24.07.2013 Views

Brand Halo: Understanding its Implications ... - ANZMAC

Brand Halo: Understanding its Implications ... - ANZMAC

Brand Halo: Understanding its Implications ... - ANZMAC

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Brand</strong> <strong>Halo</strong>: <strong>Understanding</strong> <strong>its</strong> <strong>Implications</strong>, Shortcomings and Directions for Choice<br />

Studies<br />

Flavio Freire Souza*. University of Technology, Sydney/ Centre for the Study of Choice.<br />

Flavio.freiresouza@student.uts.edu.au<br />

Paul Burke. University of Technology, Sydney/ Centre for the Study of Choice.<br />

Paul.burke@uts.edu.au<br />

Keywords: brand halo, halo effects, distortion of information, consumer choice.<br />

Abstract<br />

Humans bias their evaluations of specific personal tra<strong>its</strong> in line with an overall perception of<br />

the subject being assessed (Thorndike, 1920). This phenomenon, named <strong>Halo</strong> Effects,<br />

opposes the classic view in psychology regarding the sequence of attitude formation,<br />

contending that affect can influence beliefs. In a marketplace setting, halo effects lead to the<br />

proposal that a consumer’s valuation of specific product characteristics are inflated or deflated<br />

in a manner that echoes their attitude towards the brand. This distortion may induce violations<br />

in the economic assumption of rationality in decision-making. This study reviews brand halo<br />

effects in consumer choice and <strong>its</strong> measurement, proposing boundary conditions that may<br />

moderate this bias and affect modern market research techniques that rely on untangling a<br />

product’s utility. It also attempts to explain why the importance of accounting for individual<br />

perceptions about brands in developing predictive choice models is a worthwhile endeavour.<br />

Introduction<br />

<strong>Halo</strong> effects refer to a cognitive bias introduced in a judgement task, resulting from<br />

inaccuracy in judgement whereby an assessor’s overall evaluation of a subject taints their<br />

evaluation of specific individual aspects under scrutiny (Bagozzi, 1996; Nisbett & Wilson,<br />

1977; Russo, Meloy, & Medvec, 1998; Thorndike, 1920). From a psychology perspective,<br />

this phenomenon occurs as consumers rely on attitudes toward an object to construct beliefs<br />

regarding the evaluation of specific aspects of that object, which differs from the classical<br />

approach that assumes beliefs foster the creation of attitudes (Ajzen, 2005; Bagozzi, 1996).<br />

Under these set of assumptions, classic models would predict that an attitude would be<br />

formed towards an employee based on various cues such as performance indicators. In<br />

contrast, halo effects would suggest that if the employee was held in a favourable light (i.e.<br />

positive attitude), the judgement of the employee’s performance would be biased in a way<br />

that reinforces this liking.<br />

Marketing literature holds that consumers perceive brands as receptacles of information: they<br />

personify these brands (Aaker, 1997) and develop relationships with them (Fournier, 1998).<br />

Although there is consensus regarding the importance in defining a brand’s equity, great<br />

debate regarding <strong>its</strong> conceptualization and measurement is still existent. Different<br />

perspectives assign value to different aspects of this brand equity.<br />

One area of investigation has pursued how the valuation of brands by consumers is inherently<br />

related to understanding how they influence subsequent decision tasks. Choice studies<br />

indicate that brand equity enters a decision process at different stages, such as the definition<br />

of consideration sets (Erdem & Swait, 2004), sensitivity to product features and prices


(Erdem, Swait, & Louviere, 2002), introduction of brand associations and quality<br />

expectations (Van Osselaer & Alba, 2003), among others.<br />

A halo effect in consumer choice would, therefore, relate to the information distortion<br />

introduced in the judgment of alternatives (Russo et al., 1998); in the pursuit of reaching a<br />

final outcome, consumers involuntarily rely on their perceptions about brands, biasing a<br />

rational assessment of specific attributes. Beckwith and Lehmann (1975) point out that<br />

individuals who favour an alternative tend to rate it high on all desirable attributes, whereas,<br />

disliking that alternative influences in making specific attributes more negatively assessed,<br />

possibly in pursuit of evaluative consistency (Ajzen, 2005). <strong>Brand</strong> halo would be the<br />

component of <strong>Brand</strong> Equity that “blinds” consumers into assuming aspects of products of a<br />

specific brand are better/worse than others, in line with the global assessment of that brand.<br />

<strong>Implications</strong> of this phenomenon suggest that consumers are unable to rationally judge the<br />

alternatives presented, and interpretation, rather than the functional attribute, plays an ever<br />

important role in consumer judgment, contrary to what is expected from the rationaleconomic<br />

view. Managerially, this effect highlights the role brand equity exerts in addressing<br />

shortcomings of specific aspects of products.<br />

Additionally, when products are described by a myriad of specifications and consumers can<br />

only hope to grasp comprehension of a handful for undertaking a choice, a valuable brand<br />

impression would provide an advantageous starting ground, independently of the aspect being<br />

analysed. Russo and colleagues (Russo et al., 1998) point out the risk of consumers selecting<br />

an inferior alternative due to judgment bias introduced in the selection task.<br />

The objective of this paper is to develop a conceptual foundation across choice-related<br />

disciplines to investigate the role of brand halo in consumer choice. Defining this component<br />

and <strong>its</strong> moderating variables enables a deeper understanding of this phenomenon and<br />

facilitates further research into validating <strong>its</strong> adequacy to consumer choice studies, measuring<br />

<strong>its</strong> impact in a given choice task, and identifying <strong>its</strong> boundary conditions and constructs<br />

related. The development of this topic will take place in the following sections.<br />

<strong>Brand</strong> <strong>Halo</strong> and Choice<br />

Humans strive for mental coherence: a cognitive response to a state of psychological conflict.<br />

This pursuit is justified differently according to various streams of social psychology, ranging<br />

from conceptualizing it as an instrument to avoid cognitive dissonance to claims that it is an<br />

almost inevitable consequence of the way the human brain functions (Ajzen, 2005). Psychic<br />

tension is resolved by the readjustment of cognitive or affective aspects of an assessed<br />

element towards a coherent evaluative attitude behaviourally consistent (Ajzen, 2005).<br />

The classic approach in social psychology contends that different sources of information<br />

provide the input for the construction of personal beliefs, which in turn are concatenated to<br />

produce an attitude towards the elements assessed. In a marketplace, a decision-maker<br />

constructs beliefs regarding products, which provide a foundation for the construction of an<br />

overall attitude towards this alternative (Leuthesser, Kohli, & Harich, 1995).<br />

The classic perspective of attitude formation is consistent with economic studies and multiattribute<br />

attitudinal models: their premise is to decompose the value of a product into<br />

observable parts and measure how these specific aspects weight on the attitude regarding a<br />

product/ brand (Beckwith & Lehmann, 1975; Meyer, 1981). Random-Utility Models aim to


identify value based on a consumer’s judgment, assuming consumers are utility-maximisers<br />

and will base their evaluations on the precise, thorough and complete use of information. The<br />

value perceived for a focal brand, Qantas for example, may be derived from the assessment of<br />

attributes such as Fare, Food and Flight Frequency, represented by the A arrows in Figure 1.<br />

Fare<br />

Flight Frequency<br />

A<br />

A<br />

Food Qantas<br />

Fare<br />

Flight Frequency<br />

A<br />

D<br />

D<br />

D<br />

Figure 1 – Overview of <strong>Brand</strong> Attitude Formation<br />

The attitudinal response for Qantas, in this approach, is based on beliefs concerning the<br />

company’s attributes. A consumer relies on various sources of information to assess valued<br />

aspects of a brand/product, such as advertising, word-of-mouth, or even previous experiences<br />

and memories. A brand’s image construed by an individual, via mental associations of<br />

functional and perceived characteristics, is defined by the consumer behaviour literature as<br />

brand equity (Aaker, 1991; Keller, 1993). <strong>Brand</strong>s that detain a more favourable position in a<br />

consumer’s mind possess more equity, which translates into purchase decisions by facilitating<br />

choice processes (Erdem & Swait, 1998).<br />

A contrary, perhaps complementary, flow in the evaluation process then occurs. Termed halo<br />

effects by social psychologists, modifications in beliefs take place on the basis of attitudinal<br />

responses (Beckwith & Lehmann, 1975; Johansson, MacLachlan, & Yalch, 1976; Nisbett &<br />

Wilson, 1977; Thorndike, 1920). A few studies have extended this effect to the marketing<br />

literature, under the term brand halo (Bagozzi, 1996; Leuthesser et al., 1995; Russo et al.,<br />

1998). In this context, the phenomenon would occur when the brand equity construed reenters<br />

the evaluation process retroactively, as portrayed in Figure 1 by the arrow labelled B.<br />

To maintain coherence in their assessment, a consumer may introduce inferences regarding<br />

already valued attributes, or even shift importance to accommodate attitudinal assessments,<br />

either to previously valued or possibly unvalued attributes hitherto, in a pursuit for coherence<br />

towards the evaluation of the favoured brand. This would generate an inflation/ deflation of<br />

beliefs in alignment with the individual’s attitude (Ajzen, 2005). In this sense, a consumer<br />

who favours Qantas would justify higher fares by assuming the catering of better tasting<br />

meals, reassessing the importance of the considered attributes, or introduce assumptions that<br />

were outside the scope of the evaluation such as better in-flight entertainment and more<br />

comfortable seating.<br />

B<br />

C<br />

Food JetStar


The phenomenon has further implications as brands are compared to one another. In Figure 1,<br />

the arrow labelled C represents the process that occurs as a new brand enters a consideration<br />

set. At this stage, in a pursuit to maintain coherence in judgement, consumers who favour<br />

Qantas, or simply hold it as a status quo in their decision-making, will modify their<br />

perceptions of attributes to fit their evaluation against a competing company. In the airflight<br />

example, a consumer might shift importance to Qantas’ Flight Frequency, which had not been<br />

prioritized in the decision task until now, and/or construct negative inferences regarding<br />

JetStar’s Flight Frequency, one of the D arrows in Figure 1. Erdem, Swait and Louviere<br />

(2002) show that consumers have different price sensitivities for products with distinct brands<br />

despite their equivalent configurations, which could be a function of different consumer brand<br />

valuation <strong>its</strong>elf, as well as a result of different expected utilities from specific aspects of<br />

products from different brands.<br />

This same process would be complemented by the opposite effects, with JetStar as the focal<br />

brand of analysis. Hence, beliefs regarding attributes and attribute levels of the product<br />

category would mould the attitude towards the brand JetStar. This brand attitude would<br />

feedback into the evaluation process both in regards to <strong>its</strong> own attribute beliefs, but also to the<br />

beliefs of additional brands in the consideration set, in this example Qantas.<br />

Measurement of <strong>Brand</strong> <strong>Halo</strong><br />

The extension of halo effects to brands is a not new concept, with initial steps in the literature<br />

circa 1970s (Bass & Talarzyk, 1972; Bass & Wilkie, 1973; Beckwith & Lehmann, 1975,<br />

1976; Huber & James, 1978; Johansson et al., 1976; Wilkie, McCann, & Reibstein, 1974).<br />

Albeit these studies stressed the value in comprehending this phenomenon with multiattribute<br />

attitudinal models, the operationalization of these proved a more complicated<br />

endeavour, especially in regards to the delineation of this brand halo in a choice context.<br />

Since <strong>its</strong> identification, halo effects have been verified by observance of inter-attribute<br />

correlations of rating data, where factor analysis is used to identify highly related attributes,<br />

characterized by having 0.6-0.7 as a rule of thumb for defining high level of bias in judgement<br />

(Leuthesser et al., 1995; Thorndike, 1920). Wu and Petroshius (1987) mention that the<br />

effectiveness of this alternative rating procedures as a halo reduction tool is elusive, which<br />

receives support from Bagozzi (1996), who distinguishes affective-ignited brand halo from<br />

simple inter-attribute correlations because the latter are an empirical observation that cannot<br />

be objectively be ascertained as a result of the former.<br />

A more refined approach was presented by Russo, Meloy and Medvec (1998), with<br />

experiments designed to investigate the distortion of information in consumer judgment. In<br />

this study, one group received a favourable irrelevant additional piece of information,<br />

prompting positive attitude towards a specific fictitious brand, whereas for another condition<br />

different brands were presented at each choice set, eliminating the possibility of formation of<br />

brand-specific affective response (Russo et al., 1998). Russo, Meloy and Medvec (1998)<br />

stress that understanding distortion of information is particularly important for studies that<br />

rely on conjoint measurement techniques: the increased use of discrete choice experiments<br />

and econometric modelling techniques in the marketing discipline begs for a refinement on<br />

how this phenomenon can be addressed for a utility-model implementation.


Research Propositions<br />

Identifying sources of halo in choice may have implications in creating more reliable<br />

attitudinal models via the manipulation of distortions. This section highlights variables that<br />

can alleviate the introduction of brand halo in judgment.<br />

Firstly, Nisbett and Wilson’s (1977) studies report that assessors claimed not to have been<br />

influenced by halo effects, despite their results indicating high correlational levels. If interattribute<br />

correlations and real halo have some common ground, this implies that halo<br />

distortion is involuntary. Additionally, Bagozzi (1996) reports that both positive and negative<br />

beliefs could be influenced by emotional arousal in the context of blood donation. Despite<br />

contextual limitations, Bagozzi’s (1996) results seem to indicate that prompting consumers’<br />

awareness of halo effects could incline them to make more cognitive-driven assessments.<br />

That is, if consumers were conscious of the potential halo effect-type bias and <strong>its</strong><br />

consequences, it is likely that decision makers will attempt to minimise <strong>its</strong> practice. This<br />

leads us to our first research proposition.<br />

P1: Prompting raters’ awareness of own halo effects will reduce brand halo in choice.<br />

Providing accurate information to respondents regarding diagnosticity of attributes assessed<br />

should reduce halo effects (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Van Osselaer & Alba, 2003). Russo et al.<br />

(1998) selected attributes purposefully with low diagnosticity to prompt reliance on brand in<br />

the choice task. Moreover, Leuthesser et al. (1995) and Russo et al. (1998) highlight the<br />

importance of attribute specification for understanding halo effects, although manipulation<br />

regarding diagnosticity of information showed that halo effects were still present in spite of<br />

highly diagnostic information. Leuthesser et al. (1995) further points out that halo may be<br />

induced in a task if attributes are few or lack concreteness. This motivates the proposition of<br />

the following hypothesis.<br />

P2: Experts will be less prone to brand halo in choice than novices.<br />

Consumers seem to modify their judgments in line with the confidence in a choice (Russo et<br />

al., 1998). This finding supports brand signalling theory such that familiarity with a choice<br />

context motivates consumers to rely on the signals emitted by credible brands (Erdem &<br />

Swait, 1998; Swait & Erdem, 2007). If this is the case, more brand halo is expected when<br />

consumers have familiarity with brands presented on the basis of preestablished decisionsetting<br />

preferences. This leads to the third proposition.<br />

P3: Confidence in decision setting increases brand halo.<br />

Final considerations<br />

The extension of the halo effects literature seems to complement some of the findings in the<br />

consumer behaviour literature. It provides support to the idea that consumers have different<br />

price sensitivities to products described by the equivalent aspects (Swait, Erdem, Louviere, &<br />

Dubelaar, 1993), in a manner consistent with compensatory inferences (Chernev, 2007;<br />

Chernev & Carpenter, 2001; Chernev & Hamilton, 2009), a strategy employed by consumers<br />

to justify the price/ benefit relationship expected from different products. This paper<br />

consolidates the literature on brand halo through a literature review in economics, marketing<br />

and psychology, laying foundations to the conceptual development of the phenomenon.<br />

Current measurement techniques and their shortcomings were also debated. Finally, research


propositions were elaborated, highlighting variables to be considered in the studying of this<br />

phenomenon.<br />

References<br />

Aaker, D. A. (1991). Managing brand equity: Capitalizing on the value of a brand name:<br />

Free Pr.<br />

Aaker, J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 347-<br />

356.<br />

Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes, personality and behavior: Open Univ Pr.<br />

Bagozzi, R. P. (1996). The role of arousal in the creation and control of the halo effect in<br />

attitude models. Psychology and Marketing, 13(3), 235-264.<br />

Bass, F. M., & Talarzyk, W. W. (1972). An attitude model for the study of brand preference.<br />

Journal of Marketing Research, 9(1), 93-96.<br />

Bass, F. M., & Wilkie, W. L. (1973). A comparative analysis of attitudinal predictions of<br />

brand preference. Journal of Marketing Research, 262-269.<br />

Beckwith, N. E., & Lehmann, D. R. (1975). The importance of halo effects in multi-attribute<br />

attitude models. Journal of Marketing Research, 265-275.<br />

Beckwith, N. E., & Lehmann, D. R. (1976). <strong>Halo</strong> effects in multiattribute attitude models: An<br />

appraisal of some unresolved issues. Journal of Marketing Research, 418-421.<br />

Chernev, A. (2007). Jack of all trades or master of one? Product differentiation and<br />

compensatory reasoning in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4),<br />

430-444.<br />

Chernev, A., & Carpenter, G. S. (2001). The role of market efficiency intuitions in consumer<br />

choice: A case of compensatory inferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(3),<br />

349-361.<br />

Chernev, A., & Hamilton, R. (2009). Compensatory reasoning in choice The social<br />

psychology of consumer behavior (pp. 131-147).<br />

Erdem, & Swait, J. (1998). <strong>Brand</strong> equity as a signaling phenomenon. Journal of Consumer<br />

Psychology, 7(2), 131-157.<br />

Erdem, Swait, J., & Louviere, J. (2002). The impact of brand credibility on consumer price<br />

sensitivity. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19(1), 1-19.<br />

Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (2004). <strong>Brand</strong> credibility, brand consideration, and choice. Journal of<br />

Consumer Research, 191-198.<br />

Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: Developing relationship theory in consumer<br />

research. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 343-353.<br />

Huber, J., & James, W. (1978). A measure of halo. Advances in Consumer Research, 5(1),<br />

468-473.<br />

Johansson, J. K., MacLachlan, D. L., & Yalch, R. F. (1976). <strong>Halo</strong> effects in multiattribute<br />

attitude models: some unresolved issues. Journal of Marketing Research, 414-417.<br />

Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand<br />

equity. The Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1-22.<br />

Leuthesser, L., Kohli, C. S., & Harich, K. R. (1995). <strong>Brand</strong> equity: the halo effect measure.<br />

European Journal of Marketing, 29(4), 57-66.<br />

Meyer, R. J. (1981). A model of multiattribute judgments under attribute uncertainty and<br />

informational constraint. Journal of Marketing Research, 428-441.<br />

Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration<br />

of judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(4), 250.<br />

Russo, J. E., Meloy, M. G., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). Predecisional distortion of product<br />

information. Journal of Marketing Research, 438-452.


Swait, J., & Erdem, T. (2007). <strong>Brand</strong> effects on choice and choice set formation under<br />

uncertainty. Marketing science, 26(5), 679.<br />

Swait, J., Erdem, T., Louviere, J., & Dubelaar, C. (1993). The equalization price: a measure<br />

of consumer-perceived brand equity. International Journal of Research in Marketing,<br />

10(1), 23-45.<br />

Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological ratings. Journal of Applied<br />

Psychology, 4(1), 25-29.<br />

Van Osselaer, S., & Alba, J. (2003). Locus of equity and brand extension. Journal of<br />

Consumer Research, 539-550.<br />

Wilkie, W. L., McCann, J., & Reibstein, D. (1974). <strong>Halo</strong> effects in brand belief measurement:<br />

<strong>Implications</strong> for attitude model development. Advances in Consumer Research, 1,<br />

280-290.<br />

Wu, B. T. W., & Petroshius, S. M. (1987). The halo effect in store image measurement.<br />

Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 15(3), 44-51.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!