20.07.2013 Views

slides - Linguistics

slides - Linguistics

slides - Linguistics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

The Role of Canonicity in Shaping Perceptual<br />

Biases in Speech Processing<br />

Charles B. Chang 1 and Alan Mishler<br />

University of Maryland, College Park<br />

1 cbchang@umd.edu<br />

13th Conference on Laboratory Phonology<br />

July 27, 2012


Outline<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

1 Background<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

2 Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

3 Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

4 Discussion<br />

Nature of Language Transfer<br />

Future Directions<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Non-veridicality of non-native speech perception<br />

Non-native (L2) listeners tend to be worse than native (L1)<br />

listeners at perceiving phonemes in a language.<br />

[Nábělek and Donahue 1984, Bradlow and Pisoni 1999, Garcia Lecumberri and Cooke 2006, Cutler et al. 2008]<br />

cognitive constraints<br />

[Goh 2000]<br />

partial knowledge of the L2 vocabulary, grammar, and culture<br />

[Bloomfield et al. 2011]<br />

phonological interference (viz., transfer) from L1<br />

[Lado 1957, Best 1994, Best 1995]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Native language biases as obstacle<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

L1 transfer results in L2 performance that is either worse than L1<br />

performance (“negative” transfer) or not very different from L1<br />

performance (“positive”, i.e. neutral, transfer). [Odlin 1989]<br />

Negative transfer: L1 processing of similar sounds as the same<br />

phoneme becomes a hindrance in an L2 where the sounds are<br />

different phonemes. [Flege 1995, Cutler 2001, Best and Tyler 2007]<br />

L1 Japanese speakers have trouble with English /l/ and /r/,<br />

even after learning to pronounce the distinction reliably.<br />

[Goto 1971, Sheldon and Strange 1982, Yamada and Tohkura 1992, Yamada 1995]<br />

L1 English speakers have trouble with Japanese short and long<br />

consonants and vowels. [Han 1992, Tajima et al. 2008]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Native language biases as innocuous<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Neutral transfer: parallelism between L1 and L2 phoneme<br />

inventories results in little trouble with perception of L2 sounds.<br />

Japanese: [l] and [r] are variants of the same phoneme ><br />

great difficulty distinguishing between English /l/ and /r/.<br />

German: /l/ and /r/ are different phonemes > little difficulty<br />

distinguishing between between English /l/ and /r/.<br />

[Iverson et al. 2003]<br />

Possibility of advantageous L1 transfer?<br />

Test case: perception of unreleased stops.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Final stops in English: released or unreleased<br />

Frequency (Hz)<br />

Frequency (Hz)<br />

6000<br />

6000<br />

0<br />

0 p u p p u tt<br />

1<br />

Time (s)<br />

0<br />

0 p u p p u tt<br />

1<br />

Time (s)<br />

released:<br />

[p h 5p (h) ] vs. [p h 5t (h) ]<br />

unreleased:<br />

[p h 5p^] vs. [p h 5t^]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Frequency vs. canonicity of unreleased stops in English<br />

Final stop variants in American English demonstrate a disparity<br />

between frequency (rate of occurrence) and canonicity (relative<br />

status).<br />

Final voiceless stops are frequently unreleased at all POAs.<br />

/p/: produced without release 40–60% of the time<br />

/t/: produced without release 43–72% of the time<br />

/k/: produced without release 15–75% of the time<br />

[Byrd 1993, Davidson 2011]<br />

Released stops are recognized as canonical.<br />

unreleased [t^] more frequent than released [t (h) ] word-finally<br />

released [t (h) ] more effective prime [Sumner and Samuel 2005]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Canonicity of unreleased stops in Korean<br />

Final stops in Korean are obligatorily unreleased (i.e., frequent +<br />

canonical). [Sohn 1999]<br />

Final stop contrast has a heavy functional load (many minimal<br />

pairs/triplets).<br />

[p ^ ap^] ‘rice’, [p ^ at^] ‘field’, [p ^ ak^] ‘outside’<br />

[t ^ ap^] ‘answer’, [t ^ at^] ‘close’, [t ^ ak^] ‘chicken’<br />

[k ^ ap^] ‘pay back’, [k ^ at^] ‘be similar’, [k ^ ak^] ‘each’<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

Native language biases as advantageous?<br />

Question: Are unreleased stops in American English better<br />

perceived by Americans (L1 English speakers) or by Koreans in the<br />

U.S. (L2 English speakers with L1 Korean)?<br />

Factors influencing listeners’ performance favor both groups.<br />

Americans: greater experience with American English<br />

efficient processing of English speech<br />

familiarity with language-specific properties of unreleased stops<br />

[Tsukada 2006, Tsukada et al. 2007, Tsukada and Roengpitya 2008]<br />

Koreans: greater experience with unreleased stops<br />

bias to attend to formant transition cues to final stops<br />

[Abramson and Tingsabadh 1999, Cho and McQueen 2006]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Transfer in L2 Speech Perception<br />

Unreleased Stops: English vs. Korean<br />

Research Questions<br />

A canonicity benefit in speech perception<br />

Hypothesis: Canonicity leads to a perceptual advantage.<br />

Prediction: Koreans will be better than Americans at perceiving<br />

unreleased stops in English.<br />

without lexical information (Experiment 1)<br />

with lexical information (Experiment 2)<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Participants<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Talkers:<br />

2 native speakers of English from Maryland<br />

both m., age 19 and 25 yr<br />

no experience with obligatorily unreleased stops<br />

Listeners:<br />

25 native speakers of English born/raised in the U.S.<br />

11 m., mean age 21.1 yr (SD 5.4)<br />

no experience with obligatorily unreleased stops<br />

25 native speakers of Korean born/raised primarily in S. Korea<br />

11 m., mean age 27.6 yr (SD 7.5)<br />

mean AOA to the U.S. = 21.1 yr (SD 9.0)<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Stimuli<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

56 bisyllabic English nonce words of the shape C1V1C2V2C3<br />

C1 = ô, V1 = {5, @}, C2 = z<br />

V2 = {i, u, A, eI, oU, AI, Aô}, C3 = {p^, t^, k^, ∅}<br />

stress = initial or final<br />

e.g., ô@"zAIp^ (rezipe) v. "ô5zAIp^ (ruzzipe)<br />

Recorded in a sound-attenuated booth.<br />

Zoom H4N audio recorder, Audix HT5 head-mounted mic<br />

44.1-kHz sampling, 24-bit resolution<br />

items presented via English spelling<br />

Releases removed and intensity normalized using Praat.<br />

[Boersma and Weenink 2011]<br />

ensured that oral closure was realized<br />

“dereleased” stops perceived similarly to unreleased stops<br />

[Lisker 1999]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Procedure<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Task: speeded 4AFC identification of finals in English nonce words<br />

(played in a carrier sentence: sample trial)<br />

stimuli presented through headphones in E-Prime<br />

responses entered on a serial response box<br />

Analysis: logistic mixed-effects modeling of identification accuracy.<br />

[Jaeger 2008, Dixon 2008]<br />

starting with random-effect terms for Participant and Item<br />

adding fixed-effect terms for Final (unreleased stop or<br />

non-stop), Group (Americans or Koreans), and a Final x<br />

Group interaction<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Identification of unreleased stops in English<br />

Accuracy (%)<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

stop sonorant<br />

Final<br />

Group<br />

Americans<br />

Koreans<br />

Model improved by:<br />

Final<br />

[χ 2 (1) = 106.71, p < 0.0001]<br />

Group<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing<br />

[χ 2 (1) = 4.633, p = 0.031]<br />

Final x Group<br />

[χ 2 (1) = 5.534, p = 0.019]<br />

Korean advantage for:<br />

final stops<br />

[β = 0.355, p = 0.017]<br />

No difference in RTs<br />

[F (1, 48) = 1.656, p = 0.204]


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Identification of unreleased stops in English<br />

Accuracy (%)<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

stop sonorant<br />

Final<br />

Group<br />

Americans<br />

Koreans<br />

Korean advantage for<br />

final stops applies to<br />

English syllable nuclei<br />

absent from Korean:<br />

/eI, oU, AI, Aô/.<br />

[β = 0.492, p = 0.002]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Participants<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Talkers:<br />

2 native speakers of English from Maryland<br />

both m., age 19 and 25 yr<br />

no experience with obligatorily unreleased stops<br />

Listeners:<br />

25 native speakers of English born/raised in the U.S.<br />

11 m., mean age 21.1 yr (SD 5.4)<br />

no experience with obligatorily unreleased stops<br />

25 native speakers of Korean born/raised primarily in S. Korea<br />

11 m., mean age 27.6 yr (SD 7.5)<br />

mean AOA to the U.S. = 21.1 yr (SD 9.0)<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Stimuli<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

48 minimal pairs of monosyllabic English words<br />

stop/stop (e.g., lip–lick), stop/zero (e.g., peek–pee)<br />

balanced for spoken frequency using COCA [Davies 2008-]<br />

Recorded in a sound-attenuated booth.<br />

Zoom H4N audio recorder, Audix HT5 head-mounted mic<br />

44.1-kHz sampling, 24-bit resolution<br />

items presented via English spelling<br />

Releases removed and intensity normalized using Praat.<br />

[Boersma and Weenink 2011]<br />

ensured that oral closure was realized<br />

“dereleased” stops perceived similarly to unreleased stops<br />

[Lisker 1999]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Procedure<br />

Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Task: speeded AX discrimination of English minimal pairs<br />

(contrasting in terms of final: sample trial 1, 2)<br />

stimuli presented through headphones in E-Prime<br />

responses entered on a serial response box<br />

Analysis: linear mixed-effects modeling of perceptual sensitivity, as<br />

indexed by d’. [Macmillan and Creelman 2005]<br />

starting with random-effect term for Participant<br />

adding fixed-effect terms for Contrast (stop/stop or<br />

stop/non-stop), Group (Americans or Koreans), and a<br />

Contrast x Group interaction<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Methods<br />

Results<br />

Discrimination of unreleased stops in English<br />

d'<br />

2.0<br />

1.5<br />

1.0<br />

0.5<br />

0.0<br />

stop/stop stop/zero<br />

Contrast<br />

Group<br />

Americans<br />

Koreans<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing<br />

Model improved by:<br />

Contrast<br />

[χ 2 (1) = 63.862, p < 0.0001]<br />

Contrast x Group<br />

[χ 2 (1) = 8.274, p = 0.004]<br />

Korean advantage for:<br />

stop/zero contrasts<br />

[β = 0.321, p = 0.041]<br />

No difference in RTs<br />

[F (1, 48) = 1.675, p = 0.202]


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Nature of Language Transfer<br />

Future Directions<br />

Positive effects of phonological transfer<br />

Koreans were better than Americans at perceiving unreleased stops<br />

in English.<br />

Experiment 1: better at identifying unreleased stops in English<br />

nonce words.<br />

Experiment 2: better at discriminating between the presence<br />

and absence of an unreleased stop in real English words.<br />

Advantageous L1 transfer:<br />

Canonicity of unreleased stops in Korean ≫ high frequency of<br />

unreleased stops in American English + L1 processing<br />

advantage + L1-specific phonetic knowledge.<br />

Canonicity of released stops in English seems to result in a<br />

reliance on burst cues to final stops. [Wang 1959]<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Generality of advantageous transfer<br />

Nature of Language Transfer<br />

Future Directions<br />

Koreans’ perceptual advantage was not a coincidence.<br />

Experiment 1: better at identifying unreleased stops following<br />

English vowels dissimilar from any Korean vowel.<br />

Experiment 2: better at discriminating stop/zero pairs of<br />

English words that are meaningful.<br />

General benefit of L1 transfer:<br />

Koreans abstract from their L1 experience a general ability to<br />

extract information about a final stop from formant transition<br />

cues (which can then be applied to new L2 contexts).<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Nature of Language Transfer<br />

Future Directions<br />

Variable nature of phonological transfer<br />

The conceptualization of L1 transfer in terms of negative or<br />

neutral effects on L2 speech perception is inadequate.<br />

occurrence of positive L1 transfer (boosting L2 perception<br />

beyond L1 levels)<br />

L1 transfer influences L2 perception in a manner dependent upon<br />

the alignment of properties in L1 and L2.<br />

absence of examples of advantageous L1 transfer due to the<br />

kinds of L1-L2 pairings examined in the literature<br />

favorable L1-L2 alignment > advantageous L1 transfer<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Background<br />

Experiment 1: 4AFC Identification<br />

Experiment 2: AX Discrimination<br />

Discussion<br />

Nature of Language Transfer<br />

Future Directions<br />

Advantageous transfer in other domains?<br />

L2 speakers are expected to display a perceptual advantage over<br />

L1 speakers in other cases of non-coincidence between frequency<br />

and canonicity.<br />

e.g., nasalized vowels in English vs. nasalized vowels in French<br />

Is English vowel nasalization better perceived by L1 French or<br />

L1 English speakers?<br />

The full range of transfer effects appear varied, suggesting fruitful<br />

avenues of further research in cross-linguistic speech perception.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


Acknowledgements<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Center for Advanced Study of Language, University of Maryland<br />

Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References I<br />

Appendix<br />

Abramson, A. S., Tingsabadh, K. (1999).<br />

Thai final stops: Cross-language perception.<br />

Phonetica 56, 111–122.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Best, C. T. (1994).<br />

The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: A perceptual assimilation model.<br />

In The Development of Speech Perception: The Transition from Speech Sounds to Spoken Words, ed. J. C.<br />

Goodman and H. C. Nusbaum, pp. 167–224. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Best, C. T. (1995).<br />

A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception.<br />

In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research, ed. W. Strange, pp.<br />

171–204. Baltimore, MD: York Press.<br />

Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D. (2007).<br />

Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities.<br />

In Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege, ed. O.-S.<br />

Bohn and M. J. Munro, pp. 13–34. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing.<br />

Bloomfield, A., Wayland, S. C., Rhoades, E., Blodgett, A., Linck, J., Ross, S. (2011).<br />

What makes listening difficult? Factors affecting second language listening comprehension.<br />

Technical Report TTO 81434 E.3.1, University of Maryland Center for Advanced Study of Language,<br />

College Park, MD.<br />

Boersma, P., Weenink, D. (2011).<br />

Praat: Doing phonetics by computer, Version 5.3. http://www.praat.org.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References II<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Bradlow, A. R., Pisoni, D. B. (1999).<br />

Recognition of spoken words by native and non-native listeners: Talker-, listener-, and item-related factors.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106, 2074–2085.<br />

Byrd, D. (1993).<br />

54,000 American stops.<br />

UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 83, 97–116.<br />

Cho, T., McQueen, J. M. (2006).<br />

Phonological versus phonetic cues in native and nonnative listening: Korean and Dutch listeners’ perception<br />

of Dutch and English consonants.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119, 3085–3096.<br />

Cutler, A. (2001).<br />

Listening to a second language through the ears of a first.<br />

Interpreting 5, 1–23.<br />

Cutler, A., Garcia Lecumberri, M. L., Cooke, M. (2008).<br />

Consonant identification in noise by native and non-native listeners: Effects of local context.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124, 1264–1268.<br />

Davidson, L. (2011).<br />

Characteristics of stop releases in American English spontaneous speech.<br />

Speech Communication 53, 1042–1058.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References III<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Davies, M. (2008-).<br />

The corpus of contemporary American English: 425 million words, 1990–present.<br />

Available online at http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/.<br />

Dixon, P. (2008).<br />

Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs.<br />

Journal of Memory and Language 59, 447–456.<br />

Flege, J. E. (1995).<br />

Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems.<br />

In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research, ed. W. Strange, pp.<br />

233–272. Baltimore, MD: York Press.<br />

Garcia Lecumberri, M. L., Cooke, M. (2006).<br />

Effect of masker type on native and non-native consonant perception in noise.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119, pp. 2445–2454.<br />

Goh, C. C. M. (2000).<br />

A cognitive perspective on language learners’ listening comprehension problems.<br />

System 28, 55–75.<br />

Goto, H. (1971).<br />

Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of the sounds “L” and “R”.<br />

Neuropsychologia 9, 317–323.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References IV<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Han, M. (1992).<br />

The timing control of geminate and single stop consonants in Japanese: A challenge for nonnative speakers.<br />

Phonetica 49, 102–127.<br />

Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesch, E., Tohkura, Y., Kettermann, A., Siebert, C. (2003).<br />

A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes.<br />

Cognition 87, B47–B57.<br />

Jaeger, T. F. (2008).<br />

Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models.<br />

Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434–446.<br />

Lado, R. (1957).<br />

<strong>Linguistics</strong> Across Cultures: Applied <strong>Linguistics</strong> for Language Teachers. Ann Arbor, MI: University of<br />

Michigan Press.<br />

Lisker, L. (1999).<br />

Perceiving final voiceless stops without release: Effects of preceding monophthongs versus<br />

nonmonophthongs.<br />

Phonetica 56, 44–55.<br />

Macmillan, N. A., Creelman, C. D. (2005).<br />

Detection Theory: A User’s Guide, 2nd edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References V<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Nábělek, A. K., Donahue, A. M. (1984).<br />

Perception of consonants in reverberation by native and non-native listeners.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 75, 632–634.<br />

Odlin, T. (1989).<br />

Language Transfer: Cross-Linguistic Influence in Language Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Sheldon, A., Strange, W. (1982).<br />

The acquisition of /r/ and /l/ by Japanese learners of English: Evidence that speech production can<br />

precede speech perception.<br />

Applied Psycholinguistics 3, 243–261.<br />

Sohn, H.-M. (1999).<br />

The Korean Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Sumner, M., Samuel, A. G. (2005).<br />

Perception and representation of regular variation: The case of final /t/.<br />

Journal of Memory and Language 52, 322–338.<br />

Tajima, K., Kato, H., Rothwell, A., Akahane-Yamada, R., Munhall, K. G. (2008).<br />

Training English listeners to perceive phonemic length contrasts in Japanese.<br />

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123, 397–413.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References VI<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Tsukada, K. (2006).<br />

Cross-language perception of word-final stops in Thai and English.<br />

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 309–318.<br />

Tsukada, K., Nguyen, T. T. A., Roengpitya, R., Ishihara, S. (2007).<br />

Cross-language perception of word-final stops: Comparison of Cantonese, Japanese, Korean and Vietnamese<br />

listeners.<br />

In Proceedings of the 16 th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, ed. J. Trouvain and W. J. Barry,<br />

pp. 1781–1784. Dudweiler, Germany: Pirrot.<br />

Tsukada, K., Roengpitya, R. (2008).<br />

Discrimination of English and Thai words ending with voiceless stops by native Thai listeners differing in<br />

English experience.<br />

Journal of the International Phonetic Association 38, 325–347.<br />

Wang, W. S. Y. (1959).<br />

Transition and release and perceptual cues for final plosives.<br />

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 2, 66–73.<br />

Yamada, R. A. (1995).<br />

Age and acquisition of second language speech sounds: Perception of American English /r/ and /l/ by<br />

native speakers of Japanese.<br />

In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research, ed. W. Strange, pp.<br />

305–320. Baltimore, MD: York Press.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing


References VII<br />

Yamada, R. A., Tohkura, Y. (1992).<br />

Appendix<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

References<br />

Perception of American English /r/ and /l/ by native speakers of Japanese.<br />

In Speech Perception, Production and Linguistic Structure, ed. Y. Tohkura, E. Vatikiotis-Bateson, and<br />

Y. Sagisaka, pp. 155–174. Tokyo, Japan: Ohmsha.<br />

Charles B. Chang, Alan Mishler Canonicity in Speech Processing

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!