21.02.2014 Views

Negotiation for Meaning and Peer Assistance in Second Language ...

Negotiation for Meaning and Peer Assistance in Second Language ...

Negotiation for Meaning and Peer Assistance in Second Language ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

416 MEANING AND PEER ASSISTANCE IN SECOND LANGUAGE CLASSROOMS<br />

Tasks<br />

Two similar <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation exchange tasks were used, requir<strong>in</strong>g participants<br />

to <strong>in</strong>terview their partner us<strong>in</strong>g a list of prompt questions. The Japanese<br />

L2 learners worked <strong>in</strong> twos <strong>and</strong> threes (5 dyads <strong>and</strong> 3 triads) to ask about<br />

each other’s op<strong>in</strong>ions <strong>and</strong> plans related to study abroad <strong>in</strong> Japan. The<br />

English L2 learners worked <strong>in</strong> dyads to ask their partner’s impressions of<br />

study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Engl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Data collection <strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

For both data sets audio-record<strong>in</strong>gs were made dur<strong>in</strong>g normal class times<br />

<strong>and</strong> under normal class conditions, that is simultaneously <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the same<br />

room. The first five m<strong>in</strong>utes of each record<strong>in</strong>g were transcribed <strong>and</strong> coded.<br />

Possible confirmation checks, clarification requests, <strong>and</strong> comprehension<br />

checks were <strong>in</strong>itially identified based on turn shape, with cod<strong>in</strong>g ref<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g an exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the function of the utterances as illum<strong>in</strong>ated by<br />

the context (the preced<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g turns), <strong>and</strong> by the <strong>in</strong>terlocutor’s<br />

response. Only utterances which the context <strong>and</strong>/or <strong>in</strong>terlocutor response<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated a problem with comprehension were marked as negotiation<br />

moves. Utterances which had the surface shape of negotiation moves but<br />

which had other functions, were coded as a different category. Thus we<br />

coded negotiations <strong>in</strong> three ways:<br />

1 anyth<strong>in</strong>g with the surface structure of a negotiation move as def<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

by Long (1980);<br />

2 those from (1) which were about verify<strong>in</strong>g underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g or announc<strong>in</strong>g<br />

non-underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g;<br />

3 The rest of (1) i.e. those with the surface structure of a negotiation move<br />

but not clearly related to issues of comprehension;<br />

We coded modified output <strong>in</strong> two ways:<br />

4 modified <strong>in</strong> response to (2);<br />

5 modified <strong>for</strong> any other reason, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g (3) <strong>and</strong> self-<strong>in</strong>itiated repair.<br />

To calculate the <strong>in</strong>cidence of NfM, the data were quantified by division<br />

<strong>in</strong>to AS-units (Foster et al. 2000). These are similar to the T-units <strong>and</strong> c-units<br />

used <strong>in</strong> previous NfM research, (Varonis <strong>and</strong> Gass 1985; Gass <strong>and</strong> Varonis<br />

1985; Doughty <strong>and</strong> Pica 1986; Rulon <strong>and</strong> McCreary 1986; Pica et al. 1989)<br />

but with a much more detailed def<strong>in</strong>ition that allows ‘messy’ spoken<br />

data to be coded <strong>in</strong> a pr<strong>in</strong>cipled way. They divide the spoken word <strong>in</strong>to<br />

syntactically complete units <strong>and</strong> can be used to calculate how frequently<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> features occur <strong>in</strong> numbers of speech samples. Extensive discussion<br />

<strong>and</strong> examples are provided <strong>in</strong> Foster et al. (2000). Interrater reliability was<br />

over 95 per cent on all categories on a ten per cent sample from both<br />

data sets. Follow<strong>in</strong>g Ohta (2001), the data were also exam<strong>in</strong>ed qualitatively

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!