23.12.2012 Views

Effects of Graded Texts on EFL College Students' Incidental ...

Effects of Graded Texts on EFL College Students' Incidental ...

Effects of Graded Texts on EFL College Students' Incidental ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

國立清華大學<br />

碩士論文<br />

英文文章分級及單字接觸次數對大學生<br />

非刻意單字學習影響之研究<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Graded</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>EFL</strong> <strong>College</strong> Students’ <strong>Incidental</strong><br />

Vocabulary Learning: Text Difficulty and Exposure Amount<br />

所別:外國語文學系碩士班外語教學組<br />

學號:g915253<br />

姓名:黃虹慈<br />

指導教授:劉顯親博士<br />

中華民國九十三年六月


中文摘要<br />

透過英文閱讀途徑非刻意所習得的單字(<strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary learning) 已成<br />

為成人終身學習中常見的一環;因此,我們經常鼓勵第二外語學習者培養泛讀<br />

(extensive reading)習慣以增進單字能力。然而,第二外語學習者在閱讀時卻常因<br />

為認字率不高而無法準確猜出新單字的意思,或因為新單字重複率低而難以對新<br />

單字有再次遇見及深入的瞭解和認識的機會。準備適合不同單字程度之學習者以<br />

及新單字重複率高的閱讀教材一直是英語教學界的一大挑戰。隨著字彙表相關研<br />

究以及量化語料庫分析的進步,適合不同學習者且具有 i+1 可理解輸入<br />

(comprehensible input)的閱讀教材可經由電腦分析以更快速準確的方式篩選出<br />

來。此研究旨在研發一線上閱讀環境,內含自動選文功能,融入種種線上單字學<br />

習工具,以提供學習者認字率高且新單字重複率高的閱讀教材。我們並測試受試<br />

者使用此教材的單字學習狀況。<br />

我們的研究問題包含(一)將此線上閱讀環境融入課程之可行性,(二)受<br />

試者對於自動篩選線上閱讀教材之反應,(三)在線上閱讀教材自動篩選系統的<br />

學習環境中,學習者對於學會新單字所需的接觸次數。自動篩選系統根據四種字<br />

彙表 (The General Service Word List, 大學入學考試中心高中英文參考字彙表,<br />

The University Word List, Sinorama Frequent Word List)將文章排序。系統篩選後,<br />

將較為簡單且新單字重複率高的文章排序列為優先。此外,我們也將三種線上工<br />

具:中文註解、雙語索引工具、以及以不同顏色標明文章中新單字,融入該線上<br />

閱讀的學習環境中。38 位大一非英文系學生參與此實驗,在約 12 週內閱讀 16<br />

篇由自動篩選系統所選出來的文章。實驗進行前,受試者填寫一份背景問卷。所<br />

有受試者的字彙學習情況皆接受前測及後測評量。實驗結束後,受試者填寫一份<br />

線上評估問卷及一份紙筆評估問卷。<br />

資料分析結果,顯示第一、結合種種線上工具及自動篩選過之閱讀教材對於<br />

單字學習是可行的,學生對單字認識有顯著的進步。第二、問卷結果顯示受試者<br />

對利用線上閱讀教材自動篩選系統抱持著正面的態度,並肯定其學習單字的功<br />

能。第三、在此閱讀環境中,15 次的新單字接觸次數尚不足讓學生將新單字用<br />

於理解及表達。<br />

本研究結果顯示利用英文線上閱讀教材自動篩選及泛讀學習可提升英文單<br />

字能力。此外,我們認為該線上閱讀教材自動篩選系統有助於教師融合課堂中顯<br />

性教學(explicit teaching)的精讀(intensive reading)課程以及課外隱性學習(implicit<br />

learning)的泛讀練習,使學習者的單字學習更為有效。最後,此研究提供了利用<br />

線上閱讀教材自動選文系統的教學建議以及未來系統設計修正方向。<br />

i


Abstract<br />

<strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary learning while extensive reading has been regarded as<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> for adults’ life-l<strong>on</strong>g learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a foreign language. Extensive reading is<br />

therefore encouraged to L2 learners for enhancing vocabulary ability. However,<br />

while reading, L2 learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten struggle with their lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sufficient word knowledge<br />

necessary for precise guessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meanings and the ample opportunities to<br />

encounter words repeatedly within various c<strong>on</strong>texts required for more complicated<br />

word development. Preparing texts reaching adequate familiar word coverage for<br />

specific groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners and at the same time ensuring sufficient repetiti<strong>on</strong> for<br />

target words is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten challenging. With the help <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists research and the<br />

advances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> quantitative corpus analyses using word frequency computer programs,<br />

texts with i+1 (comprehensible input) are easier to obtain. By incorporating various<br />

<strong>on</strong>line tools, an effectiveness research study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> using an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

program with careful c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary difficulty and repetitive target word<br />

exposure was c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

This study originated from an exploratory issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> what happens to the English<br />

teaching process when we incorporate an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus with<br />

graded texts for vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> purposes. The <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus was developed by graded texts, gloss, a bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, and<br />

highlights <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words. With four word lists, the General Service Word List, the<br />

Senior High Student’s Word List in Taiwan, the University Word List, and a high<br />

frequent word list, a text grader program filtered articles based <strong>on</strong> vocabulary<br />

difficulty out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the original 5008 articles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a parallel corpus, Sinorama. Easier<br />

texts were sequenced first to provide texts with i+1 and repetitive exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target<br />

words was ensured to induce quality incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. The <strong>on</strong>line<br />

ii


eading program was furthermore enhanced with gloss, a bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, and<br />

highlights <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words. Thirty-eight English-as-a-foreign-language college<br />

freshmen learners were recruited to read 16 sequenced texts using the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus within a period <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 12 weeks. A pretest, a posttest, a background<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire, an <strong>on</strong>line evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire, and an in-class evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire were used as instruments for data collecti<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong> to feasibility,<br />

our sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>cern is to which extent learners liked the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus. Finally, the study takes an interest in exploring the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure required for successful receptive and productive incidental vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> while reading extensively.<br />

Through data analyses, the following findings were obtained. First, technically,<br />

though manual efforts were laborious during certain steps in the selecti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

sequencing processes, texts with comprehensible vocabulary were successfully<br />

chosen and arranged with various <strong>on</strong>line tools. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, pedagogically, learners’<br />

word gains were found and their attitudes towards the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus were mostly positive after reading <strong>on</strong>line. Yet, learners’ inexperience in<br />

extensive reading was observed to somewhat impede the meaning making process<br />

during reading. Third, no definite answer was found with regards to the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure necessary for receptive and productive vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. It is<br />

estimated that, with texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such difficult level in the study, in order for learners to<br />

recall meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, an exposure amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> at least 15 times are needed in<br />

an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading envir<strong>on</strong>ment.<br />

The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the study showed that the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus could<br />

foster incidental vocabulary learning while reading. Moreover, it is suggested that<br />

accompanied with in-class explicit instructi<strong>on</strong>, the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

with implicit learning material, could serve as a bridge linking explicit teaching and<br />

iii


implicit learning for learners’ quality vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Finally, teaching implicati<strong>on</strong>s recommended teachers’ input <strong>on</strong> reading strategy<br />

training, word instructi<strong>on</strong>, and evaluati<strong>on</strong> for better incorporati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit teaching<br />

and implicit learning using the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus. With regards to<br />

directi<strong>on</strong>s for future refinements <strong>on</strong> developing an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus,<br />

tools which foster learner interacti<strong>on</strong> were encouraged for effective incidental<br />

vocabulary learning.<br />

iv


TABLE OF CONTENTS<br />

中文摘要 i<br />

ABSTRACT ii<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS v<br />

TABLE OF CONTENTS vi<br />

LIST OF TABLES viii<br />

LIST OF FIGURES ix<br />

Chapter One – INTRODUCTION 1<br />

Chapter Two – REVIEW OF LITERATURE 5<br />

2.1. Overview 5<br />

2.2. Computerized Gloss and <strong>Incidental</strong> Vocabulary Learning 5<br />

2.3. Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> with Word Lists 10<br />

2.4. Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> Through Sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> 13<br />

2.5. Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Exposure to a Word and Successful Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> 19<br />

2.6. Measuring <strong>Incidental</strong> Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> 22<br />

2.7. Summary 24<br />

Chapter Three—METHODOLOGY 26<br />

3.1.Overview 26<br />

3.2.Participants 26<br />

3.3.The Design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an Online Extensive Reading Syllabus 27<br />

3.3.1. The Reading Material: the Sinorama Magazine 27<br />

3.3.2. Four Word Lists 29<br />

3.3.3. Sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> 33<br />

3.3.4. Glossing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> 44<br />

3.3.5. Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interfaces 44<br />

3.4.Instruments 45<br />

3.4.1. Pretest 45<br />

3.4.2. Posttest 46<br />

3.4.3. Questi<strong>on</strong>naires 48<br />

3.5.Procedures 50<br />

3.6.Data Analysis 53<br />

Chapter Four—RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 55<br />

4.1. Overview 55<br />

4.2. Results 56<br />

4.2.1. The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 56<br />

4.2.2. The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Pretest 59<br />

4.2.3. The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Posttest 61<br />

4.2.4. The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 66<br />

vi


4.3. Discussi<strong>on</strong> 70<br />

4.3.1. The Feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Incorporating Online Tools to an Online<br />

Extensive Reading Syllabus for Vocabulary Learning Purposes<br />

70<br />

4.3.2. Learners’ Feedback towards an Online Extensive Reading<br />

Syllabus 73<br />

4.3.3. Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Exposure Necessary for <strong>Incidental</strong> Vocabulary<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> 75<br />

4.3.4. Summary 80<br />

Chapter Five—CONCLUSION 82<br />

5.1. Overview 82<br />

5.2. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Study 83<br />

5.3. Directi<strong>on</strong>s for Future Research 84<br />

5.4. Developmental Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for an Online Extensive Reading Syllabus 85<br />

5.5. Pedagogical Implicati<strong>on</strong>s 87<br />

References 90<br />

Appendix A—The Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 96<br />

Appendix B—The Pretest 99<br />

Appendix C—The Posttest 101<br />

Appendix D—An Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the VKS Scoring 107<br />

Appendix E—The Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 109<br />

Appendix F—The In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 110<br />

Appendix G—The Syllabus for the Sixteen Readings 111<br />

Appendix H—The Introductory Page <strong>on</strong> How to Use the Online Extensive Reading<br />

Syllabus 112<br />

Appendix I—The First Reading <strong>on</strong> the Syllabus 113<br />

Appendix J—A Search Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Bilingual C<strong>on</strong>cordancer TotalRecall 114<br />

Appendix K—An Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>s 115<br />

Appendix L-- Student Reading Process Recorded by Tracker 116<br />

Appendix M—C<strong>on</strong>sent Form 117<br />

Appendix N—The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 118<br />

Appendix O—The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 121<br />

Appendix P—The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire 122<br />

vii


List <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tables<br />

Table 3.1 Summary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four word lists 32<br />

Table 3.2 Target word rate and target word repetiti<strong>on</strong> rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the sixteen texts 38<br />

Table 3.3 Distributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words in the sixteen reading texts 38<br />

Table 3.4 Procedures in sequencing reading texts 39<br />

Table 3.5 A comparis<strong>on</strong> between Ghardirian’s (2002) study and the current study<br />

42<br />

Table 3.6 A comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing procedures in TextLadder and the current<br />

study 43<br />

Table 3.7 Self-report categories in the VKS elicitati<strong>on</strong> scale 46<br />

Table 3.8 VKS scoring categories: Meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> scores 47<br />

Table 3.9 Instruments used in the study 50<br />

Table 3.10 Research Procedures 52<br />

Table 4.1 Learners’ use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer and internet 57<br />

Table 4.2 Learners’ perspectives <strong>on</strong> English learning 58<br />

Table 4.3 Learners’ extensive reading experience 59<br />

Table 4.4 The independent t-test for familiar and unfamiliar words in the pretest<br />

60<br />

Table 4.5 Target words completely unfamiliar to learners 61<br />

Table 4.6 Learners’ overall vocabulary gains 62<br />

Table 4.7 The ANOVA statistics for posttest scores am<strong>on</strong>g the five groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

different exposure amount 63<br />

Table 4.8 The independent t-test for the high vocabulary group and the low<br />

vocabulary group 64<br />

Table 4.9 Target words and COBUILD frequency bands 65<br />

Table 4.10 Learners’ motivati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> future use 67<br />

Table 4.11 Learners’ attitudes towards features fostering vocabulary learning 68<br />

Table 4.12 Learners’ attitudes towards features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts 69<br />

Table 4.13 Learners’ attitudes towards interface design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the system 69<br />

viii


List <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Figure<br />

Figure 4.1 Percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> known and not known words in the pretest and the<br />

posttest 71<br />

ix


Chapter One<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

It is generally agreed by sec<strong>on</strong>d language (L2) learning researchers that<br />

vocabulary growth comm<strong>on</strong>ly occurs incidentally while learners are engaged in other<br />

cognitive exercises involving comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Am<strong>on</strong>g these cognitive activities,<br />

reading, especially extensive reading, has attracted much research attenti<strong>on</strong> due to the<br />

fact that tremendous vocabulary growth, high overall language competence, and better<br />

academic performances were observed from first language (L1) learners with pleasure<br />

reading habits (Nati<strong>on</strong>, 2001). Moreover, incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> through<br />

reading is also used to explain how children acquire mass amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> L1 vocabulary<br />

within a relatively short period <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> time.<br />

Huckin and Coady (1999) defined incidental learning in reading as a by-product,<br />

not the target, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the main cognitive activity. In the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading, learners’<br />

incidental vocabulary learning is the unc<strong>on</strong>scious acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meanings<br />

through engaging in texts for comprehensi<strong>on</strong> or understanding. In such process,<br />

word knowledge is thought to be cumulated and developed gradually through multiple<br />

exposures in various reading c<strong>on</strong>texts. Krashen (1989) further claimed that<br />

“competence in spelling and vocabulary is most efficiently attained by<br />

comprehensible input in the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading” (p. 440). Extensive reading, as a form<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensible input, has the effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> providing learners with rich c<strong>on</strong>texts ideal<br />

for vocabulary learning. During the reciprocal process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading, the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words is the result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> successes in inferring word meanings from<br />

meaningful c<strong>on</strong>text, and through more reading experiences the developed and<br />

matured vocabulary inference ability could in turn c<strong>on</strong>tributes to reading pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency.<br />

For L2 learning, extensive reading is c<strong>on</strong>sidered as beneficial for overall language<br />

1


competence (Elley, 1991; Hafiz & Tudor, 1989;1990; Krashen, 1993; Mas<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Krashen, 1997; Renandya & Rajan, 1999) and fundamental to vocabulary<br />

development as well. Researchers such as Krashen believe that similar to L1 word<br />

learning, reading is the major means for acquiring vocabulary and have thus str<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

encouraged the implementati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading programs in an L2 learning<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment (Krashen, 1989).<br />

While extensive reading does have an impact <strong>on</strong> vocabulary learning, studies<br />

have found that reading for L2 vocabulary development may lead to problems such as<br />

wr<strong>on</strong>g inferences, lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> retenti<strong>on</strong>, and most importantly, superficial vocabulary<br />

learning that impedes learners from using words actively and productively (Huckin &<br />

Coady, 1999; Hulstijn, 1993; Raptis, 1993; Wesche & Paribakht, 2000). Unlike<br />

native speakers, L2 learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten struggle with their lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sufficient word<br />

knowledge necessary for precise guessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meanings and the ample<br />

opportunities to encounter words within various c<strong>on</strong>texts required for more<br />

complicated word development. Correct inference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meanings is c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

up<strong>on</strong> accurate recogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> surrounding words and sensitive use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading<br />

strategies (Huckin & Coady, 1999). However, with previous studies indicating L2<br />

learners’ comm<strong>on</strong> insufficiency in word knowledge and lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> active use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different<br />

reading strategies (Chen, 1998; Grabe & Stoller, 1997), learners’ frustrati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> can be expected when facing overwhelming authentic texts in<br />

extensive reading, let al<strong>on</strong>e incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to learners’ limited language competence, limited reading<br />

opportunities impose yet another comm<strong>on</strong> stumbling block for incidental vocabulary<br />

learning. Since word knowledge is not an all-for-nothing piece <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning, but a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuum gradually developed with each encounter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in different c<strong>on</strong>texts,<br />

meeting a certain word <strong>on</strong>ce is clearly inadequate for bringing learners further <strong>on</strong> the<br />

2


word knowledge c<strong>on</strong>struct. <strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in L2 reading may<br />

easily fail because “<strong>on</strong>ce the immediate communicative need has been met, the<br />

learner does not undertake future mental processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word” (Wesche &<br />

Paribakht, 2000, p. 197). That is, it is possible for extensive exposure to various<br />

reading c<strong>on</strong>texts to lead to basic vocabulary recogniti<strong>on</strong>, yet, development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> further<br />

knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words, which enables learners to use words in productive mode, cannot<br />

be guaranteed (Paribakht & Wesche, 1997). Furthermore, though exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

is c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be a major factor influencing the learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary, the amount<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure needed for learning a word remains an unsettled questi<strong>on</strong> with mixed<br />

results in previous studies (Nagy, et al., 1985; Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1990; Saragi, et al., 1978; Rott,<br />

1999).<br />

The current study attempts to address the two major difficulties comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

encountered by sec<strong>on</strong>d language learners in reading texts: limited word knowledge,<br />

and limited exposures to target words, by developing an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

syllabus. With the help <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer programming, we wish to provide<br />

comprehensible reading texts by sequencing reading texts according to word difficulty<br />

level and to enhance vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> by ensuring repetitious occurrences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words. Implicati<strong>on</strong>s and pedagogical suggesti<strong>on</strong>s from previous studies <strong>on</strong><br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> via reading such as gloss, high frequency word lists, and<br />

sequenced reading texts are incorporated in order to create a reading envir<strong>on</strong>ment to<br />

facilitate vocabulary learning. Learners’ vocabulary learning is tested to verify the<br />

effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> our <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus. As the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive<br />

reading syllabus was designed with repetitious exposure to target words, the number<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encounters needed for a word to be acquired successfully was another research<br />

focus in this study. It is hoped that the study would shed some light <strong>on</strong> two aspects:<br />

the feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> developing an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus incorporable to<br />

3


<strong>EFL</strong> classes, and determining the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word exposure sufficient for<br />

Taiwanese college learners in vocabulary learning via reading.<br />

4


2.1. Overview<br />

Chapter Two<br />

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE<br />

In this secti<strong>on</strong>, issues <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incidental vocabulary learning while reading are<br />

discussed to show where the gaps are and some possible improvements CALL may<br />

help al<strong>on</strong>g this line <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> research. This chapter c<strong>on</strong>sists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four major secti<strong>on</strong>s each<br />

maintaining a possible dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitating vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language learners in extensive reading c<strong>on</strong>texts. The first secti<strong>on</strong> reviews studies<br />

related to the effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computerized gloss and incidental vocabulary learning.<br />

For designing a classroom syllabus, word lists for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> are taken up in the sec<strong>on</strong>d secti<strong>on</strong>. The third secti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerns<br />

enhancement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> through sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts. Last,<br />

the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to a word for successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> is<br />

introduced.<br />

2.2. Computerized Gloss and <strong>Incidental</strong> Vocabulary Learning<br />

C<strong>on</strong>textual guessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary while reading is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten encouraged by<br />

researchers rather than dicti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> due to the c<strong>on</strong>cern that dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use<br />

in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading could be interruptive and thus interfering the short term<br />

memory which results in impeded reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Empirical studies,<br />

however, have not provided evidence to substantiate such asserti<strong>on</strong>s. Luppescu and<br />

Day (1993) investigated the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilingual dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use <strong>on</strong> vocabulary<br />

learning by recruiting 293 <strong>EFL</strong> Japanese university students and found dicti<strong>on</strong>ary<br />

access led to superior vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> as opposed to no dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access.<br />

Support for dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use while reading and teachers’ instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary<br />

5


c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> for sec<strong>on</strong>d language learners were thus recommended. Taking the study<br />

by Luppescu and Day a step further, Hulstijn, Hollander, and Greidanus (1996)<br />

compared three reading c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s: reading with marginal glosses, reading with<br />

dicti<strong>on</strong>ary, and reading without external help. Participants reading with marginal<br />

gloss <strong>on</strong> paper in this study outperformed participants in two other groups. Marginal<br />

gloss was therefore proposed as a possible soluti<strong>on</strong> to overcome the comm<strong>on</strong> obstacle<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inaccurate meaning inference in L2 reading. Knight (1994) randomly assigned<br />

105 university students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish learners in high and low intermediate verbal ability<br />

levels into two reading c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the computer: <strong>on</strong>-line dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access and no<br />

dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access. Recalls in immediate and delayed vocabulary definiti<strong>on</strong> selecti<strong>on</strong><br />

tests showed that subjects who used dicti<strong>on</strong>aries achieved higher reading<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> scores than those who merely guessed from c<strong>on</strong>texts. For<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, the low verbal ability subjects in the dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> scored as high as subjects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high verbal ability in the same reading<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, indicating that dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access enabled learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> low verbal ability to<br />

learn as many words as their high verbal ability counterparts.<br />

The above studies attributed the significance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use and marginal<br />

gloss to learners’ additi<strong>on</strong>al knowledge source and deeper mental processing in the<br />

process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> looking up word meaning. They foregrounded the fact that though<br />

extensive reading is doubtlessly c<strong>on</strong>ducive to vocabulary growth, reading for global<br />

meaning al<strong>on</strong>e is inadequate. To initiate word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, learners’ attenti<strong>on</strong> must,<br />

at some point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the cognitive activity, be directed to form-meaning relati<strong>on</strong>ships <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words (Ellis, 1995; Huckin & Coady, 1999; Hulstijn et. al., 1996). The depth<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing hypothesis was comm<strong>on</strong>ly discussed to explicate such outcome. It<br />

states that “mental activities which require more elaborate thought, manipulati<strong>on</strong>, or<br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a new word will help in the learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> that word” (Schmitt & Schmitt,<br />

6


1995, p. 135). Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary and marginal gloss use while reading encourage deeper<br />

processing and allow c<strong>on</strong>scious attenti<strong>on</strong> to form-meaning relati<strong>on</strong>ship <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

thereby enabling learners to integrate new words into broader mental lexical network,<br />

a process indispensable for l<strong>on</strong>g-term storage. Compared to reading without<br />

dicti<strong>on</strong>ary access in which readers are likely to ignore unfamiliar words or induce<br />

incorrect word meanings, dicti<strong>on</strong>ary and gloss use could be regarded as additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mental activities engaging learners in deeper processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anchoring new words into<br />

their existing mental lexic<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In accordance with the c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing, Laufer and Hulstijn (2001)<br />

proposed the Involvement Load Hypothesis for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> operati<strong>on</strong>alizing<br />

cognitive noti<strong>on</strong>s such as depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> L2 vocabulary learning tasks.<br />

In this hypothesis, involvement load <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a task is measured by three involvement<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents— need, which is learners’ motivati<strong>on</strong>al involvement to accomplish the<br />

task requirement; search, the endeavor to find word meaning, and evaluati<strong>on</strong>, learners’<br />

attempt to specify a given word meaning by comparing it with other word meaning<br />

and surveying its surrounding c<strong>on</strong>text. The presence or absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these three<br />

elements could, as argued by Hulstijn and Laufer, explain and predict success in<br />

vocabulary retenti<strong>on</strong>. The noti<strong>on</strong> “search” defined as “the attempt to find the<br />

meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an unknown L2 word or the attempt to find the L2 word form expressing a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept” (Laufer & Hulstijn, 2001, p. 543) entailed that vocabulary retenti<strong>on</strong> is, in<br />

certain degree, c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong> the meaning searching process that learners are<br />

engaged in. Therefore, we reas<strong>on</strong> that the access for learners to engage in word<br />

searching activity should be provided even within extensive reading c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Following the main line <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> research <strong>on</strong> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary and gloss, studies have<br />

indicated that the intrusive nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use could be amended by the<br />

provisi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> marginal gloss, and the effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> marginal paper glossing was<br />

7


vastly reinforced with the ease and efficiency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning searching and greater<br />

diversity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> glossing types that computerized gloss could <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer (Chun & Plass, 1996;<br />

Davis, 1989; Leffa, 1992; Liou, 2000; Hulstijn, 1993; Hulstijn, et al., 1996; Jacobs,<br />

1994). The innovative potential <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating multimedia effects such as sounds,<br />

pictures, animated pictures, or videos into glosses led researchers into the directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

investigating specific modes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> annotati<strong>on</strong>, and found that pictures as annotati<strong>on</strong> types,<br />

especially, have received ample positive results from empirical studies (Chun & Plass,<br />

1996; Kost et al.,1999; Schnotz & Grz<strong>on</strong>dziel, 1996; Yoshii & Flaitz, 2002). Chun<br />

and Plass (1996) examined a variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> annotati<strong>on</strong>s for individual words in the form<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text, graphics, video, and sound with 103 university students learning German.<br />

The results dem<strong>on</strong>strated approximately a quarter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the new words learned by the<br />

subjects, with the participants in the text-plus-picture c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> scoring significantly<br />

higher than those in other c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Kost, Foss, and Lenzini (1999) compared the<br />

effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pictorial and textual glosses <strong>on</strong> incidental vocabulary growth for foreign<br />

language learners. Participants were divided into three c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s when reading,<br />

textual gloss al<strong>on</strong>e, pictorial gloss al<strong>on</strong>e, and text combined with pictures. Both<br />

producti<strong>on</strong> and recepti<strong>on</strong> tests delivered after reading showed that participants who<br />

were allowed to use a combinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text and picture outperformed the others.<br />

Though multimedia effects seemed promising in facilitating vocabulary learning,<br />

they do, however, ineluctably possess limitati<strong>on</strong>s when applied to extensive reading<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text. Previous studies probing into gloss effects have merely c<strong>on</strong>centrated <strong>on</strong><br />

pre-developed glosses for a small number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts, or searching and inserting<br />

picture glosses for <strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>crete words due to the fact that abstract words are<br />

complicated to express by still pictures in nature. In other words, various<br />

multimedia effects inevitably face technical challenges when applied to the diversity<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word types in extensive reading. On the other hand, plain textual gloss is more<br />

8


technically feasible for handling the abundant and distinct word types in extensive<br />

reading envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to technical c<strong>on</strong>straints, results from empirical studies in learner<br />

behavior have pointed to the directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> developing simple textual gloss for extensive<br />

reading purposes. The study c<strong>on</strong>ducted by Davis and Lyman-Hager (1997) showed<br />

that the most frequently c<strong>on</strong>sulted annotati<strong>on</strong> type was learners’ first language,<br />

English definiti<strong>on</strong>, which c<strong>on</strong>stituted 85% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the informati<strong>on</strong> accessed. Likewise,<br />

Lomicka (1998) also reported students’ str<strong>on</strong>g dependence <strong>on</strong> first language definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

when word meanings were unknown. It was suggested that though learners have the<br />

opti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various annotati<strong>on</strong> types, they nevertheless tend to rely <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> L1<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong> and ignore other opti<strong>on</strong>s for their goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> using glosses is to translate<br />

sentences in order to achieve a minimal level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Gettys, Imh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>, and Kautz (2001) further explained students’ behaviors with the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> human mental capacity. It was c<strong>on</strong>tended that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fering various<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> to readers might overrun the real needs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> readers and thus causes<br />

cognitive overload in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading. Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the limited capacity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

learners’ working memory and attenti<strong>on</strong>, providing readers with rich kinds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

informati<strong>on</strong> will likely create unnecessary distracti<strong>on</strong> and disrupti<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, the<br />

major c<strong>on</strong>cern for developing a gloss “should not be to provide as many annotati<strong>on</strong><br />

types as possible, but rather, reducing the informati<strong>on</strong> to the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> minimum<br />

sufficiency” (Gettys, et al., 2001, p. 99).<br />

The pedagogical implicati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> drawn from<br />

the studies <strong>on</strong> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary and gloss use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d language reading above underscored<br />

the following points:<br />

1. In the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading, certain amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, at some point,<br />

should be drawn to words in order for vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and ultimately L2<br />

9


learning to occur.<br />

2. Word meaning search activity could result in word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more quality,<br />

hence, search access should be provided for learners while reading.<br />

3. Gloss, especially computerized gloss is generally effective in bridging the gap<br />

between L2 text and the L2 learners’ limited cultural and lexical knowledge.<br />

Given technical difficulties and learners’ limited mental capacity, however, textual<br />

gloss is regarded as more feasible for computerized extensive reading<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

2.3. Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> with Word Lists<br />

Preliminary to discussing word list uses in vocabulary teaching, the salient term,<br />

word family, needs to be defined first. A word family is a group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words which<br />

have the same morphemic stem. It c<strong>on</strong>sists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a headword, its inflected forms such<br />

as plural, past tense, and comparatives; and its closely related derived forms such as<br />

words which are derived by adding affixes –ly, -ness, or un- to the headword (Nati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2001). In other words, items within a comm<strong>on</strong> word family would have various<br />

surface forms due to inflecti<strong>on</strong>al or derivati<strong>on</strong>al affixati<strong>on</strong>. Word families are <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten<br />

used as frequency counting units in word lists.<br />

The significance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> developing learners’ mental lexic<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an adequate<br />

vocabulary c<strong>on</strong>sisting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high frequent words was repeatedly emphasized by<br />

researchers in sec<strong>on</strong>d language learning (Carter & McCarthy, 1988; Cobb & Horst,<br />

2001; Ky<strong>on</strong>gho & Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1989; Nati<strong>on</strong>, 2001). It was estimated that the most<br />

frequent 1000 word families <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English, including words such as put, end, come and<br />

difficult, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer a coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> approximately 75% in any text (Cobb & Horst, 2001).<br />

Apparently, the return for learning a core vocabulary c<strong>on</strong>sisting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the most frequent<br />

words is significant. Based <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that learning burden could be<br />

10


minimized by learning high frequent words before less frequent words, several word<br />

lists such as Thorndike and Lorge’s list and Kucera and Francis’s list have been<br />

developed using the method <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> recording frequency counts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical items in actual<br />

samples <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> language (Ghadessy, 1979). Am<strong>on</strong>g the word lists, the General Service<br />

List (GSL, West, 1953) has remained as the most prominent word list today published.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>taining around 2000 word families, the GSL typically covers around 80% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

running words in any text (Nati<strong>on</strong>, 2001).<br />

Recent research <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>structing word lists as pedagogical material has<br />

progressed bey<strong>on</strong>d formulating a basic vocabulary list into two major aspects:<br />

empirical testing <strong>on</strong> using word lists to facilitate vocabulary learning and<br />

development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists for higher level learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various learning needs.<br />

Positive evidence for using word lists to accelerate vocabulary growth was found in<br />

empirical studies, followed by further detailed course designs based <strong>on</strong> word lists<br />

(Cobb & Horst, 2001; Flowerdew, 1993; Worthingt<strong>on</strong> & Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1996). Cobb and<br />

Horst (2001) recruited 58 ESL university students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intermediate level to study a<br />

word list roughly equivalent to the GSL, the 2,387-word Cambridge Lexic<strong>on</strong>, with the<br />

help <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, PET。2000. The results showed that learners were able to<br />

successfully acquire a gain <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 180 words within two m<strong>on</strong>ths, or 1,080 words if<br />

learning c<strong>on</strong>tinued for a year.<br />

Word counts in c<strong>on</strong>cordancers were suggested by Flowerdew (1993) as a tool for<br />

course design. He maintained that by using a corpus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> written and/or spoken text<br />

from the target communicative situati<strong>on</strong> with words appearing at different frequencies,<br />

each level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency in the c<strong>on</strong>cordancer <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fers a potential cut-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f point for<br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> and grading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items. A robust lexical syllabus accompanied with precise<br />

word counts is therefore available for classroom use.<br />

The sec<strong>on</strong>d research directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> word list vocabulary learning c<strong>on</strong>centrates <strong>on</strong><br />

11


inging learners who are competent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> basic general vocabulary further across higher<br />

lexical thresholds. The University Word List (UWL) developed by Xue and Nati<strong>on</strong><br />

(1984) with words that frequently occur in academic disciplines is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten employed by<br />

instructors for learners with basic word knowledge pursuing an academic career.<br />

Worthingt<strong>on</strong> and Nati<strong>on</strong> (1996) investigated the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how academic words in the<br />

University Word List could be sequenced for learners to meet a manageable porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words in authentic texts at appropriate intervals. With 134 academic texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various<br />

fields as corpora, it was reas<strong>on</strong>ed that it is not likely to meeting all or most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

words in UWL within a period <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a school semester. Moreover, learners without<br />

academic background would be expected to encounter a disproporti<strong>on</strong>ate large<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new words in their first few texts. Therefore, pedagogical suggesti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> or gloss help for high frequency words were proposed for learners<br />

in the initial introductory stage. Nati<strong>on</strong> and Ky<strong>on</strong>gho (1995) proposed a dividing<br />

line between a general service list and a special purpose vocabulary by using the<br />

Lancaster-Oslo/Bergen corpus (LOB) and the Brown corpus. In their study, a<br />

general service word list which served as a prerequisite for moving learners from<br />

general high frequency words to a specialized vocabulary was first determined by<br />

identifying words in GSL and high frequency words appearing more than ten times in<br />

LOB and Brown corpora. Two cut-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f points were then determined based <strong>on</strong> two<br />

criteria: words that appeared in all three sources (LOB, Brown, and GSL), and words<br />

that appeared in either two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the three sources (LOB & Brown, LOB & GSL, and<br />

GSL & Brown). These words together provided an 83.4% coverage in the corpora<br />

and were therefore, judged as words making up the general service list. Next, the<br />

words appearing <strong>on</strong>ly in <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the three sources were compared with words in the<br />

University Word List in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> percentage in the coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> corpora. The<br />

coverage figure for UWL is up to 8.5%, almost as high as words appearing in two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

12


the three sources. It was c<strong>on</strong>cluded that a general service vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> close to<br />

2000 words is suitable and sufficient for learners who are moving towards a<br />

specialized vocabulary. Also, after learners have achieved this 2000 word level, a<br />

specialized vocabulary such as UWL will c<strong>on</strong>tribute to more text coverage than the<br />

next words in the general word lists.<br />

While learning vocabulary by word lists does dem<strong>on</strong>strably result in eminently<br />

word growth, cauti<strong>on</strong> is needed for incorporating standard word lists as teaching<br />

material. As was warned by Ghadessy (1979), the idea <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> standard word lists<br />

“ignores the simple fact that the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new vocabulary items differ for<br />

various language learners because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> differences in linguistic, cultural, and<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong>al factors" (p. 25). In other words, no particular word list or sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

word list can satisfy all learners, and word lists should be c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong> and<br />

carefully tailored for learners’ specific needs.<br />

The above studies amply dem<strong>on</strong>strated the following c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1. Using high frequency word lists to select appropriate instructi<strong>on</strong>al material can<br />

substantively accelerate learners’ vocabulary growth.<br />

2. Selecti<strong>on</strong> or development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> particular sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists should be c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

up<strong>on</strong> learners’ specific needs.<br />

2.4. Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> Through Sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Vocabulary difficulties encountered by L2 students in authentic reading texts are<br />

major stumbling blocks to reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> and also further acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new<br />

words. Laufer (1989) found that learners whose vocabulary size enabled them to<br />

cover 95% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the words in a text were successful in achieving satisfactory text<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Under the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that learners are able to use c<strong>on</strong>textual<br />

inference strategies for incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, Laufer (1997) and Nati<strong>on</strong><br />

13


(1993) suggested a required 5,000 word lexic<strong>on</strong> for reaching this adequate 95%<br />

understanding in general and n<strong>on</strong>-specialized texts. However, most high school<br />

graduates in <strong>EFL</strong> countries have a far smaller vocabulary than this (Ky<strong>on</strong>gho &<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1989). With vocabulary size around 2000-3000 word level, Taiwanese<br />

college students are am<strong>on</strong>g those who are likely to fail when reading unsimplified<br />

texts (Chen, 1998). Due to the fact that authentic and unsimplified reading materials<br />

are generally written to c<strong>on</strong>vey informati<strong>on</strong> to those who are already familiar with the<br />

words in texts rather than for language learning, two essential vocabulary learning<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, intensive exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and comprehensible reading c<strong>on</strong>texts,<br />

can therefore hardly be found in authentic texts.<br />

<strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary learning from reading is not an all-or-nothing piece <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

leaning, but a gradual process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e encounter with a word adding to, strengthening,<br />

or further completing the knowledge established from previous encounters. Thus,<br />

if the small amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge is not so<strong>on</strong> reinforced by another meeting,<br />

then the previously built word knowledge will inextricably be lost. Vocabulary<br />

learning from extensive reading could therefore easily be lost, as Nati<strong>on</strong> (2001)<br />

cauti<strong>on</strong>ed “it is critically important in an extensive reading program that learners have<br />

the opportunity to keep meeting words that they have met before” (p. 156). In<br />

order to create a more optimal vocabulary learning envir<strong>on</strong>ment, sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

reading texts for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ensuring repetitive target word exposure and<br />

providing adequately comprehensible texts are thus suggested.<br />

By using newspaper articles as extensive reading materials, Ky<strong>on</strong>gho and Nati<strong>on</strong><br />

(1989) advised to sequence newspaper texts according to “running stories”, which are<br />

very closely related stories, to reduce the vocabulary load and to create an optimal<br />

word learning sequence for learners. It was postulated that newspaper stories with<br />

similar topics would provide more repetiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new words and therefore, naturally<br />

14


lead to better vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. 20 related texts from four running stories were<br />

selected from four newspapers for analyses. In accordance to the vocabulary<br />

knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> most high school graduates, which is a 2000 word level, words outside<br />

this 2000 word level were c<strong>on</strong>sidered as new words that learners have to learn in the<br />

process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading newspapers. New words in each text were calculated in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target word repetiti<strong>on</strong> for both running stories and unrelated stories. In the examined<br />

4 running stories, 17 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 62 new word families (27%) occurred more than <strong>on</strong>ce<br />

while in unrelated texts, <strong>on</strong>ly 14 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 73 new word families (19%) occurred twice or<br />

more. Compared with unrelated stories, texts in running stories were able to reduce<br />

the density <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word families and also provide more repetiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words outside<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2000 words level. If some degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge can be acquired through<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e encounter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words, then it was estimated that when learners have proceeded<br />

into the sec<strong>on</strong>d text in the running stories they would be able to reach about 94% to<br />

95% lexical coverage, a percentage high enough for reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

With the help <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer tools and analyses, large numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensible<br />

texts could be sequenced for readers’ needs. Using a self-developed computer<br />

program, TextLadder, Ghadirian (2003) was able to c<strong>on</strong>duct precise statistical<br />

analyses as the basis for sequencing and arranging large amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts.<br />

TextLadder was designed to attend to the problem <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bringing foreign language<br />

learners with limited vocabulary knowledge to the point where learners are able to<br />

comprehend authentic texts in a specific genre. Word lists were included in the<br />

program as a way to enhance vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ments. In order to<br />

achieve comprehensibility, Ghardirian first determined a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists which<br />

accounted for 95% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the running words in target texts, a percentage suggested by<br />

Laufer (1989) for adequate reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. C<strong>on</strong>sidering the vocabulary<br />

level and the needs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target learners which are learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic purposes, the<br />

15


words in this set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists were then grouped into two subsets, either a familiar<br />

word list or an unfamiliar word list. The familiar word list c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high<br />

frequency elementary words that learners have supposedly acquired before having<br />

been exposed to this reading program. On the other hand, the unfamiliar word list<br />

was comprised <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower frequency words that learners would be expected to acquire<br />

in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts.<br />

After the selecti<strong>on</strong> and grouping <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists by the researcher, Textladder then<br />

take over to operate the following processes. First, it compared the running words in<br />

each text to the word lists (which c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two subsets, the familiar and the<br />

unfamiliar word list) and trimmed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f the <strong>on</strong>es that do not meet 95% requirement.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, it automatically sequenced the remaining texts by matching the words in the<br />

texts to both subsets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists, the familiar word list and the unfamiliar word list.<br />

The <strong>on</strong>e text with the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words and the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

unfamiliar words was placed as the first <strong>on</strong> the reading list. Third, the unfamiliar<br />

target words in the first text would be added to the familiar word list for TextLadder to<br />

run the process <strong>on</strong>ce again to sort out the next text that c<strong>on</strong>tains a maximum number<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words and a least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new unfamiliar words. The sequence<br />

therefore created an order for reading texts that enabled incremental vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for learners. Moreover, TextLadder kept in track the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

occurrences for each word. A list <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words which appeared under five times will be<br />

recorded for instructors’ informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

By exposing a small amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words within meaningful texts to learners,<br />

words in the unfamiliar word list will eventually be acquired by learners after they<br />

have finished all the texts in TextLadder in order. In order to test whether<br />

TextLadder could perform the aimed functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bridging the gap between learners’<br />

limited vocabulary and a list <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary c<strong>on</strong>sisting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words from a target genre,<br />

16


simplified texts from Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America were chosen for a test-run in Ghardirians’<br />

study. After comparing the texts with a newly c<strong>on</strong>structed word list comprised <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

GSL, UWL, and frequent words in Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America, 253 texts made the 95% cut-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f.<br />

They were then sorted so that each text had a figure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new target<br />

vocabulary which appeared in it. To reiterate, the purposes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> TextLadder were to<br />

ensure first, each text was able to provide incremental vocabulary learning thus<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining a small number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new words; sec<strong>on</strong>d, words in the word list would be<br />

encountered by readers for more than five times in the selected texts for better word<br />

learning quality. Therefore, to better achieve these two purposes, some adjustment<br />

was made after the first test-run. For example, the word list coverage point was<br />

raised to 96%, and the sequence order was reversed after unfamiliar words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each<br />

text dropped to below 5%. With these adjustments, 1817 target words appeared in<br />

about 230 texts, with 1567 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target words (86%) occurring more than five times.<br />

Ghadirian (2003) menti<strong>on</strong>ed five limitati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> TextLadder. First, vocabulary<br />

learning or justifying vocabulary learning sequence was TextLadder’s main functi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

and grammar usage and difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammatical aspects in reading texts were not<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidered. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, TextLadder could <strong>on</strong>ly detect single word units, multi-word<br />

units such as “blow up”, “home run”, or “kick <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f” could not be located. Third,<br />

TextLadder’s functi<strong>on</strong> has not yet able to distinguish different meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

homographs. Fourth, proper nouns in texts were all c<strong>on</strong>sidered as familiar words in<br />

TextLadder. Last, low frequency words in the selected reading texts may <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

appear fewer than five times after learners have finished reading all 200 or so texts.<br />

Furthermore, the study was <strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>cerned with creating a reading program for<br />

dealing with the problems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the high number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfamiliar words in authentic texts;<br />

whether this sequencing way will result in measurable learning was not the focus and<br />

therefore unexplored.<br />

17


Besides the above potential limitati<strong>on</strong>s, we suspect the following additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>straints after closer scrutiny <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text sequence in TextLadder. First, from the<br />

quantitative results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ghardirian’s study, learners may have to learn 2 to 25 new<br />

words for each new text depending <strong>on</strong> the length <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the text. The percentages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new<br />

words are particularly high in the first 10 to 20 texts, ranging from 8% to 16%. In<br />

other words, if a text is about 300 words l<strong>on</strong>g, a learner has to learn about 25 new<br />

words after reading them in such a short text for the first time. Nati<strong>on</strong> (2001)<br />

pointed out that “extensive reading should c<strong>on</strong>tain no more than 5% unknown running<br />

words “ (p. 150). Compared to Nati<strong>on</strong>’s positi<strong>on</strong>, the new word percentages in the<br />

first few texts in TextLadder are still higher.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, though TextLadder tried to ensure that target words in word list could<br />

appear in reading for at least five times within about 300 reading texts, there is no<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong> when or how <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten learners will encounter those new words again. This<br />

therefore leads to another problem, the low frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> appearance for target words<br />

will degrade incidental vocabulary learning. It may be in the tenth or twentieth text<br />

that learners finally <strong>on</strong>ce again encounter the target words read in the first text.<br />

Whether words could be acquired with such interval is questi<strong>on</strong>able. Moreover, the<br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure necessary for word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> has remained a c<strong>on</strong>troversial issue<br />

with some researchers suggesting a much higher exposure amount than five times<br />

(Horst, Cobb, & Meara, 1998; Herman, Anders<strong>on</strong>, Pears<strong>on</strong>, & Nagy, 1987).<br />

Therefore, the five-time word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> threshold determined by Textladder awaits<br />

further research verificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Lastly, as was admitted by Ghadirian (2002), Textladder was not tested out <strong>on</strong><br />

real learners, hence, the actual effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequenced text <strong>on</strong> vocabulary learning is left<br />

to explore.<br />

In sum, Ghadirian’s (2002) sequencing program, TextLadder, had successfully<br />

18


created several ideal incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s within extensive<br />

reading envir<strong>on</strong>ments using computer tools. Nevertheless, enhancements could be<br />

made by providing more frequent exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words to further reduce learners’<br />

vocabulary load, and to <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer empirical evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> effective vocabulary learning<br />

through such text sequencing.<br />

2.5. Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Exposure to a Word and Successful Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Though it was persistently emphasized that enough exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a word is<br />

necessary for an incremental learning process in which various features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

could be picked up from the c<strong>on</strong>texts, previous studies have, in fact, not been able to<br />

reach an agreement <strong>on</strong> how much exposure is required for successful incidental<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Exposure from <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>ce to twenty times has been<br />

suggested by different researchers. By inserting pseudo words in texts, Saragi,<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, and Meister (1978) proposed a number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ten exposures for L1 learners to<br />

fully acquire new words. A probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning a word from c<strong>on</strong>text after <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e exposure is between .10 to .15 as estimated by Nagy, Herman, and Anders<strong>on</strong><br />

(1985). An even lower probability, .05, which is about an exposure amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

twenty times, was found in their follow-up study (Nagy, et al., 1987). Nati<strong>on</strong> (1990)<br />

reviewed previous studies related to exposures required for word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that a range from five to sixteen exposures may be needed. Rott (1999)<br />

recruited 95 learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> German as a sec<strong>on</strong>d language and assigned them into a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol and an experimental group. Each group was further divided into three<br />

subgroups based <strong>on</strong> numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to unfamiliar words which appeared two,<br />

four, or six times during reading. Learners read six passages c<strong>on</strong>taining six<br />

unfamiliar words each week either for two weeks, four weeks, and six weeks. A<br />

multiple-choice vocabulary test and a vocabulary definiti<strong>on</strong> test were delivered<br />

19


immediately after reading, <strong>on</strong>e week after reading, and <strong>on</strong>e m<strong>on</strong>th after reading,<br />

aiming to measure learners’ productive and receptive gain and word retenti<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

results indicated that <strong>on</strong>ly two encounters with unfamiliar words during reading can<br />

positively affect learners’ vocabulary growth. Yet, learners with six exposures did<br />

significantly better <strong>on</strong> both immediate, delayed receptive and productive vocabulary<br />

tests whereas two and four exposures resulted in a small difference. The findings<br />

first suggested that l<strong>on</strong>g term effect <strong>on</strong> adult L2 vocabulary growth during reading can<br />

be guaranteed under the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that texts are sufficiently rich for building<br />

vocabulary knowledge. Moreover, though two exposures during reading already<br />

result in word knowledge gain, lexical growth is more likely to be speeded up if the<br />

exposure is increased to six encounters.<br />

The major reas<strong>on</strong> leading to these disparate results with measuring encounters is<br />

the incremental nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word learning process. A single encounter with a word in<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text could already result in some amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning, while 50 exposures will still<br />

not enable learners to build the complete and complex word knowledge needed for<br />

productive use. As Huckin and Coady (1999) stressed “word-learning outcomes can<br />

range over c<strong>on</strong>tinua <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical knowledge and use competencies from partial<br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong> knowledge to precise knowledge and productive use capability” (p. 185).<br />

In other words, researchers’ c<strong>on</strong>cepts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> successful acquisiti<strong>on</strong> were different, and<br />

thus, vocabulary learning was measured in different ways.<br />

The other possibility involved in measuring word encounter frequency could be<br />

that different degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure is needed by different learners (Zahar, et al., 2001).<br />

Zahar, Cobb, and Spada (2001) investigated the issue by recruiting 144 ESL learners<br />

coming from a variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> first language backgrounds at five pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency levels ranging<br />

from beginners to bilinguals. Each student’s performance <strong>on</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>’s (1990)<br />

Vocabulary Levels Test involving matching words to brief definiti<strong>on</strong>s at five<br />

20


frequency levels were recorded as pretest results. The story The Golden Fleece was<br />

selected as the reading text since all the words in the story were classified into five<br />

levels according to Nait<strong>on</strong>’s word frequency categories and proved to be neither too<br />

difficult for beginners and nor effortless for advanced learners to read.<br />

Thirty words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various frequency levels and numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurrences in the story<br />

were chosen as target words and a word definiti<strong>on</strong> match pretest was c<strong>on</strong>ducted to<br />

ensure that learners’ pre-reading vocabulary knowledge parallels with their general<br />

vocabulary knowledge (assessed in the Levels Test). The same vocabulary test<br />

distributed in the pre-test was given in the post-test two days after learners’ reading.<br />

Each learner’s pre and post tests were then compared to identify their word gain. It<br />

was found that word occurrence frequency played a more crucial role for students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

lower vocabulary levels than those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> higher vocabulary levels. The quantitative<br />

data determined frequency as a factor three to four times more important for<br />

beginners than it was for more advanced learners. In other words, weaker learners<br />

were more dependent <strong>on</strong> word frequency than advanced learners. The study further<br />

pointed out that future studies should take learner level into c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> when<br />

examining the frequency factor in incidental vocabulary learning.<br />

To summarize, the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposures to word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> is left<br />

much to be explored, the puzzle could be further untangled if learners’ level,<br />

researchers’ c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> successful vocabulary learning, and purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary<br />

measurements are more clearly defined.<br />

21


2.6. Measuring <strong>Incidental</strong> Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the incremental nature in vocabulary learning, for words to be fully<br />

acquired by a learner after encountering it in merely several texts is clearly impossible.<br />

It would be more realistic to assume that learners acquire a degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> partial<br />

knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a target word after several exposure opportunities. Receptive and<br />

productive types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a word therefore have comm<strong>on</strong>ly been used to<br />

describe the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’ word knowledge (Nati<strong>on</strong>, 2001; Read, 2000).<br />

Receptive knowledge in vocabulary involves recognizing the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a word while<br />

reading or listening and retrieving its meaning from mental lexic<strong>on</strong>; <strong>on</strong> the other hand,<br />

productive vocabulary knowledge involves expressing a message through speaking<br />

and writing and producing the appropriate word form appropriately in c<strong>on</strong>text. It is<br />

generally agreed that learner’s vocabulary knowledge can be located in a receptive to<br />

productive c<strong>on</strong>tinuum. Generally speaking, learners will first recognize a word’s<br />

form, pr<strong>on</strong>unciati<strong>on</strong>, and basic meanings, then with further experiences or practices,<br />

their word knowledge will move al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum and finally reach the point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

able to use these words freely in productive mode. From this sense, receptive and<br />

productive types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge are not two separate word systems in learners’<br />

lexic<strong>on</strong>, but rather <strong>on</strong>e lexical store with receptive to productive c<strong>on</strong>tinuum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

different words.<br />

However, most available test measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten aim at measuring either receptive or<br />

productive vocabulary respectively. If receptive and productive vocabularies are not<br />

two distinct and dichotomous systems, then test measures that c<strong>on</strong>centrate merely <strong>on</strong><br />

measuring either learners’ receptive or productive word knowledge would inevitably<br />

overlook the diverse levels in word learning. In previous literature exploring the<br />

issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> required amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure for word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, test measures such as<br />

checklist, self developed multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s, definiti<strong>on</strong> matching, clozes,<br />

22


syn<strong>on</strong>ym selecting, translati<strong>on</strong> and the standard Vocabulary Levels Test (Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1990)<br />

have all been used for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> testing either receptive or productive knowledge<br />

(Saragi et al., 1978; Nagy et al., 1985; Rott, 1999; Zahar et. al., 2001). There seem<br />

to be no c<strong>on</strong>sistency in the way that the two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary knowledge have been<br />

measured. In other words, no c<strong>on</strong>sensus has been reached <strong>on</strong> what counts as a<br />

receptive measure and what a productive <strong>on</strong>e is (Read, 2000). Without a systematic<br />

measure tapping into both receptive and productive vocabulary and viewing<br />

vocabulary learning in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> degrees, experimental results thus varied greatly.<br />

Owing to the above reas<strong>on</strong>, an appropriate test measure that aims at assessing the<br />

quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d vocabulary knowledge is needed. Paribakht and Wesche’s (1993)<br />

Vocabulary Knowledge Scale (VKS) was adopted in the current study because “the<br />

purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> VKS is not to estimate general vocabulary knowledge, but rather to track<br />

the early development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> specific words in an instructi<strong>on</strong>al or experimental situati<strong>on</strong>”<br />

(Wesche & Paribakht, 1996. p. 33).<br />

The VKS c<strong>on</strong>sists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two scales: <strong>on</strong>e for eliciting self perceived work knowledge<br />

from the test taker and <strong>on</strong>e for scoring test takers’ resp<strong>on</strong>ses. It had been used as a<br />

tool to assess vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in several studies (Joe, 1995; Paribakht &<br />

Wesche, 1993, 1997) and proved to be a workable measure sensitive to increases in<br />

vocabulary knowledge that result from reading activities. Since the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

current study was to assess vocabulary gains from learners’ extensive reading practice<br />

and the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between learners’ vocabulary learning quality with amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

word exposure, VKS was reas<strong>on</strong>ed as <strong>on</strong>e vocabulary measure that could address our<br />

goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> investigating learners’ vocabulary knowledge depth.<br />

23


2.7. Summary<br />

We incorporated the empirical results and pedagogical implicati<strong>on</strong>s from the<br />

literature into the current study. They are succinctly underscored in the following<br />

points.<br />

1. In the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading, certain amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, at some point,<br />

should be drawn to words in order for vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and ultimately L2<br />

learning to occur.<br />

2. Gloss, especially computerized gloss is generally effective in bridging the gap<br />

between L2 text and the L2 learners’ limited cultural and lexical knowledge.<br />

Given technical difficulties and learners’ limited mental capacity, however, textual<br />

gloss is regarded as more feasible for computerized extensive reading<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

3. Using high frequency word lists to filter appropriate instructi<strong>on</strong>al material can<br />

substantively accelerate learners’ vocabulary growth.<br />

4. Selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> particular sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists should be c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al up<strong>on</strong> learners’<br />

specific needs.<br />

5. Though computerized text sequencing has its limitati<strong>on</strong>s and its effectiveness has<br />

not been tested, it possesses the potential <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reducing learners’ vocabulary load<br />

and increasing target word exposures. More refinements may induce better<br />

learning results.<br />

6. For word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposures to target words remains an unsettled<br />

issue awaiting for careful investigati<strong>on</strong> with clearly defined learners’ background<br />

and vocabulary measurements.<br />

In the current study, an extensive reading syllabus was developed by refining<br />

Ghadirian’s (2003) sequencing methods in order to provide more comprehensible<br />

reading c<strong>on</strong>text and more encounters with target words. Further, how much<br />

24


exposure is needed in order to lead to different degrees <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> was<br />

investigated. To facilitate vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, highlighting in reading texts to<br />

raise learner awareness, texts graded by specific word lists to accelerate vocabulary<br />

learning, and gloss as well as an <strong>on</strong>-line searching tool to ensure correct inference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

word meanings will be integrated. Since Zahar, Cobb, and Spada (2001)<br />

suggested learner level to be a factor influencing the frequency issue, we attempted to<br />

identify the frequency questi<strong>on</strong> within a single learning c<strong>on</strong>text and learner populati<strong>on</strong><br />

in order to limit the influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other factors. The research questi<strong>on</strong>s in this study<br />

are as follows:<br />

1. Is it feasible to incorporate <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools, gloss, word lists, and<br />

computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus for<br />

college learners’ vocabulary learning purpose? What problems or limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

will be encountered?<br />

2. What is learners’ feedback <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2a. What is learners’ attitude towards the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2b. What are learners’ difficulties and recommendati<strong>on</strong>s towards the <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

extensive reading syllabus?<br />

3. If the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus is effective in leading to vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, how much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words do learners have to encounter for<br />

successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>?<br />

3a. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word is required to lead to learners’ receptive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

3b. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word is required to lead to learners’ productive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

25


3.1. Overview<br />

Chapter Three<br />

METHODOLOGY<br />

In this study, an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus was designed and developed<br />

based <strong>on</strong> previous research implicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> effective <strong>on</strong>line vocabulary learning. We<br />

wish to probe into the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>structing an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive<br />

reading syllabus that provides comprehensible input with c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and<br />

repetitious exposure to those words and to evaluate subjects’ vocabulary learning<br />

outcome after using this tool. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, we wish to examine the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how much<br />

exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words is needed for Taiwanese college learners’ learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

receptive and productive word knowledge. A <strong>on</strong>e-group pretest and posttest design<br />

was adopted. This chapter presents the methodology for c<strong>on</strong>structing the extensive<br />

reading syllabus and the experiment in order to address incidental vocabulary learning<br />

effectiveness. Details <strong>on</strong> participants, instruments, procedures, and data analyses are<br />

discussed.<br />

3.2. Participants<br />

Participants involved 70 college freshmen students in a public university <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

northern Taiwan. These students were from two intact classes and were taking the<br />

course, Freshmen English, as a required course. Freshmen English is a <strong>on</strong>e-year<br />

course emphasizing English listening, speaking, reading, and writing. Its aim is to<br />

prepare students with reading and writing competence for academic purposes and also<br />

equip them with public speaking ability. All <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 70 participants have had at least<br />

six years <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> formal English learning experiences from junior high to senior high<br />

school and were estimated to be in the intermediate level regarding their overall<br />

English competence. Such previous learning experience in high school and the<br />

26


<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered Freshmen English course both centered around intensive reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts<br />

within explicit teaching envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

3.3. The Designing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an On-line Extensive Reading Syllabus<br />

We programmed our <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus based <strong>on</strong> Ghadirian’s<br />

(2003) work <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the sequencing procedures and added some improvements by utilizing<br />

other tools. The first step <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>structing an <strong>on</strong>-line syllabus was to find a large<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> suitable reading texts and to identify a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> useful word list tailored for<br />

our Taiwanese college learners. Moreover, the texts should c<strong>on</strong>tain a high<br />

proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the word list so as to assure frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

and to make vocabulary learning more efficient through each text that learners have<br />

read. In other words, words in the word lists should be used frequently in reading<br />

texts for learners to repetitively encounter them while reading.<br />

3.3.1 The Reading Material: the Sinorama Magazine<br />

Coady (1997) proposed that providing comprehensible input as a principal factor<br />

encouraging learners to read. Research <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the L2 reading process attributed<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> to three sources: (a) c<strong>on</strong>ceptual abilities, which is readers’ intellectual<br />

capacity; (b) background knowledge, what the reader knows about the reading topic,<br />

and (c) process strategies, readers’ strategies to comprehensi<strong>on</strong> such as knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

syntactic informati<strong>on</strong>, lexical meaning, and c<strong>on</strong>textual meaning (Nagy, 1997).<br />

Therefore, to achieve comprehensi<strong>on</strong> entails overcoming reading difficulties<br />

originated in these three aspects. With comprehensive vocabulary exposure<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trolled by word lists in our program, reading texts which are c<strong>on</strong>ceptually suitable<br />

and familiar to readers’ background knowledge would supposedly reduce readers’<br />

possible reading difficulties to the least. Accordingly, texts from the Sinorama<br />

magazine, published by Sinorama Magazine, were chosen in this study.<br />

27


The Sinorama magazine is dedicated to introducing Taiwan to the world.<br />

Articles in Sinorama are authentic materials in English, written for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

presenting a true picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Taiwan <strong>on</strong> the internati<strong>on</strong>al scene. Through special<br />

in-depth reportage <strong>on</strong> Taiwan <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all aspects, readers are hoped to gain a better<br />

knowledge and understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Taiwan. Its articles include major developments<br />

and issues in Taiwan over recent years in such areas as politics, ec<strong>on</strong>omics, arts,<br />

society, culture, ecology and the envir<strong>on</strong>ment. For readers in this study, Sinorama<br />

articles possess the advantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> first, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fering background-familiar reading texts and<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d, providing chances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning from authentic materials related to home<br />

culture, which is rare since most reading materials <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten are imported, aim at<br />

introducing foreign culture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English-speaking countries. In other words,<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> or aid for further culture knowledge is not an additi<strong>on</strong>al demand with texts<br />

from Sinorama and that reading difficulties generated from unfamiliarity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

background knowledge will be minimized. Moreover, for learners to read<br />

extensively, selecting materials that interest readers is also another major factor. A<br />

wide variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts providing various topics and themes would more likely enhance<br />

readers’ motivati<strong>on</strong> for English learning. With topics ranging from politics,<br />

ec<strong>on</strong>omics, arts, society, culture, ecology, sports, entertainment and envir<strong>on</strong>ment,<br />

texts in Sinorama possess this motivating feature. As a result, 5008 reading texts<br />

from issues <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Sinorama magazine in 1989 to 2001 were selected for analyses and<br />

sequencing.<br />

28


3.3.2 Four Word Lists<br />

As was pointed out by Laufer (1989), a 95% coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words is needed for<br />

learners to adequately understand a text. Therefore, if our goal is for learners to be<br />

able to read texts like those in the Sinorama magazine after finishing reading texts in<br />

the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus, then a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word list able to account for 95%<br />

coverage in Sinorama texts is required. Moreover, according to Ghadirian’s work<br />

(2003), word lists were further subdivided into the familiar word list and the<br />

unfamiliar word list for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing texts based <strong>on</strong> their difficulty level.<br />

Thus, judging familiar words and determining target words suitable for learners’ need<br />

was another criteri<strong>on</strong> to take into c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> in additi<strong>on</strong> to word lists with 95%<br />

coverage.<br />

Two word lists were first identified as learners’ familiar word lists in our study:<br />

the General Service Word List (West, 1953) and the High School Frequent Word List<br />

(鄭恆雄, 2002; 2003). The General Service Word List (GSL) includes about 2000<br />

English word families based <strong>on</strong> printed and written materials such as encyclopedias,<br />

magazines, textbooks, novels, and poetry. This list is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten used as a basis for<br />

English courses and simplified reading texts. Previous studies have c<strong>on</strong>firmed that<br />

knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in GSL generally gives access to about 80% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the words in any<br />

written texts (Worthingt<strong>on</strong> & Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1996). Though GSL was developed some time<br />

ago, it has remained the best <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the available lists because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> its informati<strong>on</strong> about the<br />

frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each word’s various meanings (Nati<strong>on</strong> & Waring, 1997). Based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

word families in GSL, Nati<strong>on</strong> (2001) specified the words bel<strong>on</strong>ging in each family<br />

and developed a word lists c<strong>on</strong>taining 7827 words.<br />

From the previous word list studies, we reas<strong>on</strong>ed that no <strong>on</strong>e word list could be<br />

used for all learners and c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists as teaching materials should be<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al up<strong>on</strong> specific learners’ needs. For this reas<strong>on</strong>, the High School Frequent<br />

29


Word List (鄭恆雄, 2002; 2003), which includes the most frequent and useful 4600<br />

words for high school students in Taiwan was employed in order to compensate GSL.<br />

The High School Frequent Word List (HSF) was c<strong>on</strong>structed by c<strong>on</strong>sulting previously<br />

developed popular word lists such as GSL and COBUILD (Sinclair, 1995), and<br />

modificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> frequent words in these lists were c<strong>on</strong>ducted for Taiwanese high<br />

school learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English. It was used as a guideline for writing high school<br />

English textbooks in Taiwan and therefore, college freshmen supposedly should have<br />

already acquired these words when they were in high school. Since the original HSF<br />

word lists does not provide the words bel<strong>on</strong>ging in each family, we completed the<br />

words for each family by specifying the comm<strong>on</strong> inflecti<strong>on</strong>al forms such as –ing, -ly,<br />

or –ed and affixed words such as –un, -ment, or -ti<strong>on</strong> using Visual FoxPro 7.0.<br />

Three teachers were involved in determining items in each word family to ensure the<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> for each entry was indeed frequently used. As a result, 7256 words were<br />

determined as the HSF. The GSL and HSF together are thus able to address learners’<br />

background English learning experiences and were determined as familiar word lists<br />

in our <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus.<br />

To identify target word lists, learners’ learning purpose was also our first priority.<br />

That is, depending <strong>on</strong> learners’ learning goals, learning words in the identified target<br />

word lists should take priority over learning other kinds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary. The first<br />

target word list that we selected was Xue and Nati<strong>on</strong>’s University Word List (UWL,<br />

1984). This word list c<strong>on</strong>sists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 800 word families, 3685 words, not in the GSL but<br />

frequently found in academic texts (Nati<strong>on</strong> & Ky<strong>on</strong>gho, 1995). This academic<br />

vocabulary gives an 8.5% coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic text and 3.9% coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

newspapers (Ky<strong>on</strong>gho & Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1989). Since our learners are college freshmen<br />

expecting to meet unsimplified English textbooks in future studies, adding UWL as a<br />

target word list could be an introducti<strong>on</strong> to these highly useful academic words.<br />

30


To this point, we have identified two familiar word lists, the GSL and HSF, and<br />

<strong>on</strong>e target word list, UWL, yet, after comparing all the running words in the 5008<br />

texts in the Sinorama magazine, we found these three word lists together unable to<br />

reach the 95% coverage suggested by Laufer (1989). In order to calculate<br />

percentage coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists in Sinorama texts, all proper nouns and punctuati<strong>on</strong><br />

marks were first deleted from texts. Percentages were then calculated by comparing<br />

words in each word list to all running words in Sinorama. The results showed that<br />

the GSL and the HSF together give a coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 92.1%, whereas GSL, HSF, and<br />

UWL together make a coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 93.48%. In other words, knowing all the words<br />

in these three word lists (GSL, HSF, and UWL) are still not sufficient enough to have<br />

adequate comprehensi<strong>on</strong> in all Sinorama texts. Therefore, a fourth word list was<br />

developed by directly using the corpora we have available, texts from the Sinorama<br />

magazine.<br />

We extracted the most frequently used words in Sinorama texts and excluded the<br />

<strong>on</strong>es that are already listed in GSL, HSF, and UWL. With GSL, HSF, and UWL<br />

making up around 93% text coverage, we wish to extract other high frequent words<br />

not <strong>on</strong> these three word lists but used frequently in Sinorama texts in order to make up<br />

the remaining 2% for reaching 95% coverage. In order to do this, a s<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>tware<br />

package, Visual FoxPro 7.0 was used. This s<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>tware package possesses<br />

programming functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> its built-in database. In other words, by using Visual<br />

FoxPro 7.0 we were able to store reading texts and word lists in the database and at<br />

the same time operate computer programming for manipulating texts in the same<br />

s<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>tware.<br />

To develop a new word list, first, all words used in Sinorama texts were listed in<br />

order al<strong>on</strong>g with a figure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurrences in Sinorama texts. Words<br />

that are already included in GSL, HSF, or UWL are deleted. Then, we begin to look<br />

31


for the cut <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f point in word frequency in order to form our next word list. By<br />

including the frequent words in Sinorama magazine to the other three word lists<br />

according to frequency counts and compare it to the running words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sinorama texts,<br />

it was found that 781words which appear more than 124 times in the Sinorama texts<br />

together with GSL, HSF, and UWL are able to achieve a 96.61% coverage in<br />

Sinorama texts. Since these 781 running words are bey<strong>on</strong>d the familiar word lists,<br />

GSL and HSF, we then identified this fourth word list, the Sinorama High Frequent<br />

Word List (SHF) as another target word list.<br />

In sum, there were a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four word lists needed to screen appropriate text<br />

difficulty level for our <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus, GSL, HSF, UWL, and SHF.<br />

GSL and HSF are determined as familiar words already acquired by learners before<br />

they are exposed to this extensive reading syllabus, and UWL and SHF are target<br />

word lists that learners will be expected to acquire while reading in the syllabus.<br />

There are a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 8234 words in these four lists and they are able to meet the<br />

96.61% coverage within Sinorama texts. The following table is a summary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

four word lists.<br />

Table 3.1 Summary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four word lists<br />

Names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the lists Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cumulated<br />

words<br />

GSL (familiar word list) 7827 84.7%<br />

HSF (familiar word list) 9712 92.01%<br />

UWL<br />

(target word list)<br />

SHF<br />

(target word list)<br />

12833 93.48%<br />

13614 96.61%<br />

Total 13614 96.61%<br />

32<br />

Cumulated % coverage


3.3.3 Sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts<br />

After suitable reading texts and word lists were chosen, we then began to arrange<br />

and sequence reading texts in an order that is able to provide learners with<br />

comprehensible input and increase chances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to target words. Visual<br />

FoxPro 7.0 was again used. Our programming steps are as follows.<br />

First, for the reas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> providing learners with chances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading l<strong>on</strong>ger texts<br />

and at the same time not to overwhelm learners by lengthy texts, from the 5008 texts<br />

in Sinorama, we selected <strong>on</strong>ly the texts that c<strong>on</strong>tained between 1500 and 3000<br />

running words for sequencing procedures. A total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 959 texts met this criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the 959 texts were reexamined with the four word lists that we selected, texts<br />

unable to reach 95% coverage were excluded. This sifting process left us with 856<br />

reading texts. It should be noted that at this point, each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the remaining 856 text<br />

would c<strong>on</strong>tain different proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words from the familiar word lists, words from<br />

the target word lists, and other unfamiliar words not included in the familiar word and<br />

target word lists. The 95% cut <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f point in the sec<strong>on</strong>d step ensured that the average<br />

coverage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words from the familiar word lists and the target word lists together was<br />

able to reach 95% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the running words in these 856 texts.<br />

Third, running words in the qualified 856 texts were compared to those in the<br />

familiar word lists, GSL and HSF, and the target word lists, UWL and SHF. Visual<br />

FoxPro 7.0 sifted through the texts and sequenced texts according to vocabulary<br />

difficulty. The <strong>on</strong>e text that c<strong>on</strong>tained the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words and<br />

smallest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words was placed first <strong>on</strong> the sequence. So far, the order<br />

arranging procedures are similar to those used in Ghadirian’s (2003). We added the<br />

following steps in order to increase the encounters <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words.<br />

First, to find the sec<strong>on</strong>d appropriate text, the program did not immediately add<br />

the target words in the first text into the familiar word lists like that in Ghadirian’s<br />

33


TextLadder; instead, our program put exposed target words in another newly created<br />

word list, the Exposed Word List. With this new word list, our program then sifted<br />

through texts again looking for the text that c<strong>on</strong>tained the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar<br />

words and the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, but this time a third standard was added<br />

by using the Exposed Word List. The program searched for a text which not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words and the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words,<br />

but a text that also c<strong>on</strong>tained the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words from the Exposed Word list<br />

(target words from previous texts <strong>on</strong> the sequence). That is, if the first text <strong>on</strong> the<br />

sequence order had four target words computer, internati<strong>on</strong>al, assist, and advantage,<br />

after learners read this text, these four target words would be taken out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target<br />

word list and included into the Exposed Word List. After the first text was put in<br />

order, the program then went through the remaining texts looking for the next easiest<br />

text by comparing familiar words and target words to texts, and also with <strong>on</strong>e more<br />

screening guideline, seeking for a particular text that c<strong>on</strong>tained the highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words from the Exposed Word List. The sec<strong>on</strong>d text selected then might have its<br />

new sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, for example, qualify, situati<strong>on</strong>, and paragraph, but also<br />

include target words from the first text, computer, internati<strong>on</strong>al, assist, and<br />

advantages. Compared to other texts, the sec<strong>on</strong>d text was the sec<strong>on</strong>d easiest text and<br />

the text that had the most number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> first text target words. To select the third text,<br />

the program automatically included the target words in the sec<strong>on</strong>d text into the<br />

Exposed Word List (now the Exposed Word List would c<strong>on</strong>tain the following words,<br />

computer, internati<strong>on</strong>al, assist, advantages, qualify, situati<strong>on</strong>, and paragraph, which<br />

are target words from the first text and the sec<strong>on</strong>d text) and again went through the<br />

same sifting process for the next text. In other words, the program ensured that the<br />

target words appearing in the previous texts again reappear in the following texts.<br />

After sequencing, the target words in each reading text were highlighted in red to<br />

34


aise readers’ awareness and attenti<strong>on</strong>. The exposed words from previous reading<br />

texts, <strong>on</strong> the other hand, were highlighted in green in texts to remind readers that they<br />

have seen these words before. Ideally, the words in target word lists will gradually<br />

be included into the Exposed Word List after learners have been exposed to more<br />

readings during the extensive reading practice. The Exposed Word List also keeps<br />

track <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how many times each word is exposed to learners during their extensive<br />

reading practice. It was hoped that with the above sorting process, a text which<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tains the most familiar words, the least target words, and the most exposed words<br />

would be placed in sequence.<br />

For the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> measuring learners’ vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, we selected the<br />

first sixteen texts in the sequence for learners’ extensive reading practice. However,<br />

after a test run with the Sinorama texts, it was discovered that the first sixteen texts<br />

placed <strong>on</strong> the sequence were not able to reach a satisfying repetitive target word<br />

exposure rate. The easiest text place <strong>on</strong> the syllabus reached a familiar word rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

99%, indicating that the rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words from the target word lists al<strong>on</strong>g with other<br />

unfamiliar words outside the four word lists was extremely low. As a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this<br />

test run, the rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target words in the sixteen texts placed <strong>on</strong> syllabus was<br />

relatively low, the low rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words implied that there were less target words<br />

for learners to repetitively encounter in letter texts. Am<strong>on</strong>g the first five texts, <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e word repetitively occurred in the third and fifth text.<br />

In an attempt to overcome the low repetitive rate, we arranged another screening<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>. Since a 2%-5% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfamiliar word rate within a text was suggested as an<br />

optimal incidental vocabulary learning c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for sec<strong>on</strong>d language learners (Nati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2001), the original procedure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> locating the easiest text have found texts which<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained too little target words for learners to learn. We refined our screening<br />

procedure with the 2%-5% unfamiliar word rate suggesti<strong>on</strong> in order to provide more<br />

35


target words within each text. After sifting texts which met the 95% demand,<br />

instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> immediately selecting the easiest text (the <strong>on</strong>e with the most familiar word<br />

and least target word), the program activated its new sifting guideline at this point,<br />

locating texts which c<strong>on</strong>tains 2%-5% unfamiliar words. Notice that by 2%-5%<br />

unfamiliar words, we mean that for each text, words from the two target word lists<br />

and other unfamiliar words outside the four word lists together, would range from<br />

2%-5%. To do this, each text was again compared to the four word lists. After this<br />

new procedure, the program then c<strong>on</strong>tinued to find the easiest text within the<br />

remaining texts and finished up the whole process by searching the <strong>on</strong>e with the most<br />

exposed words. Compared to the original procedure, this new criteri<strong>on</strong> ensured that<br />

the first few texts placed <strong>on</strong> the syllabus will not <strong>on</strong>ly be comprehensible but also<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained certain number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words for learners to acquire. Lastly, the<br />

highlighting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words in red and exposed words in green were incorporated<br />

after the refined sequencing procedures.<br />

During the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing, some texts may possess the same percentage<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words, target words, and repetitive words. When this occurs, <strong>on</strong>e text<br />

should be selected in order for the program to run the next text <strong>on</strong> the sequence.<br />

Human interventi<strong>on</strong> takes over the resp<strong>on</strong>sibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> deciding which text to choose at<br />

this point. While the program listed out titles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the texts with the same learning<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, the researcher selected the <strong>on</strong>e text most likely to be interested to the<br />

target reader for the program to c<strong>on</strong>tinue sifting the next text. By examining the<br />

words in the exposed word list, the new versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing process can indeed<br />

raise repetitive word rate. As a result, a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> thirty sequenced texts were placed<br />

<strong>on</strong> our syllabus. Again, for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> measuring learners’ word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly the first sixteen texts were used as reading materials for learners to read.<br />

With regards to the goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ensuring comprehensible input and target word<br />

36


epetiti<strong>on</strong>, the sixteen texts placed <strong>on</strong> the syllabus was c<strong>on</strong>trolled with an unfamiliar<br />

word rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2%-5% and dem<strong>on</strong>strated a satisfying average target word rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1.9%<br />

(see Table 3.2). The average target word repetiti<strong>on</strong> rate also reached a plausible<br />

porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 46% (see Table 3.2). In other words, for each text about half <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target<br />

words were exposed to learners in previous readings. Moreover, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 223 target<br />

words in the sixteen texts, 29 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them appeared five times or more, and 16 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them<br />

appeared more than 9 times (see Table 3.3). Compared to Ghardirins’ Textladder<br />

(2003) in which amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> five times was ensured within almost 300<br />

pieces <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts and word distributi<strong>on</strong> interval not c<strong>on</strong>trolled, the distributi<strong>on</strong> result in<br />

our syllabus differ c<strong>on</strong>siderably. As is shown from the statistic numbers, our goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trolling unfamiliar word rate and ensuring repetitive target word exposure was<br />

achieved.<br />

37


Table 3.2 Target word rate and target word repetiti<strong>on</strong> rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the sixteen texts<br />

Number Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts total running target words target exposed exposed<br />

words in<br />

words target target<br />

each text<br />

words words<br />

1 1785 43 2.4% 23 53%<br />

2 1410 48 3.4% 30 63%<br />

3 1864 51 2.7% 17 33%<br />

4 1360 24 1.7% 4 16%<br />

5 2270 48 2.1% 21 44%<br />

6 1818 34 1.8% 16 47%<br />

7 2145 39 1.8% 19 49%<br />

8 2160 39 1.8% 16 41%<br />

9 2539 49 1.9% 28 57%<br />

10 2474 37 1.5% 13 35%<br />

11 2011 29 1.5% 14 48%<br />

12 2341 50 2.1% 34 68%<br />

13 1301 24 1.8% 14 58%<br />

14 1262 16 1.3% 5 31%<br />

15 1762 31 1.7% 8 26%<br />

16 2370 44 1.8% 31 70%<br />

Average 1.9% 46%<br />

Note: Rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words= Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words / Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> total running words in each text<br />

Rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposed target words= Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposed target words / Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words<br />

Table 3.3 Distributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words in the sixteen reading texts<br />

Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word exposure Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target<br />

Groups<br />

words<br />

1 Words appearing 1-2 times 151<br />

2 Words appearing 3-4 times 43<br />

3 Words appearing 5-6 times 13<br />

4 Words appearing 7-8 times 4<br />

5 Words appearing more than 9 times 12<br />

38


A summary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> procedures in sequencing texts is illustrated in Table 3.4. This<br />

table includes the first 10 steps that we programmed in order to select the first sixteen<br />

texts <strong>on</strong> the reading sequence.<br />

Table 3.4 Procedures in sequencing reading texts<br />

Procedures Purpose What the<br />

program does<br />

1. For appropriate length <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

reading texts<br />

2. For trimming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f the texts<br />

that do not meet 95 %<br />

coverage compared to the<br />

four word lists.<br />

3. Locating the texts which<br />

have 2%-5% unfamiliar<br />

word rate.<br />

4. To select the easiest text<br />

(the first reading text <strong>on</strong><br />

the sequence) for learners.<br />

39<br />

Select texts with<br />

1500 to 3000<br />

words<br />

Compare the<br />

running words in<br />

every text to the<br />

four word lists<br />

(GSL, HSF, UWL,<br />

and SHF)<br />

Compare the<br />

running words in<br />

every text to the<br />

four word lists<br />

(GSL, HSF, UWL,<br />

and SHF).<br />

Compare words in<br />

every text with the<br />

familiar word lists<br />

(GSL & HSF) and<br />

target word lists<br />

(UWL & SHF)<br />

5. Select the easiest<br />

text that has the<br />

highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

familiar words and<br />

least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words.<br />

6. To select the sec<strong>on</strong>d text Target words in<br />

<strong>on</strong> the sequence<br />

Text #1 are added<br />

Outcome<br />

959 texts met the<br />

qualificati<strong>on</strong><br />

856 texts met the<br />

qualificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

30 texts met the<br />

qualificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Text #1 (the easiest<br />

text or the first text<br />

<strong>on</strong> the reading<br />

sequence) is<br />

selected.<br />

The Exposed Word<br />

List has target


7.<br />

into the Exposed<br />

Word List<br />

Select the <strong>on</strong>e text<br />

that meets the<br />

following three<br />

criteria:<br />

1. The highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

familiar words<br />

2. The least<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words.<br />

3. The highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words in the<br />

Exposed Word<br />

List.<br />

8. To select the third text <strong>on</strong> Target words in<br />

the sequence<br />

Text #2 are added<br />

into the Exposed<br />

Word List<br />

9. Select the <strong>on</strong>e text<br />

that meets the<br />

following three<br />

criteria:<br />

1. The highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

familiar words<br />

2. The least<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words.<br />

3. The highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words in the<br />

Exposed Word<br />

List.<br />

10. To select the fourth text <strong>on</strong> Target words in<br />

the sequence<br />

Text #3 are added<br />

40<br />

words from Text<br />

#1.<br />

Text #2 (sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

easiest text or the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d text <strong>on</strong> the<br />

sequence) is<br />

selected.<br />

The Exposed Word<br />

List has target<br />

words from Text<br />

#1 and Text #2.<br />

Text #3 (third<br />

easiest text or the<br />

third text <strong>on</strong> the<br />

sequence) is<br />

selected.<br />

The Exposed Word<br />

List has target


41<br />

into the Exposed<br />

Word List<br />

words from Text<br />

#1, Text #2, and<br />

Text #3.<br />

In order to gain a clearer picture <strong>on</strong> the differences between Ghadirian’s (2002)<br />

study and the current <strong>on</strong>e, a comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the target learners, texts, and the study<br />

goals are displayed in Table 3.5. With regards to the sequencing improvements<br />

incorporated by the current study to Ghadirian’s (2002) study, Table 3.6 dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

the adjusted procedures in bold print. Broadly speaking, inspired by Ghadirian’s<br />

(2002) TextLadder, the current study attempted to refine TextLadder’s sequencing<br />

procedures and to test the effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequenced texts <strong>on</strong> vocabulary learning by<br />

working with specified learners and analyzing a smaller number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts.


Table 3. 5. A comparis<strong>on</strong> between Ghardirian’s (2002) study and the current study<br />

Differences Ghardirian’s study The current study<br />

Target learners <strong>EFL</strong> learners (level not Taiwanese college<br />

specified)<br />

freshemen (intermediate<br />

<strong>EFL</strong> learners)<br />

Target texts Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America<br />

simplified news texts<br />

Sinorama texts<br />

Familiar word lists The first 176 words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> GSL (familiar words),<br />

GSL, and other words<br />

found in a broad range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

elementary texts<br />

HSF (familiar words)<br />

Unfamiliar word lists GSL (words other than UWL (unfamiliar words),<br />

the first 176 words),<br />

Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America Special<br />

word list, UWL,<br />

L<strong>on</strong>gman Defining<br />

Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary word list<br />

SHF (unfamiliar words)<br />

Length <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target texts 300-1500 words 1500-3000 words<br />

Goals 1. Ensuring each text<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tain reas<strong>on</strong>able<br />

small number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

unfamiliar words.<br />

2. Ensuring that most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the words in the word<br />

lists will be<br />

encountered five<br />

times or more.<br />

42<br />

1. Ensuring each text<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained reas<strong>on</strong>able<br />

small number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

unfamiliar words.<br />

2. Ensuring that target<br />

words will be<br />

encountered<br />

repetitively.<br />

3. C<strong>on</strong>trolling the interval<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word<br />

encounter.<br />

4. Testing whether<br />

sequenced texts can<br />

result in measurable<br />

vocabulary learning.<br />

5. Finding out the amount<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure needed for<br />

successful word<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.


Table3.6. A comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing procedures in TextLadder and the current study<br />

Ghadirian’s TextLadder The Current Study<br />

1. Exclude the texts that do not meet 95<br />

% coverage compared to the word<br />

lists.<br />

2. Select the easiest text (Text #1): The<br />

<strong>on</strong>e text which has the highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the familiar<br />

word lists (The first 176 words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

GSL) and the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

in the unfamiliar word lists (GSL<br />

words other than the first 176 words,<br />

Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America Special word list,<br />

UWL, L<strong>on</strong>gman Defining Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary<br />

word list).<br />

3. Unfamiliar words in Text #1 added to<br />

the familiar word list.<br />

4. Select the sec<strong>on</strong>d easiest text (Text<br />

#2): The <strong>on</strong>e text which has the<br />

highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the<br />

familiar word list (now c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

unfamiliar words from Text #1) and<br />

the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the<br />

unfamiliar word lists.<br />

5. Unfamiliar words in Text #2 added to<br />

the familiar word list.<br />

6. Select the third easiest text (Text #3):<br />

The <strong>on</strong>e text which has the highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the familiar<br />

word list (now c<strong>on</strong>taining unfamiliar<br />

words from Text #1and Text #2) and<br />

the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the<br />

unfamiliar word lists.<br />

1. Exclude the texts that do not meet 95<br />

% coverage compared to the word<br />

lists.<br />

2. Locating the texts which have<br />

2%-5% unfamiliar word rate.<br />

3. Select the easiest text (Text #1): The<br />

<strong>on</strong>e text which has the highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the familiar<br />

word lists (GSL & HSF) and the<br />

least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the target<br />

word lists (UWL & SHF).<br />

4. Target words in Text#1 added to<br />

the Exposed Word List.<br />

5. Select the sec<strong>on</strong>d easiest text (Text<br />

#2): The <strong>on</strong>e text which has the<br />

highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words<br />

(GSL & HSF), the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words (UWL & SHF), and the<br />

highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the<br />

Exposed Word List (now<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining target words in Text#1).<br />

6. Target words in Text#2 added to<br />

the Exposed Word List.<br />

7. Select the third easiest text (Text #3):<br />

The <strong>on</strong>e text which has the highest<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar words (GSL &<br />

HSF), the least number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target<br />

words (UWL & SHF), and the<br />

highest number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words in the<br />

Exposed Word List (now<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining target words in Text#1<br />

and Text#2).<br />

43


3.3.4 Glossing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts<br />

Gloss is beneficial to incidental vocabulary learning and therefore incorporated<br />

in our <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus. Luckily, we were able to obtain Chinese<br />

translati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts from Sinorama, thus, development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> gloss involved first matching<br />

and aligning sentences from two databases: the Chinese translati<strong>on</strong>, and the original<br />

English texts. After translati<strong>on</strong> was aligned with English, manual double checking<br />

was employed to make sure the correctness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each translati<strong>on</strong>. After this,<br />

JavaScript was used for displaying our gloss. Within the reading text, we provided<br />

gloss for target words and proper nouns. The meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target words and<br />

proper nouns could be retrieved by a click <strong>on</strong> the word. The gloss c<strong>on</strong>taining the<br />

appropriate Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>text would pop up in a separate box right by the<br />

clicked word. An illustrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the gloss is displayed in Appendix I<br />

.<br />

3.3.5 Reading text interfaces<br />

After sixteen texts were chosen according to our sequencing procedure, we then<br />

c<strong>on</strong>structed a website for extensive reading <strong>on</strong>-line. For the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> directing<br />

readers’ main attenti<strong>on</strong> to reading for comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, three comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were devised at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> every text (see Appendix K). Learners were able to do<br />

these questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> line and were provided with immediate feedback <strong>on</strong> the correctness<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the answers. Also, for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> providing searching opportunity, the access<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an <strong>on</strong>-line search tool, TotalRecall, which is a bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer (Wu, et al.,<br />

2003), was provided for learners who may wish to further investigate into target word<br />

use (see Appendix I and Appendix J). The <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading website first<br />

required learners to log <strong>on</strong> with their passwords, gave an orientati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> available tools<br />

and the meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> highlighting, and provided the reading sequence list (see<br />

Appendix G and Appendix H). Each reading text had the main text with target<br />

44


words highlighted in red and exposed words highlighted in green, the glossing for<br />

target words and proper nouns, access to <strong>on</strong>-line bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordsancer, TotalRecall,<br />

and three comprehensi<strong>on</strong> exercises <strong>on</strong>-line.<br />

3.4. Instruments<br />

Five types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> instruments were used in this study. First, two sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tests (a<br />

pretest and a posttest) were designed for assessing learners’ vocabulary learning after<br />

being exposed to the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus. Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, three<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naires (a background questi<strong>on</strong>naire, an <strong>on</strong>line evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire and<br />

an in class evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire) were distributed for tapping into learners’<br />

English learning background and their reflecti<strong>on</strong>s after the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

experiences.<br />

3.4.1 Pretest<br />

In the pretest, a list <strong>on</strong> target words chosen within the sixteen reading texts<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with words from the GSL and HSF lists (familiar word lists) were randomly<br />

selected and distributed for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> measuring participants’ vocabulary<br />

knowledge and ensuring target words to be unfamiliar to participants (See Appendix<br />

B). There were a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 60 items in the pretest including 50 target words (item 1 to<br />

item 50) and 10 familiar words (item 51 to item 60). The 50 target words were<br />

chosen according to the Exposed Word List in which numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each<br />

target word appeared in the sixteen reading texts were recorded. The record showed<br />

that am<strong>on</strong>g the target words, 29 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them appeared more than six times. These 29<br />

words which appeared more than six times were all included in the test. For words<br />

appearing under six times, we randomly selected ten words from words appearing 3 to<br />

4 times and eleven words from words appearing <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>ce, making up a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 50<br />

word items. The same 50 target words were tested in the posttest. In the pretest,<br />

45


participants were required put a check <strong>on</strong> the words they have seen before and try to<br />

provide an English definiti<strong>on</strong> or Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> besides these words (see<br />

Appendix B). They were to leave blank the words they have not seen before.<br />

Correct target words and familiar word were graded and transformed to respectively<br />

with 100% for each secti<strong>on</strong>. Each correct item in the target word secti<strong>on</strong> is scored 2<br />

points whereas a correct item in familiar word secti<strong>on</strong> is scored 10 points.<br />

3.4.2 Posttest<br />

In order for more precise vocabulary measurement, we selected a measure that is<br />

able to capture stages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary learning, the Vocabulary Knowledge Scale<br />

(Paribakht & Wesche, 1996). The Vocabulary Knowledge Scale is developed “to<br />

capture initial stages or levels in word learning that are subject to accurate self-report<br />

or efficient dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>, and that are precise enough to reflect gains during a<br />

relatively brief instructi<strong>on</strong>al period” (Paribakht & Wesche, 1996, p. 33). The<br />

Vocabulary Knowledge Scale (VKS) uses a combinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> self-reported and<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated knowledge <strong>on</strong> a specific word for its scaling. An elicitati<strong>on</strong> scale<br />

self-report category is as shown in Table 3.7.<br />

Table 3.7 Self-report categories in the VKS elicitati<strong>on</strong> scale (Read, 2000, p 133)<br />

____________________________________________________________________<br />

Self-report Categories<br />

------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------<br />

I. I d<strong>on</strong>’t remember having seen this word before.<br />

II. I have seen this word before, but I d<strong>on</strong>’t know what it means.<br />

III. I have seen this word before, and I think it means ____________.<br />

(syn<strong>on</strong>ym or translati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

IV. I know this word. It means ___________. (syn<strong>on</strong>ym or translati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

V. I can use this word in a sentence: _____________. (If you do this secti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

please also do Secti<strong>on</strong> IV.)<br />

____________________________________________________________________<br />

46


For each target word, learners were provided with the above self-report categories in<br />

order to assess their quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge. Possible student resp<strong>on</strong>ses and<br />

scores are showed in Table 3.8.<br />

Table 3.8 VKS scoring categories: Meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> scores (Read, 2000, p 134)<br />

_____________________________________________________________________<br />

Self-report Possible Meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> scores<br />

Categories Scores<br />

I. 1 The word is not familiar at all<br />

II. 2 The word is familiar but its<br />

meaning is not known.<br />

III. 3 A correct syn<strong>on</strong>ym or translati<strong>on</strong><br />

is given.<br />

IV. 4 The word is used with semantic<br />

appropriateness in a sentence.<br />

V. 5 The word is used with semantic<br />

appropriateness and grammatical<br />

accuracy in a sentence.<br />

From the above categories, we could see that the Vocabulary Knowledge Scale<br />

aims to determine vocabulary knowledge ranging from complete unfamiliarity,<br />

through recogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the words and some idea <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> its meaning, to the ability to use<br />

the word accurately in a sentence. Elicitati<strong>on</strong> categories I and II were determined as<br />

1 and 2 points respectively. Based <strong>on</strong> the correctness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the translati<strong>on</strong> provided,<br />

category III may lead to scores 2 (if translati<strong>on</strong> is wr<strong>on</strong>g) and 3 (if translati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

correct). Likewise, wr<strong>on</strong>g answers in Category IV and Category V could result in a<br />

47


score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2. If a target word is used semantically correct but grammatically wr<strong>on</strong>g in<br />

a sentence, then a score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 4 was given. If a sentence is c<strong>on</strong>structed with a target<br />

word used correctly in both semantic and grammatical aspects, then this word was<br />

credited with a score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 5. For sentence judging, two raters were employed and<br />

scores that they graded were averaged to achieve inter-rater reliability. The<br />

Self-report Categories were translated into learners’ native language, Chinese, for<br />

learners to fully understand how to do this test (see Appendix C).<br />

The VKS posttest c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 50 target words tested in the pretest. Based<br />

<strong>on</strong> the above VKS scoring system, each learner had a score for each target word <strong>on</strong><br />

their posttest. A real VKS scoring example is shown in Appendix D. The scores in<br />

the posttest were analyzed for two purposes. First, to measure learners’ vocabulary<br />

gain after they were exposed to the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, to<br />

determine the relati<strong>on</strong>ship <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and the quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary<br />

learning (receptive or productive word knowledge).<br />

3.4.3 Questi<strong>on</strong>naires<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>naires <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three kinds were c<strong>on</strong>structed for investigating into<br />

participants’ background and their attitudes towards extensive reading. The<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naires were written in participants’ native language, Chinese, in order to<br />

prevent participants’ limited English ability from affecting their answers.<br />

The first questi<strong>on</strong>naire, the Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire, intends to obtain<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> learners’ use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer and internet (the 1 st to 8 th item), their<br />

perspectives <strong>on</strong> English learning (the 9 th to 12 th item), and their experiences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

extensive reading (13 th to 17 th item) (See Appendix A). The other two questi<strong>on</strong>naires,<br />

the Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire and In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire, were<br />

designed to probe into participants’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

48


syllabus. The evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire includes the following four categories:<br />

system interface design, features fostering vocabulary learning, features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading<br />

texts, and future learning motivati<strong>on</strong>. It is supposed that learners’ fresh impressi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> the extensive reading syllabus would provide more beneficial feedback for some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the designed questi<strong>on</strong>s. For example, the <strong>on</strong>es related to system interface design.<br />

Therefore, questi<strong>on</strong>s closely related to specific features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus were programmed <strong>on</strong>line for learners to do immediately after they<br />

have finished all sixteen readings. Questi<strong>on</strong>s which require lesser reading<br />

experience impressi<strong>on</strong>s were collected together and distributed in class <strong>on</strong>e week after<br />

all learners have completed the sixteen readings (See Appendix E and Appendix F).<br />

The five instruments employed in the current study are summarized in Table 3.9.<br />

49


Table 3.9 Instruments used in the study<br />

Instruments Functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

1. Pretest To investigate students’ previous word<br />

knowledge and to ensure unfamiliarity<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words.<br />

2. Posttest<br />

1. To investigate the effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

(the Vocabulary Knowledge Scale) vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> through the<br />

<strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus<br />

2. To investigate the relati<strong>on</strong>ship <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and the<br />

degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’ vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

3. Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire To obtain informati<strong>on</strong> about learners’<br />

previous experiences <strong>on</strong> computer use<br />

and extensive reading experiences.<br />

4. Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire To probe into learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

toward the <strong>on</strong>-lien extensive reading<br />

syllabus <strong>on</strong> issues closely related to<br />

specific features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus.<br />

5.In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire To probe into learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

toward the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

syllabus.<br />

3.5. Procedures<br />

An <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus was first designed and programmed with<br />

systematic c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfamiliar words in the selected texts to be read before exposing<br />

participants to it. An evaluati<strong>on</strong> experiment was designed to assess the program’s<br />

effect. The experimental procedures proceeded from December, 2003 to March,<br />

2004. An in-class pretest, a c<strong>on</strong>sent form (see Appendix M), and a Background<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire were distributed at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the first semester before participants were<br />

exposed to the extensive reading syllabus. Then, participants were provided with the<br />

chance to become familiar with the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus from January, 1,<br />

50


2004 to March, 16, 2004. About <strong>on</strong>e week after the administrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the pretest,<br />

participants’ <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading course began.<br />

The main extensive reading activity took place during winter vacati<strong>on</strong> and the<br />

first m<strong>on</strong>th <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the sec<strong>on</strong>d semester. Participants were required to complete a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

sixteen reading texts during the eight-week period, that is, two reading texts were<br />

scheduled per week. An account was created for each participant and they were to<br />

log <strong>on</strong> the extensive reading website, read the text in sequence with <strong>on</strong>-line resources<br />

and gloss help, and answer four to five comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>-line. A time<br />

limit <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> five minutes for reading was set before learners is able to click to the part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s. A tracker was set up in order to record participants’<br />

<strong>on</strong>-line behaviors, and to guarantee that learners were reading texts as scheduled<br />

instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> saving all reading assignments at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the winter vacati<strong>on</strong>. It was<br />

hoped that learners would proceed with the reading process as it is in natural<br />

extensive reading manner. Weekly m<strong>on</strong>itors were scheduled during the winter<br />

vacati<strong>on</strong> each at third and sixth week for researchers to check students’ records from<br />

the trackers (see Appendix L). E-mail was sent for learners who have not proceeded<br />

with the reading as scheduled. Moreover, since the Freshmen English course is a<br />

two-semester course, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the two classes, learners from <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the class were informed<br />

in advance that their reading progress will be graded and accounted for as part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their<br />

grades in the sec<strong>on</strong>d semester. They were acknowledged that performances <strong>on</strong><br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each reading would <strong>on</strong>ly be a practice and<br />

would not be graded; however, their reading performances according to scheduled<br />

time would be crucial in grades. The learners in the other class, <strong>on</strong> the other hand,<br />

were told that the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus was a voluntarily assignment for<br />

them to improve their English ability. All participants were not informed about the<br />

vocabulary posttest. The posttest was distributed in class <strong>on</strong>e week after learners<br />

51


had finished all sixteen texts. The procedures are summarized in Table 3.8.<br />

Table 3.10 Research Procedures<br />

Period Research Procedures<br />

Preparati<strong>on</strong> 1. Programming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus.<br />

2. Participants sign the c<strong>on</strong>sent form.<br />

3. Participants take the pretest.<br />

4. Participants fill in the Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire.<br />

5. Participants learn how to use the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

syllabus.<br />

On-line Extensive 3. Participants begin their extensive reading.<br />

Reading Practices 4. Researchers m<strong>on</strong>itor participants’ progresses.<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> 1. Participants finish the Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire.<br />

2. Participants take the posttest.<br />

3. Participants fill in the in- class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire.<br />

52


3.6. Data Analysis<br />

There are two main research directi<strong>on</strong>s in this study. First, we investigated the<br />

feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating several vocabulary learning implicati<strong>on</strong>s drawn from<br />

previous studies to c<strong>on</strong>struct an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus and to evaluate<br />

subjects’ vocabulary learning outcome after learning from the syllabus. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, we<br />

examined the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how many exposures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words are needed for Taiwanese<br />

college learners’ learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> receptive and productive word knowledge. Our detailed<br />

research questi<strong>on</strong>s are as follows.<br />

1. Is it feasible to incorporate <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools, gloss, word lists, and<br />

computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

syllabus for vocabulary learning purpose? What problems or limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

will be encountered?<br />

2. What is learners’ feedback <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2a. What is learners’ attitude towards the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading<br />

syllabus?<br />

2b. What are learners’ difficulties and recommendati<strong>on</strong>s towards the<br />

<strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

3. If the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus is effective in leading to<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, how many exposures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words do learners have to<br />

encounter for successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>?<br />

3a. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word is required to lead to learners’ receptive<br />

word knowledge?<br />

3b. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word is required to lead to learners’ productive<br />

word knowledge?<br />

Quantitative analyses were c<strong>on</strong>ducted for the above research questi<strong>on</strong>s. First, by<br />

recording and observing computer programming process, we wish to report the<br />

53


technical feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating <strong>on</strong>-line resources, gloss, word lists, and<br />

computerized sequencing techniques into a pedagogically appropriate <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

extensive reading syllabus. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s and difficulties <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> developing an <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

extensive reading syllabus for vocabulary learning purpose are discussed to answer<br />

our first research questi<strong>on</strong>. To answer research questi<strong>on</strong> 2, data collected from the<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire were analyzed. To answer research questi<strong>on</strong> 3, learners’<br />

performances in both pretest and posttest were analyzed. The pretest aimed to<br />

double check subjects’ unfamiliar knowledge <strong>on</strong> target words, and its outcome was<br />

compared with posttest performances. Learners’ performances <strong>on</strong> posttest and<br />

recordings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the times that target words appeared throughout the extensive reading<br />

practice were employed to answer research questi<strong>on</strong> 3. In order to do this, the<br />

posttest scores and the frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words were compared with<br />

ANOVA.<br />

54


4.1. Overview<br />

Chapter Four<br />

Research Results and Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

By developing a graded <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus and incorporating it to<br />

an <strong>EFL</strong> college freshmen course, this study attempts to explore the following research<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

1. Is it feasible to incorporate <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools, gloss, word lists, and<br />

computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus for<br />

vocabulary learning purpose? What problems or limitati<strong>on</strong>s will be<br />

encountered?<br />

2. What is learners’ feedback <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2a. What is learners’ attitude towards the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2b. What are learners’ difficulties and recommendati<strong>on</strong>s towards the <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

extensive reading syllabus?<br />

3. If the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus is effective in leading to vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, how much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words do learners have to encounter for<br />

successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>?<br />

3a. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word are required to lead to learners’ receptive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

3b. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word are required to lead to learners’ productive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

Subjects were recruited from college freshmen students who were taking a general<br />

English course in two classes. To ensure equal reading practice effects, participants<br />

who failed to complete all sixteen texts were excluded from further analysis.<br />

Besides, students who were absent for either pretest or posttest were eliminated. As<br />

55


a result, valid subjects dropped from 70 to 38 college freshmen.<br />

In this chapter, the results gathered from the background questi<strong>on</strong>naire, the<br />

pretest, the posttest, the <strong>on</strong>line questi<strong>on</strong>naire, and the in-class questi<strong>on</strong>naire were<br />

analyzed and presented first. Then, the feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> introducing an <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus to <strong>EFL</strong> learning envir<strong>on</strong>ment for vocabulary purposes, learners’<br />

attitudes towards the <strong>on</strong>line syllabus, and times <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary exposure needed for<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading envir<strong>on</strong>ment are discussed.<br />

4.2. Results<br />

The research data in this study come from four sources: (1) participants’<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the background questi<strong>on</strong>naire, (2) participants’ pretest scores, (3)<br />

participants’ posttest scores, and (4) participants’ resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the <strong>on</strong>line evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire, (5) participants’ resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the in-class evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire.<br />

The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> data analyses are presented as follows.<br />

4.2.1 The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the background questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Before learners were exposed to the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus, copies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the background questi<strong>on</strong>naires were delivered for obtaining informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> learners’<br />

use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer and internet (the 1 st to 8 th item), their perspectives <strong>on</strong> English<br />

learning (the 9 th to 12 th item), and their experiences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading (13 th to 17 th<br />

item).<br />

A very high percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners (94.7%) used computers frequently and all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

them had a PC at school. Also, during winter vacati<strong>on</strong>, 97.3% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the students had<br />

Internet access at home. Despite learners’ frequent use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computers, merely 7.9%<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them used Internet resources to learn English <strong>on</strong> a regular basis and <strong>on</strong>ly 26.3% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

them had past experience <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> using computers to assist language learning in English<br />

classes (see Table 4.1).<br />

56


Table 4.1 Learners’ use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer and internet<br />

Item 1~8 The use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer and internet Yes No<br />

1. I use computers frequently. 94.7% 5.3%<br />

2. I have a PC at school. 100% 0%<br />

3. I have a computer to use during winter vacati<strong>on</strong>. 97.3% 2.6%<br />

4. I have internet access during winter vacati<strong>on</strong> . 94.6% 5.4%<br />

5. What is the internet c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> during winter vacati<strong>on</strong>? Regular<br />

bandwidth<br />

37.1%<br />

57<br />

Wide<br />

bandwidth<br />

62.9%<br />

7. I use Internet resources for learning English regularly. 7.9% 92.1%<br />

8. In the past English classes, my teacher used computer<br />

assisted language learning methods in class.<br />

26.3% 73.7%<br />

On questi<strong>on</strong>s regarding learners’ English learning, about 31.6% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded that they like English learning, however, <strong>on</strong>ly 5.3% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them was c<strong>on</strong>fident<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their English ability. As for feelings and learning towards respective skills,<br />

vocabulary and grammar were reported as the most difficult skills while reading was<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidered the easiest. Moreover, the <strong>on</strong>e skill that learners wish to sharpen the<br />

most is speaking, followed by vocabulary (see Table 4.2).


Table 4.2 Learners’ perspectives <strong>on</strong> English learning<br />

Item 8~12 Learners’ Perspectives <strong>on</strong> English Learning<br />

8. The way I feel<br />

about learning<br />

English.<br />

9. The way I feel<br />

about my<br />

English ability.<br />

Like<br />

it very<br />

much<br />

5.3%<br />

Very<br />

good<br />

0%<br />

11. The way I feel about the<br />

following English<br />

skills: (1. Very difficult<br />

2.Difficult 3.OK<br />

4.Easy 5.Very easy)<br />

12. The <strong>on</strong>e skill that I want<br />

to improve the most.<br />

Like it<br />

26.3%<br />

Good<br />

5.3%<br />

58<br />

No<br />

preference<br />

60.5%<br />

OK<br />

52.6%<br />

Dislike it<br />

5.3%<br />

Bad<br />

28.9%<br />

Mean<br />

Listening 2.5 Speaking 2.5 Reading 3.1<br />

Writing 2.6 Vocabulary 2.4 Grammar 2.4<br />

Listening<br />

39.5%<br />

Speaking<br />

23.7%<br />

Reading<br />

5.3%<br />

Writing<br />

5.3%<br />

Dislike it<br />

very much<br />

2.6%<br />

Very bad<br />

13.2%<br />

Vocabulary<br />

26.3%<br />

For questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> learners’ past extensive reading experiences, up to 78.9% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

students had been encouraged to form the habit <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading regularly by their English<br />

teachers before, yet, <strong>on</strong>ly 13.2% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them had extensive reading habits. The reas<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for not having extensive reading habit were mainly because they did not have time for<br />

reading (28%), they could not find suitable reading materials (24%), and that most<br />

reading materials were too difficult to comprehend (20%) (see Table 4.3).


Table 4.3 Learners’ extensive reading experience<br />

Item 13~15 Learners’ Extensive Reading<br />

Experiences<br />

13. In the past English classes, my<br />

teachers have encouraged me to<br />

form extensive reading habits.<br />

Yes 78.9% (30/38)<br />

No 21.1% (8/38)<br />

14. I have extensive reading habit. Yes 13.2% (5/38)<br />

No 86.8% (33/38)<br />

15. The reas<strong>on</strong>s why I do not have<br />

extensive reading habits.<br />

4.2.2 The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the pretest<br />

59<br />

No time to read 28% (14/50)<br />

No time to find suitable material<br />

24% (12/50)<br />

Most materials are too difficult to<br />

comprehend 20% (10/50)<br />

Cannot find interesting materials<br />

16% (8/50)<br />

Have no interest in learning English<br />

12% (6/50)<br />

The pretest was designed to tap into learners’ initial word knowledge with an aim<br />

to locate learners’ unknown words for use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> measuring the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word exposure<br />

leading to vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. In the pretest, learners were to put a check by the<br />

word that they have seen before and try to provide a Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> for it. Two<br />

sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words were included in the pretest, familiar words which included ten words<br />

randomly selected from the familiar word lists, GSL and HSF, and unfamiliar words<br />

which were fifty randomly chosen target words grouped according to their amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure in the sixteen readings. The 60 words were graded and transformed<br />

respectively to 100% with each correct familiar word scoring 10 points and each<br />

correct unfamiliar word scoring 2 points. An independent t-test was run to<br />

determine whether learners’ performances <strong>on</strong> these two sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words differ. With<br />

mean scores <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 84.73 and 39 respectively, significant differences between familiar and<br />

unfamiliar words were revealed (t(2/38)=12.737, p


estimati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’ vocabulary level when we used the four word lists as criteria <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> (see Table 4.4).<br />

Table 4.4 The independent t-test for familiar and unfamiliar words in the pretest<br />

n Mean SD t value df p<br />

Familiar Words 38 84.74 15.89 12.737 74 .000*<br />

Unfamiliar Words 38 39.00 15.41<br />

Note: p


Table 4.5 Target words completely unfamiliar to learners<br />

Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Groups Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Words in the<br />

Exposure Group<br />

Group 1 (1-2 times) 2 times m<strong>on</strong>opoly Noun<br />

1 times recruitment Noun<br />

61<br />

Word Classes<br />

Group 2 (3-4 times) 4 times startling Adjective<br />

4 times deputy Adjective<br />

Group 3 (5-6 times) 6 times calligraphy Noun<br />

5 times epidemic Adjective<br />

Group 4 (7-8 times) 8 times faculty Noun<br />

Group 5 (more than 9 times) 15 times entrepreneur Noun<br />

4.2.3 The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the posttest<br />

15 times hypnosis Noun<br />

10 times administrative Adjective<br />

A posttest was c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong>e week after learners have finished sixteen reading<br />

texts for two purposes: to measure overall word gain and to determine the necessary<br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure for incidental word learning. Items designed as the Vocabulary<br />

Knowledge Scale (Paribakht & Wesche, 1996) including the 50 target words tested in<br />

pretest was adopted as posttest. Scorings in VKS were based <strong>on</strong> learners’<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> self-report knowledge <strong>on</strong> each word. Knowledge from never<br />

having seen the word before to being able to make a correct sentence with the target<br />

word were scored <strong>on</strong> a scale <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1-5. For the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> examining word gains,<br />

words with scores from 1 to 2 were classified as words that are “not known” to<br />

learners and words with scores from 3 to 5 were the <strong>on</strong>es c<strong>on</strong>sidered as “known”<br />

(Paribakht & Wesche, 1997). Referring to the VKS scoring scale, with a correct<br />

Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> provided for a certain word, a score ranging from 3 to 5 could be<br />

obtained, and therefore, the word could be c<strong>on</strong>sidered “known” by the learner.<br />

Otherwise, the word remains “not known”. Our pretest and posttest, though differ in<br />

format, were comparable due to the comm<strong>on</strong> distincti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> determining unknown


words from known words. The provisi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a correct Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> for a target<br />

word was the indicati<strong>on</strong> in both tests. Percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> known words in pretest and<br />

posttest were analyzed in paired t-test. As is apparent from Table 4.6, highly<br />

significant overall vocabulary gains (t(2/38)=8.849, p


Post hoc analysis using Least Significant Difference (LSD), procedures was used<br />

to locate the differences am<strong>on</strong>g groups (see Table 4.7). The results indicated<br />

learners’ performance in group <strong>on</strong>e significantly inferior to group two, group four, and<br />

group five. Again, learners’ word gain in group two was extremely high compared<br />

to those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other groups. Leaving group two aside, a significant word gain was<br />

observed in group four and group five. While group three did not result in<br />

significantly different word gain compared to group <strong>on</strong>e, group four and five reached<br />

a level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> significance, indicating word exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more than 7 times able to bring<br />

learners from score 1 to score 2 <strong>on</strong> VKS scale.<br />

Table 4.7 The ANOVA statistics for posttest scores am<strong>on</strong>g the five groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

different exposure amount<br />

Mean SD Group1 Group2 Group3 Group4 Group5<br />

Group 1 (1-2 times) 1.68 .74 * * *<br />

Group 2 (3-4 times) 2.37 .94 *<br />

Group 3 (5-6 times) 2.05 1.35<br />

Group 4 (7-8 times) 2.08 .67 *<br />

Group 5 (9~ times) 2.39 1.03 *<br />

Note: * Sig. p


vocabulary level, <strong>on</strong> the other hand, learners with scores below average were in the<br />

low vocabulary level group. With an average <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 49.5% known words in the pretest,<br />

21 learners were allocated to the high vocabulary level group. Learners in the high<br />

vocabulary level group had an average <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 50% known target word and 90% known<br />

familiar word initial knowledge when taking the pretest. 17 learners were allocated<br />

to the low vocabulary level group. These learners had an average 25% known target<br />

word and 78% known familiar words as their initial word knowledge. An<br />

independent t-test was executed to compare the mean VKS scores <strong>on</strong> the ten target<br />

words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners from the two different vocabulary levels. The results indicated<br />

that the two groups significantly differed from <strong>on</strong>e another with the high vocabulary<br />

level group achieving a mean <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2.17 <strong>on</strong> the 5-point VKS scale and the low<br />

vocabulary group reached a mean <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1.79 (see Table 4.8), c<strong>on</strong>firming our speculati<strong>on</strong><br />

that with the same amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to target words, learners with higher<br />

vocabulary entry level were capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquiring completely unknown words better<br />

than learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower vocabulary level. Learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> low vocabulary level, <strong>on</strong> the<br />

other hand, showed greater dependency <strong>on</strong> frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure while learning new<br />

words.<br />

Table 4.8 The independent t-test for the posttest performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the high vocabulary<br />

group and the low vocabulary group<br />

n Mean SD t value df p<br />

high 21 2.17 .97 4.210 378 .000*<br />

low 17 1.79 .76<br />

Note: p


the COBUILD Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary (Sinclair, 1995), which organizes words into five frequency<br />

bands with band five being the most frequent easy words and band <strong>on</strong>e the least<br />

frequent difficult words. A table <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words in the study and the COBUILD<br />

frequency bands that each word bel<strong>on</strong>gs to is showed in Table 4.9<br />

Table 4.9. Target words and COBUILD frequency bands<br />

Number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Groups Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Words in the COBUILD<br />

Exposure Group<br />

frequency bands<br />

Group 1 (1-2 times) 2 times m<strong>on</strong>opoly Band 1<br />

1 times recruitment Band 3<br />

Group 2 (3-4 times) 4 times startling Band 2<br />

4 times deputy Band 4<br />

Group 3 (5-6 times) 6 times calligraphy Not found<br />

5 times epidemic Band 2<br />

Group 4 (7-8 times) 8 times faculty Band 2<br />

Group 5 (more than 9 times) 15 times entrepreneur Band 1<br />

15 times hypnosis Band 1<br />

10 times administrative Band 2<br />

As can be seen from Table 4.9, except for words in group two, “startling” and<br />

“deputy” which bel<strong>on</strong>g to band 2 and band 4 respectively, most words were found in<br />

band 1 or band 2, the least frequent and most difficult two bands. This word<br />

difficulty difference was also dem<strong>on</strong>strated in posttest results. While the mean<br />

scores in other groups showed a gradual rise in word gains with the increase <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure, group two displayed an unusually high mean score, almost as<br />

high as words in group five, the <strong>on</strong>es which appeared for nine or more times,<br />

indicating that words in group two could be easier to acquire than others.<br />

65


4.2.4 The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

After the 38 learners have completed all sixteen reading texts <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>line<br />

extensive reading syllabus, two evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naires: an <strong>on</strong>line <strong>on</strong>e and an<br />

in-class <strong>on</strong>e, were distributed to probe learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the syllabus. Due to<br />

the reas<strong>on</strong> that learners’ fresh impressi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

would be more beneficial for some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the designed questi<strong>on</strong>s, questi<strong>on</strong> items closely<br />

related to specific features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus were programmed<br />

<strong>on</strong>line for learners to do immediately after they have finished all sixteen readings.<br />

On the other hand, questi<strong>on</strong>s which require lesser <strong>on</strong>line reading experience<br />

impressi<strong>on</strong>s were collected together and distributed in class <strong>on</strong>e week after all learners<br />

have completed the sixteen readings. The two evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naires include the<br />

following four categories: system interface design, features fostering vocabulary<br />

learning, features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line reading texts, and motivati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> future use. The<br />

results from both questi<strong>on</strong>naires are discussed together in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the above<br />

categories and detailed results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each questi<strong>on</strong>naire are given in Appendix O and<br />

Appendix P.<br />

For questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> learners’ motivati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> future use, about 49.9% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners<br />

said that they liked learning English after the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading experience and<br />

about 5.2 % <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners said they disliked learning English after the <strong>on</strong>line reading<br />

experience. Compared to the resp<strong>on</strong>ses in background questi<strong>on</strong>naire in which 31.6%<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners said that they liked English learning and 7.9% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners said they<br />

disliked English learning, the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners who have negative attitudes<br />

towards English learning decreased slightly perhaps because the extensive reading<br />

experience encouraged English learning motivati<strong>on</strong> (see Table 4.10). In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

81.6% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners expressed their wish <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to use similar reading<br />

syllabus in the future. Being able to improve English reading skills (42%) and able<br />

66


to facilitate vocabulary learning (33%) were the two main reas<strong>on</strong>s reported by the<br />

learners. Only 18.4% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners indicated that they will not use similar tools for<br />

learning English in the future, the main reas<strong>on</strong> being that they are not used to reading<br />

<strong>on</strong>line (51%) (see Table 4.10). Of the 7 learners (18.4%) who indicated that they<br />

will not use similar <strong>on</strong>line reading syllabus in the future, four <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them explained that<br />

they regarded the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus as a helpful tool; however, they<br />

already have extensive reading habits and satisfactory reading materials in print.<br />

Table 4.10 Learners’ motivati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> future use<br />

The way I feel about English learning 1. Dislike it very much 0% (0/38)<br />

after being exposed to the <strong>on</strong>line 2. Dislike it 5.3% (2/38)<br />

extensive reading syllabus.<br />

3. No preference 44.7% (17/38)<br />

4. Like it 42.1% (16/38)<br />

5. Like it very much 7.9% (3/38)<br />

Will I use similar <strong>on</strong>line extensive Yes, I will 81.6% (31/38)<br />

reading syllabus in the future?<br />

No, I will not 18.4% (7/38)<br />

The reas<strong>on</strong>s why I will use similar Able to improve reading skills<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus in the<br />

42% (24/64)<br />

future:<br />

Able to enhance vocabulary<br />

33% (21/64)<br />

The articles are interesting<br />

11% (7/64)<br />

There are various tools fostering<br />

vocabulary learning 16% (10/64)<br />

Other 3% (2/64)<br />

The reas<strong>on</strong>s why I will not use similar Not used to reading l<strong>on</strong>g articles<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus in the<br />

8% (1/12)<br />

future:<br />

Not used to <strong>on</strong>line reading<br />

51% (6/12)<br />

Dislike the fixed set schedules organized<br />

by the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

8% (1/12)<br />

Other 33% (4/12)<br />

67


Table 4.11 shows learners’ resp<strong>on</strong>ses regarding features fostering vocabulary<br />

learning in the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus. Learners perceived positively <strong>on</strong><br />

these features with a mean score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 3.3 <strong>on</strong> a five-point Likert scale. Repetitive<br />

exposure (mean=3.9) and highlighting with different colors <strong>on</strong> target and exposed<br />

words (mean=3.8) were c<strong>on</strong>sidered most helpful; however, gloss was thought to be<br />

least beneficial with the lowest mean <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2.9. Learners further indicated that more<br />

words should be glossed to help reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Table 4.11 Learners’ attitudes towards features fostering vocabulary learning<br />

(1.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree 2.Disagree 3.No preference 4.Agree<br />

5.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree)<br />

Mean Rank<br />

The repetitive target words is able to help me learn vocabulary. 3.8 1<br />

The highlighting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target and exposed words is able to help me learn<br />

vocabulary.<br />

The bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, TotalRecall, is able to help me learn<br />

vocabulary.<br />

68<br />

3.7 2<br />

2.9 3<br />

The gloss is able to help me learn vocabulary. 2.8 4<br />

Mean 3.3<br />

To investigate learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong> towards reading texts, Table 4.12 shows that<br />

learners agreed the text genre as being dynamic (mean=3.8) and topics being<br />

interesting (mean=3.4). Moreover, learners indicated that compared to selecting<br />

reading texts <strong>on</strong> their own, they prefer the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus to prepare<br />

texts for them (mean=3.7). Nevertheless, for the weaknesses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts, the<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g texts caused the greatest difficulties for most learners (mean=3.9).


Table 4.12 Learners’ attitudes towards features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts<br />

Strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the reading texts<br />

(1.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree 2.Disagree 3.No preference 4.Agree<br />

5.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree)<br />

69<br />

Mean Rank<br />

The genres <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the articles are dynamic. 3.8 1<br />

Compared with searching suitable reading materials <strong>on</strong> my own, I<br />

prefer the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus to prepare reading<br />

materials for me.<br />

3.7 2<br />

The topics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the articles are interesting. 3.4 3<br />

Weakness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the reading texts<br />

(1.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree 2.Disagree 3.No preference 4.Agree<br />

5.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree)<br />

Mean Rank<br />

The articles are too l<strong>on</strong>g. 3.9 1<br />

The vocabulary level in the articles is too hard. 3.3 2<br />

The vocabulary level in the articles is too easy. 2.4 3<br />

Learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong> towards the interface design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the system is shown in Table<br />

4.13. Overall, learners were satisfied with the overall layout <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus.<br />

Table 4.13 Learners’ attitudes towards interface design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the system<br />

(1.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree 2.Disagree 3.No preference 4.Agree<br />

5.Str<strong>on</strong>gly Disagree)<br />

Mean<br />

The word f<strong>on</strong>t in the syllabus is suitable for reading. 3.7<br />

The spaces between words in the syllabus are suitable for reading. 3.7<br />

The word size in the syllabus is suitable for reading. 3.7<br />

The layout in the syllabus is suitable for reading. 3.7<br />

Mean 3.7


4.3 Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

4.3.1. The feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating <strong>on</strong>line tools to an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus for vocabulary learning purposes<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s drawn from previous studies, we incorporated textual<br />

gloss, highlighting, TotalRecall (a bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer), high frequency word lists<br />

and computerized sequencing techniques to the design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus for vocabulary learning purposes. To reiterate, the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> textual gloss was<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>ed as being able to bridge the gap between L2 text and L2 learners’ limited<br />

lexical knowledge (Chun & Plass, 1997; Davis, 1989; Leffa, 1992; Liou, 2000;<br />

Hulstijn, 1993; Hulstijn, et al., 1996; Jacobs, 1994) while highlighting was used for<br />

the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> raising learners’ attenti<strong>on</strong> to target words. The bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer,<br />

TotalRecall, was viewed as a search access deepening learners’ processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

(Laufer & Hulstijn, 2001). Finally, the text sequencing procedures with high<br />

frequency word lists were designed to locate reading texts comprehensible for<br />

learners’ existing word level as well as to ensure substantive amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word<br />

exposure.<br />

In regard to the aim <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ensuring comprehensible input, the sixteen texts placed <strong>on</strong><br />

the syllabus dem<strong>on</strong>strated an average target word rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1.9%, a percentage within<br />

the optimal range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2%~5% suggested by Nati<strong>on</strong> (2001). Also, for the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

providing repetitive target word exposure, average target word repetiti<strong>on</strong> rate within<br />

the sixteen texts reached a porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 46%. Of the 223 target words in the sixteen<br />

texts, 151 words appeared more than <strong>on</strong>e time and 72 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them appeared three times or<br />

more. The distributi<strong>on</strong> result in our syllabus differ greatly compared to Ghardirins’<br />

Textladder (2003) in which an amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> five times was ensured within<br />

almost 300 pieces <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading text, suggesting that our goals <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> providing<br />

comprehensible input and multiple exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words were realized.<br />

70


Measurable vocabulary learning was dem<strong>on</strong>strated in that the percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

learners’ “known” words increased and “not known” words decreased after 12 weeks<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus (see Figure 4.1).<br />

Learners’ improved word knowledge verified the claim that such an <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus with various learning tools was adequate for effective incidental<br />

word learning. Likewise, feedback in evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naires also indicated<br />

learners’ overall positive percepti<strong>on</strong> towards the above <strong>on</strong>line tools, indicating the<br />

feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> utilizing the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus for enhancing learners’<br />

word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> while reading. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, several technical difficulties were<br />

observed throughout the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> developing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the syllabus.<br />

Figure 4.1. Percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> known and not known words in the pretest and the posttest<br />

Percentage<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

Percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> "known" and "not known" words<br />

in the pretest and the posttest<br />

61<br />

39<br />

71<br />

50.5<br />

49.5<br />

pretest posttest<br />

Unknown words<br />

Known words<br />

The first technical difficulty encountered derived from the selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> from the Sinorama magazine were originally maintained as being<br />

background-familiar to target learners due to its home culture oriented topic themes<br />

and therefore suitable for minimizing learner’s possible reading difficulties.<br />

However, these particular features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sinorama texts have resulted in unexpected<br />

difficulties in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> programming. In order to introduce Taiwanese culture,


direct ph<strong>on</strong>etic translati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Taiwanese names <strong>on</strong> objects, idioms, or customs were<br />

used in Sinorama texts, for example, shabing youtiao (燒餅油條) or jit<strong>on</strong>g (乩童).<br />

Such words were comm<strong>on</strong> but not easy to detect and isolate in Sinorama articles.<br />

They have amounted to a percentage in the texts which attenuated the effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word<br />

lists frequency and sequencing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text if not excluded <strong>on</strong>e by <strong>on</strong>e. C<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

obstacles therefore emerged during the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> locating high frequent words,<br />

calculating the percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfamiliar words, highlighting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and<br />

exposed words, and glossing for unknown words. During the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

programming, time c<strong>on</strong>suming manual modificati<strong>on</strong> and screening were required in<br />

order to tackle such problems. Though texts in Sinorama do <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer the advantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

being background-familiar, a corpus not as str<strong>on</strong>gly culture-oriented as Sinorama<br />

texts is suggested to be utilized in the future for an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

in order to simplify sequencing procedures.<br />

A sec<strong>on</strong>d technical difficulty originated during the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> glossing. In order<br />

to locate appropriate meaning for target words and proper nouns, development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

gloss involved first matching and aligning sentences from Chinese and English texts.<br />

After Chinese translati<strong>on</strong> was aligned with English, manual double checking was then<br />

employed to make sure the correctness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each translati<strong>on</strong>. However, the subtle<br />

meaning differences in English-Chinese sentences were unable to guarantee the<br />

correctness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> single word translati<strong>on</strong>. As a result, unexpectedly more manual<br />

modificati<strong>on</strong> was invested. It is supposed that a more plausible method able to<br />

generate gloss would remarkably reduce the difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating gloss to the<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus.<br />

To sum up, technically, the feasibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporating <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools,<br />

gloss, word lists, and computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive<br />

reading syllabus for vocabulary learning purpose could be more successfully realized<br />

72


if appropriate n<strong>on</strong> culture-oriented reading texts are selected and glossing problems<br />

are overcame.<br />

4.3.2. Learners’ feedback towards the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

After the twelve-week extensive reading experience with the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus, learners’ reacti<strong>on</strong> towards the syllabus were largely positive and<br />

they have approved <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the various vocabulary enhancement features as being effective<br />

for vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Improved reading and vocabulary abilities were<br />

perceived by learners and indicated as the major advantages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the syllabus. Though<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s such as the bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer Totalrecall, highlighting, and repetiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words were endorsed effective in general, a particular tool, gloss, however,<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>sidered unsatisfactory with the suggesti<strong>on</strong> that more words be glossed. In<br />

accordance with the opini<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> gloss, about <strong>on</strong>e-thirds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the learners felt that the<br />

texts were too difficult to comprehend despite the fact that they had enjoyed the<br />

reading process. Moreover, texts were regarded as being too l<strong>on</strong>g. Learners’<br />

frustrati<strong>on</strong>s were evident in their performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> multiple choice comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s as they merely achieved a comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> about 75% while reading.<br />

These feedback c<strong>on</strong>tradicted greatly with our previous attempt to ensure the<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and the estimati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learner vocabulary level c<strong>on</strong>firmed in<br />

pretest. From observing learners’ reading process, we suspect that the above learner<br />

difficulties might originate from the same problem, Taiwanese college learners’<br />

inexperience in extensive reading. Learners reported that they were not used to l<strong>on</strong>g<br />

texts and they either grew out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> patience or their eyes easily felt tired from reading<br />

<strong>on</strong>-line. Comprehensibility dropped as they struggled towards the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the texts.<br />

Thus, though the new word rate had been c<strong>on</strong>trolled by sequencing procedures and<br />

pretest had c<strong>on</strong>firmed our estimati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their vocabulary level, learner’ heavy reliance<br />

73


<strong>on</strong> gloss suggested that they still did not grow out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> habits in senior high<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>—intensive reading. From results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the background questi<strong>on</strong>naire, it is<br />

found that despite learners’ rich experience in receiving extensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

very few <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them have extensive reading habits. Without much practice, their<br />

intensive reading habits were remarkably obvious while reading texts from the <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

reading syllabus. Low tolerance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ambiguity was observed as learners tended to<br />

look up every word that they were not completely sure. From the tracking records, it<br />

is shown that learners seldom looked up target words in the bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer,<br />

Totalrecall, for more informati<strong>on</strong> besides Chinese translati<strong>on</strong>. Instead, unglossed<br />

words, including words from the familiar word lists, were the <strong>on</strong>es searched. It is<br />

speculated that instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> using extensive reading strategies such as c<strong>on</strong>textual<br />

guessing, learners immediately sought for help whenever they stumbled across<br />

sentences they did not understand. Furthermore, learners had the tendency to make<br />

clear every single sentence before proceeding to read, a habit not encouraged in<br />

extensive reading. This intensive reading habit in turn made reading l<strong>on</strong>g text more<br />

tiresome.<br />

Day and Bamford (1998) pointed out that learners’ previous experiences <strong>on</strong><br />

reading might predispose learners against extensive reading. Since intensive reading,<br />

in which all vocabulary and grammatical patterns were carefully studied within texts,<br />

was the comm<strong>on</strong> in-class practice in Taiwan, learners’ reading habits were difficult to<br />

adjust even with extensive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> implemented. It is suggested that in<br />

additi<strong>on</strong> to making extensive reading strategies explicit in class, more practice<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s should be scheduled as guidance to the extensive reading ability. Moreover,<br />

<strong>on</strong>-going time to time reminders <strong>on</strong> goals and strategies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading could be<br />

emphasized throughout the activity.<br />

One other explanati<strong>on</strong> for the c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the presumably low new word<br />

74


ate and learners’ percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> insufficient glossed words is that learners’<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> difficulty might derive from sources other than vocabulary such as<br />

complicated sentence patterns or ability to grasp main ideas (Berman, 1984; Nuttall,<br />

2000). In other words, vocabulary c<strong>on</strong>trol al<strong>on</strong>e could <strong>on</strong>ly to certain extent<br />

sequence text difficulty and thereby enhance comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, difficulties in syntax and<br />

reading strategies might emerge as vocabulary obstructi<strong>on</strong> was minimized to the least.<br />

All in all, though learners have enjoyed the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus,<br />

their extensive reading inexperience impeded fluency in reading. Due to the fact that<br />

the current study <strong>on</strong>ly lasted for twelve weeks, the questi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whether learners’<br />

reading difficulty would gradually diminish with increase in extensive reading<br />

experience awaits for further explorati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

4.3.3. Amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Exposure necessary for incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Discrepancy in amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure required for successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

suggested by various researchers has been documented in previous studies (Nagy,<br />

Herman & Anders<strong>on</strong>, 1985;1987; Nati<strong>on</strong>, 1990; Rott, 1999; Sarage, Nati<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Meister, 1978). The nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word knowledge as a c<strong>on</strong>tinuum leads to the<br />

phenomen<strong>on</strong> that merely <strong>on</strong>e exposure to target word could be able to push learners’<br />

knowledge further al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum, yet, even with exposure up to fifty times,<br />

learners’ word knowledge might still not be so satisfactory as to actively use words in<br />

various c<strong>on</strong>texts. Researchers’ discrete opini<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> what may be c<strong>on</strong>sidered as<br />

successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and the fact that previous studies might be measuring<br />

different points al<strong>on</strong>g this developmental c<strong>on</strong>struct could be the factors attributing to<br />

diverse results. Hence, by viewing word learning as a c<strong>on</strong>tinuum with incremental<br />

learning leading to learners’ receptive and productive knowledge for specific words,<br />

the current study adopted VKS (Paribakht & Wesche, 1993) as a vocabulary<br />

75


measurement tool for its functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> being able to track early word developments.<br />

Ten target words identified in pretest as completely unknown to all 38 learners were<br />

assigned into five groups according to their amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure within the sixteen<br />

texts used in the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus.<br />

In the current study, learners’ word gains were observed in all five groups,<br />

including words in group <strong>on</strong>e, the <strong>on</strong>es which appeared <strong>on</strong>ce or twice. This is in<br />

accordance to the findings from Nagy’s (1985; 1987) and Rott’s (1999) studies that<br />

though the improvements were minor, merely <strong>on</strong>e or two exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word<br />

could still c<strong>on</strong>tribute to learners’ initial word knowledge. This c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e or<br />

two encounters, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> course, was not adequate for successful word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more<br />

in-depth knowledge. In fact, the results from our study dem<strong>on</strong>strated that with<br />

scoring below 3 points <strong>on</strong> the VKS scale, learners were unable to successfully recall<br />

the Chinese meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words even with words in group five, which c<strong>on</strong>tains<br />

words with exposure amount from 9 up to 15 times. Moreover, very few learners<br />

have attempted trying to make sentences using target words in the posttest. It seems<br />

that exposure amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> up to 15 times was unable to guarantee receptive exposure,<br />

let al<strong>on</strong>g more complex and pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ound productive knowledge.<br />

The limited word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> found in our study adds yet another piece <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

evidence supported by already plentiful research that incidental vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> while reading could take place; however, its gains could be superficial and<br />

fragile (Wesche & Paribakht, 2000; Hulstijn, 1993; Huckin & Coady, 1999). Given<br />

our various endeavors to foster a vocabulary inducing reading envir<strong>on</strong>ment, learners’<br />

performance seems unsatisfactory at first glance compared to some previous L2<br />

studies assessing learners’ receptive and productive knowledge gain through reading.<br />

Four possible factors might have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to this difference—namely, the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

authentic material, learners’ depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing, target word difficulty, and individual<br />

76


differences.<br />

In Rott (1999)’s study, significant receptive and productive knowledge were<br />

documented for words which appeared merely twice. Rott (1997) attributed<br />

learners’ better word gain to the inferenceability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words within well-designed<br />

reading materials in which sufficient c<strong>on</strong>textual cues were provided. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

target words in the reading were ensured to be main idea related in the texts.<br />

Meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words were more readily accessible in such a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Our<br />

study is more comparable with Knight (1994) and Paribakht and Weshche (1997)<br />

study in that authentic texts were used for reading. Paribakht and Wesche (1997)’s<br />

study using VKS to assess learners’ word gain in reading <strong>on</strong>ly and reading plus<br />

vocabulary exercise c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. For the reading <strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in which learners<br />

read a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 24 texts with target words repetitively appearing in the texts, the<br />

knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words mainly reached <strong>on</strong>ly the recogniti<strong>on</strong> level, a result similar<br />

to the present study. With the purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>veying informati<strong>on</strong> rather than<br />

fostering vocabulary learning, authentic materials could not provide readily adequate<br />

c<strong>on</strong>textual cues c<strong>on</strong>ducive to incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Necessary amount<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure for successful acquisiti<strong>on</strong> therefore increased possibly due to the play <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

inferenceability in authentic materials. The differences emerged from using<br />

pre-written and authentic texts also point to the need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> distinguishing between<br />

authentic and pre-written texts in investigating frequency factors while reading.<br />

Another reas<strong>on</strong> influencing word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> outcome in the study could be<br />

learners’ shallow depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing. In order to prevent incorrect inference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words and to raise learners’ awareness, textual gloss <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Chinese translati<strong>on</strong>s and<br />

highlighting were available to learners. However, it seems that such preliminary<br />

noticing did not encourage learners to engage in deeper mental processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> forming<br />

lexical-semantic-syntactic relati<strong>on</strong>ship. Due to the nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading,<br />

77


learners’ processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts was presumably meaning-based. Hence, <strong>on</strong>ly when<br />

word meaning is not available at the time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> use will learners take into account<br />

surrounding c<strong>on</strong>texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and deeper analyses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words c<strong>on</strong>textual<br />

cues be processed. As was indicated by Wesche and Paribakht (2000), in the c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading “<strong>on</strong>ce the immediate communicative need has been met, the<br />

learner does not undertake future mental processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word” (p. 197). The gloss<br />

originally incorporated for ensuring correct target word meaning, though suggested by<br />

previous studies as being beneficial to word learning, attenuated the possibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

learners’ deeper mental processing.<br />

A dilemma was posed here in that preciseness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meaning and depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

processing were both desired. It could be that more exposure al<strong>on</strong>g the reading<br />

process might be able to mend learners’ superficial acquisiti<strong>on</strong>s at initial stages.<br />

However, as was found in the current study, the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure for receptive and<br />

productive knowledge should be greater than at least 15 times in an extensive reading<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text. Whether learners have ample enough opportunities for such an amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

encounter and whether the vocabulary pay<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f in extensive reading is suitable in <strong>EFL</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>text are still in questi<strong>on</strong>. Thus, a more promising alternative for quality<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> might be to use a combinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit vocabulary<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> and implicit extensive reading. Positive evidence was found for the use<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit vocabulary instructi<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with extensive reading (Paribakht<br />

and Weshche, 1997; Quan, 1996), indicating the benefits <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> certain degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> teacher<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> in sec<strong>on</strong>d language vocabulary teaching.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong> (1990) proposed that the intrinsic difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words could be a<br />

possibility underlying the ease <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. The results in the current study<br />

seem to dem<strong>on</strong>strate such effect with learners needing more than an amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 15<br />

exposure <strong>on</strong> the ten target words mainly found in the most difficult band <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

78


COBUILD word lists. Compare to Rott (1999)’s study in which an amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> six<br />

exposure was proposed as being sufficient leading to both receptive and productive<br />

word knowledge <strong>on</strong> target words with higher frequency and easier than the <strong>on</strong>es in the<br />

current study, the potential difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, therefore, could serve as the<br />

reas<strong>on</strong> for increasing demands <strong>on</strong> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure necessary for word acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

in the present study. Such result furthermore points to the directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> taking into<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> target word difficulty factor when dealing with frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word<br />

exposure issue while reading.<br />

As was explicated previously, learners’ inexperience in extensive reading was<br />

proposed as being the major factor leading to their unsatisfactory word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the current study. Pulido (2003) pointed out that for advance readers, the<br />

automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activating reading strategies “free up the attenti<strong>on</strong>al resources for<br />

further vocabulary development” (p 239). The learners involved in the present study<br />

however, seems to be readers who have not mastered reading strategies to achieve<br />

automaticity. With lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading experience and thereby insufficient text<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, learners’ influenced incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> could be<br />

suspected. Aside from previous reading experience, learners’ entry vocabulary level<br />

could also be a factor affecting incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. In accordance to<br />

the study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Zahar, Cobb, and Spada (2001), the present study also found a str<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

dependence <strong>on</strong> frequency for low vocabulary level learners. Compared to the<br />

participants in Zahar, Cobb, and Spada (2001) study, whose vocabulary ability ranged<br />

from beginners to bilinguals, learners in the current study were all c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

intermediate L2 learners. The vocabulary entry ability effect persisted, however,<br />

even with a group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> high homogeneity such as <strong>on</strong>es in this study. Taken learners’<br />

reading experiences and vocabulary ability together, it seems the complex issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure required for successful incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> could<br />

79


not be untangled without c<strong>on</strong>sidering the impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> possible individual differences<br />

factors.<br />

4.3.4. Summary<br />

summarized below.<br />

Through the data analysis, answers to the research questi<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

1. Is it feasible to incorporate <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools, gloss, word lists, and<br />

computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus for<br />

vocabulary learning purpose? What problems or limitati<strong>on</strong>s will be<br />

encountered?<br />

Technically, it is feasible to incorporate <strong>on</strong>-line searching tools, gloss, word lists,<br />

and computerized sequencing techniques to an <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus for<br />

vocabulary learning purposes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Texts</str<strong>on</strong>g> were successfully sequenced according to<br />

difficulty levels with repetitious target word exposure. Moreover, learners’<br />

measurable word gains were observed after reading <strong>on</strong> the extensive reading syllabus.<br />

However, more laborious manual efforts could be saved given appropriate n<strong>on</strong><br />

culture-oriented reading texts and a more advanced glossing method.<br />

2. What is learners’ feedback <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2a. What is learners’ attitude towards the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus?<br />

2b. What are learners’ difficulties and recommendati<strong>on</strong>s towards the <strong>on</strong>-line<br />

extensive reading syllabus?<br />

The results revealed that learners have enjoyed reading from the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus and their inclinati<strong>on</strong> for using such a reading program to facilitate<br />

vocabulary learning is str<strong>on</strong>g. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, learners’ lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> experience in extensive<br />

reading had influenced their reading fluency and comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

3. If the <strong>on</strong>-line extensive reading syllabus is effective in leading to vocabulary<br />

80


acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, how much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words do learners have to encounter for successful<br />

word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>?<br />

3a. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word are required to lead to learners’ receptive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

3b. How much exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word are required to lead to learners’ productive word<br />

knowledge?<br />

The answer c<strong>on</strong>cerning the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure necessary for successful<br />

receptive and productive word knowledge is inc<strong>on</strong>clusive. Learners were not able to<br />

recall correct translati<strong>on</strong>s even for words which had appeared up to 15 times. It<br />

could <strong>on</strong>ly be assumed that an amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> at least 15 times is required for<br />

word acquisiti<strong>on</strong> within extensive reading c<strong>on</strong>texts. Inferenceability in authentic<br />

materials, learners’ shallow depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing, difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, and<br />

individual differences were reas<strong>on</strong>ed as factors for such results.<br />

81


5.1. Overview<br />

Chapter Five<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

This study originated from an exploratory issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> what happens to the English<br />

teaching process when we incorporate an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus for<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> purposes. The <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus was<br />

developed by integrating word lists and quantitative corpus analyses using word<br />

frequency computer programs. With four word lists, The General Service Word List,<br />

the Senior High Student’s Word List in Taiwan, the University Word List, and a high<br />

frequent word list out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the original 5008 articles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a parallel corpus, Sinorama, the<br />

syllabus graded articles based <strong>on</strong> filtering texts according to vocabulary difficulty.<br />

Easier texts were sequenced first to provide texts with i+1 and repetitive exposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target words was ensured to induce quality incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

syllabus was furthermore enhanced with gloss, a bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, and<br />

highlights <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words. Thirty-eight English-as-a-foreign-language college<br />

freshmen learners were recruited to read 16 sequenced texts using the <strong>on</strong>line extensive<br />

reading syllabus within a period <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 12 weeks. A pretest, a posttest, a background<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire, an <strong>on</strong>line evaluati<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naire, and an in-class evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire were used as instruments for data collecti<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong> to feasibility,<br />

our sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>cern is to which extent do learners liked the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus. Finally, the study takes an interest in exploring the issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure required for successful receptive and productive incidental vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> while reading extensively.<br />

The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the study indicate that it is both technically and pedagogically<br />

feasible to incorporate an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus into English learning at<br />

82


<strong>EFL</strong> settings. Technically, though manual efforts were laborious during certain steps<br />

in the selecti<strong>on</strong> and sequencing processes, texts with comprehensible vocabulary were<br />

successfully chosen and arranged with various <strong>on</strong>line tools. Pedagogically, learners’<br />

word gains were found and their attitudes towards <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

were mostly positive after reading <strong>on</strong>line. Yet, learners’ inexperience in extensive<br />

reading was observed to somewhat impede the meaning making process during<br />

reading. No definite answer was found with regards to the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure<br />

necessary for receptive and productive vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. It is estimated that,<br />

with texts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such difficult level in the study, in order for learners to recall meanings<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words, an exposure amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> at least 15 times are needed <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>line<br />

extensive reading envir<strong>on</strong>ment. In the following secti<strong>on</strong>, the limitati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

current study, the directi<strong>on</strong>s for future research, the developmental implicati<strong>on</strong>s for an<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus and the pedagogical implicati<strong>on</strong>s are presented.<br />

5.2. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Study<br />

In order to address the need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners, n<strong>on</strong>-English-major college freshmen, the<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>s for selecting reading texts and word lists in this preliminary versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus were determined according to our specific learners’<br />

vocabulary level. Doubtlessly, the same texts and word lists could not be used for<br />

learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different vocabulary levels. In regards to the selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target text and<br />

determinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiar and target word lists, the decisi<strong>on</strong> was made based <strong>on</strong><br />

researchers’ experience exclusively, it is possible that despite the outcome <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> prettest,<br />

the researcher had over or under estimate learners’ vocabulary level. Similar to<br />

choosing teaching materials, the selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target texts and word lists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such an<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus depend <strong>on</strong> teachers’ understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’<br />

current vocabulary level and the judgment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’ learning priority, hence, even<br />

83


with the same group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners, different teachers might have diverse opini<strong>on</strong>s when<br />

dealing with such decisi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Moreover, from previous descripti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> sequencing procedures within the<br />

<strong>on</strong>line syllabus, it is evident that the syllabus is merely an estimati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the general<br />

vocabulary level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners rather than addressing to a particular<br />

individual learner’s need. Questi<strong>on</strong>s may be raised <strong>on</strong> how the syllabus might not be<br />

adequate for learners whose vocabulary level falls outside the norm.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to learner differences, the current <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus<br />

possesses potential limitati<strong>on</strong>s in respect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learner interacti<strong>on</strong>. With the main<br />

purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the study being to discover the effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the syllabus and to quest<br />

for an optimal sequencing method, this preliminary versi<strong>on</strong> lacks the access <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

providing learners involvement and interacti<strong>on</strong> during reading.<br />

Besides the incompleteness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus design,<br />

methodological limitati<strong>on</strong>s have resulted in an indefinite answer <strong>on</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure amount and degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incidental word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Due to the time limit,<br />

learners in this study read <strong>on</strong>ly 16 texts within 12 weeks. The highest amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

target word exposure reached 15 times, an amount proved inadequate for building up<br />

receptive or productive word knowledge. Moreover, in the current study, the<br />

intrinsic difficulty <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and the lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inferential c<strong>on</strong>textual cues in<br />

reading texts seem to increase acquisiti<strong>on</strong> difficulties for learners.<br />

5.3. Directi<strong>on</strong>s for Future Research<br />

The issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> required word exposure for effective incidental vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> within extensive reading envir<strong>on</strong>ments remained inc<strong>on</strong>clusive in this study.<br />

Without further c<strong>on</strong>firmati<strong>on</strong>, the preliminary results merely <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer a dawning<br />

understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an exposure amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> at least 15 times for prominent receptive<br />

84


word knowledge. The puzzle could be further clarified if more target word<br />

repetiti<strong>on</strong>s could be provided within reading texts. In additi<strong>on</strong>, with careful c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target word difficulty and selecti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts, future research might be able<br />

to explore into the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure issue with more generalizable answers.<br />

Future research should also c<strong>on</strong>sider the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual differences factors such<br />

as reading experiences, vocabulary level, and additi<strong>on</strong>al potential learner variables<br />

which might account for differences in L2 incidental vocabulary while reading.<br />

5.4. Developmental Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for an Online Extensive Reading Syllabus<br />

The most immediate developmental directi<strong>on</strong> for the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading<br />

syllabus is to incorporate substantive corpora and word lists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various levels in order<br />

to meet the learning demands <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different learners. Moreover, the laborious manual<br />

modificati<strong>on</strong> invested during glossing process urged a more plausible method able to<br />

generate gloss within an <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus. Besides addressing the<br />

need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> diverse groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners, the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus could be<br />

more adaptive to individual learners by introducing some extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’<br />

involvement in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> selecting texts.<br />

First, if texts could be pre-arranged according to text topics, such as sports,<br />

health, entertainment or arts, learners can be provided with the opportunity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

choosing to read texts under the topics which interest them. In order to encourage<br />

ESL students to read extensively, Coady (1997) stressed the importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> providing<br />

materials interesting to learners as the most prominent factors within a reading<br />

curriculum. Under the chosen topic, which c<strong>on</strong>tains texts a learner favor mostly, the<br />

<strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus performs the sequencing procedures with a benefit<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing fewer number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading texts. Compared to the current versi<strong>on</strong>, a<br />

syllabus which groups texts into topics would be able to <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer learner interacti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

85


also accelerate sequencing time with lesser texts.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, in the process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing, some texts may pass all the necessary<br />

requirements and still possess the same amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words and exposed words.<br />

While selecting our sixteen texts, researchers’ interventi<strong>on</strong> was involved in deciding<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g these equally qualified texts. Thus, in the future, for texts which are equal in<br />

terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary learning c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, titles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these texts could all be listed <strong>on</strong> the<br />

syllabus to allow learner involvement in selecting texts.<br />

Third, a learner portfolio which includes sensitive recordings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual<br />

reading progress and learners’ pers<strong>on</strong>al notes could be established. The current<br />

syllabus kept track <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners’ reading progress by showing learners whether they<br />

have finished a certain text or not. However, as was suggested in the evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire, most learners agreed that the texts are too l<strong>on</strong>g for <strong>on</strong>line reading and<br />

they were <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten lost in l<strong>on</strong>g texts. If page breaks could be added for l<strong>on</strong>g texts and<br />

reading history kept so that learners can be sent back to the exact page where they left<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f last time, learners’ reading frustrati<strong>on</strong> could be reduced. In additi<strong>on</strong>, learners<br />

have suggested the functi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a vocabulary notepad, which includes individualized<br />

records <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposed words during reading and also learners’ pers<strong>on</strong>al notes <strong>on</strong> specific<br />

target words or reflecti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> readings. Furthermore, a text ranking device was<br />

suggested. Learners indicated that they would be more motivated if they could rank<br />

texts and express their feelings <strong>on</strong> whether they liked the article after reading.<br />

All in all, a more sophisticated tracking mechanism should be set up to induce<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s and for learners to record self-learning progresses. The suggesti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered by the learners in the current study are in accordance with Robb’s (2002)<br />

recommendati<strong>on</strong> as he argued that a tacking mechanism built in a learning website<br />

could better motivate learners and integrate self-access learning resources.<br />

86


5.5. Pedagogical Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

In the present study, lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading skills and experiences were found<br />

as major obstacles interfering learners’ reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Moreover, learners’<br />

target word knowledge has not reached the receptive repertoire, albeit an exposure<br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> up to 15 times after reading. In an attempt to amend insufficient<br />

extensive reading strategies and enhance vocabulary learning, a paradigm <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

incorporating explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> and implicit learning was suggested.<br />

To begin with, careful orientati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s in school lab will be needed not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

to familiarize learners with the functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>-line syllabus but to explain the<br />

goals and methodology <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading. Useful strategies in extensive reading<br />

should be made explicit and practice sessi<strong>on</strong>s scheduled as guidance to the extensive<br />

reading activity.<br />

The <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus is especially useful for incorporating<br />

explicit teaching and implicit learning due to the fact that the syllabus is c<strong>on</strong>structed<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the word lists selected by instructors. The same set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word lists could be<br />

utilized both in class and for the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus. In other words,<br />

learners might have the chances to read the target words that they have learned in<br />

class while engaging in extensive reading <strong>on</strong>-line. After direct and explicit<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target words in class, the reading texts <strong>on</strong> the syllabus could provide<br />

more diverse usage and c<strong>on</strong>texts. The comprehensive exposure to target words in<br />

extensive reading could bring learners a step further al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

vocabulary learning; <strong>on</strong> the other hand, the fragile and unstable word knowledge<br />

resulted exclusively from extensive reading in turn could be more str<strong>on</strong>gly anchored<br />

to learners’ existing lexic<strong>on</strong> with explicit instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It should be cauti<strong>on</strong>ed, however, that learners may not be aware <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between these two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning. The instructor, hence, is<br />

87


esp<strong>on</strong>sible for c<strong>on</strong>necting the two by directing learners’ attenti<strong>on</strong> to the relati<strong>on</strong>ship<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word use and c<strong>on</strong>texts, the difference between intensive and extensive reading<br />

strategies, and the initiative <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> utilizing an extensive reading approach to complement<br />

in-class instructi<strong>on</strong>. In other words, learners should be taught to make use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

different strategies for different reading purposes, a skill they will need for future life<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g learning. Moreover, firmly established reading habits from prior experiences<br />

might not be easy to amend especially when learners in Taiwan still have to cope with<br />

two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading simultaneously in class and at home. Therefore, guidance<br />

should not end with the initial orientati<strong>on</strong>. On-going time to time reminder <strong>on</strong> goals<br />

and strategies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extensive reading could be emphasized in class from teachers’<br />

observati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learners reading progress and resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cerns <strong>on</strong> how the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus satisfies the learning<br />

need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> those learners whose vocabulary level are remarkably superior or inferior to<br />

the average vocabulary level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the group. Word knowledge is a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous<br />

development in which knowledge for a word is refined by each exposure and that<br />

more exposure to words entails familiarizati<strong>on</strong> with word usage in related c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

Thus, for more advanced learners, it is without doubt that reading, even with texts<br />

which are slightly simple compared to learner’s current vocabulary level, could enrich<br />

learners’ vocabulary inventory. For less competent learners, n<strong>on</strong>etheless, special<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> and instructi<strong>on</strong> from the instructor may be needed in order to overcome this<br />

individual difference.<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> is yet another aspect which inextricably demands instructor<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> since the syllabus is <strong>on</strong>ly capable to the point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequencing texts. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong> to participati<strong>on</strong> which could be recorded by a tracker, evaluati<strong>on</strong> methods<br />

could vary depending <strong>on</strong> the objectives that respective courses aim to achieve. With<br />

limited time, comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s which focus <strong>on</strong> general understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts<br />

88


may be the most time-ec<strong>on</strong>omic way to examine learners’ comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. For<br />

learners and teachers, performances <strong>on</strong> these questi<strong>on</strong>s could be a quick source to<br />

check whether learners’ interpretati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts are <strong>on</strong> the right track.<br />

Extensive reading can play a role in developing the capacity for critical thinking<br />

crucial for success in higher educati<strong>on</strong> (Day & Bamford, 1998), thus, activities could<br />

be designed in order to involve learners in active thinking, reflecting, and commenting<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts. Activities such as summarizing, research projects, oral reports, and writing<br />

reflecti<strong>on</strong>s require learners’ through understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text meanings and are therefore<br />

plausible to be used as alternative ways for evaluati<strong>on</strong> when class time allows. In<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trast to comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s, these activities serve as more authentic and<br />

meaningful purposes for learners’ reading.<br />

In sum, with instructor’s input <strong>on</strong> reading strategy training, word instructi<strong>on</strong>, and<br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong>, the <strong>on</strong>line extensive reading syllabus is capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> serving as a bridge<br />

linking explicit teaching and implicit learning for more ideal vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

89


REFERENCES<br />

Berman, R.A. (1984 ). Syntactic comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the foreign language reading process<br />

in Alders<strong>on</strong>, C. & Urquhart, A. (Eds) Reading in a Foreign Language. New<br />

York: L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Carter, R., & McCarthy, M. (1988). Vocabulary and Language Teaching. New York:<br />

L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Chen, H. J. (1998). A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Taiwanese <strong>EFL</strong> learners’<br />

vocabulary size. Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the fifteenth c<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> English teaching<br />

and learning in the Republic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> China. (pp. 193-211). Taipei: Crane.<br />

Chen, H. J. (2003). Developing a technology-enhanced self-access reading<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2003 Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> English<br />

Teaching and Learning in the Republic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> China. (pp. 103-116). Department<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English Language, Literature, and Linguistics, Providence University<br />

Shalu, Taichung County.<br />

Chun, D. M., & Plass, J. L. (1996). Research <strong>on</strong> text comprehensi<strong>on</strong> in multimedia<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments. Language Learning & Technology, 1(1), 60-80.<br />

Coady, J., & Huckin, T. (1997). Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Vocabulary Acquisit<strong>on</strong>: A Rati<strong>on</strong>ale<br />

for Pedagogy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Cobb, T., & Horst, M. (2001). Reading academic English: Carrying learners across<br />

the lexical threshold. In J. Flowerdew & Peacock (Eds.), Research<br />

perspectives <strong>on</strong> English for academic purposes (pp. 315-329). Cambridge,<br />

UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Davis, J. (1989). Facilitating effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> marginal glosses <strong>on</strong> foreign language reading.<br />

The Modern Language Journal, 73(1), 41-47.<br />

Davis, N., & Lyman-Hager, M. (1997). Computers and L2 reading: Student<br />

performance, student attitudes. Foreign Language Annals, 30(1), 58-72.<br />

Day, R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the sec<strong>on</strong>d language classroom.<br />

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Elley, W. (1991). Acquiring literacy in a sec<strong>on</strong>d language: The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> book-based<br />

programs. Language Learning, 41(3), 375-411.<br />

Ellis, N. (1995). C<strong>on</strong>sciousness in sec<strong>on</strong>d language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: A review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> field<br />

studies and laboratory experiments. Language Awareness, 4, 123-146.<br />

Flowerdew, J. (1993). C<strong>on</strong>cordancing as a tool in course design. System, 21(2),<br />

231-244.<br />

Gass, S. (1999). Discussi<strong>on</strong>: <strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary learning. Studies in Sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

Language Acquisit<strong>on</strong>, 21, 319-333.<br />

Gettys, S., Imh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>, L., & Kautz, J. (2001). Computer-assisted reading: the effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

glossing format <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> and vocabulary retenti<strong>on</strong>. Foreign<br />

90


Language Annals, 34(2), 91-101.<br />

Ghadessy, M. (1979). Frequency counts, word lists, and materials preparati<strong>on</strong>: A new<br />

approach. English Teaching Forum, 17, 24-27.<br />

Ghadirian, S. (2003). Providing c<strong>on</strong>trolled exposure to target vocabulary through<br />

the screening and arranging <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> texts. Language Learning & Technology,<br />

6(1), 147-164,<br />

Grabe, W., & Stoller, F. (1997). Reading and vocabulary development in a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language: A case study. In James Coady & Thomas Huckin (Eds) Sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Groot, P. (2000). Computer assisted sec<strong>on</strong>d language vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Language Learning & Technology, 4(1), 60-81.<br />

Hafiz, F., & Tudor, I. (1989). Extensive reading and the development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> language<br />

skills. ELT Journal, 43(1), 4-13.<br />

Hafiz, F., & Tudor, I. (1990). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Graded</str<strong>on</strong>g> readers as an input medium in L2 learning.<br />

System, 18(1), 31-42.<br />

Harley, B. (1996). Introducti<strong>on</strong>: Vocabulary learning and teaching in a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53(1), 3-11.<br />

Hazengerg, S., & Hulstijn, (1996). Defining a minimal receptive sec<strong>on</strong>d-language<br />

vocabulary for n<strong>on</strong>-native university students: An empirical investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Applied Linguistics, 17(20), 145-163.<br />

Henriksen, B. (1999). Three dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary development. Studies in<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Learning, 21, 303-317.<br />

Holley, F. (1973). A study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary learning in c<strong>on</strong>text: The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new-word<br />

density in German reading materials. Foreign Language Annals, 6, 339-347.<br />

Horst, M., Cobb. T., & Meara, P. (1998). Bey<strong>on</strong>d a clockwork orange: Acquiring<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d language vocabulary through reading. Reading in a Foreign<br />

Language, 11(2), 207-233.<br />

Huang, C. C. (2000). A threshold for vocabulary knowledge <strong>on</strong> reading<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Seventeenth C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> English<br />

Teaching and Learning in the Republic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> China. pp132-145.<br />

Huang, C. C. (2001). An investigati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ESP students’ vocabulary knowledge<br />

and reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Selected Papers from the Tenth Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Symposium <strong>on</strong> English Teaching. (pp. 436-445). Taipei: Crane.<br />

Huckin, T., & Coady, J. (1999). <strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language: A review. Studies in Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, 21, 181-193.<br />

Hulstijn, J. H. (1993). When do foreign-language readers look up the meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

unfamiliar words? The influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> task and learner variable. The Modern<br />

Language Journal, 77(2), 139-147.<br />

91


Hulstijn, J. H., Hollander, M., & Greidanus, T. (1996). <strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary learning<br />

by advanced foreign language students: The influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> marginal glosses,<br />

dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use, and reoccurrence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unknown words. The Modern Language<br />

Journal, 80(3), 327-339.<br />

Jacobs, G. M. (1994). What lurks in the margin: Use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary glosses as a<br />

strategy in sec<strong>on</strong>d language reading. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 5,<br />

115-137.<br />

Joe, A. (1995). Text-based tasks and incidental vocabulary learning. Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language<br />

Research, 11, 149-158.<br />

Krasheh, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

evidence for the input hypothesis. The Modern Language Journal, 73(4),<br />

440-455.<br />

Krashen, S. (1993). The case for free voluntary reading. The Canadian Modern<br />

Language Review, 50(1), 72-81.<br />

Knight, S. (1994). Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary use while reading: The effects <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> and<br />

vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different verbal abilities. The Modern<br />

Language Journals, 78(3), 285-299.<br />

Kost, C., Foss, P., & Lenzini, J. (1999). Textual and pictorial gloss: Effectiveness <strong>on</strong><br />

incidental vocabulary growth when reading in a foreign language. Foreign<br />

Language Annals, 32(1), 89-113.<br />

Ky<strong>on</strong>gho, H., & Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. (1989). Reducing the vocabulary load and encouraging<br />

vocabulary learning through reading newspaper. Reading in a Foreign<br />

Language, 6(1), 323-335.<br />

Laufer, B. (1989). What percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexis is essential for comprehensi<strong>on</strong>? In C.<br />

Lauren & M. Nordman (Eds.), Special Language: From humans thinking to<br />

thinking machine (pp. 69-75). Cleved<strong>on</strong>, UK: Multilingual Matters.<br />

Laufer, B (1997). The lexical plight in sec<strong>on</strong>d language reading: Words you d<strong>on</strong>’t<br />

know, words you think you know, and words you can’t guess. In J. Coady &<br />

T. Huckin (Eds.). Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: A Rati<strong>on</strong>ale for<br />

Pedagogy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Laufer, B., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2001). <strong>Incidental</strong> vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language: The c<strong>on</strong>struct <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> task-induced involvement. Applied Linguistics,<br />

22(1), 1-26.<br />

Leefa, V.J. (1992). Making foreign language texts comprehensible for beginners: An<br />

expreimnet with an electr<strong>on</strong>ic glossary. System, 20(1), 63-73.<br />

Liou, H. C. (2000). The electr<strong>on</strong>ic bilingual dicti<strong>on</strong>ary as a reading aid to <strong>EFL</strong><br />

learners: Research findings and implicati<strong>on</strong>s. Computer Assisted Language<br />

92


Learning, 13(5), 467-476.<br />

Lomicka, L. (1998). To Gloss or not to gloss: An investigati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>line. Language Learning & Technology, 1(2), 41-50.<br />

Luppescu, S., & Day, R. (1993). Reading, dicti<strong>on</strong>aries, and vocabulary learning.<br />

Language Learning, 43(2), 263-287.<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>, B., & Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as a foreign language.<br />

System, 25(1), 91-102.<br />

Nagy, W.E. (1997). On the role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text in first- and sec<strong>on</strong>d-language vocabulary<br />

learning. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Descripti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, pedagogy (pp. 64-83). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anders<strong>on</strong>, R. C. (1985). Learning words from c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233-253.<br />

Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anders<strong>on</strong>, R. C. (1987). Learning word meanings form<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text during normal reading. American Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research Journal, 24,<br />

237-270.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. (1990). Teaching & learning vocabulary. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. (1993). Vocabulary size, growth, and use. In Robert Schreuder & Bert<br />

Welterns (Eds.), The bilingual lexic<strong>on</strong>, pp115-134. Philadelphia: John<br />

Benjamins Publishing Company.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in anotherlLanguage. Cambridge, UK:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, P., & Coady, J. (1988). Vocabulary and reading. In R<strong>on</strong>al Carter and Michael<br />

McCarthy (Eds). Vocabulary and language teaching: 97-110. New York:<br />

L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Nait<strong>on</strong>, P., & Ky<strong>on</strong>gho, H. (1995). Where would general service vocabulary stop and<br />

special purposes vocabulary begin? System, 23(1), 35-41.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. & Waring, R. (1997). Vocabulary size, text coverage and word lists. In N.<br />

Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Descripti<strong>on</strong>, acquisiti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

pedagogy (pp. 64-83). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Nuttall, C. (2000). Teaching reading skills in a foreign language. UK: Macmillan<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Paribakht, T. S. & Wesche, M. B. (1993). Reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> and sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

language development in a comprehensi<strong>on</strong>-based ESL program. TESL<br />

Canada Journal, 11, 9-29.<br />

Paribakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. (1997). Vocabulary enhancement activities and<br />

reading for meaning in sec<strong>on</strong>d language vocabulary development. In Coady,<br />

J., & Huckin, T. (Eds.). Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Vocabulary Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

93


Rati<strong>on</strong>ale for Pedagogy (pp. 174-200). New York: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Pulido, D. (2003). Modeling the role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d language pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency and topic<br />

familiarity in sec<strong>on</strong>d language incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> through<br />

reading. Language Learning, 53(2), 233-284.<br />

Qian, D. (1996). ESL vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: C<strong>on</strong>textualizati<strong>on</strong> and<br />

dec<strong>on</strong>textualizati<strong>on</strong>. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 120-142.<br />

Raptis, H. (1997). Is sec<strong>on</strong>d language reading vocabulary best learned by reading?<br />

The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53(3), 69-75.<br />

Read, J. (2000). Assessing Vocabulary. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Renandya, W., & Rajan, S. (1999). Extensive reading with adult learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English as<br />

a sec<strong>on</strong>d language. RELC Journal, 30(1), 39-60.<br />

Robb, T. (2002). Examining self-access. Paper presented at Sec<strong>on</strong>d Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> the Curriculum, Testing and New Technologies, Sultan<br />

Qaboos University, Muscat, Oman.<br />

Rott, S. (1999). The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure frequency <strong>on</strong> intermediate language<br />

learners’ incidental vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and retenti<strong>on</strong> through reading.<br />

Studies in Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, 21(4), 589-620.<br />

Saragi, T., Nati<strong>on</strong>, P., & Meister, G. (1978). Vocabulary learning and reading. System,<br />

6, 72-80.<br />

Sinclair, J. M. (1995). Collins COBUILD Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary (2 nd edn. ). L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>:<br />

HarperCollins.<br />

Schmitt, N., & Schmitt, D. (1995). Vocabulary notebooks: Theoretical underpinnings<br />

and practical suggests. ELT Journal, 49, 133-143.<br />

Schnotz, W., & Grz<strong>on</strong>dziel, H. (1996). Knowledge acquisiti<strong>on</strong> with static and \<br />

animated pictures in computer-based learning. Learning and Instructi<strong>on</strong>, 3,<br />

247-249.<br />

Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T. (1996). Assessing sec<strong>on</strong>d language vocabulary<br />

knowledge: depth versus breadth. The Canadian Modern Language Review,<br />

53(1), 13-39.<br />

Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T S. (2000). Reading-based exercises in sec<strong>on</strong>d language<br />

vocabulary learning: An introspective study. The Modern Language Journal,<br />

84(4), 196-213.<br />

West, M. (1953). A General Service List <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English Words. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: L<strong>on</strong>gman, Green<br />

& Co.<br />

Worthingt<strong>on</strong>, D. & Nati<strong>on</strong>, P. (1996). Using texts to sequence the introducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new<br />

vocabulary in an EAP course. RELC Journal, 27(2), 1-11.<br />

Wu, J. C., Yeh, K. C., Chuang, T. C., Shei, W. C., & Chang, J. S. (2003). TotalRecall:<br />

94


A bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordance for computer assisted translati<strong>on</strong> and language<br />

learning. Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 41 st Associati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Computati<strong>on</strong>al Linguistics<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference, Sappora, Japan, July 7-12.<br />

http://candle.cs.nthu.edu.tw/totalrecall/c<strong>on</strong>cordance/c<strong>on</strong>cordance.aspx<br />

Xue, G., & Nati<strong>on</strong>, I. S. P. (1984). A university word list. Language Learning and<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>, 3, 215-229.<br />

Yoshii, M., & Flaitz, J. (2002). Sec<strong>on</strong>d language incidental vocabulary retenti<strong>on</strong>: The<br />

effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text and picture annotati<strong>on</strong> types. CALICO Journal, 20(1), 33-58.<br />

Zahar, R., Cobb, T., & Spada, N. (2001). Acquiring vocabulary through reading:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency and c<strong>on</strong>textual richness. The Canadian Modern<br />

Language Review, 57(4), 541-572.<br />

鄭恆雄 (2002),大學入學考試中心高中英文參考字彙表,台北:大學入學考試<br />

中心。<br />

鄭恆雄 (2003),大學入學考試中心高中英文參考字彙表之編輯方法及原則。<br />

Proceedings the 7t Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> Multimedia Language<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>. (pp. 514-529). Taipei: Crane.<br />

95


第 1-6 題 電腦方面<br />

Appendix A<br />

The Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

請回答以下問題,並在符合敘述的 □ 內打勾。謝謝 !<br />

1. 我是否常使用電腦 是 □ 否 □<br />

2. 我在宿舍裡有個人電腦 是 □ 否 □<br />

3. 寒假期間家中是否有有電腦可使用? 是 □ 否 □<br />

(答「是」者請答第 4 題,答「否」者跳答第 7 題)<br />

4. 家中電腦是否能上網? 是 □ 否 □<br />

5. 家中的電腦是用何種方式連接網路? 電話撥接 □ 寬頻系統 □<br />

6. 寒假期間可以聯絡到你的 e-mail ______________________________________<br />

7. 平常是否會利用網路上的資源學英文? 是 □ 否 □<br />

8. 在過去的英文課中,英文老師是否曾經利用電腦輔助英文教學?<br />

否 □<br />

是 □ (答 “是” 者請繼續回答第 8(1)及 8(2)題)<br />

8(1). 老師曾經利用電腦輔助 閱讀 □ 寫作 □ 單字 □<br />

(可複選) 文法 □ 聽力 □ 會話 □<br />

96<br />

其他 □ ______________________<br />

(請寫出輔助的教學內容)<br />

8(2). 老師的教學方式 在電腦教室裡集體教學□<br />

(可複選) 在家中上網自行學習□<br />

其他□ __________________________<br />

(請寫出教學方式)


第 9-12 題 英文學習方面<br />

9. 我對學習英文的感覺<br />

非常喜歡 □ 喜歡 □ 普通 □ 不喜歡 □ 非常不喜歡 □<br />

10. 我覺得我的英文程度<br />

非常好 □ 很好 □ 普通 □ 不好 □ 非常不好 □<br />

11. 我對學習下列英文能力的感覺<br />

(1)聽 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

(2)說 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

(3)讀 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

(4)寫 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

(5)字彙 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

(6)文法 非常困難 □ 困難 □ 普通 □ 容易 □ 非常容易 □<br />

12. 我目前最想加強學習的英文能力: (單選)<br />

聽 □ 說 □ 讀 □ 寫 □ 字彙 □<br />

第 13-17 題 英文泛讀方面<br />

泛讀是指利用空閒時間閱讀大量自己有興趣的小說、文章、雜誌、報紙…<br />

等,也就是閱讀課外的英文讀物的習慣。<br />

13 在過去的英文課程中,老師曾經鼓勵我培養英文泛讀的習慣 (老師曾經規<br />

定我在課堂外自行閱讀英文雜誌或小說)<br />

否 □<br />

是 □ (答是者請繼續回答 13(2),13(3))<br />

我曾經閱讀過的英文讀物:(可複選)<br />

13(1). 英文小說 □ ________(請寫出你曾經閱讀過幾本小說)<br />

13(2). 英文雜誌 □ (請繼續勾出閱讀過的雜誌名稱。如果你用雜誌<br />

練習聽說能力,則不包括在內。)<br />

空中英語教室□ 普及美語□ 長春藤美語□<br />

大家說英語□ Time□ Newsweek□<br />

其他□_________________________(請寫出名稱)<br />

13(3). 英文報紙 □ (請繼續勾出閱讀過的英文報紙名稱)<br />

China Post□ Taipei Times□ Taiwan News□<br />

97


Student Post□ 其他□____________(請寫出名稱)<br />

14. 我目前有英文泛讀的習慣 是 □ 否 □<br />

(答「是」者請答第15 題,答「否」者跳答第17 題)<br />

15. 我通常利用閒暇時間閱讀 (可複選)<br />

15(1). 英文小說 □<br />

15(2). 英文雜誌 □ (請勾出你習慣閱讀的雜誌名稱。如果你用雜誌<br />

練習聽說能力,則不包括在內。)<br />

空中英語教室□ 普及美語□ 長春藤美語□<br />

大家說英語□ Time□ Newsweek□<br />

其他□_________________________(請寫出名稱)<br />

15(3). 英文報紙 □ (請勾出你習慣閱讀的英文報紙名稱)<br />

China Post□ Taipei Times□ Taiwan News□<br />

Student Post□ 其他□____________(請寫出名稱)<br />

16. 我多常閱讀英文讀物<br />

每天□ 每週一~三次□ 每週一次□<br />

每兩週一次□ 每個月一次□ 不一定,有時間才閱讀□<br />

17.我沒有英文泛讀習慣的原因 (可複選)<br />

英文太差,文章大都看不懂□ 沒時間閱讀□<br />

對學習英文沒興趣□ 沒找到有興趣的文章□ 沒時間找合適的文章□<br />

其他 □_______________________________ (請寫出原因)<br />

Thank You for Your Time !!<br />

98


Appendix B<br />

The Pretest<br />

Please check the blank next to the words that you have seen before and provide all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

its translati<strong>on</strong>s that you know. Thank you!<br />

請在你看過得英文單字旁打勾並提供中文翻譯。寫出關於此單字所有你知道的翻<br />

譯或解釋。謝謝!<br />

English Words (英文單字) Chinese Translati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(翻譯)<br />

1. entrepreneur<br />

2. institute<br />

3. calorie<br />

4. hypnosis<br />

5. perform<br />

6. administrative<br />

7. immune<br />

8. finally<br />

9. c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

10. psychiatric<br />

11. sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

12. virtual<br />

13. therapy<br />

14. faculty<br />

15. achieve<br />

16. c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

17. series<br />

18. combat<br />

19. strain<br />

20. pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

21. calligraphy<br />

22. clinical<br />

23. potential<br />

24. symbolic<br />

25. negotiate<br />

99


26. optimistic<br />

27. c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

28. interview<br />

29. epidemic<br />

30. inadequate<br />

31. altitude<br />

32. startling<br />

33. deputy<br />

34. accommodate<br />

35. accumulate<br />

36. embark<br />

37. initial<br />

38. precisely<br />

39. import<br />

40. inevitably<br />

41. insufficient<br />

42. m<strong>on</strong>opoly<br />

43. undertake<br />

44. sustain<br />

45. abolish<br />

46. implicit<br />

47. innovative<br />

48. recruitment<br />

49. sophisticated<br />

50. eloquently<br />

51. acceptable<br />

52. believe<br />

53. occasi<strong>on</strong><br />

54. opini<strong>on</strong><br />

55. reduce<br />

56. extensi<strong>on</strong><br />

57. favorable<br />

58. keenly<br />

59. luggage<br />

60. tensi<strong>on</strong><br />

100


Appendix C<br />

The Posttest<br />

各位同學<br />

先謝謝你們參與這次的測驗。以下的 50 個單字中各有五個選項,請你依照自己<br />

對各個單字的認識圈出符合自己對此單字認識的描述。如果圈出的選項需要寫入<br />

中文翻譯或造句請一併填入。造句時,你可以改變單字的時態、詞性、單複數以<br />

符合句子需要。謝謝各位。<br />

Example 1 :<br />

Apple:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III<br />

我看過這個字,意思可能是___蘋果___________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

Example2:<br />

read:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是___閱讀__________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V.<br />

我會用這個字造句。 I read newspapers very <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

101


1. entrepreneur:<br />

VI. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

VII. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

VIII. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IX. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

X. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

2. institute:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

3. calorie:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

4. hypnosis:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

5. perform:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

102<br />

6. administrative:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

7. immune:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

8. finally:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

9. c<strong>on</strong>stant:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

10. psychiatric:<br />

VI. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

VII. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

VIII. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IX. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

X. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)


11. sessi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

12. virtual:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

13. therapy:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

14. faculty:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

15. achievement:<br />

VI. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

VII. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

VIII. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IX. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

X. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

103<br />

16. c<strong>on</strong>cept:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

17. series:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

18. combat:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

19. strain:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

20. pers<strong>on</strong>nel:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)


21.calligraphy:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

22. clinical:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

23. potential:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

24. symbolic:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

25. negotiate:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

104<br />

26. optimistic:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

27. c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

28.interview:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

29. epidemic:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

30. inadquate:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)


31. altitude:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

32. startling:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

33. deputy:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

34. accommodate:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

35. accumulate:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

105<br />

36. embark:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

37. initial:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

38. precisely:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

39. import:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

40. inevitably:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)


41.insufficient:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

42. m<strong>on</strong>opolistic:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

43. undertaking:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

44. sustain:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

45.abolish:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

106<br />

46. inhibit:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

47. innovative:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

48. recruitment:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

49. sophisticated:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

50. eloquently:<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

VI. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

VII. 我看過這個字,意思可能是<br />

_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

VIII. 我確知這個字,它的意思是<br />

_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IX. 我會用這個字造句。<br />

___________________________________<br />

___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選


1 point<br />

Appendix D<br />

An Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the VKS Scoring<br />

entrepreneur: (item <strong>on</strong>e circled)<br />

○I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II. 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

2 points<br />

entrepreneur: (item two circled)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是_________________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

entrepreneur: (item three circled, but the meaning provided was incorrect)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是____娛樂__________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

entrepreneur: (item four circled, but the meaning provided was incorrect)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是______________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_____娛樂_____(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

entrepreneur: (item five circled, but the meaning and the sentence provided was incorrect)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是______________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_____娛樂_____(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。Watching movie is my entrepreneur.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

107


3 points<br />

entrepreneur: (item three circled and the meaning provided was correct)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是_______企業家_____(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_________________ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

4 points<br />

entrepreneur: (item four circled and the meaning provided correct)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是___________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_____企業家____ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 ___________________________________.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

entrepreneur: (item five circled, the meaning provided correct, the sentence is semantically<br />

correct but grammatically wr<strong>on</strong>g)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是___________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_____企業家____ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 I with to be ○a entrepreneur in the future.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

5 points<br />

entrepreneur: (item five circled, the meaning provided correct, the sentence is both<br />

semantically and grammatically correct)<br />

I. 我對這個字沒印象。<br />

II 我看過這個字但不知道它的意思。<br />

III. 我看過這個字,意思可能是___________(請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

IV. 我確知這個字,它的意思是_____企業家____ (請寫中文翻譯)。<br />

V. 我會用這個字造句。 I with to be an entrepreneur in the future.<br />

(如果你造了句子請同時完成第四個選項)<br />

108


Appendix E<br />

The Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

109


Appendix F<br />

The In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

各位同學好,首先謝謝你們填這份問卷。本問卷目的在了解同學們閱讀泛讀網站<br />

的過程及感想,希望可藉此修正並提供同學真正需要的指導。對於下列的問題,<br />

請同學以下列五種選項表達你的意見。<br />

1 非常不同意 2 同意 3沒意見 4 同意 5 非常同意<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

1. 這個閱讀網站讓我在寒假時也能繼續學英文 □ □ □ □ □<br />

2. 這個閱讀網站安排在寒假的閱讀進度適當 □ □ □ □ □<br />

3. 這個閱讀網站讓需要學習的單字重複出現<br />

可以幫助我學習新的單字 □ □ □ □ □<br />

4. 這個閱讀網站的文章內容及類型相當多元 □ □ □ □ □<br />

5. 這個閱讀網站的文章大部分都有趣 □ □ □ □ □<br />

6. 比起自己找尋合適的文章,我比較喜歡這個閱讀網站<br />

幫我安排合適的文章 □ □ □ □ □<br />

7. 在接觸過網路泛讀後,我對學習英文的感覺<br />

非常喜歡 □ 喜歡 □ 普通 □ 不喜歡 □ 非常不喜歡 □<br />

8. 如果上網方便,未來是否會繼續使用類似網站來加強英文能力?<br />

會 □ 不會 □<br />

(答「會」者請答第 9 題,答「不會」者請答第 10 題)<br />

9. 我會繼續使用類似網站的原因<br />

可以加強英文閱讀能力 □ 可以加強英文字彙 □<br />

文章多元有趣 □ 可以知道需要學習的單字有哪些 □<br />

比起一般讀物,這樣的網站有單字查詢功能 □<br />

其他 □_______________________________________________________<br />

(請寫出原因)<br />

10. 我不會繼續使用類似網站的原因<br />

不習慣看長篇英文文章 □ 不喜歡自己讓網站安排文章和閱讀進度 □不<br />

習慣在電腦上閱讀 □<br />

其他 □_________________________________________________________<br />

(請寫出原因)<br />

11. 如果可以的話,你認為此閱讀網站的設計應該如何改進?<br />

______________________________________________________________<br />

______________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________<br />

110


Appendix G<br />

The Syllabus for the Sixteen Readings<br />

111


Appendix H<br />

The Introductory Page <strong>on</strong> How to Use the Online Extensive Reading Syllabus<br />

112


Appendix I<br />

The First Reading <strong>on</strong> the Syllabus<br />

* The target words were highlighted in red and exposed words were highlighted in<br />

green.<br />

* The word “expand” was glossed with Chinese translati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

* The access to the bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer, Total Recall, was at the bottom <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

interface.<br />

113


Appendix J<br />

A Search Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the bilingual c<strong>on</strong>cordancer TotalRecall<br />

* The word “expend” is searched.<br />

114


Appendix K<br />

An Example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

115


Appendix L<br />

Student Reading Process Recorded by Tracker<br />

* The first table records each student’s performance <strong>on</strong> the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for every reading.<br />

* The sec<strong>on</strong>d table provides students’ emails and records how many readings each<br />

student have completed.<br />

116


親愛的同學<br />

Appendix M<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sent Form<br />

我們和資工系教授共同製作了一套網路閱讀教材,供同學課餘提昇英文程度之<br />

用。總共有十六篇,提供單字解釋及「雙語關鍵字前後文檢索系統 TotalRecall」,<br />

並有一些文意測驗之選擇題,希望你們在 1/10-3/10 間分次完成。我們同時需要<br />

你們做兩次課堂之單字練習及回答兩份問卷。這項活動有助於我們了解你們學英<br />

文情形,也同時能提昇你的英文能力。<br />

如果你順利全程做完寒假部份之六篇,可得下學期大一英文期末成績 5%,另 10<br />

篇在 2/16-3/10 期間完成,佔下學期成績 5%。先謝謝你們。如有任何進一步問題,<br />

我很願意為你們解答。<br />

外語研究所學姐 黃虹慈敬上 g915253@oz.nthu.edu.tw<br />

指導教授及下學期大一英文老師 外語所劉顯親老師 hcliu@mx.nthu.edu.tw<br />

如你同意參與,請簽名_______________________________<br />

117


第 1-6 題 電腦方面<br />

Appendix N<br />

The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Background Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

1. 我是否常使用電腦 是 94.7% (36/38) 否 5.3% (2/38)<br />

2. 我在宿舍裡有個人電腦 是 100% (38/38) 否 0% (0/38)<br />

3. 寒假期間家中是否有有電腦可使用? 是 97.3% (37/38) 否 2.6% (1/38)<br />

(答「是」者請答第 4 題,答「否」者跳答第 7 題)<br />

4. 家中電腦是否能上網? 是 94.6% (35/38) 否 5.4% (2/38)<br />

5. 家中的電腦是用何種方式連接網路? 電話撥接 37.1% (13/38)<br />

寬頻系統 62.9% (22/38)<br />

6. 寒假期間可以聯絡到你的 e-mail ______________________________________<br />

7. 平常是否會利用網路上的資源學英文? 是 7.9% (3/38) 否 92.1% (35/38)<br />

8. 在過去的英文課中,英文老師是否曾經利用電腦輔助英文教學?<br />

否 73.7% (28/38)<br />

是 26.3% (10/38) (答 “是” 者請繼續回答第 8(1)及 8(2)題)<br />

8(1). 老師曾經利用電腦輔助 閱讀 14.3% (2/14) 寫作 28.6% (4/14)<br />

(可複選) 單字 0% (0/14) 文法 7.1% (1/14)<br />

聽力 35.7% (5/14) 會話 14.3% (2/14)<br />

其他 0% (0/14)<br />

8(2). 老師的教學方式 在電腦教室裡集體教學 63.6% (7/11)<br />

第 9-12 題 英文學習方面<br />

9. 我對學習英文的感覺<br />

(可複選) 在家中上網自行學習 36.4% (4/11)<br />

非常喜歡 5.3% (2/38) 喜歡 26.3% (10/38) 普通 60.5% (23/38)<br />

不喜歡 5.3% (2/38) 非常不喜歡 2.6% (1/38)<br />

118


10. 我覺得我的英文程度<br />

非常好 0% (0/38) 很好 5.3% (2/38) 普通 52.6% (20/38)<br />

不好 28.9% (11/38) 非常不好 13.2% (5/38)<br />

11. 我對學習下列英文能力的感覺<br />

Mean 1.<br />

非常困難<br />

聽<br />

2.5<br />

說<br />

2.5<br />

讀<br />

3.1<br />

寫<br />

2.6<br />

字彙<br />

2.4<br />

文法<br />

2.4<br />

18.4%<br />

(7/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

7.9%<br />

(3/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

10.5%<br />

(4/38)<br />

2. 困難 3. 普通 4. 容易 5.<br />

非常容易<br />

31.6%<br />

(12/38)<br />

50%<br />

(19/38)<br />

10.5%<br />

(4/38)<br />

23.6%<br />

(10/38)<br />

31.6%<br />

(12/38)<br />

42.1%<br />

(16/38)<br />

31.6%<br />

(12/38)<br />

34.2%<br />

(13/38)<br />

65.8%<br />

(25/38)<br />

60.5%<br />

(23/38)<br />

34.2%<br />

(13/38)<br />

42.1%<br />

(16/38)<br />

12. 我目前最想加強學習的英文能力: (單選)<br />

119<br />

18.4%<br />

(7/38)<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

23.7%<br />

(9/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

聽 39.5% (15/38) 說 23.7% (9/38) 讀 5.3% (2/38)<br />

寫 5.3% (2/38) 字彙 26.3% (10/38)<br />

第 13-17 題 英文泛讀方面<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

泛讀是指利用空閒時間閱讀大量自己有興趣的小說、文章、雜誌、報紙…<br />

等,也就是閱讀課外的英文讀物的習慣。<br />

13 在過去的英文課程中,老師曾經鼓勵我培養英文泛讀的習慣 (老師曾經規<br />

定我在課堂外自行閱讀英文雜誌或小說)<br />

否 21.1% (8/38)<br />

是 78.9% (30/38) (答是者請繼續回答 13(2),13(3))<br />

我曾經閱讀過的英文讀物:(可複選)<br />

13(1). 英文小說 28.3% (19/67)


13(2). 英文雜誌 41.7% (28/67) (請繼續勾出閱讀過的雜誌名稱。如<br />

果你用雜誌練習聽說能力,則不包括在內。)<br />

空中英語教室 39.4% (28/70) 普及美語 2.8% (2/70)<br />

長春藤美語 9.9% (7/70) 大家說英語 28.2% (20/70)<br />

Time12.7% (9/70) Newsweek4.2% (3/70)<br />

Live ABC 1.4% (1/70)<br />

13(3). 英文報紙 29.8% (20/67) (請繼續勾出閱讀過的英文報紙名稱)<br />

China Post 31.4% (11/35) Taipei Times 8.6% (3/35)<br />

Taiwan News 8.6% (3/35) Student Post 51.4% (18/35)<br />

14. 我目前有英文泛讀的習慣 是 13.2% (5/38) 否 86.8% (33/38)<br />

(答「是」者請答第15 題,答「否」者跳答第17 題)<br />

15. 我通常利用閒暇時間閱讀 (可複選)<br />

15(1). 英文小說 14.3% (1/7)<br />

15(2). 英文雜誌 71.4% (5/7)<br />

(請勾出你習慣閱讀的雜誌名稱。如果你用雜誌練習聽說能力,<br />

則不包括在內。)<br />

空中英語教室 40% (2/5) 普及美語 0% (0/5)<br />

長春藤美語 0% (0/5) 大家說英語 0% (0/5)<br />

Time 40% (2/5) Newsweek20% (1/5)<br />

15(3). 英文報紙 14.2%(1/7) (請勾出你習慣閱讀的英文報紙名稱)<br />

China Post 0% (0/1) Taipei Times100% (1/1)<br />

Taiwan News 0% (0/1) Student Post 0% (0/1)<br />

16. 我多常閱讀英文讀物<br />

每天 0% (0/5) 每週一~三次 80% (4/5) 每週一次 0% (0/5)<br />

每兩週一次 0% (0/5) 每個月一次 0% (0/5) 不一定,有時間才閱讀 20% (1/5)<br />

17.我沒有英文泛讀習慣的原因 (可複選)<br />

英文太差,文章大都看不懂 20% (10/50) 沒時間閱讀 28% (14/50)<br />

對學習英文沒興趣 12% (6/50) 沒找到有興趣的文章 16% (8/50)<br />

沒時間找合適的文章 24% (12/50)<br />

其他 0% (0/50)<br />

120


Appendix O<br />

The Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Online Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>s 非常<br />

不同<br />

意<br />

1 這個閱讀網站的字型適合 0%<br />

閱讀<br />

(0/38)<br />

2 這個閱讀網站的文字間距 0%<br />

適合閱讀<br />

(0/38)<br />

3 這個閱讀網站的文字大小 0%<br />

適合閱讀<br />

(0/38)<br />

4 這個閱讀網站的文章太長 0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

5 這個閱讀網站的文字難度 0%<br />

太高<br />

(0/38)<br />

6 這個閱讀網站的文字難度 13.2%<br />

太簡單<br />

(5/38)<br />

7 這個閱讀網站的及時查單 15.8%<br />

字功能方便<br />

(6/38)<br />

8 這個閱讀網站提供的查詢 10.5%<br />

網站 Total Recall 可以幫助<br />

我更瞭解單字的用法<br />

(4/38)<br />

9 這個閱讀網站將新單字和 5.3%<br />

學過的單字以顏色區分可<br />

以提醒我哪些是重要,需要<br />

學習的單字<br />

(2/38)<br />

121<br />

不同意 沒意見 同意 非常<br />

同意<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

10.5%<br />

(4/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

47.4%<br />

(18/38)<br />

28.9%<br />

(11/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)<br />

7.9%<br />

(3/38)<br />

18.4%<br />

(7/38)<br />

21.1%<br />

(8/38)<br />

18.4%<br />

(7/38)<br />

28.9%<br />

(11/38)<br />

47.4%<br />

(18/38)<br />

34.2%<br />

(13/38)<br />

26.3%<br />

(10/38)<br />

26.3%<br />

(10/38)<br />

15.8%<br />

(6/38)<br />

52.6%<br />

(20/38)<br />

52.6%<br />

(20/38)<br />

52.6%<br />

(20/38)<br />

44.7%<br />

(17/38)<br />

31.6%<br />

(12/38)<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

44.7%<br />

(17/38)<br />

44.7%<br />

(17/38)<br />

57.9%<br />

(22/38)<br />

15.8%<br />

(6/38)<br />

15.8%<br />

(6/38)<br />

15.8%<br />

(6/38)<br />

23.7%<br />

(9/38)<br />

7.9%<br />

(3/38)<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

13.2%<br />

(5/38)


Appendix P<br />

Results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the In-class Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

1 非常<br />

不同意<br />

1 這個閱讀網站讓我在寒假時也能 0%<br />

繼續學英文<br />

(0/38)<br />

2 這個閱讀網站安排在寒假的閱讀 0%<br />

進度適當<br />

(0/38)<br />

3 這個閱讀網站讓需要學習的單字 0%<br />

重複出現可以幫助我學習新的單 (0/38)<br />

字<br />

4 這個閱讀網站的文章內容及類型 0%<br />

相當多元<br />

(0/38)<br />

5 這個閱讀網站的文章大部分都有 2.6%<br />

趣<br />

(1/38)<br />

6 比起自己找尋合適的文章,我比 0%<br />

較喜歡這個閱讀網站幫我安排合 (0/38)<br />

適的文章<br />

122<br />

2 同意 3 沒意見 4同意 5非常<br />

同意<br />

2.6%<br />

(1/38)<br />

7.9%<br />

(3/38)<br />

10.5%<br />

(4/38)<br />

0%<br />

(0/38)<br />

5.3%<br />

(2/38)<br />

7.9%<br />

(3/38)<br />

31.6%<br />

(12/38)<br />

47.4%<br />

(18/38)<br />

18.4%<br />

(7/38)<br />

36.8%<br />

(14/38)<br />

55.3%<br />

(21/38)<br />

26.3%<br />

(10/38)<br />

52.6% 13.2%<br />

(20/38) (5/38)<br />

34.2% 10.5%<br />

(13/38) (4/38)<br />

55.3% 15.8%<br />

(21/38) (6/38)<br />

50% 13.2%<br />

(19/38) (5/38)<br />

31.6% 7.9%<br />

(12/38) (3/38)<br />

47.4% 15.8%<br />

(18/38) (6/38)<br />

7. 在接觸過網路泛讀後,我對學習英文的感覺<br />

非常喜歡 7.9% (3/38) 喜歡 42.1% (16/38) 普通 44.7% (17/38)<br />

不喜歡 5.3% (2/38) 非常不喜歡 0% (0/38)<br />

8. 如果上網方便,未來是否會繼續使用類似網站來加強英文能力?<br />

會 81.6% (31/38) 不會 18.4% (7/38)<br />

(答「會」者請答第 9 題,答「不會」者請答第 10 題)<br />

9. 我會繼續使用類似網站的原因<br />

可以加強英文閱讀能力 42% (24/74) 可以加強英文字彙 19% (12/74)<br />

文章多元有趣 11% (7/74) 可以知道需要學習的單字有哪些 14% (9/74)<br />

比起一般讀物,這樣的網站有單字查詢功能 16% (10/74)<br />

其他 3% (2/74) (沒時間找文章時很方便)<br />

10. 我不會繼續使用類似網站的原因<br />

不習慣看長篇英文文章 8% (1/12)<br />

不喜歡自己讓網站安排文章和閱讀進度 8% (1/12)<br />

不習慣在電腦上閱讀 51% (6/12)<br />

其他 33% (4/12) (已有泛讀習慣和合適閱讀文章)<br />

11. 如果可以的話,你認為此閱讀網站的設計應該如何改進?


123

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!