07.04.2015 Views

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

cortex 44 (2008) 1146–1160 1147<br />

the current study we focus on developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia<br />

and show that at least three different loci on the lexical<br />

route can be impaired, causing three different subtypes<br />

of developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia, which differ in the patterns<br />

of performance in various tasks.<br />

When the lexical route is unavailable, readers may be<br />

forced to rely on the grapheme-to-phoneme route for oral<br />

reading. Looking more closely into what can cause the lexical<br />

route to be unavailable, several possible loci of impairment<br />

emerge: one might be the orthographic input lexicon<br />

or the access to it ( in Fig. 1). Another possibility is that<br />

the orthographic input lexicon itself is intact and accessible,<br />

but its output is damaged: either its output both to<br />

the phonological output lexicon and to the semantic system<br />

(marked in Fig. 1), or only the output to the phonological<br />

lexicon (marked in Fig. 1), with intact access to the semantic<br />

system.<br />

Grapheme-to-phoneme reading can also result from an<br />

impairment to the semantic system, to the phonological output<br />

lexicon, or its access to the phonemic output buffer (Jackson<br />

and Coltheart, 2001) but in these cases, <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia is<br />

part of a more general language or semantic impairment, and<br />

is not restricted to reading, and we will not explore these subtypes<br />

in the current study.<br />

The three impairments (1–3 in Fig. 1) are expected to<br />

yield a similar pattern of reading aloud. Since all three impairment<br />

loci cause reading via conversion rules, all three<br />

impairments should result in regularizations in reading<br />

aloud. However, importantly, the three impairments are<br />

expected to differ with respect to their effect on lexical decision<br />

and comprehension. The first impairment – involving<br />

lack of access to the orthographic input lexicon, or impaired<br />

orthographic input lexicon 1 – would result both in<br />

difficulty in lexical decision and in impaired comprehension.<br />

Namely, this deficit will result in inability to determine<br />

whether a letter string forms an existing word,<br />

especially when it can be sounded out via grapheme-tophoneme<br />

conversion as an existing word. The comprehension<br />

in this type of impairment will rely solely on the<br />

phonological output of the grapheme-to-phoneme conversion.<br />

Thus, words that are regular and nonhomophonous might<br />

be understood correctly, but irregular words and homophones<br />

would either not be recognized as a word and<br />

thus not be understood, or be recognized as a different<br />

word and misunderstood. For example, if a word like<br />

‘‘yacht’’ would be read as yakt, the reader might say that<br />

she does not recognize the word. A word like ‘‘sale’’,<br />

when identified solely on the basis of the phonological lexicon,<br />

might be defined as ‘‘To move along the sea with<br />

a boat’’, and a word like ‘‘too’’ might be defined as ‘‘The<br />

number after one’’.<br />

Individuals with an impairment of the second subtype –<br />

impaired connection from the orthographic input lexicon to<br />

both the phonological output lexicon and the semantic<br />

1 Ascribing a deficit to the orthographic input lexicon still leaves<br />

the question open whether the deficit is in processes operating in<br />

and on the lexicon, or whether the representations in the lexicon<br />

are impaired (for example, ‘‘faded’’, and require additional activation<br />

to be accessed).<br />

semantics<br />

orthographic-visual analysis:<br />

letter identification letter position letter-word binding<br />

2<br />

orthographic<br />

input lexicon<br />

2<br />

1<br />

3<br />

phonological<br />

output lexicon<br />

phonemic buffer<br />

grapheme-to-phoneme<br />

conversion<br />

Fig. 1 – A model of single word reading. The numbers<br />

indicate possible loci of impairment that lead to<br />

developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia.<br />

system – are expected to be able to decide whether a letter<br />

sequence is a word or not, even when it is a pseudohomophone,<br />

as they have access to the orthographic input lexicon.<br />

Because they do not have access to semantics, they<br />

will fail in comprehension in much the same way as the<br />

first subtype.<br />

Finally, individuals with an impairment of the third subtype<br />

– whose impairment results from a disconnection between<br />

the orthographic input lexicon and the phonological<br />

output lexicon – are expected to perform well both in lexical<br />

decision tasks and in comprehension tasks, when these tasks<br />

do not involve oral reading. This is because they have intact<br />

access to the orthographic input lexicon and from it to the<br />

semantic system. Only when they read aloud will their <strong>surface</strong><br />

dyslexia be manifested, because they will be forced to<br />

use the grapheme-to-phoneme route. 2<br />

Thus, in order to determine the locus of impairment for<br />

each individual with <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia, reading aloud is not<br />

enough. The performance in lexical decision, and specifically<br />

the ability to reject pseudohomophones (nonwords<br />

that can be sounded out like real words, such as fone), would<br />

indicate whether the orthographic input lexicon is accessible.<br />

Comprehension tasks of homophones might speak for<br />

whether or not there is access to the semantic system<br />

from the orthographic input lexicon (Marshall, 1984a).<br />

In a seminal study of acquired <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia, Coltheart<br />

and Funnell (1987) identified seven loci that might<br />

lead to acquired <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia. They showed that their<br />

2 Theoretically, when the pathway between the lexicons is impaired,<br />

one can also use the route from semantics to the phonological<br />

output lexicon in order to read aloud. However, whereas<br />

this route is the natural route for word retrieval, it does not<br />

seem to be a natural route for reading, and this is why the arrow<br />

appears dashed in Fig. 1. It seems to be a last resort, used only<br />

when no other route is available for reading, as is the case in<br />

deep dyslexia. We will return to this point in the Discussion, on<br />

the basis of our results.<br />

1

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!