21.01.2015 Views

Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the ... - CI Wiki

Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the ... - CI Wiki

Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the ... - CI Wiki

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

SUPERADDITIVITY IN MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION<br />

NEUROREPORT<br />

1.0<br />

Saccadic RT and <strong>in</strong>verse effectiveness<br />

12.0<br />

Manual RT and <strong>in</strong>verse effectiveness<br />

Normalized (re: visual) RT<br />

0.8<br />

0.6<br />

0.4<br />

0.2<br />

Vis<br />

Aud<br />

V + A<br />

Percentage of reduction <strong>in</strong> RT<br />

10.0<br />

8.0<br />

6.0<br />

4.0<br />

2.0<br />

VA low<br />

T low A low<br />

VA high<br />

VT low<br />

T high A high<br />

VT high<br />

0.0<br />

−21 dB −18 dB −12 dB −6 dB<br />

Auditory signal to noise ratio<br />

Fig. 4 Saccadic reaction time and <strong>in</strong>verse e¡ectiveness. This ¢gure,<br />

which was adapted from Figure 10 of Corneil et al. [7], illustrates <strong>the</strong><br />

relationship between stimulus strength and <strong>the</strong> decrements <strong>in</strong> saccadic<br />

reaction time (RT) result<strong>in</strong>g from comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g visual and auditory stimuli.<br />

Human <strong>in</strong>dividuals were <strong>in</strong>structed to make saccades as quickly as possible<br />

to a visual stimulus, an auditory stimulus, or a spatially congruent<br />

visual^auditory stimulus. Auditory stimuli were presented at di¡erent<br />

signal strengths ( 21, 18, 12, and 6 dB) with respect to background<br />

noise. Plotted are mean RTs for saccades to <strong>the</strong> auditory stimulus alone<br />

(white bars) and <strong>the</strong> auditory^visual stimulus pair (gray bars) as a function<br />

of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g auditory signal strength. All mean RTs are normalized relative<br />

to that for saccades to <strong>the</strong> visual stimulus alone (black bars ^ value of<br />

1.0). As expected, mean RT for <strong>the</strong> auditory stimulus (white bars) decl<strong>in</strong>es<br />

monotonically as a function of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g auditory stimulus strength<br />

(<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g S/N ratio from left to right). Note, however, that S/N noise<br />

ratio also modulates <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>£uence of <strong>multisensory</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration such<br />

that mean RT for <strong>the</strong> cross-modal pair (gray bars) is shorter than that<br />

for <strong>the</strong> auditory stimulus alone (white bars) for <strong>the</strong> weakest auditory<br />

stimulus ( 21dB), but not for <strong>the</strong> most <strong>in</strong>tense auditory stimulus<br />

( 6 dB), a ¢nd<strong>in</strong>g consistent with <strong>in</strong>verse e¡ectiveness. It is also important<br />

to note that <strong>the</strong> apparent RT bene¢ts associated with cross-modal<br />

stimuli were not exclusive to <strong>the</strong> lowest S/N ratio, but appear to be<br />

present and of <strong>in</strong>termediate magnitude for <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>termediate S/N ratios<br />

( 12 and 18 dB).<br />

(eyes, head, ears, body) to <strong>the</strong>se stimuli (see [28,29] for<br />

reviews). It is reasonable to presume that any stimulusrelated<br />

factor that leads to an augmented neural response <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> superior colliculus also <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>the</strong> salience of that<br />

stimulus, and with it <strong>the</strong> probability that it will elicit an<br />

orient<strong>in</strong>g response. It is well established that highly salient<br />

stimuli (e.g. bright, loud) provoke behavioral responses<br />

more reliably and more rapidly. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong>se same<br />

stimuli evoke neural responses that are of shorter latency,<br />

more vigorous, and less variable whe<strong>the</strong>r considered from<br />

<strong>the</strong> perspective of a s<strong>in</strong>gle neuron or populations of<br />

neurons. In many <strong>in</strong>stances, it seems justified to <strong>in</strong>fer a<br />

causal relationship from such behavior/neural correlates<br />

(e.g. greater activity level, shorter reaction time).<br />

Consider<strong>in</strong>g superadditivity as a context-limited phenomenon<br />

with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> broader spectrum of <strong>multisensory</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>teractions, it seems self-evident that <strong>multisensory</strong> behavioral<br />

phenomena, at least those mediated by <strong>the</strong> superior<br />

colliculus, are not wholly dependent on supral<strong>in</strong>ear <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

between <strong>the</strong> senses. Whereas one might reasonably<br />

expect that such <strong>in</strong>teractions contribute to <strong>the</strong> most potent<br />

behavioral effects observed when very weak unisensory<br />

stimuli are comb<strong>in</strong>ed, it should be recognized that simple<br />

0.0<br />

V+A V+T T+A<br />

Fig. 5 Manual reaction times and <strong>in</strong>verse e¡ectiveness. Shown here are<br />

<strong>the</strong> decrements <strong>in</strong> manual reaction time (RT) result<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>the</strong> addition<br />

of a stimulus from a second modality (i.e. cross-modal versus modalityspeci¢c).<br />

They are <strong>in</strong>versely related to stimulus <strong>in</strong>tensity. The data are<br />

taken fromTable 4 of Diederich and Colonius [6]. In <strong>the</strong>ir experiment, human<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals were <strong>in</strong>structed to depress a response button with each<br />

hand upon detect<strong>in</strong>g any stimulus, and manual reaction times were measured.<br />

Stimuli consisted of a visual £ash, auditory pure tone, vibratory<br />

tactile stimulus, or some comb<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>the</strong>se three stimuli. Plotted are<br />

<strong>the</strong> % reductions <strong>in</strong> RT for <strong>the</strong> visual^auditory, tactile^visual, and tactile^<br />

auditory stimulus comb<strong>in</strong>ations. For each pair<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> percent reduction<br />

<strong>in</strong> mean RT is plotted with reference to that for <strong>the</strong> modality-speci¢c<br />

component stimulus that yielded <strong>the</strong> shortest mean RTaccord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong><br />

general formula: % reduction <strong>in</strong> RT¼m<strong>in</strong>(RT 1 , RT 2 ) (RT 1 + 2 )/m<strong>in</strong>(RT 1 ,<br />

RT 2 ) 100, where RT 1 and RT 2 are mean RTs for responses to each of <strong>the</strong><br />

stimulus components and RT 1 + 2 is <strong>the</strong> mean RT for responses to <strong>the</strong><br />

stimulus comb<strong>in</strong>ation. Note that for each cross-modal stimulus, <strong>the</strong> higher<br />

<strong>in</strong>tensity comb<strong>in</strong>ation (gray bars) yielded a proportionally lower reduction<br />

<strong>in</strong> RT than did <strong>the</strong> lower <strong>in</strong>tensity comb<strong>in</strong>ation (black bars), thus<br />

illustrat<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> concept of <strong>in</strong>verse e¡ectiveness applies to manual<br />

RT. Note also that all stimuli were suprathreshold, illustrat<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong><br />

behavioral bene¢ts for <strong>multisensory</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration are not exclusive to near<br />

threshold stimulation and that behavioral bene¢ts, albeit attenuated, are<br />

also observed for higher <strong>in</strong>tensity comb<strong>in</strong>ations. Stimulus pair<strong>in</strong>gs VA low ,<br />

VA high , VT low , VT high , T low A low , T high A high correspond to VA 70 , VA 90 , T 1 V,<br />

T 3 V,T 1 A 70 ,T 3 A 90 , respectively.<br />

summation, and even subl<strong>in</strong>ear comb<strong>in</strong>ations, of <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

<strong>in</strong>puts would be expected to have behavioral manifestations<br />

by virtue of yield<strong>in</strong>g substantial <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> superior<br />

colliculus activity. The predicted <strong>in</strong>verse trend has been<br />

demonstrated empirically for behavior; <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence of<br />

comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g stimuli on behavioral measures such as localization<br />

or reaction time does, <strong>in</strong> fact, decrease with <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g<br />

salience of <strong>the</strong> unisensory components, and this pr<strong>in</strong>ciple<br />

seems to apply equally well to orientation of gaze (e.g. [7];<br />

see Fig. 4) or limb movement (e.g. [6]; see Fig. 5). Note,<br />

however, that <strong>the</strong> examples depicted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se illustrations<br />

suggest that <strong>the</strong> behavioral effects of <strong>multisensory</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegration,<br />

whereas certa<strong>in</strong>ly greatest for <strong>the</strong> weakest stimuli, are<br />

not exclusive to such comb<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

The concept of <strong>in</strong>verse effectiveness as it applies to<br />

behavior is <strong>in</strong>tuitive and, as discussed above, an analog<br />

(and perhaps its neural correlate) is evident <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> responses<br />

of superior colliculus <strong>multisensory</strong> neurons: comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

of very weakly effective modality-specific stimuli result <strong>in</strong><br />

proportionately larger enhancements than do comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

of highly effective stimuli (see Figs 1–3). Given that<br />

<strong>multisensory</strong> phenomena arise via convergence (and, <strong>in</strong><br />

Vol 18 No 8 28 May 2007 791<br />

Copyright © Lipp<strong>in</strong>cott Williams & Wilk<strong>in</strong>s. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!