07.04.2015 Views

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

cortex 44 (2008) 1146–1160 1149<br />

grapheme-to-phoneme conversion, although some conversions<br />

might be favorable. Even 3-letter words might theoretically<br />

have several thousands of possible readings. For<br />

example, in the word , QBS, each of the three letters can<br />

be converted to either of two consonants, and the vowels<br />

are not represented, so after each consonant letter one of six<br />

vowels can be used. Together with the two possible stress positions,<br />

this leads to 2 2 2 6 6 6 2 ¼ 3456 theoretical<br />

ways to read this word, which the orthographic lexicon narrows<br />

down to three lexical options.<br />

Another result of this structure of Hebrew orthography that<br />

is important for the description of <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia is the abundance<br />

of potentiophones (Gvion and <strong>Friedmann</strong>, 2001; Lukov and<br />

<strong>Friedmann</strong>, 2006). We use the term potentiophones for word<br />

pairs that are written differently and sound differently, but<br />

whose letter sequence can be mapped onto the same sound<br />

string. Therefore when such a word is read solely via grapheme-to-phoneme<br />

conversion, it can be read aloud as the other<br />

existing word, which sounds differently. An example in English<br />

is the word now that can be read as no or know when read<br />

via grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (other examples for<br />

English potentiophones are resent–recent, come–comb, bear–<br />

beer, angle–angel, talk–talc, and whose–hose). Examples for<br />

potentiophones in Hebrew are the pairs (KtR–QTR,<br />

/katar/–/keter/, locomotive–crown), (QMO–KMO, /kmo/–/<br />

kamu/, like–woke up), and (XOL–XBL, /xol/–/xevel/, sand–<br />

rope).<br />

Potentiophones are valuable for the diagnosis of <strong>surface</strong><br />

dyslexia, because reading aloud of these words can already<br />

indicate whether or not the reader used her lexical route<br />

for reading. They are better for the detection of <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia<br />

than homophones, because homophones sound the<br />

same and thus reading aloud cannot indicate whether<br />

they were read correctly or not, and they thus require comprehension<br />

tasks. Furthermore, potentiophones might be<br />

more sensitive to <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia reading than other irregular<br />

words because the reading of irregular words that do<br />

not have potentiophones results in a nonlexical response.<br />

Because some individuals with <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia have<br />

a strong tendency to produce real words as output (Patterson<br />

et al., 1985, p. 12), this nonlexical response may be<br />

identified by the reader as incorrect, and corrected immediately<br />

in an attempt to reach a lexical response. An incorrect<br />

reading of a potentiophone, however, yields an existing<br />

word, and hence does not allow the detection of the<br />

error.<br />

In the current study we used these characteristics of Hebrew<br />

orthography to examine <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia, and more specifically<br />

three subtypes of developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia. We<br />

also explored the way <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia is manifested in<br />

a highly irregular language like Hebrew.<br />

2. Experimental investigation<br />

2.1. Participants<br />

2.1.1. Participants with developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia<br />

The participants with <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia were individuals who<br />

had developmental dyslexia, who were diagnosed with<br />

‘‘learning disabilities’’ or ‘‘reading disabilities’’ prior to the<br />

study. We included them in the study based on the number<br />

and types of errors they made in single word reading. Participants<br />

were included in the study only if their reading aloud<br />

(see Section 2.2.1) included significantly more errors than<br />

the control group, and the type of errors they made in reading<br />

were errors that result from reading via graphemeto-phoneme<br />

conversion: regularizations, errors in vowel<br />

pattern of unvoweled words or words with ambiguous vowel<br />

letters, and potentiophone errors. The comparison of each<br />

participant to her/his control group was done using the<br />

Crawford and Howell’s (1998) t-test for the comparison of<br />

a single participant to a group.<br />

This created a group of 17 participants, six female and<br />

11 male. Their background information is presented in<br />

Table 1. All the participants with developmental <strong>surface</strong><br />

dyslexia had normal IQ, and studied in regular schools<br />

and regular classes. They had normal language, their spontaneous<br />

speech was normal and none of them was diagnosed<br />

with SLI (specific language impairment). Four of the<br />

participants (TM, OM, BZ, and YD) were tested with spoken<br />

language test batteries for the diagnosis of syntactic impairments<br />

(BAMBI, <strong>Friedmann</strong> and Novogrodsky, 2002; BAFLA,<br />

<strong>Friedmann</strong>, 1998), and a test battery for the assessment of<br />

phonological abilities (BLIP, <strong>Friedmann</strong>, 2003), showing performance<br />

within the norm in syntax and phonology. In order<br />

to avoid <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia-like reading that resulted from<br />

lack of sufficient exposure to reading, we only included participants<br />

who were in fifth grade and higher, and who had<br />

effective classroom instruction. Eleven of the participants<br />

were in fifth or sixth grade, four were in middle school,<br />

and two were adults. All of them had Hebrew as their<br />

mother tongue, and one was bilingual and had both Hebrew<br />

and English as mother tongues. They had no hearing<br />

impairment, and none of them had a history of neurological<br />

disease or head trauma that might have led to acquired<br />

Table 1 – Background information on the participants<br />

with developmental <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia<br />

Participant Age Grade Gender Handedness Remedial<br />

teaching<br />

SH 10;8 5 M R Yes<br />

GL 10;10 5 F R –<br />

OF 11;2 5 M R –<br />

YR 11;0 5 M R Yes<br />

TM 12;0 6 F R Yes<br />

NT 12;8 6 M R Yes<br />

OM 14;10 8 M R Yes<br />

BZ 43 Adult M R –<br />

AS 11 5 M R Yes<br />

OS 11 5 F R –<br />

AK 13;9 8 M R Yes<br />

AM 10;7 5 F R –<br />

AL 11 5 M R Yes<br />

KR 11 5 F R –<br />

NF 14;5 8 M R Yes<br />

IR 15;10 9 M L Yes<br />

YD 21 Adult F R Yes

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!