10.08.2013 Views

Transforming education: the power of ICT policies - Commonwealth ...

Transforming education: the power of ICT policies - Commonwealth ...

Transforming education: the power of ICT policies - Commonwealth ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Digging more deeply into <strong>the</strong>se and o<strong>the</strong>r student outcome studies, it becomes clear that <strong>the</strong> connection between<br />

<strong>ICT</strong> and student learning is a more complicated relationship than one based on mere availability or use. Despite<br />

an o<strong>the</strong>rwise negative relationship, when Fuchs and Woessmann (2004) compared <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> home computers for<br />

communication or <strong>education</strong>al uses <strong>of</strong> home computers, ra<strong>the</strong>r than for gaming, <strong>the</strong>y found a positive relationship<br />

with achievement. In a more recent study in OECD countries (CERI, 2010), <strong>the</strong>re was also a signifi cant correlation<br />

between frequency <strong>of</strong> home use <strong>of</strong> computers and achievement on an international science assessment, even when<br />

socioeconomic contexts are controlled. School use is ano<strong>the</strong>r matter: in <strong>the</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> OECD countries, students<br />

<strong>of</strong> different levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>ICT</strong> use in schools all performed similarly on <strong>the</strong> science assessment. However, it was found<br />

that while a majority <strong>of</strong> students participating in <strong>the</strong> study were regular users <strong>of</strong> computers at home, <strong>the</strong>y did not<br />

use <strong>the</strong>m regularly in school.<br />

Even <strong>the</strong>se analyses must be qualifi ed. It matters how <strong>ICT</strong> is used and what is tested. Student assessments were<br />

specifi c to <strong>the</strong> learning <strong>of</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matics and reading in <strong>the</strong> Fuchs and Woessmann study and <strong>of</strong> science in <strong>the</strong> CERI<br />

study. But <strong>the</strong> data collected on computer use was general; even <strong>the</strong> <strong>education</strong>al use computers was not specifi c<br />

to math, reading, or science. Some studies have looked at this more-direct relationship between <strong>the</strong> topic <strong>of</strong> use <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>ICT</strong> and <strong>the</strong> topic tested. For example, <strong>the</strong> Wenglinsky (1998) study cited above, measured <strong>the</strong> amount computers<br />

were used in ma<strong>the</strong>matics classes and scores on math tests. The study found a positive relationship between <strong>the</strong><br />

use <strong>of</strong> computers and learning in both 4th and 8th grades. Similar positive relationships were found in more recent<br />

studies where computers were used and students tested in ma<strong>the</strong>matics (NCES, 2001a; Cox 2003), science (NCES,<br />

2001b; Harrison, et al., 2003), and literacy (Harrison, et al., 2003). Still, some studies in ma<strong>the</strong>matics found negative<br />

relationships between computer use in math and math scores (Angrist and Lavy, 2001; Pelgrum and Plomp, 2002).<br />

Even here, <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se studies must be qualifi ed. Conclusions in <strong>the</strong>se studies are limited by <strong>the</strong> fact that<br />

<strong>the</strong>y use correlation analyses. This is <strong>the</strong> most common type <strong>of</strong> <strong>ICT</strong> study. But with this type <strong>of</strong> analysis, factors are<br />

simply associated with each o<strong>the</strong>r; causality cannot be established. Yet, this is <strong>the</strong> issue <strong>of</strong> most concern to policymakers.<br />

It cannot be assumed that positive results in such studies were due to computers because, for example,<br />

it may be that <strong>the</strong> brightest students use computers more than less able students and it is student ability that<br />

accounts for higher scores ra<strong>the</strong>r than computer use. Causality can only be assured with controlled experiments,<br />

where one group uses computers or uses <strong>the</strong>m in a certain way and an equivalent group does not. An example <strong>of</strong><br />

this type <strong>of</strong> experimental study was conducted in Vadodara, India (Linden, Banerjee, and Dufl o, 2003) in which<br />

students in primary schools used computer ma<strong>the</strong>matics games two hours a week and students in equivalent<br />

schools did not. The students who used computers scored signifi cantly higher than <strong>the</strong> comparison students on a<br />

test <strong>of</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matics. The bottom group <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> students benefi ted most, and girls benefi ted as much as boys.<br />

Even with experimental studies, conclusions can be drawn with confi dence only when results are consistent across<br />

a substantial number <strong>of</strong> such studies. Kulik (2003) looked at a large number <strong>of</strong> studies in <strong>the</strong> USA that were carefully<br />

designed and he combined <strong>the</strong> results in a meta-analysis to statistically compare outcomes across studies. His<br />

fi ndings across 75 studies can be summarized as follows:<br />

apple Students who used computer tutorials in ma<strong>the</strong>matics, natural science, and social science scored signifi cantly<br />

higher on tests in <strong>the</strong>se subjects. Students who used simulation s<strong>of</strong>tware in science also scored higher.<br />

However, <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> computer-based laboratories did not result in higher scores.<br />

apple Primary school students who used tutorial s<strong>of</strong>tware in reading scored signifi cantly higher on reading scores. Very<br />

young students who used computers to write <strong>the</strong>ir own stories scored signifi cantly higher on measures <strong>of</strong> reading<br />

skill.<br />

apple Students who used word processors or o<strong>the</strong>rwise used <strong>the</strong> computer for writing scored higher on measures <strong>of</strong><br />

writing skill.<br />

Means, et al. (2009) also conducted a meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>ICT</strong> studies. This team examined carefully designed studies<br />

<strong>of</strong> online versus face-to-face learning in K-12 settings published between 2004 and 2008 and identifi ed 51 effects.<br />

Across studies, online learning was signifi cantly more effective than face-to-face. Situations that blended online<br />

learning and face-to-face learning were even more effective.<br />

16 | <strong>Transforming</strong> Education: The Power <strong>of</strong> <strong>ICT</strong> Policies

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!