20.07.2013 Views

Gestural Alignment Constraints and Vowel Devoicing in ... - Linguistics

Gestural Alignment Constraints and Vowel Devoicing in ... - Linguistics

Gestural Alignment Constraints and Vowel Devoicing in ... - Linguistics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Gestural</strong> <strong>Alignment</strong> <strong>Constra<strong>in</strong>ts</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Vowel</strong> <strong>Devoic<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> Andean Spanish<br />

The variable, gradient process of unstressed vowel devoic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> proximity to voiceless<br />

consonants observed <strong>in</strong> a number of languages such as Korean, Montreal French, Greek <strong>and</strong><br />

Turkish can readily be expla<strong>in</strong>ed as the result gestural overlap with<strong>in</strong> the framework of<br />

Articulatory Phonology (see Beckman 1994 for a summary). As devoic<strong>in</strong>g primarily affects high<br />

vowels <strong>and</strong> is associated with fast speech <strong>in</strong> these languages, it is ascribed to a rate-based<br />

decrease <strong>in</strong> the distance between gestures that causes the glottal abductions of voiceless<br />

segments to imp<strong>in</strong>ge upon adjacent vowels. High vowels are most vulnerable due to their short<br />

duration. While unspecified <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> gestural overlap dur<strong>in</strong>g fast speech account for such<br />

rate-sensitive high vowel devoic<strong>in</strong>g, this approach cannot expla<strong>in</strong> several features of vowel<br />

devoic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Andean Spanish: * (a) <strong>Devoic<strong>in</strong>g</strong> is not rate-dependent <strong>and</strong> frequently occurs <strong>in</strong> slow<br />

speech (b) Word-<strong>in</strong>ternally, /e/, a mid-vowel of <strong>in</strong>termediate duration, is devoiced <strong>in</strong> a proportion<br />

similar to that of the shorter high vowels /i/ <strong>and</strong> /u/ (/e/=13% devoiced, /i/ =13%, /u/=22%) (c) In<br />

word f<strong>in</strong>al syllables with /s/ <strong>in</strong> coda position, which are predom<strong>in</strong>antly plural morphemes, all<br />

vowels exhibit high rates of devoic<strong>in</strong>g (/i/=20%, /u/=19%, /e/=20%, /o/=18%, /a/=15%).<br />

I argue that gestural alignment constra<strong>in</strong>ts of the type proposed by Gafos (2002) express<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of Articulatory Phonology <strong>in</strong> Optimality Theoretic terms can account for the unusual<br />

characteristics of vowel devoic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Andean Spanish. These constra<strong>in</strong>ts specify a range of<br />

possible coord<strong>in</strong>ation relationships govern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ter-segmental tim<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> facilitate discussion of<br />

cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic phas<strong>in</strong>g differences. Gafos shows that different configurations of his constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

CC COORD effectively model the types of consonant-consonant phas<strong>in</strong>g found <strong>in</strong> various<br />

languages, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those that require release of the first consonant <strong>in</strong> order to maximize<br />

recoverability <strong>and</strong> others that allow for close CC transitions (Fig 1). I propose that the consonant<br />

to vowel <strong>and</strong> vowel to consonant relationships posited <strong>in</strong> Articulatory Phonology <strong>and</strong><br />

conceptualized as CV COORD <strong>and</strong> VC COORD by Gafos can also differ across languages <strong>and</strong><br />

between dialects. If the CV <strong>and</strong> VC COORD constra<strong>in</strong>ts of Andean Spanish are assumed to<br />

allow greater overlap between vowels <strong>and</strong> consonants, the rate <strong>in</strong>dependence of vowel devoic<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> this variety can thus be understood as follow<strong>in</strong>g from its basic tim<strong>in</strong>g patterns. The gradient,<br />

variable nature of vowel devoic<strong>in</strong>g is modeled by formulat<strong>in</strong>g these alignment constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of phase w<strong>in</strong>dows (Byrd 1996b) (Fig 2b) which <strong>in</strong>clude the canonical relationships that<br />

produce fully voiced vowels shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 2a. Follow<strong>in</strong>g previous claims that vowelconsonant<br />

homorganicity can affect gestural overlap (Hall 2003,) the tendency to devoice /e/ but<br />

not the other non-high vowels /o/ <strong>and</strong> /a/ word-<strong>in</strong>ternally is attributed to the greater amount of<br />

overlap permitted between this [+coronal] vowel (Clements & Hume 1995) <strong>and</strong> the most<br />

frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g voiceless consonants <strong>in</strong> Spanish which are also [+coronal]. This effect is<br />

formalized as a constra<strong>in</strong>t limit<strong>in</strong>g the overlap between heterorganic vowels <strong>and</strong><br />

consonants,*OVERLAP V//CHET (see Fig. 3), that outranks CV <strong>and</strong> VC COORD.<br />

The devoic<strong>in</strong>g of all vowels <strong>in</strong> word f<strong>in</strong>al syllables closed by /s/ is expla<strong>in</strong>ed by extend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Gafos’ constra<strong>in</strong>t schema to <strong>in</strong>tra-segmental tim<strong>in</strong>g. Studies reviewed <strong>in</strong> Krakow (1999) <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />

that consonants’ secondary gestures tend to occur earlier <strong>in</strong> relation to their oral gestures <strong>in</strong> coda<br />

compared to onset position. I propose that, although /s/’s glottal open<strong>in</strong>g gesture is normally<br />

simultaneous with its oral constriction (Silverman 1997), its laryngeal abduction is sometimes<br />

phased to anticipate oral closure <strong>in</strong> syllable f<strong>in</strong>al position <strong>and</strong> thus impedes voic<strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

preced<strong>in</strong>g vowel (Fig. 4a). Evidence support<strong>in</strong>g the explanation of high devoic<strong>in</strong>g rates <strong>in</strong> this<br />

context as a result of the early occurrence of coda /s/’s glottal open<strong>in</strong>g gesture rather than /s/’s<br />

status as a plural marker or word f<strong>in</strong>al position itself comes from a decrease <strong>in</strong> the frequency of<br />

devoic<strong>in</strong>g as word f<strong>in</strong>al vowel-/s/ comb<strong>in</strong>ations are placed <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al position of progressively<br />

larger prosodic doma<strong>in</strong>s (<strong>in</strong>tonational phrase, utterance). Presumably, this <strong>in</strong>verse relationship<br />

between devoic<strong>in</strong>g rate <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al position <strong>and</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> size reflects the extent to which phrase f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

lengthen<strong>in</strong>g counters the syllable position effect <strong>and</strong> separates the preced<strong>in</strong>g vowel from /s/’ s<br />

glottal open<strong>in</strong>g gesture (Fig 4b). In addition, vowels followed by an /s/ <strong>in</strong> coda rather than <strong>in</strong><br />

onset of the follow<strong>in</strong>g syllable are significantly more likely to devoice word <strong>in</strong>ternally as well as<br />

word f<strong>in</strong>ally. As the consequences of syllable position for <strong>in</strong>tra-gestural tim<strong>in</strong>g may differ across<br />

languages (Kochetov 2006), the proposed “coda /s/ effect” is expressed <strong>in</strong> terms of the<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts OS COORD (default oral-secondary gesture coord<strong>in</strong>ation) <strong>and</strong> OG COORDC (oralglottal<br />

coord<strong>in</strong>ation, coda).<br />

* Observations regard<strong>in</strong>g Andean Spanish are based on record<strong>in</strong>gs of 180 <strong>in</strong>formants from Cusco, Peru


Figure 1 Consonant to Consonant Coord<strong>in</strong>ation Adapted from Gafos (2002)<br />

Consonant Cluster with Open Transition Consonant Cluster with Greater Overlap<br />

CC COORD: ALIGN (Center C1, Onset C2) CC COORD: ALIGN (Release C1, Target C2)<br />

Center C1 Release C1<br />

Onset C2 Target C2<br />

Figure 2a Canonical CV, VC Coord<strong>in</strong>ation with no devoic<strong>in</strong>g (Browman & Goldste<strong>in</strong> 1990,<br />

Gafos 2002)<br />

Center C1 Release V<br />

CV COORD: ALIGN (Center C, Onset V) C gesture<br />

VC COORD: ALIGN (Release V, Target C) V gesture<br />

Onset V Target C2<br />

Figure 2b Proposed CV, VC Coord<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong> Andean Spanish, Incorporat<strong>in</strong>g Phase W<strong>in</strong>dows:<br />

CV COORD: ALIGN (Onset ~ Center C, Onset V) V Onset may align with any po<strong>in</strong>t rang<strong>in</strong>g from Onset to Center <strong>in</strong> C<br />

VC COORD: ALIGN (Target ~ Release V, Target C) C Target may align with any po<strong>in</strong>t rang<strong>in</strong>g from Target to Release <strong>in</strong> V<br />

Example of <strong>Devoic<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> CV sequence: Example of <strong>Devoic<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> VC sequence:<br />

Target C Center V<br />

Onset V Target C<br />

Figure 3 *OVERLAP V//CHET: The plateau of a consonant may not overlap the plateau of an<br />

adjacent heterorganic vowel.<br />

*OVERLAPHET vacuously satisfied *OVERLAPHET violated<br />

e8 Ccoronal o8 Ccoronal<br />

Figure 4a Word-f<strong>in</strong>al (V8s) Figure 4b Intonation Phrase F<strong>in</strong>al (Vs)<br />

Oral Gestures<br />

Glottal Gestures<br />

Selected References<br />

Beckman, Mary. 1994. When is a Syllable not a Syllable? In Phonological Structure <strong>and</strong><br />

Language Process<strong>in</strong>g. Takashi Otake & Anne Cutler (eds.) Mouton de Gruyter, New York<br />

Byrd, Dani. 1996b. A Phase W<strong>in</strong>dow Framework for Articulatory Tim<strong>in</strong>g. Phonology 13,139-69.<br />

Clements, G. N. & Elizabeth Hume. 1995. The Internal Organization of Speech Sounds.<br />

In The H<strong>and</strong>book of Phonological Theory. John Goldsmith (ed).<br />

Blackwell, Cambridge MA. 245-306.<br />

Gafos, Adamantios. 2002. A Grammar of <strong>Gestural</strong> Coord<strong>in</strong>ation. Natural Language <strong>and</strong><br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Theory 20, 269-33<br />

Hall, N. 2003. Gestures <strong>and</strong> Segments: <strong>Vowel</strong> Intrusion as Overlap. Ph.D. dissertation,<br />

University of Massachusetts, Amherst.<br />

Krakow, Rena. 1999. Physiological Organization of Syllables: A Review. Journal of Phonetics<br />

27, 23-54

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!