28.01.2015 Views

human factors

human factors

human factors

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Aviation Maintenance 26-9<br />

8. Human-<strong>factors</strong> guide for aviation maintenance. With so much research and development activities<br />

on <strong>human</strong> <strong>factors</strong> in maintenance and inspection, there is an obvious need to get usable information<br />

for the nonspecialists within the system. Since 1992, a guide has been under development to<br />

codify the <strong>human</strong>-<strong>factors</strong> principles, techniques, and findings for the system participants, such as<br />

managers and supervisors of maintenance and inspection. This guide was produced in CDROM<br />

and hard copy forms in the mid-1990s (Maddox, 1995), and has formed the basis of training and<br />

interventions in the industry.<br />

9. Workcard redesign. As existing workcards were often found to be unsatisfactory from a <strong>human</strong><strong>factors</strong><br />

viewpoint, a project was undertaken to show how they could be improved. The first phase<br />

of this project (Patel, Drury, & Prabhu, 1993) used the <strong>human</strong>-<strong>factors</strong> literature to the determine<br />

principles of information design applicable to workcards, and to design new workcards embodying<br />

these principles. These new workcards were developed as job aids for two distinct types of<br />

inspection. For a C-check, which is a heavy inspection conducted infrequently, inspectors need<br />

detailed guidance on what defects to expect and which areas to search. For the more frequent<br />

A-checks, the inspection is typically the same every day (or more accurately, every night), and<br />

hence, a layered information system is needed. Here, a checklist provides procedural and sequence<br />

information to prevent procedural errors, and more detailed information is available behind the<br />

checklist for reference as needed. Evaluation of the C-check prototype showed highly significant<br />

improvements when the inspectors rated the workcard design (Patel, Drury, & Lofgren, 1994).<br />

Since that study, there has been much interest in using AECMA Simplified English for workcards.<br />

Chervak, Drury, and Ouellette (1996) showed that Simplified English did reduce comprehension<br />

errors. Later, Chervak and Drury (2003) demonstrated that maintenance errors were also<br />

reduced in a simple maintenance task. A design tool for workcards (documentation design aid<br />

[DDA]) was developed by Drury and Sarac (1997), and has been used extensively (available for<br />

download at http://hfskyway.faa.gov). A more recent study of the DDA in outsourced aviation<br />

maintenance (Drury, Wenner, & Kritkausky, 1999) showed that it could reduce half of the comprehension<br />

errors. Currently, Simplified English is one of the several techniques being tested in overseas<br />

repair stations, as a defense against language errors in maintenance (Drury & Ma, 2004).<br />

10. Restrictive space changes. Many aircraft inspection tasks must be performed in restrictive spaces<br />

owing to airframe structural constraints. A study at an airline partner measured the effect of<br />

restrictions on postural accommodations (e.g., movements), perceived discomfort, and perceived<br />

workload (TLX). It was found that it is possible to differentiate between good and poor workspaces<br />

using these measures, and to use the findings to initiate countermeasures in the form of<br />

improved access equipment (Reynolds, Drury, & Eberhardt, 1994). A classification scheme for<br />

restricted spaces was developed to assist this work, and was tested using laboratory simulations of<br />

inspection tasks (Reynolds, Drury, Sharit, & Cerny, 1994).<br />

11. Visual-search training. A comprehensive series of projects used a workstation-based visual<br />

inspection simulator (Latorella et al., 1992) to test the various hypotheses about improvement<br />

of inspection training. For visual-search training, both improvements in defect conspicuity and<br />

improvements in search strategy were sought (Drury & Gramopadhye, 1992). Current inspection-training<br />

procedures are largely either classroom-based, covering theory and regulation, or<br />

on-the-job practice. Neither technique is most appropriate to the skills required in inspection,<br />

particularly, the search skills. One experiment tested a technique of practice on a visual-lobe<br />

testing task and showed that this practice transferred to search the performance for both similar<br />

and perceptually similar defects. The second experiment evaluated both performance feedback<br />

and cognitive feedback as techniques for improving search strategy and performance. It was<br />

found (Drury & Gramopadhye, 1992) that the two types of feedback have different effects, and<br />

hence, both may be needed to obtain the best results.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!