 182; class differences in<br />

norms, 190-91<br />

Frcope from Freedom, 224<br />

Ethnic groups: values and norms <strong>of</strong>. 14,<br />

69, 70, 73: violence in, 14, 71, 80


Subject Index<br />

Erhnogiaphies, <strong>of</strong> Americnnmarriages, 157<br />

Exchangr model <strong>of</strong> marriage (Scanzoni),<br />

170 ---<br />

Exchange theory. 205"<br />

Expressive violcncc: ZZn, 108. 111-12,<br />

113; in children's lirerutuie, 53; in fiction.<br />

58; in middle class, 204: in mass<br />

media, 214<br />

Exrended family: and nuclear family, 18:<br />

conflici in, 135"; srability <strong>of</strong>, 219. 225<br />

Family: and aggression, 7; and stress, 8. 11.<br />

33. 103, 104, 107; stiucrural features<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 8, 85, 115, 136;supportin, 11, 18,<br />

218; involuntary membership in, 17.<br />

120: relationships sndconflict,49,115,<br />

156:values,73;functions<strong>of</strong>, 104;uiban.<br />

110; as 'dehumanizing" agent. 139:<br />

politics, 139, 173, 174% 175n; private<br />

nature <strong>of</strong>, 152; and children. 16243;<br />

and power, 201; as training ground for<br />

violence, 215-18<br />

Family impact analysis. 219<br />

Family life: complexity <strong>of</strong>. 10, 22; idealiraiion<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 11; characteristics <strong>of</strong>, 41<br />

Faihei: dominance <strong>of</strong>. 68: relarions wirh<br />

chiidren. 73. 74, 75. See also Ilusbnnd<br />

Fcmalc aggressors, in children's books, 62<br />

Female dominance. 157<br />

Female-centcied family, 77<br />

I'emininiry: compulsive. 225; and employmenr,<br />

225<br />

Feminisc movcmenr, see Women's movemrnf<br />

Fiction, and violence, 57-58, 63<br />

Final Say Power Index, 193<br />

Frustration: reduction <strong>of</strong>, 108, 227: and<br />

mess. 108. 109;andviolence. 109,112;<br />

causes <strong>of</strong>, 110-1 1; and aggression, 227<br />

Frusrration-aggresrion theory, 111, 116<br />

General mess model. 95, 105, 108, 111,<br />

112,113, 114<br />

Goal attsinmenc, 109, 180, 181, 185,<br />

189<br />

Goal bloclage. in children's books, 5940<br />

Goal-direcred rule violaiions, 145<br />

Goals: and violence, 105; in conflict, 125<br />

Guns, and domestic violence, 25, 214-15,<br />

229<br />

Happiness, and marital stability. 9<br />

Head <strong>of</strong> household: male as, 86, 156, 177,<br />

179: rrsponsibilities <strong>of</strong>. 168: changes<br />

in. 221, 222, 228. 230<br />

Hedonistic iclrvance. 151-52<br />

High resource group, 200<br />

Hitring license norm, 212. 220<br />

Homicide, 8, 11, 1415, 40,45,48, 55, 213<br />

liosriiity: and inrimate ielariunships, 9.16,<br />

116, 118, 128; production <strong>of</strong>, 20, 129,<br />

130;suppression <strong>of</strong>, 20, 116, 118, 119-<br />

22, 126-27. 133; and conflicr <strong>of</strong> inter-<br />

est, 117. 118. 133; subjecrive, 117.<br />

129: expression <strong>of</strong>. 124, 125; outcome<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 127: reduction <strong>of</strong>. 128; and expres-<br />

sive conflict, 130<br />

Household rasks, 148: and violence, 179;<br />

distribution <strong>of</strong>, 223, 224<br />

Housewife, 181: power in home, 167<br />

<strong>Husband</strong>: and power, 8-9: and faiher role.<br />

53; and violence, 200, 202, 203, 208"<br />

<strong>Husband</strong> bearing, 31-33<br />

Identiry: in inrimace relationships, 140,<br />

143. 149; rhreatmed, 144; and rule<br />

violsrions, 146; support <strong>of</strong>, 146: an ro-<br />

cia1 phenomenon, 154n; and srabiiiry.<br />

225<br />

Illegirimare strategy: in intimarr relation-<br />

ships, 128; in conflicrs <strong>of</strong> inrciesi. 131;<br />

in conflicr siiuations, 133<br />

Income. and family violmce. 227. 231<br />

individualism, 188: and achievement, 53:<br />

and violence. 87: protection <strong>of</strong>, 139; in<br />

U.S.. 199<br />

Indusiriaiized nations, and violence. 52<br />

Instrummtal behavior, 139<br />

Instrumental conflict: in inrimsie groups.<br />

122; defined, 123, 124-25; consequen-<br />

ces <strong>of</strong>. 124: intent in, 125; in conflicrs<br />

<strong>of</strong> inrerest, 131; effeciiveness <strong>of</strong>, 131;<br />

and resolution, 131: as result <strong>of</strong> hosri-<br />

liiy, 133; leading ro aggression, 134<br />

lnstrumenral dependence, on family, 120<br />

lnstiumenral inverrmmrs: and horriliiy,


Subject Index<br />

119; in family, 120; in inrimarepups,<br />

122<br />

lnsrrumenral strategy, 125<br />

Instrumental violmce, 2211, 107, 111-12:<br />

in children's liieruruir. 53, 64; in fic-<br />

tion. 58; and srress. 113; andclass, 203,<br />

204. 205; in mass media, 214<br />

Inreni, 143, 14748: <strong>of</strong> violmce, 136,137,<br />

138. 141, 150, 153; <strong>of</strong> aggression. 141<br />

Inrimacy, 110, 115, 117. 118: in family,<br />

9, 10. 128, 136; and identity. 17~18,<br />

146-47; and authoriry, 73;and conflict.<br />

116. 126. 183, 185, 218: and aggres-<br />

sion. 129, 152<br />

Intimate Enemy, <strong>The</strong>, 220<br />

lniimaie groups: paradoxical struciuie <strong>of</strong>,<br />

116, 122. 133: dissolurion<strong>of</strong>, 122.129:<br />

and expressive conflicr. 130; and con-<br />

flicts <strong>of</strong> inteiesi, 133; and violence,<br />

137.172<br />

Intimate relationships: knowledge <strong>of</strong> inti-<br />

mazes, 129, 148; rules <strong>of</strong>, 13940, 149;<br />

maintenance <strong>of</strong>. 141; and rule viola-<br />

tions, 144. 145. 146; and threat per-<br />

ception, 147; andatrriburion, 147.151;<br />

and equaiiiarianiun, 172, 183, 186<br />

Invasion <strong>of</strong> privacy, 24<br />

irony: <strong>of</strong> family violence, 10-11, 21-22,<br />

85: and egalitarian family sysrcms. 87<br />

Jewish-American families, violence in, 77-<br />

79, 80<br />

Juvenile delinquency, 15, 165<br />

Law, see Criminal justice system<br />

Leadership norms, in American sociery,<br />

198<br />

Lcadrrahip role: <strong>of</strong> husband, 199, 227;<br />

and physical foicc. 227<br />

Literature: and violence, 11, 214: reflect-<br />

ing cultural norms. 42; influencing so-<br />

ciery. 52, 53<br />

Living iogerher, and violence. 31<br />

Love: in family structure, lo, 11, 24, 34.<br />

41. 218: and use <strong>of</strong> violence, 14, 24,<br />

34, 42, 217, 218, 230:sndaurhoriraiivn<br />

personality, 217 '<br />

Macho image, in working class, 204<br />

,Male chauvinism, 165<br />

Malc dominance, 92, 172, 173, 180, 188:<br />

and conflicr, 16; in tiansirion, 19; when<br />

male is threaiened, 19; and violence.<br />

19, 36, 77, 86, 87. 161-62, 165, 166,<br />

169, 186, 200, 202, 203, 221; and<br />

physical force, 30, 91, 189, 202: in<br />

Mexican-American fmiilies. 68, 74;<br />

rhrough insrirurional aiiangemcnrs, 86;<br />

in urban society, 171; and conflicr.<br />

176, 177, 179-80; failuietoacraiii, 182:<br />

and equalirarianism, 185: social class,<br />

190, 203, 204, 205; resisiance to, 201.<br />

See airn Women, suboidinaiion <strong>of</strong><br />

Male power, and violence, 183, 200. 202<br />

~Mariiage: and happiness. 9; as long-term<br />

,,relationship. 17; counseling, 43, 220,<br />

221; srudies <strong>of</strong>. 47; and encounter<br />

movrmenr, 221; contract, 222<br />

Marriage license, as hirring license, 13, 39-<br />

50, 63. 211-12<br />

Masculiniry, 32, 87-88, 156, 205,221, 228:<br />

compulsive, 930, 180, 182, 185, 225<br />

Masochism. 229<br />

Mass media and carhaisis rheory, 52; vio-<br />

lence in, 52, 214, 215, 229; and sen<br />

roles, 181-82<br />

Mcdiaror. See Third parry arbitraiion<br />

Mexicani\merican college scudenrs, sur-<br />

veyed, 74-75<br />

Mexican-American familicr: male domi-<br />

iinncs in, 68, 73: violence in, 73-77,<br />

80. 81n<br />

Middle-class norms. andviolence. 190. 191,<br />

196, 200, 201, 203, 204, 205<br />

Miliraiy violence. 213<br />

Money: as resource, 166. 172; and marital<br />

role, 169; and power, 169, 178; and<br />

male dominance, 182<br />

Moniicys, and violence. 217<br />

Moihei: iolc <strong>of</strong> and work, 163; idmtifica-<br />

cion wich, Z80<br />

Motion pictures: and nonviolence myth <strong>of</strong><br />

family, 11; and violence, 214<br />

Murder. see Homicide<br />

Myih: <strong>of</strong> family nonviolence, 11, 41, 49.<br />

63. 64, 65; <strong>of</strong> violence as socially wor-<br />

thy. 63; <strong>of</strong> single-parent household,<br />

89


Subject Index<br />

Nonconscious ideology, 38<br />

Nonrealistic conflict, 123<br />

Nonviolence <strong>of</strong> family, see Myrh<br />

NOW <strong>Wife</strong> Assault Program, 226-27<br />

Nuclear family: violence in, 13, 17; insta-<br />

bility a€, 17; compared with extended<br />

family. 18; srrainson, 21 1<br />

Occupational groups, classification <strong>of</strong>,<br />

207% 208"<br />

Occupacional discrirninarion, 88, 222,223,<br />

230<br />

Occupaiional idenfificarion, 225, 231<br />

Old, dominance by, 73<br />

Oprimum stress level, 8. 99<br />

Parent-child violence. 13, 47, 63<br />

Parenrhoad: by-product <strong>of</strong>, 165-66; rc-<br />

sponsr ro, 174"<br />

Passive aggression. 137<br />

Passive-cangeniul marriage, 21. 127<br />

Percepiion: and stress, 96: <strong>of</strong> aggression,<br />

137: <strong>of</strong> violence, 141, 151<br />

Personality involvement: and conflict <strong>of</strong><br />

inceiest, 117; and hostiliiy. 118; in in-<br />

chafe irlarionships, 118, 127, 129,<br />

134n; and rcplacemenr <strong>of</strong> group mem-<br />

bers. 119; and expressive conflict, 130<br />

Personaliry rraits: and poivet, 193; by sex,<br />

229<br />

Physical affection, 217-18<br />

Physical punishment: ro conrrol children,<br />

13; consequences <strong>of</strong>, 34; role-modeling<br />

function <strong>of</strong>, 34, 40, 49, 215-16, 230;<br />

and husband-wife violence, 35; Irgiri-<br />

miring violence, 35. 40; in Tepozilan,<br />

70; in Mexican-American families. 73;<br />

andsrress 103:elirninarion<strong>of</strong>. 216,217<br />

Physical srrmph: and husb:banil braring, 32;<br />

and injury, 33; and mdr dominance,<br />

182-83<br />

Physical <strong>Violence</strong> lndrx. 25<br />

Police, see Criminal jusiicr<br />

Power: struggles. 29. 160, 165, 166-67:<br />

division <strong>of</strong>, 87. 166. 201; claims on,<br />

123. 176; in marriage, 177: souicrs<strong>of</strong>,<br />

177. 178: and rrnploymenr <strong>of</strong> wife,<br />

179; definition <strong>of</strong>. 186n;nnd resources<br />

and violence, 190-209<br />

Pregnancy: and violence. 33, 172; and se,<br />

roles, 165, 170;response to, 165, 174"<br />

Piivacy: related to violence, 18: in family<br />

218<br />

Pueiio-Rican families, power in, 74<br />

Race: and artirudes <strong>of</strong> authorities. 43: anc<br />

violence in children's books, 61<br />

Rnpr: wirhin marriage, 47; number in U.S.,<br />

55; and power, 88: reduction <strong>of</strong>, 93"<br />

Rarionsl problem solving. 77<br />

Relationd complacence. in intimate rela-<br />

rionships. 149, 150<br />

Relsiional rules: interpretarion <strong>of</strong>. 139:<br />

violarion <strong>of</strong>, 140-50:ininrimarcgroups,<br />

140-54<br />

Relative Power lndrx, 193, 194<br />

Rrlarivc Resource Index, 193<br />

Relativr resources: and power. 196; and<br />

violence. 198, 201<br />

Release mechanism. 104. 111<br />

Religious beliefs, and children. 163<br />

Resolution: and srraregy choicc, 131; and<br />

conflicr. 219, 221<br />

Resouice theory: and violence. 105, 106,<br />

196; <strong>of</strong> power, 116. 178,189:tojusrify<br />

dominance, 166<br />

Resources: and male suprrioriry. 87; and<br />

physical force, 87: and strms, 105, 114;<br />

and subjecrive hostility, 118; claims<br />

over. 123: <strong>of</strong> wife, 156. 179, 198, 201,<br />

202; definirion <strong>of</strong>, 178. 205n; md<br />

power and violence, 190-209; measures<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 193;<strong>of</strong> husband. 198<br />

Response capabilities ro mess, 97.99, 100,<br />

104-5, 106, 110, 113<br />

Responsibility, and aurhority, 169<br />

Retsliarion, 143, 144<br />

Rcriicmenr, 17<br />

Retiibucion. 143<br />

Reward power, 192<br />

Roles: scx-typed, 16, 223, 224, 225, 228,<br />

229. 230; and violence, 34. 221: in<br />

tiansiiion, 76-77, 181; in mairi%ge, 87,<br />

165,181; conflicrs in. 104: and respon-<br />

sibiliry, 168; clarity <strong>of</strong>, 180, 181, 182,<br />

185; adoprion <strong>of</strong>, 181<br />

Romantic love, 120<br />

Rulc violations: accidenral, 14041: ai-


Subject Index<br />

riagc, 9; violencc used ro enforce. 59:<br />

contradictory, 123.<br />

Verbal aggression. 135n. 137. 187n. 220<br />

Verbal conflicr. 185<br />

Verbal expression. 79<br />

Victimizntion: <strong>of</strong> children. 93n:<strong>of</strong>women.<br />

20, 33.41<br />

Vioiencr, family: incidence <strong>of</strong>, 4; and . psy- .<br />

chopathology, 4, 8, 13, 152, 211; as<br />

socially learned and patreined bchsvior,<br />

4, 11, 14, 38, 107, 215;prcvalencc<strong>of</strong>.<br />

8; measurement <strong>of</strong>, 8, 25, 27, 191;underreporting<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 8, 30, 32, 191: Irgiiimate,<br />

13, 22% 25, 38, 39, 41, 53, 58,<br />

106-7; parent-child, 13, 47. 63; ikgitimate,<br />

14. 22". 53; iime factor, relared<br />

to, 15, 18; definirion <strong>of</strong>. 22n, 124;<br />

"deserved," 24; normal. 24. 30, 43,<br />

173; imiracion <strong>of</strong>, 32; jusiification <strong>of</strong>,<br />

35; arrests and, 44-45, 212: rolemodrls<br />

for, 49, 213, 230; and sporis, 52; fictional,<br />

and social control <strong>of</strong> deviance,<br />

57-61; sibling, 63, 216, 217; imirarion<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 71; as last resort, 77: peiperuation<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 80, 81"; curing <strong>of</strong>, 94; prcdisposiiion<br />

to, 103; as response to srrers, 105:<br />

alrrrnarives ro, 106. 115; social psychological<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong>, 114, 137. 151; as<br />

subtype <strong>of</strong> aggression, 130; atrribution<br />

<strong>of</strong>. 134". 136. 137, 138-39. 141-50;<br />

accidcnral, 137, 138; and rule violations.<br />

144; and marital politics, 157-75;<br />

and resources and power, 190-209;<br />

governmcnrai, 213-14: and peers, 216-<br />

17; avoidance <strong>of</strong>, 223; unrmployrnenr<br />

and, 227, 228<br />

<strong>Violence</strong>. gcnrial, in US.. 55<br />

<strong>Violence</strong> index, 49<br />

Voluntary iclationshipn, 119, 170<br />

War: and children's literamre, 57; and vio-<br />

lence, 64, 213-14: sloiies, 66"<br />

<strong>Wife</strong>: role, 89; stlying home, 166; as vic-<br />

tim, 171; as dominant, 171-72, 173;<br />

economic dependence <strong>of</strong>, 176, 222,<br />

228; and power, 198-99; and violence,<br />

200. 201-2. 208n: resources <strong>of</strong>, 200,<br />

202<br />

<strong>Wife</strong>, employment <strong>of</strong>, 92, 150, 156, 159,<br />

172. 176. 178-79, 223. 224.225, 228,<br />

230: and male dominance, 19. 179-80;<br />

consequences <strong>of</strong>, 176. 177; and vio-<br />

lence, 179, 180; and resources, 185<br />

<strong>Wife</strong>-bearing: as accrpiabie, 23, 24, 71;<br />

definition <strong>of</strong>, 24; index <strong>of</strong>, 25, 27: ex-<br />

tent <strong>of</strong>. 27-31; causes <strong>of</strong>. 33-36; pre-<br />

vention <strong>of</strong>, 35, 212-13, 231. 232; and<br />

sexual inequaliry, 86; as reflection <strong>of</strong><br />

societal violence, 213-15; sex role and<br />

sexism, 221; toleration <strong>of</strong>, 226; risks<br />

<strong>of</strong>, 232<br />

Wirhdrawal, red Avoiiiancc<br />

Women: subordination <strong>of</strong>. 41. 165; as ag-<br />

gressors, 62; in violent acts, 66": in<br />

,Mexican-American culrurc. 74; negn-<br />

rive self-images <strong>of</strong>. 89; as children, 89-<br />

90, 224, 225; righrs <strong>of</strong>, 92; and suc-<br />

cess, 92; and power, 199<br />

Women's movement. 181, 222: and wifr-<br />

bearing. 24; goal <strong>of</strong> reducing violence,<br />

90-91; conrriburions <strong>of</strong>, 91; men's<br />

reactions to, 178; resistance ro. 225<br />

Work-cthic, 66"<br />

Working class, 190-91, 194, 196. 200-5<br />

passim<br />

Working wife, See <strong>Wife</strong>, rmploymenr <strong>of</strong>


About the Authors<br />

CRAIG 8. ILLEN 1s ZP Instznctoz in Soci3LJgy a? 10x2<br />

State Uni7ersiCy. AB t25 done zesearch on fsaily<br />

zPtervactiOn techniqoes, pazental discipline 328 13cas <strong>of</strong><br />

Control iP children, and childrsnqs ocdeling <strong>of</strong> cartcoo<br />

characters. Cnrrently, h? is studying relationships among<br />

measures <strong>of</strong> conjogal power to dere=m:ne if indexes based 3n<br />

con?estod paver are more sensitivs indicators 3f the mazitel<br />

power balance than indexes based on uncorteste3 povsr. 9s<br />

15 the au'hor <strong>of</strong> "Role Specializatiol and Locus <strong>of</strong> Contr~l,"<br />

PrEsBnted at the Third Ancoal Pamilp Reselrch Corference,<br />

Brigham Young <strong>University</strong>. 1974.<br />

BRUCE Y. BRORR is hssist31t PTO~SSSO? Of SO~i0103Y at<br />

Xilkes College in wilkes-Barre, Penrsylvacia. Ee has done<br />

TESO~TC~ on historical a?+lysis <strong>of</strong> the family, femily<br />

measuresent techniques. and discipline <strong>of</strong> childrec. 9e is<br />

COY &ling a stndy <strong>of</strong> how family life has been depicted ir<br />

magizinc advertising sicce 1920, which eaphasizss both<br />

historical and life cycle changes in family companionship<br />

+nd irtimacy.<br />

na19 marriage<br />

Be is the author <strong>of</strong> "Tha Cimngicg Bole 3: Zhe<br />

Partner as D~picted in ths 20th ceztory<br />

rarriage a+rualn (New York state Soci~logicsl Rssociition,<br />

1973); "Edncation, Employment, and Prescriptiors for<br />

Marit21 D~c1sion-i3ekixg: 1900-1974" (8msricao Sociologicel<br />

nssociation. 1977): "nagazioe Advertising: Its Use i~ the<br />

exstorical stndy <strong>of</strong> the Psmily" (Pennsylvaoia Sociological<br />

Society. 1979): "<strong>Wife</strong>-Enploysent an6 the Emergence <strong>of</strong><br />

Equa1l:arzan Earit21 Pole Prescr:pticns: 1900-1974"<br />

(zsgm& Of Com~arative Fzmilv Stcdls, Sprirq 1978); and<br />

"Parent's Discxpline <strong>of</strong> Children in Poblic Placesu ( E a _ ~ ~ & ~<br />

coortinatar, July 1979)- He is co-author, with Burray A.<br />

straos, <strong>of</strong> SG:&V ngaggrsgn: TPchnQggs (university <strong>of</strong><br />

Minnesota rzrss, 1978); and vith Joseph A. Pznzanaro. <strong>of</strong><br />

"Compulsory Pre-Karital Educatioo?" (Rilkss College<br />

Quarterly, summer 1979).


JOSEPH CARROLL is ar Pssistart Pr<strong>of</strong>esso; in Sociclogy<br />

at Colby-Sawyer College ir New LorAon, Anv Aaapsb.irn. Pe<br />

has done research on the icterosnera~ional transmisa~nn <strong>of</strong><br />

faaily violence, and cliaa+alogical factcrs associated with<br />

aggression. He is now doing a study <strong>of</strong> causes <strong>of</strong><br />

psychological distress and family problems that emphasizes<br />

the effect <strong>of</strong> varying degrees <strong>of</strong> intsgrati5n into one.5<br />

community. Ee is the author <strong>of</strong> "<strong>The</strong> Intergeneretional<br />

Transmission cf Family <strong>Violence</strong>: <strong>The</strong> Long-term Effects <strong>of</strong><br />

Aggressive Behavior." 1977). and the "Effect <strong>of</strong> Climate on<br />

Homicide and Suicide: Euaidity, Reat, and Cold" (Annual<br />

meetlng <strong>of</strong> the Society for the Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Problems,<br />

1977).<br />

KE<strong>IT</strong>H PBRRLNGTOB is a memher <strong>of</strong> the Department <strong>of</strong><br />

Sociology and Anthropology at whitman College. R i s research<br />

interests include the causes and consequenc?s <strong>of</strong> social<br />

Stress, labeling and deviance, and violsnt behavior. Re is<br />

the author or co-author <strong>of</strong> '<strong>The</strong> Scheduling <strong>of</strong> Personal<br />

Crises: Seasonal Changes in the Pace <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Ictivities<br />

and Aelp-seeking at mental Bealth Clinicsw (presented st the<br />

annual mPeting <strong>of</strong> the Society for the Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong><br />

Problems, New York City. August 1976). '?Family Violelce and<br />

Household Density: Does the Crowded Home Breed Aggression?"<br />

(presented at the acnual meeting <strong>of</strong> the Society for the<br />

Study <strong>of</strong> Soclal Problems. Chicago, Seprenher 1977). and "In<br />

Search <strong>of</strong> the nisslng Conceptual Framevork in Family<br />

Sociology: <strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Conflict Frameworkm (pfesert3d at thj<br />

annual maeting <strong>of</strong> the National Council on Paerly Rslations,<br />

San Diego, October 1977). He is currently doicg a<br />

macroscopic analysis <strong>of</strong> variations in the pace <strong>of</strong> social<br />

life and their effects.<br />

JOYCE E. POSS is an Assistant Pr<strong>of</strong>essor in Sccio13gy at<br />

Sangamon state State university in springfiela, Illinsis.<br />

She has done research on sex roles +rd mencll health, the<br />

impact <strong>of</strong> women's Studies courses on attitudes toward<br />

women's roles, and community perceptions <strong>of</strong> hospital<br />

Services. Currently she is doing a study <strong>of</strong> the types <strong>of</strong><br />

power assertions used by husbands and wives, uhich<br />

emphasizes relationships between structural and processual<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> power. She is the author <strong>of</strong> "Sex Differences in<br />

the Use oi mental Pealth Clinics: Real Illness or Patient<br />

Behavior?" (Society for the Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Problems Ccnual<br />

aeeting. 1974) ; and co-author <strong>of</strong> "Review Essay: "=pop<br />

Cor.structIon and the Sgoipgy gg t he Pamilx. by Nesl?y R.<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 'Eissing' Conceptual Framework in Family Sociology:<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Conflict Framework" Annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the


GERRLD T. EOTRLIRG is an Instructor in SOCiOlOgy at the<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> vcrnont. R i s research interests include<br />

Family <strong>Violence</strong>. Uental Health and the Family. and Sex Roles<br />

and Health Behavior. Ee is a member <strong>of</strong> the editorial board<br />

He is the author <strong>of</strong> "Family Vulnerability to Stigma as<br />

a Factor ir. Aelp-Seeking Behavior" (Society for the Stuay <strong>of</strong><br />

soclal Problems meetings 1975), "Facilitating Tiol€nce: Why<br />

Intimates Attribute Rggressionn (presented at the Rational<br />

Council 02 Family Relations meetings 1975). "<strong>Social</strong> Class<br />

and Value Orientation in the Phase Bovement <strong>of</strong> Faellies<br />

Through the Rental Realth System" (Society for the Study <strong>of</strong><br />

social Problems meeting 1976). "Sex Differences in<br />

~elp-Seeking Behavior" (Society for %he Stuay <strong>of</strong> social<br />

problem^ meeting 1978, with Saundra Atwell and Joyce 2.<br />

Poss) , and "'dolescent Life Changes and Illness: A<br />

comparison <strong>of</strong> Three nodels' (Eastern Soci~logical S~ciaty<br />

meetings 1978. to be<br />

Adolescence, 1978).<br />

publlshed hy the Journal <strong>of</strong> Youth<br />

BARTAA D. BUGGINS is an Instructor at Union College in<br />

the Department cf Sociology. She recently spent two years<br />

as a Yisiti3q Pr<strong>of</strong>essor i~ t h ~ Department <strong>of</strong> SJciology at<br />

the Universidade Federal de Pernambuco. Brazil. <strong>The</strong><br />

VisitlCg PrOteS~orship was funded by the Fletcher School <strong>of</strong><br />

Law and<br />

Foundation.<br />

Diplomacy, Tufts Uriversity,<br />

<strong>The</strong> title <strong>of</strong> her dissertation<br />

and th? Ford<br />

is "Punishaent<br />

without Crime: Deviaccr, <strong>Social</strong> Change and <strong>Social</strong> Control<br />

in Pernambuco, Brazil, 1860-1922." She is the co-author<br />

(with Scott G. HcNall) <strong>of</strong> "Guerilla Warfare: 4 Prsliminary<br />

study <strong>of</strong> Predisposing and Precipitating Factcrs," and <strong>of</strong> a<br />

paper presented at the 1977 Eastern Soci~logical society<br />

meeting on "<strong>The</strong> Relatiorship between <strong>Social</strong> 3na Political<br />

Conditions and<br />

Recife, Brazil."<br />

Historic6l Trends in Deviant Behav:or in<br />

RALPH LaROSSB is an lssistant Pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong> Sociology at<br />

Georgla State university in htlanta. Re has done rese%rch<br />

on the psycho-social aspects <strong>of</strong> marriage and first pregnancy


and is currently invclved in two projects: a participact<br />

observation study <strong>of</strong> the aging process and in in-deprh<br />

ir~iervieu studv <strong>of</strong> tno tzansition to parenthood. He is the<br />

8URRP.Y A. STBAUS has beec Pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong> Sociology at ?he<br />

Unlvsrsity <strong>of</strong> Nsu Rampshire since 1968. Ee is thf auLhor <strong>of</strong><br />

about 80 articles in the sociology <strong>of</strong> the family, violence.<br />

south Asia. rural sociology, and research methods: and ths<br />

fo110~ln9 books: Socioloqical hnal~sis: An E~pirical<br />

~ ~ ~ r a a Through c h - ~eplicetioc (1968); Family 8eas"rement<br />

Techniques (1969. revised ediLior, 1977) ; Family Bnalyais:<br />

Readinqs acd Replicaticns <strong>of</strong> Selected Studies (1969):<br />

Famlly Problem Solvinq (1971): end Violerce in the Family<br />

(1979). He<br />

+nd founding<br />

was Assistant Editcr <strong>of</strong> Sociological Lbntracts<br />

editor <strong>of</strong> the journal Teaching Soci3logy.<br />

straus was president <strong>of</strong><br />

Relations, vica-presidert<br />

the National Council on Family<br />

<strong>of</strong> the rasterr Sociolsgical<br />

Society, and member cf the Council <strong>of</strong> the Enerican<br />

Rssociatioz to= tte Fdoa~cemelt <strong>of</strong> Scierce. Ee was recently<br />

given the E. V. Burgess award for cutstandirg csctributions<br />

to research on the family. Togethsr with Bichara Gelles and<br />

Suzanne Steinmetz, he is the author <strong>of</strong> 3 book 3n PiPLgaCP LC<br />

*&g &AgrLc&Q Ej~iLy based or data from :he ca+.ionslly<br />

representstise sample <strong>of</strong> 2,143 families acscribsa io the<br />

article in thls issue.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!