05.08.2013 Views

Stability Across Cohorts in Divorce Risk Factors - Bishop Ireton High ...

Stability Across Cohorts in Divorce Risk Factors - Bishop Ireton High ...

Stability Across Cohorts in Divorce Risk Factors - Bishop Ireton High ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 331<br />

STABILITY ACROSS COHORTS IN DIVORCE RISK<br />

FACTORS*<br />

T<br />

JAY D. TEACHMAN<br />

Over the past quarter-century, many covariates of divorce have been identified. However, the<br />

extent to which the effects of these covariates rema<strong>in</strong> constant across time is not known. In this<br />

article, I exam<strong>in</strong>e the stability of the effects of a wide range of divorce covariates us<strong>in</strong>g a pooled<br />

sample of data taken from five rounds of the National Survey of Family Growth. This sample <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

consistent measures of important predictors of divorce, covers marriages formed over 35<br />

years (1950–1984), and spans substantial historical variation <strong>in</strong> the overall risk of marital dissolution.<br />

For the most part, the effects of the major sociodemographic predictors of divorce do not vary<br />

by historical period. The one exception is race. These results suggest that the effects associated with<br />

historical period have been pervasive, simultaneously alter<strong>in</strong>g the risk of divorce for most marriages.<br />

he <strong>in</strong>creased prevalence of divorce, comb<strong>in</strong>ed with its well-known consequences for<br />

the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of men, women, and children (Amato 2000; Cherl<strong>in</strong>, Chase-Lansdale, and<br />

McRae 1998; McLanahan and Sandefur 1994), has spurred efforts to understand its determ<strong>in</strong>ants.<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g various methods and data from different historical periods, researchers<br />

have l<strong>in</strong>ked numerous social, demographic, and economic variables to divorce. DaVanzo<br />

and Rahman (1993), Faust and McKibben (1999), and White (1990) reviewed this extensive<br />

literature. The list of variables considered is long and the results have varied, but a<br />

small number of sociodemographic variables have been l<strong>in</strong>ked consistently to the risk of<br />

divorce. These variables <strong>in</strong>clude age at marriage, education, premarital births and conception,<br />

religion, parental divorce, and race.<br />

Unfortunately, little research has attempted to ascerta<strong>in</strong> whether the determ<strong>in</strong>ants of<br />

divorce are <strong>in</strong>variant across historical time even though there are reasonable theoretical,<br />

albeit slimmer empirical, grounds for expect<strong>in</strong>g the effects of various predictors of divorce<br />

to vary across time. In my study, I tested the equality of effects across time us<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

pooled sample of data taken from five rounds of the National Survey of Family Growth<br />

(NSFG); the NSFG <strong>in</strong>cluded consistent measures of important predictors of divorce, covered<br />

marriages formed over 35 years (1950–1984), and spanned substantial historical<br />

variation <strong>in</strong> the overall risk of marital dissolution. With the exception of race, I found that<br />

the effects of the major sociodemographic predictors of divorce do not vary across time.<br />

These results <strong>in</strong>dicate that the effects of historical period have been pervasive, at least<br />

over the period covered by this research.<br />

WHY SHOULD EFFECTS VARY OVER HISTORICAL TIME?<br />

There is no generally accepted theory of divorce, although the predom<strong>in</strong>ant perspectives<br />

all rely on some notion of the exchange of expressive and <strong>in</strong>strumental goods and services<br />

between husbands and wives (Becker 1991; Becker, Landes, and Michael 1977;<br />

Oppenheimer 1994, 1997; Ruggles 1997). The assumption is that marriage is beneficial<br />

because mutual <strong>in</strong>terdependence generated by exchange <strong>in</strong>creases the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of each<br />

*Jay D. Teachman, Department of Sociology, Western Wash<strong>in</strong>gton University, Bell<strong>in</strong>gham, WA 98225-<br />

9081; E-mail: teachman@cc.wwu.edu. I thank Kyle Crowder, Mick Cunn<strong>in</strong>gham, and the anonymous reviewers<br />

for their many helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article.<br />

Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002: 331–351 331


332 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

spouse beyond what would be achieved if they were not married. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, anyth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that dim<strong>in</strong>ishes the real or perceived ga<strong>in</strong>s to marriage constitutes a risk factor for marital<br />

disruption.<br />

In the face of chang<strong>in</strong>g rates of divorce, to assume that the determ<strong>in</strong>ants of divorce<br />

have rema<strong>in</strong>ed constant over time is to assume that changes <strong>in</strong> the real or perceived ga<strong>in</strong>s<br />

to marriage have occurred uniformly across all marriages. This is a strong assumption to<br />

make without the support of substantial prior evidence, and there are reasonable theoretical<br />

arguments that lead to the expectation that the effects of some predictors of divorce<br />

will vary across time. I outl<strong>in</strong>e a few of these arguments next.<br />

First, consider the possibility that <strong>in</strong>dividuals with similar characteristics make different<br />

decisions about divorce because they react differently to changes across time <strong>in</strong> the<br />

nature of the exchange between husbands and wives. For example, consider the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

changes <strong>in</strong> the context surround<strong>in</strong>g marriage. Over the past half-century, attitudes toward<br />

divorce and nonmarital liv<strong>in</strong>g have become dramatically more forgiv<strong>in</strong>g (Cherl<strong>in</strong> 1992;<br />

Thornton 1989), and opportunities to form <strong>in</strong>timate unions and raise children outside marriage<br />

have expanded tremendously (Bumpass and Sweet 1989; Bumpass, Sweet, and<br />

Cherl<strong>in</strong> 1991; Mann<strong>in</strong>g and Smock 1995; McLanahan and Sandefur 1994; Morgan and<br />

R<strong>in</strong>dfuss 1999; South 1999). In addition, <strong>in</strong>creased equality <strong>in</strong> gender roles and <strong>in</strong> the<br />

structure of the economy have weakened the traditional economic <strong>in</strong>terdependence of men<br />

and women (Bianchi 1995; Oppenheimer 1994, 1997; Ruggles 1997). Thus, concurrent<br />

with <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> alternatives to marriage, there has been a decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> the mutual dependence<br />

between husbands and wives on the basis of a traditional division of labor between<br />

market and nonmarket activities.<br />

However, these changes have not affected all marriages uniformly. For example, South<br />

(2001) suggested that <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>in</strong>stitutional supports for unmarried mothers, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

with more liberal gender-role attitudes, have made it easier for employed wives to use<br />

their economic resources to leave unsatisfactory unions. South also noted that decreased<br />

occupational sex segregation has <strong>in</strong>creased the opportunity for employed married women<br />

to meet men who may be better matches than their current partners. The result of these<br />

changes should be an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly positive effect of married women’s employment on<br />

divorce, an expectation consistent with South’s empirical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

The second possible l<strong>in</strong>k between changes <strong>in</strong> the context of marriage to the risk of<br />

divorce is even more straightforward. When there are few options to marriage and divorce<br />

is uncommon, the effects of any traits that are positively l<strong>in</strong>ked to the risk of divorce will<br />

be suppressed. However, as the barriers to divorce wane and alternatives to marriage become<br />

more common, the effects of these traits may beg<strong>in</strong> to be expressed <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

risk of marital disruption. Some evidence for this process has come from behavioral geneticists,<br />

who have found little to no evidence for the heritability of divorce when rates of<br />

divorce are low (Turkheimer et al. 1992) but substantial evidence for heritability when<br />

divorce is more common (Jock<strong>in</strong>, McGue, and Lykken 1996; McGue and Lykken 1992).<br />

Behavioral geneticists argue that heritability operates through the <strong>in</strong>heritance of personality<br />

traits that <strong>in</strong>crease the risk of marital dissolution. Indeed, a number of personality<br />

traits have been l<strong>in</strong>ked to the risk of divorce (Eysenck 1980; Jock<strong>in</strong> et al. 1996; Johnson<br />

and Harris 1980; McGue and Lykken 1992; Rockwell, Elder, and Ross 1979). It is not<br />

illogical to expect that at least some of the common demographic predictors of divorce are<br />

l<strong>in</strong>ked to different personality traits. For example, Kiernan (1986), us<strong>in</strong>g data from Brita<strong>in</strong>,<br />

found that women who marry younger exhibit higher levels of neuroticism, and higher<br />

levels of neuroticism are l<strong>in</strong>ked to an <strong>in</strong>creased risk of divorce. A variety of research has<br />

l<strong>in</strong>ked other measures of social psychological function<strong>in</strong>g, such as self-esteem, to sociodemographic<br />

characteristics like early and premarital fertility (Coley and Chase-Lansdale<br />

1998; Plotnick 1992; South 1999) and premarital cohabitation (Ax<strong>in</strong>n and Thornton 1992;<br />

Clarkberg, Stolzenberg, and Waite 1995; Thomson and Colella 1992).


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 333<br />

A third alternative may be that changes <strong>in</strong> the context of marriage have not occurred<br />

uniformly for all Americans. For example, over the past 40 years, blacks have been more<br />

likely than whites to delay marriage, to experience premarital fertility, and to cohabit<br />

outside marriage (Ellwood and Crane 1990; Teachman 2000). To some extent, these differences<br />

have emerged because the marriage markets <strong>in</strong> which blacks and whites operate are<br />

substantially different (Lichter, LeClere, and McLaughl<strong>in</strong> 1991; Lichter et al. 1992) and<br />

have diverged over time (McLanahan and Casper 1995), with blacks <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly subject to<br />

conditions less favorable to the formation and ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of marriages. One consequence<br />

of these different rates of change may be that, relative to whites, blacks who marry are an<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly select group who are relatively more committed to the <strong>in</strong>stitution of marriage.<br />

This result suggests a slower <strong>in</strong>crease over time <strong>in</strong> rates of divorce for blacks than for whites,<br />

an expectation that is consistent with f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs presented by Sweet and Bumpass (1987).<br />

PREVIOUS RESEARCH<br />

Unfortunately, much previous research has been constra<strong>in</strong>ed by the use of data, such as<br />

the National Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Survey of Youth or the National Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Study of the <strong>High</strong><br />

School Class of 1972, that have provided limited <strong>in</strong>formation about divorce across historical<br />

time (Becker et al. 1977; Booth and Edwards 1985; Bumpass and Sweet 1972;<br />

South and Lloyd 1995; South and Spitze 1986; Teachman 1986; Teachman and Polonko<br />

1990). In other <strong>in</strong>stances, when data, such as the Current Population Survey, conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation on divorce across a broader range of historical time have been utilized, it has<br />

been common for researchers to account for historical shifts <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce by<br />

simply add<strong>in</strong>g a control for marriage cohort (or some other <strong>in</strong>dicator of historical time) <strong>in</strong><br />

their models (Bumpass, Mart<strong>in</strong>, and Sweet 1991; Heaton 1991; Lehrer and Chiswick 1993;<br />

Waite and Lillard 1991). The implicit assumption has been that any historical change <strong>in</strong><br />

the risk of divorce affects all marriages equally.<br />

Carlson and St<strong>in</strong>son (1982) conducted one of the first studies to f<strong>in</strong>d that the effects of<br />

one or more predictors of divorce may not be stable across time. They found that the<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> marital dissolution across time was the greatest among women who married as<br />

teenagers. However, three other studies (Mart<strong>in</strong> and Bumpass 1989; Morgan and R<strong>in</strong>dfuss<br />

1985; Thornton and Rodgers 1987) have failed to replicate this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Mart<strong>in</strong> and Bumpass (1989) reported an <strong>in</strong>teraction between marriage cohort and both<br />

education and premarital fertility. Specifically, women with more education were <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly<br />

less likely to divorce, and women with children who were born before marriage were<br />

first less and then more likely to divorce. Other research has found some evidence to<br />

suggest that the effects of premarital cohabitation (Schoen 1992), parental divorce<br />

(Wolf<strong>in</strong>ger 1999), and race (Sweet and Bumpass 1987) have effects that vary across time.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong> the most recent effort to identify effects that vary across time, South (2001)<br />

found that the impact of wives’ employment on divorce has <strong>in</strong>creased over the past 25<br />

years (although the effect of wives’ education has not changed).<br />

Overall, the empirical evidence is limited but suggests that some covariates may have<br />

effects on divorce that vary over time. The number of studies that have explicitly considered<br />

this possibility rema<strong>in</strong>s small, however. Moreover, these studies have tended to focus<br />

on a s<strong>in</strong>gle covariate of divorce and generally have been restricted to a consideration of<br />

marriages that were formed after 1959. I extend previous research by consider<strong>in</strong>g both a<br />

wider range of time and a wide range of covariates.<br />

DATA AND METHODS<br />

Data<br />

The data for this study were taken from the five rounds of the NSFG, conducted <strong>in</strong> 1973,<br />

1976, 1982, 1988, and 1995 (National Center for Health Statistics 1998). The first two


334 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

rounds of the NSFG were nationally representative samples of women aged 15–44 who<br />

were ever married or who had a child of their own liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the household. The last three<br />

rounds were nationally representative samples of all women aged 15–44. The sample sizes<br />

were 9,797 for round 1; 8,611 for round 2; 7,969 for round 3; 8,450 for round 4; and<br />

10,847 for round 5.<br />

Each round of the NSFG obta<strong>in</strong>ed retrospective <strong>in</strong>formation about the marital history<br />

of all the respondents. I used this <strong>in</strong>formation to calculate the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g and end<strong>in</strong>g dates<br />

of all first marriages. I selected all first marriages that were formed between 1950 and<br />

1984, yield<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation on divorces that occurred between 1950 and 1995. Over this<br />

period, there were substantial shifts <strong>in</strong> age at marriage, marital dissolution, education,<br />

out-of-wedlock childbear<strong>in</strong>g, premarital cohabitation, and divorce.<br />

Marriages formed before 1950 were excluded because they <strong>in</strong>cluded only women who<br />

married prior to age 21. I excluded marriages formed after 1984 to obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation on<br />

at least 10 years of potential exposure to the risk of divorce for all marriages. Of course,<br />

earlier marriage cohorts have much longer potential marital durations. In results not reported<br />

here, however, my conclusions were not affected by the arbitrary truncation of all<br />

marriages after 10 years of exposure to the risk of divorce. 1 After I selected first marriages<br />

formed between 1950 and 1984, there were 27,296 marriages (7,611 divorces)<br />

available for analysis.<br />

Each round of the NSFG collected retrospective marital histories <strong>in</strong> a consistent fashion,<br />

limit<strong>in</strong>g the possibility that changes <strong>in</strong> the risk of marital dissolution across time are<br />

artifacts of the survey design. It is possible, though, that some historical differences <strong>in</strong><br />

marital dissolution are due to differences <strong>in</strong> samples and collection procedures. To m<strong>in</strong>imize<br />

this possibility, I constructed a set of dummy variables <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the survey round<br />

from which each respondent’s data were taken. I estimated all models with these dummy<br />

variables <strong>in</strong>cluded to provide protection aga<strong>in</strong>st survey-specific effects.<br />

Each round of the NSFG also collected different types of background <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

that can be used to predict divorce. However, there is a set of basic sociodemographic<br />

predictors available <strong>in</strong> all five rounds. These predictors are all fixed at the time of marriage<br />

and <strong>in</strong>clude wife’s age at marriage, husband’s age at marriage, education of the<br />

wife, education of the husband, premarital fertility status, parental divorce status, religion,<br />

and race. 2 From this set of variables, it is also possible to calculate estimates of the<br />

age homogamy and educational homogamy of the spouses. Although this is a somewhat<br />

limited set of predictor variables, it represents a broader array of covariates that could<br />

<strong>in</strong>teract with historical time than has hitherto been considered.<br />

To simplify the analysis, I did not consider time-vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation on marital births.<br />

Although marital fertility has been l<strong>in</strong>ked to the risk of divorce (Morgan and R<strong>in</strong>dfuss<br />

1985; South and Spitze 1986; Waite and Lillard 1991), childbear<strong>in</strong>g decisions are not<br />

fixed at the time of marriage. Because children can be born at any marital duration and<br />

their effects on marital dissolution vary accord<strong>in</strong>g to their age and number (Waite and<br />

Lillard 1991), isolat<strong>in</strong>g the effects of marital childbear<strong>in</strong>g as they vary accord<strong>in</strong>g to marital<br />

duration and historical period is not straightforward. It rema<strong>in</strong>s the task of a subsequent<br />

research project to tackle this problem.<br />

1. I truncated all marriages at 10 years’ duration by cod<strong>in</strong>g marriages of longer duration as be<strong>in</strong>g censored<br />

at that po<strong>in</strong>t. I then reestimated each of the event-history models described later, and found no substantive<br />

differences.<br />

2. In most cases, these predictor variables were measured consistently across rounds of the NSFG. However,<br />

there are a few differences <strong>in</strong> measurement that should be noted. Parental divorce status was measured at<br />

age 14 for the respondent <strong>in</strong> the first four rounds (Was the respondent not liv<strong>in</strong>g with both biological parents<br />

because of divorce?) but us<strong>in</strong>g a full parent history <strong>in</strong> round 5 (I used this parent history to code whether the<br />

respondent was not liv<strong>in</strong>g with both parents at age 14 because of divorce). Wife’s education was measured at the<br />

time of marriage <strong>in</strong> rounds 1, 2, and 4 and the highest level atta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> rounds 3 and 5. Husband’s education was<br />

measured at marriage <strong>in</strong> all rounds except round 3, when it was measured as the highest level obta<strong>in</strong>ed.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 335<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, rounds 4 and 5 of the NSFG conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation on premarital cohabitation<br />

status. This <strong>in</strong>formation allows a more limited test of the possibility that the effect of<br />

premarital cohabitation has changed across time us<strong>in</strong>g marriages formed after 1969. The<br />

availability of this <strong>in</strong>formation also allows a consideration of the possibility that the effects<br />

of the other predictor variables are confounded with premarital cohabitation.<br />

Methods<br />

I exam<strong>in</strong>ed the effects of the measured covariates on divorce us<strong>in</strong>g a Cox proportional<br />

hazards model (Cox 1972). Completed spells of marriage were measured by the duration<br />

<strong>in</strong> months between the date of marriage and the date of separation. Censored spells were<br />

measured <strong>in</strong> months between the date of marriage and the date of the survey or between<br />

the date of marriage and the date of death of the spouse for the small number of widowed<br />

women <strong>in</strong> the NSFG. The Cox model takes the follow<strong>in</strong>g form:<br />

h(t) = λ 0(t)exp(Xβ),<br />

where h(t) is the hazard of divorce at marital duration t, λ 0(t) is an unspecified basel<strong>in</strong>e<br />

hazard rate at marital duration t that is shifted upward or downward by the vector of<br />

measured covariates X <strong>in</strong> amounts equivalent to exp(β). The Cox model is easy to fit to<br />

the data and requires no assumptions about the shape of the hazard rate as it varies accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to marital duration.<br />

To measure the effects of historical context, I <strong>in</strong>cluded an <strong>in</strong>dicator of the year <strong>in</strong><br />

which each respondent was first married. Without further consideration, this measure<br />

may be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as the effect of either the marriage cohort or the historical period.<br />

Either <strong>in</strong>terpretation is possible because the historical period is equal to the effect of the<br />

marriage cohort (the year <strong>in</strong> which the respondent was first married) plus marital duration,<br />

and, as estimated, the Cox model allows the risk of divorce to vary simultaneously<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the year of marriage and marital duration. As described by Allison (1995:<br />

142–43), the effect of historical time is perfectly captured by the coefficient for year<br />

married.<br />

The question becomes: should the effect of time, as <strong>in</strong>dicated by the coefficient for<br />

year married, be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as the effect of historical period or marriage cohort? On the<br />

basis of the results provided by Thornton and Rodgers (1987), I <strong>in</strong>terpreted the coefficient<br />

for year of marriage <strong>in</strong> terms of the effect of historical period and assumed that the<br />

effects of cohort (either marriage cohort or birth cohort) on divorce are m<strong>in</strong>imal. I conducted<br />

several tests to ascerta<strong>in</strong> whether this was a reasonable assumption and found no<br />

evidence to suggest that it was not. 3<br />

3. First, follow<strong>in</strong>g Thornton and Rodgers (1987), I plotted the risk of marital dissolution for two marital<br />

durations (the first 18 months of marriage and months 54–72) across marriage cohorts. If marriage cohort and<br />

marital duration act additively to <strong>in</strong>fluence the risk of marital dissolution (i.e., there is no effect of historical<br />

period), the ratio of the risks at the two marital durations should be constant across marriage cohorts. That is not<br />

the pattern that was observed (results not shown). Rather, the ratio of the two risks <strong>in</strong>dicated first divergence and<br />

then convergence. Without consider<strong>in</strong>g an effect of historical period, the only way that this pattern could be<br />

observed would be if one assumed a strong <strong>in</strong>teraction between marriage cohort and marital duration. Second,<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g the work of Ono (1999), I estimated a model with effects for year of marriage (to measure historical<br />

period) plus an <strong>in</strong>dicator specific to marriage cohort (average age at first marriage for wives for the year <strong>in</strong><br />

which the respondent was married). Contrary to Ono’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, this model did not <strong>in</strong>dicate an <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

effect of marriage cohort. F<strong>in</strong>ally, I estimated a model that <strong>in</strong>cluded effects for year of marriage (historical<br />

period) plus an <strong>in</strong>dicator specific to birth cohort (gross domestic product for the year <strong>in</strong> which the respondent<br />

was born). This model failed to provide any evidence for the effects of birth cohort. Of course, these results rely<br />

on the choice of <strong>in</strong>dicators for either marriage cohort or birth cohort. Different results could hold if alternative<br />

measures of these cohort effects were constructed.


336 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

DESCRIPTIVE RESULTS<br />

Age at marriage for both husbands and wives was coded <strong>in</strong> years; education was coded <strong>in</strong><br />

years of completed school<strong>in</strong>g; premarital birth was coded 1 for women who entered the<br />

first marriage with a birth, 0 otherwise; premarital conception was coded 1 for women<br />

who gave birth to a child with<strong>in</strong> the first seven months of marriage, 0 otherwise; wife<br />

older was coded 1 when the woman was two or more years older than her husband, 0<br />

otherwise; husband older was coded 1 when the husband was five or more years older<br />

than his wife, 0 otherwise; wife has more education was coded 1 if the wife had at least a<br />

high school degree and at least two years of school<strong>in</strong>g more than her husband, 0 otherwise;<br />

parents divorced was coded 1 if the woman was not liv<strong>in</strong>g with both biological<br />

parents at age 14 because of divorce, 0 otherwise; Catholic was coded 1 for women who<br />

reported be<strong>in</strong>g Catholic, 0 otherwise; black was coded 1 for women who reported they<br />

are black, 0 otherwise; and other race was coded 1 for women who did not report their<br />

race as either white or black.<br />

Each of these predictor variables has received considerable attention <strong>in</strong> the literature<br />

(see the reviews <strong>in</strong> DaVanzo and Rahman 1993; Faust and McKibben 1999; Teachman,<br />

Tedrow, and Crowder 2000; White 1990), and I did not develop separate rationales for<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong> the analysis. In general, it has been found that the risk of divorce decl<strong>in</strong>es<br />

with <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> age at marriage and education. <strong>Divorce</strong> is also less likely among<br />

Catholics and whites, <strong>in</strong> age- and educationally homogeneous marriages, and <strong>in</strong> marriages<br />

not preceded by a birth or conception or a parental divorce.<br />

Although the sample is large, the number of divorces that occurred for women who<br />

were married <strong>in</strong> a given year was small. Thus, the number of divorces accord<strong>in</strong>g to a<br />

s<strong>in</strong>gle year of marriage showed substantial random fluctuation. To alleviate this problem,<br />

I grouped marriages <strong>in</strong>to five-year cohorts: 1950–1954, 1955–1959, 1960–1964, 1965–<br />

1969, 1970–1974, 1975–1979, and 1980–1984. I calculated a cont<strong>in</strong>uous measure for year<br />

of marriage by cod<strong>in</strong>g 1 for marriages formed <strong>in</strong> 1950–1954, 2 for marriages formed <strong>in</strong><br />

1955–1959, and so forth to 7 for marriages formed <strong>in</strong> 1980–1984. This is the measure of<br />

year of marriage used <strong>in</strong> all the models.<br />

All results are shown us<strong>in</strong>g unweighted data. I do not provide descriptive statistics<br />

based on weighted data because I know a priori that the distributions of the predictor<br />

variables by marriage cohort are not nationally representative of each cohort. The purpose<br />

of this article is not to document accurately changes across marriage cohorts <strong>in</strong> their<br />

composition but, rather, to ascerta<strong>in</strong> whether the effects of important predictor variables<br />

change across time. As I describe later, I conducted a number of sensitivity tests to ascerta<strong>in</strong><br />

the robustness of my results. F<strong>in</strong>ally, results from the Cox proportional hazards models<br />

are shown as a function of the unweighted data to preserve the appropriate calculation<br />

of standard errors. 4<br />

Descriptive statistics for the sample are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 1. Although the results are<br />

biased by the fact that age at marriage is truncated <strong>in</strong> early marriage cohorts, they are<br />

consistent with previous research. In particular, age at marriage and education have<br />

shifted upward for both husbands and wives. In addition, more marriages are preceded<br />

by a premarital birth, and more women come from families that are disrupted by divorce.<br />

There is also some <strong>in</strong>dication that heterogeneity <strong>in</strong> marriage has shifted toward more<br />

women be<strong>in</strong>g older and hav<strong>in</strong>g more education than their husbands. Variations <strong>in</strong> racial<br />

composition and religion are more difficult to <strong>in</strong>terpret but likely reflect the overall retreat<br />

from marriage by blacks and the rapid <strong>in</strong>crease of the Hispanic population <strong>in</strong> the<br />

United States. As a result, blacks constitute a smaller share of all marriages, and there<br />

4. In results not shown here, the coefficients estimated from weighted data were not substantially different<br />

from those estimated from unweighted data.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 337<br />

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics of the Sample of Marriages Taken From Five Rounds of the National<br />

Survey of Family Growth<br />

_________________________________________________________________________<br />

Year Married<br />

Variable 1950– 1955– 1960– 1965– 1970– 1975– 1980–<br />

Age at Marriage<br />

1954 1959 1964 1969 1974 1979 1984<br />

< 19 51.6 42.3 39.3 33.7 33.2 24.3 18.9<br />

20–21 29.5 26.9 27.6 28.9 29.0 26.1 21.7<br />

22–23 14.3 17.6 17.7 19.8 19.4 21.9 20.5<br />

24–25 4.1 8.2 7.5 8.9 9.3 14.0 15.2<br />

25+<br />

Husband’s Age at<br />

Marriage<br />

0.4 5.0 7.9 8.6 9.1 13.9 23.8<br />

< 19 13.7 11.1 11.2 10.9 10.9 8.8 6.2<br />

20–21 24.8 22.2 22.2 21.8 22.2 17.6 14.9<br />

22–23 22.7 24.4 24.5 25.2 24.6 21.5 17.3<br />

24–25 17.0 17.0 15.6 16.8 16.9 18.8 17.3<br />

25+<br />

Woman’s Education<br />

21.9 25.3 26.5 25.2 25.4 33.3 44.2<br />

< 12 years 45.0 37.8 34.7 25.7 23.1 16.1 16.5<br />

12 years 42.7 44.7 44.7 46.1 43.8 40.7 38.2<br />

13–15 years 10.1 11.7 13.8 18.1 20.1 24.4 25.7<br />

16+ years<br />

Husband’s Education<br />

2.2 5.9 6.7 10.0 12.9 18.5 19.5<br />

< 12 years 45.0 37.9 32.3 24.1 19.7 16.8 18.0<br />

12 years 30.9 35.3 36.8 39.4 40.8 42.9 43.2<br />

13–15 years 12.1 11.6 13.9 17.4 20.5 20.7 19.1<br />

16+ years<br />

Premarital Fertility<br />

11.1 14.0 15.4 17.8 17.7 18.0 18.6<br />

Birth 7.2 10.6 11.9 12.6 13.2 14.8 18.9<br />

Conception 10.1 11.8 13.2 13.5 12.9 10.0 10.2<br />

Wife Older 1.5 2.6 2.7 2.7 4.0 5.5 6.0<br />

Husband Older<br />

Wife Has More<br />

26.2 23.1 22.4 17.7 17.7 19.6 22.7<br />

Education 13.8 13.8 13.0 13.5 13.8 19.9 20.7<br />

Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d 8.8 8.8 10.1 10.7 11.3 13.2 17.4<br />

Catholic<br />

Race<br />

21.3 21.5 22.8 23.1 24.8 28.6 30.9<br />

White 69.3 67.1 66.9 67.0 68.9 72.7 72.3<br />

Black 30.2 32.1 32.1 31.5 28.9 23.8 23.3<br />

Other 0.5 0.8 1.0 1.3 2.2 3.4 4.2<br />

N 1,962 2,901 3,857 5,545 6,481 3,345 3,205


338 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

has been a rise <strong>in</strong> the proportion of marriages <strong>in</strong> which the wife reports that she is Catholic.<br />

Change <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce over time is represented <strong>in</strong> Figure 1, where the yearly<br />

proportion of marriages surviv<strong>in</strong>g divorce is plotted for the first 10 years of marriage. The<br />

figure replicates the well-known rise <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce that occurred <strong>in</strong> the 1960s and<br />

1970s and the slow<strong>in</strong>g of this rise <strong>in</strong> the 1980s. The change <strong>in</strong> rates of divorce has been<br />

substantial. In the 1950–1954 cohort, almost 85% of marriages survived at least 10 years,<br />

whereas <strong>in</strong> the last three cohorts, only about 70% of marriages survived at least 10 years.<br />

MULTIVARIATE RESULTS<br />

I first ascerta<strong>in</strong>ed whether the effects of the cont<strong>in</strong>uous variables—age at marriage,<br />

husband’s age at marriage, marriage cohort, and education of husbands and wives—were<br />

best represented with a cont<strong>in</strong>uous cod<strong>in</strong>g or a set of dummy variables. I started by<br />

construct<strong>in</strong>g a set of dummy variables for these variables us<strong>in</strong>g the categories presented<br />

<strong>in</strong> Table 1. For each of the variables, I then estimated a simple Cox regression model<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g the dummy-variable cod<strong>in</strong>g and compared the results with two alternative Cox<br />

models us<strong>in</strong>g the variable coded cont<strong>in</strong>uously: a model with only a l<strong>in</strong>ear term and a<br />

model with both a l<strong>in</strong>ear and quadratic term. I compared models us<strong>in</strong>g both the traditional<br />

log-likelihood ratio (LR) statistic and Raftery’s (1995) Bayesian <strong>in</strong>formation criterion<br />

(BIC) statistic. 5<br />

In results not shown, I ascerta<strong>in</strong>ed that the best-fitt<strong>in</strong>g models were those that used a<br />

l<strong>in</strong>ear and a quadratic term for age at marriage, husband’s age at marriage, and marriage<br />

cohort and a l<strong>in</strong>ear term for wife’s education (based on both the LR and BIC statistics).<br />

For husband’s education, the dummy variables represented a better fit to the data. An<br />

additional analysis <strong>in</strong>dicated that a dummy variable with three categories for husband’s<br />

education fit the data best: 12 or fewer years, 13–15 years, and 16 or more years. These<br />

are the revised cod<strong>in</strong>gs for the variables that I used <strong>in</strong> the multivariate analysis.<br />

I began with a basel<strong>in</strong>e model that <strong>in</strong>cludes the additive effect of each variable (Model<br />

A <strong>in</strong> Table 2). Based on the weight of prior evidence, the basel<strong>in</strong>e model represents a<br />

reasonable specification of the effects of the covariates on the risk of divorce. I then tested<br />

a series of models aga<strong>in</strong>st this basel<strong>in</strong>e model. Each additional model, except the last,<br />

<strong>in</strong>cludes an <strong>in</strong>teraction between an <strong>in</strong>dividual predictor variable and historical time (as<br />

measured by year of marriage). For example, Model B adds an <strong>in</strong>teraction term between<br />

age at marriage and year of marriage to the basel<strong>in</strong>e model. Model C then adds an <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

term between hav<strong>in</strong>g a premarital birth and year of marriage to the basel<strong>in</strong>e model,<br />

and so on. The last model, Model N, <strong>in</strong>cludes <strong>in</strong>teractions between all the predictor variables<br />

and year of marriage.<br />

Each model with an <strong>in</strong>teraction term was compared with the basel<strong>in</strong>e model via differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> the LR and BIC statistics. Although both statistics are presented, I placed<br />

greater weight on the difference between the BIC statistics to test whether the null hypothesis<br />

of no <strong>in</strong>teraction should be rejected. I did so because it is well known that conventional<br />

test statistics are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by sample size. Thus, <strong>in</strong> reasonably large samples,<br />

such as the one used <strong>in</strong> this article, it is difficult to reject poor-fitt<strong>in</strong>g models us<strong>in</strong>g the LR<br />

statistic. More important, substantively un<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g differences between models will be<br />

judged to be statistically significant when sample sizes are large. Thus, even if the basel<strong>in</strong>e<br />

model is appropriate, use of the LR statistic, <strong>in</strong> conjunction with a large sample, may<br />

5. The BIC statistic is calculated as –χ 2 + df × ln(n), where χ 2 is the model chi-square, df is the degrees of<br />

freedom associated with the model, and n is the number of events (divorces) upon which the model is based.<br />

Note that I use the number of divorces as the value of n, not the number of marriages, yield<strong>in</strong>g a less conservative<br />

version of BIC. Models with more negative values of BIC are preferred to models with less negative values<br />

of BIC. A spirited discussion of the value of BIC as a tool for model selection is provided by Weakliem (1999)<br />

and the comments to his article <strong>in</strong> the same issue of Sociological Methods and Research.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 339<br />

Figure 1. Proportion of Marriages Not Disrupted, by Marriage Cohort<br />

Proportion<br />

1<br />

0.95<br />

0.9<br />

0.85<br />

0.8<br />

0.75<br />

0.7<br />

0.65<br />

1950–1954<br />

1955–1959<br />

1960–1964<br />

1965–1969<br />

1970–1974<br />

1975–1979<br />

1980–1984<br />

0.6<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10<br />

Marital Duration <strong>in</strong> Years<br />

lead to <strong>in</strong>appropriate conclusions—<strong>in</strong> this case, to <strong>in</strong>appropriately reject the null hypothesis<br />

of simple additive effects <strong>in</strong> favor of <strong>in</strong>teraction effects.<br />

The BIC statistic operates by penaliz<strong>in</strong>g models when they add degrees of freedom<br />

or sample size. The procedure is therefore conceptually equivalent to <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the size<br />

of a t statistic that is required to reject a null hypothesis for a s<strong>in</strong>gle coefficient. Although<br />

there are no formal cutoffs for assess<strong>in</strong>g the importance of a difference between<br />

two BIC values, Raftery (1995) provided the follow<strong>in</strong>g guidel<strong>in</strong>es. If there is a difference<br />

of 10 or more between two BIC values, there is very strong evidence that the null<br />

model should be rejected. Differences between BIC values of 6–10, 2–6, and 0–2 <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />

strong, positive, and weak evidence, respectively, that the null model should be<br />

rejected. In the case of comparisons <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g one degree of freedom (e.g., Models B<br />

through M <strong>in</strong> Table 2) and the number of divorces <strong>in</strong> this sample, these values of BIC<br />

correspond to t values of 3.86–4.35, 3.31–3.86, 2.99–3.31, respectively (squar<strong>in</strong>g these<br />

values yields the correspond<strong>in</strong>g chi-square values with 1 degree of freedom). Thus, the<br />

effect of us<strong>in</strong>g BIC is simply to require stronger evidence before the null hypothesis of<br />

no <strong>in</strong>teraction with time is rejected.<br />

Differences <strong>in</strong> LR statistics that are significant at p < .05 are presented <strong>in</strong> italic type.<br />

Differences <strong>in</strong> BIC statistics that <strong>in</strong>dicate at least positive support for reject<strong>in</strong>g the null<br />

hypothesis (< –2.0) are presented <strong>in</strong> bold italic type. On the basis of values of the conventional<br />

LR test statistic, significant <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong>volve the husband’s age at marriage and


340 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

Table 2. Estimates of Model Fit for Models Involv<strong>in</strong>g an Interaction Between Each Predictor<br />

Variable and Year Married<br />

Model χ 2 / df BIC<br />

Model Model χ 2 df Difference BIC Difference<br />

A: Basel<strong>in</strong>e Additive Model 2,929.58 22 –– –2,732.96 ––<br />

B: A + Age at Marriage × Year Married<br />

C: A + Husband’s Age at Marriage × Year<br />

2,933.14 23 3.56/1 –2,727.58 5.38<br />

Married 2,938.39 23 8.81/1 –2,732.83 0.13<br />

D: A + Premarital Birth × Year Married 2,933.14 23 3.56/1 –2,727.58 5.38<br />

E: A + Premarital Conception × Year Married 2,929.80 23 0.22/1 –2,724.24 8.72<br />

F: A + Wife Older × Year Married<br />

G: A + Wife Has More Education × Year<br />

2,930.51 23 0.93/1 –2,724.95 8.01<br />

Married 2,931.93 23 2.35/1 –2,726.37 6.59<br />

H: A + Husband Older × Year Married 2,939.01 23 9.43/1 –2,733.45 –0.49<br />

I: A + Wife’s Education × Year Married 2,932.22 23 2.64/1 –2,726.66 6.30<br />

J: A + Husband’s Education × Year Married 2,929.89 24 0.31/2 –2,715.40 17.56<br />

K: A + Catholic × Year Married 2,936.00 23 6.42/1 –2,731.44 1.52<br />

L: A + Wife’s Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d × Year Married 2,931.14 23 1.56/1 –2,725.58 7.38<br />

M: A + Black × Year Married 2,953.24 23 23.66/1 –2,747.68 –14.72<br />

N: A + All Interactions With Year Married 2,980.33 35 50.75/13 –2,667.52 65.44<br />

Notes: All models <strong>in</strong>clude a control for survey round. N for the calculation of BIC is 7,611. Values <strong>in</strong> italic type are<br />

statistically significant at p < .05. Values <strong>in</strong> bold italic type show strong evidence us<strong>in</strong>g BIC for reject<strong>in</strong>g the null hypothesis of a<br />

zero effect.<br />

the husband be<strong>in</strong>g older, be<strong>in</strong>g Catholic, and be<strong>in</strong>g black. However, the BIC statistic <strong>in</strong>dicates<br />

only one <strong>in</strong>teraction with positive evidence that the null hypothesis of no change<br />

across time should be rejected: the effect of be<strong>in</strong>g black.<br />

The results for Model N, which simultaneously <strong>in</strong>cludes all <strong>in</strong>teractions with historical<br />

time, <strong>in</strong>dicate a significantly better-fitt<strong>in</strong>g model us<strong>in</strong>g the LR statistic but not the<br />

BIC statistic. Indeed, the substantial <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the BIC statistic <strong>in</strong>dicates that this model<br />

may be overparameterized (i.e., may conta<strong>in</strong> too many nonsignificant parameters). The<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual coefficients from this model, however, are <strong>in</strong>structive: they <strong>in</strong>dicate that when<br />

all <strong>in</strong>teractions are considered simultaneously, the only significant <strong>in</strong>teraction, us<strong>in</strong>g either<br />

the LR or BIC statistic as the criterion, is that associated with be<strong>in</strong>g black (results<br />

not shown). This result provides further support for the notion that the only substantial<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction with time <strong>in</strong>volves blacks.<br />

Hazard ratios from bivariate Cox regression models, the additive multivariate basel<strong>in</strong>e<br />

model (Model A <strong>in</strong> Table 2), and the model <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>teraction between year of<br />

marriage and be<strong>in</strong>g black (Model M <strong>in</strong> Table 2) are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3. The hazard ratios<br />

shown (e β ) represent multiplicative effects on the hazard of marital dissolution at any<br />

marital duration. Hazard ratios greater than 1 <strong>in</strong>dicate a positive effect, and hazard ratios<br />

less than 1 <strong>in</strong>dicate a negative effect. The percentage difference <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce at<br />

any marital duration associated with a unit change <strong>in</strong> a predictor variable can be calculated<br />

by subtract<strong>in</strong>g 1 from the hazard ratio and multiply<strong>in</strong>g by 100.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 341<br />

Table 3. Hazard Ratios From Selected Models <strong>in</strong> Table 2<br />

Bivariate Model Additive Model Interaction Model<br />

Variable Coefficients Coefficients Coefficients<br />

Year Married 1.38 1.41 1.46<br />

Year Married, Squared 0.97 0.98 0.98<br />

Age at Marriage 0.63 0.70 0.70<br />

Age at Marriage Squared 1.01 1.01 1.01<br />

Husband’s Age at Marriage 0.82 0.92 0.92<br />

Husband’s Age at Marriage, Squared 1.003 1.001 1.001<br />

Premarital Birth 2.08 1.68 1.69<br />

Premarital Conception 1.41 1.16 1.15<br />

Wife Older 1.48 1.68 1.88<br />

Wife Has More Education 1.17 1.10a 1.09a Husband Older 1.14 1.19 1.19<br />

Wife’s Education 0.94 1.04 1.05<br />

Husband Has 13–15 Years of School<strong>in</strong>g 0.59 0.69 0.69<br />

Husband Has 16+ Years of School<strong>in</strong>g 0.39 0.56 0.55<br />

Catholic 0.69 0.90 0.89<br />

Wife’s Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d 1.64 1.39 1.38<br />

Black 1.99 1.49 1.97<br />

Black × Year Married –– –– 0.93<br />

Other Race 0.90b 0.95b 0.93b Note: All models <strong>in</strong>clude a control for survey round.<br />

a<br />

The null hypothesis that the coefficient is not statistically significant cannot be rejected us<strong>in</strong>g a BIC value equal to strong<br />

evidence (t > 3.31).<br />

b The coefficient is not statistically significant at the conventional .05 level.<br />

The hazard ratios for year of marriage and age at marriage are consistent across all<br />

three models. The coefficients for year of marriage <strong>in</strong>dicate an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g risk of marital<br />

dissolution that has slackened <strong>in</strong> pace more recently. 6 The hazard ratios for each<br />

spouse’s age at marriage reflect a decrement <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce with age that moderates<br />

somewhat at older ages (although the pattern of change across age at marriage is<br />

stronger for wives than husbands).<br />

The effects of both a premarital birth and a premarital conception on divorce are<br />

positive, with the effect of a premarital birth be<strong>in</strong>g much stronger. In both cases, the<br />

multivariate models show effects that are somewhat lower than <strong>in</strong> the bivariate model.<br />

That is, a nontrivial portion of the effect of both variables can be attributed to other<br />

covariates <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the model.<br />

6. For the sake of parsimony, I use the phrase “<strong>in</strong>crease or decrease <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce” when the<br />

correct phrase is “<strong>in</strong>crease or decrease <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce at a given marital duration.”


342 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

The effect of the husband be<strong>in</strong>g older on divorce is positive <strong>in</strong> both the bivariate and<br />

additive effect models. The effect of the wife be<strong>in</strong>g older is substantial. In the <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

effect model, if a wife is five or more years older than her husband, the risk of divorce is<br />

88% higher <strong>in</strong> comparison with a marriage that has relative age homogamy. The effect of<br />

the wife hav<strong>in</strong>g more education is also positive, although not significant us<strong>in</strong>g the BIC<br />

criterion for strong evidence, <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the risk of divorce by 9% at each marital duration.<br />

The effect of the wife’s education is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> that it reverses direction between<br />

the bivariate model and the two multivariate models. In the bivariate model, the risk of<br />

marital disruption decreases 6% for each additional year of school<strong>in</strong>g the wife has. In the<br />

multivariate models, the risk of divorce <strong>in</strong>creases 4%–5% for each additional year of the<br />

wife’s school<strong>in</strong>g. The switch from a negative to a positive effect is due mostly to the<br />

<strong>in</strong>clusion of the control for husband’s education (results not shown). More-educated<br />

women are generally married to more-educated men, and the effect of the husband’s education<br />

is substantial and negative.<br />

The effect of be<strong>in</strong>g Catholic is to reduce the risk of marital dissolution, and the effect<br />

of the wife’s parents hav<strong>in</strong>g divorced is positive. In both cases, though, the effect is reduced<br />

somewhat with the <strong>in</strong>troduction of the other covariates. In the <strong>in</strong>teraction model, at<br />

each marital duration, Catholics are 11% less likely to divorce, and couples <strong>in</strong> which the<br />

wives’ parents divorced are 38% more likely to divorce.<br />

There is no difference between whites and respondents report<strong>in</strong>g neither white nor<br />

black race <strong>in</strong> the risk of marital dissolution. Blacks are substantially more likely to divorce<br />

at each marital duration than are whites. However, this effect changes across time.<br />

In the 1950–1954 cohort, the risk of divorce for blacks is 83% greater (1.97 × .93 = 1.83).<br />

In the 1980–1984 cohort, the risk of marital dissolution for blacks is only 19% higher.<br />

This pattern results from the fact that the risk of marital dissolution <strong>in</strong>creases more slowly<br />

(by a factor of .93) for blacks than for whites.<br />

A Test for Proportionality<br />

All the models estimated up to this po<strong>in</strong>t assume that the effects of the covariates are<br />

proportional. That is, the effects of the predictor variables are assumed to be equal at all<br />

marital durations. A test for proportionality is key for the purposes of this study to account<br />

for the possibility that historical shifts <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce may be confused with<br />

nonproportionality. In particular, a variable with an effect that <strong>in</strong>creases (decreases) with<br />

marital duration may be mistaken for a variable with an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g (decreas<strong>in</strong>g) effect<br />

across historical time.<br />

In general, two rationales have been put forward to suspect nonproportionality. First,<br />

as marriages mature, <strong>in</strong>dividuals accrue more marital-specific capital (Becker et al.<br />

1977), decreas<strong>in</strong>g the risk of divorce. Thus, <strong>in</strong>dividual couples change with respect to<br />

their risk of marital disruption. Second, as marriages progress, a process of sort<strong>in</strong>g occurs,<br />

weed<strong>in</strong>g out couples who are less suited to each other or less committed to a permanent<br />

relationship (Morgan and R<strong>in</strong>dfuss 1985; South and Spitze 1986; Teachman<br />

1986). The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g marriages are therefore composed of couples who are less likely to<br />

divorce.<br />

The empirical evidence is limited, but a number of researchers, us<strong>in</strong>g different data<br />

and methods, have found that the effect of age at marriage is stable across marital duration<br />

(Morgan and R<strong>in</strong>dfuss 1985; South and Spitze 1986; Teachman 1986; Thornton and<br />

Rodgers 1987). Morgan and R<strong>in</strong>dfuss (1985) reported that the positive effect on divorce<br />

of hav<strong>in</strong>g a premarital birth decl<strong>in</strong>es at longer marital durations, but Teachman (1986)<br />

found a more consistent effect for this variable. South and Spitze (1986) and South (2001)<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated that the effect of the wife’s education changes from be<strong>in</strong>g negative to be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

positive as marriages mature. Aga<strong>in</strong>, however, Teachman (1986) found a consistent effect<br />

of the wife’s education across different marital durations.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 343<br />

Table 4. Tests for the Proportionality of Each of the Predictor Variables<br />

Model χ 2 / df BIC<br />

Model Model χ 2 df Difference BIC Difference<br />

A: Basel<strong>in</strong>e Proportional Effects Model 2,929.58 22 –2,732.97 ––<br />

B: A + Age at Marriage × Marital Duration<br />

C: A + Husband’s Age at Marriage × Marital<br />

2,930.21 23 0.60/1 –2,724.65 8.32<br />

Duration 2,930.57 23 0.99/1 –2,725.01 7.96<br />

D: A + Premarital Birth × Marital Duration<br />

E: A + Premarital Conception × Marital<br />

2,935.64 23 6.06/1 –2,730.08 2.89<br />

Duration 2,930.31 23 0.73/1 –2,724.75 8.22<br />

F: A + Wife Older × Marital Duration<br />

G: A + Wife Has More Education × Marital<br />

2,933.91 23 4.33/1 –2,728.35 4.62<br />

Duration 2,929.60 23 0.02/1 –2,724.04 8.93<br />

H: A + Husband Older × Marital Duration 2,932.11 23 2.53/1 –2,726.55 6.42<br />

I: A + Wife’s Education × Marital Duration 2,935.27 23 5.69/1 –2,729.71 3.26<br />

J: A + Husband’s Education × Marital Duration 2,947.91 24 18.33/2 –2,733.41 –0.44<br />

K: A + Catholic × Marital Duration<br />

L: A + Wife’s Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d × Marital<br />

2,929.89 23 0.31/1 –2,724.33 8.64<br />

Duration 2,934.39 23 4.81/1 –2,728.83 4.14<br />

M: A + Black × Marital Duration 2,934.96 23 5.38/1 –2,729.40 3.57<br />

Notes: All models <strong>in</strong>clude a control for survey round. N for calculat<strong>in</strong>g BIC is 7,611. Values <strong>in</strong> italic type are statistically<br />

significant at p < .05.<br />

I tested for the proportionality of effects us<strong>in</strong>g a simple <strong>in</strong>teraction between marital<br />

duration and each of the predictor variables. The results of these tests for proportionality<br />

are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 4. I used Model A <strong>in</strong> Table 2 as the basel<strong>in</strong>e and consider whether the<br />

addition of nonproportional effects yields a better-fitt<strong>in</strong>g model. Aga<strong>in</strong>, each model allow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

nonproportionality was compared with the basel<strong>in</strong>e model via differences <strong>in</strong> the<br />

LR and BIC statistics. I cont<strong>in</strong>ued to attach greater weight to the values of BIC to test<br />

whether the null hypothesis of proportionality should be rejected.<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g BIC as the criterion, I found that none of the models allow<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

nonproportionality yield a better fit to the data than the basel<strong>in</strong>e model. The LR statistic<br />

suggests nonproportionality for hav<strong>in</strong>g a premarital birth, the wife be<strong>in</strong>g older, the wife’s<br />

education, the husband’s education, hav<strong>in</strong>g a wife with divorced parents, and be<strong>in</strong>g black.<br />

With the exception of be<strong>in</strong>g black, these are not the same variables for which the LR<br />

statistics suggested an <strong>in</strong>teraction with historical time. Thus, it is not likely that<br />

nonproportionality produced these potential <strong>in</strong>teractions with historical time.<br />

With respect to be<strong>in</strong>g black, the nonproportional effect <strong>in</strong>dicates an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly positive<br />

difference across marital duration (result not shown). This pattern strongly suggests<br />

that nonproportionality is not implicated <strong>in</strong> the decreas<strong>in</strong>g effect of be<strong>in</strong>g black across<br />

time shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3. If nonproportionality was <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g the chang<strong>in</strong>g<br />

effect of be<strong>in</strong>g black across historical time, it would have <strong>in</strong>volved an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly negative<br />

difference across marital duration.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, I tested for an <strong>in</strong>teraction between year of marriage and marital duration. If<br />

period effects are present, then the effects of year of marriage should be nonproportional.


344 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

Given the rise <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce over time, I anticipated that the nature of the <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

should be a decreas<strong>in</strong>g effect of year of marriage at longer marital durations (because<br />

successive marriage cohorts experience a particular marital duration dur<strong>in</strong>g later periods<br />

when the risk of divorce is greater). The result<strong>in</strong>g model (not shown) does fit the data<br />

better than the basel<strong>in</strong>e model (Model χ 2 = 2,976.54, BIC = –2,770.98). The coefficient<br />

for the <strong>in</strong>teraction between year of marriage and marital duration is statistically significant<br />

(us<strong>in</strong>g the BIC criterion for strong evidence) and negative (e β = .999). This <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

is consistent with the notion that there is a period <strong>in</strong>fluence on the risk of marital<br />

dissolution. Inclusion of this <strong>in</strong>teraction term does not alter the coefficients (or standard<br />

errors) associated with any of the other covariates. Nor does <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

term alter any of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs shown <strong>in</strong> Tables 2 and 3.<br />

How Robust Are the F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs?<br />

I conducted several sensitivity tests to ascerta<strong>in</strong> whether the results are robust to different<br />

model specifications and restrictions on the data. First, I considered whether different<br />

results would be obta<strong>in</strong>ed if I coded age at marriage and year of marriage as a series of<br />

dummy variables <strong>in</strong>stead of us<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>uous variables. Because of the number of potential<br />

<strong>in</strong>teractions, I estimated a separate model for each <strong>in</strong>teraction between a particular<br />

age at marriage and year of marriage (us<strong>in</strong>g the age at marriage and year of marriage<br />

categories represented <strong>in</strong> Table 1). These models do not reveal a pattern of <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

between age at marriage and year of marriage that is otherwise masked by us<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>uous<br />

variables (see Appendix).<br />

Second, I considered the possibility that the results may be biased somehow by the<br />

fact that women who marry <strong>in</strong> earlier historical periods are more heavily weighted toward<br />

earlier ages at marriage. To address this issue, I reconfigured the sample and reestimated<br />

the models us<strong>in</strong>g data on marriages formed after 1959. I also formed a sample based on<br />

all years of marriage but restricted marriages to those formed before age 23, yield<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

sample <strong>in</strong> which the maximum age at marriage is consistent across year of marriage. For<br />

the sample restricted to women who marry before age 23, accord<strong>in</strong>g to values of the BIC<br />

statistic, the same <strong>in</strong>teraction as revealed <strong>in</strong> Table 2 yields a better model fit: the <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

of be<strong>in</strong>g black with year of marriage (see Appendix). The coefficients from this model<br />

are similar to those estimated from the full sample.<br />

In the sample restricted to marriages formed after 1959, the <strong>in</strong>teraction between be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

black and marriage cohort yields weak evidence of a better-fitt<strong>in</strong>g model. The important<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t to note from models estimated on this sample, though, is that no new <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

appeared when a different set of marriage ages was used. The lack of a substantial<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction between be<strong>in</strong>g black and year of marriage attests to the need for a long historical<br />

sequence to detect substantial changes <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce.<br />

Does Cohabitation Make a Difference?<br />

One substantial change <strong>in</strong> the nature of <strong>in</strong>timate relationships has been the sudden and<br />

steep rise <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>cidence and prevalence of premarital cohabitation (Bumpass and Sweet<br />

1989). Much research has l<strong>in</strong>ked premarital cohabitation to an <strong>in</strong>creased risk of marital<br />

dissolution (Ax<strong>in</strong>n and Thornton 1992; Bumpass, Sweet, and Cherl<strong>in</strong> 1991; DeMaris and<br />

MacDonald 1993; Mann<strong>in</strong>g and Smock 1994; Thomson and Colella 1992). In this section,<br />

I seek to answer two questions. First, has the effect of premarital cohabitation on<br />

divorce changed over time? Second, are the relationships between the measured covariates<br />

and divorce somehow altered by the substantial <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> premarital cohabitation<br />

over the past several decades?<br />

I made use of <strong>in</strong>formation on premarital cohabitation conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> rounds 4 and 5 of<br />

the NSFG, exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g marriages formed after 1964. I replicated the models shown <strong>in</strong><br />

Table 2, first us<strong>in</strong>g premarital cohabitation as a predictor variable and then exclud<strong>in</strong>g


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 345<br />

this variable. The results <strong>in</strong>dicated no evidence that the effect of premarital cohabitation<br />

on the risk of divorce has changed over the relatively short period be<strong>in</strong>g considered (see<br />

Appendix). Nor does the <strong>in</strong>clusion of cohabitation <strong>in</strong> any of the models alter the conclusions<br />

reached about the lack of <strong>in</strong>teraction between the other covariates and historical<br />

period. Consistent with prior research, the estimated effect of premarital cohabitation<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicates an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the risk of divorce (by about 35%). Also <strong>in</strong> results not shown, I<br />

found the effects of premarital cohabitation to be proportional across marital duration.<br />

Variation <strong>in</strong> Effects by Race<br />

I conclude the analysis by estimat<strong>in</strong>g models separately for blacks and whites. Not only<br />

does the effect of race vary by year of marriage, but there is also prior evidence to suggest<br />

that the effects of various covariates on divorce may differ by race (Mart<strong>in</strong> and<br />

Bumpass 1989; Teachman 1986). Thus, by estimat<strong>in</strong>g models separately by race, I attempted<br />

to ascerta<strong>in</strong> whether the effects of the measured covariates varied over time differently<br />

for blacks and whites (results not shown).<br />

I reestimated the basel<strong>in</strong>e model shown <strong>in</strong> Table 2 for blacks and whites only. I added<br />

to the basel<strong>in</strong>e model an <strong>in</strong>teraction between race and year married. The results <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

the strong <strong>in</strong>teraction between race and year married <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> Table 2. I then estimated<br />

the basel<strong>in</strong>e model separately for blacks and whites, thereby allow<strong>in</strong>g the effects of all the<br />

covariates to vary accord<strong>in</strong>g to race. These models were substantially less well fitt<strong>in</strong>g than<br />

a model that constra<strong>in</strong>s the effects of the covariates to be equal across race. Thus, a model<br />

that allows an <strong>in</strong>teraction between race and year married fits better than the basel<strong>in</strong>e model,<br />

and a model that allows <strong>in</strong>teractions between race and the rema<strong>in</strong>der of the covariates does<br />

not fit better. Although it is possible that a smaller subset of covariates may <strong>in</strong>teract with<br />

race, I did not search for such <strong>in</strong>teractions, given the purposes of this article and the lack of<br />

otherwise strong theoretical guidance to identify these <strong>in</strong>teractions.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g five rounds of the NSFG, I tested for changes <strong>in</strong> the effects of basic sociodemographic<br />

variables on the risk of divorce. In the ma<strong>in</strong>, most effects have rema<strong>in</strong>ed constant<br />

across a period dur<strong>in</strong>g which rates of divorce varied substantially. The results are robust<br />

to different model specifications and def<strong>in</strong>itions of the sample. Nor can the results be<br />

attributed to the nonproportionality of effects across different marital durations.<br />

Only one clear-cut <strong>in</strong>teraction with historical time was found: that <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the effect<br />

of race. Specifically, there was a convergence over time <strong>in</strong> the risk of marital dissolution<br />

between whites and blacks because the risk of marital dissolution rose more rapidly<br />

for whites than for blacks. I offer the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the <strong>in</strong>teraction between<br />

race and historical time. Specifically, the decl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g economic position of many blacks and<br />

their less favorable marriage-market conditions, when comb<strong>in</strong>ed with the growth of alternatives<br />

to marriage and greater acceptance and prevalence of out-of-wedlock childbear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and cohabitation, means that blacks who marry are an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly select subgroup of<br />

all blacks. Blacks marry at later ages and are less likely to ever marry than are whites<br />

(Cherl<strong>in</strong> 1992; Teachman et al. 2000). Indeed, the environments faced by blacks and<br />

whites have diverged sufficiently that the current situation is a reversal of historical patterns.<br />

Before 1950, blacks married earlier than whites and were more likely to have ever<br />

married (Sweet and Bumpass 1987). S<strong>in</strong>ce 1950, blacks have delayed marriage more than<br />

whites, yield<strong>in</strong>g higher proportions who never marry.<br />

The result of these changes is that blacks who marry are likely to be selective of<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals who are committed to marriage and therefore are less likely to divorce. As a<br />

consequence, although both blacks and whites have been subject to the pervasive upward<br />

surge <strong>in</strong> divorce over the past several decades, the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g selectivity of blacks <strong>in</strong>to<br />

marriage has yielded a somewhat slower <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the risk of marital dissolution.


346 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

The chang<strong>in</strong>g effect of be<strong>in</strong>g black aside, the limited evidence for change <strong>in</strong> the effects<br />

of a range of basic sociodemographic predictors of divorce constitutes impressive<br />

evidence for the pervasive impact of historical period on the risk of marital dissolution.<br />

For a wide range of variables, there is little evidence of deviation from recent historical<br />

shifts <strong>in</strong> divorce. These results mirror the sentiment of Thornton and Rodgers (1987:20)<br />

that “the degree of uniformity of the historical period effects across population subgroups<br />

has been remarkable.”<br />

Although it is important to know that the effects of historical period have been pervasive,<br />

it still begs the next logical question: what is it about historical periods that<br />

affects the risk of divorce? This question is all the more important, given the recent<br />

level<strong>in</strong>g of divorce rates <strong>in</strong> the United States (Goldste<strong>in</strong> 1999), a level<strong>in</strong>g that cannot be<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed by compositional factors or the rise <strong>in</strong> nonmarital cohabitation. There is grow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

evidence, therefore, that marriages are subject to the <strong>in</strong>fluence of powerful<br />

aggregate-level forces that are not yet well understood. Subsequent research should focus<br />

on identify<strong>in</strong>g and quantify<strong>in</strong>g the characteristics of historical periods that have led<br />

to such pervasive effects.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, as a note of caution, I rem<strong>in</strong>d readers that there may be other variables not<br />

considered <strong>in</strong> the current study that may have effects that vary across time. In particular,<br />

it would be beneficial for subsequent research to consider the impact of a wider range of<br />

variables that may be more tightly l<strong>in</strong>ked to the <strong>in</strong>strumental and expressive exchange<br />

that occurs between spouses. For example, more <strong>in</strong>formation about changes <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

of variables, such as wives’ labor force participation, occupations, and gender-role<br />

ideology and the division of household labor, could provide additional evidence about<br />

the nature of period effects on marital dissolution. On the basis of the exchange model,<br />

South (2001) outl<strong>in</strong>ed good reasons for expect<strong>in</strong>g the effect of wives’ labor force participation<br />

on divorce to change across time, and his empirical results suggest an effect that<br />

has become <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly positive. South’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate the complexity of factors<br />

that <strong>in</strong>fluence divorce and argue for care <strong>in</strong> specify<strong>in</strong>g the components of marriage that<br />

may be important to marital stability <strong>in</strong> a particular context. Although there is evidence<br />

that a wide range of important risk factors have consistent effects on divorce over time,<br />

the chang<strong>in</strong>g nature and context of marital unions argues for the cont<strong>in</strong>ued <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

of a wider range of factors that can be l<strong>in</strong>ked to marital cohesion.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 347<br />

Appendix Table A1. Estimates of Model Fit for Various Sensitivity Models<br />

____________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________<br />

Marriages Formed After 1959 Marriages Formed Before Age 23<br />

Model χ2 / df BIC Model χ2 / df BIC<br />

Model Model χ2 df Difference BIC Difference Model χ2 df Difference BIC Difference<br />

A: Basel<strong>in</strong>e Additive Model 2,056.08 22 –1,864.82 –– 2,476.85 22 –2,283.53<br />

B: A + Age at Marriage × Year<br />

Married 2,057.55 23 1.47/1 –1,856.76 8.06 2,480.60 23 3.75/1 –2,278.48 5.05<br />

C: A + Husband’s Age at Marriage<br />

× Year Married 2,059.13 23 3.05/1 –1,858.34 6.48 2,482.13 23 5.28/1 –2,280.02 3.51<br />

C: A + Premarital Birth × Year<br />

Married 2,060.83 23 4.75/1 –1,860.05 4.77 2,481.01 23 4.16/1 –2278.90 4.63<br />

D: A + Premarital Conception<br />

× Year Married 2,056.20 23 0.12/1 –1,855.41 9.41 2,477.07 23 0.22/1 –2,274.96 8.63<br />

E: A + Wife Older × Year Married 2,056.24 23 0.16/1 –1,855.45 9.37 2,478.44 23 1.59/1 –2,276.33 3.86<br />

F: A + Wife Has More Education<br />

× Year Married 2,061.30 23 5.22/1 –1,860.51 4.31 2,481.61 23 4.76/1 –2,279.50 4.03<br />

G: A + Husband Older × Year<br />

Married 2,062.50 23 6.92/1 –1,861.71 2.91 2,487.12 23 10.27/1 –2,285.01 –1.48<br />

H: A + Wife’s Education × Year<br />

Married 2,056.15 23 0.70/1 –1,855.37 9.45 2,478.96 23 2.11/1 –2,276.85 6.68<br />

I: A + Husband’s Education<br />

× Year Married 2,057.85 24 1.77/2 –1,848.33 16.49 2,477.51 24 0.66/2 –2,266.61 16.92<br />

K: A + Catholic × Year Married 2,060.55 23 4.47/1 –1,859.76 5.06 2,481.47 23 4.62/1 –2,279.36 4.17<br />

L: A + Wife’s Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d<br />

× Year Married 2,056.88 23 0.80/1 –1,856.09 8.73 2,477.57 23 0.72/1 –2,275.46 8.07<br />

M: A + Black × Year Married 2,066.44 23 10.36/1 –1,865.65 –0.83– 2,498.08 23 21.23/1 –2,295.97 –12.44<br />

Notes: All models <strong>in</strong>clude a control for survey round. N for calculat<strong>in</strong>g BIC for marriages formed after age 1959 is 6,185. N for calculat<strong>in</strong>g BIC for marriages formed before age 23 is 6,551.<br />

Values <strong>in</strong> italic type are statistically significant at p < .05. Values <strong>in</strong> bold italic type show strong evidence us<strong>in</strong>g BIC for reject<strong>in</strong>g the null hypothesis of a zero effect.


348 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

Appendix Table A2. Estimates of Model Fit for Models Includ<strong>in</strong>g and Then Exclud<strong>in</strong>g Cohabitation<br />

____________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________<br />

Includ<strong>in</strong>g Premarital Cohabitation Exclud<strong>in</strong>g Premarital Cohabitation<br />

Model χ2 / df BIC Model χ2 / df BIC<br />

Model Model χ2 df Difference BIC Difference Model χ2 df Difference BIC Difference<br />

A: Basel<strong>in</strong>e Additive Model 869.36 20 –709.19 –– 824.55 19 –672.39 ––<br />

B: A + Age at Marriage × Year<br />

Married 869.86 21 0.50/1 –701.69 7.50 824.84 20 0.29/1 –664.67 7.72<br />

C: A + Husband’s Age at Marriage<br />

× Year Married 872.56 21 3.20/1 –704.38 4.81 827.48 20 2.93/1 –667.31 4.99<br />

D: A + Premarital Birth × Year<br />

Married 869.60 21 0.24/1 –701.42 7.77 824.95 20 0.40/1 –664.78 7.52<br />

E: A + Premarital Conception<br />

× Year Married 870.87 21 1.27/1 –702.70 6.49 826.24 20 1.69/1 –666.07 6.23<br />

F: A + Wife Older × Year Married 869.40 21 0.04/1 –701.22 7.97 824.58 20 0.03/1 –664.41 7.89<br />

G: A + Wife Has More Education<br />

× Year Married 869.36 21 0.00/1 –701.18 8.01 824.55 20 0.00/1 –664.38 8.01<br />

H: A + Husband Older × Year<br />

Married 869.64 21 0.28/1 –701.46 7.73 824.76 20 0.21/1 –664.59 7.71<br />

I: A + Wife’s Education × Year Married 870.42 21 1.05/1 –702.25 6.94 825.62 20 1.07/1 –665.45 6.85<br />

J: A + Husband’s Education × Year<br />

Married 875.93 22 6.57/2 –699.75 9.44 831.21 21 6.66/2 –663.04 9.26<br />

L: A + Catholic × Year Married 870.71 21 1.35/1 –702.54 6.65 826.05 20 1.50/1 –665.88 6.42<br />

M: A + Wife’s Parents <strong>Divorce</strong>d<br />

× Year Married 869.43 21 0.07/1 –701.25 7.94 824.78 20 0.23/1 –664.61 7.69<br />

N: A + Premarital Cohabitation<br />

× Year Married 871.94 21 2.58/1 –703.77 5.42 –– –– –– –– ––<br />

O: A + Black × Year Married 870.67 21 1.31/1 –702.50 6.69 826.33 20 1.78/1 –666.16 6.14<br />

Notes: All models <strong>in</strong>clude a control for survey round. N for estimat<strong>in</strong>g BIC is 3,006. Values <strong>in</strong> italic type are statistically significant at p < .05.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 349<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Allison, P. 1995. Survival Analysis Us<strong>in</strong>g the SAS System: A Practical Guide. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.<br />

Amato, P. 2000. “The Consequences of <strong>Divorce</strong> for Adults and Children.” Journal of Marriage<br />

and the Family 62:1269–87.<br />

Ax<strong>in</strong>n, W. and A. Thornton. 1992. “The Relationship Between Cohabitation and <strong>Divorce</strong>: Selectivity<br />

or Causal Influence.” Demography 29:357–74.<br />

Becker, G. 1991. A Treatise on the Family. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Becker, G., E. Landes, and R. Michael. 1977. “An Economic Analysis of Marital Instability.” Journal<br />

of Political Economy 85:1141–87.<br />

Bianchi, S. 1995. “Chang<strong>in</strong>g Economic Roles of Women and Men.” Pp. 107–54 <strong>in</strong> State of the<br />

Union: America <strong>in</strong> the 1990s, Vol. 1: Economic Trends, edited by R. Farley. New York: Russell<br />

Sage Foundation.<br />

Booth, A. and J. Edwards. 1985. “Age at Marriage and Marital Instability.” Journal of Marriage<br />

and the Family 47:67–75.<br />

Bumpass, L., T. Mart<strong>in</strong>, and J. Sweet. 1991. “The Impact of Family Background and Early Marital<br />

<strong>Factors</strong> on Marital Disruption.” Journal of Family Issues 12:22–42.<br />

Bumpass, L. and J. Sweet. 1972. “Differentials <strong>in</strong> Marital Instability: 1970.” American Sociological<br />

Review 37:754–66.<br />

———. 1989. “National Estimates of Cohabitation.” Demography 26:615–25.<br />

Bumpass, L., J. Sweet, and A. Cherl<strong>in</strong>. 1991. “The Role of Cohabitation <strong>in</strong> Decl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Rates of<br />

Marriage.” Journal of Marriage and the Family 53:913–27.<br />

Carlson, E. and K. St<strong>in</strong>son. 1982. “Motherhood, Marriage Tim<strong>in</strong>g, and Marital <strong>Stability</strong>.” Social<br />

Forces 61:258–67.<br />

Cherl<strong>in</strong>, A. 1992. Marriage, <strong>Divorce</strong>, Remarriage, rev. ed. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University<br />

Press.<br />

Cherl<strong>in</strong>, A., P. Chase-Lansdale, and C. McRae. 1998. “Effects of Parental <strong>Divorce</strong> and Mental<br />

Health Throughout the Life Course.” American Sociological Review 63:239–49.<br />

Clarkberg, M., R. Stolzenberg, and L. Waite. 1995. “Attitudes, Values, and Entrance Into<br />

Cohabitational Versus Marital Unions.” Social Forces 74:609–34.<br />

Coley, R. and P. Chase-Lansdale. 1998. “Adolescent Pregnancy and Parenthood: Recent Evidence<br />

and Future Directions.” American Psychologist 53:152–66.<br />

Cox, D. 1972. “Regression Models With Life Tables.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society<br />

B34:187–220.<br />

DaVanzo, J. and M. Rahman. 1993. “American Families: Trends and Correlates.” Population Index<br />

59:350–86.<br />

DeMaris, A. and W. MacDonald. 1993. “Premarital Cohabitation and Marital Instability: A Test of<br />

the Unconventionality Hypothesis.” Journal of Marriage and the Family 55:399–407.<br />

Ellwood, D. and J. Crane. 1990. “Family Change Among African-Americans: What Do We Know?”<br />

Journal of Economic Perspectives 4:65–84.<br />

Eysenck, H. 1980. “Personality, Marital Satisfaction, and <strong>Divorce</strong>.” Psychological Reports<br />

47:1235–38.<br />

Faust, K. and J. McKibben. 1999. “Marital Dissolution: <strong>Divorce</strong> Separation, Annulment, and Widowhood.”<br />

Pp. 475–99 <strong>in</strong> Handbook of Marriage and the Family, 2nd ed., edited by M. Sussman,<br />

S. Ste<strong>in</strong>metz, and G. Peterson. New York: Plenum.<br />

Goldste<strong>in</strong>, J. 1999. “The Level<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>in</strong> the United States.” Demography 36:409–14.<br />

Heaton, T. 1991. “Time Related Dimensions of Marital Dissolution.” Journal of Marriage and the<br />

Family 53:285–95.<br />

Jock<strong>in</strong>, V., M. McGue, and D. Lykken. 1996. “Personality and <strong>Divorce</strong>: A Genetic Analysis.” Journal<br />

of Personality and Social Psychology 71:288–99.<br />

Johnson, J. and W. Harris. 1980. “Personality and Behavioral Characteristics Related to <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>in</strong>


350 Demography, Volume 39-Number 2, May 2002<br />

a Population of Male Applicants for Psychiatric Evaluation.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology<br />

89:510–13.<br />

Kiernan, K. 1986. “Teenage Marriage and Marital Breakdown: A Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Study.” Population<br />

Studies 40:35–54.<br />

Lehrer, E. and C. Chiswick. 1993. “Religion as a Determ<strong>in</strong>ant of Marital <strong>Stability</strong>.” Demography<br />

30:385–404.<br />

Lichter, D., F. LeClere, and D. McLaughl<strong>in</strong>. 1991. “Local Marriage Markets and the Marital Behavior<br />

of African American and White Women.” American Journal of Sociology 96:843–67.<br />

Lichter, D., D. McLaughl<strong>in</strong>, G. Kephart, and D. Landry. 1992. “Race and the Retreat From Marriage:<br />

A Shortage of Marriageable Men?” American Sociological Review 57:781–99.<br />

Mann<strong>in</strong>g, W. and P. Smock. 1995. “Why Marry? Race and the Transition to Marriage Among Cohabitors.”<br />

Demography 32:509–20.<br />

Mart<strong>in</strong>, T. and L. Bumpass. 1989. “Recent Trends <strong>in</strong> Marital Disruption.” Demography 26:37–51.<br />

McGue, M. and D. Lykken. 1992. “Genetic Influence on <strong>Risk</strong> of <strong>Divorce</strong>.” Psychological Science<br />

6:368–73.<br />

McLanahan, S. and L. Casper. 1995. “Grow<strong>in</strong>g Diversity and Inequality <strong>in</strong> the American Family.”<br />

Pp. 1–45 <strong>in</strong> State of the Union: America <strong>in</strong> the 1990s, Vol. 2, edited by R. Farley. New York:<br />

Russell Sage Foundation.<br />

McLanahan, S. and G. Sandefur. 1994. Grow<strong>in</strong>g Up With a S<strong>in</strong>gle Parent: What Hurts, What Helps.<br />

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Morgan, S. and R. R<strong>in</strong>dfuss. 1985. “Marital Disruption: Structural and Temporal Dimensions.”<br />

American Journal of Sociology 90:1055–77.<br />

———. 1999. “Reexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the L<strong>in</strong>k of Early Childbear<strong>in</strong>g to Marriage and to Subsequent Fertility.”<br />

Demography 36:59–75.<br />

National Center for Health Statistics. 1998. “National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG) Public-<br />

Use Data Files.” Available on-l<strong>in</strong>e at http://www.cdc.gov/nchswww/products/catalogs/subject/<br />

nsfg/nsfg.htm.<br />

Ono, H. 1999. “Historical Time and Marital Dissolution.” Social Forces 77:969–97.<br />

Oppenheimer, V.K. 1994. “Women’s Ris<strong>in</strong>g Employment and the Future of the Family <strong>in</strong> Industrialized<br />

Societies.” Population and Development Review 20:293–342.<br />

———. 1997. “Women’s Employment and the Ga<strong>in</strong> to Marriage: The Specialization and Trad<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Model.” Annual Review of Sociology 23:431–53.<br />

Plotnick, R. 1992. “The Effects of Attitudes on Teenage Premarital Pregnancy and Its Resolution.”<br />

American Sociological Review 57:800–11.<br />

Raftery, A. 1995. “Bayesian Model Selection <strong>in</strong> Social Research.” Pp. 111–63 <strong>in</strong> Sociological Methodology,<br />

edited by P. Marsden. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton DC: American Sociological Association.<br />

Rockwell, R., G. Elder, and D. Ross. 1979. “Psychological Patterns <strong>in</strong> Marital Tim<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Divorce</strong>.”<br />

Social Psychology Quarterly 42:399–404.<br />

Ruggles, S. 1997. “The Rise of <strong>Divorce</strong> and Separation <strong>in</strong> the United States, 1880–1990.” Demography<br />

34:455–66.<br />

Schoen, R. 1992. “First Union and the <strong>Stability</strong> of First Marriage.” Journal of Marriage and the<br />

Family 54:281–84.<br />

South, S. 1999. “Historical Changes and Life Course Variations <strong>in</strong> the Determ<strong>in</strong>ants of Premarital<br />

Childbear<strong>in</strong>g.” Journal of Marriage and the Family 61:752–63.<br />

———. 2001. “Time-Dependent Effects of Wives’ Employment on Marital Dissolution.” American<br />

Sociological Review 66:226–45.<br />

South, S. and K. Lloyd. 1995. “Spousal Alternatives and Marital Dissolution.” American Sociological<br />

Review 60:21–35.<br />

South, S. and G. Spitze. 1986. “Determ<strong>in</strong>ants of <strong>Divorce</strong> Over the Marital Life Course.” American<br />

Sociological Review 51:583–90.<br />

Sweet, J. and L. Bumpass. 1987. American Families and Households. New York: Russell Sage<br />

Foundation.


<strong>Stability</strong> <strong>Across</strong> <strong>Cohorts</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Divorce</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> <strong>Factors</strong> 351<br />

Teachman, J. 1986. “First and Second Marital Dissolution: A Decomposition Exercise for Whites<br />

and Blacks.” Sociological Quarterly 27:571–90.<br />

———. 2000. “Diversity of Family Structure: Economic and Social Influences.” Pp. 32–58 <strong>in</strong><br />

Handbook of Family Diversity, edited by D. Demo, K. Allen, and M. F<strong>in</strong>e. New York: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Teachman, J. and K. Polonko. 1980. “Cohabitation and Marital <strong>Stability</strong> <strong>in</strong> the United States.”<br />

Social Forces 69:207–20.<br />

Teachman, J., L. Tedrow, and K. Crowder. 2000. “The Chang<strong>in</strong>g Demography of America’s Families.”<br />

Journal of Marriage and the Family 62:1234–46.<br />

Thomson, E. and U. Colella. 1992. “Cohabitation and Marital <strong>Stability</strong>: Quality or Commitment?”<br />

Journal of Marriage and the Family 54:259–67.<br />

Thornton, A. 1989. “Chang<strong>in</strong>g Attitudes Toward Family Issues <strong>in</strong> the United States.” Journal of<br />

Marriage and the Family 51:873–93.<br />

Thornton, A. and W. Rodgers. 1987. “The Influence of Individual and Historical Time on Marital<br />

Dissolution.” Demography 24:1–22.<br />

Turkheimer, E., G. Lovett, C. Rob<strong>in</strong>ette, and I. Gottesman. 1992. “The Heritability of <strong>Divorce</strong>:<br />

New Data and Theoretical Implications.” Paper presented at the annual meet<strong>in</strong>g of the Behavioral<br />

Genetics Association, Boulder, CO.<br />

Waite, L. and L. Lillard. 1991. “Children and Marital Disruption.” American Journal of Sociology<br />

96:930–53.<br />

Weakliem, D. 2001. “A Critique of the Bayesian Information Criterion <strong>in</strong> Model Selection.” Sociological<br />

Methods and Research 27:359–97.<br />

White, L. 1990. “Determ<strong>in</strong>ants of <strong>Divorce</strong>: A Review of Research <strong>in</strong> the Eighties.” Journal of<br />

Marriage and the Family 52:904–12.<br />

Wolf<strong>in</strong>ger, N. 1999. “Trends <strong>in</strong> the Intergenerational Transmission of <strong>Divorce</strong>.” Demography<br />

36:415–20.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!