20.07.2013 Views

Framing effects on evaluation of coupon offers: assessing the ...

Framing effects on evaluation of coupon offers: assessing the ...

Framing effects on evaluation of coupon offers: assessing the ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

making (Puto 1987; Qualls & Puto, 1989), d<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong>s to n<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>of</strong>it organizati<strong>on</strong>s (Smith &<br />

Berger, 1995), and new or innovative product adopti<strong>on</strong> (Grewal et al, 1994).<br />

The applicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> framing to discount promoti<strong>on</strong>s has been <strong>the</strong> focus <strong>of</strong> several studies<br />

(Berkowitz & Walt<strong>on</strong>, 1980; Das, 1992; Della Bitta et al, 1981; Del Veccio et al, 2007;<br />

Gendall et al, 2006; Gourville,1998; Kim & Kramer, 2006; Lichtenstein et al, 1991; Liefeld &<br />

Heslop, 1985; Ramanathan & Dhar, 2010), which addressed <strong>the</strong> presentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> savings <strong>of</strong><br />

promoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>of</strong>fers, also known as deal semantics. Coup<strong>on</strong> framing literature <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

hand appears quite scanty. Diam<strong>on</strong>d (1992) and Diam<strong>on</strong>d and Sanyal (1990) studied different<br />

coup<strong>on</strong> face value frames but in <strong>the</strong> former case <strong>on</strong>ly in an effort to discover <strong>the</strong> latent<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong> rules used and in <strong>the</strong> latter for a combined <strong>of</strong>fer between spaghetti sauce and rice<br />

soup. More recently, Kramer and Kim (2007) proved that processing fluency enhanced<br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong>s for gain-framed face values (d<strong>on</strong>’t pay 20 % <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> final price) and not novelty<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

The present study c<strong>on</strong>siders framing <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>of</strong> three comm<strong>on</strong> alternative m<strong>on</strong>etary face<br />

value frames, “€ <strong>of</strong>f”, “% <strong>of</strong>f” and “reduced price”. It is based but also c<strong>on</strong>siderably extends<br />

<strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> Chen, M<strong>on</strong>roe, and Lou (1998). These researchers relied <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> findings that<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers tend to process expected price reducti<strong>on</strong>s relatively ra<strong>the</strong>r than in absolute<br />

m<strong>on</strong>etary amounts (Grewal & Marmorstein, 1994) and that <strong>the</strong> psychological pleasure derived<br />

from a discount depends not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> absolute amount <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> m<strong>on</strong>etary savings but also <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> price level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> promoted product (Grewal et, 1998; Heath et al, 1995) and proposed that<br />

for high-price products c<strong>on</strong>sumers will perceive a price reducti<strong>on</strong> framed in m<strong>on</strong>etary terms<br />

as more significant than <strong>the</strong> same price reducti<strong>on</strong> framed in percentage terms and <strong>the</strong> exact<br />

opposite for a low price product. Their hypo<strong>the</strong>sis was supported <strong>on</strong>ly for <strong>the</strong> perceived<br />

savings and not for purchase intenti<strong>on</strong>, probably because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> small incentive (10%).<br />

Gendall et al (2006) reached similar c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s in a replicati<strong>on</strong> study. The present study<br />

attempts to resolve this issue, by testing this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis for a medium and for a high discount<br />

size, both capable to overcome <strong>the</strong> problem.<br />

A third frame, <strong>the</strong> “reduced price” <strong>on</strong>e, is also tested al<strong>on</strong>g its two o<strong>the</strong>r counterparts. This<br />

frame has a different logic since it stresses <strong>the</strong> m<strong>on</strong>ey left to pay for <strong>the</strong> product and not <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e saved. The c<strong>on</strong>sumer has to perform a mental accounting (Thaler, 1985) to realize <strong>the</strong><br />

savings, making unavoidably miscalculati<strong>on</strong>s, or use an assimilati<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>trast process<br />

(M<strong>on</strong>roe, 1979) and never calculate <strong>the</strong> savings. Ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se, will make this frame to<br />

underperform relative to <strong>the</strong> “€ <strong>of</strong>f” frame. Primary dependent variables <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> study are three<br />

interrelated antecedents <strong>of</strong> behavior (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975): percepti<strong>on</strong>s (transacti<strong>on</strong> value<br />

which is stipulated in terms <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> savings), deal evaluati<strong>on</strong>s (attitude towards <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong>fer) and redempti<strong>on</strong> intenti<strong>on</strong>. Thus we state <strong>the</strong> following hypo<strong>the</strong>sis:<br />

H1.: For high price products <strong>the</strong> ordering <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three face value frames according to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> transacti<strong>on</strong> value, attitude towards <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fer and redempti<strong>on</strong> intenti<strong>on</strong>, will be<br />

from <strong>the</strong> highest to <strong>the</strong> lowest: “€ <strong>of</strong>f”, “reduced price” and “% <strong>of</strong>f”. For <strong>the</strong> low price<br />

product <strong>the</strong> order will be: “% <strong>of</strong>f”, “€ <strong>of</strong>f” and “reduced price”.<br />

The discount level has also been found to significantly influence buying behavior (Gupta<br />

& Cooper, 1992). In additi<strong>on</strong>, if <strong>the</strong> discount level is maintained in a reas<strong>on</strong>ably high level it<br />

is also expected that it will not dilute <strong>the</strong> framing <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> because <strong>of</strong> low processing (Grewal<br />

et al, 1996) or credibility issues (Gupta & Cooper, 1992). Thus:<br />

H2.: The discount level will significantly influence all <strong>the</strong> dependent variables but it will<br />

not affect <strong>the</strong> observed framing <str<strong>on</strong>g>effects</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

The interrelati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g percepti<strong>on</strong>s, attitudes and intenti<strong>on</strong>s and <strong>the</strong>ir traits as<br />

antecedents <strong>of</strong> actual behavior are well established <strong>the</strong>oretically and validated empirically in<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumer research (Bagozzi, 1982; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). It is expected that <strong>the</strong> same<br />

pattern will appear in this coup<strong>on</strong> face value framing setting. More specifically:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!